Freakonomics Radio - 354. How to Be Creative

Episode Date: October 18, 2018

There are thousands of books on the subject, but what do we actually know about creativity? In this new series, we talk to the researchers who study it as well as artists, inventors, and pathbreakers ...who live it every day: Ai Weiwei, James Dyson, Elvis Costello, Jennifer Egan, Rosanne Cash, Wynton Marsalis, Maira Kalman, and more. (Ep. 1 of the “How to Be Creative” series.)

Transcript
Discussion (0)
Starting point is 00:00:00 What do you think when I say the word creativity? Truth be told, creativity is having a bit of a moment. There are thousands of books on the subject, many written in the past decade. It's become a corporate buzzword, right up there with innovation and disruption. It is at the center of a whole lifestyle movement. Online classes and Facebook groups and real-life meetups. There are creativity coaches, of course, and gurus offering to rearrange your life to add more creative spice. So, we here at Freakonomics Radio got to thinking. With so many people spending so much time and money and energy in pursuit of this thing called creativity,
Starting point is 00:00:47 well, we wondered if there's anything systematic to be learned about it. What if we started by simply defining the term? We asked a bunch of academics who study creativity, as well as some artists, musicians, scientists, and inventors, how do you define creativity? You know, it's actually harder than one might think. Well, people use that word in lots of different ways to mean lots of different things. There's a huge amount of what goes under the head in creativity that just has to do with a willingness to stick to a problem and a pleasure in it. So starting with nothing, having an idea, letting the idea pull you forward, getting it down, making it right. Okay, bingo, this is how we're going to do it.
Starting point is 00:01:38 It can't just be different for the sake of being different because that's the definition of madness, I guess. Teresa Amabile is a psychologist and a professor emerita at the Harvard Business School. She spent her career studying creativity, particularly in education and work settings. But I asked her, how is it even possible to empirically study something as diffuse as creativity? Many people have the sense that it should not be studied scientifically, that you should not try to apply science and objective thinking to the magic of creativity, that it's somehow in the spiritual realm. As you might guess, I don't take that approach.
Starting point is 00:02:21 I think that it can be studied scientifically without destroying the excitement and the sense of magic about creativity. For economists, the concern hasn't been about preserving the magic of creativity. They generally avoid the topic because it seems so touchy-feely. There's sort of two questions. Why do economists care? And the answer is economists really haven't cared about creativity. David Galanson is an economist at the University of Chicago, and he does care.
Starting point is 00:02:50 His central research interest is the life cycles of human creativity. Why should they care? The single most important problem for the discipline of economics is economic growth. Why are some countries rich and others poor. And virtually all economists agree that the single most important source of economic growth in the long run, you know, what really makes a country rich is technological change. Well, technological change is just a lot of people making discoveries. So it would seem to be a very big question, how do people make innovations?
Starting point is 00:03:26 Today on Freakonomics Radio, we kick off a new occasional series on creativity. What it is, where it comes from, how it can be cultivated. Along the way, we'll answer some big questions. Is it really true that creativity and mental illness go hand in hand? Does our education system really kill off creativity? How do preposterously creative people do what they do? We'll hear from the academics who study creativity and the people who live it.
Starting point is 00:03:55 Musicians like Wynton Marsalis and Roseanne Cash and Elvis Costello, artists like Ai Weiwei and Myra Kalman, the novelist Jennifer Egan and the filmmakers Mark Duplass and Seth Gordon, the astrophysicists Margaret Geller and Saul Perlmutter, inventors like Jessica Matthews and James Dyson, and one of the people behind what may become a massive scientific revolution. As you're actually doing experiments, you realize, oh, we could do this. Oh, we could do this.
Starting point is 00:04:22 We'll get started right after this. From Stitcher and Dubner Productions, this is Freakonomics Radio, the podcast that explores the hidden side of everything. Here's your host, Stephen Dupner. Among the scholars who study creativity, there is no real consensus on who has it or even exactly what it is. I was just looking recently and reminded of that old fable that, you know, if you have people blindfolded looking at an elephant. Sharla Nemeth is a psychology professor at UC Berkeley who studies entrepreneurs and creative scientists. And one of them is, you know, looking at the tail, he thinks it's a snail he's touching. And if you have someone, you know, approaching it from the side, he thinks it's a wall.
Starting point is 00:05:33 And it's just a reminder in a way that what you focus on to some extent determines how you think creativity works. And so in many ways, it's a very difficult and still to some extent mysterious phenomenon. So if you get somebody studying it in business innovation, for example, separate from studying, you know, Nobel laureates or separate from looking at it in experimental tasks, for example, in a lab. So it's very hard to compare those because they use different definitions of the end product as being creative or not. But you've got to start somewhere. And that's why Teresa Amabile starts with a basic definition that's accepted within the field of psychology. It gets a little squidgy when we're talking about the arts, you know, what does appropriate or valuable mean in the arts. But I think even there, pure novelty just for the sake of novelty isn't really going to do it. It has to be somehow expressive of something that the artist was trying to convey or evocative of a response that the artist was trying to evoke. Novelty that works. I think that is a great starting point.
Starting point is 00:06:50 What if we run that definition past a creative type? Michael Barut, for instance. Barut is a world-renowned, much-awarded graphic designer. You've seen his logos. His work is in museums. How does he like defining creativity as novelty that works? Yeah, I mean, there's this famous formulation by the mid-century, 20th century designer Raymond Lowy. He designed the Lucky Strike Package and the livery of Air Force One, among other things. And he used, he had this like four-letter formulation, M-A-Y-A, most advanced yet acceptable.
Starting point is 00:07:29 And it was based on his theory that everyone has these two impulses. And one is the desire for regularity and comfort. And the other one is the quest for surprise and novelty, right? And so if you have too much regularity and comfort, you? And so if you have too much regular and comfort, you get bored. If you get too much surprise and novelty, you get overexcited, wired, and distracted and exhausted. And so it's the idea that it's novelty with a purpose. That purpose is the element that actually is about, you know, expectation and expectations fulfilled. And the novelty part of it is the idea that those expectations might be fulfilled in a way that you haven't seen before. It turns out that psychologists have come up with a way to test our ability to produce novelty that works.
Starting point is 00:08:25 Sharla Nemeth again. It's called the uses test. And basically, if I ask you to give me all the uses you can for a brick, you could say, you know, you could build a house, you could build a factory, you know, you could build a road. And there'd be three ideas, but they're all basically building. A more creative answer would be something like you could build a house, you could use it as a platform to hold a cup of coffee, you could use it as a missile to throw through someone's window as protest. And other tests of creativity all have that element to it.
Starting point is 00:08:55 They see if the mind tends to go wide and down different routes, and that it can even make connections through circuitous ways of getting there. That's one way to measure someone's degree of creative thinking. But how about studying creativity the other way around, starting with people that everyone agrees are wildly creative? That's the route chosen by Dean Simonton. He is a professor emeritus of psychology at UC Davis. I wanted to study creativity and genius and leadership, but I wanted to do it in a way that was different than what most psychologists did, because most
Starting point is 00:09:33 psychologists, they study really college undergraduates who happen to be taking a Psych 1 class, and they have to volunteer to participate. And those are not the people I wanted to study. And so I had to figure out a way of studying people like Michelangelo or Beethoven or Einstein when I couldn't get them to come to my laboratory, particularly since most of them were deceased, which made it kind of awkward. So I started developing various ways of studying genius at a distance,
Starting point is 00:10:13 measuring their personality, measuring their intelligence, looking at their childhood and adolescence, the nature of their career. So did you have a pretty smooth path through academia then to become a PhD? a set of college students, I had a hell of a time trying to put together a thesis committee because not everybody thought that that was even a legitimate form of doing psychological research. Because why? Because at that time, and it's still the case, in fact, in many ways, it's even more true. The laboratory is considered to be the acme of science. You know, having a lab in which you collect your data. And in the case of psychology, a lab means a place where,
Starting point is 00:11:16 for the most part, you bring in college students to study. Something about 86% of all research in psychology is based on college undergraduates. That's a very, very narrow cohort, not just age-wise, but also kind of background-wise and IQ-wise and all that. Right. That's pretty homogenous. What about just the fact that even good lab experiments, forget about all the bad ones, that it doesn't really represent the real world well enough, especially in the realm
Starting point is 00:11:42 of something like psychology, but also economics, I would say. Was that a concern for you as well, or no? Oh, yeah, it was definitely. To me, there's a distinction that is made between what's called internal validity and external validity. And internal validity has to do with the power of making causal inferences from your research. And laboratory experiments are really, really great in internal validity. But external validity is, does it really tell you anything about what's happening in the real world? And so what often happens when you want to study something in the lab that is actually in the real world, you do a simulation, but you have no way of knowing whether or not that
Starting point is 00:12:22 simulation actually represents what happens out there. For example, I wanted to find out what were the sociocultural circumstances that were responsible for why in some periods you have golden ages with lots of geniuses and other times you have dark ages where it's really hard to find anybody who even knows what they're talking about, right? Yeah, how do you do that in a lab? Right, how do you do that? I wanted to look at the impact of war.
Starting point is 00:12:49 I wanted to look at the impact of having role models in your field when you're growing up. I wanted to look at the impact of the political system, whether you had a lot of independent states or whether it was a big one unified empire. And those are things you just can't study in the laboratory. You can't say, hey, imagine you're growing up in the Middle Ages, and how creative do you feel? So how did Simonton go about studying this incredible range of factors? Well, actually, the story goes back to elementary school, believe it or not. When I was in kindergarten,
Starting point is 00:13:28 my kindergarten teacher came to our house and told my parents, I came from a working class background. In fact, my dad didn't graduate from high school. And they said, if you want your son to do well in school, you need a set of encyclopedias, world book encyclopedias. And it would really help him. And these books are designed from K through 12 and very useful writing term papers and all that kind of stuff. Those were expensive. That was a big commitment. Yeah, that was expensive.
Starting point is 00:14:00 You know, it was an amazing purchase for them to make. And I started browsing through them. And one thing that's characteristic of the Royal Book is they have lots of pictures. And I saw lots of pictures of strange-looking people, people with beards, people with long hair, people, different ethnicities, periods, whatever. And I wondered, how did they get in that? How did they get there, an entry?
Starting point is 00:14:26 Why were they so important? And particularly since I realized my parents didn't have entries and my elementary school teacher didn't have entries, Mrs. Rainer. So I was always curious about just how you become eminent enough to end up in an encyclopedia or just have someone write a biography of you. Was it, I don't want to, envy is not the word I'm looking for. Did you want to belong to that tribe and you were trying to figure out how to get there
Starting point is 00:14:55 or you were more curious about who were these people and where did they come from? Well, it was kind of a combination. I wouldn't say it was envy, but it was curiosity about how they got there and could I be in that group, you know? Do I have what it takes? And what do you have to do to get in there?
Starting point is 00:15:15 And what, yeah, what do you have to do? Yeah, what's the process? And is it something that you can develop as well? Simonton learned to pay attention to all the details in the biographies he was reading. And it would talk about, you know, where they came from. Sometimes it would mention their birth order. Sometimes it would, you know, talk about their education and maybe some of the struggles they went through and then their career.
Starting point is 00:15:41 And then, of course, histories also have a lot of information about what's going on externally, you know, like whether or not there's any wars going on, whether or not there's a dictatorship or a democratic government. There's also information having to do with personality, like this person was introverted or this person was extroverted. We know, for example, that Newton had a lot of characteristics that suggest that he was a high-functioning autistic, and he was very, very introverted. And we can see that from his behavior, as well as having some paranoid psychosis attached. All right. So, let me ask you the really obvious question. When you study people like Michelangelo and Beethoven and Einstein, who I think most people would agree were pretty good at what they did and maybe all the way up to creative genius.
Starting point is 00:16:34 Obvious question. What did you discover about creativity and genius? I guess, what do they have in common? Well, first of all, I have to say that artists and scientists are not equivalent as geniuses. But they also have things that they do share. They are all very intelligent in a general intelligence way, not necessarily in terms of taking an IQ test. But they're very sharp. They love what they're doing. They're sharp. They love what they're doing. They're absolutely committed to doing what they're doing. They want to spend their whole life, you know, discovering the nature of the universe
Starting point is 00:17:14 or creating incredible paintings, you know, on the ceiling or whatever it happens to be. And they're willing to overcome all sorts of obstacles and all sorts of struggles, you know, like Michelangelo, the agony and the ecstasy, you know, scenario. Even though he was recognized very early as a genius, he had constant struggles, and he often had major projects terminated,
Starting point is 00:17:42 like with Pope Julius II. And it's not easy to be a genius. And so all of them, whether they're scientists or whether they're artists, have that tremendous drive and commitment and determination to keep on going, even when they're failing and failing and failing. At this point, you may be starting to think, well, what Simonton's describing sounds an awful lot like what's known as the 10,000-hour rule of excellence.
Starting point is 00:18:13 In our research, we call it the 10-year rule. But in any case, that is absolutely unquestionable that you have to establish an expertise and a domain. You have to know what you're doing. You have to have the tools of the trade. Now, sometimes people enter fields where there's really not that much to learn because they're brand new fields.
Starting point is 00:18:36 So when Galileo, for example, invented his telescope and pointed to the skies and saw all these things that were not supposed to be there. There weren't supposed to be mountains on the moon. There weren't supposed to be moons circulating around Jupiter. There weren't supposed to be spots on the sun, and so forth and so on. He was creating a domain from scratch.
Starting point is 00:18:59 So he didn't have to spend 10 years learning something that was already obsolete. He was inventing his own field. So being first is always a good idea. Yeah, if you want to avoid all the hard work of studying, just be the first in a given domain. Coming up after the break, what is the most creative thing you can think of that has nothing to do with the arts? And an important question for everyone, especially if you don't consider yourself creative. Should we all strive to be creative? That's coming up. For more Freakonomics
Starting point is 00:19:31 Radio, check out Freakonomics.com or subscribe to this podcast on your favorite podcast app. And if you have a great idea for a future episode, send it along to radio at Freakonomics.com. We'll be right back. This is the first episode in a new recurring series about creativity, which, for empirically-minded people like us, is a notoriously squishy topic. The psychologist Teresa Amabile again. So, there are a few myths about creativity
Starting point is 00:20:15 that are very popular. So, let's at least clear up a few myths, shall we? First off, when most of us think about creativity, we focus on the arts, music and film, visual arts, writing, and so on. Maybe you also think of a scientist or researcher in the lab thinking up experiments to test a new hypothesis. It's all understandable since these are people who make a living through their creativity, but Amabile thinks that view of creativity doesn't go nearly far enough. Creativity is possible in all realms of human activity.
Starting point is 00:20:48 If we define creativity as doing something novel that works, that's valuable in some way, it's absolutely possible in everything that humans do. One of my favorite examples of creative thinking outside the arts is the story of John Snow, the English doctor who's considered one of the pioneers of epidemiology. In mid-19th century London, Snow was trying to identify the source of cholera outbreaks. Many doctors thought it was spread by miasma or bad air. Snow thought it maybe had to do with germs in the water supply. Using maps and statistics and common sense, Snow identified one pump whose contaminated water had caused nearly 200 deaths in a single outbreak of cholera. Over the ensuing centuries, Snow's breakthrough would help save countless lives, and that strikes me as a deeply creative act.
Starting point is 00:21:43 Yeah, I think that's right. That's an example of profound creativity that isn't, quote, artistic or artistic with a capital A. That's a filmmaker, Seth Gordon. He grew up with two social scientists as parents. Now he directs big Hollywood films and also makes lots of TV shows and smart documentaries. I think actually the most creative discipline I've ever witnessed in person is probably coding because you're creating the language that you then employ to make something happen. That is profoundly creative. And whether that's for making a game or whether that's creating a program or whether that's, I mean, in a way, maybe the most creative thing I'm aware of in the last 10 or 15 years is the Stuxnet virus. He's referring to the computer virus generally thought to be created by American and Israeli programmers, which in 2010 attacked Iran's nuclear program. Like that thing, it's unbelievable. Like what it was, what it did and how it accomplished it,
Starting point is 00:22:53 like, but I don't think anyone would call that creative in any normal sense. That's, you know, it's bad, right? But it's making something that never existed before and tricked everyone for a very long time. Like, it's amazing. Creating computer code that can shut down your enemy's nuclear centrifuges. Yes, I can see how that's creative. Some people see a lot of creativity in sports. Here's Dana Gioia, the Poet Laureate of California and former chairman of the National Endowment for the Arts. I don't like sports, but you've got to admire the energy, creativity, innovation that goes into sports.
Starting point is 00:23:35 And it's very similar to arts. It's a way of focusing human energy to create these symbolic encounters which have enormous emotional resonance, you know, to audiences. So I think it's a mistake always to talk about the arts about being about artists. The arts are also about audience. It's about community. It's about this conversation between the creator and the receiver. I mean, I think one of the things people don't understand is that every day people are creative. That's the astrophysicist Margaret Geller, who in her everyday work helped pioneer the mapping of matter in the universe.
Starting point is 00:24:13 You go in the kitchen to make dinner, you put a different spice in the dish. That's creative. You're planting your garden. You make a new flower arrangement. It doesn't have to be a big thing to be creative. Maybe not, but some practitioners of the creative arts, the humorous John Hodgman, for instance, do draw a line. You know, I would make a small distinction. It's sort of the distinction between being a stand-up comic versus being an actor.
Starting point is 00:24:43 You know, when you are cooking, you're following a recipe. When you're acting, you're following a script and direction. Those are arts, to be sure, but they're interpretive arts rather than purely creative arts where you are solely or in collaboration responsible for creating something out of nothing. You know, developing a recipe or inventing a new kind of Afghan pattern or whatever it is. If you're following a direction, right, that is an interpretive art of which many people become craftspeople and masters of, and it's a beautiful thing. You know, it's just enjoyable in and of itself. It may be enjoyable, but as Hodgman sees it, true creativity occupies a higher realm. Teresa Amabile takes a more inclusive approach.
Starting point is 00:25:31 One of my favorite things to do when I'm talking to managers is say, okay, everyone, where do you most want creativity in your organizations? They yell out R&D, marketing, advertising. Oh, great, great, great, great. Okay, so is there anywhere in your organization where you do not want creativity? Somebody will yell out accounting and then everybody laughs.
Starting point is 00:25:57 And of course people say, well, you know, you think of creative accounting and you think of Enron, you know, you think of all these examples of cooking the books. Yeah, you think of all these examples of cooking the books. Yeah, yeah, right. That's funny. And yes, we want to avoid that kind of creative accounting. And then I ask them, is it true that you don't want people in your accounting department to think about what they do creatively ever? And then I give the example of my colleague Robert Kaplan coming up
Starting point is 00:26:26 with activity-based costing many years ago and how that was a true creative breakthrough in accounting. But in a time when everyone is encouraged to be creative, are there downsides to that? Especially if there's not enough attention to go around? Can I give you one example? Dean Simonton again at UC Davis. It just happened recently, and it happened near where I live, so it's even more prominent. We had someone who, you know,
Starting point is 00:26:55 walked into the YouTube headquarters and started shooting people up because she thought that they were stifling her creativity. That she had some creativity, you know, via video uploads that was not reaching the largest possible audience. I have never seen any of her videos and you have to know Farsi to understand, I think, most of them. But the point is, is that she thought it was worth killing people because her creativity is being stifled. I think she would have been better off
Starting point is 00:27:31 just becoming an accountant or something rather than trying to be creative. And I think there's the other issue here. We live in a society where we're inundated with creativity. It's so easy to put yourself up there. You can start your own blog. You can upload your paintings.
Starting point is 00:27:51 You can upload your music. You can do all that. In earlier times, you had gatekeepers. You actually had to have a patron who was willing to pay for the marble for your sculpture. So you sound like you're, for the most part, not in favor of this total democratization of creativity. I don't really care. But, I mean, if someone is happy being a Sunday afternoon painter, by all means, do it.
Starting point is 00:28:24 Just don't give it to your relatives and force them to find some spot in the living room to hang it up. You know, when I hear that phrase, creative genius, and we do hear it a lot, it goes together. I sometimes wonder if it's a little bit of an unfortunate pairing in that people who are not geniuses don't feel they have the permission, therefore, to be creative. Well, creativity and genius are separate topics. And there are many examples of geniuses who were not particularly creative. And of course, on the other side, there's a lot of creative people out there, a lot of creativity. There are people who are just phenomenally witty in conversations
Starting point is 00:29:20 at parties. There are people who can put together an amazing recipe from just random scraps of stuff available in the pantry. And that all counts as creativity. Right. If you personally had to pick that you could only be good at one, would you be very creative or a genius? I would probably pick being creative because I think being creative is much more fun than just being a mere genius. You know, what do you do as a mere genius? You kind of wallow in your geniushood? That's not to say the genius doesn't exist
Starting point is 00:30:03 or that certified geniuses haven't come up with some pretty amazing things. Well, I do think that there's certain geniuses that truly can make mental leaps the rest of us can't. That's Walter Isaacson, who teaches history at Tulane and has written several best-selling biographies of big, big thinkers. That could be the way Einstein understood after a while that time is relative depending on your state of motion. This is something no physicist had ever even really thought of. But likewise, you can have a genius just in ordinary things when Steve Jobs figured out the iPod and how to have it be a simple, sleek, personal product
Starting point is 00:30:46 that would put a thousand songs in your pocket in its own small way. That was a leap of genius as well. On the other hand, there are some people widely considered to be creative geniuses. Hello, I'm Elvis Costello, and you're listening to Freakonomics Radio. A songwriter, for instance, whose melodies and lyrics are practically beyond belief. That's a pretty good compliment. I think everyone who knows your music, including me, would consider you an extraordinarily creative person. You're the kind of person that people use the phrase creative genius on.
Starting point is 00:31:32 That's really crazy. I think of myself, I don't really think in terms of definition like a name tag, but if you actually ask me, I say, I was a worker of a kind. I work at what I do. And then there might be moments of inspiration that visit you unexpectedly. Can you give an example? Well, any song arriving is a mysterious sort of thing.
Starting point is 00:32:01 I mean, it can range from carrying around a phrase in a notebook for four years before it joins up with some other thoughts or you know a line of melody that seems to bring it to life. And allows you to kind of you know to represent something that you want to share with people or a song can just appear, the whole thing, the words and music, time stops. Really? That's happening? Oh, yeah. I'll leave it up to you to decide whether Elvis Costello belongs on the creative genius list.
Starting point is 00:32:42 In any case, there's one characteristic that we almost universally associate with such people, that they're tortured. Either they burn so bright that they flame out early and die young, or they spend long lives wracked by schizophrenia or depression or at least garden variety neurosis. Maybe the most famous, most defining example of this is Vincent van Gogh, with his nervous breakdowns and self-mutilation and eventual suicide. Dean Simonton has looked at the incidence of mental illness
Starting point is 00:33:19 in creative types through the ages. And there is a correlation between creativity and mental illness, but it depends on the kind of creativity you're talking about. There's a relationship between how much constraint the creative genius has to operate under
Starting point is 00:33:36 and their tendency towards mental illness. A scientist operates under a lot of constraints. The scientist has to come up with theories that are consistent with the facts. It has to be logically coherent. It has to fit in with what previous scientists have been doing and so forth. And in our culture, artists don't operate that way, particularly since the Romantic period. Anything goes. But there are times and places where the arts have extremely high constraints imposed on them. Japanese haiku, for example, is a very, very constrained form. You have a certain number of syllables to work with. You
Starting point is 00:34:21 also have a certain number of themes that are considered to be more appropriate for haiku. So what's interesting is that as you get into domains that are very, very constrained, mental illness tends to be very rare. And then if you go into more and more unconstrained forms of expression, then you also do it at risk of having more mental illness, as well as having all sorts of horrible experiences in childhood or adolescence. And there's a study published on this where you can compare Nobel Prizes in physics with Nobel Prizes in literature, and they're not cut from the same cloth at all. Yeah, I think if you look at American winners in literature,
Starting point is 00:35:08 most of them are alcoholics, right? Yeah, alcoholics. They often dropped out of school. They had tremendous ups and downs in their education, if they even finished formal education. Whereas the physicists came from perfect family backgrounds, professional families. Nothing happened.
Starting point is 00:35:32 Nobody died. Excellence in physics is building within a domain that must be mastered first, and that requires a certain set of resources and skills and ability to color within the lines, yes? Right. And you're ability to color within the lines, yes? Right. And you're expected to stay within the box because the box actually defines what is science.
Starting point is 00:35:53 So interesting. I mean, I published an article in Nature a few years ago, but they wrote their own title, and they made it deliberately provocative. They said, after Einstein, genius is extinct. Whoa. And immediately I got inundated with all sorts of mail, emails mostly, from people, some who agreed with me. And did you take credit for writing the headline in those cases where they agree. Yeah, right. Exactly. But others wrote and said, what about me? And they said, I'm a genius and I'm an advocate for Einstein. In fact, I've actually disproven everything that Einstein got credit for, and I'm still waiting to get my
Starting point is 00:36:38 Nobel Prize. And they'll publish this stuff on the web, usually their own personal website, and you look at it, and it violates all the constraints of science. I mean, a basic thing is you have to obey high school algebra, for example. I know that seems really obsessive, but you have to obey high school algebra. Well, I mean, we're laughing about it, but on the other hand, in language, let's say, right, you can break language. You can go way outside the bounds of formal English or any other language, and it can be considered poetry.
Starting point is 00:37:13 But I see that science is a different ballpark. Right. And in the case of poetry, it's actually kind of astonishing how far you can push the edge. You know, like Ezra Pound, for example. You know, really pushing the edge of intelligibility. Simonton looked at the prevalence of mental illness in different types of creative
Starting point is 00:37:48 people. Visual artists and writers were on the high end of the scale, with poets the most pronounced. 87% of them experienced some kind of mental disorder. How does that compare to the general population? According to one widely accepted study, around 46% of Americans experience some sort of mental disorder during their lifetimes. So artists and writers are considerably higher than average. But Simonton found that scientists have a considerably lower tendency for mental disorder, only around 28%. And if you include all creative types in the tally, Simonton found that they have lower rates of mental illness than non-creative people. Creative behavior is in fact often a marker for good mental health. So if you're looking for some magic formula for the relationship between creativity and mental illness.
Starting point is 00:38:41 I don't think there's one formula for that. Walter Isaacson again. You know, Ben Franklin was a happy, well-adjusted kid, even though he was a runaway from his brother who tried to keep him as an apprentice. Leonardo da Vinci was pretty tortured, you know, had all sorts of manic periods of his life where he was both depressed and elated.
Starting point is 00:39:05 Somebody like Einstein, deeply focused. Steve Jobs had both demons and angels inside of his head. So I don't think you can make one blanket pattern to say creative people have some special, you know, mental challenges or abilities. Let's go back to Vincent van Gogh, who's arguably the model of the tortured artist. There have been many posthumous attempts to diagnose Van Gogh. Among the theories are manic depression, schizophrenia, and epilepsy. Many people, when they look at the fantastic swirls and the vibrant yellows and blues in Van Gogh's Starry Night, they see a picture of madness itself.
Starting point is 00:40:00 But what if the arrow is moving in the opposite direction? Consider this. Van Gogh was in and out of hospitals during the final years of his life. It's very likely that his doctors treated his epilepsy with digitalis, an extract of the foxglove plant. He even made a painting of one of his doctors posing with sprigs of foxglove. Well, one of the side effects of digitalis? The color yellow overwhelms other shades, and swirly halos can appear around objects. So it may be that Van Gogh's paintings were more influenced by the cure than the disease. Or maybe not. It's impossible to know, in part because our understanding of the mind has changed so much since then. Some recent research suggests that highly creative people often have what's called
Starting point is 00:40:50 cognitive disinhibition. Basically, they lack the filter that keeps you from getting sensory overload just by walking down the street, which means they're constantly being pinged by random stimuli. In theory, this could help you notice things that most people ignore. Cognitive disinhibition, not surprisingly, is also associated with mental illnesses like schizophrenia. So it would seem that the line between pathology and creativity, at least in some cases, can be quite fine. There are a lot of parallels between fiction writing and being schizophrenic. That's Jennifer Egan.
Starting point is 00:41:30 And I'm a novelist and a journalist. Egan won the Pulitzer Prize for fiction for her 2010 novel, A Visit from the Goon Squad. Her brother Graham was an artist who had schizophrenia and died by suicide in 2016. He heard voices all the time. But it wasn't the way I hear voices, although he was a very funny guy. He joked with you that you got paid for it. He was tormented by it. Exactly. I mean, so, you know, he heard voices intrusively as if there were radio on in his head,
Starting point is 00:42:04 and it really kept him from being able to concentrate. His voices would speak to him in very cruel ways, and that made it very hard for him to function. I mean, it just boggles the mind to think about what that would really be like day in and day out. He was sort of like in his own private war all the time. And you could really see it. I mean, he looked like he'd just come back from a tour of Iraq. He was drawn and he would be exhausted. So in that way, you know, I don't have any of that to deal with. I feel so lucky that my brain mostly seems to work as it should. Talk about a gift. I mean, a lot of people don't have that. Do you feel that the kind of cliche throughout history of, you know, the tortured artist,
Starting point is 00:42:51 the mentally tortured artist, do you think it's overblown? I don't know. I guess it's probably not. I mean, it's easy to romanticize that vision, but that romantic picture comes from somewhere. And, you know, I mean, I am lucky enough to be on a fairly even keel, but I've also had a lot of therapy. I mean, we have advantages now in terms of mental health that people didn't have 100 years ago, or really even 50 years ago. I mean, even now, honestly, if my brother were a young man now becoming symptomatic for the first time, he would have options that he didn't have then. And so maybe artists don't have to live that way now in the way that they did. treatment, including medication, a lot of creative people worry that it will, you know, quote, kill their creativity, change their creativity, change the way they think about things.
Starting point is 00:43:50 Well, I would tend to think that it's the opposite, that the craziness is actually getting in the way of the creativity more than fueling it. I mean, in my brother's case, it was so stark. I mean, he was stark raving mad without medication, absolutely out of his mind. And with medication, he still heard voices all the time, but he basically understood that they weren't real. So for him, it was the choice between having some kind of a life and having absolutely no life. So I guess I really resist the romanticization of mental illness. You know, Virginia Woolf on medication, if I had to guess, and of course I have no way of knowing, would have done all the wonderful things she did the connection between it and mental illness is far more nuanced than the stereotype, as is the case with most stereotypes. But what about the more general stereotype of the highly creative person? Essentially, the idea that they are a select few.
Starting point is 00:45:02 The idea that perhaps they're simply born that way and that we muggles should content ourselves with admiring their output, perhaps envying it a bit, but that we should generally just keep out of their way. Is that really the best way to think about creativity? Or should we all strive to be creative? That's a question we put to everyone we've been interviewing for this series. Here's Teresa Amabile, who spent her career studying the topic. Should we all strive to be creative? Yes, we should, because doing things differently in ways that work is the only way that human progress happens.
Starting point is 00:45:43 All right, then. Who's not in favor of human progress? Which means that all of us, in our way, on some dimension, have reason to sharpen our creative abilities. So how do we do this? That will be one of the preoccupations of this series, but don't expect easy answers. Too many people want a one-size-fits-all. What do I need to do to be creative?
Starting point is 00:46:09 And I'm afraid there's no one-size-fits-all. That's Dean Simonton, and here's Walter Isaacson. I do resist the type of books that say seven easy lessons to being creative or the 14 secrets to innovative leadership. I don't think you can distill everything into a list like that, which is why it's useful to read the biographies of different people. Let's put aside for the moment that Isaacson has written biographies of people like Benjamin Franklin and Steve Jobs and Leonardo da Vinci. I don't think he's just trying to sell
Starting point is 00:46:44 more books here. The leadership skills of a Benjamin Franklin came from bringing people together, finding common ground, and being very civil in his discourse when he tried to create compromises necessary to make the Constitution. That was very different from the leadership style of a Steve Jobs, who drove people crazy, but also drove them to do things they didn't know they'd be able to do. So I think it's useful to look at different creative leaders. And then after you've done so, look inside yourself and to say, I'm better off being more like Ben Franklin, or I'm better off being more like Leonardo da Vinci trying to mix art and science, or I'm better off being like Steve Jobs, driving a team crazy but driving them to do things
Starting point is 00:47:33 they didn't know they could do. And you can understand your own skills by comparing them to what great innovators and creative people have done in the past. All right, that makes sense. Given the vagaries of our topic, it also makes sense to address it both systematically and individually, interviewing creativity scholars as well as lots of creatives themselves. Sound good? I mean, that would be too intriguing an opportunity not to say yes. As these episodes unspool, we'll hear how artists come up with their ideas. There are some people who, they're only creative in the morning. There's others that can only work late at night after everybody's gone to bed. There's others that have a cue, like Schiller, who had to have the smell of rotten apples.
Starting point is 00:48:29 And so when he felt like being creative, he'd pull out a rotten apple. We'll explore failure, of course, the inevitability, the usefulness, the anxiety. It is a constant struggle with a very clear feeling that I am out of gas every day. Every day. You'll hear what it feels like to be in the moment of a scientific discovery.
Starting point is 00:48:53 It's a kind of thrill that you never forget. We'll explore the cold, practical realities of being creative for a living. So you kind of sit together, you make a plan, you make a business plan, and then you go for it or you don't. And don't forget, creativity is not always a solo project. Sometimes you have to play nice with others.
Starting point is 00:49:13 I'm listening to the bass, the drums are playing, they're playing in two different times. The drums are in six, the bass is in four. They're interacting with me. Do-do-do-do-do-do-do.
Starting point is 00:49:21 Blah-um-um. Ba-do-do-da-da-da-da. Um-um-um. So where should we start? I mean, Ai Weiwei is a giant. That, again, is David Galanson, the rare economist who studies art and creativity. Ai Weiwei is not only the most important painter in the world, he's the most important person in art. I mean, because he's done something that painting hasn't done really since Guernica and
Starting point is 00:49:46 really since the 19th century. I mean, you know, for centuries, painters made an important contribution to public life. Ai Weiwei has changed the world. I mean, Andy Warhol, I always say, would have given his eye teeth to be arrested by the government for his art. But there's no way that could happen in our system. Ai Weiwei has, you know, with his art, he has, you know, made a contribution to political discourse. This is, I mean, this is a unique person in art almost in the last hundred years. Okay, Professor Galanson, it's a deal. The Chinese-born artist and activist Ai Weiwei now lives in Berlin, and we go there to speak with him. My name is Ai Weiwei. I'm 61 years old. I was born in 1957
Starting point is 00:50:34 in Beijing, China. By the year I was born, my father was exiled. Weiwei grew up in a prison camp, his family having been declared enemies of the state. How does that kind of upbringing influence the creative spirit? Coming up next time on Freakonomics Radio, we'll get into the origin stories of our creatives. What are the early conditions that predict creative output? How well are schools equipped to encourage kids' creativity? And what can parents do to encourage creativity? Did you say that's a small topic? Yeah.
Starting point is 00:51:10 Oh, my God. That's an epic. I think that's the epic topic. There's no bigger topic than the family. That's next time on Freakonomics Radio. Freakonomics Radio
Starting point is 00:51:23 is produced by Stitcher and Dubner Productions. This episode was produced by Stephanie Tam and Matt Frassica with help from Harry Huggins. Our staff also includes Allison Craiglow, Greg Rippin, Greg Rozalski, and Zach Lipinski. Our theme song is Mr. Fortune by The Hitchhikers. All the other music was composed by Luis Guerra. You can subscribe to Freakonomics Radio on Apple Podcasts or wherever you get your podcasts. The entire archive is available on the Stitcher app or at Freakonomics.com, where we also publish transcripts and show notes and much more. If you want the entire archive ad-free, plus lots of bonus episodes, go to StitcherPremium.com slash Freakonomics.
Starting point is 00:52:03 We can also be found on Twitter, Facebook, and LinkedIn, or via email at radio at Freakonomics.com. Freakonomics Radio also plays on most of your better NPR stations, so check your local station for details. As always, thanks for listening. Stitcher.

There aren't comments yet for this episode. Click on any sentence in the transcript to leave a comment.