Fresh Air - Are Kids With ADHD Being Treated Effectively?

Episode Date: April 24, 2025

ADHD has been considered a medical disorder, treatable with drugs like Ritalin, but New York Times Magazine writer Paul Tough says recent studies question that assumption and treatment options.Also, M...artin Johnson reviews a new tribute to Anthony Braxton, who Johnson says is one of the most polarizing figures in jazz.Learn more about sponsor message choices: podcastchoices.com/adchoicesNPR Privacy Policy

Transcript
Discussion (0)
Starting point is 00:00:00 Keeping up with the news can feel like a 24-hour job. Luckily, it is our job. Every hour on the NPR News Now podcast, we take the latest most important stories happening and we package them into five-minute episodes so you can easily squeeze them in between meetings and on your way to that thing. Listen to the NPR News Now podcast now. This is Fresh Air. I'm Dave Davies. If you live in the United States, chances are good that you either are or know a parent whose child is being treated for ADHD, Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Last year, the Centers for Disease Control reported that more than 11 percent of American children had been diagnosed with ADHD, a record
Starting point is 00:00:46 high. For 14-year-old boys, the figure was 21 percent. In a recent article for the New York Times Magazine, journalist Paul Tuff examines how ADHD is diagnosed and treated, often with commonly prescribed stimulants such as Ritalin and Adderall. Though they're regarded as highly effective and thus very popular, He finds three decades of scientific studies have raised questions about their efficacy and safety and about the nature of ADHD itself. Some researchers think the notion that ADHD is a distinct identifiable brain disorder may be wrong or at least oversimplified, and the treatments other than medication should be considered. Paul Tuff is a contributing writer to the New York Times Magazine and the author of four books,
Starting point is 00:01:32 most recently, The Inequality Machine, How College Divides Us. Paul Tuff, welcome to Fresh Air. Thank you. Great to be here. You write that in the early 90s, there were rising rates of ADHD diagnoses, about 2 million American kids in 1993, roughly two-thirds of them taking Ritalin. This provoked protests from some, particularly the Church of Scientology, you know, arguing that you're drugging our kids. You write that you didn't have to be a Scientologist to acknowledge there were legitimate questions
Starting point is 00:02:04 about ADHD. What were they? At that point, the questions were pretty basic. We didn't exactly understand what this condition was and what treatments were the right ones to use. So Ritalin doctors could see, families could see that when kids took Ritalin, there was this, in many of them, this overnight change in their behavior, but we weren't sure why that was happening. And the diagnoses were expanding at such a great rate. There was also a question of why that was, why suddenly it had doubled in just a few years.
Starting point is 00:02:34 So a massive study was organized by a number of researchers. Tell us how this was put together. Yes. So this was the multimodal treatment of ADHD study, or MTA. And a number of researchers at six sites around the United States and Canada decided that they wanted to test different treatments of ADHD. So there were stimulant medications,
Starting point is 00:02:57 specifically at that point. It was Ritalin. But there were also behavioral interventions, so coaching, parent training. And they wanted to see scientifically which would work best. So they did what scientists do, which is they created this randomized controlled study. And at each site in the United States and Canada, they divided the children who were between seven and nine who had been diagnosed with ADHD into different treatment groups.
Starting point is 00:03:22 So some got behavioral training, some got Ritalin, and some were just left on their own to figure out their own treatment. Right. Results were released in 1999. What did they show? That was after 14 months of treatment. And what they showed was that the most effective treatment
Starting point is 00:03:39 for behavior, for symptoms, was Ritalin, that the kids who had taken Ritalin were doing significantly better than the other groups. Over time, of course, more and more kids were diagnosed with ADHD. And you write about a guy named James Swanson, who was at the University of California Irvine, who, among others, grew uneasy about these trends
Starting point is 00:04:01 in diagnosis and treatment. What was troubling them? So there were two things that were really troubling James Swanson, and one was that that initial expansion of diagnoses from about a million kids to about two million kids, that made sense to him because scientists thought that about 3% was the most accurate guess of what percentage of children sort of naturally had ADHD, and 2 million was pretty close to 3%. But then it kept going up. So as the study was going on, it went up to 5% to 6%, and he couldn't see a reason why that was happening. The other thing that
Starting point is 00:04:36 he found disturbing was that he and the other scientists that were running the MTA study continued after those 14 months to carefully study the children who were in the original group. And what they noticed was that the advantage that the kids in the Ritalin group had had after 14 months, it started to really fade. And by 36 months, there was no difference in the symptoms of any of the groups. The kids who had taken Ritalin weren't doing better in terms of their symptoms than the children who had been assigned to the behavioral group.
Starting point is 00:05:07 And they weren't even doing better than the kids who had not been given any treatment at all. You're right that Swanson is now 80 years old and is troubled by the way ADHD research and treatment is going. Is there a kind of fundamental theme to his concern? Yeah, I think mostly what he's concerned by is that that original study of the 14 months got a lot
Starting point is 00:05:27 of attention and the message really went out that Ritalin works, that it's the right treatment for most kids, but that the second study, the one that found that over time, those effects fade, that it did not get the same type of attention and that it's not reflected in the way that a lot of practitioners are now treating ADHD. And there's been a lot of research into what it actually is biologically and has that guided treatment at all?
Starting point is 00:05:57 I mean, I guess that's the question, is there a real connection between the understanding of the biological origins of this and the way it's treated? One of the things that's so striking in talking to scientists, including neuroscientists who have studied this, is that they say that they actually understand the biology of ADHD less than they did 20 years ago. So normally, the course of science is that as time goes on, they get a better and better understanding of what's
Starting point is 00:06:20 really going on in the brains of children with a particular diagnosis. And in this case, that's just not true. So 20 years ago, there was this belief that there were clear biomarkers, clear indications in the brain in terms of electrical signals or a particular gene that would predict ADHD or differences in the size and shape of certain parts of the brain that you could say, this kid has that biomarker, he does have ADHD, and this one does not.
Starting point is 00:06:48 And as the last couple of decades have gone on, that belief has slowly been undermined, so that now there is no clear biomarker for ADHD. And I think most neuroscientists accept that that's the case. This is such a big deal for parents. Nothing is more troubling than to see your child in pain or struggling. You have kids yourself, right?
Starting point is 00:07:09 You have two sons? I do, yeah. Yeah. So you know the stakes here. Absolutely. What are some of the things that they observe that make them so desperate for help? ADHD can be incredibly disruptive in a child's life,
Starting point is 00:07:23 in a family's life, in a classroom. When kids are having trouble sitting still, focusing, getting work done, keeping themselves organized, controlling their impulses, it makes life really difficult. And it especially makes life difficult when you are going to school. A lot of the school day, you've got to sit still, you've got to focus.
Starting point is 00:07:41 There's a lot of homework, I know, as a parent. And when you are struggling with impulse control, when you're struggling with attention, those've got to focus. There's a lot of homework, I know, as a parent. And when you are struggling with impulse control, when you're struggling with attention, those things are really difficult. And I think within families, that often leads to real conflict. And so what originally starts as just a problem in one child's life turns into a problem for a whole family.
Starting point is 00:08:00 And I guess one of the other things that's tricky about it as a diagnosis is that a lot of the things that you observe are also symptoms from other causes such as you know injury to the head or other psychological conditions, anxiety and depression, right? Yeah, so it is a very tricky disorder to diagnose for a lot of reasons. I mean the first one is that the only way to do it is by using a symptom checklist that is in the diagnostic and statistical manual. So there's this list of symptoms.
Starting point is 00:08:30 If you have six symptoms, you officially have ADHD. If you have five, you don't. There are other criteria. It has to last for a certain amount of time. It has to exist in at least two different settings. But what makes it even more tricky is exactly what you're talking about, that there is great overlap between these symptoms and the symptoms of lots of other things, of early trauma, for instance, of anxiety, of depression.
Starting point is 00:08:52 And to make it even more complicated, what the CDC has found is that there is a lot of overlap among children who are diagnosed with ADHD with other psychological problems. So about three quarters of kids who have received an ADHD diagnosis have also received a diagnosis for another psychological disorder or a learning problem. And according to the DSM, if the symptoms of a child are better explained by another diagnosis, they shouldn't be diagnosed with ADHD. So that makes it really tricky that there is sometimes overlap between two things, but you're only supposed to diagnose ADHD if it is the one that is causing these symptoms.
Starting point is 00:09:31 Right, the DSM you mentioned, that's the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, which guides treatment for a lot of practitioners. I guess one of the other things that's a little hard to understand about this is that two kinds of symptoms for ADHD are pretty different, right? I mean, there's inattention, not paying attention, and then there's hyperactivity and impulsivity. And they seem like pretty different behaviors. Why is it assumed that they arise from the same condition? Yeah, I think that's another part of the complication of this diagnosis.
Starting point is 00:10:05 There are some doctors who think these are two entirely separate conditions that each should have its own listing in the DSM. And you know, in fact, over the years, there have been lots of different sort of envelopes around ADHD. Its name has changed, the symptoms that predict it have changed. There's clearly something going on in a lot of kids who are having trouble focusing, having trouble sitting still and paying attention,
Starting point is 00:10:32 but in individual children, that expresses itself in lots of different ways. And that's another real struggle, I think, for clinicians, for families who are trying to figure out, you know, there's something going on with my kid, what is it? And ADHD, I think, has become this sort of catch-all diagnosis that we use to just put together
Starting point is 00:10:50 a lot of kids who may, in fact, be quite different. You know, it seems kind of counterintuitive that stimulants would help with hyperactivity and impulse control, right? I mean, it seems like you're pushing it in the same direction that's the problem. What's the medical explanation for that? Well, I don't think we know exactly what's going on.
Starting point is 00:11:10 But what studies of amphetamines, the drug at the root of the two most popular medications for ADHD, what amphetamines do is they help make whatever you're focused on seem more interesting. And so I think that then makes sense, right? If you're having impulse control, if you're distracted by everything else that's going on in the room, when you take this medication that makes whatever you're supposed to be
Starting point is 00:11:31 looking at seem more interesting, it makes it easier to focus, easier to sit still. So what are some of the non-drug treatments that are helpful in treating ADHD that people are discovering. Scientifically, there is not yet any real data showing that there's any particular sort of parent training or behavioral techniques that makes things better, which I think is really frustrating for a lot of clinicians
Starting point is 00:11:56 and a lot of parents. And I think, as a result, tends to make medication seem like a more attractive alternative. But lots of clinicians have found their own ways of working with kids and with families that are just about like helping to create a calmer atmosphere in the classroom, a calmer atmosphere at home, you know, things as basic as, you know, post-it notes and calendars and just ways of helping you organize your thoughts, organize your schoolwork if you're a kid in school.
Starting point is 00:12:29 You know, none of those are a perfect cure, but a lot of them seem to help. And I think they're really individualistic and, you know, a good clinician can help a family figure out the solutions or the tools, the interventions that are most helpful for that child. You read about Russell Barkley, a prominent ADHD researcher, and he has a lecture that has been viewed, what, more than four million times on YouTube, right? What is his perspective of the disorder? Yeah, so he's written a lot about it, probably the most well-known, the most prominent doctor
Starting point is 00:13:02 or scientist when it comes to popularizing our ideas about ADHD. And what he talks about in this speech is that ADHD is basically diabetes of the brain. So it's a chronic condition that you're going to have to treat for your whole life. And he tends to focus on the downsides of ADHD. He takes very seriously the real problems that can occur for kids and adults who have ADHD, including higher rates of traffic accidents, of early death, of drug addiction. And the way that he looks at ADHD is to look at it as this very much
Starting point is 00:13:38 sort of black and white yes or no diagnosis, like diabetes. If you've got it, you've got it. If you don't, you don't. And you have to treat it because there will be, you know, downstream effects that you really want to avoid. Yes, exactly. I want to talk a bit about some of the research into the nature of this disorder. In 2002, you're right, there was an international consensus statement signed by 85 researchers defending the validity of the diagnosis of ADHD because there have been questions about it. What was the thrust of their letter? Well, the main point of that consensus statement was exactly to defend the diagnosis against
Starting point is 00:14:18 critics. And so a lot of that statement is just about the basic fact that this is a real diagnosis that has real consequences. What I was drawn to in that statement was the focus on biomarkers, on particular biological signatures that could let us identify ADHD and in the process say, this is clearly a biological condition, not just a psychological one.
Starting point is 00:14:41 And it focused on three particular biomarkers. One was certain electrical signals in the brain that seemed different in kids with ADHD and without. The other was on genetics. There were early indications in the early 2000s that there was a specific single gene that predicted ADHD. And the third was about the kind of physical signatures you could see in the brain on MRIs, that there were differences in the volumes and sizes of particular parts of the brain in kids who
Starting point is 00:15:10 had been diagnosed with ADHD. And has that held up to further study? No, that part has not really held up to further study. It's become much more complicated, this search for the biomarker. And there are many scientists now who say that the search for a biomarker was just a red herring, that that's not what any scientist should be focused on. Instead, we should be focusing on the experience of kids with ADHD. So each of those three biomarkers that I mentioned have been undermined in one way or another. The study of these electrical
Starting point is 00:15:40 signals, repeated studies to try to replicate that, turned up to have no result. Genetics is more complicated. There are still indications that certain combinations of genetic qualities are predictive of ADHD. But the very sort of simple and straightforward, you've got this gene, you've got ADHD, that scientists were hoping was the case 20, 25 years ago, that has proven not to be true.
Starting point is 00:16:04 And then the third is this idea that there are differences in the volume, the cortical volume scientists say in certain parts of the brain. And that was studied in this giant global study called the ENIGMA study done by this consortium of neuroscientists and psychiatrists. And that similarly showed almost no difference between people with ADHD and people without, among adults and adolescents, no difference at all, among children, just a tiny difference
Starting point is 00:16:33 in the cortical volume of certain parts of the brain. You're right that you found a consensus of sorts among most scientists that you spoke to about this question of whether it's a biological condition. What's the consensus? Well, the consensus is that there are clearly both biological and environmental causes somehow combining to create these symptoms in kids. But beyond that, there's not much consensus. There's not a clear agreement on the relative role that environment plays and that biology plays. You know, so much of the discussion here focuses on kids, because obviously parents are really
Starting point is 00:17:07 concerned about helping their kids and helping them grow and flourish. To what extent is this a growing diagnosis among adults? To a huge extent. So in this article, I focused on kids for a variety of reasons, partly because the science is more clear, because scientists have been studying ADHD in kids for much longer. But as a phenomenon, as a diagnosis in the United States, it is among adults that all of the growth is happening. So the fastest growing groups for the diagnosis are people, adults in their 20s and 30s, but even adults in their 40s, 50s, 60s, all of those diagnosis rates are
Starting point is 00:17:45 going up really quickly, and prescription rates are going up by a huge degree as well. You know, it's interesting because you mentioned this multimodal study earlier found that there were clear benefits to Ritalin and, you know, stimulants, but that they tended to disappear after about 36 months. Do we know if that's also true of adults who take these medications? We don't. It just hasn't been studied to the same degree. I don't think there's reason to think
Starting point is 00:18:09 there's a different result that we would see among adults. But there hasn't been a similarly rigorous study like MTA for adults. This is such a common issue that we all know people that are dealing with this. And just over the past two days, just among the producers here at Fresh Air, I've had one producer who has a son
Starting point is 00:18:27 who struggles with some of this and finds the medication very helpful. And then I had another producer whose brother, when he was in second grade, had real trouble focusing and his teachers were struggling with him because he was just all over the place. And they told his mom, look, you're gonna have to put him on Ritalin
Starting point is 00:18:43 or one of these drugs or otherwise he can't come to school here. And the mom said, nope, I'm not doing that. And I know it's a public school, so you have to deal with him. He ended up in front of a school psychologist who taught him chess. And they began playing chess once a week. And at least as my friend told the story,
Starting point is 00:19:02 that was a real breakthrough. I mean, he really changed his behavior, and he's gone on. He's never taken medication. He's had a productive career as an artist and animator and lives a happy life. So I mean, an anecdote isn't the same thing as research, but it just seems like there are a lot of ways this can go. Absolutely.
Starting point is 00:19:20 Those are great stories. And yes, I've heard lots of stories as well, not only in my reporting, but just from friends before the article came out, and then in great numbers since the article came out. And I think what's hard for us is that when we hear two different stories like that, it's hard for us not to think, well, just one of them
Starting point is 00:19:37 has to be true. The reality is they are both absolutely true, and that different people have different experiences of this condition and of its treatment, there is no one-size-fits-all solution. And so to me, that is what is reassuring and even exciting about this new research by giving young people and families the message that this is not just a sort of singular biological condition that you have or you don't have, that in fact, symptoms often fluctuate over time, that there are different sorts
Starting point is 00:20:09 of treatments that work for different sorts of kids, that that gives them maybe less certainty, which can sometimes be scary, but also more of a sense of possibility, that this is something that can, like the young person you're talking to who was sort of cured by chess, that there may be ways that life can change that will really change these symptoms as well. We're gonna take another break here.
Starting point is 00:20:34 Let me reintroduce you. We are speaking with Paul Tuff. He is a contributing writer for The New York Times Magazine. His recent cover story is titled Have We Been Thinking About ADHD All Wrong? He'll be back to talk more after this short break. I'm Dave Davies, and this is Fresh Air. Browsing, mouthwatering night markets in Bangkok, or dancing to carnival in the Caribbean. With amazing beach breaks, city breaks, and bucket list trips to choose from, Air Canada
Starting point is 00:21:10 has you covered. Start planning your trip to over 180 destinations today at aircanada.com or contact your travel agent. Air Canada, nice travels. Climate change is drying up some water supplies and making others undrinkable. That's why Here and Now Anytime is covering the hunt for fresh water. From a pipeline in the Great Lakes to the science of desalination to extreme recycling that turns sewage into clean drinking water.
Starting point is 00:21:41 That's Here and Now Anytime, a podcast from NPR and WBUR. Donald Trump has an extraordinary approach to the presidency. At the NPR Politics Podcast, we're recapping the first 100 days of Donald Trump's second term from his early promises to his policy decisions and what it all means for you. Politics may not always make sense,
Starting point is 00:22:02 but we'll sort it out together over on the NPR Politics Podcast. You know, it's interesting, you're right, that the roots of treatment for this disorder go way back to an experiment, I guess, in the 1930s by a Harvard-trained psychologist in Rhode Island, right? Tell us about this. Yeah, so this is a psychiatrist named Charles Bradley who ran a home for children with psychological problems in Rhode Island. And at the time, this was in the 1930s, benzadrine, which is a kind of amphetamine, had become this really popular drug among jazz musicians actually and among college students who felt like it helped them focus and it sort of amped them up in various ways. And he thought,
Starting point is 00:22:41 maybe I should try this on these kids. There was nothing that was seeming to help them. So he did a very small but rigorous experiment where he gave 30 kids a daily dose of benzadrine. And he noticed this, in about half of them, this vast change in their behavior. So they were more placid, they were more easygoing, they could get things done, they felt better about their work, and it felt to him like this kind of miracle cure. And later researchers followed up on this and did similar experiments. What did they find? Yeah, so I spoke to this NYU researcher named Xavier Castellanos who actually told me about
Starting point is 00:23:20 the Charles Bradley paper and said that he sees the same things now when he first prescribes Brittle and Adderall to kids, that overnight there's this kind of miraculous transformation for a lot of them, that their behaviors really change. What he said was frustrating though was that when you look at the sort of academic results for kids, even though they are able to sit and focus more and get more seat work done, their test scores don't go up. And that, I think, has been this puzzle that doctors have been wrestling with for the past couple of decades, that at least in the short term, these medications seem to have a powerful effect for some kids on their symptoms, but over time they don't seem to have an effect on academic achievement, at least in measured in test scores.
Starting point is 00:24:07 Yeah. There was another test that you described involving putting stuff in knapsacks. Explain this. Yeah. So I'd never heard of this test before. It's called the knapsack test, but apparently it's pretty common in psychology and computer science. You give a kid a backpack.
Starting point is 00:24:23 In this case, it's a virtual backpack and it's just this kind of game. You give them a bunch of different things of different weights and prices and you say, what's the best way to pack this backpack to get the most value of stuff into it? So it's a little sort of logical puzzle. And these researchers in Australia gave this test to a bunch of young adults and they tested how well they did when they were on, and they tested how well they did when they were on stimulant medication and how well they did when they were not. And what they found was that when they were on stimulant medication, they, from the outside, looked to be doing much better.
Starting point is 00:24:56 They were working more quickly, they were more diligent, they were more focused. But when they looked at the results of how well they were doing on the SNAP-SAC test, they weren't doing any better. And what they saw was that that was because they weren't actually making better decisions. They were just sort of randomly pulling things in and out of their backpack instead of focusing on it. So what I think some researchers believe is that this is is a clue to why sometimes behavior in the classroom can improve on stimulant medication, but academic results don't. It's possible that these stimulants make you sit still and behave better, but they're not actually helping your brain process information better.
Starting point is 00:25:34 You mentioned some other things that were troubling about continued use of these stimulants. What were some of the other issues that came up? So, James Swanson, the researcher who helped lead the MTA study, one data point that he really focuses on in thinking about this as a long-term treatment rather than a short-term one, is that in the original MTA study, when young people took stimulant medication over the course of years, it had an effect on their growth, on their physical growth, on their height, that the kids who had consistently taken Ritalin were about an inch shorter than the kids who had not.
Starting point is 00:26:10 And so Swanson and the other MTA researchers have continued to study this group through adolescence and even into adulthood. And the most recent study looked at them when they were 25 and continued to see this height differential, that the ones who had consistently taken stimulant medication all the way through childhood and even young adulthood,
Starting point is 00:26:30 they continued to be about an inch shorter than the ones who had stopped taking it or who had never started at all. Is there an explanation for that biologically that we know of? There isn't a definite explanation. No, we don't exactly know why that is. The most sort of logical one is that these medications affect appetite in a big way. So when I would talk to young people who were taking these medications, that was the thing
Starting point is 00:26:52 that they talked about the most, that you just don't want to eat. And if you're an adolescent and you don't want to eat, you're very different than other adolescents. This is a time when kids are eating a ton and that's helping their growth. So that would make sense as part of the reason for this, but there haven't been enough careful studies to say that that's the real reason. And these are amphetamines, right?
Starting point is 00:27:11 I mean, can they be addictive? Amphetamines as a whole can be addictive. The way that the medications are formulated, especially the ones that release the medication over an extended time, over the course of a day, those are certain safeguards that make them less likely to be addicted. The young people I talked to who took these medications did not feel that they were addicting at all. They found them very easy to stop. So yes, they can be addictive. There are certainly anecdotal stories about people who become dependent or even addicted to these medications,
Starting point is 00:27:43 but that doesn't seem like a very big problem for most kids. We are speaking with Paul Tuff. He is a contributing writer for the New York Times Magazine. His recent cover story is titled, Have We Been Thinking About ADHD All Wrong? We'll continue our conversation in just a moment. This is Fresh Air. Paul Tuff, you spoke to students across the country about this with ADHD diagnosis, who'd been taking these stimulants.
Starting point is 00:28:10 What did you hear? Well, I heard a variety of things. So I think every young person's experience is unique, but mostly I felt like they had a pretty complex relationship to these drugs. So I think the way that these medications and the diagnosis in general is portrayed in the public is that this is a medical condition that has a medical solution.
Starting point is 00:28:32 And in fact, the young people I talked to saw this as much more of a kind of constant negotiation between them and their medication. A couple of the young people I talked to talked about, used the word sacrifice when they talked about their stimulant medication. They saw that there were benefits to it. They saw that it was going to help them in some significant way going forward in life,
Starting point is 00:28:54 but they didn't actually like the experience of taking them. Often not at all. I mean, one of them said, I just hate taking it. First of all, get this real rush. You feel like you're on top of the world. But then there's a real crash. And so he didn't love it, but he saw that in certain circumstances, it was the right thing to do. And that's what I heard from a lot of kids,
Starting point is 00:29:12 that they had gotten to the point where they knew what this medication would make them feel like. And rather than think of it as, say, diabetes medication, something you've got to take every day in order to survive, it was much more situational. It was something that they would take at every day in order to survive, it was much more situational.
Starting point is 00:29:25 It was something that they would take at certain times, at certain moments, and then other times not take. Yeah, there was this one guy who you called Cap who used it when he was preparing for his SAT exams and for baseball and baseball practice because he could really focus on pitches and he thought it made him a better hitter. But he didn't like it. And I guess people felt that it changed their personalities in some ways. Yeah, that was something that I heard from a lot of people, that their sort of love of life, their sense of humor, that a lot of that would change when they were on this medication, that they were, you know, at lunch they were not the happy social person that they had been. One of them said, you know, it's not like I'm unhappy, it's just like I'm kind of flat. There was one girl who I spoke to, I didn't include
Starting point is 00:30:08 in the article, who talked about how it did sort of suppress her emotions. And she said that she had been having this day a couple of days before I spoke to her when her friends were going off to college and she wanted to say goodbye to them all. And she decided that day not to take her medication because she wanted to feel. She wanted to have this emotional connection and she knew that if she was on the medication she just would feel more flat. You wrote that there are some people who believe that ADHD is a clear, you know, identifiable biological disease and therefore best treated with medication, but that increasingly people think that it may be thought of not as a condition that you have, but as something that you experience. What exactly does that mean? What are the implications of that idea?
Starting point is 00:30:54 Well, I think this is where this question gets really interesting to me. And the person who I think is sort of leading the way, the researcher who's leading the way on this, is a British researcher named Edmund Sanuga-Bark who has been studying ADHD for 35 years. And in the last few years, he has really started to change his approach. And I think that's been influential in the field. So he talks about ADHD as not having any kind of natural cutting point where you can say this person has it and this person does not have it, but instead that ADHD symptoms exist on a continuum where almost everyone has some of these symptoms and that there are some for whom it is really an extreme problem but there's not a natural cutting point.
Starting point is 00:31:38 And to him, this distinction is important not only scientifically but also kind of like psychologically and emotionally that it enables us to tell young people that this is not just a case of them having a deficit of a certain skill, and that that's a disorder that has to be treated medically, but instead that their problems may have more to do with a misalignment between their own unique brain and the situation that they're in. And if that's the case, sometimes medication can still help make that environment more tolerable, but there also might be things that we could change in their environment, that they could change in their habits and patterns that would have the same kind of positive result that medication would have. Right. I mean, school is sometimes just going to be boring,
Starting point is 00:32:25 and if your particular brain has a hard time focusing on boring stuff, I guess it's harder. What are the implications of that for treatment? You know, it's interesting. It might not have implications for treatment, but it changes, I think, the way you think about your treatment and the way you think about yourself, the way you think about your own brain.
Starting point is 00:32:43 I think when we give kids the message that this is a brain-based problem, that it's a disorder in the brain, that that's something that they take in as a sort of identity, as a message about who they are and what they can accomplish. And if instead it is portrayed, I think more accurately, as a mismatch between where you are and how your brain works, that says something very different to kids. It says maybe medication is the right thing right now, but it's also possible that in the future, this is gonna change, that you're gonna find your way
Starting point is 00:33:13 to another kind of situation that's not like high school English class, where your brain is actually gonna be really powerful and really well-suited. There was one thing that you mentioned in the research, that some children with ADHD symptoms are at greater risk of more serious issues and those are kids whose symptoms are accompanied by intense angers. What are the different risks there?
Starting point is 00:33:36 Yeah, so this is the research of a researcher in Oregon named Joel Nigg and I think this is really important. I think that one of the downsides of us having perceived this condition as this sort of yes or no, black or white disorder, is that it has obscured the fact that there are really lots of different degrees of intensity of ADHD. So, you know, the young guy I talked to who, you know, was using it for baseball as much as he was using it for school, he almost certainly does not have a really intense case of ADHD, but there are lots of kids who do. And what Joel Nick has found is that between 30 and 40% of young people diagnosed with
Starting point is 00:34:16 ADHD also have symptoms of intense anger as children. And that is often the beginning of real psychological problems. When you have both hyperactivity and impulse control and intense emotional dysregulation, that's a real warning sign. And that those kids, it's not just about changing their environment. They really do need treatment, which might include
Starting point is 00:34:39 stimulant medication. But it also might include other things. Those young people are more likely to have coexisting other disorders, oppositional, defiant disorder, depression, anxiety, and so they need a more careful set of treatments. You know, I'm sure we're going to get a lot of reaction to this interview because it affects so many people and people's experiences are all unique and there is disagreement about this.
Starting point is 00:35:03 You know, I happen to look at this magazine called Attitude, that's ADD Attude, you're familiar with this, which describes itself as the nation's leading source of important news expert advice and judgment free understanding for families and adults living with attention deficit disorder. I just went to the website and right away was struck by a story attacking your article,
Starting point is 00:35:24 calls it misrepresentative, biased, and dangerous. Have you looked at this stuff? I have, yeah. Yeah. I mean, it specifically says that in some cases you quoted people who you didn't interview. Wes Crenshaw says it, he told a Times fact checker before the article was published that the reporter's information was incorrect and asked for an interview to set the record straight, none was granted.
Starting point is 00:35:44 Generally, what do you say about this? that the reporter's information was incorrect and asked for an interview to set the record straight and none was granted. Generally, what do you say about this? Well, I'll say first that that particular claim that Wes Crenshaw asked for an interview is not true. So the reason that Attitude is responding so intensely, I think, is because I wrote about this magazine about Attitude in my article. And one of the things that I noticed in Attitude
Starting point is 00:36:03 was that there were a number of articles aimed at parents of children who were resisting the diagnosis and resisting medication treatment. And I talked to a lot of these kids, right, who were themselves being treated and who had really mixed feelings about it. And so the idea that parents need a strategy to persuade their kids to take medication felt really out of keeping with the research that I was reading.
Starting point is 00:36:30 That medication works for some kids, but it doesn't work for all kids. That on the whole, the benefits of stimulant medication outweigh the deficits for most people. But for individual kids, sometimes it is not the right choice. And I don't think that it's the job of parents to try to persuade their kids to keep taking this medication when they don't want to, as you know, the research shows that lots of them don't want to and lots of them stop.
Starting point is 00:36:53 And so these articles that from Attitude magazine that I mentioned that they're responding to, we're all in one way or another trying to give parents arguments and approaches to try to convince their kids to take this medication. And so that's what I wrote about. It's true I didn't go follow up and interview these people because I was just quoting accurately the articles that they wrote in this magazine. And I think that the magazine felt wounded by the fact that I'd identified these articles as being a part of that movement.
Starting point is 00:37:23 You know, the human mind is an awfully complicated thing, to say the least. And there are many circumstances in treatment in which medications are effective for reasons that just aren't clear. I think that was true of a lot of antidepressants for a long time. It still is.
Starting point is 00:37:40 And I'm just wondering, as you looked at this research on ADHD, are there people who are optimistic that they will get to a clearer understanding of its origin? I think so, yeah. I mean, I think there's a mix of feelings about it. I think there are hopes that that sort of, you know, intense scientific research will help us. But I think there are more people, and, including some of the researchers who I wrote about,
Starting point is 00:38:05 who said that that's not where our research focus is going to be most useful, that there may be two directions that seem most useful. One is what Joel Nigg is doing and trying to figure out if there are subtypes within this group that is diagnosed with ADHD who have different problems, different maybe genetic signatures, and different needs for treatment. And if we, instead of saying you've got ADHD or you don't, if we can be more precise in what intensity of ADHD, what type of ADHD you have, we can give you more helpful treatment suggestions. And then I think the other angle that I find really reassuring is to think about environments.
Starting point is 00:38:44 So instead of to think of this as just a biological problem that has a biological solution, think about the environmental side of it. And a lot of the research suggests that as environments change for young people, their symptoms change as well. But mostly what we see is that just kind of happens as you go through life.
Starting point is 00:39:03 There is not a big push, and I think there hasn't been a lot of study of if we are actually intentional about how we change young people's environments, can we predictably make a difference in their symptoms. My hope is that those two strands of research together will make a real difference, that we not only will be able to say, well, this is the particular signature of ADHD that you have, that we'll also be able to say, and this is the kind of intervention, whether environmental or pharmaceutical, that might help your symptoms the most. Well, Paul Tuff, thank you so much for speaking with us.
Starting point is 00:39:37 Thank you. Paul Tuff is a contributing writer for the New York Times Magazine. His recent cover story is titled, Have We Been Thinking About ADHD All Wrong? Coming up, Martin Johnson reviews a new tribute to Anthony Braxton, who Johnson says is one of the most polarizing figures in jazz. This is fresh air. Composer and multi-instrumentalist Anthony Braxton is one of the most polarizing figures in jazz. To fans he's a remarkable artist who's refused to set limits on his vision or his musical range. To his detractors he's the man who made sterile and
Starting point is 00:40:14 impenetrable music with numbers and geometric diagrams for titles instead of words. Braxton, whose won a MacArthur Fellowship and an NEA Jazz Master Award, turns 80 this year. One of his protégés, Steve Lehman, has created a tribute that highlights the composer's early work and shows the roads to and from his music. It would be impossible to do a tribute to all of Anthony Braxton's music. There are operas, there are large-scale ensembles like his Composition 84 for four orchestras, and there are iconic, intimate ensembles from the late 80s and early 90s.
Starting point is 00:41:20 But his early 70s work is great and often overlooked. Steve Lehman, who studied with Braxton at Wesleyan, then played with him for nearly a decade, has created a smart, riveting tribute to his mentor, the music of Anthony Braxton, by focusing on his early compositions, tunes that are a cornerstone of his formidable reputation. We just heard their take on a piece called 34A that showcases the composer's penchant for horn lines that coil like an Escher drawing. This is an ordinary tribute by Lehman. In addition to music by Braxton, Lehman includes two
Starting point is 00:41:54 of his own pay-ons to his mentor. Lehmann has followed Braxton's example in accepting no conventional limits. His catalog includes covers of Oteker and Wu-Tang Clan, his superb octet integrates spectral music, and he co-leads Celebeyon, a hip-hop fusion project with H. Prism of Anti-Pop Consortium, and Senegalese rapper Gaston Bandemic. A unifying theme in Lehman's varied music is force. There's a relentless urgency to his saxophone, perhaps the impact of his other key influence, saxophones Jackie McLean. So it's no surprise that he recorded this music live at the ETA Club in Los Angeles,
Starting point is 00:42:59 where Lehman presently lives. It's an implicit rebuke to those who think Anthony's music is dry and academic. There's a roaring crowd after each number, sometimes after each solo, and a you-are-there immediacy to the sound. Lehman is working with his regular trio, which features the propulsive bass of Matt Brewer and powerhouse drummer Damian Reed, plus guest saxophonist Mark Turner. The arrangements have a loose-limbed effect. Reed is swinging, Brewer is walking, and the horns play freely. Turner shares with Braxton an admiration of saxophonist Warren Marsh. Braxton has often honored his roots.
Starting point is 00:44:02 He's recorded albums of standards and paid tribute to his heroes like Lenny Tristano, Charlie Parker, Andrew Hill, and Thelonious Monk. Here Lehman's ensemble does Monk's tinkle-tinkle with a subtle nod to Anthony as a horn duet an abstraction with the tune slowly coming into view. Lehmann was born in New York City in 1978 and spent some of his youth in Paris. Like many of his peers, Jason Moran and Vijay Iyer to name two, he grew up with hip-hop as an established genre and with electronic music as an emerging one. He brings a unique rhythmic acuity into his appreciation of Braxton. He was pointed toward Anthony by his mother. Braxton was a good advisor for a young musician
Starting point is 00:45:28 with wide-ranging interest and clearly inspired Lehman and many others to follow as many of them as possible. ["Jazz on the Wall Street Journal"] Martin Johnson writes about jazz for the Wall Street Journal and Downbeat. He reviewed the music of Anthony Braxton by Steve Lehman. And to find out what's happening behind the scenes of our show and get our producers' recommendations for what to watch, read and listen to, subscribe to our free newsletter at whyy.org.freshair. Fresh Air's executive producer is Danny Miller. Our technical director and engineer is Audrey
Starting point is 00:46:15 Bentham. Our managing producer is Sam Brigger. Our interviews and reviews are produced and edited by Phyllis Myers, Roberta Shorrock, Ann Marie Baldonado, Lauren Krenzel, Theresa Madden, Monique Nazareth, Susan Yakundi, and Anna Bauman. Our digital media producer is Molly C.V. Nesbur. Thea Challenger directed today's show. For Terry Gross and Tanya Moseley, I'm Dave Davies.

There aren't comments yet for this episode. Click on any sentence in the transcript to leave a comment.