Decoding the Gurus - Special Episode: Interview with Stuart Ritchie on Hunter Gatherers in the 21st Century, covid skeptics, and bad science
Episode Date: September 24, 2021This week we have an engaging interview with the scientist, author, and public science communicator Stuart Ritchie. Stuart wrote the excellent 'Science Fictions: Exposing Fraud, Bias, Negligence and H...ype in Science' and is a prolific advocate fr better science and more nuanced public discourse.In this episode we start of by discussing Stuart's recently published review of Bret Weinstein and Heather Heying's new book 'A Hunter Gatherer's Guide to the 21st Century'. Was Stuart a fan and is now an acolyte of Weinsteinian lineage theory? Tune in to find out.Also, for those who enjoy 'challenging conversations', robust discourse, and regularly shop in the marketplace of ideas, there is an extended discussion on whether we have been a little bit too kind on the government and public health institutions. Stuart uses facts and logic and attempts to DESTROY us, so come and get your well earned vicarious catharsis but be prepared for plenty of postmodern deflection. We've learned from the best.LinksStuart's Review of Bret & Heather's new book at the GuardianWebsite for Stuart's (Excellent) Book: Science FictionsAnti-Virus: The Covid 19 FAQ Website'How the Experts Messed up on Covid' at Unherd by Stuart & Michael Story'How Covid Skeptics were duped by the Wonderdrug Ivermectin' at New Statesman by StuartChris' Tweet-thread Chapter by Chapter review of Stuart's Book (use the hashtags to find the rest)Kevin Bird's Tweet with an extract from B&H's bookThis Week's SponsorCheck out the sponsor of this week's episode, Ground News, and get the app at ground.news/gurus.
Transcript
Discussion (0)
Hello and welcome to Decoding the Guru, it's the podcast where an anthropologist and a
psychologist listen to the greatest minds the world has to offer and we try our best
to understand what they're talking about.
I'm Matt Brown and with me is Chris Kavanagh.
How's it going, Chris?
Matt, you're a professor.
Did you forget your important titles?
I mean, what's going on?
You're trying to keep it casual this week?
Yeah.
Playing it cool this week.
Playing it cool.
What the listeners can't see is that when Matt started this, he had on a
Hulk Hogan-esque bandana and he's taken it off and now has like a dark brown
kind of crazy professor hairstyle.
So maybe just calling yourself a professor would be too much.
That's why you held off, I guess.
Well, I'm rather proud of my hair.
I'm growing it long and I'm hoping to achieve.
And vertical.
And vertical at the moment, thanks to the beanie I was wearing, but I'm
hoping to achieve a Weinsteinian mop.
A bouffant.
That's oh, that is a unique hairstyle.
That would, you know, that might be the hairstyle of the 21st century.
Um, and do you, do you call beanies monkey hats?
Like do you know what a monkey hat is?
No, no, that's not a thing.
That's a Northern Irish thing at least.
We call them monkey hats.
So that's ridiculous.
I don't know why.
Well, is it Matt?
Is it any better than worse than beanie?
Well, I suppose it's not as bad as australian place names which sound famously unusual to foreigners ears but make perfect sense
to us wool bollocks cape and whatnot sprang bejang
cultural relativism is where it's at matt don't be judging other people's Sprang bejang. Cause that's creaking. Things like that.
Cultural relativism is where it's at, Matt. Don't be judging other people's cultures.
Um,
Oh, well, speaking of cultures, um, sticking to your own culture, which
is what you and I always try to do.
It's very good idea.
Yeah.
Yeah.
There's a bit, some surprising support against what is it?
Cultural appropriation, food appropriation coming from an unexpected point.
Well, well, well.
Hold your horses, Matt.
You know, there's a dark horse, caliphine, right there that we need to introduce.
So this week's episode, we're going to be talking to Stuart Ritchie about a wide
variety of things, he's going to try and prove us wrong about stuff that he believes, but he'll be, he'll be crushed by the end
of the episode of the Quivering Mask, but...
David Pérez- You have to be Arndt.
David Pérez- Yeah.
Or, or he'll destroy us.
You be the judge.
You be the judge.
That it's a contentious back and forth rip roaring interview showing the diversity
of thought available on decoding
the gurus different flavors of gray who are available and yeah so part of the thing that
we're going to be talking this year about is he he's written a review for the guardian on
brett weinstein and heller haying people People have pointed out, I say Heller Hain.
I can't remember.
Anyway, their new book about the 21st century hunter gallerist guide.
Now, we're not going to talk about that because we talked about it in length in the episode,
but something that's happened since then, which is quite funny, is that an extract from the book was shared on Twitter by Kevin Bird, and he took a little clip.
I'll just read it.
This is one of the bullet points, a kind of guide that comes at the end of the chapter.
So consider your ethnicity and look to its culinary tradition for a guide to that.
If you are Italian, look to Italian cuisine for clues as how you should eat.
If you're Japanese,
look to Japanese cuisine.
In particular,
look to the culinary traditions
of home cooking
as the foods represented
in restaurants,
while often delicious,
often represent only a sliver
of a culinary tradition's
full panoply of options.
I love that.
I love it.
Yeah, the florid language. What? Yeah. Panoply of options. David Pérez- I love that. I love it. David Pérez- Yeah. The florid language.
What?
Yeah.
Panoply of options, Matt.
Matt Therese Eberhardt Panoply.
Yeah.
And it's a lovely mixture of homespun wisdom, but there's this evolutionary
biology rationale for people sticking to their own food, which is implicit here,
Chris, am I wrong in thinking that the rationale is, or you're nodding your head.
What's.
It is evolutionary.
And the thing which people will reference to support this is lactose tolerance,
right, that in various populations where farming of cows and other such creatures
was around that, like people consumed milk and those
populations are tolerant to lactose into adulthood.
Whereas other populations were that wasn't so prevalent have lactose intolerance.
They can consume milk, but if they take too much, it can have bad consequences.
Right.
And this, this is true.
So if you take like a, a generous reading, you can say, well, they're just pointing out that, you know, there might be adaptations or there may be evolutionary consequences to the types of foods that people have eaten for generations that aren't immediately apparent.
But it's actually difficult to come up with other strong examples of this.
Like the one that everybody mentions is lactose tolerance.
And that's the only one that I hear 99% of people mention.
And also they give the example of like Italian food, but modern Italian cuisine is not exactly
something that has been
around for 10,000 years, right?
That, uh, so.
No, so let's, let's get straight in.
Like, that's the charitable version done.
Let's get straight into the uncharitable version because the, the strong
implication here is that, and this is the theme of the book, so I don't
think we're mind reading here, but the idea is that if, if a tradition has been around for a long time,
um, then it exists for a reason.
And that reason is basically a culture zeroing in on biological benefits,
evolutionary benefits to ourselves.
So according to their reasoning, because Japanese people, for instance, like raw fish and rice, and Italian people maybe don't.
Actually, it's a bad example.
Say Americans.
Yeah, they do.
Don't.
Americans also like sushi now, but yeah.
Yeah, goddammit.
That's just what I mean.
Australians, then, traditionally, haven't eaten a great deal of either.
You know what I mean?
Australians, traditionally, haven't eaten a great deal of either.
And that's for a reason.
Because there's biological differences between Japanese people and Australians. You're adapted to shrimps.
And barbecues.
And beer.
Well, in the same way, I responded on Twitter by saying that this seems like, you know,
Finlay Veal hate speech directed at the Irish, because the implication is that we are adapted to
cuisine focused around potatoes and root vegetables, you know, I'm not
disparaging Irish cuisine entirely.
You know, there's enjoyable components there and fine British cuisine,
but it does, it does feel like, you know, telling Italians that they should focus
on Italian food and Japanese, these two culinary traditions, which are widely
recognized as amongst the best in the world.
Yeah.
What about Irish people stick to your Irish Jew and potato pie,
or you know, stick in Guinness.
literal, literal violence.
That's right.
I mean, I was triggered by this too, because traditional Australian food that
doesn't involve a preparation from somewhere else, it's not good, Chris.
It's like, are you sure?
It's not good, man.
It's like, like to understand Australian traditional food such that it exists is
like take English food and then just make it worse, make it so much worse.
Can you do that?
Because that's what made it out to the colonies.
Like, so salted pepper would be the, would be the most interesting
flavors that you would come across.
So, yeah.
I think it's bad advice for, for cosmopolitanists.
Although, you know, you can explore your culinary traditions, fine, do that.
But like their version has too much of a, a strange hyper-adaptionist evolutionary tinge to it.
And a more holistic one as well.
Yeah. So, you know, but, um, look where it's just wrong in, in scientific terms is just the
amazing hyper-adaptionist view that everything that has ever happened happens for this evolutionary
biology.
If it sticks around, this is the thing.
We get into it with Stuart, but you know,
Yeah.
So I'm not going to get into it too much, but the, but the one thing that did occur
to me was that, I mean, so this was randomly dunked on and rightly so, but
it, one thing I did notice is that, you know, if you compare that to some of the
other, um, sorts of stuff that gets dunked on, like some of the more silly takes on
food appropriation, that people shouldn't cook or eat food from other cultures.
The argument is that they can eat it, but they should only eat it when it's
prepared for somebody from that, uh, an authentic thing, right?
Like the,
there's a whole spectrum of stuff, right.
Just talking about the, the sillier versions of it, which, which get
done, which get dunked upon as well.
But yeah, one thing I've noticed is that the Venn diagram of people ducking on the
Weinstein's book excerpt and the set of people dunking on the food of preparation
stuff is completely not overlapping, Chris.
There's a very slim overlap in that and it does seem a neat parallel because
it like as as kevin bird's tweet said brett and heller say no race mixing but for your food but
that could equally be appended to some of the more woke takes on you know the horrors of people appropriating traditions, other food traditions.
Well, there you go, Chris.
This is, this is peak centrism, our take on this.
There you go, folks.
You can have your milk toast centrism served at 24 degrees or 26 degrees, but you get both
kinds here.
It's great.
Yeah.
Like I can say, I wish there was a little bit more equal opportunity dunking as opposed to like ideology based dunks.
But there is also the fact that I understand when people extend charity to people that they consider to be on the whole doing things which are less harmful than other people.
to be on the whole doing things which are less harmful than other people.
And then you could take like the cartoon version and say like, oh, maybe the, you know, the neo-Nazi who's saying you shouldn't eat food outside your culture.
It isn't exactly morally the same as somebody saying you shouldn't
appropriate the food from another culture and rely on your, you
know, privilege to gain benefit that people from that culture can't have.
There's different motivations underlying that may seem important
to whether you would dunk or not.
But just broadly speaking, it would be nice to see some more consistency
on the online sphere, can we say.
David Pé say. Hmm.
Agreed.
Agreed.
Well, isn't that the perfect pivot to our crass commercialization segment?
We've just talked about the beauty of peak centrism, and we need to alert our audience
that this episode is the first one where we're going to feature an advertisement.
that this episode is the first one where we're going to feature an advertisement.
And it is for a product which in certain size could be seen as what kind of a milquetoast centrist pro product. And we all know that famous example, Matt, is some people advocate for genocide.
Some people say it's bad.
And the appropriate compromiseises, you know,
somewhere in the middle, a little bit genocide that's, that's fine.
Right.
That's that shows the beauty of adopting a perfectly balanced
centrist perspective or, or not as the case may be.
The golden mean, the golden mean.
Yeah.
The golden this.
So there, there are some issues with always seeking out a
middle ground in opinions um but
that's not what we do and that's not what the thing which we're going to advertise does is it
matt no these these penis enhancement pills will have no no effect in that regard. They'll satisfy conservatives, liberals, IDWs.
Your penis will grow to
huge levels that never before
seen levels.
Tremendous, tremendous levels.
Yeah. Okay. Yeah. So yeah, look, we've
sold it a little bit, but it'll be all right.
But not with penis pills. That was a joke.
We're not yet. We'll get there.
This whole thing is just a ruse to
attract various enhancement pills and supplements, but we haven't got the offers yet. We'll, you know, we'll get there. This whole thing is just a ruse to attract various enhancement pills and supplements,
but we haven't got the offers yet.
Yeah.
Well, we'll see what kind of package they put together for us, Chris.
Yes.
Wink.
Wink, wink.
So before that, Matt, we need to highlight how good we are and why people would want
to work with us as advertising partners.
So why don't we mention what we're actually going to promote and we'll add
some jaunty music here to illustrate that this is an advertisement, jaunty music.
Do do do.
Yeah.
So sorry.
Are we doing the ad reads happening now?
Is it?
That's it, man.
Yeah.
Okay.
Okay.
All right.
And I'll yeah.
Okay.
I'm I'm up.
You know, it's professionalism it's what you pay for what you get that's what this is i was gonna hear this except for our editor so there's this app called ground news
which allows you to compare how stories are being covered across the political spectrum.
So I gave it a bit of a go.
And yeah, it was kind of interesting.
You can search for a thing.
You can see a particular story that you might have stumbled across.
You can see whether it's being covered across the entire political spectrum from relatively mainstream or non-partisan outlets in the middle.
Not that they're better or anything, Chris.
But there's also the right-wing ecosystem, the-wing ecosystem and i tried it out and yeah you can see
that there are you know stories are really quite different so you can get a sense of you know what
people of different political leanings what their what their infosphere actually is who would have
bought it matt the right and the left cover stories differently. This is keen insight that you're providing here.
But it's more that you said that they don't cover the story at all.
Yeah, like there's like some story about Joe Biden appearing senile or something like that.
Maybe not surprisingly, it'll only exist.
You're blowing my mind here.
You're blowing my mind that you would see that kind of thing in right-wing media.
But I do like the news comparison feature because, you know, it's useful.
Whether or not the stories are good or not, just to be able to compare what's kind of trending within different ecosystems or how things are being covered.
I think that that's a good thing.
a good thing and so there's an app and there's a website called brown news that lets you identify the bias of media outlets and and see how they're framing issues compared to other news organizations
that you know that seems in line with the kind of thing we generally think is worthwhile doing right
we listen to some content that we don't like on occasion. And yeah.
Yeah, no, it's good.
So ground.news is the website.
And I'm sure you can search for ground news
and download the app too.
Cheers.
But what you should do
if you're doing that
is use our particular link,
ground.news forward slash gurus,
because then people know
that they heard this
and that's where they come from. And then you're able to see every side of every news story and make us
incredibly attractive to advertisers worldwide. Good. The important things. So there, Matt, and after that crass commercialization, we're going to turn
to an interview with Stuart Ritchie talking about how market incentives
and so on are affecting good science and reason in the discursal sphere.
Right?
Stuart Ritchie, Yes, we will.
So Matt and Chris in the past, take it away.
Hello and welcome to Decoding the Gurus.
I'm Matt Brown.
With me as always is Chris Kavanagh.
But today we had someone with us to help us figure out what is true,
beautiful, and real in this crazy mixed up world. And that person is Stuart J. Ritchie. Now,
Stuart is a Scottish psychologist. He works at King's College, London, and he's also an author,
has written a book on intelligence, and most recently has written a book called Science
Fictions, How Fraud, Bias, Neglig bias, negligence, and hype undermine the search for truth.
So obviously this is something we like to talk about.
Welcome, Stuart.
Hi, thanks for having me on.
And I can personally attest, having written an exhaustive Twitter thread for each chapter
of Stuart's work, that it's very good.
And in line with the podcast, I only have a list, Stuart, of 20 complaints that I'm
not going to read either.
That's the spirit of the book.
But thank you.
That's very kind.
Yeah, very, very good.
I do remember that back when that was happening, that there was a clear contrast because I had gotten
an advanced copy sent to me of James Lindsay and Helen Puckrose's book, Cynical Theories.
And their response to me saying, oh, I'll do a tweet thread about it was to insinuate that I
might face legal action were I to do that. And then your book came, it's fair to say it was
probably going to be a more positive review,
but your reaction was only the semi-private legal front.
So I appreciated that.
Yeah.
Any publicity is good publicity.
I remember them insinuating that they would sue you if you tweeted about their book.
I think they said, you're welcome to make us even more famous if you violate copyright or whatever.
A strange way for authors to deal with readers of the book, but fair enough.
That was also before the turn.
But with James Lindsay, that's not what we're here for.
So Matt.
One of the things we are here for is to talk about Brit and Heather, actually,
because they've written a new book.
Stuart, what's this book called?
Got it right here it's by heather
harring and brett weinstein it's called a hunter gatherer's guide to the 21st century evolution and
the challenges of modern life it's just coming out this month i don't know exactly when this
podcast is coming out but it's coming out on i think the 16th of september i see it here i've
got a little publicity thing it's got nice reviews from jonathan height says it's good and robert
sapolsky says it brett and heather are good famous people said nice things about it It's got nice reviews from Jonathan Haidt says it's good. And Robert Sapolsky says it, Brett and Heather are good. Famous people have said nice things about it.
There's a nice cover.
Yeah. It's got a little sort of caveman holding a briefcase. And the whole point of the book is
about how we're unsuited to the modern world and all the kind of health and psychological and
societal problems that that causes. Anyway, the reason that I'm bringing this up is I was asked
to write a review of it for The Guardian,
which is either,
by the time this podcast comes out,
is either online
or about to be online.
And I thought it was rubbish.
I thought it was a rubbish book.
It's a crap book.
What?
What?
Oh my God,
this is very disappointing.
I'm sort of vaguely familiar.
I'm not as familiar
as you guys are.
What?
We don't know
these people that well.
We, you know,
we've covered them
once or twice, but, you know. I'm not as obsessed with them although i have sort of become slightly
obsessed with them since reading the book and looking at their podcast and so on and yeah what
an amazingly annoying pair of people they are and that's one of the things that's in the reviews that
they come across as really annoying in the book and on the podcast and that they do as well perhaps
before we talk about the book it's interesting to talk about why this book seems like it probably will actually be very
successful because it's a very appealing concept, isn't it? The Hunter-Gatherer's Guide to the 21st
Century. It's tapping into evolutionary psychology, which I like, and I think you guys generally like
too, even though there is a fair bit of nonsense you can find as well. And it taps into a very appealing theme, which is that we're these evolved organisms that are
navigating this very artificial modern world. And in some ways we've constructed it to suit
us very well. And obviously in other ways, there are things like highly processed sugar and cocaine
or whatever that are lots of fun, but aren't very good for us. It's an appealing premise, isn't it?
Yeah, absolutely. And there are some fair points in it. They talk about things like
staying up all night looking at blue light and so on. And I'm not quite sure if there's
good evidence for that. But the point is that they talk about the weird novel things that are
in our environment. And you can see why people would find that an appealing sort of premise,
that the reason you can't sleep is you evolved in a very different place and a different set
of circumstances. And that's why the modern world doesn't quite work for you. You can see how that
would work. And the reason that you're a bit unhealthy and a bit overweight is because,
as you say, highly processed sugars and so on, it's an appealing premise. The problem is that
they have very low standards when it comes to evidence for basically everything that they talk
about. I don't have any issues with the basic rationale of evolutionary psychology, but the
problem that has occurred again and again in all the controversies over the years about evolutionary
psychology is the low standard of evidence that something is an adaptation or that something is
related to, I don't know, the ovulatory cycle. You were talking about Gad Saad talking about
that in one of your previous podcasts and the very, very small study that he had produced to
back that up. And there are problems like that in this book as well.
And it's linking of evolution to the modern world.
There's a whole literature around this kind of topic.
You could go back to Darwin.
I'm sure they would like to.
Also Desmond Morris, The Naked Ape and similar books, which often do have good insights into
them, though, like you say, there's also the issue
of speculative theorizing, which always plagues them. But in particular, you mentioned some of
the things that we've noticed, and one of them was the kind of hyper-adaptionism, which seems to
suffuse Brett and Heller's worldview, where if something exists now, it's almost like it has to have
an adaptive purpose. You also see some echoes of that in some of Jordan Peterson's stuff,
but I wonder if you can make it clear for people why there might be an issue with that. What's
wrong with seeing things that survive as likely being adaptations?
Yeah, I'm not an evolutionary biologist, but like
many undergraduates, I did spend a lot of time reading Richard Dawkins' The Selfish Gene and
Richard Dawkins arguing with Stephen Jay Gould, Richard Lewontin and people like that about
adaptation and about what adaptation means. And a kind of a, I guess you would say, philosophy of
biology conversation about what it means for something to be an adaptation,
what it means for something to evolve. From that, I learned that this is a major debate in evolutionary biology. You can't just look at some feature of an organism and say,
this evolved because there are all these other ways that things could have evolved.
There are mechanisms like genetic drift. There are features like what Stephen Jay Gould would
call spandrels, which are things which are necessary parts of the evolution of other
features, which look like they might be features themselves, but are in fact not.
The analogy is to these little parts on the side of archways inside churches,
and they've got little bits painted in them. You might think that was made for that little
scene to be painted in, but in fact, it's a necessary part of the overall archway
and it came about by accident.
Stephen Jay Gould and Richard Lewinson wrote this essay saying,
there are some aspects of things that you think might be evolved
that are actually accidents, that are actually spandrels.
And Richard Dawkins and Daniel Dennett, Stephen Pinker,
people like that argue against it and say,
well, actually, there are different ways of interpreting this philosophically.
And there's a whole back and forth debate.
And I probably would lean more to the Daniel Dennett side of it.
But that doesn't mean you just can't have the debate at all.
And it doesn't mean you can just assume that everything is an adaptation.
But Brett Weinstein thinks that he has solved this problem.
He thinks that he has come along and just totally fixed this issue with what he calls,
I think it's a three-step test of adaptation, right? And
so if a feature of an animal is complex, and he very glibly glosses over what that might mean,
but something that has to be complex, you'd think there might be an interesting philosophical
discussion about what complex actually means in this case. But if something is complex and costly,
that is if there's an energetic cost or some kind of material cost to building it or maintaining it.
And if it's been around for a very long time, then it can be assumed to be an adaptation.
Now, lots of people would say there are lots of things which organisms do, which they might have got into by accident, not through adaptation.
There are lots of things which cultures do, because by the way, he applies this same test to cultural things too.
things too. There are lots of things which cultures do, which are potentially a bit self-destructive and potentially didn't come about because they advance that culture or make it
fitter in the kind of evolutionary sense. But he just completely ignores all that and says,
well, these things are adaptations. And so therefore pretty much anything, any feature
can be described as an adaptation on his scheme. And that's what they do in the book. They talk
about essentially everything as an adaptation. And they go even further and they talk about something which i didn't have space to talk
about in my review called the omega principle have you ever encountered this and you're listening to
brett's and her stuff no no the the omega principle is about how you apply evolutionary
reasoning to culture and they talk about epigenetic regulators such as culture now of
course in biology epigenetics means things like the methylation of the genome and actual biological aspects that
regulate the switching on and off of genes and so on. But they say our definition of epigenetic
is different from that. We want to talk about something different. So they say culture is
epigenetic because it's above the genome. And I suppose in some sense you could say that,
but it's needlessly confusing anyway. And they say, from the Omega Principle, we derive a powerful concept, any expensive and long-lasting cultural
trait, such as traditions passed down within a lineage for thousands of years, should be presumed
to be adaptive, which I think is, as I've just mentioned, a ridiculous thing. But the funniest
thing to me is, and Chris, you're a cultural anthropologist, there'll be many things which
I'm sure you can think of that cultures do, which are not necessarily adaptive, even though they're
expensive and long lasting. But the funniest thing about it, in my view, is that the reason
it's called the Omega Principle is that, and here's the quote, we've chosen to use the signifier
omega to call to mind pi, like the other Greek letter, and thus indicate the obligate nature
of the relationship. Adaptive elements of culture are no more independent of genes
than the diameter of a circle is independent of that circle's circumference.
So they called it the Omega Principle because it's a Greek letter
and that will remind people of a different Greek letter
that makes them think of a circle,
that makes them think of something being related to something else.
Wow.
It's mind-tangling.
As if Greek letters are not used anywhere else in science or no one ever associates Greek letters with anything else. Wow. It's mind-tangling. As if Greek letters are not used anywhere else in science,
or no one ever associates Greek letters with anything else. Of course, we now all associate
them with coronavirus variants. They're just slightly off. Their reasoning and their way of
thinking are skew a bit. Anyone who's read the evolutionary literature, evolutionary biology
literature, wouldn't think that way about evolution. And anyone who's taught students
and stuff or used science in any real way wouldn't think that way about evolution. And anyone who's taught students and stuff or used science in any real way
wouldn't think that way about Greek letters.
It's just a weird way of thinking about stuff.
And it comes across a few times in the book.
I just think they're just a bit awful.
They just don't really think in the same way
that perhaps you or I do.
There's a couple of things that come to mind from that.
One thing, Stuart, which is hugely important to correct
is cognitive anthropologists the cultural anthropologists are unfavorable they predispose
to me so the you know we fought the culture wars for a reason in the internet i apologize profusely
cognitive anthropologists my apologies that's all right i'll accept that but your point about
cultural practices that are long lasting and must in some way be beneficial,
like what?
Like bloodletting, for example, or trephining?
Well, exactly.
There's all these things which they're passed down from generation to generation and so
on.
Yeah, all the stuff that we had before the advent of modern medicine.
It seems like there's all this literature which is sort of related to the stuff they're
talking about.
And I mean, using the Greek letters just suggests they don't know about statistics because that
would get horrifically confusing for students.
I can imagine their response to that if you brought up bloodletting or something, because
I know that they invoke things that kind of shadow the work of Joe Henrich and co, who tried to argue
that cultural taboos can actually encode adaptive information, which isn't obvious at the surface
level because there's some prohibition about women eating a specific kind of fish. And it turns out
that can build up mercury or whatever it is and have an effect. Even those examples are challenged, but I feel like it's almost hard in some respect
because they're quoting close to legitimate areas of science
and they're occasionally touching on it and referencing it,
but they just take it into this realm of their own bespoke versions,
which are much more speculative
and much less firmly grounded
in empirical evidence.
That's the feeling I get.
At the very least, you have to do the research.
You have to actually go out there and check.
It's a hypothesis and you have to go out and check.
You can't just assume.
And what they're very much pushing the reader to do is just assume that all cultural traits
have come about for a reason and all evolutionary biological traits have come about for a reason. They're asking you not to do
the work. And I feel like by just gliding over that whole aspect of evolutionary biology,
I say this in the review, that they're doing the reader a disservice. They're saying, you know,
don't worry about all that endless literature on adaptationism and what it means. Just listen to us.
It's adaptive, right? These things are all adaptations. And so now we can carry on with the rest of the book. It seems like a perfect example of what's
wrong with evolutionary psychology or pop evolutionary psychology, which is drawing
those direct causal connections in every case between, oh, you see this behavior, you see this
feature, what's it for? It must be for something tremendously important. And I teach neurophysiology, so it's totally biological,
right? It's all about those little squishy parts of the brain, how they're all interconnected
and what they do. And it's obviously based on evolutionary biology in a sense, but at the same
time, one of the things that comes home to you is that an awful lot about evolution is contingent,
that there isn't any capacity to redesign things. In a new model, of course, it has to grow like a city or something
like that in an organic sense. And that means that there are lots and lots of suboptimal
design choices that have to be made because of that process. The other thing I wanted to pick
up is that the implication of their reasoning is that if it's natural, it's good. And if it's been around for
a long time from a cultural level, it is therefore also good. So this sort of connects to your point
about this sort of 18th century Burkean conservatism. Say a bit more about that.
Yeah, I mentioned this in the review. So the central metaphor of the Chesterton's fence
thing, you come across a fence
and you think, I want to get rid of this fence. Why is this fence here? Let's remove this fence.
But then it turns out that the fence was actually doing something useful and you've made a terrible
mistake by removing it. And so you should always ask why something is there before you remove it
and before you destroy it and change it and so on. And kind of general like conservative point
that you might want to make. And it's a fairly good point. In some circumstances, you don't want to just make changes to systems
because they might have evolved or developed in ways which you haven't quite grasped,
but there might be a reason for it.
And the way that they apply that is they say,
there's a reason for all the stuff that we do culturally,
and there's also a reason for the way that we are biologically,
and that's evolution, cultural evolution.
And so we shouldn't mess around with it.
And by messing around with it, they're talking about things in the modern world,
like having a blue light on your iPhone, but also having casual sex and all sorts of things like
that, including they have these sections at the end of each chapter with a kind of a bullet point
list of things which you should and shouldn't do in a sort of a self-help kind of way. And some of
them aren't fleshed out at all. Some of them are really weird and bizarre and not mentioned in the preceding chapter at one point they say you shouldn't have the market involved in humor or
music and you go what we shouldn't pay to see a comedy gig or something or buy an album a record
it's just a very strange thing and there's no explanation for it but the point is what they're
trying to say is humor and music are there for
some kind of evolution reason, and we shouldn't mess around with it in the modern world with
market forces or something. I actually don't know what they're trying to say there. But it's all
part of this thing about things are the way they are for a reason, and you shouldn't mess around
with them. And that's the philosophy of Edmund Burke, right? It's the kind of idea of we have
all these societal systems. And so smashing the system is a very bad idea, because it might mean that you end up with unforeseen consequences and so on, which, as I say,
can be a very good point in some cases. I don't want to have a massive revolution and cause
terrible pain and suffering and so on, like many revolutions have, rather than incrementally
changing the system in some ways. I talk about this in my book about changing the way we do
science. I think it needs to be changed dramatically, but I don't want to just smash the current system and replace it with something else because we don't know whether
that something else might have bad consequences as well and things which we haven't thought about
yet. But they're just taking that and saying evolution basically supports this. And that's
the central premise of the book. But in many cases, they don't give a good rationale for why
these things are there in the first place. And they don't give a good kind of evolutionary
rationale or even a cultural rationale. And also they don't provide very good evidence for why they're bad
in the modern world. So I talk about, they mentioned fluoridation of the water to stop
dental cavities. They say that's having neurotoxic effects on kids. This would be another thing,
right? We didn't have fluoride in the water when we evolved. Now we have it. It's a bad idea to
have it. There's unforeseen consequences. It causes
neurodevelopmental problems in kids. But if you look at the study they referenced,
it's like a tiny pilot study that they referenced. They don't provide any more
evidence for that in their reference section. So they're throwing these kind of like quite
scary claims out there. And I've looked a little bit into the fluoride literature in the past,
and it's full of a lot of fear mongering stuff, but it's not very good. The research that purportedly shows that water fluoridation lowers the IQ of kids and water
fluoridation when you're pregnant will lower the IQ of the baby that's in your womb and so on.
It's very low quality research and there's no discussion of that. And obviously we can get
into this, but this is the same kind of thinking that they get into when they talk about ivermectin
and they talk about vaccines and so on. It's just very naive scientific thinking where they get into when they talk about ivermectin and they talk about vaccines and so on.
It's just very naive scientific thinking where they say there is a study, therefore it must be true. Just tell me, Stuart, did they mention sunscreen at all in the book? It comes up in
passing very, very briefly. They don't make a big deal of it, but the impression you get is that
sunscreen is bad. Yeah. That's something I've talked about before. Sunscreen isn't necessary.
This is especially triggering for me, an Australian with high skin cancer rates in the world. I mean,
look at my skin for God's sake. But apparently, yeah, sunscreen's a bad idea. Shouldn't put
sunscreen on kids. All they need to do is take five or 10 minutes time out every hour or so,
you know, give your skin a bit of a rest. It'll be good to go again.
It's exactly the same reasoning. We didn't have sunscreen in the environment of evolutionary
adaptiveness. And so therefore we don't need it now. But the thing is you can apply that to
anything and they actually do. And I think this book was written before they got into all the
anti-vaccine stuff. They mentioned vaccines in the book and they don't mention them in an entirely
positive light. Like they say there are bad effects of vaccines and so on. And of course
there are, right? And there have been cases in the past where vaccines have caused side
effects and so on. They do very much pick almost every aspect of the modern world and say,
focus on the negative aspects rather than saying, actually, vaccines are this incredible invention
that has saved untold lives. Genetically modified food is another one they talk about in a little
bit more detail. GMOs are potentially good, but you should avoid them if you can. Sometimes they can be good,
but you shouldn't eat genetically modified food, which is a point that...
It's a wrong point, though.
Absolutely. But it's a point that other gurus that you've talked about have made. Nassim Taleb,
he's a very anti-GMO as well, for this same reason that, oh, there might be a small
chance of a really big risk and they might cause terrible problems and so on. No evidence of that. And of course,
again, untold millions of people have been saved by the green revolution and all that sort of stuff,
which was genetically modified food. And they don't talk about that. It's very much the negatives
and none of the upside. I just want to mention really briefly that like another correspondence
with Basim Taleb is that kind of, because he's a conservative and I think in that Burkean mold of that sort of
belief that organic traditional systems that have grown naturally are just inherently more robust
to whatever black swan events or whatever than these sort of top-down, highly structured,
bureaucratic type systems. So it's not a criticism. I look like you. I don't agree or disagree with
that sort of position. Incrementalism is a good idea. It's good to be careful and conservative,
making changes, et cetera. I'm just pointing out an interesting correspondence there in that it can
serve as a pretty useful philosophical substrate for the naturalistic conservatism.
Yes. Yeah. No, it totally can. And the problem is that it slips into being a naturalistic fallacy.
You have to judge each case on its merits as to whether it's bad or good. And there is actually
a literature on a lot of these things like vaccines and GMOs and so on, which it would
be nice for them to refer to occasionally. But instead, you get cherry picked one or two small
studies saying that these things are bad and no real serious engagement with the scientific
literature. They take the principle of natural things are good
and things that have been around for a long time are good and changing them is bad and just apply
it in every case. There are some cases where that probably is true and some cases where it isn't.
But to have it as a general heuristic seems like a really bad idea.
Like Matt says, it's a kind of common thing that we have found amongst a bunch of gurus. There's
a kind of fetishization of tradition.
And there's a valid point about, as both of you have highlighted, that carrying systems down,
you have to consider what you're going to replace them by and so on. But there is also the point
that what's preserved in systems is often less than ideal. And there's been plenty of things
that have been preserved over long swathes of history, most of human history, which we wouldn't want to preserve, including
like the subjugation of women and so on, right? Like that's evolutionarily natural if you take
the perspective because of sexual dimorphism and that. But in any case, the one thing I have to
ask you, and I promise we'll talk to you about more than this book.
So Brett has his own theories concerning evolution.
They're not entirely well fleshed out anywhere, but Matt and I have listened to him discuss
what he refers to as lineage selection and other places he's discussed explorer modes
and the sense that we've gotten and other people have gotten,
including people like Jerry Coyne, who are better qualified to comment on that, is that
he presupposes a very teleological force behind evolution.
And, you know, he wants to seek out niches.
And whenever a species becomes too stagnant, there's kind of like this force that will
compel it forward. And he referenced
in some of the talks that we listened to with Jordan Peterson, this theory about, you know,
it's the same thing he discussed with Dawkins about the Germans, the Nazis in World War II
being the lineage and the Jews being the lineage, and that this is all best understood as lineage
competition. And my question is, does the book feature
much more exposition
about lineage selection,
lineage competition,
or is that there at all?
No, that's not really
what the book is saying.
They get the evolutionary stuff,
the kind of the theoretical
evolutionary biology stuff
out of the way near the start.
They go through the Omega principle
and all that stuff.
And then the rest of the book is They go through the Omega Principle and all that stuff.
And then the rest of the book is just how that applies to lots of different aspects of your life.
It talks about educating your kids. It talks about sex and gender, where they get a bit of the culture war stuff in there about hormones for kids and all that sort of stuff. They just
briefly mention it without really going into any detail. They make reference to the fact that
they've got theories about stuff. Like for instance, they say, we know that homosexuality is an adaptation and we're going to tease why we think that.
And then they briefly mention it in one sentence and then they kind of move on.
And you go, just a second, you've written a whole book. Can you not just give one paragraph
as to what your theory is or any link to anything? I get this kind of impression,
I get this impression from their podcast as well, that they have this kind of smirking, like, we know best. We've worked all this out. We've got our theories and we're pretty
solid on this stuff. And we know better than most evolution biologists. And again, it's just this
slightly off view of things. You've talked about the way they think about evolution is just not
really a way that people think about biology. And it's just not really a way that people think about
evolution anymore. Maybe people have thought about it that way in the past. I've actually, the only time I've ever
corresponded with Brett, I've never corresponded with Heather, was when he said on a Sam Harris
podcast, I think it was, that, I can't remember the exact quote, so I'm not going to give the
exact quote, but something along the lines of, we're all born entirely with equal capacities
for everything. Like the blank slate view, he gave the blank slate view pretty much exactly.
And I sent a tweet saying, what on earth, isn't this a bit weird and wouldn't no one have picked him up on the fact that he is meant to be a biologist but believes in the blank slate and
he came back and gave a very weird explanation like again slightly askew description of what
heritability means and he didn't really he found it very hard to explain what he meant and i think he generally just has
this off this just view that's just off and that can be very helpful in some cases because you can
look at a field from an entirely different perspective and see lots of you can be like
this genius that shakes up a field because you can see lots of the field's problems and so on
and i think that's how he thinks of himself but unfortunately it really just means that he ends
up just making mistakes.
I also get impression of a Dunning-Kruger type thing. And I know the Dunning-Kruger effect has
been reassessed recently and so on. The standard interpretation of the Dunning-Kruger effect,
which is that your level of confidence is inversely proportional to your level of expertise.
And I know that empirically that might not be exactly who it is, but the classic example,
like the karaoke singer who's belting out, I don't know, that Celine Dion song
from Titanic or something, but it's completely off key and sounds absolutely terrible, but it's
really going for it. And I get that impression when they talk about evolution, that they're
just slightly off key. They're off key, but they're really confident and they think they've
sorted it out. They think they've really fixed this problem. I've seen it recently. You highlighted
this in your podcast about when they talked about statistics as well, like meta-analysis for the ivermectin stuff. They just don't like that.
What he said about a meta-analysis, which is that you don't need to worry about the biases
of individual studies if you do a meta-analysis, is completely wrong. It's totally 100% wrong.
But he said it with such confidence and such vehemence that I can only see the Dunning-Kruger
type thing, that he just doesn't really know. So he's very confident about it because he doesn't have the expertise to know. And I think
that's very dangerous, especially when you get into talking about vaccines and so on and treatments
for COVID. And it makes it all the more frustrating and annoying to listen to them talk or read about
the stuff in the book because they think that they're taking a very rigorous scientific approach
to everything.
It's like a classic sort of pseudoscientific approach.
There's just a tissue of science on what really is just a kind of a preconception
that they're trying to justify all along.
Just to follow up on that, Stuart, whenever they were recently on Joe Rogan,
they were talking about what real science is.
And they were begrudgingly mentioning that, that yes we need numbers and we need to quantify
things but like fundamentally science is getting into the field wrestling a bat and like you know
trying to fiddle with its genitalia to work out what it's up to but there is there's always this
division between people who say you know like if you're doing studies about chimps in the wild
versus chimps in the field that the researchers have different opinions about the capacity.
So there is legitimate debates there.
But the way they take it is there's a kind of spiritual science that you just get from
being like them and going into the jungle.
And it just like seeps into you.
And then, you know, meta-analyses and numbers and all that, it's just a feeling that you get that ivermectin works no matter what the naysayers say yeah yeah i completely agree
with that and that's very much the impression you get because they give lots of anecdotes about
what happened when they went to the jungle and and so on they're in the rainforest doing all
these things and walking across a rickety bridge and all that sort of stuff but i think the
ivermectin thing and also the vaccine thing fits in very well to this world because the ivermectin, they keep emphasizing that ivermectin
has been around for 40 years and it's very safe. And these mRNA vaccines are very novel. And also
the lab leak. We shouldn't be messing around with stuff that is there for a reason. It all fits in
to this worldview. So I think if you kind of step back, they've explained why they get into all these
ridiculous situations and they like this, oh, it's just a feeling, just a vibe type thing, because that's very much their overall
overarching worldview. And they're just applying it in very predictable ways. Unfortunately,
there's been a pandemic and they've applied it and it really should not apply. Entirely new
things like mRNA vaccines have been near miraculous in dealing with the pandemic.
I've got this running hypothesis that it's a bit like the guy who played the president on the West Wing. He comes across as
more presidential, more authoritative, just more convincing as a president than any real
president in the history of presidents. And I feel that they have subconsciously or consciously
dedicated themselves to acting the intrepid scientist.
And I feel like they're good at it. Would you agree?
Yeah, yeah, totally. I think they are very good at it. They're both, and also brother,
Eric Weinstein, are all people who have this enviable capacity to be able to just talk about
stuff for endless long periods, to be able to pontificate. I don't know if there's
a psychological variable. I'm interested in this. There are clearly people in history that have this
capacity, some very good and some very bad people in history, who can just speak extemporaneously
on any topic for very, very long periods and sound very convincing and sound very authoritative.
And I mean, there are historical figures that clearly do this a lot. The first one that comes
to mind is Hitler.
I don't want to bring up, like, associate them with Hitler.
Your people and my people are famously described as having the gift of the gap. But maybe the difference is, can speak for long, but not that eloquently.
Yeah, but I think there are individuals who clearly just have this prolix thing where they can just go for long, long, long periods.
And you hear all these stories of them talking.
And that's what I get on these podcasts, these long form podcasts that they keep talking about, where it's really just one person talking for hours and hours and hours about any given topic.
If you listen to the podcasts on the mRNA vaccine or the ivermectin and all that, if you didn't know about meta-analysis, for instance, you would find that very convincing.
Like the things he says sound like scientific propositions and sound like scientific
arguments. The things that Heather Hying says about ivermectin, it works for Zika, it works for
dengue, it works for all these other diseases. And then that sounds very convincing. And she
can give this huge long list of all of the reasons that ivermectin is good. And then you just Google
it and you find these are studies in like petri dishes they're not randomized controlled trials they're the whole thing just crumbles to
dust as soon as you pick it up but it sounds so convincing and you're absolutely right it's like
playing the part of the sage scientist i don't mean sages in the british s-a-g-e group the lowercase
sage and who's telling you all this hard one evolutionary wisdom and that is why i think the
book will do very well because it's self-help for people who are a bit pissed off in the modern
world a bit maybe feeling a little bit unsatisfied in their life and they want to find out a good
evolutionary scientific reason for it and it's not their fault it's that the modern world has
brought all these things into their life that they're unsuited to. And yeah, I think it's going to be a bestseller.
So depressing, Chris.
Yeah, you know, far be it from us, Matt, to disparage long-form podcasts about niche topics
where people speak endlessly on their pet interests.
But with the hypocrisy aside, I've recently been taking a couple of courses on Coursera
related to
topics that I'm interested in, auditing them as I drive back and forth.
And they're really good, right?
They're university level courses taught by Paul Bloom or taught by some famous politics
professor.
And I can't help thinking, you know, I also consume a lot of the long form podcast type
stuff that you're talking about for interest and for
masochistic tendencies.
And it's night and day, right?
Because the difference on those courses are, you know, they're at university level standard.
So they're very clear about the standards of evidence and they're not constantly proposing
revolutionary theories for things or their own bespoke version.
They're relaying research literature.
And the thing that struck me is those courses, anyone can
go on and audit them for free.
You can pay if you want the certificate, but you can go on and audit them.
And lots of people do, especially in the developing world.
They're a good thing, I think, these MOOCs.
But that's not the thing that gets someone super famous.
It's not what pushes someone into the Jordan Peterson level of fame or the
Brett and Heller level of fame.
And that requires contrarian takes, that requires hot takes and
to wed things to politics.
It's also where things go wrong.
So you were talking about that and your review highlights the need
to give hot takes on issues. And this probably relates to your broader criticisms, you know,
about COVID skepticism and so on, that there seems to be a gravitational pull that brings people
towards, especially now with the pandemic, COVID.
And you've done a lot of work with this.
I wonder if you have some thoughts about, is it the modern ecosystem?
Is it just because we haven't had a global pandemic in a long time? Why is it that so many people are drawn to COVID,
not just ivermectin, lockdown skepticism, the whole shebang?
Yeah, the funny thing is we've already seen multiple cycles
of the same thing happening we had hydroxychloroquine last year lest we forget we had a whole
cycle of people becoming obsessed with that as the next big thing and it hasn't actually fully
gone away there are still people who are pro hydroxychloroquine i saw someone on gb news which
is this new right-leaning news channel in the UK, advocating hydroxychloroquine
just a few days ago. And I thought, my goodness, these things really are very sticky.
I think in these kind of spheres, in these kind of media spheres, there's no really checking or
saying we have now completely discarded that hypothesis. As long as it's anti-consensus or
what seemed to be the mainstream at any given time, it gives people that little frisson of
excitement when they stand up and say, I'm pro this drug that you think doesn't work, or I'm anti the
thing that you think does work, like lockdown or vaccine passport. I think a lot of it is just
memes, right? I think a lot of it is just people sense what the position mainstream is. They sense
that being outside the mainstream is to believe in ivermectin or hydroxychloroquine or whatever
it is. And then they just repeat things that are associated with that meme. They just sometimes mindlessly repeat things or have this very strong
confirmation bias to seek out evidence that's to do with that particular thing. And then they just
repeat it. And then unfortunately, the people on the other side start repeating memes pro lockdown
or whatever it is, memes, and they can go off too far along the other direction. But the issue is
you have an audience who is pushing you along on all of But the issue is you have an audience who is pushing you
along on all of these things. So you have an audience who praises you very strongly when you
talk about ivermectin, and then you want to push a little bit further in that direction because
you're getting lots of approval for it. You're getting lots of kudos from your audience. And so
you see, maybe you say something about vaccines as well, and you get lots of kudos as well from
an audience. You maybe gather more people in who are constantly praising you and so on. And I think this is probably the situation
Brett and Heather get themselves into, that they had this audience that's praising them constantly
for saying all these contrarian things. And who would want to disappoint them? Who would want to
say, actually, I wasn't right about that. Actually, I was wrong. All those genius level takes that I
thought I had were actually wrong. We've been misleading you for months and months. They've
gone too far. They've been pulled really far away from where the actual reality is. And they could
never go back now. They could never stand up and say, with all that stuff we said about ivermectin
was completely wrong. Even as in the particular case of ivermectin, it's not just that the studies
are low quality. Back in March, I was writing about how the ivermectin studies were low quality. There was a big review done by, I think, a Canadian health authority who said,
all the evidence we've got for hydroxychloroquine is bad quality trials,
either small trials or they didn't do the blinding or the randomization correctly or whatever. It's
all the issues that you might have with studies. But it's even worse now because many of those
studies have turned out not just to be low quality, but to be fraudulent. Like several big ivermectin studies are now fraudulent. If anything would change your view
on a scientific hypothesis, it would be that the evidence base that you thought you were relying on
and have referenced many times and told your thousands of followers about was fraudulent,
was fake. Someone made it up. And yet there's nothing that you can say that will change their
mind. Unfortunately, they say making that change, removing that study from our meta-analysis
does not change the results.
And they always say that.
Inevitably, that always comes out.
This massive statistical criticism you've made of our study, we've reanalyzed it.
It doesn't change the results.
It's amazing how often that happens.
I really think it's this gravitational pull of being praised by people that really drags
people off
into different directions. And I think all you can do is bear in mind that you need to try and
piss your audience off as much as you can and say things which will enrage people on either side of
any given debate, if they're true, and not just stick to one side. I think that's one of the big
problems. We've talked about this a fair bit, which is that phenomenon of audience capture.
there's a fair bit, which is that phenomenon of audience capture. We're all vulnerable to social approval and attention. But I think if one has a pretty high opinion of oneself
and maybe has narcissistic tendencies, then I think that vulnerability is magnified a hundredfold.
And arguably these new characteristics of social media and stuff, it's like the crack cocaine of
attention and popular regard.
One of the things that's really astounded me, because I've studied vaccine hesitancy for
probably 10 years now, and before COVID, it didn't really have a strong political alignment.
That's one thing that's different. It was kind of pretty much diffuse. And to the extent that it
was associated with particular subcultures. It was actually in places like Australia and
some places like on the coastal United States, more associated with kind of higher socioeconomic,
liberal, people who are interested in maximizing their health and wellness and things like that,
big associations with complementary and alternative medicine, which obviously has
these roots in counterculture, 16s kind of crystals and so on. The sort of sociology of it was very different from what we see now.
And it's interesting because it parallels the shift in the sort of tectonic shifts in
political things where right-wing parties have become much more populist.
And the sort of weird Silicon Valley, nootropics, self-optimization thing has weirdly shifted.
I think we understand why vaccines are inherently unappealing.
It's just not an appealing concept to have an injection of anything.
And it's not an appealing concept to have an injection of the toned down version of
a virus.
Whereas things like complementary medicine, pretty much like our gurus, services and treatments that are just not designed to work, but
designed to be appealing.
That's sort of how I square that circle.
They think it works.
Well, just, yeah.
One thing that that makes me think about, and Stuart, I think you will have seen
these connections as well, is I know that you were, I think at least I might be maligning you
here, but were active in skeptics circles, you know, back 10 years ago or that kind of thing,
when it used to be mainly about alternative medicine and creationism. And I know your
website, for example, is reminiscent of Talk Origins, the creationist checklist. But one thing that I've noticed, and it would be good
to get your thoughts on, is that the old skeptic movement and those kinds of topics, like Matt said,
they touched on politics. There was right-wing politicians pushing global warming or evolution
denial or whatever. It was really secondary. And the bigger thing was the pseudoscience and the
alternative medicine sphere and that kind of thing. And it struck thing was the pseudoscience and the alternative medicines for you and that
kind of thing. And it struck me as well that you still listen to Skeptic's Guide to the Universe,
I still do, still like it. But it feels like because they've ruled out addressing politics,
they don't want the focus on politics and traditional skepticism tries to avoid that arena,
that they're now missing out on a huge part where pseudoscience and alternative theories and so on
come to. So I'm just curious, especially with your website, because that's bound to have been
taken politically by some people, what your thoughts are about that shift? Yeah, so the
website is covidfaq.co. As you say, it's reminiscent of the old websites where people used to put up
creationist claims and like, you know, there are no transitional fossils or whatever.
And then you would get various references that said, well, actually, there are.
Archaeopteryx is a transitional fossil and whatever.
And you would find ways to respond to the common tropes, the common arguments that you
hear in these kind of debates.
And there's also one for global warming skeptics called Skeptical Science, I think, where it
responds to things like the world stopped heating up in 1998,
all the kinds of classic arguments that you hear in global warming skeptic circles.
And so we made this for that reason, but someone made a very insightful point just the other day on Twitter. And I think it was either an anonymous account or someone who I didn't follow. And I
can't recall who it was. So apologies. It might be you, Matt. Could have been you.
Probably me. Nobody knows who you are. Let's just assume you, Matt. Could we know you're an analyst? Yeah, it's thoughtful, thoughtful. Probably me. Nobody knows who you are.
Let's just assume it was Matt.
Yeah.
That said this.
And I'll just paraphrase.
What they said was,
and it kind of made me sit back and go,
God, that is so good.
That's so right.
The skeptics movement was very much
training you to refute arguments
that you'd already decided were wrong
or position that you already decided were wrong.
So like there were categories of people who we all agree are wrong. And then the skeptics movement
went out and criticized them. So creationists, those people are wrong. Evolution is true.
We know that. And so what you get in the skeptics movement is, and you're learning about fossils
and learning about DNA and whatever else would help you, you know, defeat a creationist and
geological stuff and whatever, anything that would help you defeat a creationist in an argument homeopaths are wrong let's learn about molecular concentrations
of stuff and and so on and learn about that and so when it comes to something new like a political
realignment or new kinds of arguments which there isn't really a clear side of and this is where the
culture war stuff can come in the skeptics movement don't really know what to do and they faulted it
and you're seeing when the skeptics movement became less of a thing was when it split up along political lines. It
split up along the kind of social justice side and the intellectual dark web side, I guess you
could kind of say is the way that the two sides of the skeptics movement sort of went. Because
there wasn't really a clear skeptics position on that stuff. And as you say, I think that's a really
important weakness in it is that they didn't have a kind of adaptability to new kinds of arguments. They just had a set of people that
they disagreed with and then they disagreed in detail with them. And I think that's a useful
service. And it's a very useful sort of almost like a consumer protection type service where
you're saying, by the way, this stuff doesn't work. This snake oil thing doesn't work. And
here's why. And I think that's really important. But when it comes to new issues, like some of the ones that we've seen in in covid it just falls to bits it's really really
difficult compared to what we all thought we were doing in the skeptics movement which was
rigorously applying critical thought to every single thing like that's not really what we were
doing it fits with my kind of general feeling these days that everything's just memes and people
repeating things that are socially acceptable like socially acceptable in the skeptics movement to say that homeopathy is bad.
And so you just learn all the things that, you know, and you make homeopathy jokes and whatever else.
And it's just socially, that's just the social thing to do.
And I think you saw this at the very start of the pandemic.
And this is where I was thinking of ways that we could argue about things and things we could disagree on. And I think possibly in recent podcasts, you've been a bit too kind to the kind of medical establishment
or authorities or whatever. At the start of the pandemic, you had our friend Eric Weinstein coming
on and saying, remember at the start of the pandemic when they said masks didn't work and
now they say they do. And you were saying, well, you know, they made a mistake. Now they've got
it right. And that's what matters. But if you think back to February 2020, it was quite an extreme thing to see all of the world's health
authorities, all of certainly in the UK and the US, I'm not quite sure where you are, what the
authorities were saying, but you had all the authorities saying in very, very vehement,
very certain terms that masks didn't work. It spread around again. It became a meme. It became
a culturally acceptable thing to say that masks didn't work. And you had all these people going
in extreme, like in capital letters on their Twitter, masks don't work. My favorite tweet
was an epidemiologist who was at a conference saying, I'm at a conference with 60 epidemiologists.
We're all in a small room. Not one of them was wearing a mask. They were saying that in a very much,
I like, you should be like us type thing.
And then it looked very embarrassing
because you can read that person's tweet
a few months later
when they had changed their profile picture
to them wearing a mask and so on.
And you had this kind of weird thing
that only Twitter can do,
which is you see the old tweet
with the person's picture
and also their display name
has like the mask emoji in it five
times and all that and it really i i found that quite a scary cultural thing because we were told
by all the authority figures and not in uncertain terms not in we think that masks probably don't
work but you saw a campaign i don't think necessarily coordinated but because i think
these things just spread through kind of cultural osmosis. But you saw a campaign of people saying that masks didn't work and in fact, make it worse, make COVID worse. If you wear a mask,
you were more likely to get COVID actually. Now they certainly didn't have any evidence for that.
And if you push back against them, they would not have been able to provide you with any evidence.
And yet it was extremely certain the evidence you got. And I think that feeds the conspiracy theorists
because later down the line, when they had to make their U-turn, the conspiracy theorists,
like the kind of COVID skeptic, lockdown skeptics type people could come out and say,
you were very certain in the past. What makes you so certain now? My remedy to that is that you say
things with uncertainty all the time. I don't have an answer to how scientists, when they're
talking to the general public and talking to policymakers, should express uncertainty. I think there's
probably people doing research into this and there must be good ways of thinking about how we
adjust our message. But clearly the way not to do it is to say things in capital letters as if they
are 100% certain, like they did with masks. I also think at the same time as that, certainly again in the UK,
I don't know what the case was with other places. And of course, Australia, for one thing,
locked down very fast, or at least closed down its borders and so on. But in the UK,
at the same time as that, our scientists were planning a herd immunity strategy, right? We're
planning that 60% of the population would catch COVID, that we needed to just let it burn through.
There was no point in trying to stop it from getting into the country, no point in trying to stop it spreading, just let it go. And only when
presented with the really catastrophic estimates of how many people would die when this was
calculated, did they kind of again do a screeching U-turn and change the way. So I actually think
that a lot of the narratives about expert failure and failure of the authorities and so on are
actually quite well borne out by the COVID pandemic. And certainly the start of the COVID pandemic, when things really,
these people really screwed up very badly, at least in the UK and in certain other places.
And we're slow to do things like lockdown and slow to do things like attempting to actually
stop the virus from getting into the country using things like quarantine and so on.
When I hear Eric Weinstein saying that stuff, I don't want to agree with him because he's Eric Weinstein. But I do think that he does have a
point. And I don't think you should dismiss the fact that our faith in the experts should have
been shaken quite a lot. And that's before you get into what people in my field, psychology,
were saying at the start of the pandemic as well, which I've written about extensively as well,
which was deeply embarrassing. But I don't know, that would be my sort of response to some of the
stuff you've been saying recently on the podcast,
which is just, I think the experts need a bit more criticism. And just because Eric Weinstein
is saying it doesn't mean it's necessarily a hundred percent wrong, even if it might be quite
annoying. Well, so I have a couple of thoughts about it. This will be good. So the first thing
is points that I think are completely spot on and that we don't really have a disagreement on is that there's a need for humility and an ability to express uncertainty.
I completely agree with that.
Also completely agree that various institutional, including public health bodies, including governments, fucked up massively, repeatedly, sent out conflicting messages in the early months.
They didn't take measures that they were advised to and so on.
I'm not saying you, Stuart, in general, but I think that's a misinterpretation of the
argument that me and Matt want to make about authorities and experts and that.
It isn't to say that the institutions did it all well and that the authorities' messaging was not confusing or counterproductive.
That I'm on board with.
Where I probably disagree a bit is that what I want to say in regards to the masking policy is that, first of all, as you mentioned, in the US and the UK, people like Fauci, he famously
had these clips played of him saying, you don't need to wear masks if you're out in
the public and so on.
And he's quite unequivocal about it.
At the same time as that was happening in Japan, the medical public health authorities
were saying people should wear masks because there was a cultural difference there.
The evidence base isn't different, but there is a difference in terms of how normal that
is and how compliant people will be in that.
And even now, in Japan now, I see all around me that people in the UK and US are having
these endless debates about wearing masks after they said that we can get vaccinated
and should we be doing that?
And like in Japan, there's none of that debate.
People just wear masks.
They wear masks in their car.
Nobody wants to do it, but there's just not this culture of that this is infringing on
your freedom because that's what people were doing anyway.
When they got a cold, they wore a mask.
What I wanted to argue and why I think Eric and stuff get it wrong is that I looked into the literature
back when this debate was happening and was quite surprised to see that the evidence for
cloth masks in community settings was pretty crap.
There's some positive study.
There's some studies that were not great quality.
Guy would characterize it as not massively different than the ivermectin literature.
And the guy who runs Slate Star Codex did a blog on it.
And he made that point.
He reviewed it and he does these things quite well.
And he said, look, the literature isn't good.
But his second point was, but given the situation, given what we know about the virus, people
should be recommending this, even if the clinical evidence isn't strong. We don't
always have the clinical evidence, and there's good reason physical barriers are obviously going
to be helpful. I agreed with that conclusion. But my point with the people who were focusing on the
public health bodies in the government is that they basically regarded it as because they think that masks work and that they should work, that the clinical evidence necessarily was there to show that.
And that wasn't there.
So because it wasn't there, I could see why a public health body would decide we're worried about shortages, but we're also worried about compliance.
And we have these people who've done reviews of the literature and said it's not very strong
about cloth masks.
We don't know.
So on the cost benefit analysis, I think that good fiat people could reach the conclusion
we don't need to promote that.
And that's the part that I want to argue is that you can say that they were wrong and
that in some circumstances
they may have hid their motivations.
They may have not admitted
that it was because they wanted
to preserve the N95 masks.
But it is also possible
for public health bodies
to reach different conclusions
when there's a messy literature
and they do different cost-benefit analysis.
And it doesn't mean
that it was all a conspiracy
to lie to the public. It's more like
data can be messy and you can reach different conclusions.
Sure. I'm not arguing that necessarily that it was a conspiracy to lie to the public. I don't
think it was a conspiracy to lie to the public. And I actually don't think it was a noble lie
either. I don't think they said, we want to preserve mask stocks and therefore we will just
tell people that they don't work. I think they thought they didn't work. I think a few people decided that was the case. And then just by pure memes, it spread
out to everyone else and everyone else. It became the thing to say that masks don't work. Oh, the
stupid people wearing masks. And you would do a tweet about, I saw someone wearing a mask today.
Haha, what an idiot. And there were plenty of them. There was no shortage of that kind of thing.
Articles in the news saying, if you're wearing a mask, there might be something a bit wrong with your brain. You're falling prey to
some sort of psychological bias or whatever. I think they genuinely believed that masks didn't
work. I think that was just wrong. If the conclusion is there's a terrible conspiracy
and our health authorities are treating us like idiots and so on, and they're all having a laugh
at our expense. No, I don't think that's the case. I think it's the cock-up theory, which is,
I think they just genuinely thought, the UK health authorities at
least thought at the start of the pandemic, that there was no way that we could stop the virus from
spreading around the population, so just let it go. And they thought masks don't work. They thought
there's not going to be a vaccine for a very long period of time. They just had all these wrong
assumptions. And you can see from their behavior that they operated on those assumptions. That's
what they did. They went out and said, don't wear a mask.
They went out and said, we're going to have to have herd immunity through infection in
the population.
They were on the news saying that.
I don't think that was some kind of special galaxy brain type liar.
I think it was just that is what they thought was true.
And that was wrong.
It would be nice if people just came out and said that they were wrong.
But the problem is that now we've had all these kind of denials and people trying to
obfuscate it and then people coming along and saying, well, actually, they were just trying to
preserve mask supplies and trying to impose a very organized way of thinking onto what was
essentially a chaotic period where people were just wrong about all sorts of stuff.
But they were saying, well, the reason that they did all that ridiculous stuff was because they
actually thought X, Y, Z thing. i think they were just chaotically wrong and
some people got right i think scott alexander as you say it's the circle that's got it generally
right which was there's pretty crap evidence right now i early 2020 but theoretically speaking it
seems like given that something could spread through droplets that come out your mouth or nose
having something physical in the way would probably be helpful even if that's quite hard to demonstrate
in a randomized controlled trial and so on so it's's probably best to wear a mask. It's a fairly low, it's not going to cause you a huge
amount of hassle in your daily life in most circumstances, probably. So you might as well
just wear one, which is my reasoning right from the start. So I don't think it's a conspiracy.
I think it's a cock up. But the fact that our authorities can cock up and yet seem so vehement
and so certain about stuff is scary enough. Look, you make a lot of good points there, Stuart.
This is one prediction.
It's like going back in hindsight and saying, I was right about this.
At the time, I remember I couldn't understand why they weren't restricting international
travel really quickly.
And I do believe that it was just a failure of imagination.
Like it just was not something that not only experts or authorities, but everybody, everybody
in the community, just the thought of just shutting down international travel at that time was unimaginable.
And like a month later, it was, well, of course we have to do that. And in a fast moving emergency
and in a crisis, there's going to be heaps of things like that. I think a good parallel is
actually the conduct of something like World War II, which is like most wars, just one cock up
after the next kind of a rolling disaster.
And you stumble through and figure it out. If you're lucky, you have a good outcome.
Dealing with a pandemic without a generations of experience in actually dealing with one is
going to include all of those things. But I take all your points about the communication
of uncertainty and the unwillingness to accept any kind of errors and so on. I think those are
significant things,
but I'm also aware that there's a difference between a scientific consensus and managing
uncertainty within a scientific community. That's one thing. And then when you actually have to
transfer that to public messaging on policy, it's an entirely different thing. And I'll give you an
example of this. In Australia, for instance, we had two vaccines available. We've got Pfizer and we've got AstraZeneca. We're very slow to get
them. Not enough Pfizer, pretty good supply of AstraZeneca. There was some evidence of very
small risk to people with AstraZeneca. Long story short, there was confused messaging about the
vaccines, just a little bit. One politician was saying this, one politician said something else.
One expert or someone within authority in the health department of us said this, and there were mixed messages.
It was just really quite small, but that threw half the population into confusion and suddenly
nobody wanted to get the AstraZeneca at all. And it was a mass, it still is a big problem.
That's just an example of how any kind of deviation from simple, clear, unified public messaging can have really bad
consequences. I guess what I'm trying to say is that I understand the impulse when it comes to
sticking to the line and falling into line when it comes to the public messaging. And you're not
saying it was a conspiracy or anything like that. And I guess I'm just pushing back on the idea that it betrays some sort of unmitigated disaster or some sort of deep fundamental problem with how things
are done. I see that kind of thing as a somewhat natural consequence of circumstances and the
demands that are on public messaging. Yeah, I totally understand why people do it. And the
folly into line thing, I can completely see because there are lots of tried and true health messages that we know are correct. And one of
them is take your vaccine for, I don't know, something like the MMR vaccine or other vaccines
that we've been using for a long, long time. Public health type people have been repeating
those messages quite rightly for a long, long time. So you can see why the impulse is there.
But I think what we've learned in this case is that when a new situation like this arises, we need to be a little bit less
willing to fall into line and just repeat the same thing again and again. Here's another example
is handwashing. In the UK, again, I'm not super familiar with what other places have done,
although actually, Chris, you're in Japan, and I think Japan have had this right for vastly longer
than the UK have. We emphasize handwashing very strongly right at the start of the pandemic. And
fair enough, back then, people thought that it might spread through touching things,
through full-mouth transmission. And so washing your hands for 20 seconds. And you had Boris
Johnson saying that you should sing happy birthday twice while you're washing your hands,
so it passes 20 seconds and so on. And that was a major thing. And obviously,
you walk through any train station and there's hand sanitizer. And I've done a bit of volunteering
at the vaccine center and they're sanitizing everyone's hands. And there's people wiping down the pens that you use to write
your name down because in case there might be some COVID on the pens and the clipboards,
you've got to wipe that. They've got volunteers coming in for the whole day, wiping down things
with antiviral disinfectant. And of course, it doesn't work really to help to stop COVID. It
might be a very tiny minority of cases that are spread through fomite transmission, but it's
almost impossible to find cases that are definitively spread through touching things.
It spreads through the air. It's an airborne virus spread through droplets and airborne
particles. And yet the public messaging was, I think we understood this in probably April,
March, April, 2020. We understood that it's in the air and we need to emphasize things like
ventilation and so on. Japan had its three C's thing about avoiding crowded and closed and close contact settings.
And those were all a recognition of the fact that the virus spreads in the air.
Although it did also talk about handwashing, but that was in there as a sort of a minor thing.
But in the UK, the handwashing thing was the first thing on our government's COVID slogan until July 2021.
We had hands, face, space as the things you should bear in mind for COVID.
They knew for a year or more that handwashing was, maybe they didn't know that it was essentially ineffective, but they must have known that it was down the list.
You looked on the NHS website and handwashing, there were like a list of 10 points on how to avoid COVID.
Handwashing was about six of them was related to handwashing. There were like a list of 10 points on how to avoid COVID. Handwashing was about six
of them was related to handwashing. Put your tissue in the bin after you've used it. Wash
your hands when you come home from work. Wash your hands, wash your hands. That's another example of
how the falling into line thing, whether you're doing the government's messaging or whether you're
working for the NHS or whether you're working for some kind of university here. I'm in my office at
university and we have a hand sanitizer station outside and so on. That was all completely a waste of
everybody's time. We should have been much more able to jump off that particular train
when the evidence showed that it was not effective. And so there's this inertia,
as well as the kind of tendency to fall in line. There's this inertia about changing things when
we've got into the way of doing things. And I'm glad that we managed to change the message on
masks. Although I think there was, again, needless inertia there because we'd been
so certain about how masks definitely didn't work. We were slow to adapt there, but we were
grossly slower to adapt away from the hand-washing message because we'd spent so much time emphasizing
it in the first place. Again, it's a bit like the gurus kind of digging themselves into a position
and getting more and more stuck there and kind of an inability to say that you're wrong and try and draw an
analogy to another part of the podcast that we previously discussed. I think the government got
themselves into, in the UK anyway, they got themselves stuck in a rut of this almost like
OCD hand-washing constantly all the time for no actual reason. Then comes the post-hoc rationale
of, well,
even if it's not good for COVID, it's good for other stuff. So we should include it in the
messaging anyway. Handwashing is helpful for norovirus. But then if you're going to put
general health messages in the COVID messaging, you might as well tell people to eat less fatty
foods and go for a run as well. Surely the COVID messaging should be about COVID.
What you just said that people should be telling is the thing which a lot of the heterodox people are saying, you're not allowed
to mention. You're not allowed to mention that people should be outdoors and be healthy and
lose weight and stuff. But on the hand washing thing specifically, because I read your article
about that at the time and in good, robust IDW sphere thing, I had some pushback for you on that.
My pushback is not to the argument that you make
that this is not a massive priority message.
That it was okay at the start that people were concerned
that they need to wash hands.
So it's understandable that they might emphasize that.
It's a relatively simple thing to implement
for places to put alcohol wipes and whatnot.
And like you said, it kills a lot of germs with one spray.
Even if it didn't work, it would be a useful thing for people to be disinfecting their
hands.
But if the issue is that you've got a limited amount of bandwidth and you want to give two
or three messages that are most important for COVID. I was convinced by your argument that what
the government should be focusing on are things like ventilation, where they haven't emphasized
that and so on. But the part that I couldn't, I don't know if I agree with you or don't,
but I'm just trying to work it out in my head. But it's that in Japan, for example,
which as a country had an emphasis on ventilation from fairly early stage,
it's still universal all over everywhere in Japan that there's hand sanitizers.
In literally every single shop.
And Japan also didn't do things like shut down pachinko parlors or nightclubs
where they were having breakouts.
So even though they had the messaging, it's kind of, you know, an illustration that the policies can be schizophrenic. But I wonder when, if you want
the public to engage in hygienic practices, and if your public health messaging says three or four
things, and one of them is avoid fries, wash your hands. This is a point that I keep coming back to
that if you followed, say, the WHO or CDC,
which are like two institutions that get regularly heavily criticized because of their mask stance
at the beginning. But while they were disparaging masks, or at least they were saying we currently
do not recommend wearing masks, they were also saying we do recommend social distancing, not
going out in crowds, not meeting other people. People act
as if if you followed their advice, you would be putting yourself in huge risk. But if you're not
meeting people outside or inside, and you're definitely not meeting people inside, you don't
need to worry so much. I wear a mask still inside and outside in Japan. It's the norm. But I'm just
speaking that in terms of messaging, surely the biggest one is the social distancing, avoiding
other people, avoiding crowds, yelling indoor with lots of people stuff. Sure. If you could follow
that, then that would be the most important thing. But I think there are a lot of people who, first
of all, there are a lot of people who can't follow that. A lot of people who never worked from home during the pandemic because their job was, they couldn't do that. And so I think messaging to people that if you've washed your hands, then you've done one of the big things against COVID. And when in fact you've done nothing, could even be damaging in that case, because I don't want to get into galaxy brain things about, it lulls people into a false sense of security and then they stop doing they could like it could but i don't know
there's not actually that much evidence you know there were all these arguments about seat belts
and things like people will drive more dangerously all these kind of things and i don't know if you
want to get into kind of like second order effects because the first order effect is like i think i
think these health messages should just have the first order effect do these things work to stop
you from getting covid and if it doesn't then it shouldn't be in the health messaging.
And I think getting into all these complicated arguments about,
I'll make people safer from other things,
is, as they say online, it's a cope.
You're coping with having made a mistake in the past.
And I think that's how people justify the hand-washing stuff now.
I agree that I don't think these organizations
were deliberately trying to mislead people,
nor do I think that if you followed the general advice,
stay at home and avoid people and keep away from crowds and stuff, if you followed that,
then you would be a hell of a lot safer. It's just that these are messages that are being put out to
a lot of people. And there's a lot of interactions happening every single day with those messages in
mind. And if you have the messaging right, then you could save some number of people's lives.
And if you've got the messaging wrong, then you're at best missing an opportunity and at worst misleading people and causing them,
lulling them into a false sense of security and so on. And that's before you get into the wasted
money, the wasted resources, the wasted time in setting up hand sanitizer stations everywhere and
pouring loads of government money into debt holes, coffers, and all that sort of stuff to set up hand
sanitizing stations absolutely everywhere,
wiping down the carriages in the New York subway and the London tube and so on with antiviral,
doing this deep clean and all that sort of stuff. It became a completely absurd industry of this kind of thing when there were probably smarter things you could have thought about to do at
the same time. Running more trains, getting people to think about when they use the train at less peak times, just keeping windows open. I think still people aren't
really that aware of the ventilation aspect. There's some polling in the UK recently that
showed that a lot of people think that hand washing is the thing that really matters and that
ventilation stuff is kind of secondary or even worse. So I think we've made a really big mistake
there by not changing the messaging. And
I'll grant, put in hand washing if you want, but it shouldn't be the first thing that people see.
And I think we really screwed our response up very badly by having it as the lead thing when
we must have known that was not the priority. Well, I mean, it sounds like essentially what
you're arguing is that public policy and messaging should follow the evidence base
as best as we know it
at any given time. There's obviously zero disagreement from me and Chris on that.
And if you're making the point also, of course, that public health authorities make mistakes.
Yeah, public advice isn't always 100% correct. Again, that's clearly true. I think one way to
resolve this is that public health messaging, it's best if it's nimble. It's best if it can change. When people like yourself are advising the public health authorities, they say, look,
we've been saying this about hand-washing. Let's not talk about that. Let's talk about this thing
instead. And maybe the whole thing could be helped by maybe upfront saying, look, our best advice at
the moment is this. Then, you know, just flagging up to people that it's subject to change. It's
not sort of drawn in stone with a chisel.
And I think people understand that.
I'm stuck on this World War II analogy, but I'm imagining London is in the Blitz or something and people are getting told, oh, you know, you should do this to whatever.
And then suddenly the next week, it's no, no, we actually should just.
I think people get it.
They can handle that.
And I think that's a good idea.
So I think where Chris and I were coming from is really when the authorities do double down
and pretend that, oh, no, we're never wrong about everything. We're perfectly infallible, like the
Pope or whatever. Then that presents this sort of chink that the gurus like to use their crowbars
to open up into. It's entirely corrupt. You can't trust them about anything. That's where Chris and
I are coming from too, which is that we're not so much defending authorities, the experts or whatever
more than anyone. We know how they get them wrong, especially Chris and I in our respective disciplines.
But what we're pushing back on is literally these anti-establishment,
anti-authoritarian guru types who are just weaponizing those issues and not keeping them
in proportion. I think problems are understandable. Mistakes are understandable. Infallibility isn't
to be expected. And we're
pushing back on people making out that it's entirely corrupt because of these mistakes.
Stuart, before you respond, I think I can make an analogous situation for you,
which has also caused me personally hassle with the lab leak. You've heard of this. So like when
I read the papers and saw the statements by various researchers, I didn't get the impression that absolutely nobody was allowed to discuss lab leaks. It was completelyredited, but it's often regarded as unfairly negative towards the possibility of a lab leak.
But when I read those papers, I didn't get that impression.
If you've read scientific papers, you know when people are saying this is where the evidence strongly leans towards.
We generally believe because of this reasons that that's the conclusion.
towards, we generally believe because of this reasons that that's the conclusion.
But they all included caveats that said, well, we cannot rule out and we'll need to do further investigation X, Y, and Z.
And just recently, two review papers have come out.
It's been a year or so since those papers have come out.
They did more review with lots more evidence.
And again, they're reaching very similar conclusions,
even reviewing the additional information that's come available,
loads of things to do with the Wuhan lab
and also the genetics of the virus and whatnot.
But they reached the same conclusion,
which is that it's likely to be zoonotic,
but we can't rule it out.
And there are people in the field who take different opinions, right?
Richard E. Bright, for example, or Jesse Bloom. But that's normal. we can't rule it out. And there are people in the field who take different opinions, right? Richard
E. Bright, for example, or Jesse Bloom. But that's normal. There's always disagreement in a field on
any issue where the evidence is still new. You're going to have these debates and things, but I feel
like people react. I'm not saying that nobody, none of the researchers have made statements which
are too strong, that they've said there's no possibility, it couldn't possibly be that, and it's all just
conspiracy theories. They do, because scientists occasionally make those kinds of statements.
But in the research literature, the learning, to me, hasn't really changed. Even that letter
that people wrote saying we need to investigate it, it said that this is the position of the WHO and the EU, which are not these heterodox bodies. So I guess my overall point
and the tie to the COVID is just, I feel that there is a level of tolerance for uncertainty
and debate, which is in scientific fields. And you've covered lots of stuff with fraud and things
where that breakdown and false consensus.
But I think that the way that's reflected through journalism and stuff,
there was a couple of months there where it was almost common knowledge
that now the lab leak was proven to be most likely.
And nothing had changed, right?
Just media narratives.
I take issue with that.
And then I get people who talk as if
i'd completely ruled out that lab leak could ever happen and if the evidence came out that was
stronger i'm just blowing in the wind with the general consensus yeah i agree that the scientific
papers do have statements of uncertainty in them and then when that gets out into the media and
certainly when it gets out into the guru sphere of people like the
Weinsteins and so on. You've made me become obsessed with them now. You've reviewed their
books. I review lots of books. I'm not going after them in any sense. But anyway, having said that,
it's not just journalists and it's not just people like provocateurs on the internet who are
responsible for bad scientific arguments getting out there and poor science being propped up. It's the
scientists themselves in many cases. And I think that Lancet letter, even if it has been criticized
overly by some people who are conspiracy theorists and so on, that Lancet letter was weird. It ended
with a sort of almost Soviet style thing about about we stand with our colleagues with an exclamation mark on the front line, stand with our Chinese Wuhan colleagues. I've never seen anything like that in a scientific paper before. I don't think I've ever seen an exclamation mark in a scientific paper before. It was just very odd. And maybe you could say there's a pandemic and people were very emotional at the time and so on. But I think there was something. I don't think all of the blame for that letter being blown up into a big controversy
can be laid at the feet of other people like journalists and internet gurus and so on.
I think that the scientists were really trying to give you the impression.
It's quite mad to believe in the lab leak hypothesis.
And that's regardless of my view on the lab leak hypothesis,
which is, I think, similar to yours, which is,
I'll just go with what the general consensus is, because I have no expertise in this matter
whatsoever. And I think general consensus at the moment is we just don't really know. And it seems
most likely that it's zoonotic, but there's some possibility that it might be a lab leak.
Or there's some sort of middle scenario where the reason that it came from bats was because
they were doing research on bats in the lab. And there's some sort of middle way. And then I don't
understand how you would tell the difference between that and a fully natural zoonotic
explanation. You know much more about this than I do, but I don't have any position on that really.
I do think it was weird the way that they wrote that letter. And I don't think you can just say
there was this lovely uncertain letter, which was spun out into madness. And I think that's the case
for lots of things. I say this in the book. If you look at the research on press releases,
for instance, scientific press releases, you find that it's not the journalists that are coming along and
taking a scientific paper and spinning it into this, I don't know, taking a paper that's in
mice and then making it sound as if it's relevant to humans, for instance, which is the classic
thing that people do. It's the scientific press release, which is often written by the scientist
or a press officer at the university or both, that often includes the hyping up of saying this is an
experiment in six mice to this is an experiment in
six mice to this is something which is important for all humanity. I just think we can't take the
scientists off entirely for this. No, look, Stuart, I agree with you that Lance had let out. The tone
of it was a bit weird. You're right. But I think you're forgetting that it was entirely a reaction
against the sort of rampaging conspiracy theories and political stuff that was
going on. And yes, scientists should be above all that and entirely unresponsive to all the sort of
cut and thrust of the politics and so on. But it's perhaps missing the mountain for the molehill
in looking at that rather than the other thing. And correspondingly, just a similar example,
I've seen a lot of commentary about how terrible the, this is getting into
politics now, but how terrible the liberal types are or science-y type people are for making fun
of people using horse paste with ivermectin. Oh, you know, horse jokes and stuff like that. It's
so terrible and whatever. Just like I agree with you about the tone of the lance of liquor. I agree.
It's not ideal, but it's kind of missing the context that this is reacting to an entirely deranged thing
yeah yeah yeah i get that i get that and i i just would prefer the kind of calm review of the
evidence type stuff which as chris says there's been a couple of papers recently that have done
that to the scientist writing an open letter thing which i just think i think i've done it once i
think i can't remember what what it was but i think i've signed an open letter maybe a couple
of times i signed the open letter and i regret it i don't think scientists should be signing open
letters i think that's just not a way that scientists should you know unless they do it
in their capacity as a normal as a citizen and it's some political thing then sure but they're
that's often not the case because you write down your doctor or professor or whatever you write
down your university name where you're from and whatever.
You're using your scientific authority and it's stolen valor to some extent, right?
You're signing some open letter on some culture war issue or whatever it is.
And even when it's in a scientist's area of expertise,
it shouldn't be convincing to us if people write an open letter, I don't think.
I think they should be convincing to us if you write a review of the evidence
or produce a new piece of evidence or some sort of statistical analysis however that's
what should convince people it should just be a wee bit frowned upon for scientists to be writing
open letters and trying to convince people especially if they have i shouldn't focus on
this because it's the sort of thing that again the weinsteins focus on but there shouldn't be
an exclamation mark in the scientific thing it's a very minor thing and it's just like, get over it. But it just
rubbed me up the wrong way. The point you make, Stuart, about, I think you make it really well
in the book about researchers hyping up research. And Chris Crandall, I think I've got his name
right, who did a whole bunch of research looking at the way language in the abstracts was being
inflated and who was responsible
for it.
And you cover it really well in your book.
I actually can happily beat up on a whole bunch of things to do with mainstream science,
right?
Like, you know, the replication crisis.
Your book is in many respects, just the documentation of how the current incentives in science,
there is validity to the criticisms that even people
like Eric, but he's not alone in that there's perfectly legitimate people who talk about
the issues with publication bias and with, you know, yeah, like any number of p-hacking
is like the least of the sins, even getting the outright fraud. And in terms of letter writing, we know that when you get 100 scientists to write a letter about how the towers couldn't have come down by the planes, that that is not good.
And we know that 50 scientists writing about how ivermectin is something that should be promoted.
We shouldn't take those things for granted,
but we don't see the same thing
when it's an issue that we agree on.
And I think that an important distinction there is
there's a difference between that
and a statistic that says 98% of climate scientists
agree with climate change.
But I'm curious that your book, when I read it,
it's depressing, right?
Because especially some of the horrifying stuff.
I mean, there's a thing when psychologists
or just get something wrong about like facial feedback mechanism
or power posing.
Sure, it can cost money and whatnot,
but it's not really doing a huge amount of damage to the world.
But when you're talking about trachea transplants
and stuff like that, those were shocking elements.
I'd recommend that anybody read your book.
But I think I put this question to you before,
and you may have answered it
in the last chapter of your book
when you do what the Weinsteins
and other people don't do,
which is you highlight open science methods,
you highlight pre-registering studies and so on.
But I was curious how you,
as somebody that's, you're a scientist, you're a promoter of science, and yet you're part of an ecosystem that you've rightly documented.
The incentive structures are terrible in them, and there's a lot of stuff in it that's bad. And I just wonder, how do you not flip into the dystopian view
of the Weinsteins or the IDW sphere? You know, I mentioned when we were talking about the book
that they'd written, the Hunter-Gatherer's Guide to the 21st Century, that they had really low
standards for evidence. So they'll just pick any old study, even if it's a pilot study or whatever,
they'll use that to back up a statement in the book. When you think think if you were writing a piece of popular science that you would want things to be
backed up by solid evidence in pretty much every case. And if it wasn't, then you would say,
I think it really is a matter of raising your standards. So I think what the replication
crisis has shown us is that scientists in many fields, not just in psychology, but in many,
many fields, and I try and cover other areas in the book, as you know, he talks about medicine and so on. Scientists have had too low standards for too
long. This means accepting very small studies, underpowered studies, studies which are badly
designed, studies which no one has tried to replicate, studies which have been analyzed
poorly, and in some cases, studies which are false, and you're not checking to an adequate
degree whether the studies are real or not. And I think if you raise your standards across the board, then you're not a dystopian Weinstein
type person. You don't become a homeopath. You don't become a vaccine denier. You don't become
a climate change skeptic. You cut out a lot of crap science and you accept that there actually
can be and there is lots of high quality science out there. And as scientists, we should be trying
to emulate that high quality science, whether
that's stuff that's been done with the open science techniques, like pre-registering your
research or sharing your data online or whatever it is, or just research that's very large
scale and done using very high quality materials or whatever it is over a long period of time,
whatever happens to be the positive aspect of it.
There is stuff you can learn from good science out there.
And raising your standards, I think, which is what I'm trying to argue in the book,
immediately cuts off all that, the stuff that we look down on in terms of the skeptics movement and so on. And it cuts out the kind of blanket skepticism, the uninformed, almost pseudo
skepticism of these kind of gurus who are saying, oh, you can't trust any science at all. Because
for instance, you would look at the vaccine trials and see that they're extremely high quality in
most cases. Look at the Pfizer trials, for instance, and you would see that the health
profile was looked at in extreme detail. You would see that the side effects were looked at,
and these were very high quality, randomized controlled trials. And you would not bring
essentially random people who claim to have invented mRNA vaccines onto your podcast to come up with all these anecdotal accounts of babies'
brains blowing up after a pregnant woman has the vaccine. You would have a higher standard. You
would have a higher standard than to rely on that sort of evidence. You would have a higher standard
than going on Joe Rogan and Joe Rogan saying, well, I took ivermectin and I feel much better now.
And then sitting nodding as a scientist, as Brett Weinstein does, nodding along and saying, yep, it's an anecdote, but
here's another couple of anecdotes that also people have said that ivermectin made them feel
better. So I think you can make lots of criticisms of science, but they have to be ones where it's
about raising the standards, not just replacing the bad science with bad reasoning of your own.
Yeah. better standards
are better. We should raise our standards. More rigorous research will be good. Very easy to agree
there. But I'm wondering what you think. I do not know the answer to this, but I am aware across
the world how tight funding is, how difficult it is to get research funding to actually fund
those good quality studies, which are expensive, and the massive pressures on academics, especially
the earlier in your career, the more pressure is on you to publish as much flashy stuff as
quickly as possible. It is an unfair question to ask, but it's clearly the cause of the problem,
but I don't know how to fix it. Yeah, people have asked, you know,
I've done a talk on talking about statistical power, the idea that you need to have studies that are large enough to answer the question that you want to ask. And sometimes
you're looking at something which might have a small effect. And so you need to have a large
study. You're talking to an audience of neuroscientists and they do research with
10 mice in their experiment. And that's dramatically underpowered for the sort of
effect that they're looking for. In most cases, even if they do find an effect, that effect is likely to be a false positive because the only thing you're
going to see in a study that's absolutely tiny is a large, probably spurious effect. You feel bad to
say to people, you're just wasting your time. I think the analogy in the book I have is that
you're trying to look for like a distant exoplanet with a little pair of binoculars or something that
you would use for bird watching. Like even if it's there, you're never going to see it. And if you do see something,
then it's probably just, you've made a mistake. You've looked at the wrong thing.
And people say to me, people say, but I have a PhD student who needs to have these papers out
exactly as you're saying there and needs to have several papers out by the end of their PhD. What
do I say to them? What do I do? They can't afford to do a hundred mice. They can only afford to do
10 or 15. And the answer to that is there isn't really an answer for PhD students right now, but there's a larger sort of restructuring that we need where people
need to be much more open to, for instance, collaborating across different labs, putting
together lots of different samples and collaborating across them using statistical techniques to
analyze these things together. That's the sort of thing that we need to have, this more collaborative
model of science where we can work together on these questions and also a move away from the culture of needing more and more
and more papers, right? So there's a couple of different analyses now where you look at 10,
20 years ago, the number of papers that the average person finishing their PhD had published,
they went on to do a postdoctoral job or become faculty. The number of papers that they were
entering that job with was two or three back
then. And now it's 10, 15, you know, in some fields, like absurd numbers, like that ridiculous
amounts of publication that you have to have done in some areas anyway. It's just anecdotes,
but I have heard, you know, really, really ridiculous numbers of papers that you need
to have published to be competitive in these fields. And it's not very useful me saying we
need to change that. I don't know how we change it. These are broad cultural things that need to move away from. We need to start assessing quality
rather than quantity and so on. But I feel like maybe this is just my hope, but just having the
conversation, just talking about it more, you can see in the scientific literature that discussions
of a crisis and so on are becoming much more common and have across the past 10 years.
It's into the public sphere. My book is just one of
the many things that have brought this to the public consciousness. You talked to Jesse Singal
a few episodes ago. He's got a book that's really good on this topic as well. Generally, people are
talking about this much more. And it has spread to some research funders. They have at least said
explicitly that they are not going to focus so much on quantity and focus more on quality. They're
not going to focus so much on flashiness and impact and so on. So there are some research funders here in the UK who have
done away with the need for having an impact statement when you write a grant application
to say, we're going to change the world with this. We're going to cure cancer. We're going to
revolutionize treatment of depression or whatever it is. They've actually done away with that
statement and instead put in place something that says, are you going to share your data?
What are you going to do to pre-register your analysis so you don't kind of fool yourself into doing some kind of
incorrect analysis? And essentially have an open science, open practices type statement that
replaced that. So I do see some kind of movement to that. And it does, to some extent, it needs to
be both bottom up and top down, right? So you need to have the bottom up kind of discussion of
scientists criticizing each other and pointing out these problems and saying like being honest with each other and trying not to worry about
hurting each other's feelings too much and saying like, sorry, that paper just, you just can't
answer the question. I had to do a review where they had 50 post-mortem brains and they were
trying to talk about numbers of neurons and cognitive abilities of the people that had died.
And obviously they measured them before they died and so on. I had to say in the review,
I'm sorry that you just can't answer a question like this with 50 brains.
And I know you've gone to great effort to get these 50 post-Morton brains,
and I'm really sorry, but you just, as a reviewer,
I have to say that this is a bad paper.
A few months later, I saw it got published somewhere else.
I didn't review it exactly the same as I had reviewed it.
No changes made whatsoever.
No caveats added to it, whatever.
But anyway, that's just how things work.
But I think we need to worry a little bit less about
hurting each other's feelings or worrying about our prestige
and our place in academia and so on and just stay these things.
And at the same time, we need to encourage top-down changes
like the funders changing, like universities valuing different things.
As always, it's far easier said than done.
I do see some reasons for optimism, even though I also see regular stories of new fraud being
discovered, new low quality papers being published, people focusing on entirely the wrong things.
I think there's reasons for both pessimism and optimism. And one just has to try and make more
people aware of this problem and more people both in science and outside science aware of the issue and just kind of hope that they'll do something about it.
I know we've used up way too much of your time and thank you for coming on and dealing with all
our harassment. But I do have one last moment of harassment while I've got you here. And to end on
like a non-controversial note, you did a review of Cordelia Fine's Testosterone Rex book, right?
A couple of years back. And you recently reviewed Carol Hoeven's Tea book. And I read Carol Hoeven,
or Hoeven, I'm not sure, book recently as well. And I couldn't see your review because it was
paywalled at the time. And I know you were critical of Cordelia Fine's book.
And I'm just, so I'm basically exploiting the opportunity
of having you on here and your goodwill to say,
what's your take on the testosterone wars and this latest salvo?
Yeah, I think what I said about the,
I don't remember exactly what I wrote about the Cordelia Fine book,
but my view was that it had lots of good points to make about low
quality studies.
Again, there's been plenty of them in the world of endocrinology and or psycho neuro
endocrinology, linking hormones to behavior in the brain and so on.
Certainly a lot of low quality stuff there.
But the general impression you get, I think, from reading, and we could talk about this
for a whole couple of hours.
I think the general impression you get from reading books by Cordelia Fine and by several other people who are generally in agreement is that there aren't really that many differences between males and females neurologically or psychologically.
And any differences that there are, you should be quite suspicious of them.
And I don't think that's true.
I think there are lots of quite big differences, even if we don't know what they mean, there are differences in the brain of males and females. One thing, males' brains have just a higher volume. And whether that means anything, there's an interesting paradox because the measured cognitive abilities of males and females are just about the same. They're just about average. And yet there's this big difference in their brain. And so what does it mean it mean? Maybe it doesn't mean anything. There's lots of research needs to be done on that.
But I do find it a bit weird that the general impression you get from those kinds of books is
that there aren't any differences. And, you know, people who are interested in these differences are
maybe a bit suspect. And so it was quite refreshing to read the Carol Hovind book where she says,
look, there's lots of uncertainty here. I'm not saying that all sex differences are explained by
testosterone or anything like that. I don't want to go back into the line of the studies that were very rightly
criticized, but there are lots of things that we can criticize on the other side of the debate as
well. And they've gone far too far. So she talks about how people have argued that there's no
correlation between testosterone levels and sports performance, for instance. And that's true within
a sex. So within males and within females, there's not that much correlation between sport performance and testosterone. But if you look
between the sexes, where there are huge, enormous differences in the level of testosterone that's
circulating. So the bell curve of the, if you look at the sort of effect size difference
are just enormously far apart. They're the huge difference, very, very little overlap.
I can't remember the exact numbers, but it's larger than the height difference between males and females, substantially larger. Then that does correlate with sport
performance. Like people with much higher levels, they're male, they'll be stronger and faster and
so on. So she talks about all that in the book. And I do recommend it. It's a really good book.
There are some aspects that I criticize. I think some of the genetic stuff is out of date and
talks about candidate gene studies, which I was not a fan of. I think that was a bit of an
unfortunate thing. Someone should have told her that we don't do that kind of research anymore
because it's underpowered, low quality research. We've moved on from that and no one seems to have
told her and she didn't seem to have noticed. But I think the general story there that testosterone
is extremely important in differentiating males from females and that males are very different in
terms of their strength and physiology and so on, and probably their aggression. And that's in some way due to higher levels of testosterone. I think she's uncertain
enough about the more detailed psychological stuff. Obviously, there's been really florid
theories of how testosterone affects absolutely everything about people's behaviors in the past,
which is often based on silly, low-quality research. But I think she does a good job of
explaining the basics of it and saying, okay, let's just step back from the political stuff. And she even has a chapter on transgender
issues and doesn't really take a side on the kind of culture war, but uses the transgender stuff
to illustrate the effects of testosterone and talk about it from a scientific perspective,
rather than going down the kind of the Brett Weinstein and Heather Hying line of saying,
this is very bad, or going down the other line of saying this is very bad or going down the
other line of saying we must focus on this is the only thing that's important in this sphere so I
think she does a really good job of being a testosterone moderate and sticking to the evidence
the testosterone centrist maybe yeah that's the impression I got admittedly I haven't looked into
studies yet but the stuff about holding infants and the effect that that can have on followers testosterone levels was interesting to me, like talking about comparing societies where
males hold infants versus those that don't. I share your feelings about a refreshing book in
some respects. Matt, last I pulled Stuart down another wormhole. Is there anything you want to say before we let him ski up?
No, I've got nothing more to say.
Two big swinging brains rub up against each other.
The real edifying spectacle.
Thank you.
Thank you.
I know it's just horrible, but that's one of those stock phrases.
You've been very cogent and informative.
Thank you, Stuart, for coming on and talking to us.
It's a great pleasure. It's weird, actually weird actually to hear you to be part of the conversation because I've listened to so many of your episodes I think probably them all maybe there's one or two that
I haven't quite got around to yet and they've been so good I love what you're doing with all
these gurus and so on yeah keep up the good work oh thank thank you Stuart and well if you've heard
a lot of them then you'll know what I mean when I say that I feel like we've transcended our differences and raised them into a higher synthesis and really engaged in some good faith sense-making today.
So well done.
Well done all.
It's a bit, we've went through gears, we've penetrated membranes and we've, you know, in a lot of ways, we've kind of sieved Western civilization.
We've gone far beyond the pitter-patter on the neocortex. That's right. We're beyond that. We've gone far beyond the, uh, the pitter patter on the neocortex.
That's right.
Oh, we're beyond that.
We've got beyond that.
Well, three nobels for everyone.
Everyone gets to have this conversation, but thanks very much, Stuart.
You're a very patient man.
And yeah.
Oh, sorry.
You are, you are a patient man.
Shouldn't have interrupted
i wanted to say that we're gonna link to some of stewart's articles and links to buy his books of
course which is very important and other interesting things in the show notes and
chris please continue your thought no that i was just gonna pivot to say that i recommend everyone
gets to work i actually use your book stewart on my course that i teach over here in japan
about research methods.
There is actually a Japanese translation in the works. We've signed the contract. I don't know
when it's going to come out. I don't actually know how long these things take to translate and so on,
but there is going to be a Japanese version of it at some point.
Yeah. And I know of one other researcher in Tokyo who also used your book as a foundation
of his course. So I'll send you details about that. But this is the back padding previous segment of the podcast, but it's accurate.
It's true.
I'm glad you found it useful anyway.
Thank you.
Yeah.
So, all right.
Since making ended, Matt, we'll call things to closure better at that than I am.
Yep.
That's right.
I'll close things off.
I can't remember what the links are.
I should have Stuart Ritchie's Twitter handle in my brain,
but it's not.
But there'll be links
to everything in the show.
Stuart, would you like to give
a Twitter handle
in case people want to follow?
Yeah, it's just my name.
It's just my name,
Stuart G. Ritchie.
So it's just my name
with my middle initial
in the middle.
That's my Twitter account.
So wonderful.
You're free to follow.
Thank you.
Fantastic.
So the music is fading in.
And Chris, you've got one thing more
you want to say to me, I think.
Oh, yeah. Grubble at the feet of your muscle master. That's music is fading in. And Chris, you've got one thing more you want to say to me, I think.
Oh, yeah. Grubble at the feet of your muscle master. That's the thing. Yeah.
Okay. All right. See ya.
Bye. Thank you.