Freakonomics Radio - Bad Medicine, Part 3: Death by Diagnosis (Rebroadcast)
Episode Date: August 31, 2017By some estimates, medical error is the third-leading cause of death in the U.S. How can that be? And what's to be done? Our third and final episode in this series offers some encouraging answers. ...
Transcript
Discussion (0)
Hey there, it's Stephen Dovner.
Coming up, the third and final episode in our Bad Medicine series.
It's called Death by Diagnosis.
We thought about calling it The Doctor Will Kill You Now, but cooler heads prevailed.
Next week, we are back with a brand new episode.
In the meantime, hope you enjoyed this one.
And if you want more, check out our other podcasts, Tell Me Something I Don't Know. There are 26 episodes to hear with 10 more coming soon. And if you want to come see us record the show live, visit TMSIDK.com. Joe's Pub, part of the Public Theater in New York City, on October 5th, 6th, and 7th.
Two shows each night with amazing guests.
So if you want to attend or if you want to be on the show, you can find all the details at TMSIDK.com.
Thanks.
This is an American condition. This is an American disease.
This has been one of the great mistakes of modern medicine.
That's David Kessler, who ran the Food and Drug Administration during the 1990s.
Surely you know the condition, the disease he's talking about.
President Obama discussed it.
This crisis is taking lives.
It's destroying families.
It's shattering communities all across the country.
But what does Kessler mean when he says this has been one of the great mistakes of modern medicine?
Drug overdoses now take more lives every year than traffic accidents.
A lot of time, they're from legal drugs prescribed by a doctor.
What Kessler is talking about is a combination of good intentions,
greed, and a complicated changing relationship
between doctors and their patients.
The result?
People dying every year from prescription drugs
that are supposed to heal us, not kill us.
It's a sick twist, isn't it?
So how'd this happen?
It's part of the recurring sense of hope and despair
associated with these drugs that are supposed to solve problems,
but they end up being problems in themselves.
The numbers are remarkable.
Prescription opioid use has gone up about 300 to 400 percent since the year 2000.
America is a world leader in the consumption of painkillers.
Here's what a 2007 report found.
We were consuming about 83 percent of the world's oxycodone in the United States.
And it is not because we had 83 percent of the world's pain.
It's because we are a consumer society that believes
in the power of a magic pill. How did medicine get taken over by consumerism?
Doctors used to practice medicine on sick and injured patients, and it was those two
players in the healthcare system. And now the same two people in the room, the doctor and the
patient, behind the room is a gigantic industry of people buying, selling, trading, bartering,
discounting, marking up all of our services. Today on Freakonomics Radio, the third and final part
of our Bad Medicine series. This time, we look at the doctor-patient
relationship. Who's got the real leverage in that relationship? They cope with their job by
giving an angry patient what they want, not what they need. What's the number one problem
in healthcare? I think the number one problem is we don't measure performance. We don't measure
the outcomes of patients in healthcare for 99 99 percent of the health care that's delivered.
And is it a better idea to just stay away from the doctor?
So I would think that you are a downright danger to your patients.
How is it that you're not?
No comment. Here's your host, Stephen Dubner.
In the first two episodes of our Bad Medicine series, we looked at some of medicine's biggest mistakes.
Drilling holes into people's skulls. It would cause a whole series of malformations and probably a lot of fetal death.
It was literally taking someone to hell and back.
And we looked at how better science
is pushing medicine not always forward,
but often backwards.
It is quite common to see practices
that end up getting reversed.
And the best estimates are that happens
about 15% of the time.
We talked about who has been excluded
from a lot of clinical trials.
The study of women in general became part of the
collateral damage. And these days, who gets included? When you look at the evidence, what you often find
is that trials are conducted in absolutely perfect dream patients, people who are by definition much
more likely to get better quickly. Now, that's very useful for a company
that are trying to make their treatment look like it's effective.
But actually, for my real-world treatment decisions,
that kind of evidence can be really very uninformative.
Today, in our final episode of Bad Medicine,
we focus on those real-world treatment decisions.
We focus on where healthcare really happens when a patient gets together with a doctor or another healthcare professional.
And what's one of the main reasons any of us might go to a doctor?
It's easy. It's because we're in pain.
Pain illuminates what is, I would argue, a general problem in medicine.
Keith Whaloo is a Princeton historian who focuses on health policy.
That is to say, who's to say what degree of pain a person is in and what constitutes truly effective relief other than the patient themselves?
And two different people might actually require different doses of medication to alleviate
the pain.
In the late 1980s and early 90s, there was a push to mandate the recognition and treatment
of pain.
This culminated in the promotion of pain as the fifth vital sign, along with temperature,
blood pressure, pulse, and respiratory rate, which made pain the only
vital sign that is determined not by objective measurement, but by the patient's own assessment.
So doctors were put in the position of having to determine whose pain was worthy of a prescription
painkiller and whose wasn't. Very often, doctors have decided, yes, in 2015, more than 650,000 opioid prescriptions were dispensed per day in the U.S.
One result of this prescription onslaught?
It is believed to have contributed to a recent uptick in mortality rates.
Anupam Jena is a physician and a health care economist at Harvard.
Mortality rates in the U.S. have risen for the first time in 10 years.
Which is striking, considering that mortality rates have been falling for at least 100 years.
The U.S. rise has been concentrated among a few groups, particularly white, middle-aged men and women.
Among white men with a high school education or less, the death rate has risen an astonishing 22%.
And the attribution of these issues is in part due to opioids.
And this is a problem that was created by medicine.
And yet you certainly can't blame your profession for that because it's an effective drug when used well, correct?
Correct, yeah. And, you know,
like many drugs in medicine, they're effective in certain situations. So, for patients who
fall at home and break their hip and have a hip fracture, opioids for situations like that are
known to be effective pain relievers. Or in patients with cancer, particularly in cancer with bone
pain because of disease that has metastasized to the bone, opioids in that situation have been
shown to be highly effective in terms of reducing pain. But opioids for low back pain or headaches
or knee pain or hip pain or just chronic pain in general, opioids are not thought to be an effective strategy.
And yet we've seen the proliferation of their use in the last decade.
So how do you maximize the use of opioids when appropriate and minimize their overuse?
That's not easy. There are a lot of confounding factors, but it's hard to come up
with good prescribing protocols for pain relievers when you don't even have good measurement for pain.
Because we don't have any objective measures for actually figuring out what works,
we are necessarily in a realm where not just subjective assessment,
but also trial and error medicine is necessary to figure out what works.
And to that end, Weilu says, But also trial and error medicine is necessary to figure out what works.
And to that end, Weilu says,
We need to think about over-medication and under-medication as not two poles of the use of pain medicine.
Because then what we do is we kind of just whiplash like a pendulum.
We go from believing that under-medication is a problem to believing that over-medication is a problem.
What we need to do is to understand that both of these things can be a problem at the same time.
The American Medical Association, hoping to address this problem,
recently turned back the clock.
It recommended that pain be removed as a fifth vital sign. But how much
will that help? Anupam Jena again. As an economist, I think about supply and demand. So there's the
demand of patients, increasing demand for patients, by patients for opioids. Once you've put the power
in the hands of the patient, or just to call it what it is, in the hands of the consumer,
it can be hard to reclaim it.
So how did we get here?
Yeah, so pain management was really emerging as a recognizable and legitimate area of medical practice and care in the 1960s, early 1970s,
with the development of multidisciplinary pain centers.
Keith Whaloo again.
There was a general recognition that you needed more than just drugs to deal with people in chronic pain. You needed social workers, you needed surgeons, you needed
psychologists, you needed a wide range of others, as well as people with pharmacological expertise.
But those multidisciplinary pain centers were really expensive.
And so, you know, one of the economic trends since the 1980s with the rise of cost containment is to sort of see drugs as the cheapest and the fastest solution to our problem.
This coincided with a big shift in how drugs are marketed to the public.
Before the 1980s, the idea that you would see prescription drugs being advertised on television was laughable.
It emerges in the mid-1980s that, you know, we're sort of seeing government regulation as the problem and the market as the solution to our problems.
Zoloft, a prescription medicine, can help.
With Adger?
Prestor.
Cymbalta? And out of this era emerges this idea that people have the right to have the information at their disposal.
Increased risk of prostate cancer, worsening prostate symptoms, decreased sperm count, ankle, feet, or body swelling, enlarged or painful breast.
About prescription drugs and to bring that knowledge into the physician's office.
Ask your doctor.
That's between you and your doctor. In order to not so much demand, but to shape clinical decision-making.
Ask your doctor.
Ask your doctor.
Ask your doctor.
Call your doctor right away.
In fact, the aggressive marketing of OxyContin as a safe pain medication
led to criminal convictions for top executives at its manufacturer, Purdue Pharma,
for misleading the FDA, clinicians, and patients about its risks.
But just because one painkiller is declared risky
doesn't mean that consumers wouldn't demand other painkillers.
Because, as Keith Wailoo told us earlier,
We are a consumer society that believes in the power of a magic pill.
And once consumers gained more leverage in the medical realm,
guess whose opinions began to matter a lot more?
Yep, the consumers.
In the form of those patient satisfaction surveys
you fill out after a doctor's visit.
Yeah, it is a problem.
You know, that's the problem when we just measure things that are easy
to measure. That's Marty McCary, a surgical director and health policy scholar at Johns Hopkins.
By putting all this attention on customer satisfaction and consumer satisfaction or
patient satisfaction, we're creating a consumerist culture in health care. People come in, they want
an antibiotic for their kid,
and they don't care what your diagnosis or explanation is. They want to walk out with
that antibiotic prescription. Or you're in pain and you want that pain script. If the doctor is
under the microscope for their patient satisfaction scores, you can imagine the perverse incentive
here. Indeed, a 2012 paper in the Journal of the American Medical Association
pointed to an unintended consequence of this perverse incentive. Physicians who do not comply
with patient requests, the authors wrote, may be the recipients of poor ratings on patient
satisfaction scores, possibly resulting in emotional, financial, and professional penalties.
So imagine this. You are a doctor and your patient asks,
maybe by name, for a prescription painkiller.
You may think the patient doesn't really need it.
You may, in fact, be worried they'll abuse it,
maybe even sell it.
But if that consumer has the ability
to punish you professionally,
well, you might just write the script.
They respond to demands. They cope with their job by, you might just write the script. They respond to demands.
They cope with their job by, you know, giving an angry patient what they want, not what they need because they have to see five patients in an hour.
I can tell you emphatically, doctors are getting crushed with record rates of burnout due to increasing overhead, higher malpractice premiums, declining pay, lowering Medicare reimbursement, being forced to see more patients in a single hour, corporate medicine.
On top of all that, McCary says, there is a brutal paradox.
Patient satisfaction is not a helpful metric when
it comes to measuring health outcomes. Well, at least not helpful in the direction you might
think it would be. A 2012 study found that the most satisfied patients had higher rates of
hospitalization and higher mortality rates. Why? The authors suggest that more satisfied patients may request more discretionary treatments, which may increase the likelihood of adverse effects.
So, you know, it's a big problem.
You know, what's important to a patient when they come to a doctor?
The doctor's patient satisfaction score?
Well, that's a piece of the doctor's quality. But really, what you're
interested in is the doctor's judgment and the doctor's skill and the doctor's ability to empathize.
Those are the sort of metrics, Marty McCary argues, that will help doctors treat patients
better. So coming up on Freakonomics Radio, now all you've got to do is collect all the data on doctors' judgment, skill, and empathy, right? But again, not so easy,
in part because of the sheer volume of that data. We are doing more than we've ever done before.
We are doing more procedures, giving more medications, hospitalizing more patients, diagnosing more
things than we ever have in the history of medicine. Also, why feedback for doctors is so
important, especially for doctors who've been practicing a while. What we find is that if you
happen to be treated by a doctor who is 10 years or 15 years out of residency, your mortality within 30 days of being hospitalized is higher.
Marty McCary, the surgeon and health policy scholar we've been speaking with, is a big advocate for medical reform.
First step, improving the feedback loop.
That is, what we know and way too often what we don't know about what actually works.
I think the number one problem is we don't measure performance.
We don't measure the outcomes of patients in health for 99% of the healthcare that's delivered.
McCary might be exaggerating a bit, but still, how can this be?
When you go in for medical treatment, don't the health professionals who treat you find out if their intervention actually worked?
The short answer is, often no.
But the longer answer is much worse.
The longer answer is that not only do medical interventions often not work,
medical interventions will sometimes kill you.
Marty McCary and co-author Michael Daniel recently published a study arguing that the
third leading cause of death in the U.S. after heart disease and cancer was medical error.
I'm going to say that again.
The third leading cause of death in the U.S.,
accounting for 10% of deaths annually, is medical error.
How can this be?
Are doctors and nurses showing up for work stoned out of their skulls?
Are they sneaking into hospital rooms at night
and smothering their most annoying patients?
Are they surreptitiously removing healthy organs to sell them on the black market?
If only.
That would make the problem so much easier to solve.
Why are so many deaths the result of medical error?
Well, I think anybody that practices medicine knows that medical errors are a function of the amount of things that we do in health care.
The amount of things meaning what?
We are doing more than we've ever done before. We are doing more procedures,
giving more medications, hospitalizing more patients, diagnosing more things than we ever
have in the history of medicine.
Right here today in the United States, we have the most medicalized, the most diagnosed
population in the history of the world.
In economic terms, you could say there's both an oversupply and an overdemand of healthcare,
since the supply and demand are both fueled by the setup of our health
care insurance system.
Patients who buy expensive insurance want to get their money's worth and may overconsume,
just as you might overconsume at an all-you-can-eat restaurant.
And doctors who make money primarily when they do stuff may tend to do more stuff.
When we're doing all of this stuff, it makes you wonder,
does that mean we're also making mistakes
proportional to the amount of stuff that we do?
Well, that sounds scary.
So first of all, I don't want to scare people out there.
Most doctors are doing the right thing and always will.
All right, but how can it be that the people we entrust to heal us, the people who've
worked their entire adult lives to learn how to heal us, may sometimes be killing us? To get to
that answer, you first have to understand that for decades, we've been making a sort of clerical error.
Our research found the methodological flaw in our country's national health statistics.
We use a billing code system to tally causes of death from death certificates.
And people don't just die from billing codes.
They die from medical mistakes, communication breakdowns, overdoses, fragmented care, closed insurance networks, preventable complications, unnecessary treatments.
And if you look collectively at this group of problems, let me call it medical care gone wrong,
it's got a significant burden in society. But those complications and oversights and errors,
McCary says, seldom wind up on the death certificate. When you fill out the death certificate,
you have to list the reason the patient died,
both the direct reason and the underlying reason the patient died.
Well, we all knew what the real reason was,
but you can't put that on somebody's death certificate.
Why not?
When somebody experiences a fatal mistake,
their heart stops, and then you do CPR, then you pronounce them dead.
What do you put on the death certificate when it says, what was the cause of the patient's immediate death?
And that's really what got us thinking, and that's what led to this study.
Because, you know, you end up putting cardiovascular arrest. And then it turns out what we put on the death certificates populates our country's national health statistics. So when the government puts out every year,
these are the most common causes of death in the United States. And by the way, that list is a big
deal. That list informs all of our research funding. It informs all of our public
health campaigns in America. That list is a big deal. So, you know, you realize we're misclassifying
medical mistakes as other causes and that medical mistakes don't even show up on the list.
So McCary and his colleagues got hold of a mountain of data and started digging.
Well, we basically looked at the best available research on the topic
from the New England Journal of Medicine and Health Affairs
and a big Medicare analysis and something called the OIG report.
It was a government report that was independent.
And what'd they find?
Before I tell you that, let me tell you this.
There is a famous report put out by the Institute of Medicine in 1999 that set a benchmark for death by medical error. That report estimated there were between 44,000 and 98,000 deaths annually in the U.S. due to medical error. Those are obviously large and frightening numbers, so large and frightening
that I've heard many, many medical professionals insist those numbers had to be way too high.
So what number did McCary come up with? At least 250,000 deaths every year in the U.S.
due to medical error, a quarter of a million people.
Before we get into the errors themselves, let's think for just a
minute about how the story of those deaths was hidden in the data. As we've noted throughout
these three episodes on bad medicine, a lot of what we take as factual and empirical within
medicine often isn't very empirical. That includes how data from clinical trials
are manipulated or misinterpreted. And as Marty McCary argues, it includes how the cause of death
is categorized. And that's why he's pushing for a fundamental reform to require that doctors,
when they fill out a death certificate, specifically indicate if a medical error was involved.
Because how do you
solve a problem if you don't even acknowledge the problem? And with medical errors, the problem is
both deep and broad. Medical errors have a complex taxonomy. That, again, is the Harvard doctor and
economist Anupam Jena. But for someone like me, I would just break them into two categories.
There are errors of diagnosis, and then there's errors of commission. When a patient has surgery
on the wrong leg, or when a patient is given an antibiotic despite it being well documented in the
medical record that he or she has an allergy to that
medication. Or when a patient receives a dose of insulin that is five times as much what it should
have been because someone couldn't read a doctor's handwriting in the chart. There are errors that
occur in the hospital because of poor hygiene and infectious disease management where people get
hospital-acquired infections.
I've got to think that those errors of commission are extraordinarily rare, though, yes?
Well, you know, you would hope so, but it turns out that they're not.
The Johns Hopkins study is actually more about those second set of errors.
So when I hear that, I, as a potential patient, I say to myself, self, unless you are bleeding heavily or unconscious,
just stay away from every doctor and certainly every hospital.
Well, hopefully if you're unconscious, you won't be making that decision.
Well, you know, I think you also have to take it a step further and say,
okay, well, when a diagnostic error occurs, what is the implication of that? Does it mean that a diagnosis is ultimately made later in a safe and effective
way? Well, that's much less alarming than if an incorrect diagnosis by a doctor leads to,
let's say, a biopsy that then causes longer-term problems. The only thing that I'd mention there
is that this is not just an issue of decisions that are made by individuals. In fact, most of the thinking on this issue
points to system-level problems that lead to diagnostic errors.
The problem is a system problem. That's Marty McCary again. Remember, here's what McCary argued
earlier. I think the number one problem is we don't measure performance.
In other words, the medical system often fails to collect useful feedback.
Simple data, simple transparency.
McCary himself is a surgeon.
99% of people that have surgery in the United States
go home and no one documents or keeps track at a systematic level, that is
national or regional or hospital level, how the patient does. At six months, are you glad that
you had your knee surgery done? At six months after hip surgery, are you walking again? Or
a year after weight loss surgery, what is your weight today? We don't keep track of those
things in healthcare for most of the procedures or treatments that we do. And the problem is that
how can you really come up with a quality metric if nobody's tracking it?
This doesn't mean doctors never check in with their patients, but the system
simply isn't designed to capture robust follow-up data. Well, there's a follow-up visit. You know,
we'll scribble some note, patient's doing well, incision has healed nicely. Who's actually
measuring the real patient-centered outcomes six months later or a year later. That is a giant opportunity in healthcare to fix the
system by creating a marketplace centered around value, not just around quantity.
That, of course, would require incentives for doctors that reward preventive care and maintenance rather than just interventions.
McCary points to a few areas of medical care where patient outcomes are well-tracked.
But it's only for a small sliver of medical care. It's like
heart surgery and cystic fibrosis outcomes.
In those cases, he says, money plays a big role.
It's almost this hodgepodge of conditions where there's been
leaders and good funding or foundation support. But absent good follow-up data and absent good
feedback in general, it's hard to tell what works and what doesn't. Of course, this is true for
anything, not just medicine. But with medicine, the stakes are high. And if you're the patient,
the stakes are practically infinite.
You are putting your life in someone's care, and you only have one life as far as we know.
Other choices might seem hard, like which house to buy, how to invest your money, how
to pick a career or a college major.
But if you've got a serious health concern, your choice of treatment and doctor is an existential choice.
So given a choice between two doctors, let's say, one fresh out of medical school and the other with 15 years experience, which one do you go for?
I think one question that any patient would have when they see a doctor is, how much experience does my doctor have?
That's Anupam Jena again. Most patients, he says, like the idea of a gray-haired physician.
Because the gray-haired physician has more experience, he's seen more patients
like me, and he's just going to take better care of me.
That makes sense, doesn't it?
But the challenge is that there hasn't been a lot of actual
high-quality evidence to assess that issue.
So Jena and some colleagues set out to gather some evidence, but it wasn't so simple as comparing patient outcomes for
experienced doctors and newer doctors. One problem that you're going to run into is the notion that
more experienced physicians will take care of sicker patients. And how do you get around that
issue? The way that we've tried to get around
it is to focus on a very specific group of doctors that are called hospitalists. Hospitalists are
internal medicine doctors who focus on hospital-based care. Jenna is himself a hospitalist.
Because of his research and teaching, he only sees patients for a month or two each year, but he's familiar with the hospitalist setup.
And the unique thing about these types of doctors is that they tend to work
either shifts or scheduled work.
For example, I might work for two weeks and then my colleague is on for two weeks
and then her colleague is on for two weeks.
Which is nice because we can isolate the effect of you, correct?
Exactly. So patients more or less end up getting quasi-randomized to physicians with different
characteristics. So for example, if you happen to get hospitalized in the first week of May,
you may be treated by a group of doctors who on average have five years less experience
than if you happen to get hospitalized in the second week of May. And we can basically see what happens if a patient happens to be treated by
a doctor who is 20 years out of residency versus five years out of residency. And what we find is
that if you happen to be treated by a doctor who is 10 years or 15 years out of residency,
your mortality within 30 days of being hospitalized is higher.
Just to be clear, if you happen to draw a more experienced doctor, you are more likely to die.
And so it does suggest that more experience actually could have a negative effect on outcomes.
And what's your best explanation for why that's the case?
So I think the most likely explanation is two things. One is that the field of medicine is
constantly evolving, and there's always new knowledge, new evidence emerging both in terms of
how to make better diagnoses and what are the right treatments for particular patients. And as you get further and
further away from residency, I think what happens is that the knowledge that you had as a resident,
which is a time when you spend 80 to 100 hours per week in the hospital, that knowledge gets
somewhat ingrained in you. New knowledge isn't picked up as rapidly. So I think what happens
is that older physicians are just less
up-to-date, if you will. I'll give you one caveat. We don't see this effect among high-volume doctors,
doctors who are seeing a lot of patients. And so what that suggests is that if you're an older
doctor who is seeing a lot of patients, you are protected from this adverse effect, which makes
sense. Unfortunately, some people come out of medical school or training thinking,
all right, I've mastered this body of knowledge or this skill set,
and I'm good to go for the next 50 years of practice.
Marty McCary again.
And the reality is, even as a tenured faculty at Johns Hopkins doing complex surgery
in a group that does, you know, the four of us do the most pancreatic surgery
of any group in the country, I'm still learning every day. And my senior, senior partner,
who's about to retire, he's still learning, you know, the year before he stops operating
every day. So medicine's a career of learning. And I think the more feedback we can get at any level.
That said, Anupam Jena's research shows that surgeons are among the subset of physicians
who do seem to improve with experience.
There's the thought that muscle skill experience in the surgical field over time improves outcomes,
and we find that as well.
But outside of surgery, when you're thinking about the care of patients that requires a lot of cognitive skill and being up to date on current medications and diagnoses, we actually find that over time, older doctors do worse.
So someone like you, so I don't mean to turn this into attack on you, but someone like you who, A, medical school is getting further in the rearview mirror as it is for every doctor.
But additionally, you're only practicing for a couple of months out of the year, not full time. So I would think that
you are a downright danger to your patients. How is it that you're not?
No comment. Well, you know, so usually when I work on service, I'm paired with someone
who is a full time clinician educator. And there's a huge
difference in the amount of knowledge. And it's very humbling to see that.
Jenna's research looks specifically at physician experience as it relates to patient outcomes. But
there's another angle to consider when we talk about experience in the medical realm, especially
if you're interested in reform. We have supposedly entered the era of evidence-based medicine.
This is still relatively new ground.
The reality was that what we were practicing was something called eminence-based medicine.
That's the physician and researcher Vinay Prasad.
It was where the preponderance of medical practice was driven by
really charismatic and thoughtful, probably to some degree, leaders in medicine. And, you know,
medical practice was based on bits and scraps of evidence, anecdotes, bias, preconceived notions,
and probably a lot of, you know, psychological traps. As outdated as that sounds now,
keep in mind that a lot of our institutions,
including medical institutions,
are still eminence-based,
which is to say, in many institutions,
many big decisions are made by the highest-ranking people who tend to have the most experience.
And people with a lot of experience
tend to have fixed views on things.
They're attracted to the status quo or some minor variation of it because that's what they know.
It's what they trust and believe in.
Also, if you want to be a bit uncharitable for a moment, we might argue that the status quo is additionally appealing to senior people because, well, because they got theirs already. They've got their job, their status, their salary.
And all that disruption that reformers like to talk about, it's messy.
It's time consuming.
It's a pain in the neck.
Like I said, that isn't a charitable view, but I'm afraid it's not wrong either.
Change can be hard.
Uncertainty can be scary.
True improvement can be elusive. But one thing that's so inspiring about all the people we've been speaking with for this series is how they embrace the notion that it's okay to challenge the very institution that you've devoted your career to. In fact, with medicine, it's a requirement. I think back to something we heard from Philip Makoviak, the doctor who unraveled the true story of 98.6. at what we're doing today and ask themselves, what was grandpa thinking of when he did that
and believed that? Look at us now, how good we are. Why weren't they that good? And they'll have
to learn all over again that science is imperfect. And to maintain a healthy skepticism about everything we believe and do in life in general,
but in the medical profession in particular.
So what happens now?
There's a long list and reasons to be excited.
Marty McCary is particularly enthusiastic about new ideas for collecting better patient feedback.
Well, Washington State has a really neat program in select hospitals.
After you have a certain operation, you get a text question.
It says, how functional are you after your surgery?
Here's a scale from one to five.
Are you glad that you had the procedure done?
Do you feel that the alternatives were adequately explained to you before the procedure?
And those few questions populate a quality database, and they keep track of outcomes.
But that's a rare thing in health care.
We need to be doing that for every procedure in the United States, everyone in the United States that has robotic surgery or a tonsillectomy or gallbladder removal or heart surgery or colon or whatever it is, should have some data that follows up and allows us to make conclusions about where we can do better, what's working and what's not working. You know, we just sort of discovered or rediscovered, if you will, recently that we don't need to
treat appendicitis with surgery.
You can come in with early appendicitis and we can give you antibiotics and it works more
than 60% of the time.
That's cool.
And we probably could have learned that if we had the right databases to look at those
conclusions to say, hey, of those patients that refused surgery
and we just gave them antibiotics for the last half century, how did they do?
McCary says a lot of this change is being driven by players outside the medical establishment.
It's happening really led by startup companies.
The startup community in America is doing great things in healthcare and they're starting
to say, hey, can we track how well someone does after surgery?
How is your experience?
How is your outcome?
How is the care that you received after whatever procedure you had done?
And over enough time and with enough patients, they're going to be actually able to make conclusions about quality using firsthand patient data.
But given the complexities of medicine, McCary warns, we shouldn't expect quick fixes.
First of all, it's different in every area of medicine, it's different. If you're a cardiologist or an OB or a psychiatrist, you can't simply implement strategies to improve
quality and standardized care in the same way.
In my own field of surgery, we believe there's something that can be done called benchmarking.
That is, we can see how we stand as a surgical
group, as individual surgeons, relative to other surgeons in our region and nationally that take on
similarly complex cases. And that's why we've proposed, and we have a grant to do this
nationally, we want the doctors' associations to come up with a metric of performance.
We want to apply it to all the doctors in that specialty.
And then we want to just share the data with the doctors individually in a confidential, peer-to-peer, civil fashion.
This is where you stand.
This is where the rest of the country stands.
And we're not making a judgment.
We just want to share with you your data.
The point, McCary says, is improvement, not punishment.
As a matter of fact, firing people for making mistakes in hospitals is the absolute wrong approach.
We need to learn from our mistakes, not send a message that if you have a concern or speak up or you do a mistake, we're going to kick you out.
Doctors are already in a tough place, under attack for nearly every quarter,
including for these past few episodes for Economics Radio.
And the fear of making a mistake or what may be construed as a mistake is already so high that doctors practice way too much defensive medicine,
that is,
tests and procedures primarily meant to avoid a malpractice suit.
A 2010 study found that U.S. hospitals and doctors spent about $45 billion a year on
defensive medicine.
But if the present looks occasionally bleak, the good news is there's plenty of optimism
about the future of medicine,
as we learned from the variety of clever and motivated people we've spoken to for this series.
Where science and medicine is going in the future is to more and more precision medicine
so that we can get closer to an autonomous and individualized diagnosis.
That's Teresa Woodruff, a professor at Northwestern and director of the
Women's Health Research Institute. When someone comes in with a cancer, there's a set of protocols,
there's the way we treat in general populations, but we can't tell the specific outcome for that
individual. How will they tolerate that drug? Will it clear the circulation faster for one
individual versus another, which means it might be
more efficacious or less efficacious on an individual basis? Those are some of that precision
medicine that eventually we have to get to. So I promise not to hold you to the prediction I'm
about to ask you to make, but in terms of precision medicine, like you're talking about, whether it's diagnostic, prescriptive, whatever it is, I just want to know what kind of time frame you see for that being a real practical everyday thing.
Is it more like two to 20 years or is it more like 50 to 100 years from now or somewhere in between or somewhere beyond?
I think science is becoming even more catalytic.
So it's going faster and faster and faster. Breakthroughs are coming about every day. And I suspect within the next
10 to 15 years, we'll really understand enough to get away from radiation and chemotherapy.
And that eventuality is the promise of basic science and medicine. And what that means is that every day as we discover more and more
fundamental biology about cells or about animals or about the way systems work, that translates
into better and better medicines that ultimately will change the patient who is seen tomorrow
versus the way the patient that's seen today. There's a lot of promise right now that in the post-genomic world
that some personalized medicine or precision medicine
will allow us to do that much better.
Jeremy Green again, a physician and historian of medicine at Johns Hopkins.
Although at present that's still highly promissory
except for in a few very well-circumstrized cases.
Some people will or won't respond to a certain drug for
hypertension. And you fish around, you try one, you try another, and then you find a cocktail
that works for them. Other people will develop allergies to specific medicines. And then you're
constrained in ways that you hadn't originally anticipated. I think the game is going to be the
same game, which is a game where, if we're really honest, perhaps a lot of the low-hanging fruit in medicine has been plucked, some of the great interventions.
And that, again, is Vinay Prasad.
Now it's a matter of sorting out interventions with medium to small benefits.
But that's okay.
But with a medium and a small benefit, you really have to be sure that you can minimize bias.
You can minimize the role of your own sort of preconceived notions,
and that's why we need careful randomized studies. But the biggest reason for optimism,
Prasad argues, doesn't have to do with better evidence or better protocols or better medicine,
per se. It has to do with better thinking, and that, he says, is happening.
I see it every day in medicine. I see it in movements like the British Medical Journal's really commitment to evidence, to transparency, to data sharing.
I see it in JAMA Internal Medicine's commitment to knowing when more is harmful, when too much medicine is harmful.
And that took many years for us to realize. are increasingly allowing people with diverse points of view in medicine, contrarians, perhaps even like myself,
to write articles in really important journals so that they can be read and thought of by other people.
So I think we're at a moment where, you know, we're much more open to different ideas on how to move medicine forward.
Here's to moving forward. And here's to your health.
Thanks so much for listening to this three-parter on bad medicine.
Coming up next time on Freakonomics Radio, we're back with a brand new episode.
Suppose you could play a game where with 99% probability, I'll pay you a million dollars.
But with 1% probability, I'm going to put a bullet in your head.
Would you take that bet?
How much brain damage do I have?
The question is, would the risk be acceptable?
And in my opinion, this study says no, it would not be acceptable.
Listen, I love my time in the NFL.
I have these amazing
experiences, but I'm
really excited to focus on
mathematics and what I'm doing at MIT.
And please leave me alone.
The NFL lineman, John
Urschel, thought he could quietly
announce his retirement at age 26.
Nice and quiet,
like a thief in the night.
But when an NFL player is simultaneously getting a Ph.D. at MIT, people tend to pay attention.
There's no story here. There's nothing going on.
Especially when your abrupt retirement comes two days after a report on long-term brain damage in the NFL. I think the longer that we sort of deny it and sort of work around it and make excuses, it's just going to delay bringing a lot of good research to this problem.
Risk and reward in the NFL.
That's next time on Freakonomics Radio.
Freakonomics Radio is produced by WNYC Studios and Dubner Productions.
This episode was produced by Stephanie Tam.
Our staff also includes Allison Hockenberry, Merritt Jacob, Greg Wazowski, Eliza Lambert,
Emma Morgenstern, Harry Huggins, and Brian Gutierrez.
You can subscribe to Freakonomics Radio on Apple Podcasts, Stitcher, or wherever you get your podcasts.
You should also visit Freakonomics.com,
where you can find the transcript to every episode
and links to the underlying research and books mentioned in our podcast.
We can also be found on Twitter, Facebook,
or via email at radio at Freakonomics.com.
Thank you for listening. you