Decoding the Gurus - Interview with Michael Inzlicht on the Replication Crisis, Mindfulness, and Responsible Heterodoy
Episode Date: May 20, 2022It's been a while but don't worry the DtG elves have been hard at work and a veritable bounty of content is on its way. The long-promised Jaron Lanier decoding is on its way next week, but this week t...he cross-overs continue as we are joined by Mickey Inzlicht, esteemed Psychologist, Research Excellence Faculty Scholar at the University of Toronto, and long term (retired) co-host of the Two Psychologists, Four Beers podcast.Mickey has now hung up his podcasting headphones but like an old prizefighter, we were able to lure him back into the limelight one last time with promises of unlimited booze and global fame. To keep Mickey from realising we could provide neither, we then subjected him to an unrelenting barrage of questions for almost two hours. Under our relentless questioning, Mickey gave up the goods on some precious long-buried information, including what it's like to work with Jordan Peterson, the details on his campaign to destroy introspection, and what he really thinks of the Gurus. We also manage to discuss some serious stuff like the state of contemporary psychology, the impact of the replication crisis, whether preregistration is always beneficial (it is, don't listen to Matt!), and to resolve the fundamental nature of the Self!Mickey is a wise egg, a funny guy, and a veteran podcaster and we really enjoyed this conversation so we hope you will too! Stick around at the end for some Tamler themed feedback and more pronunciation errors than you can shake a stick at.Back next week with Jaron Lanier!LinksMickey's HomepageTwo Psychologists Four Beers 27: Against MindfulnessBernard Schiff's Article on Jordan Peterson for the Toronto Star: I was Jordan Peterson’s strongest supporter. Now I think he’s dangerous.Inzlicht, M., & Friese, M. (2019). The past, present, and future of ego depletion. Social Psychology.Friese, M., Loschelder, D. D., Gieseler, K., Frankenbach, J., & Inzlicht, M. (2019). Is ego depletion real? An analysis of arguments. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 23(2), 107-131.Guardian article about that Facebook StudyHoehl, S., Keupp, S., Schleihauf, H., McGuigan, N., Buttelmann, D., & Whiten, A. (2019). ‘Over-imitation’: A review and appraisal of a decade of research. Developmental Review, 51, 90-108.
Transcript
Discussion (0)
Hello and welcome to Decoding the Gurus, the podcast where an anthropologist and a psychologist
listen to the greatest minds the world has to offer, and we try our best to understand
what they're talking about.
I'm Professor Matt Brown, and with me is Associate Professor Chris Kavanagh.
G'day Chris, how's it going?
Good morning, Matt.
Good day, in the lingo of your people.
How was your morning swim?
Refreshing?
It was refreshing.
I think I'm getting over the hump.
And I've told you this, but I've lost almost five kilograms.
And at least some of that is an underestimate because it's been transmuted into muscle.
Yeah.
Also known as muscle.
I was going to say, I was going to mention, but you look like someone that's lost five
kilos.
The unfortunate thing is that I'm at the beginning of that journey. I'm going to lose five kilos. So it's very annoying to hear someone report that, babe, I've just lost five kilos. Oh,
good for you, Matt. Good for you. It's all about state of mind. You just need willpower
and some moral fiber. That's all you need, Chris. Competitive drive. I've got to beat you.
That's fine.
I'll get the six kilos.
10 kilos of muscle.
But Chris, Chris, this is weird.
We're not alone.
There's somebody else here.
We're watching.
Yeah, we have an audience.
We have a guest and somebody
who I've been trying to wrangle
onto the podcast for like a year.
Not his fault.
This is my ability to schedule things. trying to wrangle onto the podcast for like a year. Not his fault.
This is my ability to schedule things. We have Mickey Inslick from formerly Two Psychologists for Beers podcast, which is still ongoing,
but without Mickey, he's retired now, an emeritus podcaster.
But he's also the professor in the Department of Psychology at the University of Toronto, Jordan Peterson's old aunt.
And I see on your website, Mickey, that you are a research excellence faculty scholar, not just a professor, research excellence scholar.
So welcome, Mickey, and thanks for coming.
Well, thank you so much for having me on.
It's quite a thrill to be on.
I listen to you guys nearly every episode, and it's just fun to be here.
And yes, I'm glad that you introduced me as being a professor in Jordan Peterson's former department.
I also co-author a number of papers with him.
This is true.
Oh, really?
I did.
So it's interesting, Ricky.
We've got some questions for you.
I guess I must say on probably the entrance to your department now that Jordan Peterson's former department fellow.
He's now an Emeritus professor as well, right?
He was given Emeritus status.
I think he just claimed Emeritus recently.
goodbye letter in the National Post, which is a conservative-leaning national newspaper in Canada,
where he exoriated the department and all that he was put through and just how terrible it is to be a professor these days in general. It was not well-received.
Yeah, but they gave him the emeritus thing anyway.
I mean, you know, I think the rules at U of T are simply, I think it's, you have to be a professor
for 20 years and then you get to essentially retire, but you have some, I think it's, you have to be a professor for 20 years and then you
get to essentially retire, but you have some, I think as an emeritus, you get like office space,
potentially you might have an ability to still conduct research if you want it. I don't think
he wants to do any of those things. I think he wants that U of T still, you know, affiliated
with his name, I'm guessing. We don't need to spend much time on this, but it did seem a very
odd thing because like Jordan Peterson,
whatever you think of him, fabulously successful, right?
Wrote huge bestselling books, has sold out tours, the lecture to young men and various
other people who might be interested in what he has to say.
But he hasn't been teaching in a university for, I would estimate, five or six years or however long since he rose
to prominence. I think it was 2015. It was probably his last, maybe possibly 2016.
Yeah. So after seven years of not teaching any classes, not like probably even being at the
university, it's not the greatest surprise in the world that the university might want the year to move on from being somebody
that's presented as an active participant of the department. The vitriol seemed, some might
describe it as ungrateful. My reaction to that essay or op-ed was, I mean, it's kind of bullshit.
My reaction to that essay or op-ed was, I mean, it's kind of bullshit.
You left because you're incredibly successful, incredibly rich, and you have a platform that's far, far larger than anything that a university classroom can offer you.
So just be open about that.
You could say that other stuff, other stuff that bothers you, but that's not the reason you quit.
That's just kind of nonsense.
Yeah.
Yeah. That was kind of obvious to me too.
it and that's just kind of nonsense yeah yeah that was kind of obvious to me too mickey this wasn't an initial question but i promise that we won't force you to just endlessly
talk about cultural figures but having jordan peterson in the department and the university
of toronto becoming for a little while at least the ground center of campus outreach, right? There were the videos with the students confronting Jordan and that kind of thing.
So I wonder, as somebody in that department and somebody that had a podcast, which also
at times touched on culture war topics and heterodox issues, how was that?
Did that affect you in any tangible way, like being at the university or the student response?
Or was it very tangential to your academic life and all that kind of stuff?
Yeah, I would say more the latter.
So the University of Toronto is such a great place to work and more or less leave you alone.
So, you know, there isn't that almost Hollywood image of what the academic life is like.
Wild faculty meetings, people, you know, conversing in the hallways.
A lot of people do that as well, but it's a little bit more.
Every professor has their own lab and they're kind of little fiefdoms.
So we're all very, very autonomous and independent.
So it didn't impact me too much.
But of course, I was influenced by it.
We would get these mass emails.
People would be very, very angry.
I would decide to email the entire department of psychology, deriding him. We actually had, I think, a philosophy grad
student once carefully break down his arguments about gender and gender identity and why they
were fallacious. At the very least, it was entertaining, but I wouldn't say negatively
impact me. My own personal response to it was at first I was a bit aghast at what he said,
but then I thought a bit more about it and sat with it a bit more. And I'm like, okay,
he had an opinion about a controversial topic and he had an incredible amount of pushback.
It seemed the pushback, at least at the time, was out of proportion with what he actually said and
did. And then I became quite sympathetic to him. And then it kind of warmed up a little bit.
But I must admit that the longer he's been in the spotlight, the less sympathy I have for him.
And now I really don't have much at all.
I saw a piece written by, I guess it's somebody in your department, the person that was like one of the primary movers in hiring Jordan originally.
And he wrote a, it was quite personal.
Ernie Schiff. And he wrote a, it was a quite personal, yeah, and a personal piece, but also relatively balanced talking about the good qualities he saw and the things that he admired in Jordan, which he still sees.
But he had these concerns about the teaching method and the tendency to mix speculative idiosyncratic theories with more established research and not carefully delineate between the two of them.
And they also covered Jordan's disdain for the IRB process or the notion that anybody
should have the right to question the ethics of your research. And I know that Matt, you and me
have all dealt with IRB boards in ethics review, and that they are often a bureaucratic nightmare to deal with.
That piece struck me that it sounded like even before his fame, that there was some
kind of conflicts in the department. Maybe it was only between him and the review board and
that kind of thing, but it sounded like there had been conflict about people complaining about
some of the content of lessons and then other people, like a very strong devotion to him
from his students.
Was that dynamic notable beforehand in the department or was it like just a very fringe
topic that nobody like would have paid attention to because everyone's in their silos?
Yeah, his nature, his polarizing nature was evident well before he rose to prominence and to fame.
So that article you're describing where Bernard Schiff was talking about Jordan complaining about
the Ethics Review Board, I was witness to this because he had cc'd a letter to the IRB and he
cc'd the entire department to complaining about what he thought was a heavy-handed oversight.
And I agree with both you and Matt
in the sense that, yes,
IRBs sometimes are bureaucratic
and sometimes they're petty,
but they're clearly required.
And some of the critiques of the IRB
that he levied were so facile.
They were like,
how can someone who's got a master's degree
criticize me?
What does the degree you have have anything to do with whether something is seen as potentially harmful to a participant? So it was just like
he was in a position of authority and only people who had his level of education or his level of
status could opine on his work. And that was just silly nonsense. But it went both ways.
He was a much loved professor.
I would say in modestly positive reviews in my teaching evaluations.
But I don't think ever, maybe one time, I've had one student say that my class changed their life.
But these were regular comments that he would get every single semester.
Like a third of his students would be spellbound by it and say, this class changed my life.
And it was a regular occurrence.
It was just bizarre.
Until you actually spend some time in a room with the man. And I know you guys talk about what it's like to be a guru. You've got your gurometer. I mean, that man just has charisma.
And by that, I mean, there's something magnetic about him. I remember very clearly, I was maybe
my second or third year professor and we had a little party. He came with his wife and he was
just in the back, not really holding court, but whoever was around him could not look away. There's
something about the way he speaks. You guys mentioned confidence. I think it's also the
musicality of the way he speaks. There's just something captivating about him. And I know one
other person in my life who also has got this special power, the special power of charisma. And no matter what he does, it doesn't matter what he does,
he will have followers. He will have people who will agree with him and go to the end of the
earth with him. Yeah. No, no. Full points for style. I mean, Robert Wright also mentioned that
he said just one thing that all of the gurus we look at have in common is that they're just good
talkers. Unlike Chris, for instance, they don't say I'm an R a lot.
And yeah, they're just very eloquent. So that's part of it. The other part of it is the charisma
and some level, yeah, they have to have it. And I've watched a few of his videos and even the
ones, which are the attack videos. Have you guys seen the one where they've spliced it,
where he's talking about rats? You're the rat and the rat goes, it's courage ready. But
even with that extremely unsympathetic edit and talking about something that's probably
nonsense, like his delivery is clearly compelling, far better than mine.
Yeah.
I'm tempted to adopt his time traveler fashion style for my lectures next year.
Like just to give me a bit, you know, I'm not going to go.
I seen where that leads, like showing up a Joe Rogan in a tuxedo, like he was just missing a rose. So I'm not going to go, I've seen where that leads, like showing up a Jewel Rogue in a tuxedo, like he was just missing a rose.
So I'm not going to go that far.
But a monocle, maybe a pocket watch?
I just don't know.
I think you should do it.
Yeah, you should do it.
You've got to go full steampunk.
You've got to look like you're in a Jules Verne novel.
Yeah, tough hype with a little clockwork clock and stuff.
Yeah.
So this is why we won't be as successful.
But Mickey, we didn't just bring you here to talk about Jordan Peterson.
That was up there on that list of very private.
But the other thing is that you are a psychologist, social psychologist.
That's all right.
That's not a slur to call you.
No, I'm a social psychologist, but I'm kind of in between social psychology and cognitive
psychology.
Yeah.
No, I'm a social psychologist, but I'm kind of in between social psychology and cognitive psychology.
Yeah.
And you've been really on the front lines, I would say, in the debates around the replication
crisis and associated methodological reform efforts in psychology.
So I think most of our audience would already be familiar with the replication crisis in
psychology, but summarize the past 10 years in psychology.
And if you were given a primer for the replication crisis and the aftermath,
what's your potted summary of the past decade or so?
Yeah, we have about 70 episodes of a podcast that cover this.
So I'm not sure I could do it in a couple of sentences.
You got five minutes, Mick.
All right, five minutes. I'll try my best.
I think the basic idea is that for a long time,
the history of our field, let's say,
and I'll talk specifically about social psychology.
I think it's broader than social psychology,
but I'll talk about social psychology specifically
because I think that's really ground zero.
That's the center of the crisis.
For our history, we mostly conduct experiments.
It's an experimental field.
And we assume that the results of these
lab experiments were reliable and they revealed little truths. I mean, I think sometimes when we
were especially proud of ourselves and grandiose, we thought maybe we revealed like bigger truths,
but truths nonetheless, small or big, these little factoids that we discovered. And then we became
so confident in our findings. You know, when I was a grad student, there was a big push in the American, actually now
it's called the Association for Psychological Science, to give away psychology.
We're so confident in the quality of our wares that we thought, you know, we need to
have a far more of an impact in the real world and start giving it away.
And we should be as influential as economists.
And I was skeptical at the time that this would do anything.
But my God, the landscape in the past 20 years
has changed dramatically for psychology
in the sense of how visible it is.
When I was a grad student,
yeah, you would see psychology in the news
every once in a while, like basic research findings,
but now it's every single day,
every single day in multiple papers
that are talked about and discussed in the media.
So we're really, by some metrics, influential.
And we became very proud of ourselves. We're doing good work. We're influencing the world, influencing public
policy, as we should be, because we're scientists. But then, really, I would say, I think the date is
2011, typically, when people think of the start of the replication crisis, or at least the awareness,
broad awareness, that, whoa, something is not right. Maybe a lot is not right.
There were a series of papers published in that year.
One that just blew my mind.
It still affects the way I think deeply.
And it's this paper written by Joe Simons, Leif Nelson, and Uri Simonson.
They have a blog called Data Collada.
They're brilliant economists and psychologists.
And they outlined that the standard operating procedures
that are done in psychology, but also in medicine, in economics, in other quantitative fields that
use inferential statistics, that these tools that we've been using, we've been using them
incorrectly. I think a lot of us kind of knew that we were using them incorrectly, but these same
folks, the data cloud of folks, they said, yeah, we kind of knew, but we thought it was, and we knew it was maybe wrong,
but we thought
it was wrong, like jaywalking as well.
Yeah, okay, I shouldn't be doing
this, but it's fine. And to translate
that for the scientists, like, yeah, okay, I probably
shouldn't be taking a couple of these shortcuts,
but the data are there. I'm just letting
the data speak to me, reveal what they
have to say. But it's not
bad like jaywalking is bad.
It's bad like robbing a bank is bad or murder is bad. It doesn't just lead us to make small errors.
It leads us to make massive errors. In fact, it's possible and very likely now that a field,
an area of social psychology that I know well, that's by some people's count has 600 empirical
studies supporting it. It's possible that all of those 600 studies are just noise mining.
Even though there's 600 supposed studies that indicate an effect of what
doesn't really matter what it is.
It's likely that none of those effects are real.
And how can that be?
And these are independent labs all over the world over a few decades doing
this.
How is that possible?
And well,
the first is what I already mentioned.
We've kind of abused our inferential tools.
By that, I mean, like,
we've been doing our statistics incorrectly.
So we've been cutting corners,
be that running small sample studies.
I see, Matt, you want to get in there.
Sorry.
Oh, I just wanted to ask you,
are you referring to that the ego depletion effect?
Is that what you're having to be?
Yeah, I was referring to ego depletion.
This is an area that I was involved with as a critic, a theoretical critic.
And I was very proud.
So I had this alternative model of how ego depletion, ego depletion is just like a model
of how self-control works.
I'm very patting myself on the back and I criticize the theory.
But for me, I still needed the evidence to be real, the phenomenon to be real.
I have a new explanation.
But then a year or two later, it turns out there's no phenomenon to even need to explain it. Yeah. Yeah. Exceedingly painful. And without overstating it,
I would say it caused dysphoria, maybe mild depression in me for a number of years. I've
been working on this, really thought this is real. Can you imagine you're working on something,
you're building something, you're building a house, you think it's solid and you're getting
other people to look at your house. Hey, beautiful house, great house. You're winning awards for how beautiful
your house is. And then one day it's like, there's no house there. Yeah. Or you built it,
you forgot to put in the foundation. So it's all a waste of time. And that's one of the most
soul-destroying parts of this, isn't it? Which is that once the process gets going, so many graduate students and professors devote so many years and so much money
to going on and building on something that turns out to be a waste of time. Yeah. So it's important.
Yeah. I think you're right to characterize it as akin to murder, major crime, and not a misdemeanor
just because of the sheer waste apart from anything else right and so i would say there
are a few ingredients to like and by the replication crisis and some people that you
whether we should even call it that some people call it a credibility revolution so you'll want
to talk about the open science movement but whatever you want to call it the the key factors
are abusing or inferential tools so otherwise known as questionable research practices or p
hacking that's one another one one. And that's,
I would say, relatively easy to solve. And I think we're on the way to doing a lot better.
We haven't solved it, but we're doing a lot better now. But some of the other factors are
much harder. So the second one is something called publication bias. And that's just this notion that
journals, editors, authors themselves, reviewers, they do not want null findings. They don't want someone to
have an hypothesis and then, oh, sorry, the hypothesis is incorrect. You have a tough time
publishing null findings, even though null findings, not always, but oftentimes are very
informative. They can tell you, oh, this idea we had and the way we operationalized this idea,
that didn't work. And if you give it a real honest try and tried multiple different ways,
you can maybe have some more confidence that maybe the idea is just not correct and that's
needed. But those, you don't see those in the literature. And in fact, the last testament I
saw this in psychology, the rate of positive findings in the field is something like 93%.
That means 93% of scholars' hypotheses are confirmed, at least in the published literature.
And anyone who does science, is an active scientist, knows that's nonsense.
I would say I have, you know, two or three failures for every one success that I have.
And I don't think I'm a particularly poor scientist.
I just think we have questions, we wonder, and oftentimes our meandering questions are wrong.
So that's the second one.
I would say a third contributor to this replication crisis,
and by that I mean things not replicating. When tried independent scholars to try it again to see if you can just get the same effect just a second time. The third factor is that no one
was doing conducting replications, or at least not what we'd call close replications or exact
replications. Some people were, but they could not be published either. So these three things led to a warping of an entire literature. And I think this culminated
in a landmark study in 2015 by the Open Science, what are they, Collaboration, I think they called
it. It was a group of 200 plus authors trying to replicate 100 established findings from two
prominent journals. And I think they were able to replicate 39%
of those studies, but in social psychology, only 25% replicated. And I don't think we should hold
that number to be, that's a holy number because there's all different reasons why that could be.
And for sure, there are going to be some errors there. And also we don't want replication rate
to be a hundred percent either. That would also be a bad sign. If you're replicating everything,
that means you're testing things that are obviously true and you're not exploring and taking risks. You need to take
risks as well. But I think we all can agree that 25 percent or 39 percent are probably true.
So, Mickey, I think you did a good job summarizing and also highlighting some of the reasons that
the whole crisis was able to unfold. But I wonder, since that situation has happened and you've
talked about, and we've talked in other
episodes about the open science movement and attempts to make pre-registration more common
and replications rewarded and many lab studies are becoming more common and that kind of thing.
I'm curious about your view of the state of the field currently.
And there's kind of two sentiments that I see quite commonly, which
operate in kind of polar opposition.
And one of them is that is prevalent in the heterodox sphere, which is that
the replication crisis has revealed how little we can trust social science,
how thoroughly politicized findings
are, and that in essence, we would be better starting from scratch or ignoring entirely
the output of the psychology.
So that's on one pole.
And on the other side, you have people who emphasize that social psychologists are the
ones that are leading most of the reform efforts.
They're the people who identified it. And even if you go back to the literature 50 years ago,
but even in 1998 and so on, there was a paper by Carr talking about harking, hypothesizing
after results are known. So it isn't that there was no one in the field that was aware of the problem, so that, you
know, wasn't pushing back about it.
And so there's maybe a more optimistic take that things are changing and there always
were people doing good work in the field and the more pessimistic one that we need to slash
and burn everything that existed.
And I wonder where you fall in your position like currently
when you look at the state of the field?
Yeah, that's a really good question.
I mean, I think I'm definitely not of the mind
that we just start over, burn it all down,
and that this is showing how corrupt psychology is
and that psychology can't be trusted,
or science in general, social science in general
can't be trusted.
I don't think that's correct.
I'm reminded of, I think it's a quote
attributed to Churchill,
democracy is a terrible form of government,
except it's better than every other one around.
I think it's the same thing with science.
It might not be a great way of knowing things,
but it's better than other ways of knowing things.
And why isn't science a great way of knowing things?
Because we make mistakes, sometimes decades-long mistakes.
The people, scientists have egos just like everyone else,
and they have their pet
theories they want to promulgate. So it's definitely imperfect. But what is beautiful about science
is that criticism is not only allowed, it's encouraged. And the peer review process for
all its failings, I think Liam Bright, I believe, was one of your podcast guests. He was saying the
peer review, you know, might not do much. I was a bit skeptical about that take, but I'm open to it.
He's a philosopher, Mickey.
He's a philosopher.
But the point is, whether it works or not, criticism is certainly open and welcome.
And when you have enough people speaking their minds and criticizing, hopefully errors can be avoided or at least eventually you can find the errors, track them down and then not start over, but make amends and do better.
And that's what we're seeing now. We're seeing a massive correction.
We're seeing a massive correction and things have improved quickly. 10 years is a really short period of time for a field to change. And we're seeing norms of change quickly in some
things like sample sizes. You'd be laughed out of any journal if you tried to publish
with what was a normal sample size just 10 years ago, you wouldn't get even in a low quality journal anymore. So that's a massive change. Open data,
open materials, that's far more common. Pre-registration I know is controversial.
I'm definitely on the pro side. I believe that. And I think I'm pro because it's good for the
scientist him or herself. You only have to conduct a write-out pre-registration and then wait a year
or two until you get your data to realize how valuable it is. You're like, oh, that's what I thought
was going to happen. That's what I was going to do. Because a year or two passes, maybe you peek
at the data, maybe you have different ideas, and all of a sudden you want to analyze things
differently or talk about things differently. And pre-registration is not a handcuff. You should
be able to analyze things as you'd like, but stick to what you planned as well. And we need that for our statistics. We need that for confirmatory statistics, which our statistics
are. So things are definitely better. Yeah. I've got a couple of thoughts. I mean,
one of them is that a lot of this discussion is centered around kind of experimental
methodologies, right? Where pre-registration makes a lot more sense and there is more focus on detecting whether
an effect is there or not. And like I myself tend to work in more large-scale survey type
population representative things where we're not really testing hypotheses, we're really
describing things and we focus more on effect size and there's a super fluidity of significant relationships. And we aren't really
concerned with that quite so much. On the other hand, I do tend to, I struggle a little bit with
the pre-registration of this kind of thing as just a working stiff statistician person.
Yeah. I mean, my response to that is I agree with you that pre-registration is not for every field
and every area, but I think even what you're describing, you can do a you that pre-registration is not for every field and every area, but I think
even what you're describing, you can do a form of pre-registration. So for example, if you're
working with a massive data set, you have the luxury of power, statistical power. So you could
explore to your heart's content on half the sample or two thirds of the sample or whatever size of
the sample, explore, find relationships, and then confirm with that sample
that you'd not examine. And then you have a robustness check. So that's a kind of pre-registration
that's happening already, and it's built into that kind of data set. I'm just going to second
Mickey and bully you because I'm probably more extreme in the view that I rarely see any instance where pre-registration wouldn't be of benefit.
And primarily, as Mickey suggests, to the researchers themselves, if not to the broader
scientific community, because one, it does do the thing of letting the outside audience
being aware of what you expected in advance.
But I find in my own experience with research groups that it reminds people what they originally
thought.
And that often changes as people are looking at data and they, you know, just because of
the way human minds work, we can trick ourselves into thinking, well, that's what I expected
all along.
But when you have a record, you're like, well, I didn't mention it though previously.
And it can help you detect motivated reasoning. So I know that there's lots of tricks people can
do, like they can pre-register things and not adhere to them, or they can make the pre-registration
very big and get a nice badge or like pre-register one study out of four and get a badge and so on.
So I'm not saying it's a silver bullet but
including with secondary data sets that just saying these are the analysis that i'm going
to run these are the patterns that we are expecting to find i find very little keys for
why it would be a bad thing to do that in advance instead of after all right i'm going to reply
and here's the caveat right i'm speaking to reply. And here's the caveat, right?
I'm speaking in very personal terms here, just to gut reactions, right?
And in principle, I'm totally on board, right?
It's definitely, it's a good thing to do.
But just for me personally, like how I work as a statistician is if I find
something, I tend to analyze it again in every different way that I can.
And if it doesn't hold up in all of the
different ways, there's multiple different ways to frame it, then I often will reject a lot of
things, right? I appreciate that a lot of people don't do that or don't necessarily have the tools
to be able to do it. The other thing too, for me personally, again, is that our pre-registration
like we do a lot of contracted research. So we have to make quite a detailed grant proposal and there's a contract
where we undertake to answer these specific questions and do this particular methodology.
There's a 40 page thing that's detailing what we are planning to do and we have to do that.
So we kind of, I'm just... That's pre-registration.
Yeah. Yeah.
That's a really interesting case
because now you're contracted to do very specific things.
And as long as that contract is detailed enough
and not super vague,
then that's a form of pre-registration.
And also I would say the other thing you're doing
is also a form of like with the multiverse analysis, right?
Trying to analyze your thing 20 different ways
and only being confident when like the grand sum of them kind of lead in the same direction.
Contrast that with, I think, what was standard operating procedures pre-2011.
And that would be, yes, I would do that.
I would test my thing five different ways.
It would work once.
And I'd only report for one.
And not even bother mentioning that I measured it three other, four other ways. The reason why I fundamentally agree with you is that I also advise a lot of graduate students who will naturally do that,
not out of any ill will or maliciousness, but just because they want a good result. They want
success in their mind. Yeah. So motivated reasoning is quite a drug, right? So you can convince
yourself very easily that this one variable that's correlated with the thing you care about, and that one's significant, that's more important than the three other ones that are consensually... consultation with maybe more experienced academics. They'll have a theory. They'll have expectations.
They'll do these literature reviews and they'll essentially be convinced that what they're looking for is there. And then when they go and do their analysis and they don't find the thing
that they expect to be there, their natural conclusion is that they have done something
wrong and they need to work harder to find the thing that should be there. So I agree with you,
really. I'm just
being annoying. I mean, yeah, science is cruel. I mean, we tested these ideas and we think we're
so right and whoa, I guess not. Or at least this version of the way we've tested it didn't work
out. I have a bit of a left field question because in part of your initial framing of this,
one of the sort of implicit assumptions there, which is that the scientific framework, right, that epistemic
is the way to answer questions about human nature and society and how humans interact with society
and so on. Now, there's a lot of fields that are adjacent to society and many psychologists or
people working explicitly in it, including some anthropologists, am I right, Chris, who don't
subscribe to that epistemology and would say that there are other ways to do it around building. It uses methods that are drawn from philosophy or even literature analysis. I've got my own opinion about this, but I'm just wondering, I see two epistemics colliding, and this is where we're venturing on culture war war territory but i think it's an interesting rubbing up of two ways of knowing yeah i mean yeah it's a really interesting
point i mean you're holding my feet at the fire about you know science the best way of knowing
things i still want to stand by it and at one point i thought you were going to say well there
are other methods for example there's qualitative approaches you could do like focus groups and i
would that's still science that's still's still a way of understanding the world.
But then you start talking about the humanities, what they can provide.
And I don't want to say they certainly provide lots of things.
I suspect the best psychology is not found in psychology.
It's found in literature.
Anthropology.
Don't give it to him.
Don't give him that point.
Okay, classical anthropology.
So absolutely, there are other ways of knowing
but where it always comes down to for me is how do we know how can we test whether this intuition
that we have or the observation that we have is some general small g general truth right and just
because i've observed it or i think it it's hard to verify that without some systematic form of
counting of having controls and again now we're talking about the scientific method so it or I think it, it's hard to verify that without some systematic form of counting,
of having controls. And again, now we're talking about the scientific method. So
again, I think these approaches absolutely enrich our lives and are absolutely needed
and reveal truths that then can be tested and verified with a scientific approach.
That would be my epistemology. Yeah. Look, I'm basically with you. I think the interesting
thing about the scientific approach
is that it relies on things you can measure.
And psychology pushes the boundaries, right?
Like we are a softer science than other sciences
and weaker in many respects
because our measures are intrinsically less concrete.
We can do our best to make them as good as possible,
but we fundamentally rely on observable, measurable
phenomena. And many things that are important to people in society are very, very difficult to
measure. And I suppose there's still space for other things. And just to, I guess, agree with
you about that other point, I certainly wouldn't include qualitative research in general as outside
of the scientific approach. In fact, Chris and I are
going to cover for our Decoding Academia series, a paper by Anna Katter, who does amazing qualitative
research, trawling through the internet, documenting all of the anti-vax tropes,
essentially, that go on. And it's extremely careful and it's a great taxonomy or just
description of that there is very little narrative building.
There is very little capital T theory involved in making these expansive claims. It is straight up textual analysis.
So I think the qualitative research is an interesting sort of hinterland or demilitarized
zone between these two contents, because some of it is like that and some of it is not.
Cherry picked.
Yeah.
some of it is like that and some of it is not yeah cherry picked yeah there's a document which is like a terrorist manifesto for the replication crisis in the form of daryl bem's advice to
grad students about how to do research and read a paper right which is still i think accessible
we'll link it in the show notes for the episode, but it essentially advises students to do
all of the things that we're saying you shouldn't do.
Right.
And it presents data analysis as a rhetorical tool.
Yeah.
A tool of finding the story and basically that your data should tell whatever story
that you can wring from it.
Right.
And you have to torture it until you can find the things, cut up the sample until you get
like the significant results in the way that line up them and then revise your hypothesis
so that it looks like you always did that and you have a neat story.
And we all know that that caused so many problems in the discipline.
And in some sense, he did a service of laying it out in the same way as he did with the
paper claiming to have demonstrated that you could influence things backwards in time, right?
He had a paper about doing the stimulus after recording the things and that the influence
could still be detected.
So time travel of stimulus, which was obviously wrong, but the results appeared to be correct
according to standard statistics at the time.
So Darrell Graham is useful in many respects because of demonstrating these things.
But that mentality, I thought, well, Matt, when you're talking about arts and humanities
and more qualitative approaches, that in some a sample of undergraduates in North America. I will not make claims about universal psychological processes and that kind of thing.
There's something of a congruence between the way Daryl Bem talked about good psychology
and the psychology that led to the replication crisis and the kind of tendency in arts and
humanities subjects to value theoretical beauty and storytelling above accepting the limitations
of empirical data.
I might be the anthropologist representative, but I think the arts and humanities approach
can be limiting in the just-so story variety.
I mean, I think narratives and stories are so compelling.
We're just so drawn to them.
So at some level, Daryl Bem's advice is good for communication, right?
It's like,
it's a nice way to communicate,
an effective way to communicate.
But science is messy.
Data are messy.
And I'm not sure
we're doing anyone a service
by making it too clean
and too much like a story.
I think you still need
the narrative when you're writing.
Otherwise, it'd be
a horrible thing to read.
But I think we should
embrace the messiness
and be honest about it and open about it. I think that's also how we gain trust from the general
public is by showing our warts. And that's why I think I disagree with Chris. The original question
was my perspective, whether we should burn it all down or there's some good stuff here. Oh,
Matt, I lost my train of thought. I talked about the early or the kind of view being that psychology is, we've reviewed so
many flaws and such bad methodology that we need to burn it all down and start from scratch.
Right.
So I was going to say that I think this process we're going through, the messiness, the fighting,
the ugliness, I think makes us more trustworthy.
Look,
we're struggling with this. We're trying to figure out what is true in a complicated,
messy world in a field that doesn't have things that are easily measured. And it ought to be messy. And we're realizing that we're figuring it out. And I don't see this as a reason to burn
it all down. I see this as, I mean, it was a massive mistake. I wish
this happened before I started grad school, but it's happening now. And it is what it is. We're
getting our house in order. So there's more reasons to trust, not less reasons.
Yeah. One of the things that bother us is how the criticisms of academia from the outside,
from the gurus, from the heterodox, is that where that academia is
kind of this echo chamber where there's reflexive defense of fields and so on.
And as this incident has shown, it's not like that at all.
Nobody wants to waste their time.
And there's just been a huge effort to try to fix these problems and a massive amount
of robust internal debate.
And yeah, we should start to see the results of it. Yeah, no, I just think it's interesting. I mean,
when I was younger and earlier in my career, I would have been much more a cheerleader for
psychology. It's a common field and rubbing up against anthropologists and philosophers and
humanities people. It's given me some appreciation for some of the intrinsic
limitations in our field. And by accepting them, it's not like giving up. It's just acknowledging
them puts us on a firmer basis. I already mentioned the issue of measurement, but I think Chris
alluded to another one, which is the issue of contextuality, right? So, you know, anthropologists
are all about that. They're all about talking about this particular
culture in a particular place in a particular time and psychologists we don't want to do that
we want to talk about human universals but we can't work with people detached from space and
time can we yeah yeah i mean in some ways the the replication crisis has and most of the discussion
has been on these technical solutions almost almost bureaucratic solutions, and they're great, but it's the
absolute lowest bar for a science to have a thing be replicable.
The next bar is, does it mean anything?
Does it map onto the real world?
How is it coherent with other little findings?
So that's, we're talking about theory, we're talking about measurement, we're talking about
applicability. So a big thing in social psychology, at least,
is these little lab studies. And I mean, for most of my career until maybe about five, 10 years ago,
I was like, I only conducted experiments because that's the only way of really knowing things.
And now I'm like, it's actually a horrible way of knowing things. Yes, we get a causality. That's
a beautiful thing. Wonderful thing. But when you leave out so much of the world, who cares whether your little thing moves something else in the lab
in the real world, it does absolutely nothing. We don't know that. So that's kind of similar to
Chris. I've heard you in one of your podcasts. I have to talk about the dangers of going from
in vitro studies to then in, you know, in vivo. And I think that's also a massive problem.
So all this stuff, all this kind of hanging is, I think, healthy and good.
And we're hopefully changing for the better.
And I think, again, it's positive.
It's been very painful, but positive.
There's a study, Mickey, that I think meets that point.
I use it usually in the introductory courses that I'm teaching, where there was the Facebook
study about influencing mood and emotion through what you display on the timeline.
And probably it wouldn't bother people so much now because there's not so much
attention on Facebook, just amongst the boomer generation, but like the, that
study was presented as if it showed that Facebook has the ability to completely
alter people's moods by simply a few tweaks to what they show on the algorithm.
And I'm on board with the people who argue that social media algorithms are important
and they can drive radicalization.
The focus on attention above all was doing a lot of damage.
But that particular study, it had the graphs that showed like, okay, this group was shown
more negative words and
they think, but like when you actually looked at what the metrics are, it was something
like one or like 0.8 extra negative words out of a hundred words in status updates.
And it was using like semantic language analysis.
So it was categorizing all these words in a very coarse way.
So it was a tiny, tiny effect.
And of course it was significant because the sample was hundreds of thousands of
people, but it was for practical purposes, it was tiny and you're basically saying
the actual result is if people see more negative things, they might in turn be slightly more
negative.
And you're like, okay, we already knew that.
And this didn't demonstrate that Facebook has the power to massively manipulate people.
This demonstrated that Facebook, through the thing that it did, has the ability to adjust
how many negative words people used out of every couple of hundred.
And that's just like, it's such a difference, right?
From the way it was presented in the media as Facebook has mind control powers versus
what people read will have a slight influence on them, maybe.
Right.
And it's such like an anodyne fighting, isn't it as well?
It's like, of course, what you read is going to impact you a little bit. Yeah. If it didn't it's such like an anodyne fighting. Is it as well? It's like, of course,
what you read is going to impact you a little bit.
Yeah.
If it didn't,
you're probably not reading.
You're not paying attention.
And it's amazing to think back at the reaction to that.
And that was a terrible,
what that,
the whole blowback to that was terrible for science because Facebook is still
experimenting all the time.
They are.
We just don't know about it now.
We don't know what the
results are, what they're doing, because of course they're going to do it because they want to
maximize their profits. Yeah. The AB testing is still going on, but it's like, I think that did
have a good issue or a good potential ethical debate about the ethics of experimenting on
people via terms and conditions, right? When you sign up on page 12, there's a
thing that say we might run social experiments on you. But that's a legitimate point. But at the
same time, these companies have access to the massive data sets and they actually could do
these really fascinating studies that would be of relevance. And now everyone is on social media. It's definitely of relevance, but like you say,
the public blowback, I'm sure it largely discourages companies from doing that because
anytime that results come out, like the stuff about Instagram and the effect on teenage
girls, it immediately becomes an outrage and there needs to be hearings and so on. And I'm not saying that social media companies don't need oversight, but I
think that there's a lot of like political stuff that goes in, which is not like
matched by what the research is actually showing or what they've been able to do.
So sorry, Matt, that's a hobby horse.
Matt Therese Pintscherr, Social media is definitely something that I moan a lot as well.
So we just finished our first study about Twitter,
which is kind of interesting.
Twitter is good, actually.
I bet that's the best thing.
What was the result, Nicky?
Breaking news.
The coding that goes gets the screw.
Take that, nature science.
Yes.
I hope they use my time.
Please tell me that.
Yes.
No surprising results.
But yeah, I mean, you know, one, logging onto Twitter is associated with less positive
affect or more negative affect.
And the effect size is about as large as the positive effect is of a social interaction.
The social interactions are generally positive, not always.
And it's about the same size, but in the opposite direction.
But massive variability, massive variability.
Lots of people show positive effects.
Some people show massive negative effects.
We also found effects, again, nothing surprising.
Effects for polarization and increased polarization.
It also, despite it having, I shouldn't say increased, I shouldn't use causal language, associated with polarization.
having it, I shouldn't say increase, I shouldn't use causal language,
associated with polarization. Despite it having, being associated with
poor, let's say, well-being,
there was a positive effect for
sense of community and belonging.
So it does seem to operate in
giving people a sense of
group and cohesion, but it's not necessarily
positive feelings
overall.
I have to say, Mickey, I have seen some
evidence for this in my immediate
network. Let's move on from replication crisis and that kind of thing. But I just, I got to say
one more thing about it. And I'd be curious about your take on this, which is that I'm really happy
that it's being dealt with. And one of the reasons I'm really happy with it is that I think that there are a lot of findings in psychology that are super robust, that are real,
that are not these negligible, tiny little effects that you can only find in a lab,
but are actually meaningful in a real life circumstance, like the thing you just talked
about with Twitter. And they get the stuff that's fake, or not fake and they get the stuff that's fake or not fake yeah
the stuff that's fake is more flashy you know it's more appealing it makes for a better science
news article headline it's more satisfying it'll be talking about ego depletion or something like
that or it could be counterintuitive whereas the stuff that I really like or the stuff that I think
is important and I'll just give an example and I just want to invite you guys to give examples of ones that might pop into your head.
But like in my field, we see these massive differences, say, between males and females
and their propensity for addictive behavior. And we see that we can identify some of the
causal mechanisms here. It's still a bit murky, but there's like DRD2 receptors and things like that.
And there's sensation seeking, rash impulsivity involved.
And we see the correlates with a whole bunch of other risky behaviors.
Also see, you know, this explains to a large degree why cross-culturally males do more
dangerous things.
They have more car accidents.
They get into more fights. There's a whole bunch of things going on. They make more podcasts.
They do make more podcasts, that's right, because we're more willing to embarrass ourselves
publicly. And that's important. It tells us something about human nature and has interesting
theoretical roots in evolutionary psychology. And it's just the kind of result that nobody thinks about much
or talks about much or thinks that psychology has anything to tell us about because it's not covered,
it's not talked about on Twitter, and it's not covered in the news because there's more
exciting, flashy stuff to talk about, which probably isn't real.
Yeah, I'm glad you're talking about this because I wouldn't want listeners to think that all
psychology or even all social psychology is bunk. I think there's solid findings in there, lots of them, and that
are meaningful and impactful. So you mentioned one. The canon that I like a lot is stuff from
judgment and decision-making, where Daniel Kahneman and Amos Tversky, a few decades ago now,
discovered that we are not rational agents like economists tell us, and that in fact,
there are predictable, irrational things that we do.
And many of them are interesting.
They're very robust and they're important.
They impact all kinds of decisions in many, many spheres of life, whether it be a general
manager picking a player for a sports team.
People getting insurance, people investing.
Right.
It impacts a wide, wide ranging impact.
So that's just one you know class of
examples but there's lots out there for sure i to complete the triangle it's the wrong way to put
it i can't think of the word i'm thinking of but to answer your question the one that would come
to mind from my own experience is that in the research on imitation and over imitation and
and this is slightly like a counterintuitive finding,
but I think one which has borne out to be correct
is that you might imagine that humans are better
and produce more cultural products
because we are more logical
and more orientated towards goals.
But a lot of the research with children and with adults
and also including comparative
psychology, comparing us with our closest evolutionary relatives, chimpanzees and other
apes, we have a propensity to over-imitate, to imitate things which have no obvious causal
or instrumental purpose.
And this means that we're very good at imitating things, but also that we do stupid stuff.
Like if you give people, the famous example is the puzzle box and you get them to tap
on the box and do various things to get a reward.
And then you show the transparent version of the box where you can see that most of
the things that you're doing, the tapping and pushing sticks into holes is not doing
anything.
And it's really only the last step that you need.
Chimpanzees and other primates are smarter in a way.
They go just for the goal and drop all the unnecessary stuff.
But children and also adults tend to copy the unnecessary behaviors because they socially
intuit that there is some reason that people are making them do that.
And that's a good reason that we can end up with cumulative culture
and other species can't.
So that's a really robust finding,
one that I think is interesting about human nature.
And I think it's good to highlight that these kind of findings
are coming from the same discipline that is dealing with
Diedrich Stapel and outright fraud.
But there is lots of good stuff there,
and that's why it is important to emphasize that as well.
Yeah.
Yeah.
It'll ultimately be a very positive procedure to winnow back the chaff and the frost.
And because the core that remains is still substantial and important.
So yeah, just ending it on a positive note.
Mickey, I have a slightly different question.
And I do want to get your take a little bit
on the gurus of the psychology world
or the ones that we deal with in general,
because I think you're also familiar
with the IDW superstars.
But before that, the mindfulness literature.
I know this has been a topic,
you covered it brilliantly on your podcast.
You've also been on papers, I believe, on the topic. But mindfulness has, for those who don't
know, is kind of a hot topic, I would say still a hot topic, though there's been some pushback
in psychology and positive psychology and clinical psychology as well, looking at using introspective practices, usually drawn from
meditative traditions as interventions for a whole range of things for wellbeing, but also
for behavioral problems and addiction and so on. And so the general reception of mindfulness,
I would say, is hugely positive amongst academics, amongst Western intellectuals.
Often it's presented as an alternative to chemical interventions, right? Antidepressants or so on.
You've had a different take and some criticisms about that. So I guess I want to ask,
what's wrong with Sam Harris? Why do you hate him, Mickey? Why do you hate Meditative Masters?
And what's your problem with Eastern Wisdom?
Enlighten us.
Yeah, that's funny.
So maybe a little background.
So we had a podcast episode a few years ago now,
Gates Mindfulness.
It might be one of my favorite ones
that I recorded with UL.
And it sprung from, well, from a few reasons.
Actually, it sprung from actually a Sam Harris podcast
where he was taking, who was it, Adam Grant to task,
who wrote, I think, an op-ed a number of years ago now
saying not everyone needs to be mindful.
And Sam Harris was trying to convict Joel and Busham
to be like, why? What's your problem?
And I just thought, well, wouldn't it be interesting
to take a more skeptical, critical take?
And to give a little bit of background, personal background.
So I was an avid meditator for four years, all through grad school.
An hour a day, 30 minutes in the morning, 30 minutes in the evening.
I went to retreats.
I went to a Zen center.
I was really into it.
And I had a few papers, a number of papers on the topic as well.
So at one point, I clearly thought there was something there. I was
very curious. That's why I started doing it. And my conscientious person just started kind of
meditating and it kind of was a thing. But after a while, at least for me, maybe I suck. I'm not a
master meditator like Sam Harris. At one point, I was like, I'm doing this for four years and
I'm still no good at this. And maybe one never really gets good at it. And I'm like, I'm still no good at this and maybe one never really gets good at it and I'm like I'm not sure
at least for me personally what the benefits are maybe it works for other people maybe they're
getting more immediate benefits maybe there were benefits and I'm just not conscious of them
whatever so I just kind of put it aside still thought there was something real and whole there
and that's why we did we conducted research on it and the research I conducted was specifically linking mindfulness as a construct or meditation
as a practice and one's cognitive ability, specifically executive attention or cognitive
control, which is a form of attention.
Can you control your attention when there are distractors, for example?
And that's actually my main line of research is on control and attention and effort.
And we found some positive results.
It got published,
highly cited now, more cited than most other things I published because mindfulness has this power. People were really, really attracted to it. But I'm not sure why at some point I just
started getting sick of it or skeptical. Maybe it's because I'm a contrarian, I'm disagreeable.
And I have this graph somewhere where it looks at the number of publications per year.
And it's like, yeah, it was around the year,
I would say 2005 or maybe a little bit after then,
where there was a massive explosion,
massive explosion, like in papers,
in the academic literature.
The Dalai Lama gave a talk at the Society for Neuroscience.
He's a religious leader giving a talk at the Society for Neuroscience.
And it just, so many people are saying this is the next best thing. No, it isn't.
So I just started looking at a little more of the literature with a little bit more of a skeptical
lens. And I'm not the only one who's going to push back. A number of other people push back.
So I think the critique are a few folds. Well, number one, like when we talk about meditation, it's thought to cultivate this
state of mindfulness.
And by mindfulness, typically that's defined as being able to attend in the moment, kind
of having a keen attentional capacity to be present right now.
And also there's a second factor, which is also evaluative or non-evaluative stance.
So paying attention to the moment, but also not evaluating good or bad what you're seeing, just accepting your experiences, whatever they might be.
And it might be that the second part is more important than the first part, even though the
first part seems to get a bit more attention. Attention gets more attention. But now, as Matt
mentioned, in our field, it's really, really, really important that we can measure things.
If we are saying that mindfulness does, like,
and what it's supposed to do, it improves sleep, it improves mental health, happiness,
makes you more productive. Of course, your attention, of course, you know,
anything under the sun probably makes you better at sex. I'm not sure about that.
Absolute sex. That's what we want.
So, and mindfulness does this, and we're being told to cultivate mindfulness we if we want to be able
to verify that claim we need to be able to measure this thing and there are numerous scales out there
numerous scales in fact small bit of trivia i think the first scale was published i'm gonna
it's gonna be like early 2000s by kirk brown and richard ryan and kirk brown was a graduate
student at mcgill when i was an undergrad there and he was the one who who introduced me to Buddhism. It's kind of an interesting connection. He developed the first
scale, the mindfulness attention awareness scale or something like that. But there are many, many
others. The problem is that these measures don't act like a mindfulness measure should act. So they
are reliable. So that's a good thing. By reliable, we mean, you know, if you ask someone how mindful
they are with these various measures, they score about the same or at least rank order is stable. So if you're
high on time one, you're going to be high in time two. That's pretty good. But in terms of validity,
in terms of it actually mapping onto the construct, it doesn't seem to act like it should.
So for example, there's one kind of humorous study showing that binge drinkers are higher in mindfulness than are
regular practicers of meditation. Okay. And that's a nonsensical finding. Now, there's also an easy
explanation of why that might be. Because it's assuming that meditation leads you to become more
mindful, which it might not, but let's assume it does. And then you're asking questions or answering
questions about how mindful you are,
how able are you to attend,
how able are you to stay focused,
how able are you to accept the content of your thoughts.
Well, the more mindful you are,
the more you might realize
how shit you are at those things, right?
So the practice of cultivating mindfulness
might lead you to score lower on this scale, okay?
So it's an awareness scale, it, a scale that's trying to get
at the quality of your awareness. And it's just a hard thing to measure.
Yeah. It's such a common thing. I remember I did my honors project way back in the day,
fourth year.
50 years ago.
Yeah. 50 years ago. In the olden days, rode to the lab on my penny farthing and
did my honors project on transformational leadership versus transactional leadership.
Right.
And now this totally different field, organizational psychology.
Nothing to do with trans topic.
No, no, no.
The popular one.
No more topic again.
But it had the same features, right?
So transformational leadership, right?
You inspire people, you show a mission and you help them grow and all this shit, right? It's sexy, right? So transformational leadership, right? You inspire people, you show a mission,
and you help them grow and all this shit, right? It's sexy, right? It's the kind of thing
psychologists and everybody wants to be true. And it's extremely appealing to people who
managers and stuff like that. They all want to be a transformational leader. Everyone wants to be
mindful. Everyone wants to be more self-aware and all that stuff. But ultimately, yeah, so it's contaminated by a massive amount of social desirability.
If you get other people to rate the manager, then it's contaminated with kind of just how
much they like them.
And so there's these simpler explanations which were just totally disregarded in favor
of this nice theoretical one, which also flattered the ambitions or the self-image of everyone involved.
So even without knowing any of the literature on mindfulness, I'm just generally skeptical to the
whole field of positive psychology, which probably unfairly, but just because it has the same quality
to me. I know the kinds of questions or the things that attract us all and the things we
want to be true. And I also know just how fuzzy these concepts are and how difficult they are to
measure. And it doesn't mean there isn't potentially some reality there. It's just that we're not well
equipped to address it. And I'm also content, my final rant, the final thing that contaminates my
thing is partly through dealing with some of these gurus
and people that I've known in my personal life. I've met people that have got into meditation
and they've been the most not very happy people, lots of problems with their relationships,
problems with their life. And you can see how Buddhism and meditation is a way to fix that.
And I've had them proclaim to me solemnly one day that they had achieved enlightenment
and they were enlightened now.
And then they would proceed to just fricking lose their temper at some tiny little thing.
Right.
Behave trivially.
Be skeptical for anyone who claims to be enlightened.
Yeah.
That's an extreme example, but I think a lot of the people that you can't help but be skeptical
and compare that to me. I do all the bad things. I don't do any meditation or any of those stuff,
and I feel I'm doing all right. I will say this is probably a good opportunity to address this
because there were people in our, well, a person in our subreddit and talking about our apparently
negative attitude towards introspective or
meditational practices. I think primarily Matt's negative attitude.
Introspection is overrated. Forget about it.
I would say here, and I think Matt, you would agree that I've got nothing against
introspective practices. Like you, Mickey, I had meditation practice.
I was very interested in Buddhism.
I studied like Buddhist history in university at SOAS.
And I still do have an interest and I find much of value in those traditions and in introspective
practices.
But I think what I would argue for is being more realistic and less aggrandizing about what they're providing,
right?
And that's what it's supposed to engender.
It's supposed to engender, like you said, Mickey, you know, greater self-awareness of
limitations and of the biases in your perceptions.
But it often doesn't come to that.
the biases in your perceptions, but it often doesn't come to that.
It's accompanied with like a perception that you are more enlightened or virtuous.
Yeah.
Self-aware than other people, even if other people don't agree with you. So I, I think that introspective practices can be great, but you have to be realistic
about what they do and what they achieve and what they have achieved for you.
But that point you made,
Mickey, about the Dalai Lama addressing an audience at a conference, a scientific or
psychology conference, that always struck me as something, especially after I'd spent time
studying Buddhism and the history of Buddhism and the fascination that the West has with Buddhism
in a very specific format,
the kind of Western Buddhism we talked with Evan Thompson about it before.
But this notion that I cannot imagine a social psychology conference
has as a keynote speaker, like a Jesuit priest,
lecturing them on contemplative prayer, right?
And saying, look, here's how we can get people to do contemplative prayer,
focus on the mysteries of the stigmata or whatever the case may be. People would not like that
because they would see it as religion intruding on research, non-appropriate manner. But when
there is a Buddhist priest, and even if they're in Buddhist robes, using terminology
from the Buddhist tradition, it's kind of given a pass, or even worse, regarded as cool.
And the only reason for it is because of ethnocentric projection that Buddhism is exotic
and interesting and Christianity is traditional and and distinctly non-cool.
Although Jordan Peterson did his best to try and reverse that.
But yeah, that double standard always struck me in, I think it's in psychology.
I think it's in Western academia in general, that Buddhism and Eastern
traditions are treated very differently than religious traditions that the West is more
familiar with, like Judaism or Islam or Christianity.
Well, I think it's because of the, you know, and Evan Thompson talks about this very clearly,
this notion of Buddhist exceptionalism.
It's not actually a religion, at least according to Western minds.
It's a philosophy.
So you can throw away all the talk about deities and gods and prescriptive practices,
and it's just, you know, it's a way of cultivating attention. So I think that's the way, that's the
sidestep that was done there. I disagree with Chris here a little bit. Like, I think Buddhism is a...
Sorry, I'm sorry. I'm sorry, Chris. I think it's objectively cooler than the other monotheistic...
Victim, you're a victim of Buddhist propaganda.
other monotheistic religion. You're a victim of Buddhist propaganda.
DT Suzuki was aiming his eyes at you.
Even allowing for all of the Orientalism and so on, the Western monotheistic
religions are fundamentally about sin and repentance and purifying your soul.
Hey, how is that different from karma and merit?
Right?
Like you're the traditional reading of Buddhism is about escaping the cycle of
rebirth and it's even worse because the people who get disabilities or like it
used to be seen as karmic demerit was why you were born a woman instead of a man.
I admit that's not very cool, but how do you get there?
It seems to me in Christianity, you get there by following the rules and not doing X, Y, and Z.
And with Buddhism, it seems like you've got to get there by more mindfulness or something.
Look, Mickey, I'm sorry you've met this Buddhist propagandist here, but the-
What about Judaism?
Which is better, Judaism or Christianity?
Let's talk Turkey.
The ancient, the...
Yeah.
But the Buddhist way to, depends on the tradition and the Western versions
have, do emphasize like introspective practices, but if you follow most of
the like Buddhists in the majority of the world,
practicing Buddhists, the right thing to do is follow a whole heap of restrictive monastic
rules about what you can eat, about who you can and cannot sleep with, what you can and cannot
do with your own hands, and including restrictions on how much hair you should have if you're
living the monastic lifestyle correctly.
Chris, those are the unimportant parts.
That's not core to the religion.
That's a timer.
It's a bugbear.
It's a bugbear.
I want to just say one last thing
about my critique of mindfulness or meditation.
So I talked about kind of the contrary to mindfulness,
but of course, you know,
we can sidestep mindfulness altogether
and just look at meditative practice
and see, you know, does it have an effect? And people have now conducted
really rigorous meta-analyses. And there's one, I think published a number of years ago now in,
I think it was JAMA, so Journal of American Medical Association, a really prominent journal.
And it found that the effects are modest, small, or non-existent, comparable to other therapies,
comparable to exercise,
compared to taking a medication.
If, you know, assuming that,
and that's not even accounting for like
all the nonsense that goes in,
p-hacking, publication bias,
that's just a straight up meta-analysis.
And we don't want to take some of these safeguards.
So if I've got a choice
and the outcomes are going to be the same,
would I want to sit on my cushion for an hour or pop a pill and cope on my day?
Or go for a jog, which we know actually has far ranging positive effects.
Yeah.
Okay.
Not just on mental health, on physical health, longevity.
It's a great thing to do.
So that hour that I spent every day for four years,
I could have been working out and have been a much more attractive human being and healthier. So I think at some level, it's also what, you know, opportunity
costs for engaging in this thing that is not clear how effective it is. And at best, it might be as
effective about other things we can do that might have other benefits. You're basically describing
my lifestyle. I get up in the morning, I swim some laps, swim my 1.7 kilometers.
Pop some pills.
Then pop in the pills and the drinking to finish off the day
and I'm as happy as Larry.
But yeah, like, you know, mindfulness in that sense
is the same as pretty much every other psychological therapy
that we try, yeah?
They all kind of work a bit.
It's hard to say which one helps more.
I did learn very recently about a very interesting finding.
I can't say who it is because it's still, whatever, it's under review and they don't want me to say that. Prominent
researchers, happiness researchers in Canada, they scanned the literature for what areas,
what things lead to happiness. So people claim gratitude leads to happiness or
meditating leads to happiness. And then they looked at how many studies in these various little effects were
pre-registered. All right. And not a single study,
there's not a single pre-registered study showing a link between,
I believe it's between mindfulness or meditation and wellbeing and happiness.
Okay. Now that's not to say that every, that's garbage if it's not pre-registered,
but again, it's a safeguard. It is guardrail being like, okay, I'm more willing to trust this
kind of study if it is pre-registered. And it's important because actually at one point I had a
little argument with a mindfulness researcher who was defensive about some of the things I was
saying on Twitter. And then I was kind of pushing back. We're trying to be trying to be kind,
which is hard for me on Twitter. And I said, does there exist? Is there any preregistered studies? This is not a study
in wellbeing. It was on, it was actually done a study sponsored by the department of defense
in the U S looking at mindfulness training among military people, which again, shows you how far
this has gone and how warped it is to some extent. And it was pre-registered. I'm like,
oh, cool. I thought this is great. I went to the pre-registration and what was pre-registered i'm like oh cool i thought this is great i went to
the pre-registration and what they pre-registered and what they did are night and day okay so i know
now that it's nine days to get to be registered but just because you've pre-registered something
doesn't mean you followed it to the t and this is a case where the results were really messy and
mixed and they didn't report all the things they even measured.
So all this stuff is like all these little hints of like,
maybe it makes you feel good, maybe.
But, you know, I like smoking cannabis.
It makes me feel good.
I'll do that instead.
Yeah, I mean, just to reiterate the point I made before,
I mean, these things can be distracting from the results that are robust and strong, like exercise.
So it's pretty easy to uncover strong effects. You just can notice them. It's not, it shouldn't
be that hard. So yeah, a bit of culling, a bit more skepticism is not just good for not believing
or not promoting something that's false or unproven, but also good for focusing people's attention
on the stuff that we do know.
We can maybe stay on message.
Psychologists aren't going to give people the meaning of life
and tell them the secrets to being a happy person.
Jordan Peterson.
Yeah.
Sorry, scientific psychologists.
But yeah, we could definitely,
we could tell them three or four things
that are definitely true, I'm sure,
and that's better than nothing.
Yeah. Since we've been quite cruel to the mindfulness practitioners Yeah, we could definitely, we could tell them three or four things that are definitely true, I'm sure. And that's better than nothing.
Yeah.
Since we've been quite cruel to the mindfulness practitioners and researchers, I'll grant Matt also the bone that I think that the emphasis on a middle path, right, within traditional
Buddhism, as well as introspection in general, that you don't need to go into the extreme
ascetic and you don't need to go into extreme hedonism.
I like that.
The moderate, the centrist, that's the correct golden mean path to follow.
And I think that is cooler than messages in Christianity or so on.
Thank you.
Thank you, Chris.
That's my vote.
All right.
I actually have a question
maybe for for chris or or matt so i know you had your little kind of back and forth argument with
sam harris about that one i guess emergency podcast about he's saying how you all need to
meditate because if you do you'll realize there's no self and and also you'll come to these particular
political views which of course is nonsense but i want to just ask about one piece of that. And it's one
that I just don't know. I'm not sure I stand. And that is this idea that if you sit long enough,
and maybe longer than I did, just an hour a day, but actually go on retreats, maybe even be alone
for a long time, do you believe that doing this will automatically, without anything else, lead
to some falling apart of the self? Some realization that the self is an illusion and that there's nothing really holding me together.
Is that a natural byproduct of this sort of introspection? Or alternatively, is it a product
of that practice plus the dharma, the teaching that that is what might happen?
Yeah, I do have thoughts on this. And I think I fall in line with Evan Thompson
and others who emphasize that
there's a lot of lifting done
by the conceptual framework
that you're introduced to in meditation.
And if you took people
and just give them the basic introduction to meditation
and didn't tell them anything else,
I think you would not see all of the things that someone
like Sam Harris might claim are just the natural process.
But the part that I would agree with him on, and I think most people who've meditated for
any amount of time would agree on, is that if you start a meditational practice, you
become aware of how unruly your mind is, how often it's projecting into the future,
looking at the past or how hard it is to exist in the moment and how hard it can be not to make your
mind go off running on tangents and that kind of thing. Just focusing on that activity is very
difficult. And I think for a lot of people that, and it was for me as well,
it's like quite revealing about the processes of how your mind is working in daily life,
that if you don't take the time to do that, that you might not notice. Now, the second part of
that, so does that lead to the awareness that the self is an illusion and that there is nothing inherently there.
I'm much more skeptical about it because I think the notion that we have an autobiographical
self concept, which is actually very important to how humans function in the world.
And yes, it depends on how you frame something as an illusion, because sure,
it's a cognitive construct that comes from psychological processes and there's
no homunculus behind the wheel at the end of the day and there, you know, there's
lots of good studies showing that we can have conflicting impulses or things
driving us that we don't notice.
But I think that if you look at people who have Alzheimer's or who get
brain damage and the impact that the destruction of a personal identity and a loss of memories
can have on an individual, it's really clear that people then say this person has lost what they
were, right? And it's very hard for the other people in their lives.
Now, if that person as well,
when it's happening to them during the process,
they're often very distressed
about what's happening, right?
They don't want to lose it.
But once they've lost the ability
in the individual moments,
they can be fine
because they don't have an awareness.
But like, I think most people,
if you said, do you want to recognize your daughter or your grandchildren, they'd regard
it as a fundamental loss. And I think this notion that people have about the destruction and
illusion of self, it doesn't recognize the importance of that cognitive component that we
have, the story of an autobiographical self. And I think becoming
aware of how you construct that through meditation is interesting, but I think it's a fundamental
component of being human that we do have a sense of an autobiographical self. So that's my extreme
take on it. I think it's an illusion in the same sense that the way we process vision is an illusion i have a take on that too which is i think you can get to that
point that understanding at least intellectually without doing any meditation at all i think i
find it maybe a little bit easier because i've got a terrible audio autobiographical memory
and i'm usually just drifting around in a haze and not really knowing what's going on. But
most people are aware that you're a different person when you're at work and when you're at
home in bed or playing with your children. And at different times in your life, you've been
different people. And if you've ever taken certain kinds of drugs, you'll know that you
can be a different person again. And I think if people are honest with themselves,
they don't really think that there is like this ineffable self,
like a kernel of a crystal somewhere that's sitting inside you,
which expresses itself differently.
I think it's layers of an onion skin.
So you can figure that out.
I mean, but they'll say, oh, no, you have to feel it.
You have to truly, which I would say, I don't know what that means.
Okay. I'm sorry. And I promise I'll let, oh, no, you have to feel it. You have to truly, which I would say, I don't know what that means. Okay.
I'm sorry.
And I promise I'll let you respond after this.
But I just want to mention that there was a cross-cultural psychology conference that
I was at.
And I seen some debates also in the literature about the self-concept in East Asia versus
the self-concept in the West, right?
I'm familiar with this research looking about the way that particularly Japanese people
use social media versus participants in North America and Europe, right?
And there was this difference in the results when you look cross-culturally that Japanese
people tended to regard it, that basically there is no fundamental self, that there is
a situational self, which depends on the context of what you show.
So, you know, what you do when your boss is watching is different than when you're with
your friend.
And they were talking about social media usage, having to navigate that, right?
That you have to present all these different types of selves and this being completely
in line with their self-concept and the self-concept in
Japan, which is very much contextual. Even the language reflects that, right? That the hierarchy
changes the verbs that you use when you're talking to someone. So you always are aware
of positional relationships. But the American respondents tended to regard people modulating
their opinions on social media
networks in order to not cause trouble with like employment or so on, to be denying
the true self because they were representing themselves differently towards different
people and sometimes like putting their opinion down.
That struck me as if you construct a view of psychology or introspective practices based
on the default being the Western North American perspectives, that it will result in you having
a very skewed, very culturally specific interpretation of like what the self is.
That it just struck me that's relevant.
Yeah.
I'm sorry.
We're not letting Mickey talk, but I just want to say I agree with you there, Chris.
I think that's kind of what I was saying that a lot of what we're referring to when
we talk about the self and so on is, is not talking about a fundamental psychological
thing, but it's like a cultural construct that for Westerners is, is a hot button
issue and just happens to fit nicely with some Eastern religions as well.
What about you, Mickey? What about you, Mickey?
What about you, Mickey?
Come on, you say something now.
Well, my take, I'm skeptical that without some extra cultural, actual teaching that
the self is an illusion that you will get there naturally, no matter how long you
meditate, I can be wrong about that.
Cause I haven't done these extreme forms of meditation, but it reminds me of this, I guess, a common occurrence for, I've never done
this, but people who take this drug called ayahuasca. It's a cactus in Peru, I believe,
in South America, more generally. And apparently, a common experience, a common visual hallucination
you might experience is the vision of a jaguar. And everyone, you know, waits to see this vision of a jaguar. And then I'm like, is there something about the drug that leads to a
vision of a jaguar? Really? Or is it that you're in South America where there are jaguars and maybe
the shaman talks about jaguars and they have some sort of mythical, you know, power or what have
you that leads you and then you're taught about it and that's what you see. So it struck me there
might be a similarity there going on. So I just kind of push against that notion that you automatically are going to have this revelation.
Only if you meditate the right way will you get there.
But again, maybe I haven't done it the right way and I better get on waking up.
Just a quick shout out on that topic of ayahuasca.
A very nice podcast, what's it called?
I Know Ross and Carrie.
They do great stuff stuff but they went
down one of them ross went down to do an iosca experience and he had a very very bad trip it's
probably worth listening to because he probably experienced it's probably evidence for the
existence of a self-concept because he completely lost it and felt that he didn't exist and was maybe dead and it was a terrifying, terrifying thing, but a great episode to
catch if in a way.
Look, Ricky and Matt, you're experienced psychonauts, so dissociative
experiences can be transcendent, but they can also be freaking scary, right?
Like I want to get back to normal.
So that's for sure.
Yeah.
Bad trips exist.
But Mickey, we've taken up a lot of your evening,
but I'd be remiss if we didn't ask you
since we've trapped you here for the time being.
So, you know, we cover gurus
and focus on the secular variety.
And you and your co-host, Yol,
I think are people that have been active
in the heterodox academy, Jonathan Haidt kind of space, right? With concerns about political
influence on particularly the psychology discipline. And I think many of the concerns
are valid because there's a huge queue in that direction. But I'm wondering with COVID and with the trajectory that the heterodox sphere has taken,
I think it's taken a lot of blows.
Definitely.
I wonder how you currently feel about that whole space and the discourse in general around
heterodoxy versus wokeism and not necessarily in academic
psychology terms, but you know, but you can talk about that just in general, how are you
feeling as a heterodox inclined person?
Right.
I mean, to clarify a little bit, so I'm a member of Heterodox Academy, but I put my
name on our website and that's essentially my involvement.
I get their newsletter, which I think I've looked at a couple of times really only.
Not because I have anything against it,
I just hit a lot of emails.
So I just don't look at them all.
So I think you're right.
Your reading of what's been happening
in the heterodox space is correct.
And I think there's a few reasons why COVID,
but I think the bigger one is this kind of
what we're calling the racial reckoning
that post George Floyd world. And that has led, I'm not sure how you've experienced it. You two have
experienced it in Japan and Australia, but in North America, I mean, it was a sweeping change.
Within weeks of that event, there were demands, letters, statements. And even though we're
somewhat removed in Canada, I mean, some of my colleagues forget
that we're a different country,
but we have a different population.
We have different demographics, for example,
but nonetheless, we experience it deeply here.
And it's become difficult for any other voices
to speak out about other issues
or even to disagree with, let's say,
some of the things that are happening
as a result of the things that are happening as a
result of the reaction to the George Floyd incident.
So I've had, you know, there have been in my department, we've had equity hires, which
were, I think we're all of us were, well, not all of us, but more or less were on board
to hire people to, again, to the faculty look like our students look like the, like, look
like the population.
There have been, you know, now there is in my department, for example,
they're asking us to reflect on
what we've done personally for diversity,
diversity issues.
And it took me a while.
I actually pushed back being like,
hey, in an earlier part of my career,
I studied intergroup relations and prejudice and stigma.
So I'm on board with diversity mandates.
I taught and I created and taught a class
in the psychology of prejudice for like nearly 15 years. So I'm on board with diversity mandates. I taught and I created and taught a class in the psychology of prejudice for like nearly
15 years.
So I'm on board with these things, but I'm not necessarily on board with being coerced
to write these statements that are almost like, you know, these struggle sessions, right?
Like where you're forced to like, say what you're doing.
So I kind of pushed back, but for me, even to push back, I'm a full professor and nothing's
going to happen.
Nothing bad will happen to me.
It took me a long time to speak up.
And I remember like unmuting my microphone and being like, oh man, am I going to do it?
Am I just going to say, I'm not sure I agree with this.
It took me a while.
So I'm not trying to say that I'm self-censoring or, you know, there's no freedom of speech.
I know that's kind of overblown.
But I also don't think it's incorrect that there is a bit of social censure about certain
topics and certain topics are off
out of bounds. And it saddens me that that's the case. I think sometimes it's good that they're
out of bounds. I mean, like I am not a free speech absolute at all. I think that's a silly notion
that we can just say whatever we want and without any consequences. I think there ought to be
consequences if there are certain kinds of speech, but we have to be, that bar needs to be really
high for someone to just express disagreement with something have to be, that bar needs to be really high. For someone to just express,
you know, disagreement with something
and to be worried about how they'll be perceived,
I think that's not a healthy environment.
So again, I think since George Floyd,
because so many people want this massive change,
and I think for mostly the right reasons,
it's been harder,
like these kind of more heterodox
kinds of organizations and people,
I suspect are having a harder time
gaining support. I was probably, and this is a good illustration of the difference, like the
thing that you're thinking about is really Heterodox Academy on kind of that wing of the
Heterodox sphere. And I was thinking a little more of the insane wing of it that became COVID contrarians and that we cover on the
gurus here. But I think that in so doing, you're highlighting an important distinction, which is
that I think there is a very important difference between a figure like Steven Pinker or Jonathan
Haidt or you and a figure like Brett Weinstein or Dave Rubin or the Gad Saad. And they all get lumped into
the heterodox, right, as a category. And I think that is something sometimes people think I'm being
guilty of, but in my head, the distinctions are always clear, but maybe not in my tweets.
And I agree with you that I find myself frustrated that there are a lot of efforts which seem to
ring the bell for equity in terms of the superficial presentation that they're helping
to achieve that. And this would include things like diversity statements and so on. But the actual practice of a lot of these things is extremely
bureaucratic. And actually, most people involved with it are aware that it's bureaucratic, right?
Diversity statements, the way that people write them on their application forms or whatever,
there's a standard format. And it isn't to say that people, the fact that it has become
relevant to hiring, it's necessarily bad.
But I think the people who push back and say, well, there's no clear evidence that it actually
impacts anything except for becoming a box ticking exercise.
That people who have the appropriate background and training that are from middle class or more
wealthy backgrounds and know how to say the right things can jump through that hurdle easier than,
say, somebody from a working class background or somebody from an immigrant background. That's
true. And I do worry that when criticism of those kind of activities tends to be regarded very suspiciously
because it is true that people who make those criticisms are often also your Brett Weinsteins
and so on as well but it would be a shame if like any pushback becomes conflated into that category
so yeah so there's a few things in there. First thing I must say,
I just ignore,
I'm like you two,
I ignore the crazy wing.
I'm not sure I even call them
heterodox thinkers.
I just call them crazy people.
And you've highlighted so well
the sheer lunacy of the Weinsteins.
I lost every ounce of sympathy
and maybe because of you two,
especially for both of them,
they're both so horrible. But I stopped paying attention a long time ago the only time i ever hear about
them is through you two and so i ignore them but i definitely take exception to for example
jonathan um height or stephen pinker being lumped in with them like some people i heard a colleague
of mine a friend of mine call jonathan height an alt-right figure like an alt-right figure i'm not
even sure he's conservative.
And it just, if you're not with me,
you're just in this box of otherness.
And to me, that is distressing.
I think that's a breakdown in just regular conversation.
So I think one should ignore those, the crazies.
They're not worried.
You guys are too smart to be talking about those two,
all those people, right?
I mean, it's entertainment.
So don't get me wrong. I enjoy hearing it. The pushback I'd have to that,
Michelo, I think overall, I agree with you that it's mentally better for people to ignore what a lot of the people that we cover up to, like nobody needs to be listening to JPC's content,
but the problem is that lots of people are. And that to me is something that there's a lot of the people that we cover.
And, you know, it's interesting academically and whatnot to look at the rhetorical techniques,
but there are also things where these people have huge audiences and they end up showing
up in places that you wouldn't expect, like on government panels or on talk shows talking about the coronavirus or so
on. And so I think there is an element where I don't think that everybody needs to pay attention
to what Alex Jones is doing. But the fact that there are people who are looking at his content
and looking at the stuff that he's pointing out or that he's advancing week in and week out.
It's a good thing.
Yeah.
Fair enough.
I mean,
you guys are doing a public service for sure.
No,
no,
not us.
I'm really,
I was thinking of knowledge fight,
the guys who listened to like four hours of Alex Church a day,
they do that.
But I think,
you know,
Matt and me do this because it is fun as well. It is entertainment.
Like we're not moral crusaders out to save the world.
We enjoy taking apart the content, but I think that there are sometimes where
it does feel like the Weinsteins and whatnot, it's an indulgence to go
into their wacky conspiracies, but they end up being repeated by
so many people that it's just like yeah they're a bellwether in some respects no but you know in
line with what mickey said i mean this was crossing my mind as we were editing the last
episode with james lindsey and not with james lindsey on james lindsey yeah we spent many hours with him and it gets to a
point where it becomes a little bit unfun because it's it's transparently crazy talk and one can
dunk on it till the cows come home or laugh at it or point out how it's gateways to fascism and so
on and all of that stuff may well be true, but it just becomes less interesting.
I think we're gravitating more towards figures
that occupy a more interesting zone,
which are not obviously crazy or wrong or stupid,
but they're either intentionally or not
indulging in some rhetorical tricks
that are kind of worth
screwing up.
That's kind of a fun puzzle for me.
Yeah.
Well, so again, I think it was Liam who recommended you cover, is it Connor Friesdorf?
He's not fond of him.
Yeah, I could tell.
And I must admit, I've read a few of his articles.
I mostly found them to be fine, but I'm wondering what the critique says.
I would be really interested in hearing a breakdown of his.
I was just going to say, I think we're moving in that direction and in line with that,
we'll be covering some more people on the left and it's a whole nother conversation,
but it is interesting how they are sort of intrinsically more challenging because to
the extent that they are, in our opinion, being nonsensical, it's kind of an institutional orthodoxy or
an academic theoretical orthodoxy that it is based on rather than a heterodox, iconoclastic
kind of thing.
So that'll be a challenge.
We'll see how we go with that.
But before we leave the culture war stuff, I'll say my piece about that and the excesses
of locusts and which is that chris and i are like self-nominated neoliberals right but i think the
socialists and the materialist communists have a good take there which is that when they see
this kind of window dressing this performative stuff being done by the middle class, essentially, which
is a way of justifying their moral superiority.
And they look at it with a very skeptical eye, and I think they're on point there.
I think there is a good component of that that we should watch out for.
To me, an interesting analogy, which probably most people would think is a stretch, is between the Victorian English middle class, this historical
culture that was in England around the time of the empire. And there was a very strong cultural
set of norms there, completely arbitrary in many cases about yeah sure what school you went to and how
you spoke but the kind of opinions and the kind of things you said and did and didn't do that
showed that you were sound yeah a sound chap right who could be trusted to run some bit of the east
india company or something and their bureaucracy was based on that.
And they had a total lack of self-awareness
that it was this grand filtering mechanism
and a moral justification for their own role
in running a large portion of the world.
And it can be a danger, I think, for fashionable academics
and people in HR departments
and people at the upper end of town, highly
educated, you know, use these topics as a way to do essentially exactly that.
You have a hot take, but...
There's an analogy, Mickey, that I thought of that relates specifically to psychology
and the double standards that I've seen around these social justice topics.
double standards that I've seen around these social justice topics.
So in social psychology, there is generally a skew towards advocacy for social justice and those kinds of topics being put front and center, right?
And I would say amongst early career researchers, this is becoming a dominant
theme and I've, I've seen this seem to be a play out in anthropology in the
seventies and the 90s.
Like it's an interesting parallel to just see it all happening again.
But I remember noticing that whenever people were talking about Turkers, right, mechanical
Turks, or the people that are taking part in any of the online crowdsourcing things.
There was a real palpable disdain for them as almost not real people, not real subjects,
and incredibly low quality.
It reflected in this concern that they were all bots at one point, right?
There was the bot crisis of whatever, 2019 or whenever it was. And that to
me was hugely overblown because what you had was some low quality data, but in large part,
a lot of the low quality data that you saw came from studies that were playing peanuts to people.
And then we're surprised to find that you'll get some percentage of data that isn't good.
And yes, there are some things where there are forms for completing things on crowdsourcing platforms.
But by and large, those are not bots.
Because the whole point of those platforms is that they're designed for tasks that bots cannot do.
Right.
That's the HIT in Amazon terms was human intelligence task because the bots can't do it.
And you can defeat most bots with just a cap check, right?
Ask it, is this a bike or what animal is in the picture or, you know, so on.
So in those respects, I saw a lot of people who might even that day have been given a
talk about the importance of looking outside
weird samples and having better representation in the discipline and being concerned about social
justice issues and then casually disparage the workers on crowdsourcing platforms. And most
of the times, it's not for me, I'm not saying that like we shouldn't criticize the over-reliance on those
samples but roller the criticism should be directed at the researchers and the people not
paying reasonable amounts or not instituting you know reasonable quality checks i've seen lots of
people recommend you give a quality check and you just kick out the people that feel and i was like
that's incredibly harsh because maybe they just feel
the attention check. My preference to that is that you pay the people who do the survey,
regardless of the quality of the data, because you cannot say in a lab setting, well, you did
the study, but you didn't do it well enough, so we're not going to pay you. And I just saw that
double standard so often that the focus on whatever is the social justice topic that has
recognition, which is associated with a political movement, gets attention. But the ones which are
maybe more impactful about just paying participants reasonable wages, treating people
online as if they are just people that are online are less sexy, less interesting.
There's not huge panels on them.
So I know that's not everyone, but I just, that struck me once as this very clear double
standard in the conferences and what the concern with social justice looked like in the psychology
field.
Yeah, that's very interesting.
I mean, it just shows you that people follow the incentives, right?
So you get the incentive, you get the points for whatever topic or way you're discussing,
but that other stuff, the actual stuff that's actually going to impact a real human being
on the other side or many human beings on the other side is not visible and not the
incentive there to do it that way. I'm sure that's true. And I'm sure there's some people that do this,
but maybe I'm an optimist. And I think generally people try to be good and, you know, we're all
hypocrites a little bit. Yeah. I don't have this as a holier than thou thing, except to say that,
if you are casting stones, you should be more careful of your own behavior.
Clean your room, Chris.
Yeah, I'm inclined.
There was an example of a project that I was involved with where there was a desire to
collect diverse data set, right?
Like not rely on college age graduates and whatnot, but they want not to pay people.
They want them to be volunteer for various reasons.
And I was thinking like, okay, but the easiest way,
the win-win for everyone is actually just pay people.
Just pay people and actually crowdsourcing platforms,
particularly in Japan where this project was going to be based,
the majority of the people using the crowdsourcing platforms, particularly in Japan, where this project was going to be based, the majority of the people using the crowdsourcing platforms are housewives.
A sample which is otherwise difficult to reach for people who might be collecting samples from universities.
So to me, it was like this project, it wanted to pay a large amount of money to a researcher
in order to arrange these volunteer efforts to ensure a diverse and underrepresented sample.
What they should have done is hire someone that can use crowdsourcing platforms, pay the people on the crowdsourcing platforms a reasonable amount, and they would contribute the data and everybody would win.
But because there was this emphasis that it has to be volunteer and underrepresented
populations we don't want to use uh online platforms it got in the way of the actual goal
so yeah right yeah i've actually struck that too chris where the ethics board came back and we
said we can't pay people that much because the person might be economically disadvantaged
and because there was such a large amount of money on offer they might feel compelled because the person might be economically disadvantaged.
And because there was such a large amount of money on offer,
they might feel compelled to participate.
That's what your ethics board said?
Yeah.
What kind of ethics board do you have?
I've had the opposite.
But you can see the logic.
It's the same logic though.
Oh yeah, you can get the opposite too. I mean, I've had the opposite where I was.
You were trying to pay slave labor.
Not slave labor.
We don't have a study where it's going to be $30 for, it was a long, a logical study,
many instances.
But my RRB was like, no man, you gotta be like 50.
I'm like 50.
Wow.
I mean, I did.
I am.
It's a good illustration of the diversity of review board.
Yeah, definitely.
It was damn master students.
Yeah.
So this is inside our baseball academic complaints as well.
But as it relates to the guru sphere and the kind of people we cover, my takeaway from
it would be that a version of responsible heterodoxy or a willingness to go against whatever the kind of dominant
trend is valuable in a lot of disciplines. And even though those people can be extremely annoying,
like Lee Jusom, for example, I think it's good to have them in the field. And that can be the
value of heterodoxy that is sometimes overlooked when we, for example,
focus on people like Brett Weinstein.
Right.
I was thinking this the other day, but I used to have a blog.
I don't really write anymore, but every once in a while I get an idea.
I get lots of ideas for things I never write.
But one was on the benefits of being disagreeable and how like we need disagreeable people.
Not many of them, mind you, just a few.
Preach.
Yeah.
In organizations.
I really do think it's important.
It's important to have people who can say no and can explain why and are willing to
argue with you a little bit.
Now, again, it's not always pleasant.
Lee Jessum, I, you know, we had him on our show and he's actually a very different person
that online as most people are.
But I really don't like his Twitter presence.
I don't follow him because I just find it annoying.
And he occasionally will like at me, but I'm glad that he's around.
I'm glad that he's stirring the pot, even if I don't agree with everything he says.
Even if I sometimes I feel he goes over the line.
I think you just, you need, Chris, I sense a strong disagreeableness in you.
What? What? need, Chris, I sense a strong disagreeableness in you. What?
Yeah, Chris.
Look, I agree with those sentiments too.
And one final nuance though, is that I think that heterodoxy has been defined too much
in terms of political orientation.
There's lots and lots of different ways to be heterodox.
And the name that always pops into my head is Peter Singer. And he's like a classic, you know, this ethicist who talks about
the ethical grounds for killing babies or whatever. It's the ultimate example of you can't
say that. That's wrong. You cannot talk about that. But I do think it's valuable. I think that,
of course, the danger is perhaps the degree to which purely academic discussions,
right?
And that's how I read Peter Singer as philosophical, like him basically putting out a very disagreeable
view that most people would find abhorrent and saying, well, prove where my logic is
wrong, right?
And then that will spark beneficial things.
Of course, the danger is that the
universities and university discourse is totally open to the world now. And I guess when the public
consumes the stuff, not so much in Peter Singer's case, but in other cases, you see it with the race
and IQ stuff, for instance, which seems like bullshit. I mean, the academic material is
intrinsically bullshit, but then it then gets weaponized even further in the public sphere. So.
Yeah. I think the world is a better place. We're in a better position because of this,
a guy like Peter Singer around, who's kind of pushing the ethical boundaries of things and
asking questions and like leading us to think maybe a little bit differently. And I find it,
you know, upsetting when actual academics are doing honest work.
By honest work, I mean they believe in it. They think it's true and they're not trying to pull
the wool over someone's eyes. And for whatever reason, it falls afoul of whatever current
orthodoxy might be. So one that brings to mind is, and this is controversial, but a number of
years ago now, I think her name was Rebecca Tuvel, wrote an article in Hypatia, a feminist philosophy journal, making an argument about the nature of trans identity and suggesting that trans identity is simply like a choice or, you know, what you identify as.
And it's not that it's not tied to sex or anything biological, that then that opens the door to other trans sorts of identities.
And she's the example of transracialism.
And there's a famous case of, I forget her name now.
Rachel Dolezal.
Yeah, Rachel Dolezal, who was the head of the NAACP in the US.
And she's white, but she's acted black and would have a spray on tan and a fake afro,
whatever.
So, and, you know, most people find her behavior abhorhorrent but it seems like she does identify as a black person i don't i don't want to take her too seriously but my
broader point is that two vowels argument was a logical one you might find it distasteful you
might not you might disagree but i find that it's a loss it's a real loss when something like that
is taken you know now everything was retracted and it's taken away and that's no longer we're
not even allowed to talk about that anymore. Meanwhile, like I remember,
it's hilarious. I've got two children, one's 13, one's 10, and they're learning a lot about trying
to date identity in school, what have you. And I was pushing a little bit and then, you know,
cause they have friends now, many friends or not many, but a few who are identified as trans.
And, and then I just, I asked them the question about, gave an example. What if I want to say I identify as a different race? How
would you feel about that? So her argument came in handy there to just kind of push their intuitions
a little bit. I think the re-age campaigns that can re-age around people can definitely take that tenor,
right, that they become just that.
And even in the cases where
people say, well, there were legitimate issues
and the paper should have been retracted. But
there's a very clear social signal
sent about the next
person that wants to write a paper like that,
right? And then, Matt, you
identified the issue
which cuts on the other side, which is when we're talking about it's good to have people that are willing to do controversial work and push the envelope and stuff.
But the race and IQ side of things also shows that you have these people who will do that.
that they clearly are in many occasions, like just so happens that they also have a lot of political views
about restricting immigration from non-white countries and so on, right?
And I think that it ends up messy
because you don't want to ignore that that exists
and that people use the mantle of like,
well, I'm just talking about controversial topics and stuff
and I'm being silenced in the same way in order to advance things which are not good science
and which are like politically violenced agendas.
And we're at the same time as you want to allow for contrarian points of view and discourse
around difficult topics.
So like it ends up being a difficult thing to walk.
And I think that the heterodox sphere, broadly speaking,
Quillette and whatnot definitely fell towards the credulous side
in dealing with any case where somebody said,
I've been silenced, this topic can't be touched, right?
Their take was always huge sympathy.
But on the other side, the social justice one can very much veer towards the, well, you've got to crack a few omelets to make an egg, right?
And I understand that, but if you're the egg that gets cracked, I think you have a
very different view about that than the, yeah, I'm not saying that Matt, you or
Nikki or me have the right answers to that, but just that I understand why it's
a fraught area, but I think I feel shitty saying this, but like having nuance is
an undervalued stance in that area.
And I know that's weaponized, so it makes me cringe even saying that,
but I still
think it is important. Yeah. You bring up a really good point. I mean, this goes back to my point
earlier about free speech. I'm not being a free speech absolutist. I think there are topics that
are off bound, but can I give you a rule about what those topics are? No, I can't. So it ends
up being really difficult to determine. So why is the race and IQ stuff out of bounds, which I
mostly agree with, but in my opinion, at least the 2 Bell stuff isn't. I don't have a good answer.
I think if you're trans, you view the 2 Bell article as threatening to your identity and
maybe dangerous. Yeah. So these are cases of people's convictions, political or moral or
whatever, impinging and it's relative isn't it people's perceptions of what's
inside and outside the acceptable boundaries i mean i have my own little cute example of this
by the way it's not culture worry but i think it's still the same kind of thing which is that
i work on addiction and gambling and the impact of gambling on health well-being and happiness
essentially and there's kind of two camps in this field. There's like a public health,
social justice, we should ban gambling, very high government involvement. Then there's the
kind of more industry positive, people are free to choose to do whatever they like,
and self-responsibility kind of model. So my findings have been unintentionally strongly aligned with the activist kind of side.
Gambling very bad.
This is all the bad things that the problems with gambling cause.
And then in a relatively minor cross-sectional paper where we were measuring the effect of different levels from people that didn't gamble at all, people that were like what we call recreational gamblers, people gambling without problems,
various levels of gambling problems.
And we just looked at their happiness and their wellbeing.
And as expected, as gambling problems increase,
of course, your wellbeing goes down.
But we had this really surprising counter-narrative finding,
which was that gamblers are a lot happier
than people that don't gamble at all.
And I suspect you'd find something similar in the alcohol research, maybe.
People who drink alcohol occasionally are happier than people that are teetotalers.
So, you know, I was like, yes, we tried to control.
I mean, I thought it was due to just more happy people.
The line of causality was the other direction, right?
People that were more hedonic, maybe younger, had the freedom, the resources to go out there and do this stuff would be happier.
We tried to control for everything and the effects still remain. My point is, of course,
I enjoyed that. I enjoyed that result. It was counter-narrative. It wasn't what we expected.
I was very happy to publish it, but I think I did notice that a lot of my colleagues in the
But I think I did notice that a lot of my colleagues that have in the anti-gambling camp just quietly and politely ignored that, not talked about.
Anyway, just a little, like, it's not a cultural thing, just a little example of the sociology,
I guess, that's at play in research.
The selectivity of support or lack thereof, depending on your priors.
Yeah.
I think it's very hard to escape, but, you know, I think that's, we all do that.
We should try hard not to, but we shouldn't fool ourselves to think that we don't do that
all the time.
Yeah.
So Mickey, you've been extremely generous with your time and it's been a pleasure to
discuss everything with you.
And I, this is a case where, you know, I heard you in my ears for years with the
Two Psychologists, Four Beers podcast.
And then now the, having the power social bubble pierced by meeting you, but I mean,
in a good way, not that my golden image was crushed.
So I hardly recommend that anyone who is interested in the kind of topics that we touched
on, that they go and look at the podcast.
And you have a lot of episodes going back a number of years that I think still really
hold up.
So I know that you retired from the podcast, but it's still going with Joel Limbaugh and
Alexa.
Yeah, Alexa Tullett. Alexa Tullett now. but it's still going with Joel Limbaugh and Alexa.
Yeah, Alexa Tullet.
Alexa Tullet now.
So I recommend it to anyone.
And is there anything else that you want to pimp, recommend?
Should people follow you on Twitter?
No, don't follow me on Twitter.
I am essentially almost off Twitter.
I've so restricted the time of usage. For many years,
I struggled with it, but I just put on these little locks on my phone, on my computer,
and I have such restricted access. And then I also do the thing of anytime anyone annoyed me on Twitter, I would unfollow them. And now my Twitter feed, I'm also in a hard ever on,
and it's incredibly boring. It really is dull.
It's like job announcements,
newspapers.
It's like no culture war stuff.
Every once in a while,
if I'm feeling masochistic,
I'll follow you,
Chris.
I'll see what you're doing.
Yeah,
that's yeah.
Just periodically unfollow and follow me.
That's the way to indulge with the culture war.
Okay,
Mickey. So it sounds like you're going
to be focusing on your actual academic
real work instead of faffing
about on social media like us.
I'm in a weird place. I don't have
nothing to pimp. No, you do. You're a Google
scholar. Check out
Mickey's Google scholar.
It's very impressive.
It's very impressive.
Big swinging H-index.
It's nice. There's stuff on mindfulness. It's very impressive. Yeah. Big swinging H index. It's nice.
There's stuff on mindfulness.
There's stuff on ego depletion.
And there's pre-registered studies, like all the things we've talked about.
So you and you are ridiculously productive academically, Mickey.
It's very annoying hearing you on the podcast,
swatting off the sabbaticals and exotic locales and, and then publishing a bunch of
papers.
Very annoying.
I'm trying really hard to correct that.
So we'll see if I can, can slow down.
Yeah.
But thank you for coming on and informing us on the replication crisis and everything
else that you did and for the podcast.
So, yeah.
Well, thanks so much for having me on. And also it's a thrill for me to be on.
I'm not going to lie.
I've also, you guys are both of you, but in my ears for, is it a couple of years
now, a year and a half, maybe?
A year and a half.
Anyway, so you're definitely up there in my, my podcast queue.
So thanks for having me on.
It's been lots of fun.
Yeah.
Oh, and one last thing, Mickey, as well, is that you were one of the first people when we were starting the podcast that we asked a whole bunch of advice for.
I forgot that.
We're podcasting Jordan Peterson.
So you deserve the credit.
I think this is your baby and your responsibility for all our heartaches.
Excellent.
Well, I'm happy that the teacher has become the student because you guys are doing such an excellent job and far surpass anything we've done.
So it's awesome.
I'll be sure.
All right.
Thanks, mate.
Catch you soon.
All right.
See ya.
Goodbye, Mickey.
Auf Wiedersehen.
Sayonara.
Tschüss.
That went back for German. Auonara. Tschüss. The word back for German.
Au revoir.
Yeah.
What's the traditional Australian goodbye?
See you when your legs are straighter?
Okay.
No, I don't know what the Australian is.
That's it.
It's in canon now.
See you when your legs are straighter.
Okay.
Okay.
That's terrible.
Sorry.
When I'm saying goodbye to you, see you when your legs are straighter. Okay. Okay. That's terrible. Sorry, when I'm saying goodbye to you, see when your legs are straighter.
See, the thing is, Chris, I've learned from watching Joe Rogan's podcast is that you can
say absolutely anything about Australia and people will believe you, especially Brett
Weinstein.
Yep.
Yep.
That's pretty true.
You're barely a real country anyway.
So it's, you know, what people imagine is happening there.
Who can say, Matt, that it isn't what is actually happening.
Yeah.
Could be a penal colony or you, you might be living in a tropical
paradise as home and away and neighbors taught me as a child.
Home and away and neighbors are pretty accurate, actually.
That's a pretty fair summary of what life is like here.
There was a Northern Irish character came into, I think, Neighbours
called Connor
and he was a car thief.
Was he a backpacker?
Because in my experience,
you guys are all backpackers here.
Maybe he was a backpacking car thief.
I don't know how he got to Australia.
I don't remember the storyline,
but I do remember being flummoxed
by somebody with a Northern Irish accent
appearing on that show.
It broke the view that it was just an alternate reality where you couldn't actually reach it.
Universe.
Yeah.
No, you can reach it, Chris.
You could come here.
That would be wild for you, wouldn't it?
Just be in Australian suburbia or a little coastal town, not too dissimilar from where
I live.
Like we have a caravan park down the road.
You know, the beach is over there.
I know about caravan parks.
I've been in a caravan park many times it's not exotic for me maybe a caravan park on the
beach but you know Japan is pretty nice the weather is nice it has beautiful scenery don't
don't worry I've seen things I've seen things yeah yeah it's exotic over there too I suppose
yeah that's good well so now, Matt, the important things.
After Mickey kindly helped us sort out the state of psychology,
we now need to sort out the state of our reviews.
With our review of reviews.
Close.
Getting closer.
Getting closer.
What state are our reviews in?
Ship shape, tip top?
Pretty, pretty good this week.
We've had a run of five-star reviews, which is good.
That's what people should be doing.
I've got two positive ones, and then I'm going to get to the negative one.
The positive ones are quite short.
That's why I'm putting two in.
Not for indulgence, Matt.
Just I think it's important to keep things balanced.
So there's a five-star one that says,
Simply Awful.
That's the title.
The way Chris constantly says,
Yeah.
And the way Matt says,
Anything and something will make you die a little bit inside every time you listen.
The actual content is still pretty good, though,
and I never miss an episode, and that's from Irish Tongue.
Well, that's nice, I think.
Say something.
Wait.
Say something.
Something.
Th.
Th.
Something.
Well, they say you say something.
I don't say something.
Something.
But that's not true.
That's a damn lie.
No, no, no.
There's a confusion here. I cannot pronounce the T-H. That's not true. That's a damn lie. No, no, no. There's a confusion here.
I cannot pronounce the T-H.
That's not what they've written.
They have written the T-H.
Oh.
And they've done the ending with a, instead of a G, a K.
So you say some think.
I say some think.
Yeah.
Do I? Maybe. i haven't noticed okay
but that was confusing hearing you reading their yeah phonetic i understand spelling of my accent
i'm still struggling to decouple it i should have thought that one out we should just let it go my
own mispronunciation issues that was so bad so um i was like god damn that sounds
like chris that's not me yeah moving swiftly on to this important one the title is let chris talk
exclamation mark and it says let chris talk let chris talk i say it half in jest love you guys
great show keep up the good work that's from noodles 115 so thank it half in jest. Love you guys. Great show. Keep up the good work. That's from Noodles
115. So
thank you, Noodles. Half in jest.
Half in jest. Yeah.
That was going well until
you said half in jest. That should have been
fully full satire.
God's sake.
That was gad sad level
satire there. I approve. Sledgehammer
satire, but he tempered it.
You shouldn't never do that.
Well, after that ringing endorsement, the two of them ringing endorsements, I will now
turn to the negative review, which is called, the title is Gaslighting Anti-Vax Shields.
And it's one star.
And it says, someone recommended
I listen to this podcast because
supposedly they break down
myths and stuff
with a scientific basis.
But I listened and all I heard
was a bunch of anti-vax nonsense
and justification
of why Joe Rogan is
actually good for society.
I think this is some gaslighting nonsense.
This is from RyRy12000.
What?
Now that's confusing, Matt.
Chris.
Do you know what's happened?
Antivax nonsense.
No, I don't.
Let me see if I can Sherlock this shit for you.
So we are not pro-antivax people. What? Wait, pro-antivax? uh so we are not pro anti-vax people what wait pro anti-vax yeah we're
not we are not anti-vax people we are not pro anti-vax people i was correct we are not in fever
of people who are anti-vax see okay stay with me old man technically stay with me But we do know someone who has recently been on our show making arguments about, well, you know, aren't there problems with the way that public health authorities have applied things?
And he also did make a case for why Joe Rogan, is he really that bad?
So what this is, Matt, this is Tamla.
We're getting Tamla's feedback.
Goddamn Tamla.
It is from Very Bad Wizards.
Damn you, Tamla.
Damn you.
It comes on our show.
This is.
It spews pro-Joe Rogan propaganda and anti-vax light talking points.
And we get the splashback negative reviews.
What the hell?
This is so unfair.
We do not endorse, Tam Laura, anything that man stands for.
He believes in ghosts.
Leave your reviews on very bad wizards.
He believes in ghosts.
That's right.
We're anti-ghosts.
We're very anti-ghosts.
We're like the ghost busters.
We're pro-anti-ghosts.
Yeah, we're pro-ghost busters.
We're ghost negative in Eric Weinstein lingo.
So that reviewer, if you're listening, which of course they're not,
they should go to Very Bad Wizards.
Leave your bad reviews there.
Also, don't bring your garbage and dump it on our lawn.
At the same time, RyRy's a bit of a moron, isn't he?
I mean, if he got that far into the episode where Tamler is, you know,
making his half-hearted defense of Rogan,
you've already listened to the intro
explaining that he's a guest
and we're openly disagreeing with him
in that part.
So that guy being confused,
is this the podcast host?
Are they advocating this?
Like, come on.
We don't want you as a listener.
You couldn't even work out who's the guest. Yeah, keep up, mate. Keep up. You've got to be on the
ball. You've got to be sharp to listen to this podcast. Well, Chris, that all makes sense. The
people that left us positive reviews were witty and wry and charming. And the person that left
a negative review is clearly an idiot. So, you know, all is right with the world no cognitive dissonance
whatsoever nope stands to reason and now matt speaking of people operating under the shadow
of cognitive dissonance our patreons our patreons lovely lovely patrons yum yum yum yum you know
that you know the one thing i don't like technically in patron lingo the people who are part of your patron are called patrons.
Patrons.
Patrons.
Oh, patrons.
They're not called patrons.
They are called patrons.
Yeah.
No, but patrons.
Patrons.
Patrons.
Is that how you pronounce that?
Like without the E?
Patrons.
That's how I pronounce it.
Okay, fine.
That's all right. Patrons. We'll just delete all that. You know, you Like without the E? Patrons. That's how I pronounce it. Okay, fine. Then it's all right.
Patrons.
We'll just delete all that.
You know, you know.
No, we won't.
No, we won't.
I'm doing the editing.
Oh, no.
Oh, no.
Yeah.
Chris, Chris, say egregious.
Say egregious.
Egregious.
It's easy, Matt.
What's the problem?
Oh, let's see.
Nobody's even heard that, Matt, because it's always been edited out.
So, yeah.
Yeah.
So anyway, with that important point noted, we have to find, the soil will save us.
These are our Galaxy Brain gurus, incidentally.
The soil will save us.
A little bit nationalist for my liking, but you know, your username.
No, no, no, no. I'm sure it's ecological.'s a ecological okay that's better nothing to do with blood yeah chris no blood in soil
yeah yeah he didn't mention blood that's true so safe there um christian david small tom v
kyle wilson kst3141, and Shane Gronholz.
Thank you one and all. Very good.
Yeah. Play the clip. I'll do that.
You're sitting on one of the
great scientific stories that I've ever
heard, and you're so polite.
And, hey, wait a minute. Am I
an expert? I kind of am.
Yeah.
I don't trust people at all.
Okay.
So those were the Galaxy Brain Gurus.
Revolutionary geniuses.
We have Hamish Buchan.
Buchan?
Buchan.
Stephen Connor.
Buchan.
Thank you.
Stephen Connor.
Connor Peck. Connor Peck. Connor Peik. Konopikik. Konopikik. Konopikik. Stefan Konopikik. Andy Seaton. God, I feel like a fucking alien trying to, you knowody, Mitchell W., and Greg Binder.
And by the way, Matt, Dag Soros, just to mention, Dag Soros is a kind of famous comedian from
Norway.
So we have a famous comedian who listens to us from Norway.
I continue to be in awe at the way you pronounce these things.
Like how anyone could pronounce Carmody. Well, yeah, I'm sorry about that, but that's what I do.
That's what I do. Pronunciation is not my strong suit. But you are revolutionary thinkers, and we thank you. Maybe you can spit out that hydrogenated thinking and let yourself feed off of your own thinking.
What you really are is an unbelievable thinker and researcher, a thinker that the world doesn't know.
Correct. Thank you, Eric.
Correct. Thank you, Eric. And last, but certainly not least, Matt, the conspiracy hypothesizers,
the people who set up the hypothesizers, hypotheses to be knocked down. They are Hashim Mudeh,
Hashim Mudeh, maybe, Heller Gewerding, Frederick Dumont, Timothy Rabin,in Jack Amira E Alan Coogan
and
No Lips or Joints
Oh, No Lips or Joints
Okay, that's not a word
No Lips or Joints
I like that last one
Yeah
Okay, yeah
Thank you, one and all
Very good
Thank you
Every great idea starts with a minority of one
We are not going to advance conspiracy theories.
We will advance conspiracy hypotheses.
Okay.
So that's that for today, Matt.
And, you know, for people interested on the Patreon, there is extra content there.
We put the interviews out.
We put out the decoding academia series where we discuss papers and that kind of thing.
And yeah, there's tons of bonus
stuff that we have there. Yeah. And coming up on the Decoding Academia series, there's got an
interesting paper suggested by Chris about different types of sort of imitation between
children and chimpanzees and the kind of links that has to cultural transmission. So yeah,
good stuff.
You should subscribe.
That's right.
Good benefits.
If you want to look at things through an evolutionary lens,
that's it.
Show you how to do that.
This is the real place to come for you. Yeah.
Well, that's that.
So, Matt, it's been fun.
I've only got one thing I need to tell you. It's a
motto for life. Consider
the disc, note the gin.
Duly considered
and well noted.
Yes.
All right. Good day to you.
Good day to you.
Sir, whoever you are.
That's right.
Consider your life choices, Matt. But not too hard, otherwise you won't come back next time. So, ciao you are. That's right. That's right. Yeah. Consider your life choices, Matt.
But not too hard.
Otherwise, you won't come back next time.
So ciao for now.
Ciao, ciao. Thank you. you