Freakonomics Radio - 468. Nap Time for Everyone!
Episode Date: July 8, 2021The benefits of sleep are by now well established, and yet many people don’t get enough. A new study suggests we should channel our inner toddler and get 30 minutes of shut-eye in the afternoon. But... are we ready for a napping revolution?
Transcript
Discussion (0)
Several years ago, we put out a two-part series, episode numbers 211 and 212, called The Economics of Sleep.
We looked at the relationship between sleep and health, both physical and cognitive.
You simply cannot think as fast and solve a problem as quickly when you're sleep-deprived as when you're not sleep-deprived.
And the relationship between sleep and income.
Generally, people who have more opportunities, more control over their lives, are also better sleepers.
None of these claims were particularly surprising, or at least they shouldn't be.
Anything the human body requires for one-third of its operating hours must be pretty important. What did surprise us was
how little good, clean, real-world data there was on sleeping. I realized that we have data on the
social experiences of individuals from childhood to middle age or older ages, and we were looking
at what social factors explain health. And they had an enormous amount of data on these individuals on two-thirds of their lives,
the waking hours.
But they didn't have anything on what's going on during that remaining third at night.
Why don't we have better sleep data?
It's quite difficult to get accurate information about people's routine sleep behavior.
What would it take to get that kind of information?
What we really need is something like an experiment for sleep.
We did learn about one sleep experiment that was just getting underway in Chennai, India, a city of 10 million people.
The researchers wanted to explore the relationship between sleep and labor productivity. Here's the economist Heather Schofield.
If people are tired enough to be sleeping in the middle of the street and the 100 degree
heat with trucks going by, it's pretty hard for them to be as productive as possible in
the labor force. So if we can help improve their sleep, our hypothesis is that it will
very much improve their ability to work
longer and work harder and work better. Today on Freakonomics Radio, we circle back to that
experiment and tell you the findings. We were shocked initially to find these results. Also,
we explore a particular mode of sleep to see whether it can help. There are some departments
that have implemented napping policies.
Napping policies? Wait a minute, aren't naps for three-year-olds? How can you sell that to
hard-charging adults? We call it the alertness edge.
Is it time for everyone to take a nap? That's coming up right after this. this is Freakonomics Radio the podcast that explores the hidden side of everything
here's your host Stephen Dubner This is one of those episodes that, I'll be honest, may say more about me than you.
But I hope that's not the case.
The thing is, I love to sleep.
And I'm pretty good at it.
Sleeping is not an Olympic sport yet.
But if that ever happens, I'm pretty sure I'll meddle.
I also love to nap just about every day, usually around 25 minutes.
It's one of the reasons I became a writer, one of the reasons I didn't want to work in
an office.
I really love that nap.
I depend on that nap.
A day without a nap is, well, it's too long.
It's too exhausting, physically and mentally.
A day with a nap, meanwhile.
It's really two days in one.
The morning day with its own rhythms and rituals,
and then the afternoon day with a different rhythm.
Given my enthusiasm for sleep,
I pay a fair amount of attention
to what the research has to say about its benefits.
There is by now a large
body of work showing that sleep is essential to good physical and mental health. Again,
not so surprising. What may surprise you is some of the research on sleep deficits.
As I mentioned earlier, good sleep data are hard to come by. A lot of the data are self-reported.
Also, a lot of small sample sizes. Some of the
more reliable research has to do with work and productivity, where the sample sizes are larger
and where, frankly, there's more incentive to measure well. Studies have shown that sleep-deprived
people can make more errors and have a hard time fending off distraction. Also, creativity can suffer.
Other studies show a relationship between poor sleep and unethical behavior, like cheating.
And if you want to look bigger picture, there is research showing that sleep deprivation
costs the U.S. more than $400 billion a year in economic losses.
That's more than 2% of GDP.
The pandemic has obviously scrambled a lot of
people's sleep patterns. People who no longer had to commute had more time at home, and some of them
certainly got more sleep. On the other hand, anxiety and stress can hurt your sleep, and the
pandemic provided plenty of that. We often hear that most of us just don't get enough sleep,
although the actual evidence for that isn't so clear. What we do know, or at least what we're
starting to learn, is that there's quite a bit of variance in how people sleep best. Some of us
are night owls, others are morning larks, ready to go at sunrise or even before.
It's funny because Lois and I, being a married couple, are typically the extremes across most of the variants in how people tolerate sleep.
Lois is a long sleeper who's a lark and typically always been a lark.
When she gets her sleep, she's brilliant.
When she gets less sleep, it's like living with a grizzly bear.
I resemble that comment.
I'm on the shorter sleep end of the spectrum.
I handle sleep disruption pretty well.
At least you think you do.
At least I think I do.
Yes, that is really, really a good point.
Lois and Stephen James are sleep researchers and professors
at Washington State University's College of Nursing.
We try to understand the impact of stressors, most prominently sleep-related fatigue and
shift work-related fatigue, on operational performance for a range of occupations where
the cost of getting it wrong is high. So we work with military law enforcement nurses
and to a lesser extent, elite athletes.
You mentioned military law enforcement nurses.
Those come to mind as professions where I haven't heard a lot about sleep requirements.
But then there are professions like aviation and even firefighting where sleep is kind of baked into the work pattern.
So is there a lot of variance in the professions
about how much attention they pay to sleep?
There's a great deal of variance.
And there are several things that factor into that.
One is shift length.
So traditionally, the professional groups
that have longer shifts have been a little bit more open
to managing fatigue and sleep requirements. So for example,
physicians and aviation and firefighting typically work longer shifts than police officers and
nurses, who in theory would have about a 12-hour max. But the other big component, I think, is cultural. There are some
real cultural differences in terms of acceptance of self-care and stigmatizing about fatigue.
Yeah, the military actually launched a new field manual highlighting sleep as an important factor
in maintaining operational readiness.
And although it's true that, you know, a firefighter, there's a culture of sleeping because of their 24, 48, 96-hour shifts, with EMS, who often share the same organization or
the same base location, sleeping on shift or napping on shift is far less common.
So how does that make sense or just doesn't? It's just an accident of history, essentially.
Partially that, yeah.
But also the idea of being proactive versus responsive.
You know, we expect our cops to be out on the street patrolling,
deterring crime, whereas firefighters are responsive.
The Jameses say that of all the vocations they have studied,
professional athletes are among the most enthusiastic about sleep and even napping.
Absolutely.
The Olympic skier Michaela Schifrin, the Olympic swimmer Ryan Lochte, the NBA legend LeBron James, they are all famously fond of napping.
The athletes I find are typically fantastic to work with because obviously they're
extremely motivated to get good sleep and to manage fatigue and they're extremely well supported.
So often we use our work with athletes to then go and talk to law enforcement to say, hey, look,
it's not weakness. You know, it's not weakness to say, I'm too tired, I need a break. You're
being a professional and you're maximizing. So we call it the alertness edge. It's a cultural
shift for sure. Thinking of sleep as a strength. When you think about the relationship between
sleep and a given profession, policing presents a series of particularly thorny issues. It's got
long periods of relative calm that still require
alertness, which are punctuated randomly by encounters that may be extremely stressful
or even dangerous. You know, we talk about the criminal justice system, but we do need to be
mindful that the system is made up of human beings with the same limits on their resources that we all have. So not to discount or make excuses or try and play down the responsibility of a police officer to get things right,
to make a good decision, but we also need to understand the impact of fatigue and so on.
So we can create policies and training and procedures and countermeasures to help support the individual in whatever operational setting they're in.
There's also the fact that policing requires shift work, including the overnight shift.
About 50% of officers report falling asleep at the wheel, and about 25% fall asleep a couple of times a month at the wheel.
And how do those numbers compare to, say, an office worker, a plumber?
They're quite a lot higher. They're comparable to other shift-working
populations, but they're a lot higher than members of the general public.
A police officer, if they're on an 8, 10, 12-hour shift, if they're on a patrol duty, will spend the vast majority of their time
driving. And to add to that really dangerous mix is the fact that they have a ton of technology
in their cockpit and almost every state exempts them from distracted driving laws. It's a perfect
storm. It strikes me that the problem is not just that you're working a lot of hours in a stressful situation, but you're working a lot of hours at night. And the more that scientists
learn about circadian rhythms, the more we learn that biology is different through the course of
day. Do you know much about how influential nighttime work is per se? Absolutely. We're
diurnal animals. We are biologically designed to operate during the
day and sleep at night. And anything we do counter to that comes at a cost, whether it's to our
health or to our safety or performance. Around the world, an estimated one in five workers
regularly works at night on what's sometimes called the graveyard shift. The World Health
Organization has called night shift work a probable carcinogen.
There are at least two major reasons why night work is so hard on the body.
The first, as I mentioned above, is our circadian rhythm, our natural body clock, which for most people generally means being awake during the day and sleeping at night.
Messing with that can trigger not only sleep disorders, but other problems like obesity
and depression. But also, when you work at night, you have to sleep during the day,
and our bodies and brains aren't primed to do that. So the quality of daytime sleep is typically
worse than nighttime sleep. Lois and Stephen James have been studying these issues empirically,
in the realm of policing, especially
in their lab at Washington State. I have large wraparound driving simulators, the type that law
enforcement train on. And then we have multiple use of force simulators, which are rooms dedicated to
encounters with the public. So there's essentially a room where you can simulate any kind of
disturbance or event that might require police attention, yes?
Yeah, so within the use of force simulators, we actually custom make our own scenarios.
The first set, I think, were developed back in 2009, and those are quite traditional shoot-don't-shoot scenarios.
And the visuals are what? They're seeing like a
live-action video, essentially. So video scenarios that we filmed with professional paid actors
in naturalistic settings based on data from police-citizen interactions. So an example is
a domestic disturbance where officers arrive to the scene.
It's quite volatile.
A man and a woman are shouting at each other, hands on each other,
and the officer needs to step in and try and de-escalate.
And the scenario can unfold a number of ways based on the actions of the officer.
And do you have different versions of this scenario filmed that mix race, age, anything like that?
Yes, we do. In addition to sleep research, my major area of work, and Steve's as well,
is in implicit bias. So these scenarios are all designed to be able to tease apart
motivations on officers' decision-making.
This is what the Jameses are really after, the connection between decision-making and sleep.
In our research, when we brought officers into the lab from four different shifts,
we found that even 72 hours off shift, so they've had 72 hours of rest,
the graveyard shift officers' performance was worse than the fatigued day shift officers because of their chronic level of fatigue.
And even 72 hours off is not enough time to recuperate. Is it possible that the police
officers who either choose to work the graveyard shift or are assigned the graveyard shift have
different characteristics
in the population of police officers who are working daytime?
That's true to some extent. The vast majority of agencies have a bid for shift based on seniority.
So when you come out of the academy, go through your field training,
you then get assigned to whatever shift needs you, and that's predominantly the graveyard shift.
So less experienced police officers typically tend to be on the graveyard shift.
Yes. There is a small cohort of officers who like it, who opt for the graveyard shift. And
when speaking with them, they say things like, well, the day shift deals with victims and we
get to deal with criminals, that kind of mentality. And we've worked with officers
who have spent 20, 30 years on graveyard shift.
And to that point, there's also great individual difference in chronotype or chronobiology,
which is how much of a morning or an evening person you are. And the people who are extreme evening persons or night owls would actually have a harder time on the day shift than they would on a night shift
if the day shift starts quite early.
And people who are naturally inclined to be morning people
are going to have a horrendously hard time on night shift.
So there is a push now to actually factor in chronotype when shift scheduling and assigning shifts.
Is napping a common practice in any police departments that you know of?
There are some departments that have implemented napping policies, but I would say it's still
considered to be an impossibility for most. As far as I'm aware, those that have implemented
policies are not all that formal.
Let's say I bring the two of you in to consult for the New York Police Department
and I say, you know, I'm persuaded that fatigue is a major downside of policing generally,
and it's a major contributor for anyone, not just police officers, to bad decision making and things
like that. The science seems pretty plain on that. And I say, I can't really control how well and long my police officers sleep when they're home
because that's their environment. But what I can do is make sure that I give them an opportunity
to catch up or to not be fatigued when they're here. So I'm going to take the whole third floor
of my building and make it nap cubicles. And I'm going to require or heavily suggest that everybody naps once during their
shift for 20 or 30 minutes. What would you expect would be the response to that? And what would you
expect would be the results? First, I would say you're hired.
Well, the evidence out there for napping improving performance, it's there, but it's limited in a way. All of the studies that
I've seen around the efficacy of napping on duty, whatever that occupation is, whether it's
firefighting, EMS, policing, shows that yes, it supports things like psychomotor vigilance tests,
you know, reaction time. It allows the officers or the worker to feel better.
We're still waiting for that study that shows it
improves operational performance. And that is kind of where I see what we need to really do.
Does it actually make it safer for the community and for the officer? Fatigue management training
isn't necessarily just beneficial for the health and wellness of officers, it is also potentially impactful for
public safety and police community relationships. Look, I'm on your team. I'm on Team Sleep by a
long shot. But I can see that a lot of people just have a baseline response that sleep is for the
lazy. And that's not me. I'm strong and I'm not going to waste my time with that. Greg Belenke, who's one of our mentors, when he was at Walter Reed Army Institute,
he did one of the most important studies I use to convince people about the need. He took a group
and he put them through different cohorts of sleep restriction. Some got nine hours,
some got seven, some got five hours, some got three. The nines
maintained their performance. The sevens dipped a little and then leveled out at about 90, 95%
of their performance. But the fives dropped down to around about 80, 85% of their baseline performance.
And then the three-hour cohort continually declined. And when I speak to law enforcement, I use the diagrams from that study to say,
hey, 80% may be okay in some professions, but I don't believe you're in an 80% profession.
Are you in an 80% profession?
Even if you are, even if you're in a 60% profession,
wouldn't you like to be better?
Wouldn't you like to know how more sleep, even a little bit more, might affect your work and the rest of your life?
After the break, the results of that Indian sleep experiment and, this is almost too good to be true, what it says about napping.
Spoiler alert, naps are awesome.
People are happier.
People have better attention and improved cognition.
We'll be right back after this brief nap.
Break.
I meant break.
We'll be right back.
In the episodes we did on sleep a few years ago, the economist Heather Schofield told us about an experiment
she and her colleagues were just starting in Chennai, India.
The focus was sleep and productivity.
Basically, we'll conduct a lottery,
and half the people in the study will be given things
to help them sleep better at night. In addition to that, we'll also ask them to take a nap in the office every day.
They chose Chennai in part because it has so many factors that conspire against good sleep.
It's really hot. Not many people have AC. It's noisy.
That's Frank Schilbach. He's an economist at MIT and one of Schofield's partners in this research.
It's extremely loud. It's crowded. People also mention psychological factors such as worries, stress, anxiety that interfere with their sleep.
So Schilbach, Schofield, their colleagues Gautam Rao, Maddy Thoma and Pedro Bessona set up a randomized controlled trial in Chennai.
There were three main questions they wanted to answer.
We really wanted to first objectively measure how much are people actually sleeping
and how prevalent is sleep deprivation in a fairly representative sample in urban India.
Second question.
What kinds of things could we do to improve people's sleep
and what impacts would these interventions have
on people's lives? And the third question. If there are large effects of sleep, why is it that
people are sleeping so little? For this experiment, they recruited around 450 low-income adults and
had them do data entry work for one month. To answer the first question, how much sleep were these folks
regularly getting, the researchers outfitted everyone with a wearable sleep monitor called
an actigraph. What'd they find? Our first main finding is that people sleep only 5.5 hours per
night. That's way below what sleep experts would tell you, which is you're supposed to sleep seven
to nine hours of night. I'm guessing you surveyed these subjects as well.
Did they suspect that they slept much more than 5.5 hours?
Yes.
And this is a common result in the sleep literature.
People tend to think that they sleep a lot more than they actually do.
This is often because people confuse how much time you actually spend in bed versus time
asleep.
Now, in our setting, there's a huge stark contrast between those two
things. People actually spend about eight hours in bed, which is what people in the US would do as
well. The percentage of time in bed that's actually spent sleeping is called sleep efficiency. The
average sleep efficiency in high income countries is 85 to 95 percent. But in Chennai, they only sleep 5.5 hours, which tells you there's a sleep efficiency
of about 70%. And sleep experts or doctors would tell you if your sleep efficiency is below 85%,
you're having sleep problems. Okay, so the people in this experiment weren't getting very much sleep.
On to question number two, could their sleep be improved? And if so, how would that
affect their productivity? This is where the randomization comes in. There was a treatment
group and a control group. For the treatment group, Schilbach and his team distributed a
variety of sleep aids. Mattress, earplugs, eye shades, table fan, blanket, pillow.
The treatment group also got information about the
benefits of sleep. Things like you shouldn't drink caffeine late in the afternoon, try to use the
bathroom before going to sleep, try to have regular sleep schedules, and so on. Schilbach and his
colleagues, being economists, also tried financial incentives. We measured people's sleep for about a
week, and then we would tell people
going forward, if you increase your sleep by up to two hours for every minute that you increase
your sleep, we're going to pay you one rupee up to 60 rupees per hour, which is about a dollar.
And there was one more intervention. We cross-randomized a nap intervention,
which means some people got the night sleep intervention only, some people got the nap
intervention only, and some people got both. Every day between 1.30 and 2 in the
afternoon, the lucky people assigned to the nap intervention were provided a private space in the
office where they were doing their data entry work. They were given everything you'd need for
a good nap, a bed, a fan, earplugs, eye shades.
And the idea was just naps would provide some very clean variation in sleep because, you
know, you can essentially control when people nap and how much they nap.
Okay, let's hear some results from this big experiment.
How effective were the nighttime sleep interventions, the sleep information, the financial
incentives, and all the sleep information, the financial incentives,
and all the sleep aids. So when you do all of those things, people in fact sleep about 30 minutes per
night more over the course of three, four weeks. And how does that compare to previous sleep
experiments? So 30 minutes increase in sleep is actually quite a bit compared to other experiments.
Now, interestingly, the increase in sleep is not coming
from increased sleep efficiency. Instead, people just spend more time in bed. So instead of spending
like eight hours in bed, they spend about eight hours and 40 minutes in bed. And out of those 40
minutes, they actually sleep 30 minutes more. So that's interesting. More sleep, but not more
efficient sleep. Okay, what about work productivity?
The researchers measured productivity by how fast and accurately these people were typing
data into their computer, how many breaks they took, and so on.
Did that extra sleep lead to higher productivity?
So for night sleep, you potentially find no effect whatsoever on these productivity measures.
There's some slight positive effects, but these are not statistically significant. At the same
time, you find relatively clear reductions in labor supply. People just spend less time at the
office and therefore also less time typing. Putting these things together, people's earnings
go down a bit. Again, that's not statistically significant. So it's not like
we can say people increasing sleep reduce their earnings. But surely we can reject the notion that
in contrast to our own predictions, people are not increasing their earnings by sleeping more at
night. That sounds like you're actually decreasing their productivity. You're trying to get them to
sleep, quote, better in order to increase their work productivity to earn more
money. But in fact, by sending them these nice mattresses and other things, you're actually
getting them to spend more time in bed, which means that they're spending less time at work,
which means that their incomes, I assume, are not rising?
Exactly. That was very surprising to us. And we were shocked initially to find these results.
I should say we also had some expert surveys in advance where we asked economists and sleep scientists to predict what might happen.
And what almost everybody, including myself, to be fair, predicted was that productivity,
so output per hour, would increase. But most experts also predicted that work hours would
go up. And the idea was, you know, maybe you're less tired, you can spend a little bit more time
at the office and so on. But instead, we find none of that. Now, you might think, isn't it just like
an economist to care so much about labor productivity? What about all the other benefits
of sleep? Schilbach and his team did look at some other outcomes. We measure their physical health,
psychological well-being, decision-making, cognition, attention, memory, and so on.
And?
We find no significant positive impacts on any of these outcomes.
So in this experimental setting, at least, with this population getting these sleep interventions,
Frank Schilbach and his team essentially failed to move the needle.
Through a lot of effort and expense,
they were able to get their subjects to sleep a bit more, but that extra sleep
didn't seem to materially improve anything. Except, except for one last intervention.
You remember the lucky group that was randomized into nap duty?
When you compare people in our study who were randomized
to receive naps versus the control group, we find significant impacts on a range of outcomes,
including pretty much all of the things that I just told you we didn't find effective night
sleep on. People produce more per hour work. People are happier. People say they're healthier.
People have better attention and improved cognition.
And how did a nap affect these workers' earnings? After all, they're taking a half-hour snooze during the workday.
Here, it depends a lot on whom you are comparing the nappers to. If you compare nappers to people
who just take a break, naps look great. People are earning more money, people are working slightly
more.
But there's less of a difference when you compare nappers to people who didn't get a
break at all.
And that's because, you know, people lose 30 minutes of their workday in the middle
of the day.
You think your afternoon is about five hours of work and you lose 30 minutes.
That's about 10 percent.
And the effects of naps are by no means as large as that.
Still, compared to the other sleep interventions the researchers tried, napping looks like
a good deal.
Yeah.
So just to give some background, increasing productivity in tasks where people need to
put in effort at work is fairly hard.
And so the main insight here is that if you increase people's wages, you tend to not find
large effects on their productivity.
So the effects of naps are remarkable. But why would a short daytime nap succeed where more sleep at night failed?
One explanation is that naps are just different naps in the afternoon and night sleep is at night.
Obviously, that's different times of the day, which might affect people differently.
The competing explanation, which if you ask me personally, I think is probably going on, is that the naps that we offer in our
study are at our study office at the workplace, and they get a pretty comfortable nap environment
where there's very little sound, it's quiet. In other words, sleep quality in a cool,
quiet environment is likely higher than the quality of sleep these same workers
were getting at home, the hotter, noisier, more crowded homes that sparked this sleep study in
the first place. So that tells us, well, what we were doing, people cannot necessarily replicate
on their own. Some employers might be able to do it, but the typical low-income worker in Chennai
is not able to, in fact, do that. This may also explain why these Chennai
findings differ from other sleep studies that do find gains in productivity and cognition.
I still think that some of the effects of sleep on mental health and well-being in the long run
might be really important, but I think it's really important to do field studies in a range of settings to understand whether the effects that are found in sleep labs extrapolate to these real world settings when people sleep in their homes and go about their lives.
And when we really do policy relevant interventions where we increase people's sleep among people who we think are sleep deprived.
And the sleep literature is not quite doing that yet. Now, I myself still nap quite a bit, and I am a believer in napping. Not sure
that's necessarily for improving productivity, but perhaps just to improve my own well-being.
Some cultures, of course, do embrace the afternoon nap or siesta. Some countries even allow their airline pilots to take naps in the cockpit, with restrictions, of course, do embrace the afternoon nap or siesta. Some countries even allow their airline pilots to
take naps in the cockpit, with restrictions, of course. There has been earlier research that
shows the cognitive and physiological benefits of naps. Researchers from the University of Athens
Medical School, for instance, found that when people stopped napping regularly, their health declined.
Let's assume that after this conversation and many other conversations that people who run institutions decide that, oh my goodness, the gains from napping are large, whether it's the
U.S. Army, police forces, teachers, medical professionals, or people working in any job, really, and that everybody should nap.
Can you imagine an environment where naps are, quote, mandatory, but people try to game the
system in order to keep getting ahead? So the issue is you can't force somebody to nap, of course.
I'd like to emphasize that what we did was, in some sense, that's not maximizing the impact of
naps, because we had just had a treatment group at the control group where in the treatment group,
everybody was napping
or at least offered the opportunity to nap.
But you know, there are some people
who are just not good nappers
or some people who just don't want to nap
on any given day.
So if you let people select into napping
on some days versus not on others,
or some people just maybe don't want to nap at all,
you might actually get more benefits of napping
for the people for whom it's most effective.
Let's say you, Frank, became the U.S.'s napping czar. Who would you suggest as the public-facing
person to endorse napping to help most cut back on any stigma that non-nappers might have?
I'm not sure about the figurehead per se, but I think it's telling that tech companies,
for example, are embracing napping quite a bit. You know, there's all these nap cabins
at a bunch of different companies. So that's one really good signal. Second, I think sports,
there's lots of NBA or other athletes who seem to be sleeping a lot, including napping.
And so if suddenly very highly respected sports stars starting to nap, I think that could really
influence people, in particular, young people who might not necessarily want to nap, otherwise.
But Schilbach thinks there needs to be a concerted focus on the main underlying problem.
It's not just that some people need to sleep more.
They need higher quality sleep, whether it's at night or during a daytime nap.
And that suggests a wider set of solutions.
One is, you know, housing policies, noise regulation, or just trying even harder than we did
to try to get people to take up devices that might improve their sleep quality,
such as earplugs, which are just hard to get people to actually use.
Second, there could be also psychological interventions
to try to reduce people's stress and anxiety
and just help people sleep better at night
for a given sleep environment.
And so if we just figured out some way
to improve sleep quality,
my prediction and my hope is that
perhaps we will in fact find larger effects
on a number of outcomes
that we're hoping to affect in the first place.
That's our show for this week.
Coming up next time on Freakonomics Radio.
If you plot GDP on one axis and religiosity on the other axis,
the U.S. is a clear and distinct outlier with high GDP and high religion.
In an individualistic society, a person is like an atom in a gas.
It can freely float about. Life is an adventure.
In societies that are tighter, there is more community building where people are willing to call out rule violators.
The U.S. is very different from other countries, so let's stop pretending we're not.
We kick off a new series that explores how America's politics, policies, and culture keep us a big
time outlier, even after all these years. That's next week. Until then, take care of yourself,
and if you can, someone else too. Freakonomics Radio is produced by Stitcher and Renbud Radio.
We can be reached at radio at freakonomics.com. This episode was produced by
Daphne Chen and Mary Deduc. Our staff also includes Allison Craiglow, Greg Rippin, Joel Meyer,
Trisha Bobita, Zach Lipinski, Brent Katz, Emma Terrell, Lyric Bowditch, Jasmine Klinger,
and Jacob Clemente. Our theme song is Mr. Fortune by The Hitchhikers. All the other music was
composed by Luis Guerra. If you like
this show or any other show in the Freakonomics Radio network, please recommend it to your friends
and family. You can get the entire archive of Freakonomics Radio on any podcast app. If you'd
like a transcript or show notes, go to Freakonomics.com. As always, thanks for listening.
It does sound like it might be a big fire or something.
And you're sure it's not your building, right?
I don't want you to burn down.
I'm pretty sure.
I think my wife would rescue me.
The Freakonomics Radio Network.