Futility Closet - 202-The Rosenhan Experiment
Episode Date: May 28, 2018In the 1970s psychologist David Rosenhan sent healthy volunteers to 12 psychiatric hospitals, where they claimed to be hearing voices. Once they were admitted, they behaved normally, but the hospital...s diagnosed all of them as seriously mentally ill. In this week's episode of the Futility Closet podcast we'll describe the Rosenhan experiment, which challenged the validity of psychiatric diagnosis and set off a furor in the field. We'll also spot hawks at Wimbledon and puzzle over a finicky payment processor. Intro: In 2002, Burkard Polster investigated the mathematics of shoelaces. A raindrop that lands on Montana's Triple Divide Peak might arrive at any of three oceans. Sources for our feature on the Rosenhan experiment: Roger R. Hock, Forty Studies That Changed Psychology, 2009. Dusan Kecmanovic, Controversies and Dilemmas in Contemporary Psychiatry, 2017. Donald O. Granberg and John F. Galliher, A Most Human Enterprise, 2010. David Rosenhan, "On Being Sane in Insane Places," Science 179:4070 (Jan. 19, 1973), 250–258. Paul R. Fleischman et al., "Psychiatric Diagnosis," Science, New Series 180:4084 (April 27, 1973), 356+358+360-369. Robert L. Spitzer, "On Pseudoscience in Science, Logic in Remission, and Psychiatric Diagnosis: A Critique of Rosenhan's 'On Being Sane in Insane Places,'" Journal of Abnormal Psychology 84:5, 442–452. Ulric Neisser, "Reversibility of Psychiatric Diagnoses," Science, New Series 180:4091 (June 15, 1973), 1116. Martin Bulmer, "Are Pseudo-Patient Studies Justified?," Journal of Medical Ethics 8:2 (June 1982), 65-71. Peter C. Gaughwin, "On Being Insane in Medico-Legal Places: The Importance of Taking a Complete History in Forensic Mental Health Assessment," Psychiatry, Psychology and Law 12:2 (2005), 298-310. Theodore Millon, "Reflections on Rosenhan's 'On Being Sane in Insane Places,'" Journal of Abnormal Psychology 84:5 (October 1975), 456-461. Maurice K. Temerlin, "Suggestion Effects in Psychiatric Diagnosis," Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 147:4 (October 1968), 349–353. Murray J. Goddard, "Personal Accounts: On Being Possibly Sane in Possibly Insane Places," Psychiatric Services 62:8 (August 2011), 831-832. Jared M. Bartels and Daniel Peters, "Coverage of Rosenhan's 'On Being Sane in Insane Places' in Abnormal Psychology Textbooks," Teaching of Psychology 44:2 (2017), 169-173. Marti Loring and Brian Powell, "Gender, Race, and DSM-III: A Study of the Objectivity of Psychiatric Diagnostic Behavior," Journal of Health and Social Behavior 29:1 (March 1988), 1–22. Jim Schnabel, "Puck in the Laboratory: The Construction and Deconstruction of Hoaxlike Deception in Science," Science, Technology, & Human Values 19:4 (October 1, 1994), 459-492. Michael Fontaine, "On Being Sane in an Insane Place -- The Rosenhan Experiment in the Laboratory of Plautus' Epidamnus," Current Psychology: Research and Reviews 32:4 (December 2013), 348-365. Mark Moran, "Writer Ignites Firestorm With Misdiagnosis Claims," Psychiatric News 41:7 (April 7, 2006), 10–12. Sandra Blakeslee, "8 Feign Insanity in Test and Are Termed Insane," New York Times, Jan. 21, 1973. Nathaniel Morris, "This Secret Experiment Tricked Psychiatrists Into Diagnosing Sane People as Having Schizophrenia," Washington Post, Dec. 29, 2017. Claudia Hammond, "One Flew Into the Cuckoo's Nest," Times, July 27, 2009, 8. Richard M. Restak, "Medicine of the Mind," Wilson Quarterly 7:4 (Autumn 1983), 112-118. Listener mail: Pierre Bertrand, "Feral Parakeets Taking Over London, Prompting Concerns They'll Push Out Native Birds," CBC News, Feb. 14, 2016. Wikipedia, "Feral Parakeets in Great Britain," (accessed May 23, 2018). James Owen, "Feral Parrot Population Soars in U.K., Study Says," National Geographic News, July 8, 2004. Hazel Jackson, "Move Aside, Pigeons: Wild Parakeets Poised for World Domination," CNN, Aug. 17, 2016. Oliver Pickup, "Introducing Rufus the Hawk: The Official Bird Scarer of the Wimbledon Championships," Telegraph, July 3, 2017. Wikipedia, "Rufus the Hawk" (accessed May 23, 2018). "Rufus the Hawk Back at Work," BT, June 25, 2013. Bryony Gordon, "Rufus the Hawk: Quails, Baths And Me-Time -- Meet Wimbledon's Biggest Diva," Telegraph, July 2, 2012. "Nosy Neighbour," finalist, 2016 Natural History Museum Wildlife Photographer of the Year, 2016. Sam Hobson's photography. This week's lateral thinking puzzle was contributed by listener Tommy Honton, who sent this corroborating link (warning -- this spoils the puzzle). You can listen using the player above, download this episode directly, or subscribe on Apple Podcasts or Google Play Music or via the RSS feed at http://feedpress.me/futilitycloset. Please consider becoming a patron of Futility Closet -- you can choose the amount you want to pledge, and we've set up some rewards to help thank you for your support. You can also make a one-time donation on the Support Us page of the Futility Closet website. Many thanks to Doug Ross for the music in this episode. If you have any questions or comments you can reach us at podcast@futilitycloset.com. Thanks for listening!
Transcript
Discussion (0)
Welcome to the Futility Closet podcast, forgotten stories from the pages of history.
Visit us online to sample more than 10,000 quirky curiosities from optimized shoelaces
to a triple watershed.
This is episode 202.
I'm Greg Ross.
And I'm Sharon Ross. In the 1970s, psychologist David Rosenhan sent healthy volunteers to 12
psychiatric hospitals where they claimed to be hearing voices. Once they were admitted,
they behaved normally, but the hospitals diagnosed all of them as seriously mentally ill.
In today's show, we'll describe the Rosenhan experiment, which challenged the validity of psychiatric diagnosis and set off a furor in the field.
We'll also spot hawks at Wimbledon and puzzle over a finicky payment processor.
In the 1950s, 60s, and 70s, some critics began to feel that psychiatric hospitals had become
prisons for eccentric people
and that the labels that were applied to them there might not be reliable. One of those critics
was David Rosenhan, a professor of psychology and law at Stanford University. He worried that
normality was rather shakily defined and that psychiatrists weren't judging it consistently.
He wrote, it is commonplace, for example, to read about murder trials wherein eminent
psychiatrists for the defense are contradicted by equally eminent psychiatrists for the prosecution He wrote, To study that question, he set up an unusual experiment.
He put together a group of eight healthy people, whom he called pseudopatients.
They included a psychology grad student, three psychologists, including himself,
a pediatrician, a psychiatrist, a painter, and a housewife.
Three were women and five were men.
Then, over the course of several years, these eight pseudopatients presented themselves to a total of 12 psychiatric hospitals in five states on the east and west
coasts. They said that they'd been hearing voices saying the words empty, hollow, and thud.
Rosenhan said he chose those words because they seemed to reflect existential concerns about the
meaninglessness of life, as if the hallucinating person were saying, my life is empty and hollow.
But in fact, they
don't actually correspond to any specific diagnosis. All the pseudo-patients gave false names and
professions, but otherwise they told the truth. And on the basis of that one symptom, all of them
were admitted to their hospitals. One hospital assigned a diagnosis of manic depressive psychosis
and the other 11, schizophrenia. Once they were inside, they stopped reporting the hallucinations.
They spoke to the
other patients and to staff members as they normally would to anyone. When the staff asked
them how they were feeling, they said they were fine and were no longer experiencing symptoms.
Each of them kept a written record of their interactions on the ward. At first, they did
that in secret, but it became clear that the staff had no objection and saw it as evidence
of compulsive note-taking. And the other patients, interestingly, often guessed that they were journalists or researchers.
The nursing reports from this time say that the pseudopatients were friendly, cooperative,
and exhibited no abnormal indications.
But Rosenhan says the staff consistently viewed them through the lens of their diagnoses.
When one pseudopatient who was a professional painter sat down one day and began to paint,
the staff called this painting behavior.
When a group
of patients assembled outside the cafeteria waiting for a meal to be served, one psychiatrist
said they were exhibiting an oral acquisitive syndrome. Generally, the suit of patients said
they felt powerless and depersonalized. On average, they spent 6.8 minutes per day interacting with
professional staff such as psychiatrists, and they were administered a total of 2,100 pills,
primarily tranquilizers, which they hid in their cheeks and flushed down the toilet.
In the end, none of them were detected. That is, no one suspected that they were normal people who
were just faking their symptoms. One was discharged with a diagnosis of schizophrenia, the others with
schizophrenia in remission. Altogether, their hospital stays ranged from 7 to 52 days, with an
average stay of 19 days.
The staff of a research and teaching hospital heard of these findings and said that they
doubted they would make the same mistake.
Rosenhan said he'd send one or more pseudo-patients to that hospital over the following three
months to see whether the staff could detect them.
Over those three months, the hospital admitted 193 patients.
Of those, 41 were judged with high confidence by at least one staff member to be
pseudopatients. 23 were considered suspect by at least one psychiatrist, and 19 were suspected by
one psychiatrist and one other staff member. In fact, as you may have guessed, Rosenhan had sent
none at all. Rosenhan wrote up these results in an article he called On Being Sane in Insane Places,
and it was published in the journal Science in January 1973. He wrote, it is clear that we cannot distinguish the sane from the insane in psychiatric
hospitals. The hospital itself imposes a special environment in which the meanings of behavior can
easily be misunderstood. In effect, he claimed that his first experiment had shown that psychiatric
hospitals can mistake sane people for insane ones, and the second experiment had shown that they can mistake insane people for sane ones. He wrote, any diagnostic process that
lends itself so readily to massive errors of this sort cannot be a very reliable one.
One particular problem, he said, is what he called the stickiness of the diagnostic label.
Once you're labeled schizophrenic, that becomes the central fact about you, and everything you do is
seen in that light. For example, when one of the pseudo-patients approached a doctor and said, pardon me,
Dr. Blank, could you tell me when I am eligible for grounds privileges? The doctor just said,
good morning, Dave, how are you today? And walked on. When a nurse found a pseudo-patient pacing
the corridor, she said, nervous, Mr. X? He said, no, bored. She hadn't seemed to realize that there
was nothing to do. In one hospital where Rosenhan himself was a pseudo-patient, he saw a nurse adjust her bra in a ward full of men, not to be
provocative, but he said, because she didn't notice us. She didn't seem to regard the people around her
as fully human. And if a patient had an outburst because he was mistreated by an attendant, a nurse
would rarely ask what had happened. She assumed that the patient's behavior was a sign of his
pathology. Rosenhan wrote, never were the staff found to assume that one of themselves or the structure of the hospital
had anything to do with the patient's behavior. And yet, as I mentioned, the other patients often
suspected that the pseudo patients were healthy. During the first three hospitalizations, 35 out
of 118 patients on the admissions ward said, you're not crazy, you're a journalist or a professor,
you're checking up on the hospital.
Rosenhan wrote, the fact that the patients often recognize normality when the staff did not raises important questions. Rosenhan acknowledged that in general, physicians prefer to make the mistake
of judging a healthy person to be sick than a sick person to be healthy. He wrote, better to err on
the side of caution to suspect illness even among the healthy. But he says that psychiatric
diagnoses are different because they carry stigma and that stigma is hard to shed. And he told the New York Times,
the psychiatric diagnoses, once they're made, are rarely changed. He said, a diagnosis of cancer
that has been found in error is a cause of celebration, but psychiatric diagnoses are
rarely found in error. The label sticks. And even beyond that, we're all anxious or depressed or
act out or behave strangely from
time to time. Rosenhan says maybe it would be best if mental health professionals focused on specific
problems and behaviors rather than applying labels that are hard to shed and that depersonalize and
dehumanize people. As you might imagine, the article brought an outraged response from the
psychiatric community. They began with a title on being sane in insane places. Sanity and insanity
are legal terms, they said, not medical ones.
Generally, being sane means being able to tell right from wrong,
so it's not clear what Rosenhan thought he was studying.
The psychiatrists argued that it wasn't their job to look for hoaxers.
In any field of medicine, a patient could show up claiming fake symptoms
and get an unnecessary treatment.
Even if the doctor is tricked, that doesn't mean the diagnostic methods are invalid.
One wrote, If I were to drink a quart of blood and, concealing what I had done, come to the emergency room of any hospital vomiting blood, the behavior of the staff would be quite predictable.
If they labeled and treated me as having a bleeding peptic ulcer, I doubt that I could
argue convincingly that medical science does not know how to diagnose that condition.
On top of this, it's extremely rare for healthy people to submit themselves to psychiatric hospitals, and turning one away could incur legal liability,
so it's not surprising that the hospitals took them seriously. At the same time, many critics
pointed out that the admitting staff at these hospitals don't seem to have followed the normal
procedure. It seems strange that so many pseudopatients could have been admitted as
inpatients on the strength of a single subjective symptom, especially if, as Rosenhan said, the
words empty, hollow, and thud were not associated with schizophrenia in any psychiatry
textbook. No one seems to have considered that the pseudopatients might be malingering,
and no one did any organic workups. Investigating auditory hallucinations can require lumbar
punctures, skull x-rays, and brain scans, and it's not clear that Rosenhan had considered that or
warned his volunteers of at-risk. Once the pseudopatients were inside, Rosenhan had written that they would
have to get out by their own devices, essentially by convincing the staff that they were sane.
But that's not what they did. They stayed passively in the ward, and they mentioned that their symptoms
had stopped only if someone asked them about it. The critics pointed out that that's not really
normal behavior. A normal person would have gone to the desk and said, Hi, I lied to get in here and I'd like to go home now, please.
Rosenhan seemed to assume that behaving normally in a psych ward would be noticeable, but it's not.
A psych ward is designed in part to allay distress,
and a psychiatric patient doesn't act floridly crazy all the time.
That's a stereotype.
Yeah, I mean, that's a good point, right?
Even if you do have a mental illness, you're not acting symptomatic 24 hours a day necessarily.
The symptoms can wax and wane.
So if you just sit there and you don't even mention that your symptoms have abated unless someone asks, that's not going to make you stand out.
Because they don't know.
You could still be having the hallucinations, for example.
Yeah.
The critics also said that Rosenhan seemed to be making a lot of guesses about the thoughts and motives of the staff without any evidence. Rosenhan himself had been admitted to several
hospitals under the name David Lurie. He wrote, I can still recall my own impulse to go up to
the nurses and say, you think I'm David Lurie. Well, I'm not. I'm David Rosenhan, professor of
psychology. It was only my anticipation of their likely response. Do you often think you're a
professor of psychology? It stopped me doing it. But the critics asked, how did he know he was right about that?
How could he know that that's how they'd respond?
Finally, many critics pointed out that the patients had been released from the hospital
with a diagnosis of schizophrenia in remission, meaning that the staff thought they weren't
psychotic, which they weren't.
That's not a failure.
The Yale psychiatrist Paul Fleischman wrote, Rosenhan's study demonstrates that despite
false historical data and the set of the hospital environment,
12 non-psychotics were observed by the psychiatrist to be non-psychotic, a record of 100% accuracy.
Although they were still given a diagnosis.
Yeah, and it's saying it's in remission.
Presuming that they had been psychotic.
Which I think would be Rosenhan's point, that you're still carrying that label out of the hospital.
Yeah.
Even if they say it's in remission, you're still schizophrenic.
Yeah.
One of the most vociferous critics of Rosenhan's study was Robert Spitzer,
who's been called the father of modern psychiatry.
He called the experiment pseudoscience masquerading as science.
Spitzer argued that the fact that so many of the pseudopatients
were given the same diagnosis when they were admitted
shows the reliability of the diagnosis,
and that the diagnosis of schizophrenia in remission when they were released
shows that the staff had noticed that the symptoms had abated. The diagnosis of in
remission was rarely given at that time. Rosenhan did have some defenders. After the flurry of
objections in science, the Cornell psychologist Ulrich Nieser wrote in to thank him for pointing
out that psychiatric diagnoses, unlike those in other branches of medicine, are rarely reversed.
He wrote, internists, neurologists, and pediatricians sometimes have to admit errors, but a psychiatrist
never does. It is not he who is remiss, but the schizophrenia which is in remission.
A medical diagnosis is much like a hypothesis in science. It should lead to further predictions
and be subject to disconfirmation. In science, hypotheses that cannot be disproved by any
conceivable evidence are not hypotheses at all. Should we not conclude that diagnoses which cannot be disproved are equally meaningless?
By showing that the diagnosis of schizophrenia is essentially irreversible, no matter how the
patient subsequently behaves, Rosenhan has dealt the scientific pretensions of psychiatry a serious
blow. Whatever we think of Rosenhan's study, it took place 45 years ago, and the field of
psychiatry has changed enormously since then.
The problems that Rosenhan pointed out began to decline as fewer people were admitted to psychiatric hospitals, particularly through the increased use of antipsychotic medications that
were discovered in the 1950s. Those reduced symptoms enough that patients can live outside
the hospital and lead relatively normal lives. And there's been a rise in community mental health
facilities, crisis intervention centers, and behavior therapies that avoid labels and focus on specific problems and behaviors.
If one of Rosenhan's pseudo-patients appeared at an emergency room today, they'd examine
him and might do some lab tests, and maybe consult a psychiatrist if that workup didn't
present anything remarkable.
And even the psychiatrist would look into the patient's background and talk to previous
providers and others who might be able to shed some light on the case.
Writing in the Washington Post, Nathaniel Morris of the Stanford University School of
Medicine said, to be hospitalized, patients need to have symptoms of a psychiatric disorder, such
as hearing voices, suffering from depression, and feeling suicidal, that are so profound as to cause
safety concerns or significant impairments in daily living, such as dysfunction at work or home.
Also, Rosenhan's study made no mention of insurance or the costs of care.
Today, insurance won't pay for a patient to remain in a hospital if they don't have symptoms.
If anything, getting hospital care today may be too hard.
One small study in 2001 followed seven people with well-documented histories of schizophrenia
who presented themselves to mental health intake offices when they were actually in crisis.
Six of those seven were denied treatment, often because of a lack of resources.
Rosenhan's study had a profound impact on popular perceptions of psychiatry when it first appeared in 1973, and it became one of the most widely cited articles in psychological journals.
But its influence seems to be waning. In the late 1980s, it was mentioned in 80% of introductory
psychology textbooks, but last year it appeared in only 45%. Rosenhan died in 2012, so we don't know what he'd
make of all this, but in 1993, 20 years after his study, he still seemed to have doubts. He said,
I don't have any data, but my impression is that no, the nature of psychiatric hospitals has not
changed much, and the issues that were brought up by that paper have been dealt with to some extent,
but it is not my view that things have changed greatly. We just wouldn't be able to commit to the amount of time that the show takes to make if it weren't for the donations and pledges we get.
If you'd like to contribute to our celebration of the quirky and the curious, you can find a donate button in the support us section of the website at futilitycloset.com.
Or if you'd like to support us in a more ongoing way, you can join our Patreon campaign,
where you'll also get access to outtakes, extra information and discussion on some of the stories,
where you'll also get access to outtakes, extra information and discussion on some of the stories,
more lateral thinking puzzles, and updates on what Sasha, the Futility Closet feline, has been doing lately.
You can check out our Patreon page at patreon.com slash futilitycloset, or see the link at the website. And thanks again to everyone who is a part of Futility Closet.
of Futility Closet.
Duncan Reynolds wrote,
Dear Sasha,
Your team's conversation about London's foxes and pigeons
in episode 195
reminded me that there are now
parrots living wild in London,
having escaped from aviaries
or, according to some theories,
from film sets or rock stars,
and set up a stable breeding population
sometime last century.
And Duncan suggested that Sasha could pass this along to us if she thought it might be
of interest.
Sasha does generally have a rather high interest in birds, especially somewhat smaller ones
like parakeets, which it turns out are the kind of parrots now living feraly in the UK.
Wow.
These British feral parakeets are specifically ring-necked or rose-ringed parakeets,
which are a non-migratory species that is native to southern Asia and sub-Saharan Africa,
and the ones in Britain are thought to be from the foothills of the Himalayas around Pakistan.
This parakeet population had been concentrated primarily in the suburban areas of London and
nearby Kent, but they are doing so well that
they are now being seen in other parts of Britain too, and as far north as Inverness, Scotland.
As Duncan noted, there are several theories on how these parakeets came to be living fairly in
the London area in the first place. Some say that Jimi Hendrix deliberately released a breeding
pair to inject more color into the London streets in the 1960s.
Others think that the birds escaped from the film set of The African Queen in 1951,
or from the London Zoo when it was damaged during the Blitz in World War II,
or from a cargo ship that capsized near London.
Experts think that the more boring but likely explanation is just that the birds are popular as pets,
and some either escaped or were released from private homes. the more boring but likely explanation is just that the birds are popular as pets, and
some either escaped or were released from private homes.
And more than one theory could be right, as the biodiversity of the wild parakeet population
suggests that the birds have escaped from captivity more than once.
Wild ring-necked parakeets were actually spotted in Britain back in the late 1800s, but they
apparently didn't survive.
It's thought that possibly the now warmer winters due to climate change,
combined with the increasing numbers of people who feed birds,
has allowed this new population to not just survive, but to flourish.
Yeah, I was going to say, I wouldn't have thought that London's climate would be the
best for parakeets, but I guess if these might be a variety that normally lives in
mountainous foothills, maybe that's not too far off.
Yeah. And with the winters being a little warmer than they used to be,
they're thinking that that helps them out a lot. And this sounds funny to say about pretty little
birds, but these parakeets are actually considered to be one of the most successful invasive species
in the world right now. As is common when a non-native species moves into an area,
there are concerns about the parakeet's negative effects on native bird species, as well as concerns about agriculture, especially orchards and vineyards.
For example, a few years ago, parakeets ate about one-third of the harvest at a vineyard southwest of London.
That's a lot.
These parakeets have no natural predators in the UK and have been recorded to live as long as 34 years.
have no natural predators in the UK and have been recorded to live as long as 34 years.
Hazel Jackson, a postdoctoral research associate at the University of Kent who studies the bird's expansion, says that the ring-necked parakeet is currently the fastest growing bird species
in the UK. She actually wrote an article for CNN entitled, Move Aside Pigeons, Wild Parakeets Poised
for World Domination. And Jackson notes that there are now
independently established breeding populations of wild parakeets in at least 65 cities in Europe,
including in Belgium, Spain, Germany, Turkey, and the Netherlands. Overall, wild parakeets are now
found in more than 30 countries across five continents, including near the St. Lawrence
River Basin in Canada, and a large
population in Bakersfield, California that is believed to stem from an aviary that was destroyed
in a storm in 1977. Many of these populations appear to be well-established and increasing,
leading to Jackson's statement that parakeets are on a trajectory to global domination.
And I will note that Sasha stands ready to defend our yard should they decide to spread here,
especially if they will fight fair and stay on the ground.
So while parakeets might represent a future problem,
pigeons are currently the nuisance birds for many cities,
though as we discussed in episode 195, sometimes the answer for that is another bird.
Christian Clarkson tweeted at us,
On bird control, the All England Tennis Club keep pigeons away from Wimbledon with a resident hawk.
He's called Rufus. And Rufus the hawk has been the professional Wimbledon pigeon
frightener since 2007, taking over at that time from Hamish the hawk. And Rufus is tasked with
keeping pigeons from interrupting the championship games. Though Rufus is responsible for keeping the All England Club's 42-acre grounds pigeon-free year-round,
he has also found the time to scare off pigeons from Westminster Abbey,
as well as hospitals, airfields, and landfills.
What a strange job.
To be a pigeon frightener.
Yeah, I mean, I wonder what he thinks he's doing.
I guess he's just being a hawk, but it's got like certain boundaries on it.
Like it's not just a regular natural.
Right, and they have to actually train him and feed him well enough so that he doesn't eat the pigeons.
He just frightens them off.
It must be quite difficult to manage that, yeah.
In June 2012, Rufus was birdnapped from the back of his owner's car while it was parked on a private street,
and the theft had significant media coverage with Rufus being called the world's most notable bird.
He was found three days later with a sore leg but otherwise quite well,
and was fussed over quite a bit by his very relieved owner Imogen Davis by being given baths and his favorite treat of quail.
Davis actually did sound quite attached to him and was pretty upset while he was missing.
She was quoted in the Telegraph as saying that he was going to have to sleep next to
her pillow at night so that she could make sure he was safe.
Rufus has accounts on Twitter and Facebook as well as a Wimbledon security photo card
pass with his official job title of bird scarer on it.
No word on whether he'd be up for scaring off parakeets, though I would guess that he would. And on the subject of Britain's
urban foxes from episode 195, Charles Hargrove wrote, I thought you might like the photo from
the Wildlife Photographer of the Year exhibition at the London Natural History Museum last year.
Photographer of the Year exhibition at the London Natural History Museum last year.
And Charles sent a link to a photo taken by Sam Hobson, a photographer who specializes in urban wildlife. The photo, which we'll have a link to in the show notes, is of a cute little fox using
his front paws to hold himself up so that he can just peek over the top of a wall in Bristol.
The text with the photo says that Hobson wanted to capture the inquisitive
nature of the urban red fox and that he spent time gaining the trust of this fox family so
that he could get his photo. It probably helped that this family of foxes is used to people as
they are regularly fed by several local residents, which I guess implies that at least some people
seem to like the foxes. It turns out that Sam Hobson has a website of photos of urban wildlife,
including a whole series on foxes and a rather dramatic photo of feral parakeets flying over a
darkened cemetery. I thought that many of the photos were quite arresting, like seeing a full
grown male deer in the middle of a nighttime London street or a badger framed against a large
headstone as it climbed out
of a grave. Hobson has managed to catch many interesting moments and can really make you
aware of how much urban wildlife there actually is. Link to the site, of course, in the show notes.
Thanks so much to everyone who writes in to us. We learn so much from our listeners.
If you have anything that you'd like to share, please send it to us at podcast at futilitycloset.com. It's Greg's turn to try to solve a lateral thinking puzzle. I'm
going to give him an odd sounding situation, and he has to try to figure out what's going on,
asking only yes or no questions. This puzzle comes from one of our more regular puzzle
contributors,
Tommy Houghton, who says, here's a new puzzle and spoiler, no one dies. Thought this was just
fascinating. We'll see how long it takes Greg to get this. Here's Tommy's puzzle. A payment
processing company regularly denies transactions based on information entered into the name field.
Why? Wow. Is this true?
Okay.
Payment processing, do I need to pursue that?
That's just what I think it is?
Like an online payment processor.
Yeah.
Regularly denies transactions based on... Information entered into the name field.
Okay.
Information entered, does that mean something other than a name?
No.
Okay.
So someone enters their name into that field.
Yes.
And in some circumstances, this processor will reject the transaction on that basis alone.
Is that fair to say?
Yes.
Okay.
Oh, that's very interesting.
Is it based on gender or sex?
Yes.
Really?
Yes.
So just so I know, does it deny women?
It denies female names, people who enter female names.
Okay, so just to nail this down. Women? It denies female names, people who enter female names. Okay.
So just to nail this down, is that what I'm looking for?
I mean, are there other criteria that I also need to work out?
No.
That's it?
Yes.
Okay.
So I need to work out why they would do that.
Yes.
Under what situations or why they would do that.
Payment processor means these women were trying to
purchase something? Yes. Do I need to know what that is? Yes. Okay. Wow. All right. So women are
trying to buy something. If a man had bought the same thing, he would have been allowed to? Yes.
So that means the merchant, the person who's selling it, doesn't want women customers? Is that right?
That's incorrect.
Okay, so the merchant would be glad to get these women as customers?
It's the processor that's turning them down?
Would you say that?
I wouldn't say that.
Would you say the merchant doesn't want female customers?
I wouldn't say that. Well, someone doesn't want female customers? I wouldn't say that.
Well, someone doesn't want them.
Do I need to work out what they're trying to buy?
Yes.
Okay.
I don't think it's right to say that they don't want female customers.
I don't think that that's quite the right way to say it.
Do they expect the women not to pay?
That's partly something along those lines.
So you'd say they'd rather sell to men than women?
I guess I could say that, yeah.
All right, and I'm trying to see if I need to distinguish between the processor and the merchant.
Yeah, it's the payment processing company
that's denying the transactions.
But are they doing that at the request of the merchant?
I'm just trying to sign that.
So does the merchant know this is happening?
I don't know.
I mean, Tommy just knew about this happening at the payment processing company doing it.
Okay.
So I don't know what the merchant's thoughts are or feelings are on the whole business.
I'm just trying to work that out.
So possibly the merchant doesn't even know this is happening, but it's the payment processor
that's making the judgment.
Yes.
Yes.
Do I need to know about like geography or the regime where this is like?
No.
I don't need to know where this is happening.
That's correct.
With currency or anything like that.
Right.
Okay.
Why the payment processor then?
And this is for a certain class of
merchandise would you say
yes
so what a woman
wants to buy something and it's turned down
but a man isn't is what this comes down to
yes that's actually
that is what it comes down to
see I'm the naturalness thing would be to try
to figure out what she's trying to buy but that
would take a long time yeah i'm gonna say merchandise is not quite the right word a
service um i'm not sure what you would call this somewhere between merchandise and a service i'm
not sure exactly what you would call this like renting or leasing something like getting the
use of something no no no, not quite like that.
Or like a subscription or something?
Like an online access to something?
Yes.
More like that.
Yes.
Okay.
Yes.
Okay.
So that would be granted to men but not to women?
Yes.
And it's the payment processing company that's denying it, not the merchant themselves. And you were saying that has something to do with
them expecting those women
to be a greater, I guess,
risk, you'd call it? Yes.
Expecting the people who
put women's names into the
name field to be a greater risk.
That's artfully
worded. That was artfully worded.
Meaning they might think they're not truly women?
That's, yes.
Is that the concern?
That's part of the concern.
Well, if they're not women, they would be men.
So they think that some men are posing as women who,
and they're trying to prevent that?
That's not quite right.
No, that's not quite right.
But they're worried that for some reason that some of these women are not really women?
Yes.
Is that the essence of their concern?
Well, it's more than that. I mean, they don't care about that just in general terms. They don't care
if men are posing as women for ethical reasons or whatever but they they have a specific concern as
a payment processing company okay um is this like a dating app something no with that no
or like you know uber some kind of online service that has online customers not like uber not like an uber kind of thing
you were saying before is it like something that you would get online access to and i was saying
yes that was that was correct the the payment processing company is concerned about fraud
they don't want to not be paid so they are regular you know regularly denying transactions
if people enter women's names into the name field
because they think there's a much higher chance that they're not going to get paid, that it's
going to be fraud. And you're saying at least part of the time it's because they're not really women.
Yes. But that's not the whole of it. Well, that's it with what I just told you, that they're worried about the fraud angle.
So what is it that they think, okay, it's legitimate if a man puts his name in the field,
but if we see a woman's name in the field, then this is unlikely to be legitimate, is what it comes down to.
Does this have to do with sex?
Yes.
Pornography?
Yes, that's exactly it,
which is that apparently extremely few women pay for pornography.
And so Tommy says,
according to the neuroscientist who wrote A Billion Wicked Thoughts,
women do not often pay for porn.
Authors Ogus and Gaddam wrote,
according to CC Bill,
the billing service most commonly used by the online adult industry,
only 2% of all subscriptions to pornography sites are made on credit cards with women's names.
In fact, CC Bill even flags women's names, female names as potential fraud,
since so many of these charges result in an angry wife or mother demanding a refund for the misuse of her card.
It's just so rare for women to actually be legitimately buying the service. That if they get a woman's name, they just assume it's just so rare for women to actually be legitimately buying the service.
That if they get a woman's name, they just assume it's fraudulent.
They just assume it's most likely to be fraudulent.
Wow.
So thanks to Tommy for another of his non-fatal puzzles.
If anyone else has a puzzle for us to try,
please send it to podcast at futilitycloset.com.
And if you want to specify who should read the puzzle,
you can put that in the subject line.
And if you want to specify who should read the puzzle, you can put that in the subject line. everyone who's pitched in to help keep it going. You can check out our Patreon page at patreon.com slash futilitycloset or see the support us section of the website at futilitycloset.com.
While you're at the site, you can also check out Greg's collection of over 10,000 quirky curiosities, browse the Futility Closet store, learn about the Futility Closet books, and see
the show notes for the podcast with links and references for the topics we've covered. If you have any questions or comments for us, you can email us at podcast
at futilitycloset.com. Our music was written and performed by Greg's awesome brother, Doug Ross.
Thanks for listening, and we'll talk to you next week.