Freakonomics Radio - 489. Is “Toxic Positivity” a Thing?
Episode Date: January 6, 2022In this special episode of No Stupid Questions, Stephen Dubner and Angela Duckworth discuss the consequences of seeing every glass as at least half-full. ...
Transcript
Discussion (0)
Hey there, it's Stephen Dubner.
A couple years ago, I got to be good friends with Angela Duckworth, a psychologist at the
University of Pennsylvania and author of the book Grit.
We decided to make a podcast together.
We called it No Stupid Questions, and it became the first spinoff in the Freakonomics Radio
Network.
So you may ask, how's it going?
Yeah, not bad.
Today, No Stupid Questions gets more than a million listens every month.
If you are one of those million, thank you.
And if you're not, well, we plainly haven't made our case yet.
Perhaps today's episode will change your mind.
You are about to hear a new episode of
No Stupid Questions made especially for our Freakonomics Radio listeners. Every week on
No Stupid Questions, we try to answer a question like, if everybody hates meetings, why do we have
so many of them? Or when is it okay to tell a lie? Or how can you stop comparing yourself to other people?
Some of these questions come from listeners.
Some are just rattling around in Angela's head or my head.
In any case, Angela and I love having these conversations.
And if you enjoy hearing them, I have good news.
You can get No Stupid Questions on any podcast app for free,
just like Freakonomics Radio. I hope you'll do that. Thanks for listening and happy new year.
They really want to complain and all you want to do is dance around them in a jig of joy.
That's exactly what I did last night. So you're saying that was a bad thing.
You're listening to No Stupid Questions,
the podcast that explores the weird and occasionally wonderful ways
in which humans behave.
Here are your hosts,
Stephen Dubner and Angela Duckworth.
Angela, question here from a listener named Christina.
Okay.
She writes to say, as we all know, there are those who see the glass as half empty and
those who see the glass as half full.
Then there are those like me who see it not only as half full, but as reason to celebrate.
I wake up most mornings happy and honestly feel kind of excited about my day.
I have to say, I'm starting to wonder here if Christina is really Angela Duckworth.
I was gonna say, girlfriend, let's go out.
I absolutely identify with her, completely.
Christina continues, I am familiar with the idea
that most people have a set range
for their resting state of happiness
to which they eventually return,
almost regardless of what life events befall them. And I've come to believe that I am just blessed with a very high level.
It's not like nothing bad has ever happened to me. I am a middle-aged woman. I've gone through
the deaths of those I love, divorces, tough financial times, etc. But especially now,
in my more mature years, I always seem to return to a place of joy. Until recently, I figured I had
just won the lottery in this respect. I mean, isn't feeling good a natural goal of being human?
It motivates and sustains and allows us to direct goodwill toward others too.
However, she writes, you knew there was a however.
There's going to be a but. There's got to be.
Everybody's got a big but, as they like to say.
However, the other day, Christina writes, I came across the idea of toxic positivity
and was quite taken aback.
Is it possible to have too much optimism and joy in my life. You can just feel the air going out of Christina's bouncy, joyful balloon.
So Angie, I may be bringing this question to the wrong person
because you are a proponent of what is literally called positive psychology.
But I'll ask anyway, is it possible to have too much positivity?
Christina, she's going to bounce right back no matter what we say, by the way.
But you want to smack her down for a minute?
Stephen, you'd be the one to rain on this parade.
I would just be like, let's get more streamers.
This is amazing.
And we can rain a little bit on this, but I do believe it's great to be a happy and
optimistic person.
Now, there is a condition called hypomania, correct, which I
assume is too much darn happiness? Well, coincidentally, I've been talking about hypomania
a lot with Marty Seligman, and this precedes getting a letter from Christina about her own
levels of joy and optimism. Marty Seligman, my PhD advisor and a world-renowned psychologist, has come to the tentative hypothesis
that history is changed for the better by the hypomanic. So what is hypomania?
Stephen, you've heard of mania, right? As in manic depression?
Stephen Even I have heard of mania.
Even you. Okay. Well, this extreme form of happiness, when it reaches what clinical psychologists would say is like a pathological level of happiness, is not only feeling like you're on top of the world, but unfortunately, and this is why therapists think it's dysfunctional, is that you are, you know, talking without stop in a way that people can't follow you. You have no need to sleep, and therefore you don't sleep.
You are also, and this is maybe the worst part of being in a manic episode,
you are also taking ridiculous risks.
Basically, it's a complete lack of impulse control.
You might decide that it would be a really good idea to take all your clothes off
and run around the block and see what that's like, and you would just do it.
And that's what an extreme manic episode is. By the way, mania tends to cycle in and out with the opposite, depression.
That's the origin of the term bipolar manic depression. Hypo, meaning less than or low
levels. Hypomania is a lower, milder form of this state of joy.
And what Marty is especially interested in is stable hypomania.
So not just a one-month state or a two-month state, but just dispositionally being high energy.
So you are sleeping, but maybe not as much as most people,
and cheerful and optimistic and all the rest.
So before we go forward with Christina's
concerns about toxic positivity, I want to go back to a couple of things that Christina wrote
that I found interesting. She wrote, I'm familiar with the idea that most people have a set range
for their resting state of happiness to which they eventually return almost regardless of what
life events befall them. Is that true? That is true. There is this
kind of center of gravity to our mood states. However, it's a center of gravity that can shift.
So if you think about your childhood and you're like, you know what, I always was a little bit
of a melancholy kid. It doesn't mean that you're going to have the same center of gravity in terms
of your mood states as you move on and as things happen to you.
So yes, there is some equilibrium that we return to, but that equilibrium itself can change.
Okay. So Christina's correct largely in that statement. Here's another one though. I just want to run past you. She wrote, isn't feeling good a natural goal of being human? I think it's
a nice idea, but would you agree that that's a natural goal from a psychological
perspective, at least? I think she actually is more right than wrong there. Every language in
the world and every history and every philosophy and every religion has talked about happiness and
its opposite, despair. So the universality of this is, I think, indicative of the fact that we all
know what it's like to feel good, and we all know what it's like to feel bad, and all of us would
like to feel good more than we feel bad. I remember having a conversation with Danny Kahneman,
the Nobel laureate, about optimism and happiness, and he said that if there were a fairy godmother that only granted you one wish for your newborn child,
absolutely, you would ask for optimism slash joy, right? And those aren't exactly the same thing,
but they're highly correlated, and they're very causally interrelated. And I think this
idea of being somebody who, you know, the glass is half full and it is full of something I like.
And what's in your glass? I'm sure it's great. I think overall, the reason why we want that for
our children and for ourselves is that it is the end in itself. Many philosophers have said that
the pursuit of human action is happiness. So if indeed it is human to seek out happiness, we get to Christina's
dilemma. And what I find interesting about her question is this underlying assumption
that sounds close to guilt, as if her, quote, toxic positivity is necessarily numbing or
blinding her to the real concerns of the day. So I guess the first question would be, can't you
have both? I've learned from my friend Angela Duckworth that instead of either or, both and
is a really good option. Both and is always the right option. You know, Christina talks about
going through the deaths of those she loves. Divorce is plural, I noted. Tough financial times.
It's not that she's living a Pollyanna life and
nothing terrible happens to her. I'm guessing she knows what it's like to feel sadness,
grief, loss, insecurity, jealousy, anxiety, worry.
You're bumming me out just saying those words. I have to say, no, I'm serious.
Really?
I feel my shoulders slumping.
Or, you know, hunching.
I'm doing both. I'm so brought down by that list.
I'm hunching and slumping.
Both and.
I think that it is not only universal to seek good days and not bad days.
It's not only universal to want to be happy and want to be unhappy less.
It is also universal to experience positive and negative emotion.
And here's actually a fun fact from the scientific literature that was really kind of shocking when it debuted a few decades ago, which is that for a long time, psychologists thought that unhappiness and happiness were almost just the photo negatives of each other. Like, if you're unhappy, you're not happy. But it turns out that the correlation between
positive emotions and negative emotions is only negative 0.3. In other words, if it were truly
the case that being happy was simply the opposite of being unhappy, you would actually be seeing a
correlation of negative one. And what this suggests is that human beings are fully capable
of feeling maybe even in the same moment moment some happy emotions, some negative emotions, mixed emotions.
But one thing that Christina noted is that not only does she seem to experience more positive than negative, so she has a center of gravity more in these positive mood states than these negative ones, but she also said something about having a lot of stability around that, right? And when we study this scientifically, we not only want to know the center of gravity of somebody's mood state, but also how much variation is there. And what we find with people who are what we would call neurotic is that you have rollercoaster emotions. You have a lot of variability around that mean. And so what I'm hearing from Christina in her letter is that
she's got a high mood center of gravity and she hovers around there pretty stably.
I will say Christina's email did make me examine my own state of positivity.
Yeah. What about you, Stephen?
So here's the thing about Christina's question. I do love encountering ideas that literally make you stop and think.
And this one did.
It made me reflect on some of the work we've been doing on Freakonomics Radio the last
couple of years about negativity, about the power of bad, for instance, this psychological
notion that bad things have more salience than good things.
We did another piece recently on the power of negative media, which contributes
to all sorts of problems on the personal and institutional levels. So I guess I have been
paying attention to negativity and pessimism and judging it to be potentially harmful. And I do
feel I've got a positive disposition personally, maybe not quite as positive as Christina or you, but overall quite positive.
I have a few friends who I stereotype as Eeyores, basically. And if I'm given the choice between seeing them one evening and staying in and reading a book, I'm going to stay in and read the book
because I don't want to swim in that negativity for a whole night. You know, life's too short and too hard. Why make it harder than it already is? But Christina's email did make me think about the potential downsides to this pursuit of happiness and perhaps a state of what she calls toxic positivity. I know there's some literature, including by a colleague of yours, Maurice Schweitzer.
Same department, even. Who has written about the notion of happiness being associated with naivete, right?
Yeah.
I know there's also research that shows that in a competitive setting, positivity or a
willingness to collaborate can be seen as a sign of weakness.
So you can see in the business sphere, being positive might come across as something that
could be exploited.
I think another fear is that if you seem too happy, toxically happy, it may be an indication
to others that you are low on empathy.
And I believe there's psychological research about that as well.
June Gruber.
June Gruber, precisely.
So all of these checks on my happiness do lead me to wonder if indeed Christina is
barking up just the right tree. On the other hand, if you do have the quote problem of being very,
very happy and you want to bring a little bit more either empathy or concern into your life,
certainly there is for everyone, no matter which side of the scale you're starting on,
a golden mean. Isn't that the real pursuit? Not to be happy all the time, but to find that golden mean where you can enjoy your life while acknowledging that on any given day, there is
unbelievable cruelty and genocide and other bad things.
So I'm going to sidestep for a moment the question of the golden mean of happiness,
the ideal number on a scale from zero to 10. Maybe it's not 10. Is it eight? How about nine?
I'm going to sidestep for a moment and just say, what is going on with dispositionally happy people?
You say that with a tone as if you don't understand it.
What's going on with these people, these people who are just like me?
Well, I have to say that I have some familiarity with June Gruber's research and Maurice's,
their colleagues and friends, and I also have some intrinsic interest in this.
What's going on with these dispositionally happy people who may be perceived by others to be
naive and who might be sometimes insensitive to people who are not in a very good mood at the
time? I think it comes down to attention. What it is to be a dispositionally optimistic, happy person
at the core is to have a kind of bias to pay attention to the good sides. Like, look, the glass is half full. It's
equally true to say that the glass is half empty, but a dispositionally optimistic and joyful person
has this attentional bias. That explains the naivety finding from Mary Schweitzer. It's like,
hey, they're looking over here. While they're looking at the glass being half full,
we can exploit this blindness that they have to sell them a horse.
This is also why it's been shown that people get happier on average as we get older.
Until close to the end.
Until close to the end.
But well into your healthy older adulthood, you reliably find that adults are happier than they were when they were younger. Now, why? Laura Carstensen's research at Stanford would suggest that it's because
older adults tend to selectively look at the pretty picture and not the ugly picture.
So I think that this attentional engine that's always filtering out our reality
and then pushing our feelings around and our thoughts,
attention is the gatekeeper for everything.
I think that's what's really going on. And so then when you ask me, well, what's the golden mean? I agree. It surely
can't be the optimal life to ignore inequality, death, sadness, loss. We don't want to be that
person, even if we could be 10 out of 10 on happiness, because there's something other
than happiness like character. But having a tilt in our
attentional focus to sort of look all around, see the glass half full, see it half empty, but then
preferring to focus on the fact that it's half full, I think that's pretty defensible.
Still to come on No Stupid Questions, Stephen and Angela wonder whether the U.S. is
disproportionately optimistic, and if so, why?
And they admit that what appears to them to be good today may be superseded by something better tomorrow.
That's coming up on this special episode of No Stupid Questions
from the Freakonomics Radio Network.
Before we return to Stephen and Angela's conversation about toxic positivity,
let's hear some of your thoughts on the topic.
We told listeners to tweet us their views on the benefits and detriments of a positive mindset.
A listener with the Twitter handle at Bitter Malone writes,
when someone complains about a problem, toxic positive people say things like,
don't worry about it, you gotta stay positive,
or you just have to push through, try harder, which only invalidates a person's concerns and
makes them feel worse. At Hender Kevinson writes, my wife is an optimist, which is great, except
when she's backing out of parking spots because she's convinced she's not going to back into the wall, or a shopping cart,
or another car. Optimists have higher insurance rates. Hender Kevinson, your observations are acute. Research from the Journal of Accident Analysis and Prevention concludes that existence
of robust optimism appears to be related to an exaggerated sense of control, which can decrease the probability of appropriate anticipatory avoidance responses.
A.K.A. checking to see if there's a shopping cart behind you before backing out of a parking space.
And finally, at Twist F*** writes,
Expect the best, prepare for the worst.
It's more work than having blind faith that everything will work out,
or absolute defeatism that it's not worth bothering.
It's a blend of the better part of each.
Let's call it optimism.
Wise words, Twist.
Wise words.
If you'd like your thoughts to appear on an upcoming show, follow our Twitter account, at NSQShow.
Now, back to Stephen and Angela and their conversation about whether it's damaging to live
a life where you constantly see the glass as half full. One could imagine that the choice set is
even larger than it's half full, it's half empty. It could be that Christina or you or maybe even me
on a good day could say, not only is the glass half full, but I should find another glass because I've got so
much happiness, I'm going to fill up two. It's not so much, well, we're looking at the same picture
and we're seeing slightly different versions of it. It reminds me of this book by Barbara Ehrenreich,
the journalist who's most well-known for a book called Nickel and Dimed, which was an amazing
piece of reporting about low-wage work in America. And this was probably 15 or 20
years ago, so it was quite prescient. But she wrote another book called Bright-Sided, which
is a play on blindsided. And it came about because she'd been diagnosed with breast cancer,
and she was suddenly being assaulted with all these optimistic and inspirational messages about,
you know, keep your head up, everything's going to be okay. And she was more like, hey, shut up.
I got cancer here.
The book was called A Knockdown of America's Love Affair with Positive Thinking.
And it's a bit of a knockdown of the kind of work that you do.
Yeah, and very explicitly a positive psychology.
But I definitely empathize with that view. And I definitely empathize with
the idea that if someone is going around being happy all the time, that it'd be very easy to say,
you know, when people are happy and they're relatively successful in their lives. And look,
let's not lie. You and I, we've both had a lot of good things happen in our lives.
We've had good health. We've had good education. We enjoy our work. You and I, we've both had a lot of good things happen in our lives. We've had good health.
We've had good education.
We enjoy our work.
We have families that we love and maybe even love us back sometimes.
And so I think it's very easy from the outside to say, well, of course you're happy.
You've got those things working.
So what do you say to that argument?
That happiness is a privilege.
I think that's an excellent point. But as many
philosophers have pointed out, you do not have to be a rich person to control where you pay
attention. It's not to negate that some people just have worse lives, objectively speaking.
But I think at the same time, both and, Stephen, right? The optimists of the world tend not to be
like hanging out on their couches, just staring blissfully into their half-filled glasses.
They're starting nonprofits and becoming teachers and so forth.
I really do think it's both.
And you can acknowledge that there are objective disadvantages and also say, I'm going to look at the good side, too.
You want a little data on optimism versus pessimism in America?
Absolutely. Yes, please. This is from a 2013 survey by Time Magazine
of around 800 people. So let's just say it's not particularly scientific. But anyway, the question
was, do you consider yourself an optimist or a pessimist? Do you have any guesses before I give
you the numbers? I'm going to say that 90% of people consider themselves optimists. Good God, you're optimistic about the optimism survey. I should also say you could answer somewhere in between. There's also don't know or refused. If somebody refused, I would put them down as a pessimist personally.
Exactly. Just acknowledge that this is extremely unscientific. According to this poll, 50% said they were optimists.
43% said they're somewhere in between.
Only 4% said they're pessimists.
50 to 4, so it's 10 to 1.
50 to 4.
Hey, that's about the same as my estimate of 90%, by the way.
Now, that shocks me.
What did you think?
I think maybe I'm hanging out with the wrong crowd because I know a lot of pessimists.
Grinches.
We've been doing a lot of episodes on Freakonomics Radio this last year about America.
You know, I came to this conclusion over the course of many years of reporting and writing
that whenever we would look at a policy idea or solution for whatever it is, universal
child care, the way health care is paid for and so on.
The solution almost always was, well, just copy the Danish model or copy the Hong Kong model or whatever.
And when you start to look at how you can try to overlay a solution like that, the answer
was usually you can't because we're just too different.
And when you look at the US on many, many dimensions, culture, history, politics, etc.,
we're just a major
outlier.
Along those lines, I do wonder if we are an outlier as optimists as well.
I'd like to read you a couple sentences from Democracy in America, Alexis de Tocqueville's
mind-bogglingly interesting book written in the 1830s from the perspective of a French visitor.
He wrote, they, Americans, have a lively faith in the perfectibility of man. They all consider
society as a body in a state of improvement, humanity as a changing scene in which nothing
is or ought to be permanent, and they admit that what appears to them to be good today
may be superseded by something better tomorrow. So it really does make me wonder if what you and
I are talking about now is really not optimism in the human, but optimism in the American human.
It's an excellent question. I don't know the answer. I'm guessing there have to be
national differences in optimism. We know for sure that there are international differences in happiness because for some years now, there are these annual happiness polls happy country on the planet. So we know happiness
varies by national culture. My guess is that optimism would, too. It may be more pronounced
in the United States. Benjamin Franklin, he had this parable of the handsome and deformed leg.
Have you heard of this parable? So, you know,
Ben Franklin. Him I've heard of. Yeah, but I was going to say, I'm at Penn, so we quote Ben
Franklin a lot. So I can read to you this little story. Franklin is speaking about pessimists here.
An old philosophical friend of mine was grown from experience and carefully avoided any intimacy
with such people. He had, like other philosophers, a thermometer to show him the heat of the weather
to mark when it was likely to prove good or bad.
But there being no instrument invented to discover at first sight
this unpleasing disposition in a person, that of being a pessimist,
he for that purpose made use of his legs,
one of which was remarkably handsome, the other, by some accident, crooked and deformed.
If a stranger at the first interview regarded his ugly leg more than his handsome one, he doubted him.
If he spoke of it and took no notice of the handsome leg,
that was sufficient to determine my philosopher to have no farther acquaintance with him.
Everybody has not this two-legged instrument, but everyone with a
little attention may observe signs of that carping, fault-finding disposition and take the same
resolution of avoiding the acquaintance of those infected with it. I therefore advise those critical,
querulous, discontented, unhappy people that if they wish to be respected and be loved by others and happy in themselves,
they should leave off looking at the ugly leg. I'll stop there, but obviously this is Ben Franklin
well before June Gruber's and Maurice Schweitzer's research saying that I think what's going on here
is a tension. And, you know, he's famously a founder of this country and perhaps not only
somebody who shaped our culture,
but also the fact that we keep quoting him and keep his parables alive. Maybe it is a sort of
particularly American, I won't say uniquely American, but maybe particularly optimistic
American bias. I can imagine one other large reason why America may be more optimistic than many other countries, which is that we have
overall a very strong religious belief in this country and a very strong belief in an afterlife.
I mean, if you want to think about the ultimate example of a happy ending, that's what heaven is.
And if you want to think about a reason to be optimistic,
it would be the belief that your ending will be good, will be better than now. So I wouldn't be
shocked if America is indeed near the top of the optimism scale, if it's related in some
significant measure to that wide belief in heaven.
Maybe. I don't know. You have a religious tradition that emphasizes that things will ultimately be very good, but also maybe itself a product of a bias to want to see that kind of
thing. Hence the difficulty of studying culture, right? It's very hard to do this scientifically.
What you're saying now relates back to what
Franklin is suggesting. Franklin was essentially saying, smile and you will feel happy. And now
you're saying that if you tell yourself a certain kind of story, it may make you feel happy or
optimistic, but it may not have a basis in the reality. But at the end of the day, does it matter?
I think there could be virtuous cycles
here where the causal arrows go every which way. Maybe when you're a happy person like Franklin
and maybe Christina are saying people are attracted to you, they're drawn to you. Maybe
if you have people who are drawn to you, you have even more reason to be happy.
There are these virtuous cycles and there's a lot of very good data now
on the causal influence of happiness on future outcomes. So we do know at least that the causal
arrows go from happiness to, for example, doing better at work. And then if you're doing better
at work, you can just imagine, and there's research on this too, that makes you happier.
So this upward spiral is real. But when you go back to Christina's worry, can you be too optimistic? Can this get
toxic? If we go back to this attention hypothesis that even Franklin had centuries ago, if you are
ignoring other people's pain, if you are ignoring the gradient of privilege, if you are kind of
tone deaf, I guess, for example, a roommate comes home and they've just had a day and they really want to complain.
And all you want to do is dance around them in a jig of joy.
That can be bad.
That's exactly what I did last night.
So you're saying that was a bad thing.
But look, both and right.
It's both possible to have a high center of gravity when it comes to positive emotion and to be pretty stable around that. And I think it, you should tone it down a little bit for the sake of others.
And that pessimists should maybe jack it up a little bit because it's a free pass toward improving your outcomes.
Is that the advice?
I'm going to give the advice my way, which is that rather than thinking about toning down your behavior and your voice
and trying to get to something which is going to be good for everyone,
I want to actually focus on attention.
I think that if you diagnose yourself, as I guess most Americans apparently do as an optimist,
then you might want to make sure you are at least noticing when the glass is half full
and for whom the glass is half full and who doesn't have a
glass at all and who has two glasses. So just attention-wise, make sure you're not neglecting
a part of the bigger picture, even if you're going to go back to the good parts. That's my
recommendation for optimists. My recommendation for pessimists is, again, notice your attention.
What part of the picture are you obsessively
dwelling on? Have you failed to notice that the glass is half full? Have you failed to notice
other things that might actually improve your mood? And I would also draw the attention of
the optimists and the pessimists listening here to the reality of these cycles, because I really
do believe there are virtuous and vicious cycles and being unhappy, being negative and always being hypercritical, et cetera, can really be a spiral downward.
If you do pay attention to that, you could say like, hey, look over here.
It's this virtuous upward spiral.
I think I'm going to join that one.
Has this conversation about the possibility of too much optimism dimmed your own optimism even one watt?
Not even half a watt, Stephen.
No Stupid Questions is produced by me, Rebecca Lee Douglas.
And now, here's a fact check of today's conversation.
Angela describes pathological mania as a symptom of bipolar disorder, and she notes that during
manic episodes, a person may have no need to sleep and could lose all impulse control.
This sort of episode is generally tied to bipolar 1 disorder. Although individuals with this illness
may not experience these exact symptoms, less severe hypomanic episodes are common with bipolar 2 disorder. Hypomanic episodes
and periods of depression are also associated with cyclothymic disorder. We'll link to more
information in the show notes at Freakonomics.com. Also, Angela recalls that Togo was, at one point,
the least happy country on the planet. In 2015, the West African nation indeed ranked dead last,
158th out of all 158 countries measured in the World Happiness Report, which uses data from the
Gallup World Poll and the World Risk Poll. However, in the 2021 report, Togo ranked 136th.
The change may be attributed to a steady drop in infant mortality
and an increased number of children receiving education.
Afghanistan is currently ranked as the least happy country in the world,
with Zimbabwe right above it.
The happiest country in the world, as Angela suggested, is Finland,
with Denmark as a close second.
The United States sits at number 19 on the list,
a slight drop from its rank as number 15 in the 2015 report.
Finally, Stephen surmises that the United States
may be one of the most optimistic countries in the world.
According to a 2020 survey from market research firm Ipsos,
this is true.
Americans ranked fifth out of 28 participating countries
when it came to optimism about their nation's future. China was ranked as the most optimistic
country, and Italy was ranked as the most pessimistic country. That's it for the Fact Check.
Hello again, Freakonomics Radio listeners. I hope you enjoyed this special episode of our spinoff podcast, No Stupid Questions, where Angela Duckworth and I hold conversations every week about happiness, cognition, and all the weird and wonderful ways that humans behave.
You can get No Stupid Questions wherever you get your podcasts.
If you are new to the show, there are 80 episodes waiting for you. If you would like to submit a question to
Angela and me, write to nsq at Freakonomics.com. Coming up next time on Freakonomics Radio,
are you the kind of person who knocks on wood to prevent bad luck? Do you refuse to clink the glass
of someone who's drinking just water? Perhaps you even believe in the sweater curse.
It is irrational, and yet it's very popular.
Many people are superstitious,
probably more than admit to it.
Before you laugh it off entirely, consider this.
The embracing of superstition at the official courtroom level
isn't totally rejected in the American legal system.
The legalities, history, and and yes, economics of curses. That's next time on Freakonomics Radio.
Until then, take care of yourself, and if you can, someone else too.
No Stupid Questions and Freakonomics Radio are part of the Freakonomics Radio Network,
which also includes people I mostly admire and FreakakonomicsMD. All our shows are
produced by Stitcher and Renbud Radio. This episode was mixed by Eleanor Osborne. Our staff
also includes Allison Craiglow, Greg Rippin, Zach Lipinski, Ryan Kelly, Mary J. Duke, Jasmine
Klinger, Emma Terrell, Lyric Bowditch, and Jacob Clemente. The No Stupid Questions theme song is
And She Was by Talking Heads. Special thanks to
David Byrne and Warner Chapel Music. If you want to read the show notes or a transcript,
go to Freakonomics.com. As always, thanks for listening.
All right, I'm ready when you are.
Yes, go.
You asked the question, though.
No, you're asking me the question, aren't you?