Hidden Brain - Radio Replay: Yum and Yuck

Episode Date: December 22, 2018

We dig into the psychology that determines the foods that make us salivate and the scents that make us squirm. ...

Transcript
Discussion (0)
Starting point is 00:00:00 This is Hidden Brain, I'm Shankar Vetantam. Hi, how are you? Thank you! Sarah and David Risa recently bought a place outside of Washington DC. They sound excited because this is the first time they're hosting both their families. We are making today a vegetable frittata with tomato, basil and goat cheese. We've got home fries. Sarah and David wanted to start a new tradition. Cook brunch with ingredients from their home garden.
Starting point is 00:00:28 The home fries have rose married from our garden and the potato has basil from our garden. This is Sarah's way of going back to her family's roots. She remembers her mother doing the very same thing. I remember growing up my mom had a garden and she would always come back with like these tiny little tomatoes and be like, I provided for my family, look, family, I grew this and we're like, yeah, that's the smallest tomato ever. But there's something about being able to grow stuff and being able to give it to people that you love that's pretty exciting. So yeah.
Starting point is 00:00:57 My grandmother would always say that this is Sarah's brother's act. The sharing food is important, but I'd say the most important part is being around the table. But food was the lure to get everyone up. Yeah, exactly. We kept us there. Yeah, exactly. Food brought us together, but it was the company and the conversation.
Starting point is 00:01:19 Today on Hidden Brain, the profound role that food plays in our lives. Food is not just nutrition that goes in your mouth or even pleasant sensations that go with it. It connects to your whole life. We look at the culture and psychology that determine what we eat, what we spit out, and when we come back for more. Paul Rosen has been studying the psychology and culture of food for more than 40 years.
Starting point is 00:01:45 He works at the University of Pennsylvania. Early in his career, Paul found himself pondering a question that few of us might think to ask, why do so many people across the world enjoy the hot, stinging pain of chili peppers? This question took Paul to a small village in Mexico, where chili peppers were as common as salt and pepper in the United States. They don't think their food tastes good without it, and the little kids don't like it. So something happens somewhere between two and five years of age. At the meals where everybody adult is eating the hot pepper and the older children are, and they're enjoying it and a little kid is thinking it's terrible and after a while some magic occurs and the little kids like it. So I thought well we had there a number of possible accounts.
Starting point is 00:02:38 Some accounts involve something fundamentally biological. And presumably the biological explanation would be if you start eating it long enough, you're eventually just going to like it for biological reasons. Yes, that's right. The brain compensates for all sorts of things. We adapt to things. This is a case of more than adaptation, where you're turning something that's negative into something positive.
Starting point is 00:02:57 I mean, to me, that's an amazing thing that we can start with something that's innately negative and make it really positive. So I said, let's take a look at the animals in the village because the dogs and the pigs eat Mexican food. They're eating tacos and hot sauce and beans and all of that's love. So I went around the village and I first asked people, do you have any dogs or pigs that like hot pepper? And they said, what are you crazy? I said, well, yeah, I mean they eat it.
Starting point is 00:03:23 And they said, that's ridiculous. I I said, well, yeah, I mean, they eat it. And they said, that's ridiculous. I said, would you mind if I give the dogs a little piece of cracker with some hot sauce on it and without and see what they choose? So they said, go ahead. And so I did, but go around the village with pigs and dogs. I put up one cracker in front of them with hot pepper sauce and another without, and I'd see what they did.
Starting point is 00:03:45 And it turns out that none of them ate the hot pepper first. They all ate the one without pepper because they're hungry, but their first choice was the one without hot pepper. So that I couldn't find an animal in the village that did what everyone over five years old in humans did, which was to gobbelist up out and prefer it. So that suggested to me that it seems to be uniquely human. And so this is not just a question of people getting used to it and getting to like it,
Starting point is 00:04:13 because the dogs and pigs are eating the garbage, the garbage is laced with chili peppers. If that was the case, that would be true for the animals as well. So what was happening in the humans, in the human brain, if you will, that allowed five-year-old children to fall in love with chili peppers and kept their canine cousins from liking it. Well, that's indeed exactly the question. We don't know how this happens, but we do know that where it happens is at the meals
Starting point is 00:04:37 where the kids are eating with their parents and their older siblings, and they keep eating it because the social pressure to eat it. Now, the animals keep eating it because the social pressure to eat it. Now, the animals keep eating it too because they're hungry, so it can't be just that they keep eating it. So, it's really, to me, a miracle. And what I realized is that it's a miracle that takes place in humans all over the world,
Starting point is 00:04:59 not just about hot pepper. It's also about liking coffee, which is bitter, and people don't like it originally, and they like it. And it goes outside the food domain. People like to go on roller coasters. Now, a roller coaster is a very negative experience the first time you have it, right? You think you're crashing to death, and your heart is pounding, and yet people pay to do this. Can you imagine a dog paying to go on a roller coaster? I mean, you know, it just won't happen. So we found that there's a whole range of things in which humans enjoy what is originally negative and they come to enjoy it as positive.
Starting point is 00:05:35 This includes the fatigue of running, it includes said movies, it includes being afraid in horror movies. This seems to be only humans. So our thought, and it's only a thought, is that what humans are enjoying is the very fact that their body thinks that something is bad, but they know it's okay. We call it benign masochism. The human have this special ability to appreciate the fact that they know that something that their body is saying is bad is actually good. And we have evidence for that. So for example, for chili pepper, the favorite degree of hotness is just below the level of unbearable pain.
Starting point is 00:06:12 They're pushing as far as they can to get their body to really screen, get this out of here, and yet know that it's okay. So it seems to be a very general feature of humans, which we tapped into by asking water a couple of billion people like hot pepper. So in some ways the theory is that you start out not liking it and then social pressure if you will, convinces you to try it and then you try it often enough that your body learns to adapt to it a little bit, but then eventually you start to like it for actually a slightly different reason.
Starting point is 00:06:44 You're no longer liking it just because of the pure pressure or the social interactions you're liking it because it gives you a sense that you're coming close to the edge of something thrilling. Is that the argument? Yes, there's something thrilling that is not threatening. Right. So for example, if something is threatening, you don't get to like it. Like people don't get to like serious pain. Right. It has to have something of the sense of it's not really threatening. And it even becomes funny. So in the case of disgust, people don't like disgusting things, right?
Starting point is 00:07:14 But they make exceptions. They eat smelly cheese. And they come to like that smell in the context of the cheese. Even though their body is saying, get this out of your mouth, it stinks. You once performed an experiment on yourself. You were with your wife, I believe, at a Korean restaurant in New York City, and you didn't interview with NPR back in 2015, where you told the reporter that it was one of the hottest things you'd ever eaten.
Starting point is 00:07:39 Tell me that story. Well, my wife at the time was a cookbook writer, and she was very interested in cuisine. We went to a Korean restaurant in New York, and the people around us, they were ordering some dish that we didn't recognize. So we said to the waiter, we would like that. And he says, you don't want that. I said, no, no, we said, we really like to try new foods.
Starting point is 00:08:01 He says, you don't want to try that food. So this went on, and we won, of course, and he brought this dish and it was unbearably hot. And we ate it because we were shamed into eating it by our own insistence that it was good. I have another story of a different sort. It also happened in New York. I had a colleague who had a dog and the dog liked to eat dog poop. So they were going to Washington Square Park and with the dog and the dog, we just hunt out a dog poop and eat it and it was just awful. You know, they hated it and the dog smelled it.
Starting point is 00:08:34 So they went to a vet and the vet said, why don't you put hot pepper on the dog poop because dogs don't like hot pepper. So they go into Washington Square, wanting to get this image. Barbara's holding the dog on a leash and her husband goes with a shaker of hot pepper, finds a dog turn and seasons it in front of everybody with all this hot pepper. Okay. And I mean, the idea of someone seasoning a dog turn
Starting point is 00:08:59 is really pretty good. And then they let the dog go and the dog ate it. And did the dog stop eating dog poop? No, no, because the dog liked dog poop more than it does like the hotness. You know, people will put up with pain to do something they really like, right? Just as Paul has shown that the sense of taste is shaped by the brain, he's also done work that shows the same thing for appetite. In one study, he set a meal down before patients who had amnesia. Yes, the general view in most of the people who work on hunger
Starting point is 00:09:35 is that hunger comes when your body reserves or low, or maybe your glucose is low, various hormones come out, and you feel this sensation of hunger and you eat until it disappears. Now there's some truth in that, of course, but there are many other higher order things. So for example, the cultural definition of what's a meal is very important.
Starting point is 00:09:56 After you finish the meal and have dessert, you stop eating. So to try to show this, we dealt with the two amnesic patients who were totally amnesic. They didn't remember anything that happened more than 30 seconds ago, but they were quite intact otherwise. So we fed them lunch in a situation where they were in a room without a clock. And we said lunchtime, and it was lunchtime, and we brought them a favorite lunch of this.
Starting point is 00:10:23 We'd asked them what their lunch was, and they, they of course ate it. Then we took the plate away. There were no more signs that they'd eaten. And ten minutes later we brought another lunch and said lunchtime and they all ate it three times each. The second lunch completely. Then we took that away, waited ten minutes and brought a third lunch. Each of these were full-sized lunches. And most of the time they ate the third lunch, twice I think one of them said, I'm getting a little stuffed, meaning their stomach was really getting full. But the point was that when you serve the meal, it looks palatable, you tend to eat it.
Starting point is 00:10:59 I was on a plane once, flying to Chicago, and at three in the afternoon, they served the full lunch. Now, everyone had eaten lunch already. But six out of the nine people who I could see ate that lunch because it was lunch and it was food and it looked good. Probably wasn't good if it was airplane food, but they ate it. So a lot of what we do is we eat when there's good food around
Starting point is 00:11:22 and when the situation is appropriate. Now, if we had left their first lunch and they saw the plate in front of them with a, you know, there was pieces of chicken bone, they might have realized that I've just eaten. And when we give people, normal people, a second meal in the same way I just described, they don't say I'm not hungry, they say, I just ate.
Starting point is 00:11:42 So it's fascinating because I think what you're saying really is that memory plays a huge role in whether we think we're hungry. Yes, being hungry is only one reason that we eat. So if you go for a full dinner, halfway through that dinner you're not hungry anymore. Right. But you're still eating the rest of the meal. That's right.
Starting point is 00:11:59 So hunger can't institute meals. The lack of it will probably discourage you from eating more, but there are other things that influence you too. When we come back, the role that culture plays in our experience of food, we also look at how you can serve more memorable meals. Stay with us. You're listening to Hidden Brain, I'm Shankar Vedantam. This is Hidden Brain, I'm Shankar Vedanta. This is Hidden Brain, I'm Shankar Vedanta.
Starting point is 00:12:29 I'm talking with psychologist Paul Rosen, who has spent many decades examining the interplay between food, identity, and culture. Food is not just nutrition that goes in your mouth, or even pleasant sensations that go with it. It connects to your whole life and it's really a very important part of performing your culture and experiencing your culture. When Paul asked people about their favorite meals, they suddenly mentioned eating at great restaurants, but they also talk about memories of meals that they shared with family and friends.
Starting point is 00:13:03 Here's a very common answer that's very short for a home meal. Every Christmas Eve, my Italian grandfather and Greek grandmother would cook a meal, consisting of creamy carbonara with bacon pieces throughout, homemade spinach pie and sausage. It was always amazing. Now, that's a lovely one, right? And it's not fancy, but you can see the emotion and the pleasure of it. And it's connected, of course, to the pleasure of family, not just to the pleasure of food. Yes, it's very social.
Starting point is 00:13:37 Pleasure is only one factor when it comes to what we eat and how much we eat. Contrast and context can be important. Consider the difference between a tapestile meal and a meal that's built around one large entree. In both cases, you can fill up your stomach. But it turns out, these have very different effects on your brain. As social scientists have found, most of us find it difficult to tell the difference between the tenth bite and the eleventh bite of the very same food. There's a whole line of modern decision research, most associated with Daniel Kahneman, who got a Nobel Prize for it, and Amos Torsky showing that people are not so rational as you
Starting point is 00:14:19 might expect them to be. And one of the features of it is called duration neglect, which is people don't remember how long an experience is. They just remember the experience. So if you had a pain for 12 hours or a pain for one hour, two weeks later, what you remember is the experience of the pain, not how long it was. So this applied to food means that if you have the same food, a lot of the same food, it won't produce a very different memory from having a little of that same food because it's the memory of eating the food. So this raises a very important question that Coniman originally brought up, which is the distinction between your experience and your memory for the experience.
Starting point is 00:14:59 Coniman and others have shown, we've done some work on this too, that the ending of an experience is particularly important. others have shown, we've done some work on this too, that the ending of an experience is particularly important. So when you remember something you're more likely to remember the end of it. And also by the way, to some extent more likely to remember the beginning of it. And that would mean if you want to produce the best memory for food, you should put the best foods at the beginning and the end, whereas most people think the entree is the best, which is in the middle and the end, whereas most people think the entree is the best, which is in the middle, and is the least remembered.
Starting point is 00:15:28 We had Danny Kahneman on Hidden Brain recently, and of course he talked about the peak and rule, and also about the difference between the experiencing self and the remembering self. Yes. And one of the implications of this, as you point out, this difference between experiencing a meal and remembering a meal, is it points to the difference between people who go to restaurants and order their favorite dish every time and people who go to a restaurant and order a new dish every time. How do these two strategies essentially catering to two different psychological impulses? Well, that's an issue that we have looked at and it's pretty clear. If you're going to order your favorite food and you know you're going to order that before you go to the restaurant,
Starting point is 00:16:07 there are actually three aspects. There's your anticipation, this comes from Conor. The anticipation, the experience, and the memory. Your anticipation of a meal is going to be higher if you order your favorite food, because you know you're going to have something great, right? Whereas if you can order something new, it's not even clear what you can imagine,
Starting point is 00:16:23 because you don't know what it is. At the meal itself, you're probably gonna enjoy your favorite dish more than I knew this, because it's one of their best dishes. You love it. There's a little risk in ordering something, though. But if you order your favorite dish, you're not gonna create a new memory.
Starting point is 00:16:38 Whereas if you order a new food, you're gonna create a new memory. We are looking at this, but we don't know yet, for sure. If you're a person who generally values memories then you're going to try to create more memories by creating new experiences. Whereas if you value anticipation and experience more you will keep doing the same wonderful thing. So how you value anticipation, memory and experience affects how you're going to choose what activities
Starting point is 00:17:05 you do. Let's just take an example. Massage. Okay, I like massage. I go once a month, I get a massage. It's pretty much the same. I can't tell you last month's massage was exactly this. So I'm going for the anticipation and the experience, which is very positive.
Starting point is 00:17:21 I don't really create much memory from this. And I do it, and I like it, but it's very different from the way I eat, where I'm always trying to say, I want to enrich my mental menu listing, my life experience of food. But people differ on this. We've been interested in trying to see
Starting point is 00:17:42 if people are consistently different. We don't know yet. Think of a seven-day Caribbean vacation at a resort. Almost nothing happens, right? You're feeling good. It's sun is good. You go in the water. It's nice.
Starting point is 00:17:54 But there's no. If people say, tell me about your vacation, it's going to be a very short thing, right? So there's two kinds of vacations. Those that are really high on experience, and vacations that will give you a lot of experience, but there will also be some hardship. You'll get tired, something may not work, something might be closed when you thought it was open. All kinds of things can happen. So it won't be a totally positive experience, but it will be a
Starting point is 00:18:20 bunch of good memories. You've thought a lot about the differences between the American attitude toward food and the French attitude toward food, and you say that the French are more focused on what happens in the mouth, and Americans are often more focused by what happens in the bloodstream. I want to play you a short movie clip that illustrates this idea. Wow! If I isn't dying or girl! I'm going to get you guys.
Starting point is 00:18:45 What can I get here that has no sugar, no carbs and is fat free? Water. Water. Was that the problem? So that was from the movie A Cinder Ella Story Paul, and I'm wondering if you can just talk a little bit about this, the American attitude toward food versus the French attitude toward food. I go to France a fair amount, and they seem to be enjoying their food more than we were.
Starting point is 00:19:07 And interestingly enough, they're marginally healthier than we are. I mean, it's not a big difference, but they live a little longer and they have less heart disease. And yet they eat a diet that's higher in animal fat than we do. It's OK if we're worrying about food
Starting point is 00:19:21 and the consequence of that is that we're going to live longer. But we seem to be worrying about food and not living longer. So that seems like a bad exchange. So we started a study of how French and Americans differ in the way they eat. And it's basically got two parts to it. One is how they think about food and the other is how their food world is set up. The French think about food as an oral experience. They think about eating as something that is giving them
Starting point is 00:19:49 pleasure. They don't tend to think about what's going into their bloodstream, how much sugar is in their animal fat. So they're getting more pleasure out of food, because they're not worrying about it. So for example, if we ask French, when you think of heavy cream, do you think of whipped or unhealthy? They will say usually whipped
Starting point is 00:20:11 and Americans will say unhealthy. Now it's the same thing, but they're thinking about it as an experience and we're thinking about it more as a health event. And in some ways that actually might be a good thing, right? Because presumably when you take more pleasure in food, you're focused on it, you're not necessarily just focused on getting stuff in your mouth or focusing on nutrition. The French, for example, seem to pay more attention to portion control than Americans, except that I don't know if they're thinking about this as control. They're thinking about this as in the enjoyment of food. And once I'm done enjoying this bite, I'm done with it. Well, they eat more slowly, first of all. So they have more mouth experience
Starting point is 00:20:47 because they don't swallow as quickly. They savor the food. So I mean, if you have a chocolate bar, you can bolt it down in a couple of minutes so you can make a 10 or 15 minute experience out of it. And they're more inclined to the latter. We actually were able to measure how, in McDonald's in Paris, how long people sat
Starting point is 00:21:10 and ate compared to the McDonald's in the United States. Okay, and we made sure they were French people in the McDonald's, they were talking French. The French people sitting in McDonald's sit there for longer than the American people sitting in McDonald's. So they're than the American people said in McDonald's. So they're eating more slowly, they're talking more, you know, they're not just bolting down food.
Starting point is 00:21:30 Food is not fuel. Americans often, not always, treat food as fuel. Whereas the French think about it not as a fueling, but as an event and experience. Now in the food world, the big difference between the French and Americans is portion size. French traditionally serve smaller portions. If you look at a French cookbook, the amount of meat for four people per person is less than an American cookbook. In McDonald's, in France, the portions are smaller.
Starting point is 00:21:58 Now if you remember our discussion of the fact that you eat what's in front of you, if it's pleasant, the amnesic patients, the French put less food in front of you. And so they're eating less and enjoying it more. And that seems to me to be a good formula. Now they have other features of it. The French meal is a much more elaborate event. People don't get up, especially at home. They don't get up in the middle of the meal and just leave the table.
Starting point is 00:22:24 Everybody eats the same thing. So it's a social event. So I would say the French deal with food well in the face of the modern world, where there's so much good food around that we can easily stuff ourselves and eat everything under the sun. They have managed to have a tradition
Starting point is 00:22:41 which keeps it moderate and very pleasant. Paul Rosen has been studying the psychology and culture of food for over 40 years. He's currently at the University of Pennsylvania. Paul, thank you for joining me today on Hidden Brain. It's been a pleasure. Pleasure is not the only feeling we experience when we sit down for a meal. Food can also evoke other emotions, like disgust. After the break, we are going to delve more deeply into this emotion.
Starting point is 00:23:14 Why do some things, like smelly cheese, delight us, while other smelly things, like sneakers, disgust us? We pose that question to research a Rachel Hurts and find out why she calls disgust. The instinct that has to be learned. You're listening to Hidden Brain, I'm Shankar Vedanta. This is Hidden Brain, I'm Shankar Vedanta. There's a simple explanation for why we have the emotion of disgust.
Starting point is 00:23:46 It's a defense mechanism against things that could contaminate us. Where we often have flawed intuitions about how disgust works, starting with what's clean and what's dirty. In her book, That's Disgusting, psychologist Rachel Hurst talks about how the cleanliness of toilet seats compares to buttons on an ATM. The psychologist Rachel Hurts talks about how the cleanliness of toilet seats compares to buttons on an ATM. Things that we actually touched a lot and have no idea or don't really think about how
Starting point is 00:24:11 disgusting they are, things like ATM buttons actually are cell phones, are just petri dishes of pathogens and germs, and most of us do not clean our cell phones very often. And yet we have this idea because of the associations that we know very specifically between toilets and waste from the human body, that toilets are really, really filthy. And yet we don't give a second thought to things like keyboards or cell phones and other sorts of things that people are touching
Starting point is 00:24:39 all the time and could be very dirty and contagious. I wanna talk about a certain quality that disgusting things have, Rachel, which is that they tend to almost infect the things around them. As the psychologist Paul Rosen, who's done a lot of work on disgust says, a single cockroach will destroy the appeal of a bowl of cherries, but a single cherry doesn't make a bowl of cockroaches appealing. Why do disgusting things infect the things around them? Well, first I want to make a sort of fleshed out
Starting point is 00:25:09 a point there that I think is really interesting. And that is that good does not overpower evil. The way evil can overpower good. And so in fact, in other experiments, he found that the idea, for instance, of how could you purify something like Hitler's sweater. So people are told that Hitler owned the sweater and would you be willing to wear it under varying conditions?
Starting point is 00:25:31 Or would you be, how when could it become okay? And no form of cleaning could make it okay. Mother Teresa, wearing it, could make it a little bit okay. But in the case of Hitler's sweater, it had to be totally destroyed and burned in order for it to be okay. So good does not sanct. But in the case of Hitler's sweater, it had to be totally destroyed and burned in order for it to be okay. So good does not sanctify bad in the way that bad
Starting point is 00:25:49 can sanctify good. Whereas just one spot of something bad, like a cockroach, for instance, in a glass of water or milk or anything else, has the capacity to destroy the whole thing. Because negativity is much more pervasive and powerful from the way that we are built.
Starting point is 00:26:06 And this is in fact adaptive because it's better to be worried about things that can harm us than overly excited about things such as might be benevolent. I remember doing a story some years ago Rachel that looked at a slightly related idea. This was worked by the sociologist Anne Bowers and she was studying the market for used wedding rings. And she found that people were really reluctant to buy a wedding ring when they learned that the couple who had previously owned that wedding ring had gotten divorced. And people tended to want wedding rings where, you know, there had been some tragic love story, but the couple had been very happy together. So I'm not sure if it's exactly the same concept, but it really feels as if inanimate objects
Starting point is 00:26:49 can sometimes carry with them the spirit, if you will, it's an unscientific idea, the spirit of living things. Well, actually it is very much the same concept, Shankar, and what you're talking about is something called sympathetic magic, which definitely plays a role in discussed and what we're disgusted by through association. The idea of once in contact, always in contact.
Starting point is 00:27:12 So for example, the ring that was on the finger of the woman who had a terrible marriage and ended up getting divorced, somehow even though that ring is now in a jewelry shop, has nothing to do with that original couple whatsoever. The essence of that bad marriage is somehow still in the ring. And therefore, wearing that ring will therefore impart the bad marriage onto the new wearer. And somehow this spirit, as you said, will transcend and infect the new marriage.
Starting point is 00:27:39 So it's again, it's a form of infection. You have a very simple and interesting thought experiment in your book, which is, I'm not disgusted at the thought that when I drink a glass of water, that there is saliva in my mouth. But if I spit in a glass of water, and then I drink that glass of water, that seems disgusting. And of course, in both cases, it's exactly the same outcome.
Starting point is 00:28:02 I have water in my mouth with saliva in my mouth. Why does it feel more disgusting in one case and not the other? So that's exactly a great point about the idea that discussed is about the outside coming in and contaminating our inside. So while the saliva and the water is in our mouth, it's inside of us, and even just take water out of the equation, just the fact that you have saliva in your mouth right now, we're all okay with that. But as soon as we spit that exact same saliva
Starting point is 00:28:27 that's in our mouth into the glass, and then look at that, and then I'm telling you, now you should drink that, it all of a sudden, even though it's only been out in the air for seconds, has become contaminated by the outside and bringing it back into our body is now an entirely different proposition.
Starting point is 00:28:43 And the fact also, I think in this case, that even though I know it's my saliva, it could be your saliva, it could be anybody else's saliva. Suddenly it becomes equalized with all this saliva that I know is out there, that I definitely do not want in my mouth. So if someone, for instance, is talking at you and spitting while they're talking enthusiastically, we're disgusted by the fact that some spit could touch us. Just like once our spit is outside of our mouth, it becomes much less pleasant to think about taking it back in. There was a really interesting study done about sharing your toothbrush. And this
Starting point is 00:29:18 study found that, so again the idea that you know you're having to have something in your mouth that somebody else had in their mouth. And it was found that the person that one would least like to share your tooth brush with is the boss that you don't like. Followed by, so if you don't like your boss, you definitely do not want to share your tooth brush with him or her. However, the anchor person on your local news station, if they were attractive, was pretty fine to share a toothbrush with. And people in your direct family or your best friends were also okay to share a toothbrush with. And so this speaks to two things. One, familiarity and emotional connection. So people in your family or your best friends, you feel positively towards them, you're very familiar with them,
Starting point is 00:30:01 and therefore you sort of feel safe around their saliva. The good looking person on the television, beauty equals health equals okay. But the person that you dislike or the person who's a stranger is a lot likely to be either more contaminating or because you don't like them purely because you don't like them. They could be good looking and you could know they're very healthy but if you don't like them purely because you don't like them. They could be good looking and you could know they're very healthy, but if you don't like them, then you don't want anything about them coming into you as in their toothbrush. Contacts is key when it comes to disgust. In October 1972, a plane crashed in an isolated region of the Andes mountain
Starting point is 00:30:46 range. There were 45 people on board, most of them rugby players traveling to Chile for a competition. Many passengers died upon impact, more died in the coming days because of the freezing cold and dwindling food supply. In the end, 16 people had to survive more than two months in the mountains, and their story has inspired movies, books, and documentaries like this one from the History Channel. I said Nando, there isn't anything left in the storage compartments where we kept the chocolates and the can of sardines that we had. And Nando looked me in the eye and said, garlitos, I want to eat the pilot. The people that had this idea and wanted to convince
Starting point is 00:31:32 the other members that this was the only way they were going to survive did two things. One, they told the people to think of this as what you're doing is eating just meat. So don't think about this as a person, kind of connected to, we don't think about eating cows and pigs, so much as we think of eating pork and beef. So this is just meat.
Starting point is 00:31:51 And the second thing is that they try to justify this in another kind of a moral way by saying that the death of their compatriots was, would have been sort of a complete waste if it couldn't be used in this way that potentially aid in their survival. And so, reluctantly at first, but then everybody joined in, ate the dead remains of the people that were around them, and another thing they did. We made a pact, and we did what people do now. People give blood to friends, to family members.
Starting point is 00:32:24 They make organ transplants, you know? And we made a pact. We said, okay, hand in hand. If I die, please use my body. So at least one of us can get out of here. So the fact that this is about cannibalism and that the people willingly resorted to this is something which has really caught our imagination because willingly resorted to this is something which is really caught our imagination because it really leads to this question of what
Starting point is 00:32:49 would you do in that circumstance? Would you also, you know, traverse the line into this worst taboo? One of the things that I find interesting is that when I personally think about their behavior, I don't necessarily feel disgusted by what they did. In the same way that if you told me that someone was munching on their neighbor's arm, there's something about essentially having your hand forced by circumstances, you're acting in a way that is the only way you can possibly survive, that changes the way I think about whether this is disgusting. Exactly. It's only really disgusting when it's a willful, unnecessary behavior. So like you said, you kill your neighbor and then decide to eat them.
Starting point is 00:33:35 But someone who is forced into this situation and the only way that they can survive is by resorting to an opportunistic situation that the person is already dead. That's a lot different. But the act itself, eating someone who's dead, eating another dead human being, is the same. And again, this is how our mind changes the behavior from being okay to something completely abhorrent. Rachel, you describe a case that took place in the 1990s.
Starting point is 00:34:02 A man was stomp to death after propositioning another man, and when the case was taken to court, the defendant successfully used something known as the gay panic defense. You say this idea is rooted in a misconception about disgust? So this idea that in this sense, using the gay panic defense. So in this case, someone was able to convince the jury that the idea of this man making a homosexual advance to him was so repugnant that being motivated to kill this person was somehow justified. And like you said, it actually was successful in the trial. Now, the concept that I think, so this is sort of, this is moralizing and this is getting
Starting point is 00:34:45 people to become less judgmental towards the act of murder because of the fact that they felt a kind of a sympathy towards the feeling of a version that this person must have had and therefore this sort of outburst of rage is somehow justified. The thing that's different about this, and I think this is an interesting point about disgust in what makes it different from anger, which I think it's often confused with, is that disgust is about recoiling from, moving away from, avoiding the stimulus that's making you disgust it. And if someone were truly disgusted by somebody else, they would not want to get all over them and beat
Starting point is 00:35:20 them to get death and get their blood and everything else all over them, because that would be even more disgusting if you're already disgusted. Instead, if I'm really angry, if I'm enraged by something, then I attack, then I approach, then I can demolish you and get all covered in you and it doesn't matter because I'm just in a rage. So really, the idea about this being discussed is wrong, and what it was, is this person was affronted, somehow morally, personally, whatever the case might be, and incited into such a rage that then he wanted to murder this person. So the idea of using disgust in this way is actually flawed because the person is not
Starting point is 00:35:56 disgusted, they're just enraged. So that example of homophobia, Rachel ritual makes me think about an idea. In almost every society you go to, you see patterns of disgust that are modeled on social hierarchies. In many countries, you have the rich who are disgusted by the poor or upper caste who are disgusted by lower caste or people who are native citizens being disgusted by foreigners. What do you think explains this? It has to do with something more insidious
Starting point is 00:36:29 and that's related to our feelings about our social environment and the people that are in it. And the idea that foreigners and strangers and so forth are threats to our social normal order and that that then becomes somehow connected to our ideas about contamination and protecting us somehow. And the idea is that this somehow justifies and rationalizes racial prejudices and other kinds of prejudices, because if we stay away from the unfamiliar and the foreign, because
Starting point is 00:37:01 we don't know what those immigrants, you know, they could be diseased, and in fact, disease was often used as a way of anti-immigrant propaganda. This somehow justifies negative attitude towards them when there is no relationship between their ability to actually make a sick or not. In much of our life, disgust feels instinctive. When we see a cockroach and a kitchen counter counter or smell rotten food in the trash can, our revulsion feels hardwired. But Rachel says it's not. Our sense of disgust is learned. The things that disgust us not only reveal a lot about our culture,
Starting point is 00:37:37 they reveal a great deal about our minds. She remembers an instructive episode from her own childhood. As we were driving in the car, and it was a beautiful sunny summer day, and the windows were rolled down, and we were going by fields, and everything was very pretty. My mother from the front seat said, oh, I love that smell. And so, as I was smelling the same thing, and all kinds of nice things were in my visual scene, and my mother who I love said, I love that smell. I thought, okay, this is a great smell. And I then learned a few years later that saying that in response to that smell was a very big mistake. So the smell turned out
Starting point is 00:38:15 to be skunk. So when I said, I love that smell on the playground with all bunch of little kids around me and they went, ew, disgusting, that skunk, oh you're so gross and so forth and ran away from me. Again, so I become now, this skunk, because they all ran away from me. I realized that was not the socially appropriate thing to say and so I kept that to myself for quite a while. But it turns out I'm not alone.
Starting point is 00:38:37 There are actually people who like the smell of skunk and that also leads into something interesting about our sense of smell. In fact, we don't all smell skunk in the same way. So both our minds and the way our noses in fact react to the chemical that makes up skunk is different. So unless you have an identical twin, your receptors in your nose are actually only yours and nobody else shares them, even though there's a lot of overlap.
Starting point is 00:39:02 So I buy the idea that we're all not smelling the same thing when we smell skunk that are the receptors in our noses and the way our brains work, you know, we might be smelling different things. But it's also, what I find really fascinating about the story is that there is so much about disgust that is actually learned. You heard your mother, you have an association
Starting point is 00:39:23 between your mother and the beautiful scenery around you, and you learn in some ways whether something that's a strong smell is a positive smell or a negative smell. And in many ways, this runs counter to the way most of us think about disgust. We think of it as being this innate drive that if I find something disgusting, you're going to smell it and you're going to find a disgusting as well. So I think that brings up a great way to think about the idea of disgust and it is that it is the instinct that has to be learned. So once we learn what something disgusting is, we then feel disgusted by it like for
Starting point is 00:39:57 instance bodily products and toilet training and you know poop and so forth. We don't have a question about whether or not it's disgusting especially if it's from somebody else where also is where meaning and context come into play. But we did not think it was disgusting from the get go. So that is to say, we were not hardwired or born thinking that poop was gross. In fact, many infants both like to play with poop. They like the smell of poop. They don't have any reactions to things.
Starting point is 00:40:23 For instance, that adults in the same community think of as positive or negative. In fact, a great demonstration I like to do is showing these facial expressions of babies getting either the smell of sort of sweaty socks and vomit versus the smell of vanilla. And some babies' faces to the sweaty socks and vomit are making big smiley faces, and others to the smell of vanilla are making what we would call disgust faces. So you know, there's nothing hardwired about the reaction either to smells or to things that are disgusting.
Starting point is 00:40:52 But once we learn what the meaning is, we then stick to it except for when the context can make it very confusing or the context changes the way we perceive something. I love this idea that in many ways our notions of disgust are constructed. I understand that Americans like winter green flavor chewing gum, but people in Britain not so much. So in the UK, winter green mint is used exclusively in toilet cleaning products and in some medicinal bombs, like things you would rub on your skin if you're in pain. And so they're connected, the smell is connected to either
Starting point is 00:41:28 being in pain or cleaning the toilet. So not good. In the US, however, the scent is used in candies and in gums, it's connected to the taste of sweet and sweet actually is innately positive. So tasting sweet plus smelling something that's gonna be good. And we don't have any connection to cleaning the bathroom or being in pain when we smell this odor.
Starting point is 00:41:50 And so as a function of the connections that we have learned to it and what the meaning is, this odor which in and of itself is totally, you know, agnostic, it doesn't have meaning one way or the other becomes good or becomes bad as a function of that. So the British and the Americans are divided on the subject of wintergreen. But I'm absolutely sure they'd come together when it comes to eating bugs. Eating happened. My heart's pounding oh my god. They look like crickets. There's no sugar coating. I'm about to throw up if I stay here.
Starting point is 00:42:31 I don't want to do it. like crickets. There's no sugar coating. I'm about to throw up if I stay here. Those are reactions from a couple of our NPR co-workers when we presented them with a plate of dried crickets and asked them to take a bite. Can I get the old my water bottle? Can I have a chaser? I'm cool bugs as long as they're outside. I'm not sure how cool I am with them being in my mouth. It's disgusting. It's insects. Look at it. There's exoskeletons everywhere, pieces of legs. This is gross. People eat this. There's a couple of things which disgust us about bugs. One of them is that we don't consider them to be food.
Starting point is 00:42:56 Now, there are cultures, especially in Southeast Asia, that do consider all kinds of bugs as food. And if you were to go to Thailand, you'd see Madagascar hissing cockroaches on display in the market where people are actually buying them to eat them up for meals and prepare them in all kinds of ways which apparently, although I don't know, personally, can be quite delicious. But the idea of what food is is a really interesting concept because this again speaks to how disgust is learned. And when we decide that something is edible, then it's okay to eat and other cultures
Starting point is 00:43:27 think differently about that. The other thing about bugs is that they move in a way that seems to be related to one of the aspects of disgust underlying why we don't like them. And this is actually somewhat connected to bodily fluids. So bugs move in a kind of a jerky or slithery or slimy kind of way. And those kinds of things actually have a similarity
Starting point is 00:43:51 to body products that we are also disgusted by. So being disgusted by slugs, for instance, is to do the fact that slugs are similar visually to feces. So there is this kind of continuum about our association between what bugs are and what bodily fluids are that were disgusted by. So there's a variety of reasons why bugs are disgusting. One of the things in your book that caught my eye was even in the same country in different periods of time, our attitudes about what's disgusting have changed or can change enormously. I understand that a few centuries ago, lobster was not quite seen the way that it was in Massachusetts. No, lobster was actually considered vermin of the sea and only suitable for slaves. And in fact, there was a slave uprising against the idea that it was completely cruel
Starting point is 00:44:44 and unusual punishment to give people lobster to eat more than three times a week. And today we might say, oh, how lucky to be able to afford to have lobster three times a week. So it's conceivable then, maybe in a few decades or maybe in a couple of centuries from now, we will think of eating cockroaches and crickets very differently than we do today. In fact, there are some people who would argue that insects are a very efficient source of protein, and they are plentiful, and in some ways consuming insects might actually
Starting point is 00:45:12 be good for the environment in all kinds of different ways. I'm personally a vegetarian, so I don't know if I'll ever subscribe to eating crickets and cockroaches, but presumably our attitudes towards eating insects could change. Absolutely. So the idea also, one of the things that made lobster, disgusting, and unappealing is that it was slave food, it was prisoner food. So these people, these are bad people in the kind of the hierarchy of humans. And therefore it's okay for them to eat things that are possibly that we would consider disgusting, but not okay for me, you know,
Starting point is 00:45:45 the elevated person, whatever I think of myself as being. And right now, the way we think about insects is similar to that. So people who are not like us, people who have to resort to the, you know, the last measure is possible for survival, they're gonna, you know, debase themselves to eat insects.
Starting point is 00:46:01 But if we think about it entirely differently, so if we remoralize the story and and instead make eating insects about saving the planet and make it a virtuous thing to do, rather than somehow a debased thing to do or a last resort thing to do, we could in fact make it a very positive thing. While some of our co-workers shrank it first when they held the cricket in their hand, the more you look in his eyes, the less you want to eat it. The looks of fear and disgust faded after the first crunch. Cheers. Cheers.
Starting point is 00:46:36 It's just like chips kind of. It's good, it's a little earthy. It's like sunflower seed or something. Yeah, it's like maybe like a basket of these like before my little pangs. Along with the whole crickets, we also provided samples of delectable protein bars made of cricket flour. I chose peanut butter and chocolate. If you didn't tell people this was a cricket, they wouldn't know.
Starting point is 00:46:58 Good. It's not like there's big chunks of cricket in it, so I like it. Not bad at all. I'm trying to to be so like... American, you know, because everyone else eats crickets. It is a luxury to be able to be disgusted. From the very basic level that if you don't have anything else to eat, other than someone who's dead beside you or the cockroach on the floor, then you just do not get disgusted by that because you have to do it in order to survive.
Starting point is 00:47:26 So it's a privilege to be able to say, no, I'd rather not have that dead person or that cockroach. I'd rather have that hamburger or the steak or that beautifully prepared dish of Portobello mushrooms. So the ability to say that we are disgusted by something actually comes from the privilege and the luxury of having abundance and having enough to sustain us that we can make the decision that we don't want this and we'd rather have that. Rachel hers is the author of That's Disgusting and the cent of desire. She also had a book out at the end of 2017. Why you eat what you eat?
Starting point is 00:48:04 Rachel hers, thank you for joining me today on Hidden Brain. It's been a pleasure, Slankhark. Thank you for having me. This week's show was produced by Thomas Liu and Parth Shah. It was edited by Tara Boyle. Our team includes Jenny Schmidt, Raina Cohen, Laura Quarell and Camilla Vargas Restrepo. Renek Cohen, Laura Quarell and Camilla Vargas-Restrepo. Special thanks to Monique Laborde. Anya Grunman is NPR's Vice President for Programming. For more hidden brain, you can follow the show on Facebook and Twitter. Please be sure to subscribe to our podcast.
Starting point is 00:48:37 I'm Shankar Vedantum. Thanks for listening. I'll see you next week.

There aren't comments yet for this episode. Click on any sentence in the transcript to leave a comment.