Freakonomics Radio - The U.S. Is Just Different — So Let’s Stop Pretending We’re Not (Ep. 469 Replay)

Episode Date: August 4, 2022

We often look to other countries for smart policies on education, healthcare, infrastructure, etc. But can a smart policy be simply transplanted into a country as culturally unusual (and as supremely ...WEIRD) as America?

Transcript
Discussion (0)
Starting point is 00:00:00 Hey there, it's Stephen Dubner. Today on the show, we are revisiting the first episode in a series we made last year about American culture and just how unusual it is. We have updated facts and figures as necessary. Also, we need your help for a new episode we're producing about bosses who have business degrees. We want to hear your stories specifically about how your company or department changed when someone with an MBA showed up to start running things. Did the situation get better, worse, weirder? How? You can send an email
Starting point is 00:00:40 to radio at Freakonomics.com or even better, a voice memo. And we might use your voice on the show. Please record your voice memo in a quiet place. Keep your mouth close to the phone. Please include your name, where you live and what you do. And given the nature of this topic, if you need to stay anonymous, we will honor that. You can send the voice memo to that same address, radio at freakonomics.com. Thanks in advance.
Starting point is 00:01:09 And now I hope you enjoy this episode. How much time have you spent thinking about what makes America, America? It may help if you're not originally from here. When was that moment that America became the most American America it could possibly be? Have you ever noticed how Americans are not stupid? I was so excited to be in America, I couldn't sleep. The comedians John Oliver, Hannah Gadsby, and Kumail Nanjiani all grew up outside the U.S. When you are trying to understand the nature of something, an outside view can be extremely helpful. Did you know there is an entire academic field called cross-cultural psychology?
Starting point is 00:02:04 It's a subfield of psychology that tries to understand what's universal, what's similar, and what's culture-specific. Michelle Gelfand is one of the premier practitioners of cross-cultural psychology. After 25 years at the University of Maryland, she moved to the business school at Stanford. Why the business school? We're fiercely interdisciplinary. We do lab experiments, field experiments, computational modeling. We bring in neuroscience to understand all things cultural. You might think that someone who studies cross-cultural psychology also grew up abroad,
Starting point is 00:02:37 or at least in some big city with a melting pot vibe. But no. I grew up on Long Island. You have to pronounce it right. Long Island, New York is the birthplace of the American suburb. And I had that typical, you know, New Yorker view of the world, the cartoon where there's like New York and there's New Jersey and then there's, you know, the rest of the world. When it was time for college, Gelfand went all the way to upstate New York, Colgate University. She was majoring in pre-med, but then she took a semester abroad to London. I really had a lot of culture shock.
Starting point is 00:03:12 I was on the phone with my dad, and I said, you know, it's really crazy all the differences between the UK and the US. Now, keep in mind, this was London, English-speaking London, not Uzbekistan or Botswana, even Mexico. Still, Gelfand's horizons were suddenly expanded, and her curiosity was triggered. The next day, I booked a trip to Egypt. I was there, and later on, and travels the Middle East, and working on a kibbutz and elsewhere, that I started recognizing this really powerful force of culture that was incredibly important but really invisible. I came back to Colgate and I shifted from pre-med into what turned into a career of cross-cultural psychology. In 1990, when Gelfand was a graduate student, she followed the news as Iraq invaded Kuwait. U.S. President George H.W. Bush
Starting point is 00:04:05 made clear to Iraq's Saddam Hussein that this wouldn't stand. But Bush also wanted to avoid going to war with Iraq. I was watching this negotiation between Tariq Aziz and James Baker. Baker was Bush's secretary of state. Aziz was Hussein's deputy prime minister.
Starting point is 00:04:24 President Bush had framed these negotiations as going an extra mile for peace. And there was discussions in the cross-cultural psychology community about how James Baker's kind of unemotional communication style was received as, this is not so serious, in terms of Tariq Aziz's understanding of Americans' intentions. But it was serious. The negotiations didn't work out. The U.S. assembled a coalition of allies. Just two hours ago. And invaded Iraq.
Starting point is 00:04:57 Allied air forces began an attack on military targets in Iraq and Kuwait. These attacks continue as I speak. And I thought if these kinds of cultural differences are happening at the highest levels, we better start understanding this stuff. The reason we reached out to Michelle Gelfand is that I want to understand this stuff better too. Let me give you a little background. On many Freakonomics Radio episodes, we'll hear about some idea or policy that works well elsewhere in the world but hasn't taken root in the U.S. In Germany, for instance, labor unions often have a representative on company boards, which can radically change the dynamic between companies and employees. We have interviewed dozens of academic researchers about lowering health care
Starting point is 00:05:46 costs or improving access to child care or building smarter infrastructure or creating a more equitable economy. And so often they'll just point at some other country on the map. They'll say the Scandinavians have great childcare and family leave policies. Or they'll say, China has built more high-speed rail in the past few years than the U.S. has even thought about. So naturally, the next question is, can't the U.S. just borrow these Scandinavian and Chinese and German ideas and slap them on top of the American way of doing things? The answer to that is usually usually no, you can't. Why not?
Starting point is 00:06:29 Because for all the so-called globalization of the past half century or so, the U.S. still differs from other countries in many ways, historically, politically, and yes, culturally. Culturally, maybe more than anything. One of the defining features of Americanism is our so-called rugged individualism. You might even call it wild individualism. It is part of our founding DNA. This individualism has produced tremendous forward progress and entrepreneurial energy, but it can make life harder for the
Starting point is 00:07:05 millions of Americans who aren't so entrepreneurial or rugged or individualistic. The American model is among the most successful and envied models in the history of the world, but it is also a tremendous outlier. You can see this on many dimensions. How we work and travel. How we mate and marry. How we care for our children and our elderly. How we police. How we conceive the relationship between the individual and the state. Even how we manage death. So it's hard to simply transplant another country's model for education or healthcare, no matter how well it might seem to fit.
Starting point is 00:07:48 This realization is what led to today's episode of Freakonomics Radio. We'll call it, The U.S. is Very Different from Other Countries, so Let's Stop Pretending It's Not. It is the first in a series of episodes where we'll look at different pieces of that difference. Today, an overview of the cultural differences. It is the first in a series of episodes where we'll look at different pieces of that difference. Today, an overview of the cultural differences. We will learn which countries are tight, which are loose, and why. We'll find out what it means to be weird, although not weird in the way you're thinking.
Starting point is 00:08:21 And yes, we will talk about what makes America, America. At least as seen through the eyes of Kumail Nanjiani, who was born in Pakistan. I was so excited to be in America, I couldn't sleep. My uncle's like, hey, I have something to show you. My first day in America, he showed me the Macy's Thanksgiving Day Parade. And I was like, this is every day in America! As advertised. This is Freakonomics Radio, the podcast that explores the hidden side of everything.
Starting point is 00:09:12 With your host, Stephen Dubner. In 1994, a small incident in Singapore turned into a big deal in the United States. Michelle Gelfand again. This American teenager from Ohio, Michael Fay, was in Singapore and was arrested and charged with various counts of vandalism and other shenanigans. Michael Fay wasn't a tourist. He was living in Singapore with his family, attending an American school. And it caused a real international crisis because the Singapore government gave him what was then classic punishment, which was caning. Caning as in a spanking, basically, on the bare buttocks
Starting point is 00:09:43 with a half-inch thick rattan cane. In the U.S., various newspapers covered the story. The TV networks, too. A young American has been sentenced to a caning for an act of vandalism. And it got the attention of President Clinton. This is the first I've heard of it. I'll look into it. Clinton went to negotiate to say, hey, this is just totally inappropriate, this punishment. And Singaporean government's reaction was, look, this is our culture. If you're violating the social order, you't have to be said. It was, and your culture, your American culture, is very different. At this point, we should probably define terms. What is culture?
Starting point is 00:10:36 Yeah, it's a classic question. It was back in grad school that Michelle Gelfand first asked herself this question. I was planning to become a cross-cultural trainer to work at the State Department and train people to understand culture. This interest goes back to those negotiations between Jim Baker and Tariq Aziz. But Gelfand saw an even bigger question. How can you understand culture if you don't know exactly what it is? So what is it? It's like that story of two fish where they're swimming along and they pass another fish who says, hey, boys, how's the water? And they're like, what the heck is water?
Starting point is 00:11:11 Here's another culture metaphor, another watery one from the Dutch culture scholar Kertjaan Hofstede. Culture is the ripples on the ocean of human nature. It's the tiny differences in sociality. Culture is about if you are a part of a society, you're like one drop in the Mississippi River. You may decide to go another way, but that doesn't make the river change. Scholars in this realm have a general agreement on what culture is and what it's not. Culture is not genetics or biology or individual characteristics. It's more about how individuals are acted upon by the people and institutions around them. And they often don't even realize they're being acted upon. Culture is information stored in people's heads that got there via some kind of learning process, usually social learning. That is Joe Henrich, a professor of evolutionary biology.
Starting point is 00:12:07 And this can include motivations, heuristics, biases, beliefs. It's trying to include all the stuff that we acquire as a consequence of growing up in different environments and contrast that with things like our sex drive, which doesn't seem to be acquired by observing others. So culture is about values, beliefs, absorbed ideas and behaviors. But here's the thing about culture. It can be really hard to measure, which is probably why we don't hear all that much about the science of culture.
Starting point is 00:12:38 When something is not easily measured, it often gets talked about in mushy or ideological terms. Michelle Gelfand wasn't interested in that. She did want to measure culture and how it differs from place to place. She decided that the key difference, the right place to start measuring, was whether the culture in a given country is tight or loose. All cultures have social norms, these unwritten rules that guide our behavior on a daily basis. But some cultures strictly abide by their norms.
Starting point is 00:13:10 They're what we call tight cultures. And other cultures are more loose, they're more permissive. Are you the creator of the looseness-tightness system for looking at culture? Well, we can look back to Herodotus. He contrasts places like Egypt that had strict rules for authority and gender impurity with the Persians, who, using my terminology, he would have said that they were quite loose. Later on, fast forward to Pietro Pelto, who's an anthropologist. He wrote a paper about it, and this paper was basically sitting in the shelves of libraries for many years. And when I started to work with Harry Triandus, who was one of the founders of
Starting point is 00:13:43 the field, I thought, wow, this is a super interesting construct, so let's try to measure this. Gelfand and several colleagues undertook a massive research project, interviewing some 7,000 people from 33 countries on five continents. They made sure to include a variety of ages, occupations, religions, social and economic classes. Here is one of the questions they asked. If someone acts in an inappropriate way, will others strongly disapprove in this country? Here's another. Are there very clear expectations for how people should act in most situations? In 2018, Gelfand published a book of these findings called Rulemakers, Rulebreakers, How Tight and
Starting point is 00:14:27 Loose Cultures Wire Our World. Tight cultures, she writes, are usually found in South and East Asia, the Middle East, and in European countries of Nordic and Germanic origin. Loose cultures tend to be found in English-speaking countries, as well as Latin American, Latin European, and formerly communist cultures. The United States, you may not be surprised to learn, is on the loose end of the spectrum, although not in the top five. The five loosest countries, according to this analysis, were Ukraine, Estonia, Hungary, Israel, and the Netherlands. The Netherlands, despite its
Starting point is 00:15:06 Germanic roots, Australia and Brazil are also loose. The five tightest countries are Pakistan, Malaysia, India, South Korea, and our old friend Singapore. China, Japan, and Turkey are also tight. Now, let's pull back and make an important point. Labeling a given country tight or loose is an overall aggregate measurement. Within countries, there is, of course, enormous variation. There are plenty of looser people in tight countries and vice versa. But remember what Hofstede told us. You're like one drop in the Mississippi River. You may decide to go another way, but that doesn't make the river change. I asked Michelle Gelfand to talk about why
Starting point is 00:15:51 a given country is loose or tight. In cross-cultural psychology, we study how ecological and historical factors cause the evolution of differences. And there's large differences around the world, for example, on how much cultures are exposed to chronic threat. And that really can help explain some variation, not all, but some variation in norms and values. Chronic threat, meaning a country is prone your nation been invaded over the last hundred years? Groups that tend to have threat tend to develop stricter rules to coordinate. So between not having been historically a terrible recipient of viruses, and also by dint of having an ocean on either side of us, etc., etc., and being a really big and really rich country, it sounds like the U.S. must have one of the lowest inherent threat levels.
Starting point is 00:16:50 We've had our share of threat, but just not chronic threat. Compared to other countries, including places like Japan, Singapore, Germany, we can afford to be more permissive. When I look at the loosest country in the data, I see Ukraine. And I think, holy cow, Ukraine is surrounded by threat, including its next door neighbor, Russia. The data suggests that those countries in Eastern Europe are extremely loose, almost normalist, we might say, because after the fall of the Soviet Union, these countries did a pendulum shift. And that happens a lot. What we saw in Egypt was very similar.
Starting point is 00:17:28 We had a very tight social order. When they took up Mubarak, this went to the opposite extreme, to almost enemy normalcy. As for the U.S., Gelfand says we are not only loose, but that for the most part, we've been getting progressively looser. We analyzed shifts in tightness over 200 years. says we are not only loose, but that for the most part, we've been getting progressively looser. We analyzed shifts in tightness over 200 years. We developed these linguistic dictionaries to analyze language reflective of tight and loose in newspapers and books. Tight words like restrain, comply, adhere, enforce, as compared to words like allow and leeway, flexibility, you know, empower. And we can see a strong trend that looseness has increased over the last 200 years. But even a loose country will tighten up when a threat arises.
Starting point is 00:18:15 Like during 9-11, during world wars, we see increases in tightness. During the Cold War, we had a lot of struggles with tightening during COVID, clearly. Gelfand says the countries that were most aggressive in trying to contain COVID tended to be tighter countries. Singapore, for instance. Wearing masks is a way of life now in Singapore. For the last few months, the city-state has seen just a handful of COVID-19 cases. Tightness and compliance would seem to go hand in hand, but there's more than compliance going on here.
Starting point is 00:18:50 Listen to the Dean of the Public Health School at the National University of Singapore. We have a tradition of having national campaigns to galvanize people to proceed in a common direction. And I think this community spiritedness has been built in us since we were very young. Michelle Gelfand and several co-authors recently published a study in The Lancet about how COVID played out in loose versus tight cultures. Controlling for a variety of other factors, they found that looser countries, the U.S., Brazil, Italy, and Spain, have had roughly five times the number of COVID cases and nearly nine times as many deaths as tighter countries.
Starting point is 00:19:34 But let's look at the pandemic from a different angle. Which country led the way in producing the most effective COVID-19 vaccines. Coming up after the break, what other benefits come with looseness? And what can the Muppets teach us about American culture? People who went out to California, I would say they were those kind of chaos Muppets because they were risk-seeking.
Starting point is 00:20:02 We'll be right back. We've been talking today about what makes America so American. The cross-cultural psychologist Michelle Gelfand has been explaining the differences between tighter countries like Singapore and Germany and looser countries like the U.S. Tightness may produce compliance, especially during a public health emergency like COVID-19, but looseness can drive innovation and creativity. The U.S. is one of the most creative places on the planet. Like you saw in the U.S. trying to locate COVID in sewage. That's a crazy creative solution to try to deal with the pandemic. Gelfand has spent a lot of time trying to understand how a given country's looseness or tightness affects everyday life. Once you begin looking for evidence,
Starting point is 00:21:00 you see an almost infinite array of examples. Investing, for instance. There's some research coming from University of Georgia that found that buying and selling of stocks was more synchronized in tighter cultures as compared to looser cultures. A tight country like Germany tends to set strict limits on noise
Starting point is 00:21:20 with mandated quiet hours. New York City, meanwhile, has been called not just the city that never sleeps, but the city that never shuts up. Tight countries tend to have very little jaywalking or littering or, God forbid, dog poop on the sidewalks. You can even see the evidence in the clocks that appear on city streets. In Germany and in Japan, the clocks are really synchronized.
Starting point is 00:21:46 In Brazil and Greece, you're not entirely sure what time it is. You might think that these relatively minor differences don't add up to much. Gelfand would disagree. She says these are merely visible indicators of a country's tightness or looseness, and it's what you don't necessarily see that shapes a given
Starting point is 00:22:06 country's culture. By the way, Gelfand doesn't really take a position on whether loose or tight is superior. She argues that both styles have their upsides and their downsides. A loose country, like the U.S., tends to do well in creativity and innovation, in tolerance and openness, in free speech and free press. The downsides of looseness are less coordination, less self-control, more crime and quality of life problems. In societies that are tighter, there's more community building where people are willing to call out rule violators. Here in the U.S., it's actually a rule violation to call out people who are violating norms. She sees the lack of self-control in loose countries as particularly worrisome. So that has a lot of other effects on debt, on alcoholism, on recreational drug use.
Starting point is 00:23:01 It also is related to obesity. Apparently, like over 50% of cats and dogs in the U.S. are obese. Get out of here. My own sweet Portuguese water dog, Pepper. I mean, that dog is just gigantic. She likes to eat human food and she doesn't love to exercise. She's not very disciplined. You could argue that Pepper's owner is the one who isn't very disciplined. I hate to call out Michelle Gelfand, but even in the loosest of cultures, dogs don't typically have unfettered access to food. But maybe that's part of living in a loose culture, too. We ascribe agency even to our pets.
Starting point is 00:23:40 In any case, here's how Gelfand breaks down the upsides and downsides of tight cultures. Essentially, they are the opposite of the loose attributes. Tight cultures have more coordination and more self-control. If you're in context where there's a lot of rules, you develop from a very early age that impulse control. This leads to less obesity, less addiction, and there's less crime in tighter cultures. Those are the upsides. The downsides? Less innovation, less openness to ideas that challenge the status quo, and less tolerance for differences in religion and race. In one experiment, Gelfand sent a bunch of research assistants to different places around the world. They were trained to ask for help in city streets and in stores.
Starting point is 00:24:29 And in one condition, I had them wearing these fake facial warts, like you can buy them on the internet. In another condition, they were wearing tattoos and nose rings and purple hair. And in a third condition, they were wearing just their face. Gelfand wanted to learn where they would get the most help. And it was fascinating because when people were wearing the normal face, there was no difference. No difference that is between tight and loose cultures. But when people were wearing those really weird nose rings or those
Starting point is 00:24:54 facial warts, they got far more help in loose cultures. This far less stigmatization of people in terms of their race, their religion. This does not mean that no one in a loose culture like the U.S. is stigmatized or mistreated. We have a lot of work to do, there's no question, but relatively speaking, we have more tolerance. Again, it's worth repeating that no culture is a monolith. How do racial and ethnic minorities fit into the American looseness? We hear these terms like America's melting pot or folks who talked about salad bowls that describe what America is. That is Mark Anthony Neal of Duke University.
Starting point is 00:25:39 You have no real other example of a country that has brought together so many different national and ethnic and racial backgrounds. Neil is a professor of African and African-American studies. He has written several books about what music and other pop culture has to say about the broader culture. We think about improvisation in the context, obviously, of creative and musical terms. But it's also a way of always having to adapt to the changing political, social, and cultural realities. And I think that is a hallmark of African American culture in this country. And how does a scholar like Neil think about culture per se? I think it's helpful to think about culture in terms of a big C and a little c, the little c being those everyday things
Starting point is 00:26:26 that we sometimes don't elevate to a level of culture. And then there's the big C, the stuff that we have these big conversations about, that we do these incredible studies about, which is really about the worldview of groups of people coming together in a community, in a nation, in a family, right? But the big C, in my mind, is very different than the little c.
Starting point is 00:26:49 Groups that are of lower status tend to live in tighter worlds. Michelle Gelfand again. And that suggests that minorities, women, people of different sexual orientation, when they violate the same rule, might be held to a higher accountability, to more strict punishment. For example, we asked bank managers some years ago to look through scenarios of people violating organizational rules, like coming to work late, staying on the phone too long, maybe checking their email. And we manipulated whether their names were like Jamal or Letitia versus Brad and Lorna. These are stereotypical names. And we found that people from minority or even women backgrounds were seen as valuing
Starting point is 00:27:30 something more severely and were subject to higher punishment without even people realizing this. So the general rules of a loose or tight culture may not be consistently applied to all populations. And there are other inconsistencies, especially in a country as large and diverse as the U.S. For instance, where you live. We have a whole new map of the U.S. where we can actually rank order the U.S. 50 states in terms of how much threat they have. Because remember, threat is what can drive tightness. Places in the South have tended to have more natural disasters. They tend to veer tighter on our measures than places on the coast.
Starting point is 00:28:09 Also, the people who settled in different areas in the U.S. brought with them their own cultural norms and values and set the stage for different levels of tight-loose within the nation. Where is the loosest place in America? I would say it tends to be California. Now, California is a really exception because it has a lot of threat. But somehow that diversity and that early celebration of permissiveness has overridden that. People tend to be super creative and there's a lot of
Starting point is 00:28:38 negotiation of rules. Mobility also produces looseness because it's harder to agree upon any norm. The people that came to New York early on in the early 1800s, they were from all sorts of different cultural backgrounds, and that's helped to produce the looseness that exists to this day. People who went out to California, I would say if we give them the tight, loose mindset quiz, they were probably on the looser mindset. They were those kind of chaos Muppets because they were risk-seeking. What's a chaos Muppet, you ask? The lawyer and journalist Dahlia Lithwick once argued that every living human can be classified according to one simple metric. Every one of us is either a chaos Muppet or an order Muppet.
Starting point is 00:29:22 Essentially, loose or tight. Consider the prominent Muppets, Bert and Ernie. Ernie, Ernie, don't eat those cookies while you're in your bed, huh? Why not, Bert? Because you get crumbs in the sheets, that's why. And if there are crumbs in the sheets, they'll get in your pajamas. And if you get crumbs in your pajamas, they'll make you itch. Oh, gee. I don't like to itch, Bert. We should note that Bert and Ernie, despite their differences, are very dear friends. This suggests that looseness and tightness can coexist.
Starting point is 00:30:00 It suggests that, as in most things in life, balance is desirable. I do work with the U.S. Navy and other organizations that are trying to have that kind of balance, like the military should be tighter than tech. Nevertheless, you might be able to intentionally create pockets of looseness so you can have more balance. That's what we call tight, loose ambidexterity. What does an institution like the Navy see as the upsides of more looseness? Having more adaptability, more innovation. Innovation requires coming up with a lot of ideas that is generated by looseness. We can think about extraordinarily loose contexts like Tesla or Uber that probably need a little more structure. You can think about it at the household level. To be told, I veer somewhat loose. My husband is an attorney. He veers tighter. And how does that work out? Well, it requires a lot of negotiation.
Starting point is 00:30:47 He's kind of horrified by my dishwasher loading behavior. I think that's a good litmus test of tight, loose. Oh yeah. I'm with him. I also teach negotiation. And the whole point about negotiation is you figure out what's your highest priority in the situation. What domain is so important for you in terms of your tightness or your looseness?
Starting point is 00:31:08 And then negotiate accordingly. We do this on vacations with my siblings. There's a huge variation in how much spontaneity people like versus how much structure they want. And it drives us crazy. And I'm guessing you're the spontaneous type. Yeah, for the most part. I find that people who don't load dishwashers carefully are usually pretty loose with the planning. I'll just say that there are also other contexts where we naturally tighten. When you have teenagers, you're like tight, you know, at least for me. I'm like, we're going to go to Singapore if you people don't behave. Coming up after the break, how America's creative looseness has produced a strange global effect. The scientific discipline of psychology is dominated by Americans.
Starting point is 00:31:47 In the meantime, a bit more from the comedian Hannah Gadsby, who grew up in Tasmania. Have you ever noticed how Americans are not stupid? I've been led to believe, by you, that you are as dumb as bricks. And then I meet you all all and then you're not I mean you've got your quota that's how we all but you're not do you know what you are you're culturally confident good on you I say good on you you know who else had that skill set the ancient Romans and things worked out well for them for a bit.
Starting point is 00:32:31 We'll be right back with the U.S. is very different from other countries, so let's stop pretending it's not. I'm Stephen Dubner, and this is Freakonomics Radio. The cross-cultural psychologist Michelle Gelfand has been telling us about loose and tight cultures around the world. The U.S. is overall relatively loose, but there's something else to be said about American culture. We are supremely weird. Not just regular weird, we are acronymically weird. Capital W-E-I-R-D, which stands for Western Educated Industrialized Rich and Democratic. In case you missed it, that's Western Educated Industrializedized, rich, and democratic. And here's one of the people who created the weird designation. I'm Joe Henrich. I'm a professor of human evolutionary biology at Harvard University.
Starting point is 00:33:39 Henrich and a couple of colleagues came up with the weird label when he was teaching at the University of British Columbia. He was a professor in both the economics and psychology departments, which was weird in its own way, lowercase weird, since Henrik had never taken a course in either subject. He started out as an anthropologist, but he began mixing and matching disciplines to suit his curiosity. Here is how he describes himself these days. I'm a researcher who tries to apply evolutionary theory to understand human behavior and human psychology, and particularly culture. So how it is that we acquire ideas, beliefs, and values from other people, and how this has shaped human genetic evolution,
Starting point is 00:34:18 and I'm particularly interested in how it's shaped our psychology. In 2016, Henrik published a book called The Secret of Our Success, how culture is driving human evolution, domesticating our species, and making us smarter. Stripped of our culturally acquired mental skills, he writes, we are not so impressive when we go head-to-head in problem-solving tests against other apes, and we certainly are not impressive enough to account for the vast success of our species. Henrik recently followed that book with another one called The Weirdest People in the World, How the West Became Psychologically Peculiar and Particularly Prosperous. And the case I make is it's been highly unsuccessful to just pick up institutions that evolved in Western societies and transport them to drop them in Africa or the Middle East or places like that, because there needs to be a fit between how people think about the world, their values, worldviews, motivations, and the afford side of the idea we started out with in this episode. That is why it's hard for the U.S. to simply import successful policies from elsewhere.
Starting point is 00:35:30 Henrik is saying that the export of American ideas isn't necessarily easier. If it were, Afghanistan and Venezuela, even Iran, might be U.S.-style democracies by now. It's hard in either direction, not just because some cultures are tighter than others. Henrik argues that national psychologies can be quite particular, but that you may not appreciate that if all you read is the mainstream psychological research. And that's because the vast majority of the research subjects are weird. One study of the journals in social psychology shows that 96% of all subjects in social psychology come from societies that are Western-educated, industrialized, rich, and democratic.
Starting point is 00:36:17 If you just look at Americans, it's 70% American. So the scientific discipline of psychology is dominated by Americans. And why is that a problem? Because Americans and Westerners more generally are psychologically unusual from a global perspective. So if you only want to talk about American psychology, you're fine. But if you want to talk about humans, homo sapiens, then you have a generalization problem. Can you give me a good example of an idea or a theory that I might come across in a Psych 101 textbook that would just be so American that it wouldn't really be useful if you actually care about humans? This probably wouldn't be in a psych textbook, but something like the ultimatum
Starting point is 00:36:58 game. The ultimatum game is famous among social scientists. It's an experiment developed in the early 1980s by, among others, the German economist Werner Guth. Here's how it works. Two players divide a sum of money, so say it's $100, and the first player can offer a portion of the $100 to a second player. So they might offer, say, 10 out of the 100. The second player is given a choice between accepting or rejecting. The two players don't know each other, they don't even see each other, and this is a one-time interaction, so there won't be another round of the game where the second player can punish or reward the first player. If they accept the offer, they get the
Starting point is 00:37:40 amount of the offer, so $10 in this case. If they reject, both players get zero. Okay, you get the gist, right? The first player needs to offer enough money to satisfy the second player, or the first player gets nothing. If you're an economist, you might think that offering even $1 out of the hundred would be enough, because $1 is more than zero, so the second player would still be better off. But if you're not an economist, if you're a regular human being, you can see why the second player might reject a $1 offer. It is a small price to pay to punish the first player for being so stingy. So how much would you offer?
Starting point is 00:38:27 That's what the ultimatum experiments set out to find. As with most experiments like this, the research subjects were weird. Usually, they were students at the universities where the researchers worked. So, the usual result that economists found in lots of university populations in Europe and the U.S. is many people offer 50-50, so you end up with mean offers around 45% of the total. And I was interested in this, and I thought maybe it would tell us something about an innate human psychology for reciprocity or something like that. That is a pretty interesting result. One stranger giving away roughly half their money to another stranger when theoretically 10 or 20% would keep the second player from rejecting the offer. Some researchers looked at these results and came up with a new label
Starting point is 00:39:12 for humans in this context, homo reciprocans. This was in contrast to the economist's label of homo economicus. That version of humans is more self-interested, less reciprocal. But Joe Henrich wanted to see how the ultimatum experiments worked when it wasn't just a bunch of weird college students. I was doing research in the Peruvian Amazon. So I did the experiment there with an indigenous population called the Machiganga. And the Machiganga were much closer to the predictions of homo economicus, where you make low offers and never reject. So they would offer a mean of about 25, 26%. There were a number of low offers of 15%, which didn't get rejected. And this led to this project
Starting point is 00:39:56 where we did it in lots of places, hunter-gatherers, pastoralists, Africa, Papua New Guinea. And we found the full spectrum of variation. Offers went up as high as 55 or 60 percent in some places, and then down around 25 percent in other places. I remember once, years and years ago, when I was reading this research that you were doing, speaking with Francisco Gil White, who was then at Penn, and he told me that when he was running this ultimatum experiment, I don't remember where, I want to say Mongolia. Yeah, he was in Mongolia. But the research subjects, they gave him a lot back, and they thought it was going to him. And he said the reason was that he was, you know, a young,
Starting point is 00:40:42 whatever, postdoc, and he had holes in his jeans. And the research subject explained to him that, oh, I feel so bad for you that you can't afford pants without holes in them that I can't take the money from this poor American kid. And it struck me as a way in which this experiment could be, you know, perverted. So, Francisco is a good pal of mine, and he's also a very charming fellow, so I have no doubt that his subjects really liked him. I do think that that particular story is idiosyncratic to his experience, but we tried to address that. So after we ran that first project, we redid the entire project, and we took concerns like the one Francisco had, and we made sure that the subjects knew that the money was coming from an organization,
Starting point is 00:41:25 that the giver did not get any of the money. We ratcheted up our levels of anonymity. We put in a bunch of other checks and controls. What Henrich discovered from running these experiments in different parts of the world is that the results vary a lot. This suggests that every time a social scientist runs an experiment whose research subjects are weird, that's capital letter weird, the results of that experiment may be meaningful in the U.S. and some other places, but quite likely not in others. So, again, if you want to talk about Americans, you're OK.
Starting point is 00:42:03 But if you want to talk about humans, then you have a problem. This feeds back into what Michelle Gelfand was talking about earlier in the context of geopolitical negotiations. If these kinds of cultural differences are happening at the highest levels, we better start understanding this stuff. So if you base your understanding of a given culture on a body of research that fails to include them, you'll likely fail to understand how that culture thinks, whether we're talking about another country or a group within your own country. This failure leads to confusion at the very least, but quite possibly deeper misunderstandings,
Starting point is 00:42:40 perhaps all the way up to hatred and violent conflict. So, yeah, that is weird. Joe Henrich's research into national psychologies led him to an even more fascinating conclusion. This is where he combines all his academic interests, not just economics and psychology, but also anthropology and evolutionary biology. Remember what he said earlier? So, how it is that we acquire ideas, beliefs, and values from other people, and how this has shaped human genetic evolution. Really? Can that possibly be true? Our culture shapes our genetics?
Starting point is 00:43:19 Henrik says yes. Here's how he puts it in his latest book. You can't separate culture from psychology or psychology from biology because culture physically rewires our brains and thereby shapes how we think. One example he gives is literacy. In a society in which 95% of adults are highly literate, he writes, people have a thicker corpus callosum than a society in which 95% of adults are highly literate, he writes, people have a thicker corpus callosum than a society in which only 5% of people are highly literate. The corpus callosum is the bunch of nerve fibers that unites the two brain hemispheres. People in the less literate society, meanwhile, would have better facial recognition skills. Here's another example.
Starting point is 00:44:04 People from more individualistic societies tend to focus on central objects. Meaning, if you grew up someplace like the U.S., when you look at an image, you're more likely to pay attention to what's in the foreground, in the center. Someone raised in an Eastern culture might focus more on the image as a whole and less on the central object. This cashes out in an ability to make better abstract or absolute judgments. So if you ask people to judge the absolute lengths of two lines, people in more individualistic societies tend to get that right, whereas people from less individualistic societies tend to be better at making relative-sized judgments. Michelle Gelfand has another example of how culture shapes perception.
Starting point is 00:44:46 One of the areas of cultural study that first hooked her had to do with optical illusions. Classic things like the Mueller-Lyer illusion, which is these two lines where one looks longer than the other. These are the two lines that are the same, but one has arrows going out and one in. Exactly. And they were finding that people in Africa were not falling victim to this illusion. Part of it is that when you live in a world that has carpented environments like right angles, where we live in houses in the States, makes us focus on those right angles and
Starting point is 00:45:16 it produces this illusion. I was floored. If basic things like visual illusions are not universal, what about other phenomena? Yes, what about other phenomena? Like how things smell to us? Joe Henrich again. In some societies, people really attend to scent, and they have a complex set of language terms that have the equivalent of basic color categories for scents. They're able to make finer distinctions in terms of their olfaction, whereas we usually describe a scent by saying something that it smells like. There are also auditory differences. Some people grow up speaking languages like Mandarin, where you have to learn to distinguish words just by the tone, and that's going to cultivate certain tonal abilities which could
Starting point is 00:45:58 feed into certain kinds of music and things like that. Henrik's next example is more behavioral than physiological. It has to do with conformity. There's something called the Ash Conformity Test, where you have confederates of the experimenter give the same wrong answer to an objective problem. And then you see how often the subject wants to go along with the other people as opposed to give the answer they would give if they were by themselves.
Starting point is 00:46:22 When they're by themselves, the vast majority of people who do this experiment get the right answer, like in this archival tape of an Ash conformity test. Three. Three. But then the experimenter's confederates come in. But on the third trial, something happens. Two.
Starting point is 00:46:42 Two. Two. Two. Two. Two. Two. Two. Two. The subject denies the evidence of his own eyes and yields to group influence. When Americans did this experiment, a third of them conformed and gave an obviously wrong answer. The same experiment was done in other non-weird countries like Zimbabwe and Japan. If you go to other
Starting point is 00:47:06 societies, people are much more willing to give the same wrong answer to go along with others. It turns out that Americans were among the least likely to conform. Relatedly, Americans place a high value on being consistent across different situations. You want to be the same self, regardless of who you're talking to or what context you're in. Whereas in other places, it seems to be okay to morph and shift your personality depending on your context. So the picture that emerges from these findings is that Americans are less likely to conform in the name of social harmony, and we also treasure being consistent, expressing our true selves regardless of the context. If you wanted to reduce this to a slogan of Americanism, it might be something like,
Starting point is 00:47:55 I am me, deal with it. This fits quite snugly with the fact that the U.S. has been found to be the most individualistic culture in the world. We may not be the very loosest culture, but we are number one in individualism. Which doesn't mean egoism, but it could go that way. It means, I did it my way. This man has proof of our individualism. We met him earlier, but just briefly.
Starting point is 00:48:25 Here's a proper introduction. My name is Gert-Jan Hofstede. I'm a professor of artificial sociality at Wageningen University in the Netherlands. The study of culture is a family business for Hofstede. His late father was a social psychologist who devised a system to rank countries on several dimensions, including their level of individualism versus collectivism. In an individualistic society, a person is like an atom in a gas. It can freely float about and life is an adventure. The best thing you can become is yourself. And in a collectivistic
Starting point is 00:49:05 society, a person is like an atom in a crystal. Whether proud or not, whether happy or not, it has a position and it should stay there. On the next episode of Freakonomics Radio, Hofstede will guide us through the other dimensions of sociality. He will pinpoint the ways in which the US is an outlier among the nations of the world. To hold you over till then, we'll leave you with a patriotic tribute from one last transplanted U.S. comedian. When was that moment that America became the most American America it could possibly be. John Oliver grew up in England. Baseballs were hit from the deck of a warship, from a needlessly inflatable batting cage,
Starting point is 00:49:52 out into the ocean where they were caught by people on jet skis. That is not just the most American thing that's ever happened. Those should be the new words to your national anthem. We'll be back next week. Until then, take care of yourself, and if you can, someone else too. Freakonomics Radio is produced by Stitcher and Renbud Radio. We can be reached at radio at Freakonomics.com. This episode was produced by Brent Katz. Our staff also includes Neil Caruth, Gabriel Roth, Greg Rippin, Zach Lipinski, Ryan Kelly, Rebecca Lee Douglas, Julie Canfor, Morgan Levy, Eleanor Osborne, Jeremy Johnston, Jasmine Klinger, Emma Terrell, Lyric Bowditch, Jacob Clemente,
Starting point is 00:50:45 and Alina Kullman. Our theme song is Mr. Fortune by the Hitchhikers. All our other music was composed by Luis Guerra. If you like this show or any other show in the Freakonomics Radio network, please recommend it to your family and friends. You can get the entire archive of Freakonomics Radio or any of our shows on any podcast app. If you'd like to read a transcript or look up the show notes, you can find that at Freakonomics.com. As always, thanks for listening. I'm guessing economists are the tightest of the social scientists, yes? I mean, I don't have the data,
Starting point is 00:51:18 but I would bet the college education funds for my kids on that. The Freakonomics Radio Network. my kids on that.

There aren't comments yet for this episode. Click on any sentence in the transcript to leave a comment.