Freakonomics Radio - 167. The Three Hardest Words in the English Language

Episode Date: May 15, 2014

Why learning to say “I don’t know” is one of the best things you can do. ...

Transcript
Discussion (0)
Starting point is 00:00:00 Hey, it's Stephen Dubner. Our new book is out, Think Like a Freak, and we want to talk about it with you. So, we are forming the Think Like a Freak Book Club. How's it work? You send in your questions about the book, and we will answer them on this podcast. There are nine chapters in the book, so why don't we do the first installment of our book club with chapters one, two, and three. Those are what does it mean to think like a freak? The three hardest words in the English language and what's your problem? Just send your questions to radio at Freakonomics.com with the subject line book club. And if we choose your question for the podcast, we will send you a signed copy of Think Like a
Starting point is 00:00:42 Freak or a limited edition Think Like a Freak t-shirt. Thanks. Hey, Levitt, can I read you a story? Sure. Okay. So, I'm going to tell you this story. A little girl named Mary goes to the beach with her mother and brother. A little girl named Mary goes to the beach with her mother and brother. A little girl named Mary goes to the beach with her mother and brother.
Starting point is 00:01:07 They drive there in a red car. They drive there in a red car. At the beach. They swim. Eat some ice cream. Play in the sand. And have some sandwiches for lunch. So that's the end of the short story.
Starting point is 00:01:21 Now, Levitt, let me ask you a few questions about it. So now I'm going to ask you the questions. Ready? Number one, what color was the car? I think it might have been red. What color was the car? The car was red. Fantastic.
Starting point is 00:01:35 Number two, did they have fish and chips for lunch? No, they had sandwiches. Yes, they did have fish and chips for lunch. No, they do not have fish and chips for lunch. No, they do not have fish and chips for lunch. What did they have? Sandwiches. They had sandwiches. Number three, did they listen to music in the car?
Starting point is 00:01:56 No, they didn't. I'm sure they did. Doesn't everyone listen to music? Did they listen to music in the car? No, they didn't listen to music in the car? No, they didn't listen to music in the car. And number four, did they drink lemonade with their lunch? No, they didn't drink lemonade with their lunch. Probably not. Most people drink soda. What flavor ice cream did Mary have?
Starting point is 00:02:21 I don't know. Mary had chocolate flavor ice cream. What flavor ice cream did Mary have? Mary don't know. Mary had chocolate-flavored ice cream. What flavor ice cream did Mary have? Mary had vanilla ice cream. What flavor ice cream did Mary have? You can say I don't know. Strawberry? From WNYC, this is Freakonomics Radio, the podcast that explores the hidden side of everything. Here's your host, Stephen Dupner. Steve Levitt and I have just published a new book called Think Like a Freak. One chapter is called The Three Hardest Words in the English Language.
Starting point is 00:03:20 Now, what do you think those words are? In the past, some may have argued that people have a hard time saying, I love you. I don't think so. People run around saying, I love you all the time to friends, pets, strangers. So what are those three most difficult words? So, Levitt, one piece of business advice that people give all over the web, LinkedIn, and you just read all the regular kind of job searching career advice place. One big piece of advice I give is when you're in a job interview, there's one thing you should never say to the person interviewing you, which is, I don't know. You should never admit you don't know the answer to a question.
Starting point is 00:04:04 Think that's pretty good advice? That actually probably is good advice for an inner Jew. Just say inner Jew? That probably is good advice for a job interview, but it is terrible advice for life. And why is that such terrible advice for life? Let me try to answer that by example. So we wrote Freakonomics, and because it was deemed a business book and we sold a lot of copies, that made us business experts. And since we wrote that book, I've been asked a ton, you as well, to go talk to companies and give them advice. And what's amazing to me, and I've kept very close attention over the last decade almost,
Starting point is 00:04:46 is I could count on one hand the number of occasions in which someone in a company, in front of their boss, on a question that they might possibly have ever been expected to know the answer, has said, I don't know. Within the business world, there's a general view that your job is to be an expert. And no matter how much you have to fake or how much you're making it up, that you just should give an answer and hope for the best afterwards. And I've seen it teaching the business school students that they're incredibly good, the MBAs, at faking like they know the answer when they have no idea. But that's so counterproductive. I mean, think about it. It might keep your job for another week or another month. It might make people think you're good. But that's not the point. I mean, what fun is life if all you do is go through life trying to fake like you're something that you're
Starting point is 00:05:37 not, when really the goal is to be good and to improve and to learn and to make things better. And the only way to do that is to start by saying, I don't know. When I don't know, I don't know. All right, so it's easy to ridicule the fakers and bluffers of the world, but we can understand their motivation, can't we? Everyone wants to look smart, look competent, look like we belong. But wouldn't it be even better if, as Levitt says, we could improve and learn, actually make things better? If you think so, listen on. Let's try to figure out where this reluctance to say,
Starting point is 00:06:20 I don't know, comes from. Hi, Amanda. Hey, Stephen. Amanda Waterman teaches developmental psychology at the University of Leeds. A lot of her research is about people's willingness, especially children's willingness, to say, I don't know. Her early research looked at whether kids would say,
Starting point is 00:06:39 I don't know, when asked a patently ridiculous question. What do feet have for breakfast? Toenails if you chop them off. Smelly socks. You can say I don't know. They have your... They have your skin. They have a walk for breakfast.
Starting point is 00:07:00 Shoes and socks for breakfast? OK, next question Is red heavier than yellow? Yellow Is red heavier than yellow? No, because yellow is my favourite colour. Bella, what do you think? I think it is heavier than yellow because... Because, because, because...
Starting point is 00:07:35 Um... Red is in hell. And then yellow's in the sky, isn't it? Yeah, so it's down below, down there. Is a jumper angrier than a tree? No. Is a jumper angrier than a tree? No, because trees look like they have faces.
Starting point is 00:08:05 Yes, a jumper is angrier than a tree because you can pick jumpers up and that's how they move. And trees just go like this. They sway from side to side in the wind. So, Amanda, is a sweater, what you there call a jumper, angrier than a tree? Okay, well, that's perhaps where this research started, where we asked children very silly questions about whether a jumper or indeed a sweater was angrier than a tree to see whether they would indicate in any way whether they were confused by the question or whether they would
Starting point is 00:08:49 just go along with the sort of fun spirit of the question. And we found that often children did want to decide whether a jumper was angrier than a tree. But we moved on after that. I was more interested in looking at whether... You got bored by that stuff, didn't you? I got bored. Jumpers are obviously more angry than trees, right? Oh, really? I thought trees. No, no, jumpers were definitely angrier because I think they're kind of used by people. You know, they have to be put on and off all the time. I see. Okay.
Starting point is 00:09:14 But we were more interested in looking at perhaps something that would be a little bit more applicable to real life because generally adults don't ask you very silly questions all the time. But you do come across questions to which you do not know the answer. So it might be a perfectly sensible question, but you just happen not to know the answer to it. So the research went on from silly questions to what we've called unanswerable questions. Which brings us back to the story you heard earlier
Starting point is 00:09:39 about the little girl named Mary. A little girl named Mary goes to the beach with her mother and brother. This story was part of a study that Amanda Waterman ran to see how children will answer a question that is essentially unanswerable, a question to which the most sensible answer is, I don't know. In the story about Mary and her family going to the seashore, we learn several facts. The car was red. They ate sandwiches for lunch. But other facts are not given to us.
Starting point is 00:10:10 Did they listen to music in the car? We aren't told that. Did they have lemonade with their sandwiches? We don't know. Did they drink lemonade with their lunch? No, they didn't drink lemonade with their lunch. Did they listen to music in the car? You can say I don't know.
Starting point is 00:10:32 No, they didn't listen to music in the car. The vast majority of children pretend they know the answer to unanswerable questions. It has varied between studies. If you're looking at maybe two-thirds to three-quarters of children, and we're talking in the age range here of about five to eight years old, would say yes or no to a yes-no question that we know they don't know the answer to. So these kids are preparing themselves very well for careers in business or politics, maybe as a pundit of some sort, or wherever else
Starting point is 00:11:07 in society people pretend to know more than they do. Now, why is this impulse so strong among children? Personally, what I think is going on is that the children are very used to a classroom situation. Obviously, you do get asked questions by your parent as a child, but a lot of question answer exchanges happen in school. Teachers ask kids questions all the time to ascertain what they're picking up from the lesson etc and generally in that situation it's sensible to have a go you know you don't generally earn many points for saying i don't know when a teacher asks a question so having a go and and actually feeling good that maybe you have that piece of knowledge and that you can show, you know, that you are clever. So I think that when it seems like a test situation,
Starting point is 00:11:51 they fall back on what they've already started to learn from the context that they spend a lot of time in. I guess when you describe that as an academic setting, even an academic setting among children, it reminds me of certainly academic settings among older people, university and so on. But it also reminds me of settings in the workplace where very often when a question is raised, someone is expected to know the answer and maybe the asker of the question, if they don't necessarily know the answer, they certainly have strong opinions about it. So, I'm curious about how you would connect what you see in young children with what you see among the rest of us adults in whether it's the work world or politics or policy, things like that. Yeah, well, I think that ties back into this whole social aspect of any kind of conversation or dialogue or question answer exchange where you, a lot of how you respond
Starting point is 00:12:42 is affected by how you perceive the expectations of the person asking the question. So it's not just about giving a straight factual answer as to the state of your knowledge. It's actually trying to work out all the other contexts that surround why that question has been asked, what you think the interviewer wants from you. And I think children do start to do that from a very young age I think one of the things that happens with kids but you can actually definitely apply this to adult conversations is that there's quite a power differential between a child and an adult who's asking them a question and the child will feel that the adult is the person that has more of the power and therefore perhaps they feel slightly disadvantaged they feel like they want to show what they can do.
Starting point is 00:13:26 They don't feel as comfortable admitting when they don't know something. That clearly applies across into the work context with adults where you have a boss with an employee or you have somebody who's higher up the hierarchy than you are and you feel that expectation that, as you say, somebody should know the answer to this. As it turns out, Waterman also tested some adults with unanswerable questions. How'd they do?
Starting point is 00:14:07 We found that about a quarter of the adults tried to answer the yes, no unanswerable questions. At least that's better than the three quarters of the kids who couldn't bring themselves to say, I don't know, but still not very encouraging. Think about it for a minute. When's the last time someone you know, maybe even yourself, gave a firm answer to a question that you really didn't know. Maybe a week ago? An hour ago? Maybe just now? Coming up on Freakonomics Radio, the consequences of not being able to admit what you don't know. It's easier to spend a billion dollars pretending they knew than to actually find out the answer. And what we can do about it.
Starting point is 00:14:50 There's only one way to learn, and that's through feedback. From WNYC, this is Freakonomics Radio. Here's your host, Stephen Dubner. One of the first steps in learning to think like a freak is learning to say, I don't know. Why? Because until you can admit what you don't know, it's virtually impossible to learn what you need to. Because if you think you already have all the answers, you won't go looking for them. But let's be clear. Simply saying, I don't know, isn't a solution. It's just a first step. You have to figure out what you don't know and then work like a dog to learn it.
Starting point is 00:15:53 And how do you learn? There's only one way to learn, and that's through feedback. That's Steve Levitt again. The thing about always faking is that if you fake like you know the answer, you don't have the freedom to explore other possibilities. But if you actually care about the outcome and the truth, saying I don't know is critical. One thing we've learned is that the only way to learn is through feedback. That whether you're a human being, an animal, an organization, the way that you learn is by trying different things and seeing the outcome when you try different approaches and comparing those outcomes.
Starting point is 00:16:34 Feedback comes in many flavors. It might mean going out and asking a lot of questions. It might mean gathering a lot of data. If the data don't happen to be just lying around, waiting to be plucked, you may have to generate data yourself. How? One great way is to run some kind of an experiment. If you want to think like a freak, you need to fall in love with experimentation. Now, scientists have been doing this for centuries.
Starting point is 00:17:07 The rest of us, not so much. But why should scientists have all the fun? So something that puzzled me for the longest time when I started working with firms, that I would preach to them the gospel of experimentation, of either randomized experiments or accidental experiments, all the things that I do. And always it was met with great enthusiasm. Of course, yeah, you know, experiments, that's the way to go. And then when it actually came down to it, we could almost never convince anyone to do things experimentally. Why don't they want to do it?
Starting point is 00:17:38 What are they reluctant about? Are they afraid it's going to be expensive, that they won't know how to do it, that it's going to embarrass them? What's the problem? For years, I struggled with trying to understand what it was. And I thought, was it expensive? Well, no, I could design it in ways that it really wouldn't cost them anything. Would it take too long, too many resources? I mean, it was none of that. And it finally hit me what was totally obvious, is that in order to run an experiment, you have to start from the premise
Starting point is 00:18:03 that you don't know what the right answer is. So I don't know if I should do the red version or the blue version. Should I price high? Should I price low? Should I give people a money-back guarantee or should not? So all of those are questions that might be perfect for trying to answer with an experiment. But in order to be willing to run the experiment, you have to say, well, geez, I don't know the answer. Wait, but you're the expert. You're in charge of marketing. You're in charge of pricing. You're supposed to know the answer. And so the people in the firms we were working with felt so much pressure to be the expert that they wouldn't run the experiments because that was an admission that they weren't really as expert as they maybe
Starting point is 00:18:40 had been pretending to be or other people wanted them to pretend to be. Levitt, in addition to teaching at the University of Chicago, helps run a consulting firm. One of my favorite early stories, this might have been the very first company that I went to visit. This was a company whose unwillingness to experiment, to admit what they didn't know, had pretty big consequences. It was a huge multinational retailer. They spent many millions of dollars in advertising in the U.S. alone, but they weren't sure how effective the advertising was. So far, they'd come to one concrete conclusion, that TV ads were about four times more effective dollar for dollar than print ads. Now, how would they know this? That's what their sales data told them.
Starting point is 00:19:34 They advertised every week in newspapers. But because TV advertising is so expensive, they only bought ads three times a year, Black Friday, Christmas, and Father's Day. So then I said, I think I might understand why your TV ads seem to have a return on investment that's four times as great as your newspaper ads. And they said, why? I said, well, every single time you go and do a big advertising campaign, your sales go through the roof right afterwards. And like, yeah, Father's Day, Thanksgiving, Christmas. I said, but don't you think maybe even if you didn't do the ads,
Starting point is 00:20:12 you would sell more stuff around Christmas and on Black Friday? And they said, yeah. So basically, this company is spending hundreds of millions of dollars on TV advertising to persuade people to go to their stores to buy stuff on the three times of the year when people were already going to be going to stores to buy stuff. Exactly right. So what about the print ads? This retailer had run newspaper ads pretty much every Sunday for 20 years in more than 200 markets. How effective were they? It was hard to know. So I go to them and I say, I think you cannot learn from the existing data the answer to your question. And to their credit, they said, well, geez, this is a really important question for us. How could we learn the truth? And I said, well,
Starting point is 00:20:55 TV ads are tougher. I don't even know how we'll ever learn that. But the newspaper answer, that's an easy one. So you said you have a couple hundred markets. And I did some calculations about how much power we would have if we did a randomized experiment. And I think I would need you to give me about 40 of your newspaper markets. And I would randomly assign 20 of those to a control group where we wouldn't do any newspaper inserts for about three months. And we will compare the sales in the markets where you kept on doing the inserts to the ones where you didn't do inserts. And by doing that comparison, we would learn with a great degree of confidence what the impact of newspaper inserts on sales. And did you expect them to say, hey, that's a fantastic idea. And
Starting point is 00:21:36 just think of all the money we'll save by not buying the ads that's even cheaper. Did you expect them to be enthusiastic about it? They were spending hundreds of million dollars a year on these newspaper inserts. They had no idea if they worked. And they even had this belief, which I'm almost certain was erroneous anyway, but they had reason to believe the newspaper inserts weren't that good relative to TV. So I thought for sure, this is a no brainer. This is easy. Of course, you know, let's get rolling. How fast can we get it started? And what was their response? So we went to them with the proposal. And their reaction surprised me.
Starting point is 00:22:08 They said, are you crazy? We can't not advertise in 20 markets. We'll all get fired. And they went on. One of the other guys chimed in and said, yeah, you know, we once had this summer intern, an MBA that we had hired. And one of his jobs was to do the ordering of the media in different cities. And the guy was so incompetent that for reasons we never really figured out, he just forgot
Starting point is 00:22:33 to order the newspaper inserts for a big chunk of Pittsburgh for the entire summer. And just kind of joking around with him, I said, well, I hope you hired that guy. And he said, hired him? He was terrible. We would never hire him. Why should we hire him? I said, well, at least he was terrible in just the right way that you're all too chicken to be terrible because, I said, what happened to the sales in your stores in Pittsburgh
Starting point is 00:22:57 when this guy forgot to do the newspaper instance? So what did happen? To their credit, they went and they looked at the data and they called me with a spring of excitement in their voice and they said to me you're not going to believe what we found we found no impact whatsoever on the sales in pittsburgh when this guy didn't put in the ads and i said wow that's amazing all right so when can we start the experiment again i just need 40 markets and i'll randomly assign them and we'll stop the ads for three months. And they said, after a pause, they said,
Starting point is 00:23:28 are you crazy? We can't not advertise in 20 markets. The CEO will kill us. And it's truly amazing that the only piece of real learning they had, the only piece of real feedback that they were given, is that the newspaper inserts didn't work. And yet, it's now five years later, and they've probably spent close to another billion dollars putting ads in the newspaper. And the reason was because if they tried to do an experiment to see whether newspaper inserts worked or not, it would be admission that
Starting point is 00:24:02 they did not know whether newspaper inserts worked. It's easier to spend a billion dollars pretending they knew than to actually find out the answer. It's easy to criticize people who get locked into pretending to know answers that they don't. But as we said at the beginning of this program, those three little words, I don't know, can be really hard to say. First, it takes a lot of confidence to be willing to say, I don't know. And I think actually it's easy for someone like me to say, I don't know, because, you know, I've had some success. We sold some books. I've done well in academics. So if I say, I don't know, people say, oh, isn't that cute? The Midwestern self-deprecating, blah, blah, blah. But if you're just, you know, a middle manager
Starting point is 00:24:56 in a firm and you say, I don't know, people look around, they're like, this guy doesn't know? What's wrong with him? So the incentives to fake it, to never admit you don't know an answer, are pretty strong. But maybe there's a way to flip those incentives, to encourage people to say I don't know when they really don't know. Remember Amanda Waterman, the British psychologist who studies kids? We went back to her with this. So in our new book, we make the argument that people should be much more willing to admit what they don't know, especially in settings, whether it is business or academia or politics, where you're dealing not just with a simple set of facts or even a simple set of beliefs, but with a process or, you know, when you're trying to
Starting point is 00:25:45 tease out cause and effect, which can be very complicated, not only in the, certainly in the future, what policy will work, but even trying to understand what has worked in the past. And we make the argument that it would be better if more people admitted what they didn't know, because that is kind of the first step on the road to learning and discovery and getting feedback and running some experiments, perhaps, to find out what's real. I'm just curious whether you would endorse that argument of ours, or whether you think that's not really a principal obstacle. No, I absolutely endorse it. And I would go back a little to saying that I guess at least part of the responsibility lies with the people who are
Starting point is 00:26:26 at the top of whatever hierarchy you happen to be talking about. If you create a culture where it's okay to say, you know, I don't know the answer to that, but wouldn't it be good to find out? Then I think you can, that then perpetuates through the organization. But I think the problem is, I guess that the people that have come up to the top are maybe used to having got to the top, possibly by not admitting when they didn't know the answer. So perhaps that you get that cycle. But no, I absolutely agree with you that saying you don't know appropriately is a good thing. So let's break that cycle, shall we? Let's start with the generation that really matters. You can say I don't know.
Starting point is 00:27:14 Uh, uh... I don't know. I don't know. I don't know. I don't know, miss. I don't know. That's perfectly fine. Hey, podcast listeners.
Starting point is 00:27:43 On next week's show, we stop making fun of children and start learning from them. Sometimes doing things differently and simply and with a kind of joy and triviality leads you to a really special place that as an adult, you don't get to go to very often. Why we should all spend some time thinking like a child. That's next time on Freakonomics Radio. Freakonomics Radio is produced by WNYC and Dubner Productions. Our staff includes David Herman, Greg Rosalski, Greta Cohn, Beret Lam, Shruti Pinamaneni, Susie Lechtenberg, and Chris Bannon, with engineering help from Jim Briggs. If you want more Freakonomics Radio, you can subscribe to our podcast on iTunes or go to Freakonomics.com, where you'll find lots of radio, a blog, the books, and more.

There aren't comments yet for this episode. Click on any sentence in the transcript to leave a comment.