Freakonomics Radio - 376. The Data-Driven Guide to Sane Parenting

Episode Date: May 2, 2019

Humans have been having kids forever, so why are modern parents so bewildered? The economist Emily Oster marshals the evidence on the most contentious topics — breastfeeding and sleep training, vacc...ines and screen time — and tells her fellow parents to calm the heck down.

Transcript
Discussion (0)
Starting point is 00:00:00 Hey there, podcast listeners. Have you ever had a question about how Freakonomics Radio gets made? If so, we want to hear from you. Any question at all about how we choose our topics, why we interview who we do, how the whole process works. I'll be sitting down to answer your questions with Alison Craiglow, our executive producer, for a series of episodes called The Hidden Side of Freakonomics Radio, which we'll put out on Stitcher Premium. That's the subscription service where you can listen to our entire archive ad-free and hear bonus material like this. Just go
Starting point is 00:00:36 to stitcherpremium.com slash Freakonomics. And to submit a question, send an email to radio at Freakonomics.com with the subject line hidden side. Thanks. If you've ever had a child or ever been a child, you know there's a lot of parenting advice out there, much of which is not very nuanced. No one's in the middle and everyone and people are yelling. The first person's like, well, I did that and, you know, my kid's amazing. And then someone will be like, well, actually, if you do that, there's a very good chance your baby will die. And only someone who hates their baby would do that. Some parenting decisions are controversial and we hear about them all the time.
Starting point is 00:01:24 Vaccines do not cause autism. We have a tremendous amount of data showing that that is not true. There are many other decisions that don't get much scrutiny. And the guidelines seemed really arbitrary. Like the restrictions on certain foods during pregnancy, and alcohol and caffeine. Whether or not to let the baby cry it out, and the long-term effects of daycare, of screen time, of eating nothing but buttered noodles for the first 10 years of life. Wouldn't it be nice if someone out there could cut through the dogma and the old wives' tales
Starting point is 00:01:59 and use data to help parents make decisions? And so I really started digging into like, okay, well actually, what should we do here? Today on Freakonomics Radio, The Economist's Guide to Child Rearing. From Stitcher and Dubner Productions, this is Freakonomics Radio, the podcast that explores the hidden side of everything. Here's your host, Stephen Dubner. We're speaking today with Emily Oster.
Starting point is 00:02:50 I am a professor of economics and public affairs at Brown University. You're also, we should say, married to another Brown economist, Jesse Shapiro, yes? Yes, I am. That's my husband. We should also say your parents are also both economists. Yes, that's also true. Can you talk just for a second about the degree of inbreeding among economists and whether it's typical in the social sciences or academia writ large? So it's interesting. I think a lot of female economists are married to male economists. There aren't as many female economists. So some of the male economists are not able to marry
Starting point is 00:03:23 female economists. And, you know, whether that's as common in other academic fields, I'm not sure. But if you like it, it's great. I mean, I think it's pretty fun to get to do work stuff in addition to home stuff with my spouse. A lot of Oster's research has been related to health care. Studying people's health behaviors and trying to understand why people don't always behave in the ways that our economic models would suggest. And so why don't people always seek out information about their health? Studying people who are at risk for Huntington's disease and why they don't always get genetic testing, looking at why people don't seem to always diet when we think that they should, or what kind of health recommendations people do or do not respond to. What sort of tools does Oster use for this kind of research?
Starting point is 00:04:17 There is a lot of emphasis on big data sets, using administrative data, looking at questions that can be sort of well answered in those data. So several years ago, when Oster was pregnant with her first child, she naturally went looking for good data to help her make good decisions. And even around something like prenatal testing, trying to understand, should I have this screening test for genetic disorders or this other screening test. And the guidelines seemed really arbitrary and to be based on age and not on anything about preferences, which is totally outside of how I think about decision making. And so I really started digging into like, OK, well, actually, you know, what should we do here? And what I came up with
Starting point is 00:05:01 suggested, you know, some of the data was really flawed. The people were using for recommendations. The recommendations didn't even really make any sense, even given the flawed data that they were using. It isn't like there's some secret repository of knowledge about babies that, you know, you're missing out on. Oster went back and started reading the underlying studies that contribute to the conventional wisdom on pregnancy and child rearing. She found that a lot of the studies were built around small sample sizes or incomplete data. A lot of the analysis didn't control for things like income and education level. Consider, for instance, one of the most controversial topics of early motherhood, breastfeeding. So higher income women, particularly in the U.S., women with more education, they're more likely to breastfeed.
Starting point is 00:05:47 And so if later you look and their kids are doing better in school or are thinner, those are things which are also correlated with parental education or parental income with resources that the family has. And so it's really hard to learn from that comparison about the real causal relationships. And that comes up all the time, not just in breastfeeding, in everything. So Oster read the breastfeeding studies,
Starting point is 00:06:13 including one that was based on a large randomized trial. What'd she conclude? So there are some, you know, small but not zero benefits in the short run, particularly around improving digestive health, lowering episodes of diarrhea, and maybe some evidence that lowers rates of ear infections in the first year of life. But many of these claims that people make, like breastfeeding is going to give your kid an IQ bump, breastfeeding is going to make your kid thin, it's going to
Starting point is 00:06:40 prevent allergies or asthma later, these things are just not supported in the data. To say something is not supported in the data doesn't mean that it's not true. Just that when people say it is true, their argument is more likely to be based on some sort of wish or belief as opposed to scientific evidence. Oster did find some compelling evidence
Starting point is 00:07:02 about breastfeeding, but not as a benefit to the baby. Yeah, so this is like the one sort of long-term effect where it looks like maybe we have some good evidence is suggest that it may actually lower breast cancer risk for the mother. And what's the mechanism for that? Mechanisms are always hard, but in this case, I think we have some sense that it changes some of the composition of the cells in the breast in a way that may help protect against breast cancer. Okay. How real is nipple confusion? The idea that if you feed your baby with a bottle, it'll get used to that nipple, then be confused if you try to breastfeed later? Nipple confusion is made up. It's made up. So particularly around something like pacifiers, is you don't give your kid a pacifier because they won't nurse. This is just no evidence for that.
Starting point is 00:07:43 So even a not very brain-developed baby is able to differentiate between a breast and a pacifier. That's right. Yes. Amazing. Okay. What about dietary restrictions for breastfeeding mother? So mostly none. And the question people ask me all the time is like, is it okay to drink while I'm breastfeeding? And the answer is not like a sailor, but some, yes, totally fine. The concentrations are very low in breast milk. Caffeine, fine. Some babies are very sensitive to some things like caffeine.
Starting point is 00:08:17 And so if you find that you drink a cup of coffee and you nurse your kid and they get like totally crazy, then you may need to adjust. What about medication, especially antidepressants? So antidepressants do pass through breast milk to the baby. And so this is something that women have to sort of talk to their doctors about. But in general, many antidepressants are safe for use. And also postpartum depression is a very significant issue. And if you need treatment, you need treatment. And that is something that should be paramount. So on balance, the benefits of breastfeeding are what? I think you want to
Starting point is 00:08:53 imagine that there are some small benefits to breastfeeding in the short term and that, you know, those may be enough to try. And also, I should say a lot of women enjoy breastfeeding and they find it to be a nice way to bond with their infant. And that is, I should say a lot of women enjoy breastfeeding and they find it to be a nice way to bond with their infant. And that is, of course, a great reason to do it. I think the thing that is too bad and is not great is when people sort of build up breastfeeding in their mind as like, this is the only way to give my baby the best start. And if I don't do this, that's giving them a bad start. And that just isn't true. As Oster sees it, one big problem with parenting is that many of the conversations instantly
Starting point is 00:09:33 devolve into a level of partisanship that can make our political discourse seem courtly. For me, the thing that really encapsulates this is the sort of like Facebook conversations and somebody will ask a question like, you know, oh, my three-week-old baby is not sleeping well and I'm thinking about keeping them in the bed with me. You know, what do you ladies think about this? Pretty soon, the shouting starts. And then, you know, somebody will be like, okay, we have to shut down this post because like this is too much. And some of what I try to do is sort of push against that and say, you know, somebody will be like, OK, we have to shut down this post because like this is too much. And some of what I try to do is sort of push against that and say, you know, like you could each make different choices and they could both be right.
Starting point is 00:10:14 And just because it's not the same choice doesn't mean it's wrong. Economists believe in preferences. And they also believe it's perfectly sensible for different people to have different preferences, as long as they're making decisions with a full knowledge of the costs and the benefits. And the knowledge that most decisions do have both costs and benefits. And yet somehow, when it comes to parenting... I think that there is a knee-jerk to be like, well, if anyone ever said that this might be dangerous, no one should ever do it ever. I think that there is sometimes a discomfort with facing up to evidence and also to the uncertainties that come with data that sort of lead doctors, medical professionals, medical organizations to want to make more blanket statements than are
Starting point is 00:11:02 always appropriate and to be sort of less comfortable with explaining nuance to their patients than they might otherwise be. Oster had no such discomfort with nuance. She set out to explore the parenting terrain using data as her guide. The result has been two books. The first, published a few years ago, is called Expecting Better, Why the Conventional Pregnancy Wisdom is Wrong and What You Really Need to Know. The new book is called Cribsheet, a data-driven guide to better, more relaxed parenting from birth to preschool.
Starting point is 00:11:35 Oster appreciates that there are systemic reasons for the medical field to be cautious. Remember, first, do no harm. Also, there's the threat of a malpractice suit. But Oster wanted to think about risk rationally, not as a doctor hoping to avoid liability or even as a parent wanting nothing bad to ever happen to her children. Instead, she just wanted to think about risk as an economist. Like, first of all, let's interrogate a little bit whether those risks are really real and are really significant. And then also to interrogate, you know, like you have to trade off the risks maybe against some other benefits. And, you know, in something like pregnancy, you think about like treating, you know, really severe nausea. There's a sort of thing be like, oh, don't take anything to that, just like suffer through it. It's like, well, actually, you know, that can be really debilitating. And it may make sense for people to take something even if we are not 1000% sure that there are absolutely no risks to it, because it may outweigh some other risks. And I think we sometimes forget that.
Starting point is 00:12:38 And what about facing head on risks that you're describing as relatively small while totally ignoring other, you know, let's say daily risks that are actually relatively large, like getting in a car. Yeah. I mean, getting in a car, I am constantly comparing things to getting in a car because getting in a car is very risky. And I think that there are many kinds of risks that people talk about in pregnancy and childhood, which are far less risky than getting in a car where people are like, oh, only somebody who's a terrible parent would even consider doing that. It's like, well, actually, you know, like, do you get in the car? When it comes to advising parents on risks, one problem Oster identifies is that the advisors,
Starting point is 00:13:17 doctors primarily, aren't necessarily practiced in risk-reward calculations. There's relatively little training on data analysis in medical school. And now that, of course, does not mean that doctors are not data literate. Many of the doctors that I know are very data literate and think very carefully about these kind of issues. But it is true that generally this is not a kind of training that gets much play in medical school. And I think there are some good reasons for that, which is, you know, there are many things about being a doctor that are about doing things correctly and understanding how this biology works,
Starting point is 00:13:52 which are much more important and you can't teach everybody everything. You can't teach everybody everything. Fair enough. But if there's one group of people in the world who think they can learn just about anything, it's economists. This is a longstanding complaint
Starting point is 00:14:12 among other academics and scientists and assorted smart people. The field of economics does carry an air of triumphalism. Many economists feel they can contribute insights to areas that lay well outside their own expertise. Areas like education, criminology, medicine. There's also a lot about, really about decision theory and about how do you structure a decision in a way that helps you make a good choice. And that's really economics. Developmental psychology
Starting point is 00:14:41 and obstetrics and pediatrics are not sciences of decision making. And so I think especially around the issues where you've got to think about what's the best for your family, you need someone doing decision science. There's something to be said for this. Economists have analytical tools that are useful on many topics. They're really good at working with very large data sets. And there can be a big upside in having an outsider's perspective on hard problems. But economists' triumphalism, or maybe you'd call it colonialism, it also has its downsides, as Emily Oster discovered firsthand. Years ago, she was trying
Starting point is 00:15:18 to understand why the ratio of males to females was so off-kilter in many places, especially Asia. Most previous explanations pointed to violence against women and girls, or the selective abortion of female fetuses, or even infanticide. Oster offered another explanation. I wrote a paper in graduate school which argued that parents who are carriers of the hepatitis B virus have more male children, and then this explained some gender imbalances. The theory was that a pregnant woman with hepatitis B was much more likely to give birth to a boy than a girl,
Starting point is 00:15:54 although the mechanism wasn't clear. It could have been that female fetuses were more likely to be miscarried when exposed to the hepatitis B virus. And then subsequently some research came out which suggested that basic fact was not true in the data. And then I did some sort of subsequent follow-up research which also showed that that was not true in the data. She had to walk back her earlier conclusion, which had gotten a lot of attention, including from the authors of Freakonomics. We too walked back her conclusion. That mistake, that error, that episode has had a big impact on how I think about my work and how I think about the importance of being careful.
Starting point is 00:16:34 And so I try to be careful. Did it kind of make you feel that the whole goal of establishing causality was much harder than you used to think it was? Yeah, I think it taught me a little bit to be more cautious about some of the, you know, I thought I had a really good set of causal evidence around this problem. And then it, you know, turned out not to be right. And of course, you know, sometimes things are, things are not right. And so I, I think that it did make me, it did, you know, give me pause about some aspects of causality. It's probably a good idea to take some pause about causality. Causality is often much harder to establish than it might first appear,
Starting point is 00:17:08 especially when the data aren't abundant, especially when the topic is something as universal and controversial as parenting. Some kinds of things people tell you are just completely made-up old wives tales. You know, like, if your belly sticks out to the front, that's a boy. And then, you know, there are some things where it'll be like sort of food restrictions, where, you know, the answer is the restrictions come from from data. But there's a wide variety of quality in, you know, how good the data is and how good the conclusions are. Okay, so let's talk about some of these conclusions, the conventional wisdom, and how solid it
Starting point is 00:17:49 is or isn't. Let's start with pregnancy and perhaps the most obvious don't from the do and don't list. In the U.S., there is a blanket no alcohol during pregnancy. Even a small amount of alcohol can be dangerous. And having read the underlying studies, what did Oster conclude? It is definitely true that drinking a lot of alcohol is very bad. And even one or two times having a large amount of alcohol can be very dangerous. But the data does not support the conclusion that occasional alcohol consumption, say, you know, no more than a glass
Starting point is 00:18:21 at a time, a few times a week, is dangerous for your baby. And you made a lot of new friends by writing that, did you not? So many friends, yes. Not everyone was very happy with that. The National Organization on Fetal Alcohol Syndrome called Oster's conclusions, quote, deeply flawed and harmful. But Oster stands by her conclusion, and it seems as if the obstetrics community is moving in her direction. The truth is about half of the obstetricians in the U.S. say that they tell their patients it's fine to have an occasional glass of wine. And so my guess is that more people listen to that after reading my book than before. What about caffeine?
Starting point is 00:18:57 So caffeine, again, the restrictions are very, very stringent. And I think some people take that to mean none, like no caffeine. The truth is there's certainly no evidence that having two cups of coffee a day is dangerous. And there really isn't much evidence that going up to, say, three or four cups a day has any negative impact either. When you get into eight cups a day, you know, the data is a little more complicated. There's a long list of foods that some pregnant women avoid. Yes. No deli meats, no soft cheeses, no sushi, etc. There are a few things that you should avoid. Probably deli turkey, things that sit around in a steam table, not so good.
Starting point is 00:19:42 Probably unpasteurized soft cheese, also worth avoiding. But many of these things, you know, sushi, ham that are on the restricted list, you know, most women are likely to look at the evidence and think it's actually fine. And why deli turkey, but not other deli meats? Deli turkey is more likely to harbor listeria than other deli meats. And then talk about smoking and nicotine. And I want you to kind of handle them separately, if known. Smoking, it sounds as though it's pretty indisputably bad. Smoking is bad. Yeah, smoking is particularly bad for birth weight.
Starting point is 00:20:14 And the evidence out is pretty good. And we should say low birth weight is a very good proxy for baby health generally, yes? Yes, it is generally the good proxy we use for that. But then what do you know about nicotine? Because obviously there are other ways to deliver nicotine. And there are those who argue that nicotine itself is actually a pretty nifty drug in moderation. Yeah, I think the issue is we don't actually know that much about sort of how should we think about, you know, nicotine replacement therapy as like, you know, relative to cigarettes or e-cigarettes relative to cigarettes. It just hasn't been studied much.
Starting point is 00:20:48 Note to self, that would be a good topic for another episode, the risks of nicotine itself, now that vaping has become so popular. Anyway, back to the risks and alleged risks surrounding pregnancy and childbirth. One of the most controversial topics around childbirth is C-section versus vaginal delivery. Yes, and I think what we know there is that basically in the short run, the recovery from a vaginal delivery
Starting point is 00:21:15 is on average a bit easier than a C-section. So women tend to be sort of up and about a few weeks faster. In the long run, actually recovery is very similar. It is also true, though, that for later deliveries, having had an earlier C-section can increase some complications. So people sometimes will ask, you know, well, is there really any downside to having a C-section? I think the answer depends a lot on whether you want to have more kids. If you want to have more
Starting point is 00:21:39 kids, the downsides are more salient than if you're kind of done. And what about outcome on the babies, whether cognitive, physiological, whatever? We just don't have any evidence suggesting there's any differences in outcomes at all. People say things like, you know, it's important to have the microbiome and you have to rub the vaginal secretions on the baby. We just, we don't have any evidence that that works or not. And when you say we don't have any evidence, meaning there's not enough evidence really to think clearly about it, or there's quite a bit of evidence and it just doesn't suggest a difference?
Starting point is 00:22:06 I would say this is more, and I think that's an important distinction, I would say this is more in the category of we just don't have enough evidence. And so the evidence that we do have doesn't suggest large differences. And in the sort of general question about many of the kinds of outcomes, like survival and so on, we have good evidence that it doesn't matter. On some of these more subtle things like, you know, disease resistance, allergy, immunity, I don't think we have great evidence. So you can start to see why so many parents or would-be parents get so confused by the avalanche of information coming at them. Information that's often not very well sourced,
Starting point is 00:22:46 or that's got an agenda attached to it, or, and this is pretty common, information that used to argue for one decision and now argues for the exact opposite. There's a 2003 book by Ann Hulbert called Raising America, Experts, Parents, and a Century of Advice About Children. It does a great job showing how
Starting point is 00:23:06 many flip-flops there have been over time. Consider, for instance, how parents today are urged to engage and intellectually stimulate their children. And there's a lot of evidence that things like the early acquisition of language is incredibly powerful. But in the early 20th century, one of the most renowned pediatricians of his time, L. Emmett Holt, cautioned that a baby is not a plaything and there should be, quote, no forcing, no pressure, no undue stimulation during the first two years of life. Holt's argument was that the brain was growing so vigorously during that period that overstimulation might cause, quote, a great deal of harm. He also believed a baby should be left to cry for 15 to 30 minutes a day. It is the baby's exercise, he wrote. Coming up after the break, if you're Emily Oster and you're searching for actual data
Starting point is 00:23:59 about parenting, those flip-flops can come in handy. Because of these flip-flops, we actually have some pretty good evidence. So, for example, you can see in places that have flip-flopped, the death rates also flip-flops can come in handy. Because of these flip-flops, we actually have some pretty good evidence. So, for example, you can see in places that have flip-flopped, the death rates also flip-flop. That's coming up right after this. Some people argue the world is in such a state that it's irresponsible to bring any more humans into it. There is a British movement called Birthstrike, for instance,
Starting point is 00:24:34 composed primarily of women who have declared their intention to, quote, not bear children due to the severity of the ecological crisis and the current inaction of governing forces in the face of this existential threat. Indeed, the global fertility rate is about half of what it was in 1962, which turns out to have been the peak year for global population growth. The fertility rate is particularly low in wealthy places like Japan and many Western European countries,
Starting point is 00:25:06 the U.S. rate somewhat higher. But overall, the global population continues to grow, roughly 7.5 billion people today, up from less than 4.5 billion in 1980, 3 billion in 1960, and well under 2 billion in 1900. A few years ago, for an episode called Why Do People Keep Having Children, I asked Emily Oster that question. I think this is probably an open question for debate. I think
Starting point is 00:25:32 many people would tell you that it's the biological imperative. I think that some people would tell you, you know, kids are enjoyable. I think that some people would tell you, particularly in developing countries, people have kids as an investment in their old age or even, you know, to work on their farms when the kids are young. So I think those are probably the leading candidate explanations. That, at least, is how an economist like Oster sees it. She and her husband have two kids, a daughter, Penelope, who's eight, and a son named Finn, who's four. And they're great. But the scarcity of data about parenting led Oster to seek out what she could find and write it up in two books, Expecting Better, about pregnancy, and now Cribsheet, which essentially says, okay, you've had your baby, now what?
Starting point is 00:26:20 I think one of the biggest challenges that we face when we parent now is the perception that if you are doing something for yourself, it must necessarily not help your baby or your small child. And I think so much of the rhetoric around this almost martyr-like approach to some aspects of parenting, it's like, you know, well, I haven't slept in three years, but you know, that's because I love my baby. And I think it should be fine to say, look, I don't care if my kid doesn't sleep because I like getting up in the middle of the night with them. You know, some people will say that. I think that's like totally fine. But the idea that
Starting point is 00:26:58 somehow that is the thing that makes you a good parent is something that I think we should move away from. And, and know, really, rather than thinking about somehow, like, all of my sacrifices are how I prove I love my kid. You could just, like, love your kid and also sleep. Like, that's also fine. So babies and little kids sleep a lot. And I'm guessing that parents have many questions and confusions about the do's and don'ts of baby sleeping. So why don't you tell us what are some of the big issues that people have and then how they should be thinking about it? The two biggest questions in sleep are where the baby should sleep and whether you should let them, what is colloquially called cry it out, whether you should do some kind of sleep training with
Starting point is 00:27:39 them. So, you know, on the first question, the issue is like, should you let your baby sleep in your bed? And a lot of people are tempted to do that because actually many babies sleep better in the bed. And then if you need to like breastfeed in the middle of the night or do something else, it is easier to sort of like roll over and just not have to get out of bed and get on your bathrobe and go down the hall and get your baby. So that's like the plus. But, you know, there are pretty strong restrictions on, you know, you shouldn't have your baby sleeping in the bed with you because, like, you could roll over them and that that's a risk factor for SIDS. SIDS stands for Sudden Infant Death Syndrome.
Starting point is 00:28:18 This is a sort of general term for the condition in which an infant dies in the crib or in the bed without other obvious risk factors. The American Academy of Pediatrics says babies up to one year old should sleep on their backs on a firm surface without any pillows or bedding. Yes, sleeping on the back is a good idea. The Back to Sleep campaign has been very good at preventing SIDS. Okay, now that said, for people of a certain age, they will remember that the advice on this is flip-flopped a lot of times.
Starting point is 00:28:51 How persuaded are you that sleeping on the back is the definitive good idea? I was pretty persuaded. I mean, I came into this sort of thinking, like, I wonder if this is one of these many things that is not super supported. But actually, because of these flip-flops, we actually have some pretty good evidence. So, for example, you can see in places that have flip-flopped, the death rates also flip-flop. It's suggesting that back sleeping is very important. The American Academy of Pediatrics also suggests that babies sleep in the same room as the parents but not sleep in the parents' bed.
Starting point is 00:29:22 The degree to which that is a risk depends a lot on the other kinds of behaviors that you're engaged in. Meaning like smoking and drinking primarily? Smoking and drinking, yeah. Smoke is around the baby that's already compromising the respiration. And because that kind of compromises also a risk for SIDS,
Starting point is 00:29:40 that that's the issue there. And for drinking, it's simpler that just if you're intoxicated, you are more likely to roll over on the baby and not notice. So basically, a baby in the bed, if there's not smoking and drinking going on, is very low risk, correct? It's low risk. Most of the evidence would suggest that there is some risk to that, but it's small. Apparently, there's a fair amount of sleep sharing, meaning parent and baby
Starting point is 00:30:04 sleeping together on a sofa versus a bed. How bad is that, if so? That is extremely dangerous. Of all of these things, sleeping on a sofa with your infant is something you should not do. Why? I mean, how do bad things happen there? People fall asleep sitting up on the sofa with their baby because they are trying to stay awake and not fall asleep in the bed with their baby or for some other reason. And then you sort of the baby falls over. The sofa is very soft.
Starting point is 00:30:28 People kind of fall over. They fall over on the baby. The risks there, suffocation. So that's yes, that's really risky. So the way you just told that story, it's the the desired avoidance of co-sleeping in a bed that leads to the bad thing. Yes. Yes.
Starting point is 00:30:44 And I think that's that's something that doesn't get enough. You know, people are really, they're trying very hard to stay awake, but of course you're exhausted. And I think we're better off telling people, you know, that they would be better off sleeping safely in a bed than sleeping on a sofa for sure. So you're a super smart person. And I'm curious to know how you would describe the caliber of your thinking during
Starting point is 00:31:07 that haze of the, let's say, the first year of your first kid. I mean, just terrible. Just terrible. I think part of it was I was constantly trying to like figure out what was the right thing to do. Not so much around these data things, but just in like what exactly works for your kid. There's this sort of tendency to be like looking for patterns all the time. Like, OK, they slept for six hours. And so like what did I do? Was it like this particular song? I think you should probably sing that song again.
Starting point is 00:31:35 And then what about sleep training and crying it out or other methods? So here, the thing that people will tell you is that, you know, if you do this, your kid will be forever damaged and unable to form adult relationships. But there isn't any evidence for that. And there actually is a lot of, you know, randomized trial evidence on the impacts of sleep training programs on infant sleep. And there's just no evidence to suggest that sleep training has any negative consequences. It does make your kid sleep better. So if letting a baby cry it out leads to better sleep and baby sleeping presumably leads to better maternal sleep and maybe paternal sleep, I am curious about the relationship between the baby's sleep and maternal depression and whether, you know, the cost of letting your kid cry it out might be really, really, really worth it in the long term to the parents especially.
Starting point is 00:32:29 Yeah. So, I mean, what's interesting about these studies of baby sleep is the main outcome they're interested in is maternal depression or parental functioning. And so you actually see in the randomized data that one of the outcomes of, you know, doing a sleep training program with your kid is lowered maternal depression. And some of those effects are really big because, you know, sleep deprivation, we know it's very hard to look positively at your life when you're exhausted. And, you know, people whose kids really don't sleep well, the depression is a very significant risk. And so I think that that gets lost a little bit in some of these discussions, that there are some real benefits. It's not just something you selfishly do because you're like hoping to go out to the club.
Starting point is 00:33:12 Let's talk about the big landmark events for children that parents watch out for, walking, talking, and so on. What would you say is the single biggest misperception about those landmark events? Those kind of physical milestones, there is a very wide range of normal, I think much wider than people perceive. And being on one or the other end of the ranges of normal is like not worse. So kids who walk late are no less likely to, say, be able to walk later or have lower IQ or anything like that. So people get very focused on physical milestones.
Starting point is 00:33:45 And if they're walking early, you know, maybe they're going to go to the Olympics. Your kid's not going to the Olympics, probably. What can you tell us about kids and germs and the hygiene hypothesis? So the hygiene hypothesis refers to the idea that you should expose your kids to germs because then they will be healthier later and they will have fewer allergic reactions. I think that there's sort of some evidence to suggest that that's true. And so for that reason, when your kid gets sick when they're toddler age, usually we don't worry too much. And, you know, you probably don't need to be like super obsessive about never exposing them to any germs. When your baby is very, very little, it actually is a good idea to avoid germs because
Starting point is 00:34:25 if they get sick, then it sort of sets off a cascade of interventions, which will happen even if they just have a cold, which you want to avoid. But is exposure to germs, let's say in toddlerhood and up, is it actually long-term beneficial then? I think to the extent we have evidence to suggest it probably is somewhat long-term beneficial, at least in preventing them from getting sick later. Okay. Same questions then about allergens, because there's certainly been a lot more attention paid to allergies.
Starting point is 00:34:52 Peanuts is maybe the most famous one. It would seem to be there's a big spike in these, but it may just be these were previously undiagnosed. So what can you tell us about exposure to allergens and long-term costs or benefits? Yeah, so this is like one of the biggest changes, even in the last like five or ten years, has been the recognition that the best way to prevent your kid from developing allergies to things like peanuts and eggs and wheat is to give it to them when they are little, not to avoid it. And so people were told, don't give your kids peanuts until they're, you know, two,
Starting point is 00:35:25 because it could be an allergen. It turns out that is a great way to produce allergies in people. And a good way to prevent allergies is to give them peanuts very early on. And so that advice has totally switched. So I can imagine thousands of parents listening to you right now and shuddering with the idea that, oh, the last thing I'm going to do is like give my kids peanut butter when they're like three months old because I barely got used to the kid and now I'm endangering him or her. So how do you do that early exposure while protecting yourself against possible downside? If you're very anxious about this, sometimes they'll tell
Starting point is 00:36:01 people like, you know, bring your kid to the ER and give them some peanut snacks. You know, unless your family has a high risk of allergies, like unless you have a reason to think your kid is at a high risk of allergies, they almost certainly are not going to have that kind of reaction. And so, you know, it's probably not something to actually worry that much about. So what do you know about the incidence of, let's say, peanut allergies now versus 50 years ago? Is it truly higher? It has gone up. And why is that? Because I would think that more kids would have access to peanuts earlier now than they used to. So there was a long period in which they told people not to expose people to peanuts. And so I think that was not great.
Starting point is 00:36:34 So the supposed prevention was part of the problem, then, you're saying? I believe that it's part of the problem, yes. Gotcha. Okay. Let's talk about vaccinations, which didn't used to be very controversial, but has become so in the last maybe 10 or 15 years. Talk about, I guess, the controversy, the beliefs and where you feel the evidence lies. Childhood vaccinations are designed to prevent diseases like pertussis or measles. There has been a lot of discussion in the last two decades about the possibility that vaccines cause autism or other kinds of negative consequences. There was a very, very damaging paper by a guy named Andrew Wakefield in The Lancet, which suggested that the measles, mumps, and rubella vaccine contributed to autism.
Starting point is 00:37:18 It turns out that was not only wrong, but also completely made up, fraudulent. He lost his medical license. But still, those concerns have been really, really prevalent and have contributed to lower rates of vaccination. There is no evidence for those kind of negative consequences of vaccines. Vaccines do not cause autism. We have a tremendous amount of data showing that that is not true. They do not cause autism. The kinds of things people cite as risks are simply not there. And also vaccines do prevent disease. What are the greatest downsides of the suite of vaccines that are commonly applied? So the biggest risks of vaccines, and I should say, like, you know, I am trying hard to actually take seriously the concerns that people have about
Starting point is 00:38:03 these. I mean, I think it's very clear you should vaccinate your kid, but I think we also do people a disservice by not explaining to them, you know, what is the real truth about the risks, which turn out to be very small. So the big thing is that when you vaccinate your kid, they may get a fever. That's a very common reaction to the measles, mumps, and rubella vaccine in particular, that it's an immune challenge. A very small share of kids, if you get a fever very fast, can have a seizure, which is very scary, but actually also has no long-term consequences and is, again, a very rare complication. And there's a small number
Starting point is 00:38:35 of other things that can happen, say, to a severely immune-compromised child, which are cited as risks of vaccines. But of course, if your your kid is very immune compromised, they will not be vaccinated. Talk about discipline for a minute. And I'm especially curious about the value of consistency. Yeah. So when we look at evidence on discipline, the consistency emerges as the most important thing. So there's a lot of different
Starting point is 00:39:05 strategies. But in almost all cases, it's just you want to pick a strategy and stick with it because you want your kid to know what to expect. Like if you say, you know, if you don't put down that toy, like the following thing is going to happen, you kind of have to make sure that thing happens, which is why like if you're on an airplane with your kid and you say like, you know, if you don't stop kicking the chair, I'm going to leave you on the airplane, that's actually not a good threat because you're not going to leave them on the airplane. Says you. Yeah, unless you're prepared to leave them on the airplane, don't say that. And then how great is spanking? I assume it's wildly effective. Spanking is not effective. There is no evidence that it is good
Starting point is 00:39:39 and a fair amount of evidence that it's bad. And how does that bad evidence manifest itself? That when you spank a kid, there are more behavior problems later. Can you talk for a second about, I guess, the tradeoff between a parent wanting their kid to be happy and safe also and wanting to do what's long-term best for them when it comes to child rearing? Yeah, this is a very hard thing. And I think it comes up even in something like discipline, where like in the moment, you don't want your kid to be sad. And you don't want to like punish them. But you know, you need to kind of do that, because that's how they learn how to
Starting point is 00:40:16 behave correctly and how to like be a successful adult. But it's hard because you know, you love your kids, you like want them to be happy all the time. Do you have any advice for people who are really torn between those two poles? No. That's quite hopeless. I mean, I think, no, that's, I mean, it's not true. So, you know, when you do some of these sleep training things with your kids, it can be very hard. You know, people like to listen to their kids cry. And, you know, I have like a WhatsApp channel with my best friends and you'd be like, okay, I'm doing this. Like, can you please tell me that like, it's okay. And they'll be like, yes, you can do it. Like, it's great. I harp a lot on like, you know, moms can be mean to each other and parents can be judgmental. But of course, there's also a camaraderie that comes with parenting that's
Starting point is 00:40:55 really special and can be really, really important for kind of surviving. Talk a little bit about media consumption, TV and or, you know, screens and all the media that those can deliver. What's the kind of sensible way for a modern parent to think about, again, the upsides and downsides? So this is another place where evidence is not very good. So on the one hand, it is very hard to imagine that having your kid watch a half an hour of TV a week while you like take a shower is going to be bad for them. And indeed, the evidence would suggest that that is fine. It is also the case that if your kid is only watching TV all the time, we will generally think that is probably not good. You know, some of the questions you would be more interested in answering, which is like, you know, well, what about 90 minutes a day or,
Starting point is 00:41:38 you know, a couple hours a day? Is that a problem? We do have some evidence on that. Some of it's actually by my husband, oddly, which suggests that watching TV in that range of time is not damaging, doesn't have impacts on later IQ or test scores. What we're missing, I think, is answers to questions like, well, what about iPad games? You know, what about screen time on the phone? And that just hasn't been very well studied because those technologies are relatively new. So in terms of figuring out how the baby or kid will be cared for and really who's going to take care of the child, you advocate creating a decision tree. What share of your readers do you think will actually create a decision tree and follow it?
Starting point is 00:42:28 A hundred percent, obviously. No, I mean, a lot of what I'm trying to do is just sort of say, look, there's like a framework, like a way that you can think about these decisions. And just structuring the way you think about it a little bit, it can sometimes be very helpful to coming to the choice that works best for you. What do you know about the relationship between daycare and cognitive and behavioral outcomes? Again, it's very hard to study. And to the extent that we see data that suggests that, you know, maybe there's a little bit of kind of negative impacts on some behavior from very, very early on, and maybe some positive impacts from having your kid in daycare later. So if you sort of summed it up, it's like if you said,
Starting point is 00:43:10 I'm going to either have a nanny until school or I'm going to have daycare, it's basically a wash. So kind of do what works for you. Are kids who go to daycare less attached to their parents? No, they are not. So you write the following about going back to work yourself as an economist. The eighth hour at my job is better than the fifth hour with the kids on a typical day. And that is why I have a job, because I like it. It should be OK to say this.
Starting point is 00:43:35 Is it not OK to say this among certain friends or family members? Yeah, I think sometimes it's not or I think it can feel often in these conversations that parents have with each other, particularly moms, that if you work, you're supposed to say, oh, well, of course, I have to work. I'd love to spend more time with my kids. And if you don't work, you sort of say, well, I have to stay home. And I think you get a lot of judgment on both sides in a way that I think is really not helpful. Well, what can you tell us about the benefits of a stay-at-home parent? There really aren't many differences across kids, depending on how, you know, the sort of work configuration of the household. I should say this is one of those examples where it's very hard to
Starting point is 00:44:20 study because whether parents choose to work is not assigned randomly. It correlates with income. It correlates to all other kind of stuff. But to the extent that we know, we certainly don't have any good evidence suggesting that one kind of parenting, whether it's stay at home or not stay at home, that one of those is better than the other. You write that the U.S. has subpar maternity leave policies, and you compare that unfavorably to European and other countries. So what kind of effect do those relatively long-term parental leave policies have on kids' outcomes? Yeah, so I want to distinguish between sort of two things. One is going from, say, no maternity leave policy, which is what a lot of people have in the U.S., to something like
Starting point is 00:44:59 four months, which would be sort of the kind of low end of what you would get in these European countries. And I think there we do have good evidence from policy changes in the U.S. and from other places that that is good for infant health, that having some maternity leave opportunities, some ability to be home early in life is good. When we then think about sort of like going from four months to, say, two years there, we don't see much evidence that that influences long-term outcomes. I think that going to everybody having a year is probably not as important as trying to make sure that people have a few months. You write the following, by the time I had Penelope,
Starting point is 00:45:36 which is your first kid, I was 31. Up to that point in my life, there had been surprisingly few instances in which I could not defeat a problem with hard work. So how did the problems of parenting differ? Were your work ethic and intellect as effective in this realm? And I'm curious how it played out. I think the introduction of another person really limits the way that work can help. And so I think I wrote that in the context of thinking about infant crying. And, you know, this sort of moments with your baby early on where like you think, OK, why don't you just like get on the breast and nurse? They just won't do it. And, you know, it doesn't matter how hard you work.
Starting point is 00:46:16 You can't get them to do it. Or, you know, later when it's something like potty training, it's sort of like, OK, my kid doesn't want to poop on the potty. Like I can't make them. So I think there are many moments like that in parenting where you're just like, I can't make you fall asleep. Like, I can't close your eyes for you. And I'm working so hard, but there's nothing I can do. I'm curious what you think that says about humans generally. You're going to say, what does that say about me?
Starting point is 00:46:48 Nothing good. Well, because like, you know, considering how long our species has been around and considering how well we do at passing along some types of information from generation to generation, you know, math comes to mind. Euclid did a lot so that I don't have to, right? But do you think like as a species, we've been not all that successful in passing along parenting information because it seems like every generation is newly flummoxed. Yeah, I think part of it is just like infants are really hard. So particularly like little babies, like, you know, what are you going to say? Like the baby cries a lot and a lot of times you can't figure out what they want and every baby is a little bit different. I mean, if you sort of think about your parents giving this advice, it's actually really hard to remember, particularly this very early time, which is sort of out of your control.
Starting point is 00:47:30 And also, you know, nobody wants to listen to their mom when she's like, oh, why don't you try this? Like, you know, that's what's worked with you. Get out of my face. That was the Brown University economist Emily Oster. Her new book is called Cribsheet. Coming up next time on Freakonomics Radio. As you may have heard, college tuition is out of control.
Starting point is 00:47:51 There have been 36 consecutive years of increases here. Where's all that money going? Water features and climbing walls and concierge services. Mitch Daniels, the former governor of Indiana, now the president of Purdue University, is fighting this tuition bloat. The all-in cost of attending our school in 2021 will be less than it was in 2012. One tool Purdue is using is catching on elsewhere, too. The school is free until and only if the students find a job that's over $40,000. And once they do find a job that is paying them a nice salary,
Starting point is 00:48:29 it's 17% of your income for three and a half years. Presidential hopefuls are talking about loan forgiveness and free college tuition for all. What are the actual college people talking about? That's next time on Freakonomics Radio. Freakonomics Radio is produced by Stitcher and Dubner Productions. This episode was produced by Matt Frassica. Our staff also includes Allison Craiglow, Greg Rippin, Harry Huggins, Zach Lipinski, Matt Hickey, and Corinne Wallace.
Starting point is 00:48:56 Our theme song is Mr. Fortune by the Hitchhikers. All the other music was composed by Luis Guerra. You can subscribe to Freakonomics Radio on Apple Podcasts or wherever you get your podcasts. The entire archive is available on the Stitcher app or at Freakonomics.com, where we also publish transcripts and show notes. If you want to hear our entire archive ad-free, plus lots of bonus episodes, go to StitcherPremium.com slash Freakonomics. We also publish every week on Medium, a short text version of our new episode. Go to medium.com slash Freakonomics Radio. We can also be found on Twitter, Facebook,
Starting point is 00:49:31 and LinkedIn, or via email at radio at Freakonomics.com. Freakonomics Radio also plays on many NPR stations. Please check your local station for details. As always, thank you for listening.

There aren't comments yet for this episode. Click on any sentence in the transcript to leave a comment.