Criminal - Pants On Fire
Episode Date: February 14, 2014For nearly a century we've been trying to read someone's truthfulness by the way they act. Be it through machines, or our own intuition. The police have tried. The FBI has tried. The CIA has tried. Bu...t the fact is… most of their efforts just don't work. Are we doomed to ignorance? Maybe not. We talked to forensic psychologist Andy Morgan about the difficult truth regarding the industry around lying. Say hello on Twitter, Facebook and Instagram. Sign up for our occasional newsletter, The Accomplice. Follow the show and review us on Apple Podcasts: iTunes.com/CriminalShow. We also make This is Love and Phoebe Reads a Mystery. Artwork by Julienne Alexander. Check out our online shop. Episode transcripts are posted on our website. Learn more about your ad choices. Visit podcastchoices.com/adchoices
Transcript
Discussion (0)
Support for Criminal comes from Apple Podcasts.
Each month, Apple Podcasts highlights one series worth your attention,
and they call these series essentials.
This month, they recommend Wondery's Ghost Story,
a seven-part series that follows journalist Tristan Redman
as he tries to get to the bottom of a ghostly presence in his childhood home.
His investigation takes him on a journey involving homicide detectives,
ghost hunters, and even psychic mediums, and leads him to a dark secret about his own family.
Check out Ghost Story, a series essential pick, completely ad-free on Apple Podcasts.
Your own weight loss journey is personal.
Everyone's diet is different. Everyone's bodies are different.
And according to Noom, there is no one-size-fits-all approach. Thank you. for a healthier lifestyle. Stay focused on what's important to you with Noom's psychology and biology-based approach.
Sign up for your free trial today at Noom.com.
What about those that have been trained to lie?
You know, they know how to do this.
They know how to lie in an interrogation.
Is that possible?
Are we able to train people to lie?
Oh, people lie all the time.
I don't know if you have to be trained.
I think people learn as they grow up.
There's all kinds of lies people can tell, right?
You can tell lies about the past, the future, yourself, other people.
You can lie for fun.
You can lie to not be punished.
You can lie because it's your job.
If you're a professional working undercover, you have to lie.
You can tell lies about what you did or what you will do.
Those are all different kinds of lies. So in the context of when people are being questioned by
the police, there's a number of ways in which a person could look, if they are deceptive,
could look absolutely normal. Whereas a truthful person might actually be frightened of the police
and they ironically would look deceptive.
There's this thought that if you want to know if someone is lying,
you should be able to tell by just looking at them,
that people give off signals of dishonesty.
This is huge in TV shows like Sherlock and Lie to Me.
Classic one-sided shrug.
Translation, I have absolutely no confidence in what I just said.
The body contradicts the words. He's lying. It feels so compelling that you should be able to recognize, you know, fear, disgust, anger, sadness, and that these are universal expressions.
And I think that it's true that around the planet, by and large, many emotions are expressed similarly by the facial muscles.
Not entirely, but similarly.
What's really, I think, more questionable scientifically is whether or not these expressions or micro facial expressions,
these quick flashes of human emotions on your face,
are actually signals of lying.
So if your body doesn't matter the way we've thought it has for so long,
then what does?
I'm Phoebe Judge, and this is Criminal.
For 20 years, Andy Morgan has been studying human memory and deception. He's worked with the FBI, CIA,
the military, law enforcement groups. And it goes without saying that all those people Morgan worked with, they really like to know when someone's lying to them. They will do some crazy, expensive,
exhaustive things to find out if someone's lying to them. But what we're learning now
is that a lot of those things they've been trying, they just don't work.
We've been looking for physiological clues since the 1920s with the invention of the lie detector.
This is the Reed polygraph, a lie detector.
In the past 20 years, an estimated 200,000 persons have staked their futures, many of them their lives on this machine.
The polygraph, as you know, is a device that records how fast you're breathing, sort of your
respiration rate, and how much your chest is moving. They put tubes around your chest. And
the general belief in law enforcement and within the polygraph community for years has been rooted
in the idea that we call the fear and alarm hypothesis.
That variations in blood pressure and pulse rate
are present during and after the act of lying.
That telling a lie is threatening in some way,
and that threat to you will trigger a difference, a shift.
The conscious act of lying creates an emotional disturbance.
And cause your blood pressure to rise or your skin conductance to go up or your respiration
rate to change, like holding your breath or maybe breathing more rapidly.
Now, it may not shock you that the polygraph isn't a perfect tool, but we were actually
surprised to learn just how inaccurate the machine is. We actually know the polygraph in the way it is mainly used is not better than chance
at detecting deception, maybe slightly at 52, 52%, 53%.
You could literally flip a coin and be just as accurate as the polygraph at detecting a lie.
But the idea that your body gives you away is still very much alive.
It's still being used in police departments
all over the country.
Cops are trained to read body language.
Some investigators claim they can watch
an interrogation with the sound off
and know if someone's lying.
But what Andy Morgan says is that when put to the test,
people aren't any better at sensing a lie than chance.
And if that's true, that we can't see a lie, that we can't hear a lie or smell a lie,
or physically sense in any way when someone's playing us, then what do we do?
About a decade ago, Morgan and some colleagues decided to test a different technique.
They started interviewing people from all over the world, about 1,200 of them.
So ranging from white Brits to Chinese, East Indians, Lebanese, Jordanians, Moroccans, Sudanese, Afghans, Iraqis, Vietnamese, Russians.
My colleagues and I have tried to study people inside and outside the United States.
And we've mainly used a particular technique called cognitive interviewing.
Cognitive interviewing is based on the premise that memory doesn't work like a video camera.
There are sights and sounds and smells stored deep in your brain
that can all be recalled if pushed hard enough.
So every time Morgan sat down to interview an American or Jordanian, some of them would be
lying and others would be telling the truth. And he would ask them to do something pretty simple.
To tell us a story about what they've been doing.
The most memorable concert that I've been to. It's not the best. It's the most memorable.
So obviously this isn't actually tape from Morgan's research.
We just put some of our friends to the test. What was Yaysayer about a year ago?
I went to a Foo Fighters concert in 2005 for the In Your Honor tour.
And then say, so if you imagine for a minute that I was there with you,
what would I have seen if I had been there with you the entire time?
They had the lights programmed with the music,
and so the mirrors really reflected the light,
and so it would form shapes.
Then we say when they're done.
So I think I'm getting a better picture in my head.
So imagine for a moment I was with you, but this time I was blind,
and I could only listen.
What would I have heard during that
time? So it's more of a shuffling of like rubber against pavement there. Go down metal steps,
go down to the car, our car doors open. Depending on the context, you might say,
what would I have smelled? There was this girl that was standing next to me who was in a sweater
dress who did not wear deodorant. And then we say, starting with the very last thing that happened, what do you remember happening right before that?
Leave dorm.
Right before that.
Go walk backwards to car.
Right before that.
Get in car, come back to dorm.
Kind of having them just walk us backwards through their memory.
Crunching guitars and awesome drum stuff.
What's been found over the years in many studies of cognitive interviewing
is that using those mnemonic prompts,
those sensory prompts,
what you would have seen, heard, smelled,
thought, touched, or tasted,
they trigger more memory recall.
You get more detail.
The picture becomes much more rich and complex
without suggesting anything specific
to the person you're interviewing.
But here's where things get interesting.
Turns out, if you're telling a lie, a made-up story, even one that's well-rehearsed,
you can't complete this interview, not without giving yourself away.
If you tell a very simple lie, there's not much work to do.
You go, I don't know, and you have nothing to say.
Kids do that all the time.
But if you're telling a story that's reasonably complicated and
it's supposed to be believable, because if I'm lying to you, my goal is to sell you the story
I'm telling you and then to leave before you figure out it's not true or not to bring up
anything that might lead you to suspect that it's not true. I'm trying to tell my story and stick
to it. We find that liars are often worried that if they're inconsistent, they will be thought to be lying.
So what they do is they tell you a story.
So if I say, tell me everything that happened, they might tell me quite a wonderful story.
I went to see Florence and the Machine.
It was like in the spring.
But then when I say, so let's go back to the beginning.
So imagine I was with you.
What would I have seen?
You know, heard, thought, smelled, touched, tasted, or walked through it backwards?
The overall result is they have very little to say.
Do you remember the car ride home?
Not really.
They will say things like, well, pretty much just like I told you before.
And they will repeat very closely the same thing they've already told me
so that I'm not learning anything new over time.
The thing is, I don't even really remember.
It's sort of like comparing a digital photograph of your house with the tree in the front yard
and a child's picture of it where there's a house and there's a tree
and there's clouds and there's birds, but there's not a lot of detail.
And what we tend to do when people tell us lies is I think we fill in the blanks.
And that, Morgan says, is where the problem lies.
We fill in the blanks because, of course, we want to believe people.
So Andy Morgan's challenge was to find a way to take these interviews
and analyze them without filling in the blanks.
To do this, he had to put a little distance between himself and the interesting person telling the story.
The way we've analyzed that in most of our studies is we record the interviews and we do a transcript,
and we let the computer just count the number of words that comes out of a person's mouth in the interview
and the number of unique words that comes out of a person's mouth in the interview and the number of
unique words that comes out of their mouth. So if you think about the phrase,
one small step for man, one giant leap for mankind,
one giant leap for mankind, there are 10 words, but you've used one and four twice, so there's only eight unique words.
As you're thinking harder about monitoring what you're saying, it has the side effect of reducing the richness of what you have to say and shortens the thing that you have to say.
All the computer is doing is counting those two variables. And when we sort people based on those two variables,
response length and unique word count,
the computers write typically 80 to 85% of the time.
Whereas our professional raters,
whether they come from Homeland Security or the FBI or the DIA
or other intelligence groups or law enforcement,
our human raters are rarely better than 54% correct.
It's kind of a crazy idea that someone with a transcript and a tally sheet could tell us
whether or not what we're remembering is accurate,
while someone with decades of FBI interrogation training could be flying blind.
And this idea could have all sorts of huge implications.
Think about a jury.
A jury is just a bunch of people trying to figure out who to believe.
But people, according to Andy Morgan, are terrible at figuring out who to believe.
Isn't it possible that a juror with a transcript of the trial,
who's never laid eyes on the accused,
might actually do a better job at figuring out who's lying, who to trust?
For Andy Morgan's part, he's glad our bodies aren't the end-all be-all.
It's reassuring to know that although there's so many books on nonverbal behavior
and people say 90% of communication is nonverbal,
based on the science,
I think that the best way to sort out the truth
is to listen to what people have to say.
And to know that maybe our fate won't hang on sweaty palms
or a pounding heart,
that our words really do matter.
Thanks to Andy Morgan, who's a forensic psychologist at Yale,
and just in case you were curious, during his interview with us, he used more than 7,200 words.
And enough unique words to make us trust he wasn't lying.
The show is produced by Lauren Spohr, Eric Menel, and me.
I'm Phoebe Dredge, and this is Criminal.
Support for this show is brought to you by Nissan Kicks. We'll see you next time. revamped for urban adventure. From the design and styling to the performance, all the way to features like the Bose Personal Plus sound system, you can get closer to everything you love about
city life in the all-new, reimagined Nissan Kicks. Learn more at www.nissanusa.com
slash 2025 dash kicks. Available feature, Bose is a registered trademark of the Bose Corporation.
Do you feel like your leads never lead anywhere?
And you're making content that no one sees, and it takes forever to build a campaign?
Well, that's why we built HubSpot.
It's an AI-powered customer platform that builds campaigns for you,
tells you which leads are worth knowing, and makes writing blogs, creating videos, and posting on social a breeze.
So now, it's easier than ever to be a marketer. Get started at HubSpot.com slash marketers.