Hidden Brain - The Choices Before Us

Episode Date: May 5, 2020

An abundance of choices is a good thing, right? In the United States, where choice is often equated with freedom and control, the answer tends to be a resounding 'yes.' But researchers say the relatio...nship between choice and happiness isn't always so clear-cut. This week, we talk with psychologist Sheena Iyengar about making better decisions, and how she's thinking about the relationship between choices and control during the coronavirus pandemic.

Transcript
Discussion (0)
Starting point is 00:00:00 From NPR, this is Hidden Brain, I'm Shankar Vedanta. From the moment you get up in the morning, your brain is making choices. Do you hit the snooze button? Do you hit it a second time? For breakfast, are you going to have cereal or eggs? Coffee or tea? Every day, you grapple with hundreds of choices. To many people an abundance of options is a good thing.
Starting point is 00:00:31 It signifies freedom and having control. You get to choose whether to spend your Saturday at a movie or at a baseball game. You decide whether to try the new restaurant down the block or stay in and cook. It's your call whether to take the job with the higher pay or the one with better work-life balance. But in recent months, with the spread of the coronavirus pandemic, the number of choices in our lives has suddenly shrunk dramatically. And going to a grocery store has become the equivalent of a special outing. Imagine looking this good for who?
Starting point is 00:01:12 One mark. Look at this good for what? The constraints on our choices have been very hard. Millions of people are out of work, and millions more feel their lives are precarious. If we had a choice to swiftly go back to the old normal, we'd jump in it. But with all this hardship, many people also report a new relationship to the limited things they do have. Being homebound has given me the opportunity to not feel so rushed to stick to the rigid schedule that my family is used to.
Starting point is 00:01:52 I've had to notice that I seem to be enjoying the mundane tasks of daily life a lot more than I used to do. Take the leaves out of my flower beds, make chocolate chip cookies with my tile. The other night I actually enjoyed just doing the dishes kind of spacing out into that. Would we be better off when our regular lives resume if we had fewer choices? Why do we crave so many options of ice cream, cars and vacation destinations? This week on Hidden Brain, what are choices say about us? Why we fail to understand the pernicious effect of too many choices? And how to make choices more wisely.
Starting point is 00:02:58 Shina Ayangar is a psychologist at Columbia University. She is best known for a bold idea that many people find surprising. Having more options doesn't always lead to better or happier decisions. In her book The Art of Choosing, she explores how history, culture and psychology shaped the way we think about making choices. Shina Aengar, welcome to Hidden Brain. Thank you, it's wonderful to be here. A number of psychological studies have examined the well-being and motivation of people when they get to make choices about what they do. What have these studies generally found about the effects that choice and autonomy have on what's called intrinsic motivation? There's a wealth of studies that have been done with both animals
Starting point is 00:03:42 and humans that show that when we have a feeling of control or a sense of autonomy over our lives, this makes us strive more, so we're more intrinsically motivated, it makes us healthier, it makes us happier, I mean, just lots of benefits when it comes to psychological and physical well-being. There's a famous study that looked at British bureaucrats that examined the differences in health between those who got to make decisions for others who were the bosses and those who had to follow orders, people who were underlings. What did the study find, Gina?
Starting point is 00:04:18 The people that end up making more choices or having more autonomy and control over their schedules by and large were less likely to suffer from heart disease or strokes and lived longer lives. People who seemingly had less to worry about because all the choices were made for them, they were simply following orders, tended to be more stressed out and suffered more physical consequences for it. There are studies that show that giving people even trivial or superficial choices can improve their health. Alan Langer and Judy Rodin ran a remarkable study at a nursing home and can etiquette many
Starting point is 00:04:54 years ago. Can you describe that study and tell me what it found? If you take people at a nursing home and you give them trivial choices, like would you like a plant in the room? Would you like to decide which night you watched the movie? You know, it's really trivial choices. It turns out that they end up having better health and they live longer, was essentially what they found.
Starting point is 00:05:17 And you see these kinds of effects in all kinds of ways, right? If you give children a choice of when they're going to do their assignment, they spend more time on it, they're more motivated to do it. If you go into a restaurant that has higher ceilings, it gives you more delusion of freedom or a sense of space and liberty, again, people are happier. When you step back and look at the value of having choices, you've identified sort of three broad drivers that make choice valuable to have in our lives. What are those three drivers, Gina? I think choice plays three fundamental functions for us. First, it's just psychologically motivating. We feel happy when we have the feeling that we have autonomy or some kind of control. That's universal. The second is that compared to any other species on the planet, we can take a bunch of options
Starting point is 00:06:16 and say, you know, which one is better or worse and make trade-offs. The function it plays is that it enables us to engage in judgment, decision-making. The third function that choice makes is it's because of our ability to choose that we're able to create new choices. We were able to imagine a future and turn that imagination into choices that we can now make. Having the freedom to imagine choices and to make those choices is a good thing. But is there a point at which we can have too much of this good thing? Many years ago, Shena was on a plane. The passenger sitting next to her Excitedly told her about a study she had read that had to do with jam
Starting point is 00:07:12 What the passenger didn't know was that Sheena had conducted that study herself Lots of people do describe the jam study to me and they often refer to it as the jam study I did that study in the 1990s. There was this upscale grocery store called Draggers, which specialized in offering people lots and lots of choice. It had like 250 different types of mustard, vinegars, mayonnaisees, and over 500 different types of fruits and vegetables. In discussions with the manager, I asked the store managers to whether this model of offering
Starting point is 00:07:46 people all this choice really worked. And after a series of discussions, we decided to do a little experiment, which has now been dubbed the Jam Study. So we set up a tasting booth where we put out six different flavors of Jam or 24 different flavors of Jam near the entrance of the store. And we looked at two things.
Starting point is 00:08:07 First in which case were people more likely to stop and sample some jam. It turned out more people stopped when they were 24 on display, 60% versus 40%. And then we looked at in which case were people more likely to buy a jar of jam. And it turned out that more people were likely to buy a jar of jam. And it turned out that more people were likely to buy a jar of jam when they encountered six than when they encountered 24. So again, the experiment had two conditions. In one, the researcher set up a table with six jams. In another, they had 24. More people stopped at the jam display when they were more options. more. More people stopped at the jam display when there were more options, but more people actually bought jam when there were fewer options.
Starting point is 00:08:51 And this was really the first documentation that even though people are more attracted to having more choices, when it comes down to making a choice, they're more likely to make a choice when they have less than when they have more. I want to stay a moment with sort of the evolution of the idea after you did the jam study. You got a call from the Vanguard Group, which is one of the largest mutual fund companies in the world, and they were looking at some 900,000 employees at various companies, and they were looking at the number of people who were signing up to save for their retirement and 401k plans, and they were finding that as they provided more options
Starting point is 00:09:26 to people, more mutual fund options, the number of people who were signing up to save for retirement was dropping. Tell me about the study you did with them and what it found. So it was about 900,000 people. They were all in the United States, about 650 different institutions.
Starting point is 00:09:44 And these companies ranged in size from a few hundred up to about 100,000 employees. They were all carrying a vanguard. That is a common factor across all of them. But essentially what we found was that these plans offered people anywhere from two to roughly 60 options. And what we found was that as plans offered, people more options participation rates dropped. And so the more choices you gave people,
Starting point is 00:10:11 the lower the participation rates. And by the way, these are all cases where employees could expect a match from their employers. So they were literally giving up free money by not participating. When we looked at people who actually did participate to actually made the choice, even there we found that giving people more choices
Starting point is 00:10:32 didn't actually make them better choosers. It turned out that the more choices we gave them, the more likely they were to avoid investing in equities and put more of their money in bonds and money markets. And you know, most of the time when you're talking about long-term financial well-being, you really shouldn't be avoiding equities. Vanguard wasn't the only company interested in questions of choice. Corporations seized on the idea that having fewer options could boost sales.
Starting point is 00:11:05 Proctor and Gamble reduced the number of options of their anti-Dandra shampoo and saw sales surge. It was a counter-intuitive pivot in a culture where the abundance of choice was seen as an inherent good. There was another domain where Shina found that having more choices didn't turn out the way people expected. As an undergraduate, she had spent time in Germany when the Berlin Wall came down. Thousands of East Germans came across the border today, perhaps more than a hundred thousand. So many, the border police. of a hundred thousand so many the butterflies. I was fascinated by Berlin and I was there when the
Starting point is 00:11:46 wall fell and participated in all the big partying and you know it's a wall fell and everybody's partying and hugging each other and this was a moment where freedom had prevailed it was a big deal. But when she now went back to Germany in the earlys, she found the euphoria had worn off. East Berliners would say, yes, I have more choices in terms of where I could go for vacation, more choices of what I can eat, but I've lost access to the better quality asparagus, because apparently better quality asparagus came from the East. And so they were questioning paragus came from the East. And so they were questioning whether having more choice was really worth it, that they felt that at some level they had lost out on some of the more high quality choices in
Starting point is 00:12:34 the drive to have more choices. One of the most telling examples of the mismatch between our perception of the importance of choices and the reality of those choices is a personal story that you tell about your own parents. Tell me that story, Shina, and what you took away from it. When I was growing up in this culture, you always taught that you choose everything. I mean, you always ask what do you want. And of course, you're the one who decides how you're going to look and what you're going to do with your life in terms of your, you know, how you're going to look and
Starting point is 00:13:05 what you're going to do with your life in terms of your career and obviously whom you're going to marry. And you know, my parents met each other on their wedding night. It was really decided by my two grimmas. And when I was growing up and I would tell everybody, yeah, my parents met each other on their wedding night, I kid you not, it never failed. Every single friend of mine, every single friend's parent would always say, Oh my God, if that ever happened to me, I would die.
Starting point is 00:13:32 And, you know, I certainly understood that that was the view. But my parents never really looked like they were ready to die. They, you know, they were just mom and dad. You know, they were just mom and dad. Contrary to the theories of her friends, Sheena's parents were not miserable. It was yet another example of the strange conundrum. When you limited people's choices, they didn't always turn out worse.
Starting point is 00:14:02 When we come back, what are choices say about who we are and why many of us crave more choices, even when they make us unhappy? Psychology Shina AYANGAR teaches at Columbia Business School. She has studied the choices we make, why our choices don't always produce the results we intend, and how to make better choices. Shina, one idea you explore has to do with the perception that we all have about ourselves, that we are unique. What does this perception have to do with our desire for choice and how we make choices? So we all have an innate desire to be someone, right? We don't want it just to be a member of some massive crowd unrecognizable from everybody else. So we want to somehow stand out from everybody else. We don't want to become this lonely minority, but we do want to somehow be distinguishable from the person next to us.
Starting point is 00:15:12 And how do we do that? We do that through the expression of identity and more and more in the modern world that expression of identity is coming through the choices that we make. The more choices we have, the more we believe that we can identify and pick a choice that is the perfect expression of who I am and what I want. And so that's really the link between my desire to uniquely be known as a type of person and how choices are used to establish my uniqueness. What does the psychological research say about our perception of uniqueness? Is it actually true? Well, it's interesting that you ask that question.
Starting point is 00:15:56 So let's say I give you a bunch of things like names to call your children or sunglasses or shoes or ties for the case of men. And I ask you to just rate these on how unusual they are, how common they are. And I also ask you not only to rate on how unusual and common they are, but I also ask you how much do you like them, how much do you think other people would like them? And we did a series, we did this with a series of different kinds of decision-making scenarios. And what we found was that most people really believe that they like more unique things than everybody else.
Starting point is 00:16:35 They also believe that they are more unique than everybody else. But when you actually look at their preferences for what they like as compared to what other people said they like, essentially we all like the same thing. We want that which is considered to be slightly unique. Nothing to plan, nothing to bizarre. So like if you look at ties, right? We don't want to pick the tie with the, you know, with the orange disco balls on it, that's a little too weird. We also do want to pick just the solid color, that might be a little too boring. We want something we just a little bit of a kick to it.
Starting point is 00:17:16 So we believe that we are unique and we need choices to express that uniqueness, but we then sometimes, as you say, find ourselves in the awkward situation of making choices that are not very different from others, which then challenges our notions of being unique. You tell a funny story about waiting in line for hours at an Apple store to buy an iPhone for your partner, and he gave you very specific instructions on what he wanted. Tell me what happened next, Gina. So this is my ex-husband, yes. For his birthday, it was just at the time when the Apple iPhone had just come out.
Starting point is 00:17:51 And for his birthday, he wanted the original iPhone. So what happens was on that day, I don't know if you remember, but when the Apple iPhone first came out, there were these long lines that would go down many, many blocks. Six o'clock this morning. Well, more horrible, yeah, he spent the last 12 hours waiting at night. So I had to get up at the middle of the night and go down to the Apple store and stand in line
Starting point is 00:18:11 to get him this phone that he really wanted for his birthday. And he had given me some very specific instructions that he needed the black iPhone. I gave him all these specifications, blah, blah, blah. He wanted the black one because he had said that it, you know, it's less likely to get dirty and it looks sleek, etc. Well, by the time I got to the front of the line, he caught up with me, he got out of bed, came to the Apple store and this is the one on Fifth Avenue, he got out of bed, he got to me and he's like,
Starting point is 00:18:39 just as I'm putting in the order for the black phone, he's like, no, no, no, switch that to the white. And I'm like, what do you mean you went the white? You told me you went at the black and you told me all these reasons why you wanted the black. And I didn't really care about black or white. What did I care? And he's like, I don't know, I haven't even looked around. Everybody else has got the black one. I want the white one. Now, mind you, we're only talking about black versus white. So as far as I was concerned, I really couldn't see the big deal here. Now, you know, and he was in the loan.
Starting point is 00:19:09 There were lots of people who regularly make that decision just to which one's going to somehow make them stand out. Now, today, my understanding is he doesn't even carry any apple phone in part because they would make him look too cut-form-s. So it doesn't actually believe in apple products. So, so yes, we often do that. There are experiments along these lines of people sitting in a restaurant deciding what to order. Sometimes they get to be the first to make the order. Sometimes they have to wait for everyone around the table to order.
Starting point is 00:19:40 Tell me what these experiments find and how satisfied people are with what they order depending on whether they go first or they go last. Well my favorite study is this study that we've done in a brewery where people are at a table and these are just normal customers at a table and they would ask people to order and so the first person would order the second person, third person, fourth person, fifth person. The orders of the first person were much more homogenous, meaning you were more likely to order similar things when you looked across the first orderers. But when you looked at the people who ordered it, each table subsequent to the first person, now
Starting point is 00:20:20 you saw greater heterogeneity. Right? So the second, third, fourth, fifth person, they're all ordering something slightly different from the person who just ordered right before them. Interestingly enough, the person who's most satisfied with the beer that they ordered is the first person who ordered because they weren't paying any attention to what everybody else had already ordered.
Starting point is 00:20:40 Another issue with having choices is that it forces us to grapple with options that we have to forego. I'm wondering how this worry might affect the world of dating when we have so many people we can potentially choose from, but making a choice involves foregoing other choices. Oh, it's a huge problem, right? It's what we call FOMO, Barry Schwartz and I first identified it in the domain of job search, right? It's what we call FOMO, very short tonight first identified it in the domain of job search, right? That when people get more job opportunities, even though they actually do
Starting point is 00:21:12 better in many objective senses, right? They get higher salaries, they get better packages, they're less happy than the people that had fewer job offers. And the reason is not so much that they're convinced that they didn't pick the best of the bunch that they had. Most often they are convinced that they picked the best, but they are convinced that there might actually still be something better out there because they compare what they chose against some imaginary option. And the same thing is happening at a much larger level when it comes to dating, right?
Starting point is 00:21:47 Because you have so many options. And you know, in many ways, these are incomparable options. Because you're comparing humans. And of course, it doesn't make a difference how long you go. There'll always be more options on the table. So this becomes a prescription then for indecision or a prescription for essentially dragging the decision out endlessly. Absolutely. So sometimes the choices we make are trivial, but other times they're also potentially heartbreaking.
Starting point is 00:22:24 You describe in your book how researchers have looked at one of the worst tragedies to befall new parents and examine differences between countries like the United States and France. Can you tell me about those research studies and what they found, Shina? Yeah, those were some of the most heartbreaking studies I did, and yet so much of what we learned in those studies are so applicable to modern day society. Essentially, we did a study with parents in France
Starting point is 00:22:52 and parents in the United States who all had given birth to a child that was suffering from cerebral anaxia. Cerebral anaxia is a type of injury where the brain is deprived of oxygen. As a result, Sheena says, these children were born with little or no brain function. In the case of France, the doctors would make the decision of taking these babies off of life support. And in the case of the United States, the doctors would tell the parents that look, you know, the likelihood that this child will ever walk or talk or engage in any kind of normal life is close to zero. And so you can
Starting point is 00:23:32 either take it off life support or it will probably die anyway. And the American parents would have to sign the consent form in order for that final decision to be made. And so we compared those cases where the decision was made in both cases for the child to be made. And so we compared those cases where the decision was made in both cases for the child to be removed off of life support and in general in this kind of decision-making, it's very rare for a child not to be taken off of life support. And so when they are taken off of life support, they typically die within hours. Now what we did was we followed these parents over the course of the next year, and what we found was that the parents in the United States had a much harder time coping with the death of their child as compared to the ones in France. So the parents in France were much
Starting point is 00:24:19 more likely to say things like, you know, Noah was here for so little time, but in that time he gave us so much. Now by contrast, the American parents were still suffering even a year later. And they said things that were really heartbreaking like, you know, I keep wondering, what if, what if, what if, or they said things like, I can't believe they got me to do that.
Starting point is 00:24:42 So it was almost like they were still struggling with the guilt and wanting to put their responsibility somewhere because they were struggling with the fact that essentially they felt they had killed their child. So did the American parents wish that they had not been given that choice in that situation? Well, the paradox in that study was that even though having made the choice, had made these Americans so miserable when they were asked, would you have rather have had the doctors make the choice for you?
Starting point is 00:25:15 They all said no. And I think this comes back to the fact that in this culture, choice has a really special elevated place in our lives. Because for these American parents, to say no to choice would have made them question everything that they had come to believe was important to them and what it meant to be a human being. So even when we know that our choices can lead to great unhappiness, we cannot help wanting to have those choices anyway. We still
Starting point is 00:25:50 believe we should be the one to make the choice. Questions of choice in autonomy are raging around us today in the context of the COVID-19 pandemic. Some people chafed being told to stay home and see it as a curtailment of their personal autonomy. I ask Shina how questions of choice are shaping our response to the pandemic. Well, I think that one of the lens through which we see choice around the globe has to do with to what extent we value the notion of individualism versus collectiveism. And so when you go to places like China,
Starting point is 00:26:35 having a decision dictated to you in the interest of the collective feels more natural. In the case of the United States, having a choice dictated to you in the interest of the collective feels a bit foreign because this is a culture that above all else values individual rights. That is the collective stand. And now we're asking people to do something that's kind of antithetical to that, which is put aside your own individual interests for the sake of the collective.
Starting point is 00:27:14 Whether you're living in a culture that prizes the collective or prioritizes the individual, there are ways to make better decisions. When we come back, how to make WIZER choices? Shina Iangar studies how we make choices and how we can make better choices. One of our earlier studies involves students at an elementary school in San Francisco. One of my first studies on choice looked at the differences between Asian and Anglo-American children. And essentially, we took a very traditional paradigm in psychology where in one condition children are given a choice, which one of these six activities do you want to do, and
Starting point is 00:28:03 in the other condition the children are told which one of these six activities do you want to do? And in the other condition, the children are told which one of these six activities to do. We then added a third condition, where the children were shown the choices, but were told that their mothers had made the choice for them, as to which of the six activities they would do. And these are seven year old kids. And what we found was that with Anglo-American children, we got exactly what prior research had expected us to find. Anglo-American children were more motivated to do the activity and performed better on
Starting point is 00:28:36 the activity when they made a choice. And it didn't matter whether it was an experimenter who made the choice for them or their mothers. If anything, they were more angry when they were told that their mothers had made the choice for them or their mothers. If anything, they were more angry when they were told that their mothers had made the selection for them. And so Anglo-American children were more likely to say things like, you asked my mother, I can't believe you asked my mother. By contrast, the, and I can relate now
Starting point is 00:28:58 as a mother of a teenager. I mean, I think you would kill somebody if they said that mom had the right to make a choice for him. Now, by contrast, the Asian American children, they actually performed the best for mother. When they thought mom had made the choice, second best when they got to choose for themselves, and you know, just as badly as the Anglo-American children when the choice was made for them by this experiment or whom they had never met before. What we observed there was that for the Asian American children, having mom
Starting point is 00:29:33 make the choice was not this awful thing. If anything they wanted mom to be told that they had done it exactly the way she had said. Because for them it wasn't this moment of someone taking away their empowerment. If anything, they felt more empowered by having Mother make the choice, because now they felt more confident that they were making the right choice. So some of this has to do with our notions of how much we think of ourselves as individuals, how much we see ourselves as part of a collective. It's not necessarily that, you know, Asian and Anglo children are biologically different from one another.
Starting point is 00:30:08 But the experiment that you are citing really reveals that we have in some ways different frames. So autonomy means something different for the Anglo kids than it did for the Asian kids. So what was it? Well, I think in American culture, we have a very deep assumption, which goes like this. If a choice affects me, I should be the one to make it. By God. I'm the one who should make that choice. If it's going to affect me, how could you possibly think otherwise?
Starting point is 00:30:37 And in the Asian culture, they have an equally valid assumption, which says, if a choice affects me and is important, then by God, somebody really important to me is going to tell me, you're a sister, you're a guide me on how to make that choice. How could you do it any other way? And that's an example of how a choice should be made, what constitutes a good or bad choice-making method? These are cultural constructs.
Starting point is 00:31:05 So the takeaway here is not only do cultures differ in how many choices people get, they also differ in how desirable they think it is to have choices. Some cultures see less choices, the root to happiness and strive to limit choices and other cultures see more choice as the path to happiness. You encountered this in your own life when you were working in Japan and at a restaurant one day you asked for a cup of tea. Can you tell me what happened next? So I was in Japan. This was in 1995. I was a PhD student and I was in Kyoto. And I, you know, as an Indian, I grew up with tea that had sugar. Of course, you put sugar
Starting point is 00:31:42 in your tea. And so I go to this Japanese restaurant and I order green tea and I say, can I please have some sugar? And the waiter says, oh, I don't know, you don't put sugar in tea. And I said, yeah, I understand in Japan, you don't put sugar in your tea, but can I please have some sugar with my tea? And in Japanese style, this leads to this big discussion and how multiple people are coming to me verifying I want sugar. Ultimately the manager comes to me and says I'm sorry we don't have sugar. And so then I'm like well I don't want green tea if I can't have sugar so I order a cup of coffee. And they bring me a cup of coffee and with the cup of coffee they brought me two packets of sugar.
Starting point is 00:32:28 And it wasn't that they were trying to be obtuse or rude. They were just trying to protect me from, you know, making a fool of myself. I mean, you can't put sugar in green tea. It's just not done. I was just trying to protect me from my base or instincts. One thing I take away from the story is that there isn't a one-size-fits-all rule for everybody when it comes to choice. So in other words, an arranged marriage might work for one couple, but it might be a disastrous idea for another.
Starting point is 00:32:54 The love marriage and the arranged marriage as tempting as it is to want to compare them. They're just very different models, right? An arranged marriage assumes that marriage is conceived in fate. Kind of like a parent and a child, right? It's conceived in fate. This is not something you break. And so when you're stuck with somebody, you just bring a different mindset to it, right? You know you have to work it out and adapt. A love marriage is conceived in choice. And so you're going to treat it differently, you know,
Starting point is 00:33:31 because you have different expectations from it. So I think that's one thing to pay attention to that, you know, they are conceived of in different ways and so they're gonna bring different expectations for the two sets of participants. I think the second thing to keep in mind is that they are matching people based on different criteria. In an arranged marriage, they are going to take care of the stuff that could cause problems pretty up front, right? They're usually people of the same religion, the same caste, the parents know each other,
Starting point is 00:34:08 generally speaking, they're similar demographics, economics, et cetera. And so by and large, you do have people that are a bit more similar coming together. So at least you get a bunch of those things out of the way that they're probably not gonna fight about. And remember, they don't have this goal necessarily of feeling of, well, a good marriage is one where I have to want to, you know, take this person's clothes off every day.
Starting point is 00:34:35 That's just not their vision of what the ideal marriage is, right? Whereas in the love marriage, it's, you know, it's about being compatible in terms of, oh my God, I have this special feeling every time I see this person. And at the very least, I want to tear off their clothes. And on top of that, we have some amazing conversations. And we get along and we share in values. They're just going to judge them by very different criteria. Sheena has come up with a number of strategies to help people make better choices.
Starting point is 00:35:12 One insight came from a study she conducted where people were given the option to customize the cars they were buying. Let's imagine you are custom making your car. And you have to make roughly 60 decisions, right? The rear view mirror, the steering wheel, the roof, the interior decor, the exterior car color, yeah, yeah, yeah, yeah. Now, per decision, you have a variable number of choices, right?
Starting point is 00:35:39 So for an engine, you might have two, for exterior car color, you might have 56 different colors. So it turns out that if I organize your choosing experience so that you go from easy choices, meaning you're choosing two engines and then 10 different types of, say, steering wheels and then 21 different types of interior decor, steering wheels and then 21 different types of interior decor. It's just getting gradually more complex until you get to the 56 different car colors.
Starting point is 00:36:12 Versus, you go from high choice scenarios, 56 car colors and then it's gradually getting less and less and less such that the last choice you make you're choosing two engines. It turns out if you go from low choice to high choice, you remain motivated and interested in that call. How do we know that? Well, per decision, if you don't know what to choose, you hit the default button. And it turns out that when people go from low choice to high choice, they continue to choose. When they go from high choice to low choice, they start off with enthusiasm, but somewhere in the middle of the choosing process, they just start hitting the default button over
Starting point is 00:36:57 and over and over again. They're also less satisfied with the card that they have just put over 30,000 euros to buy if they go from high choice to low choice, much more satisfied when they go from low to high choice. Now, what are the ramifications of that? It means that there's a, you know, we like customization, that's the whole point of modern living, right? You can have personalization, which enables us to be unique and have it exactly the way we want and apps, et cetera, and make it a lot easier for us to do that. But it also means that you have to make
Starting point is 00:37:34 that choosing experience curated in a way that they're actually intrinsically motivating, right? You can't just throw me into the deep end. Take me in little by little by little. Start me off with an easy choice, then gradually make it harder and harder. You need to start me off so that I know what I'm choosing. I'm starting to imagine what I'm putting together.
Starting point is 00:37:58 The more excited I get, the more I'm able to imagine this thing I'm creating. The more I'm going to be able to put in the mental energy that it takes to deal with those increasingly more complex choices that you're going to give me. I'm wondering if there is an element of this that's also about the forest and the trees. You've also, I think, talked about this idea that it's important to choose sort of bigger categories rather than start out with smaller categories. So maybe you want to decide do I want a hybrid or do I not want a hybrid before you get bogged down and sort of the differences between the specifics to make the higher level choices
Starting point is 00:38:35 first. Do you think the two ideas are connected? Do the forests before the trees? Oh absolutely. I mean when you're going to give people tons of choices, hands down those websites which will have a real competitive advantage is the way they organize the choices, making the categories you have to make the categories meaningful, not cutesy. You have to make the categories one where I clearly know without much thought, oh, that one is more applicable to me and that one is not. The more people can feel confident about saying no to the stuff they don't want. And yes, I am interested in something more in that category and I can start whittling it down.
Starting point is 00:39:16 Happier I am in the choosing experience. By the way, I think to the extent that anybody can do that with the dating apps, the better off people will also be, because literally the swiping right and left, it's not actually helping, even though it might feel good to keep swiping, it's not actually good for the experience of feeling confident or competent during that choosing process. One of the things that you've talked about, and I understand you're writing a new book about the subject, is that choice gives us the ability to imagine futures that we might not have been able to imagine
Starting point is 00:39:51 otherwise. Can you talk about this utility that choice gives us and how it's connected to the new book you're working on? So I really think that the real power of choice does not come from the ability to pick and find. I mean, obviously we spend our lives picking and finding from morning until night, but I think the real power of choice, what really distinguishes us from everything, whether it be other animals, whether it be AI and machine learning, is our ability to imagine and turn what we imagine into a real choice. So one of my favorite quotes is a quote by the French polymath Anouille Procaré who said that invention consists of avoiding the constructing of useless combinations and consists of the constructing of useful combinations
Starting point is 00:40:48 which are an infinite minority. To invent is to discern is to choose. And so I often tell my students a corollary. And my corollary is that to choose is to invent, that we can use choice to construct those most meaningful combinations. And that's when we really experienced the power of choice. That choice is not about being reactive to whatever is in front of you.
Starting point is 00:41:20 It's being able to be proactive about creating those choices that enable you to go from who you are today to whom you want to be tomorrow. And so that's my link to imagination. So in many ways this leads me to a personal question I wanted to ask you, Shina. As a child, you had dreamed of becoming a pilot. I want you to tell me why that childhood dream did not pan out. And what that disappointment taught you about the world of choices and what you were just talking about a second earlier, the world of constructing your own choices. So when I was a kid, I was diagnosed with a written-itis pigmentosa, and so I had a rare form of it,
Starting point is 00:42:06 and so my parents were told that I would go blind sometime during my school years. And, you know, as often happens when parents are confronted by this, they don't really know what to do, they don't know what options they, we might have, and in many cases they were kind of hoping it would go away, and it was kept a secret. And I remember the first time when I was in school I was in second grade and our teacher had just finished telling us that we could grow up and do and be whatever it is we wanted to be as long as we put our hearts and minds to it. And so she asked us what did we want to be when we grew up.
Starting point is 00:42:41 And I raised my hand, I was about like eight years old at the time, and I said I was gonna be a pilot, and everybody's snickered. And because you know, the teacher knew that I had some visual challenges and the kids knew, and that was actually probably the first time when I really began to understand that you can't just put your heart into it. There are constraints.
Starting point is 00:43:08 You are going to have to figure out what choices are available to you. And I think that's true for all of us, right? We're all trying to figure out what our limitations are. And in part of that has to do with being able to differentiate between our perceived and true limitations. And that was certainly one of my big struggles growing up. I was figuring that because there were lots of people that had lots of views about what a blind person could and could not do and what choices they had and didn't have. And so I had to look at that information and I had to look at the information of, well, what could I do and not do? Isn't it possible that there are other choices available that go
Starting point is 00:43:51 beyond this choice set that other people are telling me about? You write in your book that to choose means to turn ourselves to the future, to try to catch a glimpse of the next hour, the next year. Right now in the midst of the COVID-19 pandemic, many of us can't see what's ahead. We don't know when a vaccine is going to come, what's going to happen to the economy. Many of us are experiencing a deep loss of control. I'm wondering what your own experience with blindness may have taught you about having control,
Starting point is 00:44:29 not having control, and making choices that would produce happiness in the future. So I would say that there are three things that I keep as sort of models in my life. One is you always have to be choosing about choosing. And that is that there's so many things coming at you, you have to somehow remind yourself, what are the three most important things right now?
Starting point is 00:44:57 And I try to keep it to no more than five and really prefer three because my brain can't handle more than that. And by the way, everything I'm saying I certainly things that I try to practice right now during the crisis. The second thing is, each day I ask myself, what is the bare minimum that I absolutely have
Starting point is 00:45:19 to accomplish today? That's really important to accomplish today. Everything else I get done is icing. And, you know, that makes life a lot easier because then you understand, okay, of, you know, what's the bare minimum? Like, make sure 14 year old son stays alive. It's bare minimum. No, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, control over. And so you're better able to let go of the stuff you don't have as much control over. The third thing that I keep in mind is that, you know, in life we have lots of dreams. And one of the great things about being a human being is that we're endlessly dreaming. And we always have great dreams. And most of those dreams we have to take off the table.
Starting point is 00:46:23 They're gone. They're just not possible. And that's really sad. But the reality is that as humans, dreams come in an endless supply pack. And so you can focus on the dreams that you can make and you can take advantage of. And that's really where the power of choice comes from. advantage of, and that's really where the power of choice comes from. Psychologist Shina Aengar works at Columbia University. Shina, thanks for joining me today on Hidden Brain. Thank you.
Starting point is 00:47:09 This episode was produced by Lu Shik Waba and Paath Shah. It was edited by Tara Boyle and Raina Cohen. Our team includes Jenny Schmidt, Thomas Liu, Laura Quarelle and Kat Schuchnick, production support from Gilly Moon. Hidden Brain has a request for you. We'd like to better understand who's listening and how you're using podcasts. Please help us out by completing a short, anonymous survey at npr.org slash podcast survey. It takes less than 10 minutes and really helps support the show. That'sung hero this week is Kate Turner. Kate works as an assistant to Sheena Iangar
Starting point is 00:47:49 and she was essential to making our interview possible. Before the interview, Kate took time to test out an app to record the conversation and then made sure we were getting the best sound quality possible. She deleted many megabytes of files from her phone to clear space for the interview recordings. Thank you for your diligence and your patience. For more hidden brain, you can find us on Facebook and Twitter. If you like today's
Starting point is 00:48:15 show, please share it with a friend. I know I ask you to do this every week, but I really mean it. If you like today's episode, please think of someone who doesn't know about hidden brain and share it with them. I'm Shankar Vedantam and this is NPR. you

There aren't comments yet for this episode. Click on any sentence in the transcript to leave a comment.