Freakonomics Radio - 262. This Is Your Brain on Podcasts
Episode Date: October 13, 2016Neuroscientists still have a great deal to learn about the human brain. One recent MRI study sheds some light, finding that a certain kind of storytelling stimulates enormous activity across broad swa...ths of the brain. The takeaway is obvious: you should be listening to even more podcasts.
Transcript
Discussion (0)
Let me warn you right off the bat. While today's episode of Freakonomics Radio is a regular brand new episode, it is also a fundraiser.
One of those episodes where we ask you to send money to WNYC, the public radio station that produces our show.
We'll talk about that more later. But if you just want to get out of the way now, all you've got to do is go to Freakonomics.com and click the donate button.
Or you can text the word FREAK to the number 69866, and they'll text you back a donation link.
In either case, you'll find all kinds of Freakonomics Radio swag available only to
donors of this show. Thanks in advance. Now, moving on. As you surely know, there is a lot of amazing research going on these days into the human brain.
And a lot of amazing brain researchers.
Hi, this is Jack Gallant.
So why did we choose Jack Gallant from UC Berkeley to speak with?
I could pretend it's just because he's very good at what he does and very versatile.
I have done everything. I mean, I've done computer science.
I've done psychology.
I've done neurophysiology.
So I call myself a computational and cognitive neuroscientist.
And all that would be true.
But that's not why we chose Jack Gallant.
We chose Jack Gallant because, well, because he's on our team.
Even if he didn't set out to be.
And what team is that, you ask?
It's Team Podcast.
In his lab at Berkeley, Jack Gallant stuck research subjects in an fMRI machine,
that stands for Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging, and he had them listen to podcasts.
There's no way I'm going to excel drawing this naked man's essence.
And I'm about to cry.
Does she need Mary Kay or does she need Jesus?
And I thought, no, that can't be it.
That can't be all there is.
And I quit the job.
We use them because they're compelling.
You pay attention to them.
You want to know the resolution to them.
They're very powerful stories.
So on today's show,
we'll learn some things we didn't know about how the brain works,
especially when it comes to language.
The brain areas involved in comprehending the meaning of language are very, very broadly distributed.
And we'll hear how people are impacted by the language in a podcast like, say, Freakonomics Radio.
I don't think I would have made the Olympics if I hadn't listened to that podcast.
Let's just put it that way.
From WNYC Studios, this is Freakonomics Radio, the podcast that explores the hidden side of everything.
Here's your host, Stephen Dubner.
We're starting today with Jack Gallant. He is a psychology professor who has his own
brain research lab at Berkeley. Let me ask you an extraordinarily naive question. Let's pretend we take the brain
in its totality as a three-dimensional machine that does all this stuff and is connected to
all these things and has a history to evolutionarily. And if we were to take that
machine and turn it into either a pie chart or a graph that's got a 100% and it's got a 0%. Where are we on that graph
of truly knowing what's going on in the brain? Are we above 50% yet?
That's the way I think about the problem too. You know, there's 80 billion neurons in the brain.
So here's the larger context. The brain is a computer, but it's not like your desktop computer,
right? The brain processes information, it represents information,
it represents information about the world and it allows you to interact with the world.
So it's an information processing device and therefore it's a computer.
But it's a wet, squishy computer that evolved according to its own principles and its own history
and its operating principles are very, very different from the desktop computer that you have.
So a desktop computer is what's called a von Neumann architecture,
where the design of the system is such that the hardware, the transistors,
and the software, the program that runs on the hardware,
are as independent as they can possibly be.
The brain works by very different principles.
The brain is essentially just a collection of wires,
and everything's just wired to everything else in this big, gigantic, tangled mess.
Every neuron is connected to between 1,000 and 10,000 other neurons.
There are feed-forward and feed-back loops.
There's a multi-scale organization of the brain,
so there are individual neurons.
These are organized into local circuits.
Those circuits are organized in layers and columns.
Those layers and columns are organized in areas.
Your brain contains, no one's quite sure probably something on the order of two to five hundred distinct functional areas and any given area or piece of the brain has about a 50 chance of being
connected to every other piece so it's a hugely highly interconnected network and it takes about
20 minutes or so to grow a new synapse. So as you're
listening to me speak and your thoughts are fleeting from one thing to another, you're essentially
having those different thoughts as an, those different thoughts are an emergent property
of information flowing over this fixed set of wires. So the brain has this dynamical property
where information flowing over a fixed set of wires can interact with itself in order to
give rise to this new emergent property of thought. And we have no idea how that kind of system works
at this point. You described the brain as a mess. If you were to be tasked with redesigning it,
let's say we'll give you a budget of, you know,
a couple billion in two and a half years and a staff of 40. What do you do? What are the
most significant differences? That is one of the most entertaining questions I've ever heard.
You're welcome. I wouldn't redesign the brain because I don't think we have enough information
to redesign the brain. Okay. But if I'm going to give you the two and a half billion dollars
anyway. Well, I would just use it to do basic neuroscience. Okay, but if I'm going to give you the $2.5 billion anyway...
Well, I would just use it to do basic neuroscience.
And what do you want to find out when you say basic neuroscience?
It's obviously much less basic than I would imagine.
So in any field of science, including neuroscience,
there are sort of two large kinds of problems that can limit your progress.
One is how much data you have about the system.
And one is what your theory is about the system.
And of course, in the end, if you have a complicated problem, you're limited by both
of these things. But at any given point in time, one of these is more problematic. And in neuroscience,
the real thing that's limiting our understanding of the brain right now is not theory, it's data.
We have plenty of theories about the brain. The problem is we can't constrain any of
those theories with data because we don't have good enough data. After we collect an enormous
data set, then it becomes a sort of a modeling theory machine learning problem to troll through
those data and try to understand the basic principles that gave rise to them.
I've had this totally idiotic theory for about 10 years now that I'm sure is wrong. And it's
not really a theory. It's more just a metaphor. And I I'm sure is wrong and it's not really a theory
It's more just a metaphor and I've said it to people and it sounds smart
So they always nod
But I want to run it by you because you'll be able to prove I think how idiotic it is
But I'd like to improve the the theory so I'm coming to you for feedback
Go for it when I think of the human today in 2016 and i think of the stimuli
that any given person deals with on a given day and granted there's a huge variance if i live in
new york city or if i live in you know one of any other million places on earth and depending on
everything what kind of business what kind of family what kind of political structure and so
on there's obviously huge variants but we're dealing with a whole lot of 7 billion, you know, pretty similar animals
who have this computer in our heads, as you put it. And I always think of that computer as being
pretty good and fairly reliable hardware that is relatively old, because it's been evolving, you know, quite slowly for a long time,
but that the stimuli that we're responding to on a given day, which has changed a lot faster than we
physiologically evolve. And those stimuli include all kinds of transactions and interactions and
responses and behaviors that, you know, our ancestors never could have dreamed
of. And I sometimes feel as if we're just trying to run, you know, version 18 million point four
to four software on hardware 1.0 and that we do our best to accommodate, but that it's really hard.
And that would explain a lot of our biases and
heuristics and so on, not all of which are bad, but would explain why we're not,
I don't want to say optimal, but why we sometimes don't act as though the most rational people
among us argue we should act. And I'm just curious if there's any merit at all to that metaphor and
assuming not, if you could offer me a better metaphor to impress people within the future.
Well, that's an interesting problem.
I guess I would have two things to say about that.
First of all, human society has been evolving very rapidly, you know, for 50,000 years since the dawn of agriculture.
Everything has been different since then, continuously different.
The pace of change is maybe accelerating, but things have constantly been different since then continuously different the pace of changes may be accelerating
but things have constantly been different and we've been dealing with these societal changes
for that whole time because evolution has given us a very flexible computer system we kind of have a
fixed brain but during development and even when we're adults, we can learn to flexibly use that system
to solve novel problems. That doesn't say the system can't be overwhelmed or confused or,
you know, operate suboptimally, but it's a pretty damn flexible system. And I think that's why
humans have managed to push culture much farther than any other animals. I mean,
certain species of non-human
animals have culture in the sense that small groups of them will learn behaviors that they
will pass on to their mates and that don't influence their genetics directly except perhaps
by increasing their fitness. But humans, you know, this is all of human existence is culture at this
point, right? Now, one more thing I'll add is remember that in the human brain, the hardware
and the software are intimately linked, right? So the hardware can only run the programs that are
conferred by the hardware. It's very different from your desktop computer. So if you think about
your desktop computer, if you have, you know, an Atari 64, you could try to run OSX on it. It
wouldn't work very well. You could maybe hack on
the OSX long enough for a few years and get it to limp along on an Atari 64, but it would not
behave well. We have, in some sense, a worse situation because we have an Atari 64 computer
in our head, but it's running Atari 64 software. We're just trying to use it to solve modern problems. Okay, so plainly, you know a lot about how we
use our brains and how the brain works. I do just want to hear you talk about vision for a bit.
Okay, so vision is a very interesting sense. Humans rely on vision more than any other sense.
At the same time, vision seems completely trivial,
because, you know, you open your eyes, you see, what's the problem? I mean, you just walk around,
you do stuff, you play sports, everything's trivial. It's trivially easy. So how hard can
vision be? Well, it turns out vision is a very, very difficult computational problem. And the
reason humans are so good at it is that about a quarter of your brain is solely or largely devoted to vision.
In humans, we think there are probably something on the order of 50 to 70 distinct visual areas.
There are a lot of brain areas devoted to vision that are simply involved with mapping
the incoming stimulus that lands on your eyeball into the motor commands you need to move the
muscles to, say, pick up an object near
you, right? If you think about it, if you look at your desk and there's a coffee cup, where the
coffee cup falls on your eye is completely irrelevant to you. What you care about is where
the coffee cup is relative to your hand and how you need to operate the pulleys in your muscles
of your arm and your hand to grab the coffee cup. So transforming from this sort of eye-centered coordinate system
to this arm-centered coordinate system is a very complicated problem
that's solved completely seamlessly in your brain.
So vision is a nice system because we know what it's trying to do.
It's trying to do vision, right?
If you think about looking at the prefrontal cortex of a human
where, you know, there are brain areas involved in abstract thought and moral reasoning and planning.
I mean, we have some vague idea of what they're trying to do, but it's very difficult to get your handle on that.
Vision is a very solid system and easy to understand.
And we share our visual system with a lot of other animals that have very similar visual systems. So as a consequence, scientists have learned an enormous amount
about how the visual system is organized in non-human animals
over the past 50 years.
And that data can be used to help us understand
the human neuroimaging data we're getting from this fairly new technology, MRI,
which has really only been around 20 years.
So the whole reason everyone uses fMRI today,
functional magnetic resonance imaging,
the whole reason people use it
is because it replicates the results in vision
that we know should be there from animal studies.
And that justifies using this MRI method
to study other things that are less well understood than vision.
Other things like language.
It turns out that language is a very interesting system for two reasons.
A, just like vision, language is hierarchically organized.
So when you hear speech, it comes into your cochlea in the form of a sound spectrogram,
which is just a frequency by time.
And then from that sound spectrogram, you extract phonemes and morphemes,
and you can extract syllables and words and syntax and semantics and narrative.
All of that information can be extracted from spoken narrative that you hear.
And that means since you can think about all of those levels of information they must be represented somewhere in the brain. So we
decided to take the tools that we had developed for vision and to apply them
to language. This led to a research project which led to a paper published
this year in Nature by Gallant and his co-authors Alexander Huth, Wendy Tahir, Thomas Griffiths,
and Frederick Tunison. It's called Natural Speech Reveals the Semantic Maps that Tile
Human Cerebral Cortex. So our stimuli came from the Moth Radio Hour, and this is essentially
stand-up storytelling, right? Professional, semi-professional storytellers get up in front
of the audience. They tell stories meant toers get up in front of the audience.
They tell stories meant to sort of excite and interest the audience.
But when I got close to about 40, I suddenly thought, oh, my God, this could be it.
This could be what I end up doing.
This could be on my tombstone, Tom Weiser, custom database application engineer.
And I thought, no, that can't be it.
That can't be all there is.
So I get a phone call from my mom, and she tells me that my father is about to get on an emergency life flight from our home in Montana to go to Denver to get an emergency liver transplant.
Suddenly, I was just thinking, does she need Mary Kate or does she need Jesus? It was
really, really idyllic. Snow and Vermont and all this other stuff. And Michael, we're out on this
little deck outside of it. And Michael's like, look at that buck. Look at that buck. Get the
shotgun, Ethan. Get the shotgun. And I'm like, what for? Two and a half weeks later, a black funeral wreath
was delivered to me at my office with a note that said, in memory of our son. These are largely
autobiographical stories about love and loss and redemption. They're great stories. Did you use
them because they're great, or did you just use them because they were stories? We use them because
they're compelling. They're interesting stories.
You pay attention to them.
You want to know the resolution to them.
They're very powerful stories.
So previous people had used these stories.
Mainly Uri Hassan at Princeton had started using these stories.
And he found that they elicited a large amount of brain activity
because people are paying close attention to the stories.
One of the problems you have in MRI experiments
is oftentimes they're very boring.
If you put somebody in an MRI scanner,
which is a very uncomfortable place to be,
and then you flash a word at them every five seconds for an hour,
they get bored out of their skull.
But when I got close to about 40...
Suddenly I was just thinking,
this could be me. Just see me in the black.
These stories are very interesting.
You just lie in the magnet.
You listen to these people telling these stories.
You get lost in the stories.
It's the best MRI experiment ever.
And in fact, this is the only MRI experiment we've ever done where we didn't have to pay people to be in the study.
They were just happy to lie there and listen to the stories.
And that means you get a lot of signal, and in a regime like fMRI where we're signal limited, getting more signal is always better. It means we're
going to have more information to model the brain.
Okay. And the information that you gleaned from the study in order to model the brain,
how fruitful is that really for you?
Oh, it's, the data we got from this experiment is really quite
remarkable. I'm now at the point where in order to explain the results, I have to explain the method.
Let me tell you how we analyze the data because that's important. So people are lying in the
magnet. They listen to a couple hours of stories. We measure brain activity. We're measuring changes
in blood flow and blood oxygen at 50,000 or so different locations across the cerebral cortex
while they listen to these stories.
And the essential problem is to figure out
for each location in the brain that we measured
what information in the stories is driving activity
at that location in the brain.
Gallant and his colleagues divided the stories
into two linguistic categories,
syntax, or the grammatical
structure, and semantics, or the story's meaning. So now we can probe each of the locations we
measure in the brain to find out if it responds to different kinds of syntax, or if it responds
to different kinds of semantics, or both. So this is a data-driven approach in which each location in the brain will tell us
in this procedure which specific kinds of features it prefers. And when you play this game,
you find out that exactly as you'd expect, these very simple features like spectral features and
phonemes are represented in primary auditory cortex, which is the first location in the cortex
where auditory information comes from the ears. But they also found something they weren't
expecting. This higher level semantic information, the meaning of the stories,
isn't really represented in primary auditory cortex at all. It's represented further downstream
in a large constellation of brain areas that represent different aspects of meaning.
And that's actually the most interesting thing about this study is the representation of semantics.
We have information about the representation of all these different feature spaces,
but the one very surprising thing from this study is that semantic information,
the meaning of the stories, is represented broadly across much of the brain. All of those
various areas of the brain
represent different aspects of semantic information
in these really complicated maps
that are very, very rich,
but fairly consistent across different individuals.
Okay, here's my lay interpretation of what you're saying.
Extremely lay, super lay interpretation would be
podcasts or radio make your brain hum with mystery and delight.
That's how I interpreted what you said.
They make your brain hum.
Whether that humming is mysterious and delightful kind of depends on whether you wanted your brain to hum or not.
If you're trying to sleep, that might not be so good. But what I really want to know is how anomalous or how typical is this, I don't know, cross-network
or broad humming in the brain? Well, I have several things to say about this. First of all,
it is traditionally thought the lore in the language sort of world is that language is
very left-lateralized and very localized and not largely
distributed. And that is true for production. The key sort of brain nodes you need to produce speech
at the sort of the after semantics, the actual translating meaning into speech, those are left
lateralized and those are bottlenecks and damage there will cause severe problems with speech
production. But remember, we're not doing speech production here.
We're doing speech comprehension.
And the brain areas involved in comprehension,
comprehending the meaning of language, are very, very broadly distributed.
I think more broadly than anyone had expected.
So yes, when you're listening to someone tell an interesting story,
an enormous swath of your brain is being activated.
For example, imagine I tell you a story about a dog.
Well, okay, you know a lot of things about a dog.
All of this different information, both the information in the stories
and the information that is primed by the stories,
the sort of memories that are dredged up by a story,
are represented in a constellation of many, many different brain areas. Auditory information tends to be represented in sort of certain locations in the brain and not
others. Olfactory information is represented in certain locations in the brain and not others.
Mathematical operations tend to occur in certain parts of the brain and not others. And if you're
listening to a story that involves, you know, a dog barking and a dog smelling bad and a pack of dogs, well,
that a certain number of dogs, like four dogs, then that would activate different brain areas
associated with all these different aspects of the stories. I'm guessing you're not going to
want to give me any advice as to how to make this podcast stimulate enormous swaths of the brain,
but I would be an idiot to have you on the line and not ask.
So, you know, are there words or ideas I should embrace? Should I favor dogs over cats? It sounds
like you're very pro-dog. Should I, for instance, stop using, you know, long words like externalities
and heterogeneity? Do you have any advice for me, Jack? Well, the underlying subtext of your question is that evoking large amounts of brain activity
is good. And I have no idea if that's true, right? So let's just start there, right? If you ask me,
how can I evoke lots of brain activity? I can answer your question. If you ask me,
should you? I have no idea, right?
You really don't have any idea or all right, okay, I'll take what you got. How can I?
Exactly.
If you choose to create a story that elicits as much brain activity as possible,
actually, you already know how to do this.
All journalists know how to do this.
There's an old trope in journalism, if it bleeds, it leads.
Because journalists all know that human interest stories,
especially involving something nasty like a violent thing,
attract people's interest. And one of the facts that we know about the brain from the last sort of 10 years or so of MRI
is that not only are there these rich representations of brain activity,
but that these representations are modulated and actually transformed by what you attend to.
So social information is represented at many, many different locations in the brain,
and people attend to bad social things that happen. So if you wanted to evoke a lot of
brain activity, you'd put a murder on the front page. And that would attract everyone's attention,
and it would evoke activity in all of the socially related parts of the brain,
and you would have your solution. Okay, so here's what I've learned
from talking to Jack Gallant.
In order to ensure the ongoing success of this podcast,
I should probably murder someone live on the air.
But I'm not willing to do that.
Maybe that's surprising to you, but I am not.
So plan B?
Plan B is much better.
It's simple.
It's less bloody. Not even illegal.
All it takes is for you to make a donation to WNYC to help them keep producing Freakonomics Radio.
Even though this show attracts great advertisers to whom we're very grateful, The public radio business model also relies heavily
on listener donations. So please go to Freakonomics.com, click the donate button,
or even easier, you can text the word FREAK to the number 69866, and they will send you a link
to donate and to claim your Freakonomics Radio swag. We've got t-shirts, mugs, and our brand new Freakonomics Radio logo Titleist golf balls.
All of which are only available to donors.
So, please do this, would you?
Either at Freakonomics.com or by text.
Again, the number is 69866.
Then you text the word FREAK.
Now, if I were you, I'd be thinking, how much money should I donate for a product that I
habitually get for free? That is a great question. It's a question that a Freakonomics Radio listener
named Gil Adler asked himself. I decided to apply the economic theory that I've learned
in Freakonomics and apply it to how much I should donate.
So I started with a theory that I would pay $1 per episode.
And if I really enjoyed the episode, then I would donate extra.
And if the episode was below the standards that I had come to expect
from Mr. Dubner and Mr. Levitt, then I would subtract from that dollar.
There was maybe an episode or two
where I was so disappointed
and so felt taken advantage of for my time
where I felt as if I should have been paid
to listen to those two episodes.
So what number did Adler finally land on?
It comes out to about $238, I believe,
for all the episodes that were produced
at the time I had submitted my worksheet.
Personally, I think Gil is being a bit generous.
I'm sure there are many more lousy episodes than he's saying,
but we do try hard around here,
and we do appreciate the generosity of Gil Adler
and the many, many thousands of others who support WNYC.
I sure hope that you are one of them.
So, remember, go to Freakonomics.com and click the donate button or text the word FREAK to 69866.
And thanks.
Coming up on Freakonomics Radio, we hear from a bunch of Freakonomics Radio listeners who
have used this show to their advantage. Honey, what's the ROI of buying a new mattress? I was
having trouble with deep, complex thinking, and this is where your podcast comes in. You know,
I listened to one probably halfway through, and then I stopped. Uh-oh. Guess we need to do better.
Anyway, that's coming up right after the break.
Welcome back to our special fundraising episode.
We already heard from the Berkeley cognitive neuroscientist Jack Gallant about how podcasts affect the brain.
We also asked Freakonomics Radio listeners how our podcast specifically has affected their brain or other body parts.
Hi, Freakonomics Radio. My name is Davina. I'm from Grenada, but I live in Austin, Texas. I was actually really inspired by your Hidden Side of Online Dating episode, and I started my own online dating account. And actually, a few days later, I met Philip, who is on track to become my first and most serious relationship. So it's crazy for me to think that a podcast may have led to me finding real love.
That was Davina Bruno. Here's another listener named David Bell.
It was probably less than a year ago, and I would just blissfully fall asleep wondering why the sky was blue or what that coworker meant in that last confusing email. Now when I lie in bed, I'm wide awake because
of Freakonomics and I'm thinking about something, trying to engage my wife in conversation like,
what are the optimal names for our future children in order to maximize their earning potential?
Or, honey, what's the ROI of buying a new mattress? And here is Kelsey Warren.
I was in a car accident in March of 2013.
I broke my back, my neck, my femur, punctured a lung, and most dramatically, suffered a trauma called brain shear.
My brain injury had knocked me back in time.
Basically, I thought I was a kid, and I acted like one.
When I'd progressed closer to normal, I'm 26 years old, I hit a wall.
I was having trouble with deep, complex thinking.
And this is where your podcast comes in.
I started listening to Freakonomics and after a few months, I developed into a more inquisitive, culturally aware, and thoughtful person. I can talk about anything now with sincere
curiosity and patience, very much interested in the hows and whys that I was oblivious to just a
year ago. So, thank you. You're welcome, Kelsey, and I hope your healing continues. There's one
more Freakonomics Radio listener I'd like you to hear from, a pretty new listener. Hey, Anders, it's Stephen Dubner. How's it going?
Not too bad. How about yourself?
Good. Nice to meet you.
Nice meeting you, too.
Anders Weiss is a 23-year-old athlete. His sport is rowing.
When you first started rowing, what were your aspirations?
I wanted to go to Youth Nationals and then get recruited to college.
You know, getting recruited for rowing, you know, it simplifies the process quite a bit,
but I wanted to do well at youth nationals.
In my senior year, I placed third,
so I considered that a success.
So I wasn't the best when I was growing up,
but I wasn't the worst.
Weiss got recruited to row at Brown University in Providence,
and he qualified for the international under-23 competition,
the U23s.
I think the moment I decided I wanted to go to the Olympics and knew I could go for it
was at U23s when one of the coaches said, you know, I expect to see a lot of you at 2020.
And I said to myself, oh, that's way too far away. That's not going to happen. I'm going for 2016.
You know, you're a young kid. You haven't really faced a lot of defeat. And you're just,
I was arrogant at that time. I was like, I'm going to do it.
And this was how long before the actual 2016 Olympics?
This was in 2013.
So you were 20 years old or so.
Yeah.
Right. And you're thinking, no.
Yeah, I'm going to get it this time.
That's so far away. No way.
After he graduated from Brown, Weiss did get an invitation to go to Princeton to work out with the U.S. rowing squad and try to qualify for the 2016 Olympics on either the eight-man boat or the four-man boat.
He knew his chances weren't terrible.
I would say like 25 percent because, you know, going from collegiate to the senior team, it's a whole different ballgame.
And I was never really exposed to that level of speed. You just can't get exposed to that level of speed
before you actually immerse yourself in it. These are guys are best in the world. Some have been
training out after college for four plus years. A lot of them have Olympic medals. Three of them
had Olympic medals. So it's a, it was a very, very strong crew. And so being the young guy who had
good success at Brown, I was like,
all right, I'm just going to transfer that here. That's not always the case.
And let me ask you, what is the average or what is the median age at which rowers peak,
would you say?
I would say around 28, 27.
So you are hoping to qualify for the four of the eight, you're one of 26 people with 12 slots, right?
Talk about decision time, when the team actually gets chosen, and walk me through that.
I remember the coach has sent the boats out on the water, and he's like, all right, you three guys or you four guys, you're going to go erg, which is the indoor rowing machine.
I was like, oh, no.
He's like, oh, I need to speak to you after practice.
And I was like, oh, God. You never want to hear that.
And he said, you know, basically, you're not going to make the eight or four.
I think you have a lot of talent for 2020, but right now you're out of contention for the eight and the four.
And I was like, oh, God.
How did you respond internally and externally?
I was angry.
I was very angry.
But, you know, you can't blame the coaches.
That's, you know, of course they're trying to make the fastest boat. And if they don't see you being the fastest person, then, you know, that's the way life goes.
Now Weiss was facing a long and sad drive home to Rhode Island from the Olympic training camp in Princeton. I was so upset. I said to myself, I can't spend four and a half hours
listening to Bieber or Taylor Swift or something like that.
I needed something to take my mind off Rowan.
Had you ever listened to a podcast?
You know, I listened to one probably halfway through,
and then I stopped.
And then what made you end up listening to a Freakonomics Radio show
on that very happy trip back to Rhode Island?
I opened up my phone.
I went to podcast to look at the ones to download.
And, you know, Freakonomics, one of my friends from college listened to you guys.
And I looked at the titles you had.
And I was like, all right, this one sounds interesting.
That one sounds interesting.
Let's load them up.
What was the name of the episode?
Do you know?
It was How to Be More Productive.
Gotcha. And why did you pick that title?
Because everyone wants to be more productive.
Good answer. Yeah.
So I was like, all right, here we go.
I'm always trying to learn something new, and this seems like a good podcast to listen to to do that.
We released that How to Be More Productive episode in April as part of what we called Self-Improvement Month.
It featured an interview with the writer Charles Duhigg.
It was a very small segment of the podcast.
I think it was like five minutes where it talks about Marine Army Rangers, I believe, and how to get leaders out of them.
They didn't say you're a natural leader or something like that.
You said you were hardworking and your success is built off hard work and not talent or not how a natural leader you are. So this drill sergeant told me
that he never tells someone who's a natural athlete that they just ran a good race. He only
tells like the small kind of wimpy kids that they just did a great job running. The core as a whole
never tells anyone that there's such a thing as natural born leaders. Because that implies that you don't have any control over whether you're a leader or not.
Instead, what they do is they compliment shy people who take a leadership role.
And they say to them, look, I know it was hard for you to do that, but you did a great job.
And growing up, I was always decently athletic and I always had pretty good success in athletics.
And the same was true in high school and college.
And so I put in the work.
You know, you can't not put in the work, especially at Brown.
So it was like, okay, I put in the work.
But at the end of the day, all my success is going to be attributed to how good my body is for rowing.
And after that podcast, I sort of had to do a, you know, a 180 on that. It was more of a, okay, my talent's
definitely helping me, but my total success is going to be determined by how much work I put in.
And there was also another podcast there. I think it was how to be great at anything, how much
dedicated practice I was putting in. Yeah. It sounds like you were a hard worker,
but if I'm reading you correctly, it sounds like you're saying that even though you worked hard,
A, you could work harder and B, you could work kind of more strategically or engage in what we call deliberate practice.
So was that kind of the light bulb that went off for you, which is, oh, yeah, it's not like I'm lazy, but I can get a lot more out of me than I have been in the past?
Yeah. So rowing is basically steady state.
That's how we train.
It's a lot of steady state.
And, you know, if you sort of let your mind wander during all those hours, instead of,
okay, this is what I need to do to fix my technique or keep you engaged. You can row the
hours and you can get the heart rate that you need to be, but you're not going to make the
technical changes that you need to. And it's not like I was zoning out, but it was like, okay, you know,
this technical change isn't really working that well. I'm going to just go back to steady state
press instead of, all right, I'm going to practice this over and over for the, you know, two hours
I'm on the water now. And then the two hours I'm on the water in the afternoon, I'm going to nail
it down no matter how long it takes. And so it was sort of a shift away from doing the steady state to do the steady state.
You know, get the heart rate to doing the steady state to improve.
To put in the hours that actually would improve my speed instead of relying on how well my body was made to row.
By the time he got home to Rhode Island, having failed to qualify for the 2016 Olympics in the four-man
boat or the eight-man boat, Anders Weiss was already thinking about one more option. He could
try to qualify for the two-man boat, the coxless pair. In that event, the coaches don't decide who
qualifies. It's only your time that matters. He got hold of a potential partner, a veteran U.S.
rower named Narek Geregian.
The partner I got, Narek, he is a very hard worker.
And so he drew up our training plan, and I was like, all right, let's do this.
This is what's going to make us win the trials.
And I think if it was before that, before the podcast, it would have been, okay, I'll do this work,
but I don't know if it's really going to make us faster.
I think it's really decided by our body build and how talented we are at rowing.
And so it was, you know, I attacked the workouts that we had with a little more purpose.
Maybe I would have pulled the same splits on the erg.
But the technical changes I made on the water with this new mindset stuck.
And we got so much faster as a result of that.
And it's sort of that mentality shift instead of, you know,
working hard to work hard, you know, you're working hard to, to win a race.
And did you tell Narek that you had a new approach?
Uh, no, I didn't want him making fun of me. Uh, he was one of the older guys. So,
and I was one of the younger guys. I was just like, all right, let's do this.
What would you imagine that he would say? Like, oh, you're finally discovering, Anders, that you actually have to work on your technique?
Yeah, pretty much that.
He would have made fun of me for a bit.
And I'm just curious, like, did you come to feel at some point in this process that, man, I really have just been relying on my genes and not really, you know, I don't know if trying is the right word.
But did you feel that you'd been kind of
failing to tap a lot of your potential? Yes. Yeah. I mean, I could even see that,
which is how much speed the pair that we were rowing got. You know, we were pretty quick to
start with, but we just, we really started improving our speed, our top-end speed, quite a bit throughout the months we had together.
Weiss and Goregian had to go to a series of time trials.
Finally, the moment of truth, the Olympic qualifying race,
with only one team going to Rio to represent the United States.
We got to the starting line, and, you know,
I think my heart rate there was like 160, 165 just before the start.
We had a very good start and we did what we did best and kept pushing.
There was a pair from a club in Pennsylvania from Philadelphia that was incredibly fast
and we knew they were going to be our main competition because they beat us in the time trial.
There were how many boats in the final?
There was four. And so we were feeling pretty confident,
but that other boat was always, how fast are they going to be?
And they beat us in the time trial, so anything could happen.
One of those things where it's do or die.
And so we got to the 1,000-meter mark.
We were a little bit ahead of them, and I wanted to win very badly.
But Nare know the the pair
partner that chose me he's been training for six years and so I said you know I gotta do it for him
and that was sort of that extra kick to keep going even when you know I couldn't really see that well
and and my body was just saying please stop please stop please stop but to be the best and
to eventually beat the best you have to go to those lengths. Can you just talk about, is this a commonly known danger among rowers?
Is that your body is doing such crazy stuff that you literally lose your sight temporarily?
You know, I wouldn't say it's very common, but it has happened before.
And it's one of those, you know, usually happens at the Olympics or leading up to the Olympics or world championships.
You have people that go crazy, that want to win and will do anything to win.
That's the lengths they need to go to to win.
That's the first time it ever happened to me, and it's not going to be my last.
So what did it actually look like from your perspective then?
The way I experienced it was sort of the peripheral started going,
and then there's one spot just took pretty much center stage, and that was all I could see.
And yeah, it was just, it wasn't sudden, but it was definitely noticeable once it was like, okay, I can't really see anything besides this one little dot on my partner's back.
How long did it last?
I think it was like 300, 400 meters to go, which is, I think, a little over a minute.
And then once we stopped, I probably had my vision back, I think, like 15 to 20 seconds
afterwards after I could just lay back in the boat and do nothing.
Were you worried for a moment that you had somehow lost your eyesight for real?
Or did you just know that you would be okay?
I knew, you know, at that point I was like, yeah, we're going to the Olympics.
Yeah, we did. I remember when I was a little kid, you know, watching the Olympics and being like,
oh, I want to go that. I want to, I want to be an athlete. I want to do that. And I hadn't picked
up rowing at that time. So I was looking at basketball, all those other sports.
And it was one of those things where it's just so cool.
You're going to be on the biggest stage in the world
doing what you probably do best
against people who are the best in the world.
And so when I crossed that line, I gave a little shout,
and then I sort of just passed out a little bit.
All right, so you're realizing now you're going to the Olympics,
you're going to Rio. Me, I'm listening to your story a little selfishly. I'm thinking,
if I understand your story correctly, because Freakonomics Radio played this role in reorienting
you and getting you to take up Coxless Pair, go on the two-man boat and eventually make the Olympics.
If I'm reading it correctly, I think our role in this means that essentially I'm also
an Olympian. Would you say that's fair? No, I would definitely agree with that.
I don't think I would have made the Olympics if I hadn't listened to that podcast. Let's just put
it that way. Anders Weiss and his partner, Narek Geregian, didn't medal in the Rio Olympics, but they did pretty well, making it to one of the final heats.
And you should definitely keep an eye out for Weiss in the 2020 Olympics.
Now, we can't promise this Olympian level of helped you pass the time in a slightly more meaningful way.
If so, do me a favor and make a donation to WNYC so that we can keep producing this show. Just go to Freakonomics.com and click the donate button
or text the word FREAK to the number 69866. And thanks.
Coming up next week on Freakonomics Radio, is innovation overrated? Should we be spending more
time and more money on maintenance? People always think more about how new ground can be broken
than they think about how existing institutions can be sustained
or existing facilities can be maintained.
In praise of maintenance.
That's next time on Freakonomics Radio.
Freakonomics Radio is produced by WNYC Studios and Dubner Productions. This episode was produced by Caitlin Pierce. Our staff also includes Jay Cowett, Merritt Jacob,
Christopher Wirth, Greg Rosalski, Noah Kernis, Alison Hockenberry, Emma Morgenstern, and Harry
Huggins. You can subscribe to this podcast on iTunes or wherever you get your podcasts.
And please come visit Freakonomics.com
where you'll find our entire podcast archive
along with a complete transcript
of every episode ever made.
Also, music credits and lots more.
Thanks for listening.