Huberman Lab - Dr. Charan Ranganath: How to Improve Memory & Focus Using Science Protocols
Episode Date: September 30, 2024In this episode, my guest is Dr. Charan Ranganath, Ph.D., professor of psychology and neuroscience at the University of California, Davis, and a world expert on the neuroscience of memory. We discuss ...how memory works, what causes diseases of dementia like Alzheimer’s, and science-based strategies to reduce age-related cognitive decline. We also cover the essential role of curiosity and the dopamine-curiosity link that can assist memory formation and neuroplasticity. We then discuss challenges with attention and focus, and how to overcome them, as well as how to manage task-switching and create home and work environments more conducive to cognitive health and longevity. Additionally, we explore the emotional aspect of memories, tools for overcoming rumination, and strategies for reframing past negative experiences. This episode will be of interest to anyone seeking to improve and maintain their cognitive health, focus, and memory across their lifespan, as well as for those struggling with ADHD. Access the full show notes for this episode at hubermanlab.com. Thank you to our sponsors AG1: https://drinkag1.com/huberman David Protein: https://davidprotein.com/huberman Levels: https://levels.link/huberman Waking Up: https://wakingup.com/huberman Function: https://functionhealth.com/huberman Timestamps 00:00:00 Dr. Charan Ranganath 00:02:06 Sponsors: David, Levels & Waking Up 00:06:48 Memory: Past, Present & Future; Sleep 00:13:23 Self, Memory & Age, Neuroplasticity 00:18:50 Tool: Curiosity & Dopamine 00:26:55 Dopamine, Forward Movement 00:33:09 Sponsor: AG1 00:34:22 Dopamine, Learning; Curiosity & Appraisal 00:40:31 Memory, Hippocampus 00:43:34 Prefrontal Cortex & Memory, Aging 00:50:07 Aging, Prefrontal Cortex & Memory; Depression, Rumination 00:58:53 Sponsor: Function 01:00:40 Tool: Lifestyle Factors, Minimizing Age-Related Cognitive Decline 01:09:39 Exercise, Brain Function; ADHD 01:17:26 Sense of Purpose, Tool: Values, Goals, Navigating ADHD 01:23:31 Forgetting, Intention vs. Attention 01:30:10 Tool: Smartphones, Task-Switching, Forgetfulness 01:36:36 Tool: Pictures, Memories, Intention 01:45:46 Deep Focus, Dopamine 01:49:36 Hearing, Vision, Oral Hygiene, Inflammation, Brain Health, Alzheimer’s 01:59:51 Déjà Vu 02:09:00 Serotonin, Reframing Memories, Trauma 02:19:05 Psychedelics, Neuroplasticity, Perspective, Group Therapy 02:27:53 Rumination, Trauma, Nostalgia, Narrative 02:30:30 Music, Pavlov’s Dogz Band 02:36:27 Zero-Cost Support, YouTube, Spotify & Apple Follow & Reviews, Sponsors, YouTube Feedback, Protocols Book, Social Media, Neural Network Newsletter Disclaimer & Disclosures
Transcript
Discussion (0)
Welcome to the Huberman Lab Podcast,
where we discuss science
and science-based tools for everyday life.
I'm Andrew Huberman,
and I'm a professor of neurobiology and ophthalmology
at Stanford School of Medicine.
My guest today is Dr. Charan Ranganath.
Dr. Charan Ranganath is a professor of psychology
and neuroscience at the University of California Davis.
He is one of the world's
leading researchers in the topic of human memory. And memory, of course, is an essential component
to our entire lives. Memory isn't just important for remembering things that we learn. It's also
vitally important for setting the context of our entire life. Meaning only by understanding
where we come from, who we were and who we are currently,
can we frame what we want to do in the next moments,
the next day, the next years,
and indeed for the rest of our life.
This is why, for instance,
that people who have deficits in memory,
either due to brain damage
or due to age-related cognitive decline
or diseases like Alzheimer's dementia suffer so much,
not just in terms of not being able to remember things
for sake of daily tasks,
but also for sake of placing themselves
in the larger context of their life.
Recognizing family members isn't just about
being able to relate to those family members
on a day-to-day basis.
It's also about understanding the full context
of all one's memories with those people
and what meaning a given interaction brings
to any of life's experiences.
So today you're going to learn how memory works.
You're going to learn about things like deja vu.
You're going to learn ways to offset age-related cognitive decline, what the research really
says about that and ways to prevent things like Alzheimer's dementia.
We also talk about ADHD or Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder.
And Dr. Ranganath shares his own experience with ADHD, how it relates to memory and the
tools that he has used in order to combat his own ADHD.
Dr. Ranganath has an exquisite ability to describe research studies in clear terms and to combine
that with his own narrative and life experience in a way that really frames for you practical
tools that you can apply in your daily life.
Before you begin, I'd like to emphasize that this podcast is separate from my teaching
and research roles at Stanford.
It is, however, part of my desire and effort to bring zero cost to consumer information
about science and science related tools to the general public.
In keeping with that theme, I'd like to thank the sponsors of today's podcast.
Our first sponsor is David.
David makes a protein bar unlike any other.
It has 28 grams of protein,
only 150 calories and zero grams of sugar.
That's right, 28 grams of protein
and 75% of its calories come from protein.
This is 50% higher than the next closest protein bar.
These bars from David also tastes incredible.
My favorite bar is the cake flavored one.
But then again, I also liked the chocolate flavored one
and I liked the berry flavored one.
Basically I like all the flavors.
They're all incredibly delicious.
Now for me personally,
I try to get most of my calories from whole foods.
However, when I'm in a rush or I'm away from home
or I'm just looking for a quick afternoon snack,
I often find that I'm looking
for a high quality protein source.
And with David, I'm able to get 28 grams
of high quality protein with the calories of a snack,
which makes it very easy to hit my protein goals
of one gram of protein per pound of body weight.
And it allows me to do so
without taking on an excess of calories.
Again, I focus on getting most of my food
from whole food sources throughout the day.
But I typically eat a David Bar in the late afternoon
when I get hungry between lunch and dinner,
sometimes also mid morning if I get hungry then.
And sometimes I'll use it as a meal replacement,
although not a complete meal replacement,
it can get me to the next meal.
So if I need to eat in a couple of hours,
but I'm really hungry, I'll eat a David Bar.
As I mentioned before, they are incredibly delicious.
In fact, they're surprisingly delicious.
Even the consistency is great.
It's more like a cookie consistency,
kind of a chewy cookie consistency,
which is unlike other bars,
which I tend to kind of saturate on.
I was never a big fan of bars
until I discovered David bars.
If you give them a try, you'll know what I mean.
So if you'd like to try David,
you can go to davidprotein.com slash Huberman.
Again, the link is davidprotein.com slash Huberman.
Today's episode is also brought to us by Levels.
Levels is a program that lets you see
how different foods affect your health
by giving you real-time feedback on your diet
using a continuous glucose monitor.
Now, one of the most important factors
in both your short and long-term health
and your energy levels each day
is your body's ability to manage blood glucose.
To maintain energy and focus throughout the day,
you want to keep your blood glucose levels steady
without big spikes or crashes.
Now I first started using levels about three years ago
as a way to understand how different foods
impacted my blood glucose levels.
And it's proven incredibly informative
for determining what food choices I make,
when best to time my food intake around things like workouts,
both cardiovascular training versus resistance training, and when and what to eat close to sleep or not so close to sleep. When I wake up
in the morning, if I'm fasting or breaking a fast, et cetera. Indeed using levels has helped me shape
my entire schedule. So I have more energy, more cognitive focus. My workouts are better. My sleep
is better. Everything got better when I understood how different things, especially food were
impacting my blood glucose levels.
So if you're interested in learning more about levels and trying a CGM yourself,
you can go to levels.link slash Huberman.
Right now, Levels is also offering an additional two free months of membership.
Again, that's levels.link, L-I-N-K slash Huberman,
to try their new sensor and two free months of membership.
Today's episode is also brought to us by Waking Up.
Waking Up is a meditation app that offers hundreds
of guided meditation programs, mindfulness trainings,
yoga nidra sessions, and more.
I started practicing meditation when I was about 15 years old
and it made a profound impact on my life.
And by now there are thousands
of quality peer-reviewed studies
that emphasize how useful mindfulness meditation can be for improving our focus, managing stress and anxiety, improving
our mood and much more. In recent years, I started using the waking up app for my meditations because
I find it to be a terrific resource for allowing me to really be consistent with my meditation
practice. Many people start a meditation practice and experience some benefits, but many people also
have challenges keeping up with that practice. What some benefits, but many people also have challenges
keeping up with that practice.
What I and so many other people love
about the Waking Up app is that it has a lot
of different meditations to choose from.
And those meditations are of different durations.
So it makes it very easy to keep up
with your meditation practice,
both from the perspective of novelty,
you never get tired of those meditations,
there's always something new to explore
and to learn about yourself
and about the effectiveness of meditation.
And you can always fit meditation into your schedule,
even if you only have two or three minutes per day
in which to meditate.
If you'd like to try the Waking Up app,
please go to wakingup.com slash Huberman
where you can access a free 30-day trial.
Again, that's wakingup.com slash Huberman
to access a free 30-day trial. And now for my discussion with Dr. Charan Ranganath.
Dr. Charan Ranganath, welcome.
Thank you.
Speaking of memory, we go way back.
We do, we do.
I was a graduate student when you were first hired
as an assistant professor,
which for those that aren't familiar
with the academic nomenclature and trajectories,
assistant professors are professors
that have not yet received tenure,
but now of course you're a full professor
and you are a world expert in memory,
something that I think occupies the minds of all of us,
even if we're not trying.
So that's actually the segue to my first question,
which is as we move through our day,
how much of our cognition, our perception
is focused on things that are happening in the present
as opposed to being driven by prior memories?
Studies ever been done that evaluate
how often our brain switches you know, switches to thoughts
about the past.
Of course we learn about things that are in our present.
I know this as a cup because I was taught that
at some point, but what I'm referring to is
how much of our thinking on a day to day basis
is literally in the past?
Well, it's interesting.
I mean, it's, first of all,
it's a great question to start off with, and it's interesting because
I actually don't think memory is about the past.
I think memory is about the present and the future.
It's about taking selectively what you need from the past to make sense of the present
and to project to the future.
I know you're a vision guy, right?
And so if you look at people's just eye movements,
right? The first time I came into this room, I'm sure I wasn't aware of it, but I'm sure
my eyes were going all over the place. Now, if I came back to Visi, you say, if you're
like, oh, that was an awesome interview or whatever, right? Hopefully, but maybe not.
But let's say I do, right?
Chances are.
Yeah. So I go and my eyes will probably go right to the Rick Rubin photo.
Then I'll go right to something else,
or to the espresso machine.
And so my memory allows me to make predictions
about where things are.
And it's almost pre-conscious, so that it's
happening without our awareness.
And it's confirmatory.
We're grabbing the important stuff
and making sure everything is where it's supposed to be.
And you can see this play out in phenomena also like change blindness.
It's a little bit of a different phenomena.
But basically in change blindness, there's a famous example where they show a video of people playing basketball.
And they're passing the ball back and forth.
And then this guy in a gorilla costume just walks behind them.
And about, I think it's 40% of the people who watch this video don't see the gorilla.
And the reason is, is that you're generating these serious expectations about what's in
front of you.
And so you're not literally seeing what's in front of you.
You are creating an internal model, a simulation really of what the outside world is.
And memory, whether it's semantic memory, which we'll talk about, I'm sure, your knowledge
about the world, like the cup thing, if it's episodic memory, which is your memory of what
happened, let's say just a minute ago, it's all coming into play in terms of your sense
of where you are.
If I just ask you what day is it? You will use episodic memory for that.
Tomorrow morning, I'm going to wake up in a hotel room.
If I don't have episodic memory, I will freak out because I'll be like, where am I?
Did I get kidnapped?
Why am I here?
And that's really the experience of people with memory disorders.
I mean, they have to be in really familiar environments because it's
frightening otherwise, right? So even I wouldn't necessarily say that we were never seeing
the present. Of course we are, right? But our understanding of the present is so informed
by the past that it allows us both to focus on what's important, what's non-redundant with what we already know.
And it also allows us to detect surprises
and find out the things that are unexpected
and grab the most informative stuff as well.
Yesterday, I took a brief nap in the afternoon.
I do this practice of non-sleep deep rest
in the afternoon.
I gotta have you teach this to me sometime.
Yeah, it's very restorative
for mental and physical energy, I find.
But I fell asleep toward the end of it.
And when I woke up, I was in a dark room,
but I didn't know where I was for about,
felt like 10, 15 seconds, somewhat scary.
But I had forgotten that I was in my solo studio.
I had turned the room lights down.
What is it when we have these lapses of memory as we emerge from sleep or sometimes
if one has been severely jet lagged, you can experience this disorientation of place?
Do we know what that is? Well, a lot of your sense of where you are comes from episodic
memory. Now, there's a school of thought that says that episodic memory, which is your ability
to remember past events, comes from your ability to understand where you are. And we have some
interesting data from sea lions, actually, that speaks to this issue. Sea lions? Sea lions, yeah.
We'll get back to that. We'll get back to the sea lions. But I would argue that to remember where you are when you first get up,
you have to engage in an active episodic memory retrieval.
That is, you have to figure out, well, how did I get here?
And that takes a moment.
Orienting yourself takes a moment.
Because it's a little bit of a controlled memory search, right?
It's not something that's in front of you that reminds you of where you are initially.
And you're also in this little bit of a fog when you wake up.
I don't know enough about sleep to say, but I would suspect that people probably are in
some kind of a stage one-ish or just high alpha.
These brain waves that are like very much associated with kind of
grogginess as you're getting up.
Neuromodulators are probably super low.
So you're, you know, so basically not much adrenaline.
Yeah.
Yeah, exactly.
And so that's going to lead you to really be slow in doing that memory
retrieval that you need to orient yourself.
So like in the clinic, if you want to ask, if you want to understand whether someone
has a memory disorder, one of the simplest things
is to ask them what day of the week is it?
What month is it?
Who's the president?
Who's the president, yeah.
Right now that's a loaded question.
Well, it depends on what time of year
relative to the election you ask, right?
Exactly, yeah.
Very interesting.
I'm curious also why it is that most all of us
have a stable representation of who we are.
So my understanding is that even people
with very severe memory deficits
don't wake up in the morning and wonder,
who am I or who is this person in this body?
That somehow we remember that we have a self,
that we are separate from other selves,
that that kind of knowledge might be innate,
we might be born with it,
and that the representation of self in memory
is very stable, is that true?
Well, here's what I'll say,
it's a really interesting and complex question.
Everything's, you always talk to a scientist,
you get that it's complicated.
But I'll give you as simple of a thing as I can,
which is, so if you look at patients with amnesia,
so they have a memory disorder where they can't form new memories,
they have a sense of who they are, as you mentioned, right?
It's not like they don't know who they are.
And I mean, like, they know their names,
they know their biographies and so forth.
But what happens is, at the time, And I mean, like, they know their names, they know their biographies and so forth.
But what happens is, at the time, let's say if you had gone swimming and you nearly drowned,
you had a hypoxia incident or a cardiac arrest or, you know, you had, like, a traumatic brain
injury, severe memory deficit, right?
Your sense of self doesn't update.
It gets kind of stuck.
And so there is kind of a sense of looking
and not expecting yourself to be as old as you used to be,
as you are, because you're stuck in your sense
of who you were.
And I do think, I talked, my good friend,
Rick Robbins at Davis is a personality psychologist,
and he studies the development of personality.
And it does develop, you know, it kind of stabilizes in these adolescent years and that's
actually also interestingly related to memory. But it does change. People do change in really
interesting ways. So one thing is that people grow more optimistic on average as they get
older.
Is that right?
Yeah, yeah, that's true. So Laura Carstensen, your colleague at Stanford actually has done some really cool work on
that topic.
They become more optimistic and yet I would argue that we become more quote unquote set
in our ways because neuroplasticity, the ability to reshape our neural circuits diminishes
with age.
Well, you know, so I think that's overdone a little bit.
I think you're right.
You know, you definitely see less dopamine activity for instance as people get older.
But what I'll say is that people have gobs.
If you have a healthy aging person, they have gobs of neural plasticity.
But often what happens is, yeah, you get stuck in your ways and that could be related to
a few things.
One is that you get changes in the prefrontal cortex and that leads you to be less cognitively
flexible.
It can be also because people just build up so much prior knowledge about the world that
it just becomes kind of ingrained that this is the way it is and it's harder to be surprised.
I mean, you kind of see this with old scientists, right?
They go like, nothing's new.
Everything's been discovered in 1960,
and nothing new has happened since then.
And by the way, for folks listening
who are considering a career in science,
nothing could be further from the truth.
In fact, prior to recording, you told me a saying
that I've never heard before.
I don't know if it's cynical or optimistic,
but if I recall the quote that Dr. Ranganath passed along,
which does not come from him,
it descends from somebody else, not to be named,
is that, quote, science progresses one funeral at a time.
Very, very actually, very interesting statement.
It could be examined from a number of directions,
but I agree, I agree.
I mean, there's some wonderful,
let's call them aged scientists
with tremendous knowledge and excitement.
I mean, one only has to listen to
the Nobel Prize winner, Richard Axel,
talk about his lovable faction and perception,
and you can sense his delight.
And he's getting up there.
Sorry, Richard, but it's true.
He's in his seventies, right? Hopefully he'll live a very long time.
And certainly science progressed as a consequence
of him being alive and working on the olfactory system.
But I think what you're referring to is really important.
Neuroplasticity doesn't necessarily shut down as we age.
It might even stay open to the same degree
as early adulthood.
But if I understand what you're saying correctly,
you believe that it's because people tend
to seek out less new knowledge as opposed to lacking the ability to create new knowledge.
I believe that's true, but that's kind of, that's an opinion.
I don't have data on that per se, but someone's probably looked at this, but that would be
my sense is, is that a lot of what happens with the way
people's lives play out as they get older has to do with their environment and their
experience. And that's not to say that – I mean yes, neuroplasticity does change as you
get older but it doesn't account for the degree to which sometimes people can get stuck
and set in their ways. And you know, your example of the scientists is such a beautiful example because I look
at the scientists who don't get stuck in their ways, right?
And they constantly challenge their beliefs.
They surround themselves with a diverse group of people who stimulate them and they're
also open to prediction error.
That is they're open to saying something could be violating
my knowledge of the world or my understanding of the way the world works. So here's just
an example. I know I'm going to be free associating all over the place.
Please.
You're going to get into that, but it's like what are the coolest studies that we ever
did? And I totally credit my postdoc, Matthias Gruber, for this. He came into my lab. He was originally German, came in from University College London.
And he told me he wanted to study curiosity and its effect on memory.
I'm like, this is just – I am being totally closed-minded.
I said, this is just a dumb topic.
You know, it's – everybody knows if you're curious about something, you'll remember
it better.
It's just because you're interested, right?
So he said, no, no, no.
This is really interesting.
And so he did this experiment and I got on board with it.
I, you know, we really kind of collectively, it was just this beautiful
thing where I was exposed to something new and I got excited about it.
And so the idea was we would give people these trivia questions.
And so it's kind of like a pub quiz, right?
You sit in a pub quiz.
Sometimes you get a question and it's like, I don't know the answer.
Sometimes you get it and you know, I know and it's like, I don't know the answer.
Sometimes you get it, you know, I know it.
Sometimes you go, I don't know.
But God, I really need to know the answer to this.
And you get this itch, right?
Or sometimes your listeners, I mean, they're probably very curious people.
That's why they listen to this.
And maybe some of them go to your show notes afterwards because they want to learn more,
right?
So we actually scan people's brains using functional MRI.
And so we scan them when they get questions.
And sometimes they had said, I'm really
curious to find out the answer to these questions.
Sometimes they weren't curious.
And then we make them wait about eight seconds,
or 10 seconds, I think it was, something like that.
And then we show them the answer.
So they're kind of in suspense, kind of like you're watching Breaking Bad
or something back in the day.
People at commercials, and so you're like,
oh, now I gotta find out what's gonna happen to Walt, right?
So you're in suspense.
You need to know the answer to this, or you don't care.
Sometimes you just don't care.
You're just sitting around.
So we show a little face, and we say,
hey, how likely is it you think this person
knows the answer to the question?
Now, this is a totally dumb thing to do because they don't know this person.
They're just looking at a face.
They're just making some arbitrary decision.
But I'll get to why we did that because that was, I think, the coolest part of the
experiment.
But let's first get back to the trivia question.
So we found that when we looked at brain activity, when we give people the question,
right afterwards there is a burst of activity throughout the so-called reward circuit of the brain.
There's a series, it's not really a reward circuit as we've discussed offline.
It's really these areas of the brain that process the neurotransmitter dopamine.
And unlike many other neuromodulators that go all over the place, dopamine is much more
restricted in its effect.
And so in the midbrain near the ventral tegmental area, sorry, I'm geeking out on this.
No, we've talked about that in this podcast.
I think the key statement that you made that people should hold on to as we progress through
this is that dopamine is not dumped everywhere.
It's not sprinklered all over the brain. It's released in a fairly restricted sites
in order to drive particular processes.
That's right.
I think that's sufficient for now.
Yeah, yeah.
And so when we look at functional MRI,
we can't measure dopamine,
but what we see is activity in the dopaminergic midbrain area,
meaning the area of the brain around the midbrain.
And you see it in the nucleus accumbens or what's called the ventral striatum, which
is another area that's super high dopamine reward processing area.
The more curious people are, like on a one to six scale, the more activity you see.
It's just like this beautiful relationship, right?
And it's not driven by the answer.
Now there's a reason we probably didn't get it for the answer, but it's driven by the question.
So it's not like they're like, oh, I learned something new.
It's like, I want to get this knowledge.
And so that's part one of the story.
Part two of the story is we show that face right after the question.
And if people are curious to find the answer to the question, they get a bump in memory
for the faces relative to if they're not curious.
Now the faces have nothing to do with the trivia question, but it's being in that
curious state that drives this dopaminergic activity in the membrane.
So there's a whole lot of other studies, findings from that study. But basically, I think, god, I gotta,
you know how sometimes you do a lot of studies,
I mean, I published like 180 studies,
so it's like, I'm trying to remember the exact,
I think it was like functional connectivity
between the hippocampus and the red brain
during the face was predicting better memory
for these faces in general, something like that.
We can put a link to the paper, of course,
in the show note capture.
So let me make sure I understand
that when people are prompted with a question,
that drives the release of dopamine.
The amount of dopamine is proportional
to how curious they are to get the answer
to that particular question.
And then the dopamine itself, if elevated,
because they are very curious,
can increase the probability
that they will remember the answer.
It creates a milieu, an environment for better memory,
but that can confuse us and make us think
that dopamine improves our memory,
but it's that curiosity increases dopamine,
which increases the capacity to store
information that comes subsequent to curiosity. Beautiful synopsis, but I'll do two cheerful
amendments. So one is, um, technically we're not measuring dopamine. So I have to be very clear
about that. This is bold signal, meaning it's, you know, metabolic activity, but it's following
all the usual suspects of where you'd expect it to be.
The second thing is I
do think that dopamine is playing a part and I mean it definitely facilitates plasticity.
So I do think it helps in learning the answer for sure. And there's a whole theory called synaptic tagging
which basically says that if you just release a bunch of dopamine and then you have these
which basically says that if you just release a bunch of dopamine and then you have these potentiated synapses that you can dry plasticity in those synapses,
even if it's not happening at the same time.
But what's really cool is the face has nothing to do with the trivia question.
The theory that we have is when you get that bump in dopamine activity,
you're motivated, you're energized to get the answer, and you're driven towards a state of plasticity.
And now giving you something that has nothing to do
with this question, and boom, you got it.
So when people ask me, and they ask me a lot,
how best to elevate their brain dopamine,
one reasonable answer based on this study
is curiosity, to engage curiosity.
Do you know the quote by Dorothy Parker?
The cure for boredom is curiosity.
There is no cure for curiosity.
I believe it was Dorothy Parker.
I love that.
If it wasn't, I'm sure we'll find out quickly
in the comments on YouTube.
Older people show the effect just as much as younger people do. If it wasn't, I'm sure we'll find out quickly in the comments on YouTube.
Older people show the effect just as much as younger people do.
Kids show it just as much as older people do.
It's just something that sticks around.
So speaking to your point, if you are surrounding yourself with things that will stimulate your
curiosity and if you're open to that curiosity, we can talk about knowledge gaps and all these
things that stimulate curiosity
Novelty is another one Richard Morris has some beautiful data on this with rats
But emma do so and I too have some data with humans
Surprise all of these things. I have a little chapter in my book on this
Drive that system. So the dopamine system dopamine system
So basically if you expose yourself to
opportunities to be proven wrong, you expose yourself to new people, places,
situations, and you allow yourself to be energized by these things and not be
scared and anxious, not be like, oh this person's saying something that I
disagree with, I can't deal with this, you know, or oh we figured this out 30 years
ago, we don't need, nothing's new here. If you can be open to that, I can't deal with this, you know, or, oh, we figured this out 30 years ago. We don't need nothing's new here.
If you can be open to that, I would argue that you're going to be engaging lots of plasticity
and that's something that's preserved in old age.
Recently we had one of the world's experts on romantic relationships on this podcast,
Esther Perel to be specific.
And we talked about a lot of things related
to romantic relationships,
but she said that one of the most sustaining factors
for romantic relationships over long periods of time
is a sense of curiosity, both about the other person,
but also about oneself and how one changes
in the context of the relationship,
and also curiosity about where the relationship
could eventually go, where one to continue to invest in it.
So this word of curiosity seems to be a resounding theme.
I'm struck by, although it makes total sense,
that curiosity would drive dopamine release
in these pathways, that novelty would drive dopamine release
in these pathways, and that also in the physical realm,
dopamine is so important for physical movement.
I don't think this is a coincidence, right?
Somehow evolution organized this neuromodulator dopamine
to be involved the way I think about it
is in both a physical movement,
it's required for it in fact,
as well as cognitive movement.
What we're really talking about is cognitive forward movement
if there is such a thing.
Is there a, we're both neuroscientists,
but you're the memory researcher.
Is there sort of a word or a framework
for thinking about cognitive movement forward?
Meaning as opposed to just recycling past ideas
and memories, the notion of taking memories
and actually putting them, as you said earlier,
into the present to anticipate the future,
actually forward mental movement.
Huh, that's a really interesting question.
Well, first of all, I want to be careful
in not to say dopamine does this
because it's a trap, right?
Well, to be clear, you observed heightened activity
in a dopaminergic circuit.
So the idea that it would not involve dopamine
is a bit of a stretch,
but you didn't directly show that it was dopamine.
So you're being very exact.
I wouldn't be exact and just say that assigning
a single function to a chemical is risky.
But that said, I do believe there's a link.
One of the things that you see is in Parkinson's disease,
dopaminergic neurotransmitter mission
is shot.
And depression is also a symptom of Parkinson's disease.
It's quite a severe one, in fact.
And so what I think one theory goes is that dopamine energizes us to seek rewards or to
seek information, right?
So a big part of movement is you move to get something.
It's approach, right?
They talk about approach and avoidance as basic kind of things that you want to program.
And so, a person with Parkinson's disease has a problem with willful movement, tremors and stuff too.
But I think that dopamine is involved in this kind of energizing you to move.
I think it's involved in energizing you to seek information.
I think it's involved in energizing you to seek rewards.
And so I do think there's some kind of a common pathway there.
And it speaks to this issue of the difference which you've talked about and I talk about a lot as
wanting versus liking. And so Kent Barrage at Michigan's great work on those recent
gobs and gobs of manipulations of dopamine activity and what he finds is an animal
Let's say that it's deprived of dopamine
It will go for rewards just fine. It just won't work for them
It won't do the work that you need to get
a reward. But if you just put it in front of them, they'll take it. So what dopamine
– it is heavily involved with these opioid systems that does drive reward responses and
it's heavily involved in learning about rewards and that's why you get a big dopaminergic
bump when an animal gets a reward because you're learning about
the reward and what predicted the reward. There's a little bit of a credit assignment process
that takes place. What's interesting is you get this too with actually my colleague at
Davis, Brian Wilchin, some beautiful work where he's looked at trace conditioning, which
is when you have like a, let's say if you play a tone and you wait a long time, and then the animal gets a shock, right?
And so what you find is that the animal
learns to be afraid of the tone.
But it's such a long time in his thing,
I think it's on the order of 10 seconds or above,
the animal has to be somehow doing something
to be able to blame that tone for getting shocked, right?
And so what he found was that there's this burst of dopamine activity in the locus rullius,
which is actually known for norepinephrine, but there's really cool work on dopamine
in the LC now modulating hippocampus.
Sorry to get all nerdy here, but...
No, no.
Trust me, this audience likes nerdy.
Okay.
I think that's part of why they're listening. A locus rullius is just an area of the brain that tends to sprinkler large brain areas
with epinephrine, nor epinephrine nor adrenaline for alertness.
So somewhat distinct from the dopamine system, but you're telling us it can also release
dopamine.
Yes, that's right.
And sometimes they co-release from the same neurons, from what I understand.
And so what seems to be happening is, and he's studying this now, but what seems to
be happening is it's not that the animal's going, oh, I just heard a tone, I heard a
tone and then they get shocked. It may be more like they get a shock and then they get
an immediate what just happened and then they get a memory retrieval of the tone. And that
allows them to put the two together
to learn that this tone caused the shock, right?
And dopamine seems to be playing a part
in that learning process too.
So it's not just about reward.
It's really kind of, you know,
the next time you hear that tone,
you might, if it were a real threat,
you could actually escape from it, right?
And there's this whole act of avoidance literature
that you can look at with these approach circuits that
are actually quite useful for avoiding threats
and avoiding punishment.
So it's really, to me, I see this role for energizing.
And that's often rewards.
I like rewards as much as the next guy.
Look at how much coffee I drank when I got here.
So it's not that, but it's just that
it's mobilizing you, I think.
I'd like to take a quick break
and acknowledge our sponsor, AG1.
By now, many of you have heard me say
that if I could take just one supplement,
that supplement would be AG1.
The reason for that is AG1 is the highest quality
and most complete of the foundational
nutritional supplements available.
What that means is that it contains not just vitamins and minerals, A-G-1 is the highest quality and most complete of the foundational nutritional supplements available.
What that means is that it contains
not just vitamins and minerals,
but also probiotics, prebiotics, and adaptogens
to cover any gaps you may have in your diet
and provide support for a demanding life.
For me, even if I eat mostly whole foods
and minimally processed foods,
which I do for most of my food intake,
it's very difficult for me to get enough fruits
and vegetables, vitamins and minerals,
micronutrients, and adaptogens from food alone.
For that reason, I've been taking AG1 daily since 2012 and often twice a day,
once in the morning or mid morning, and again in the afternoon or evening.
When I do that, it clearly bolsters my energy, my immune system,
and my gut microbiome.
These are all critical to brain function, mood, physical performance, and much more.
If you'd like to try AG1,
you can go to drinkag1.com slash Huberman
to claim their special offer.
Right now they're giving away five free travel packs
plus a year supply of vitamin D3 K2.
Again, that's drinkag1.com slash Huberman
to claim that special offer.
One thing that we talked about just briefly earlier
was this non-sleep deep rest protocol
that in yoga tradition is called yoga nidra or yoga sleep
because you lie down, it's self-directed relaxation,
long exhale breathing to slow your heart rate,
et cetera, et cetera.
I called it not an NSDR, not to appropriate it,
but because the language of yoga nidra
is a little bit of a separator for people,
it sounds a little bit esoteric, right?
And non-sleep deep rest tells you what it is, right?
There's a study at the University of Copenhagen,
I can provide a link to it in the show note captions,
and I'd love your thoughts on it,
that show that people who do this practice,
this is a PET imaging study,
so positron emission tomography for those that don't know,
and they see significant increases in striatal dopamine
in the condition of people that do this
self-directed relaxation,
as opposed to a more traditional meditation.
And this is why I say that NSDR is useful
for restoring mental and physical vigor,
which translates to this idea that dopamine prepares us for,
or is a reservoir for potential movement,
typically toward rewards.
And I love that we're talking about
some of the other facets of dopamine
because all too often people think about it
as pleasure or motivation,
and certainly it's involved in motivation.
And I'm very happy to learn
that it's also involved in learning.
I think that's a novel perspective on dopamine,
and we hear so much about dopamine.
Do you think that when dopamine is released
as a consequence of curiosity
in a way that primes the memory system
that we become entrenched in particular behaviors
or routes of pursuing curiosity to the exclusion of others?
What I'm thinking about here is a kid.
We've seen these data, kids with ADHD
actually have terrific ability to focus
if it's something that they're really excited about,
really curious about.
So you give a kid with ADHD who loves video games
a video game, and they're like a laser.
So it's not that they lack the capacity to focus,
it's that they have a harder time dropping into focus.
But it seems that because of the learning
Dimension to dopamine that these circuits could potentially quote-unquote learn that it's video games that provides that feeling of focus
to the exclusion of other things meaning how does one keep a diversification of
Inputs to the dopamine system so that we're continually driving dopamine
through lots of different things
as opposed to just social media or just video games
or just pick your favorite thing.
Because becoming a functional human being
involves the requirement to focus on many things,
not just the things we were curious about.
Yeah, yeah, yeah.
I mean, to me, I talk about this a little bit in the book, but to me, and if you look
at the literature too, you can see this, a big part of being curious is the appraisal
process, so to speak.
And what I mean by that is saying something happens, right?
Let's say something in your environment happens.
If you're going to, you have a decision to make, is this interesting?
Is this important? Is this scary? And I think the thing is that you need to be open to that
possibility that it's interesting.
So let me just give you like an example that I often give. Let's say you're walking
in a neighborhood. You're traveling like you do for many of your events and you walk
into a new neighborhood
you haven't been to.
It's nighttime, kind of poorly lit and you hear a loud noise, right?
You could be like, well, that's a gunshot.
I better hide or I better run or you could be like, oh, maybe there's a club nearby
and there's like a cool band playing.
I should go check this out.
That appraisal is really critical for how you respond. And so it's not just a matter of curiosity happens.
It's a process of cultivation and it's a process of appraisal.
And so, I mean, this is I think, I'm not a wellness guru or anything, but it's like,
I think this is one of the cool things about mindfulness training is it forces you to take
the mundane and be curious about
it. And when you start paying attention to your breathing, my friend, Mishi Jha has really
kind of turned me on to this. She wrote a book on mindfulness and meditation. And one
of the things that happens is you're breathing and you realize, wait a minute, this one isn't
the same as the last one, right?
Or you can do these meditations.
I'm sure you've done this, right?
This part of like, the sound is different.
I'll sit in the backyard doing, thanks to you, I do this morning 10 minute thing.
And so I'll be out in the backyard and I'll be like hearing some sound and I'll be like,
oh, that sound, there's a bird there.
I didn't even notice that, you know, and then there's some other sound.
I'm hearing the freeway.
That's annoying, but I heard it.
So these – it's really a matter of paying attention in some ways and being open to it.
And I think this speaks back to this thing about as you get older, sometimes people find
it scary to be in a new place.
People find it scary to meet a person who's different from them or so forth.
I mean, I love listening to music that's a little bit out of my comfort zone, so people hate it,
you know? So I think some of it is sort of cultivating being comfortable with discomfort.
I think it's such an important theme. I feel like nowadays, in part because of the algorithms
on social media, we are fed things that feed our
progressively greater and greater scrolling
and dwell time, as it's called.
The algorithms are measuring clearly how long we dwell
on a given image and what's in that image and et cetera.
But it'd be nice to cultivate an algorithm for curiosity.
Surely it can be done.
I mean, you got all these smart computer scientists
and AI folks.
And we come into this world naturally curious.
All primates, including humans,
will visually fixate on anything that's novel, right?
And study it.
And try and make predictions and gain understanding.
Maybe now would be a good time for us to discuss
a little bit about the circuitry involved in memory
so that we have that as a template
to digest some other themes in memory.
Most people are probably familiar
with the so-called hippocampus, which is, I mean,
seahorse. It looks a little bit like a seahorse, although the anatomists had a little bit of an
imagination there in my opinion. But hippocampus, let's add to it prefrontal cortex, which you've
already mentioned, and then these neuromodulatory systems. So if we were going to assign a one sentence definition,
functional definition to each of those areas,
what would you say the hippocampus does?
If you need more than one sentence left.
Well first of all, I'll add a whole lot
of other neocortical areas we can talk about.
Please, and let's add those in,
but I think if you can start with three,
I think then folks can digest it.
Yeah, so the hippocampus is controversial.
I mean, it's the most studied area of the brain,
arguably, except for maybe V1.
Um,
Visual cortex.
V1.
Yeah.
Uh, and, uh, but I believe in my colleagues do, I wrote a big paper with
Howard, I can delete Howard, I can bomb and Andy on Lenis on this who, um, you
know, from Davis and, uh, we believe that it's about linking various experiences
to a context.
And what I mean by that is you've got information about smell, high level vision, high level
semantic knowledge information, right?
And the hippocampus' wiring is really set up to not understand what's going on.
So the late David Morrow's pioneer in computational neuroscience proposed that what the hippocampus is about
is what's he called simple memory. It's
basically saying, I know Andy Huberman. Sorry.
He's okay.
He used to call me Andy. That's fine.
Yeah.
Long story.
That's a Davis thing. You would understand.
So, um, uh, so I know Andy Huberman, right?
But to have a memory of this moment that's separate
from, let's say I saw you at some neuroscience retreat when we were in, when
you were in grad school, I have to have some part of the brain that doesn't know
who you are to some extent, right? Because I got to keep them separate. And so there
are the hippocampus, what it'll do is it'll form a memory that's not an Andy Huberman memory
It's an Andy in this place at this time in this context
And that's what allows it to support what's called episodic memory, which is your ability to say I
Went to Washington DC once and I remember going to the Smithsonian as
Opposed to your knowledge about what generally
happens in Washington, DC, or the presidents there.
Oh, that's where a lot of politics happen.
Oh, the Smithsonian is a place in DC.
It's a memory of your being there
at a particular place and time.
Now, there's other parts of the brain
that allow you to associate that information in a meaningful
way and to be able to actually expand on that context and create these narratives and these
stories about it.
Where the prefrontal cortex comes in, and it's a huge area.
It's about one-third of the primate brain.
So it's just massive.
There are a lot of people who go, well, there's no real,
there's a bunch of different areas and I'll do different things. And I subscribe to the view
that that is very true. And at the same time, there's a global function of the prefrontal cortex,
which is what's called cognitive control. It's this ability to say, I'm going to regulate my movements, and I'm going to regulate my perceptions and my thoughts
based on what's important to me in terms of this higher order
goal, right?
So when I've tested, for instance, patients
with prefrontal lesions, I'm sure Mark Desposito talked to you
about this.
It's like the hallmark of them, they used to say,
well, the prefrontal cortex, it's
important for working memory. and you could record from neurons in the prefrontal
cortex or look at fMRI signal and if a person or an animal is holding something in their
mind like a phone number, neurons or bold signal and MRI will be highly elevated.
Their activity will be elevated throughout this period of time
where they're holding in mind. But it turns out if you just ask somebody with a major prefrontal
lesion, here's a bunch of numbers, five, two, seven, eight, you know, I ask you to tell them back to me
in right order. They can do it just fine. But now I start to distract them, I move my hands around,
there's a plane going on, you know, flying outside the window.
I mean, I had that literally happen once.
Now they start to bomb it because their attention is not controlled by their goal.
It's controlled by the environment around them.
And so this is where things get really interesting.
So I once tested a patient and I'd heard about this, but until you see it, it like doesn't
register.
It really blew my mind.
So there's a test called the Wisconsin card sorting test and we don't have to get into
all the details of it.
But basically it's this test where people learn some rule about where to put a card
on a table, right?
And they don't get told the rule.
They just learn it.
And patients learn this with prefrontal damage, learn it just fine, right? And they don't get told the rule, they just learn it. And patients learn this with prefrontal damage,
learn it just fine.
Is it that they get a error signal or a correct signal
if they're doing it in the right direction over time,
they just kind of, the brain figures it out.
Yeah, yeah, so maybe I'll give a little bit more background,
but I don't want to go in the weeds.
No, that's okay.
I mean, if I'm correct, if I'm wrong,
I forget the Wisconsin Card Soaring Test details,
but you know, like they're told to just start
showing the cards and that the correct algorithm will reveal itself
by a series of error and correct signals.
And so maybe I'm taking all the red cards
and putting in one pile of black cards
and putting them in another,
I'm getting error signals,
so then maybe I go odds, evens.
Maybe I divide by suit,
if it's depending on what kind of cards they are.
Maybe I organize by even odd alteration.
And sooner or later, the brain figures it out.
Yeah, exactly, exactly.
And you don't need a prefrontal cortex to do that,
which is surprising, but you don't, you can do it.
And- So there's context dependent action and learning
without the prefrontal cortex.
Yes, but let's unpack this context then, right? So now you've been, let's say, putting all the diamonds
in one pile. You've been putting all the spades in another pile. So now I changed the rule,
but I don't tell you and you put the queen of diamonds in the diamond pile, let's say. And now
I say, nope, that's wrong. So now you have to say, wait a minute,
that was right all this time, what's going on?
This is like life.
This is like life, right?
The thing that used to work for you no longer works.
So you keep doing this, and a person with an intact brain
will eventually figure out, OK, that's not working.
I'll try another strategy, and then they'll
learn the new rule, right?
It's not easy, it's pain, but people will do it.
This patient in particular kept on using the old rule.
And so you have to give a series of hints going like,
what's your strategy here?
And they're like, they'll tell you I'm putting it according to the color.
And then you, okay, well, does that appear to be working for you?
And they'll go, no.
They'll just keep doing it. They perseverate.
They perseverate.
But the interesting thing is he knows it's not working, but he can't help himself from
doing it.
And so what the prefrontal cortex is, it's not about this declarative knowledge about
what you should do.
And I think this is very deep because I think often we get more realistic about people's
actions, especially for people who have head injuries or something like that.
And it's like you can have all of these beliefs that you want to have, but you need the prefrontal
cortex to translate these high order beliefs, things that are very abstract, into actual
concrete action.
Otherwise, what you do is not going to be dictated by that knowledge.
So how this relates to memory is we're constantly barraged by information. I think it might have said something like 35 terabytes. I don't know. But it's a big number and the estimates get bigger
and bigger every year. So we're barraged by information. There's no way you can even pay attention to it all, right?
So you really rely on the prefrontal cortex
to be able to say, this is what I'm doing right now.
And everything else, it's noise.
Here's the signal that I need to focus on.
And that's super important for memory,
because one of the things you see in old age
is older people are bad at most memory tests.
But it turns out in labs we kind of overestimate that.
And the reason we overestimate it is we're giving them a test, which is something hard.
It requires a lot of focus and it's not something they do every day.
But Karen Campbell and Lynn Hasher, these great cognitive psychologists, did this cool
experiment where they had a bunch
of other stuff that people were supposed to ignore
in this memory task, where they're studying
a bunch of things, they're trying to memorize
a bunch of stuff, but there's stuff going on
they're supposed to ignore.
The older people were just as good as the younger people
at remembering the stuff they were supposed to ignore.
They were just bad at the stuff that they were supposed
to pay attention to.
That's so interesting.
Maybe you could say that another time.
You said it very clearly, I got it,
but say it one more time, because if anyone missed it,
this is super important.
Older people can.
They were bad at remembering the stuff
that they were supposed to remember,
but they were just as good as the younger people,
maybe even better, but definitely as good
as the younger people at remembering the things
that they weren't supposed to pay attention to gosh it speaks to
almost two
Parallel processing streams for memory if I'm not mistaken
Or maybe so what's going on there? Is it that one form of memory involves the suppression of?
Information and that circuit is actually quite active
in these older people and young people.
Whereas curiosity for, and the ability to remember
and integrate new information is somehow diminished
in older people.
Earlier we were talking about how that's not the case.
That curiosity, if curiosity is intact,
memory is intact and growing.
Yeah, well, okay, I should say the benefit of curiosity on memory is intact in older people. I got that wrong
I don't know Mattias could tell me if I just
Email him if at a break or something, but I don't know if curiosity itself is as high in older adults
I would self-reported or? I would guess no,
but this is why I asked about movement earlier.
Curiosity is also linked to your ability
to access novel scenarios.
Of course, online you can just thumb scroll or click
and access all sorts of novelty.
Is there any, there must be data
as to whether or not people in their 70s, 80s and 90s
are scrolling social media
to the same extent that younger people are.
I don't know, but I can say two things to this.
One is that definitely there's a lot of work
on media multitasking.
The short answer is bad for memory.
Okay, so scrolling is bad for memory. Well, media multitasking is bad for memory. Okay. So scrolling is bad for memory.
Well, media multitasking is bad for memory.
The tech thing is a super fascinating area in general.
It's really how we interact with the tech that's bad.
But if you're an older adult, your frontal function is not going to be as good.
You will be more distractible.
You will be more likely to go off course.
And so that scrolling is going to be more potent,
because as you pointed out, the algorithms are all
designed to suck up our attention.
So psychologist Herb Simon came up with this beautiful term
called the attention economy, right?
And so the idea is that the more information
that you have in front of you, the more impoverished
you are in terms of your attention.
So there's no such thing as free speech, because it's like you have a limited supply of attention.
So everything has a cost.
So the more information you have in front of you, the harder it is to pay attention
to what's important.
And that's where I think the older adults really lose some of their functioning because
basically I talk about in the book, and it's not a perfect analogy,
is neurons are functioning kind of like a democracy
in the sense that real democracies involve
these political coalitions or alliances, right?
I mean, people talk about the right and the left,
but that's dumb because it's like there's really just
alliances between people who like different things,
and they just form these convenient alliances with each other.
But let's just imagine neurons kind of do this in the brain, right?
And so you have, in theory, to be able to pay attention to something, some coalition
of neurons has to be firing a lot that is corresponding to the thing that you're trying
to pay attention to. But if something is salient, bright, shiny, loud, it's just grabbing your attention.
What's going to happen is, is that those neurons start to shout down the neurons
that are trying to keep you on what's not shiny, but it's important.
Right?
And so what happens is with the prefrontal cortex, you can bias that competition.
Now that's the term that people cortex, you can bias that competition.
That's the term that people have used in literature.
That allows you, so what people have found for us
is just a really cool finding again
is you can find in the visual cortex neurons that fire
when you're seeing something red,
neurons that fire when you see something blue, let's say,
right?
I'm kind of distorting the picture, but you get the idea.
So if an animal is trying to hold in mind something, I'd say hold on a mental picture
of something that's blue, what happens is the blue neurons are firing in the visual
cortex even though the animal is not seeing blue, right?
It's just they're thinking about blue.
You damage the prefrontal cortex, nothing.
So you lose that selectivity.
So what's happening is the prefrontal cortex
is biasing the competition and saying, I know blue's not shining in front of you. There's no shiny
blue thing in front of you right now, but I need these neurons to stay active. And so it's doing
this modulation to help out the neurons that are keeping the information that's goal relevant. So what happens when that communication goes,
let's say due to hypertension, diabetes,
you get all this white matter damage
that happens with old age.
And this is really a big thing that is very preventable
with the right protocols, so to speak.
I'll just bring it, white matter are the fiber tracks,
the wires that essentially that connect neurons
across long and short
distances.
Exactly.
Yeah.
And so if you damage those long range tracks, the prefrontal cortex is not efficiently able
to bias that competition.
And so now the inane gets remembered at the expense of the important.
That's I think the key thing and a lot of that's why people talk
about the prefrontal cortex as the central executive. As anybody who's worked a job knows,
it's like the executives are useless, right? He's trying to get an executive to do, I mean,
except for some who are useful, but then they don't really run companies very well.
There's some CEOs that are doing spectacular things, but-
Yeah. Well, okay. we won't go there.
Controversial, shall we say.
But anyway, so a good executive, their job isn't to micromanage.
Their job is to say, here's the big picture, here's my vision for the company, and I want
everyone to be working towards this goal, not sifting through the mailroom, not paying the
bills.
And so what happens is that when you have certain kinds of things that happen with aging,
like damage to the white matter, that happens through essentially tiny cerebrovascular events,
most likely.
And we've done some research on this in our lab in collaboration with Bill Jagus,
who's now in Berkeley, and Charlie DeCarli.
You can measure this in MRIs with a measure called white matter hyperintensities.
You use a scan that shows up little bright spots where the white matter is probably damaged.
What you find is that these people with white matter hyperintensities actually have memory
performance that's as poor as people who
have hippocampal atrophy probably in the early stages of Alzheimer's.
And they're also bad at controlling information even when they don't have to remember something.
So it's like a double whammy and it's kind of like the executive is trapped in a remote
place and they got no internet access and no phone and so they
can't communicate with the company.
It's just doing their own thing, right?
That's a little bit of what can happen with aging.
It doesn't have to but that can happen.
You see this to a really great extent in many disorders.
This is why so many disorders really affect control and frontal function, multiple sclerosis, diabetes,
many kinds of things.
We talked about brain fog and many kinds of inflammatory conditions will affect it.
Depression, clinical depressions.
I've seen people, older adults with depression, who are cognitively more impaired than people
in the MCI stage of Alzheimer's.
Really?
Yes.
So depression is dangerous for memory.
It's terrible for memory,
and it seems to be a risk factor for Alzheimer's as well.
Do you think that depression is dangerous for memory
and a risk factor for Alzheimer's
because it is, by definition, anti-curiosity?
I would probably not say that.
I would say also I don't know what...
I mean, once you kind of get into these things in the epidemiological world, everything interacts
with each other, and there's genetics, and there's environment, and blah, blah, blah.
Depression means poor sleep, which means poor learning, which means that's all...
I think that's a big part of it.
I think that's a big part.
But you do...
Okay, so let's go back to your question question because I do think curiosity is affected by depression. I don't know the research
on this but I would be shocked if it isn't. And I do think that dopamine activity is disrupted
in depression and your motivation to get anhedonia is the – one of the hallmark symptoms of
depression as is rumination, by the way,
which is memory retrieval
of preferential negative retrieval of negative events
and cogitating over them.
I'd like to take a quick break
and thank one of our sponsors, Function.
I recently became a Function member
after searching for the most comprehensive approach
to lab testing.
While I've long been a fan of blood testing,
I really wanted to find a more in-depth program
for analyzing blood, urine, and saliva
to get a full picture of my heart health,
my hormone status, my immune system regulation,
my metabolic function, my vitamin and mineral status,
and other critical areas of my overall health and vitality.
Function not only provides testing
of over a hundred biomarkers
key to physical and mental health, but it also analyzes these results and provides insights from top doctors on your results.
For example, in one of my first tests with Function, I learned that I had two high levels
of mercury in my blood. This was totally surprising to me. I had no idea prior to taking the test.
Function not only helped me detect this, but offered medical doctor-informed insights
on how to best reduce those mercury levels,
which included limiting my tuna consumption,
because I had been eating a lot of tuna,
while also making an effort to eat more leafy greens
and supplementing with NAC, N-acetylcysteine,
both of which can support glutathione production
and detoxification and worked to reduce my mercury levels.
Comprehensive lab testing like this
is so important
for health.
And while I've been doing it for years,
I've always found it to be overly complicated and expensive.
I've been so impressed by Function,
both at the level of ease of use,
that is getting the tests done,
as well as how comprehensive and how actionable
the tests are, that I recently joined their advisory board
and I'm thrilled that they're sponsoring the podcast.
If you'd like to try Function,
go to functionhealth.com slash Huberman.
Function currently has a wait list of over 250,000 people,
but they're offering early access to Huberman Lab listeners.
Again, that's functionhealth.com slash Huberman
to get early access to Function.
You probably think a fair amount about age-related
cognitive decline and Alzheimer's.
And I'm just curious at a personal level,
what are the sorts of things that you do
to try and offset cognitive decline?
You seem to be a very vivacious and curious person.
I've known you a long time,
and I don't know whether or not you were caffeinated
every time we met, but you have a lot of energy.
You're a very curious person.
You just wrote a book, we'll talk more about,
and you're going on podcasts, you're doing a lot of things
besides running a world-class research laboratory.
So clearly a lot of curiosity.
What did the data say about ways to maintain
or enhance one's memory capacity?
With the understanding that curiosity is probably involved
as we talked about earlier, but at a really basic level.
I mean, a number of things leap to mind,
but I'm just curious what your,
if you had to pick like three to five things
that are clearly substantiated in the data
as supporting the maintenance or enhancement of memory
as we get older, what are those?
I mean, as a memory researcher,
I almost find myself like ashamed
when I talk about these things,
because as you know,
so many of the most important factors are ones
that are related to just health.
So for instance, you mentioned sleep, that's a big one.
Actually, there's a beautiful study that speaks to this that was done, 29,000
subjects in China and they followed them up for 10 years.
Now at the beginning of, so they divided people into three groups.
They said, okay, here's, well, what they said is there's six lifestyle factors that
we're going to investigate.
One was I think engagement in cognitive activities, I think one with
social engagement, one was physical exercise, not smoking, I think no alcohol,
but they identify these lifestyle factors that were basically just kind of
good lifestyle factors. So they get people who have four to six of these
lifestyle factors going versus zero to one of these
lifestyle factors.
I'll just take the extreme.
When they start, they're all the same.
Ten years later, the people with four to six lifestyle factors going for them are performing
almost twice as high on memory tests as the people with zero to one lifestyle.
Wow.
So these are people exercising, paying attention to their sleep, social engagement.
What are some of the other,
I'm guessing low inflammatory diet.
Yeah, yeah, definitely not smoking.
And smoking and alcohol, I think were big ones.
The smoking one is interesting
because we know smoking can cause cancer
and cardiovascular risk is real there.
Although there are some data, as I understand,
that nicotine itself, not smoking, vaping, dipping,
or snuffing, but that nicotine can be pro-cognition
and maybe even pro-memory.
And nowadays, people are using nicotine more and more.
I'm not a big proponent of this
because of the blood pressure increase
and the typical routes of administration are dangerous, But nicotine, I've been told is protective
for Parkinson's and Alzheimer's. Is that true?
Well, let me, by the way, I just have to say I forgot healthy diet, healthy diet was a
big one too.
Which I define as people wonder what is that? And there are all these online debates about
vegan, vegetarian, carnivore, blah, blah, but but but I think most people in the world
Are omnivores most and I think it's very clear that the number one thing for healthy diet is to try and get most of one's
food from
Non-processed or minimally processed sources. Yeah, we're set you in the right direction. Yeah. Yeah
So I was actually emailing with Dean and I issue sure Zai who really talked a lot about this
Yeah, they do great work So I was actually emailing with Dean and Aisha Shirzai, who really talk a lot about this.
Yeah, they do great work.
They do great work.
And so they were actually sending me some stuff
and I had known about some of this,
but like Mediterranean diet has worked really well.
Olive oil, fruits, vegetables, fish, eggs,
limited amounts of meat.
Although I happen to, I'm half Argentine, so I'm a.
Yeah, you like your steaks.
I do.
Yeah.
Which I think, you know, is,
let me come back to this point because I think it's super important,
but leafy greens were a big one.
They pointed out a Rush Presbyterian study that I didn't know about that put people on,
I think it was called the DASH diet, and it included leafy greens as a big part of it,
and that had a dramatic increase in cognitive, I mean, dramatically preserved cognitive performance in people
who are on that diet.
So the healthy diet is a big part.
Now nicotine is interesting.
So if you notice a lot of people with schizophrenia smoke, and one of the things that's been
found is that nicotine does seem to improve functioning in people, cognitive functioning
in people with schizophrenia.
Now I think the big thing to remember about any kind of drug, and this goes for food effects
probably too, but especially drugs, is there's huge individual differences, huge.
And so, I mean, just to give you an example, I could not function without that coffee that I had this morning and then coming in here.
But my daughter wouldn't not be functional after those cups of coffee.
Some people really are affected by these different things differently.
And then of course, there's always a dose response curve and they
often follow these inverted U's.
So Mark Desposito, who was my postdoc mentor, did a lot of work with
dopaminergic drugs.
And a lot of people had done these drug studies early on on cognition.
They would find no effects or sometimes it would make people worse.
And what he found was that if you looked more carefully, there was an inverted U effect
where some people, and it depended on their working memory capacity, were actually benefiting
from the drug.
And then these other people who were, let's say, I can't remember, higher or lower, were
doing worse.
And there's a genetic component to that, unsurprisingly.
Dopamine transporters play a role and so forth.
And so now you start to get into all of these gene environment drug interactions that are
just – I would really caution people against saying nicotine is
good, nicotine is bad.
I think it really, really is a much more complex issue.
It's just like marijuana, right?
So you can look at smoking weed in adolescence.
For people who are at high genetic risk for psychosis, it dramatically increases your
risk for psychosis.
That's my understanding.
Yeah.
Although the times I've said that on the internet,
I caught a lot of pushback from some
of the cannabis researchers, but then having invited
one of them on this podcast, I then got subsequent input
from other researchers, which counter their narrative,
which we can both say because we're both research
scientists, that's what you call a field.
Yeah.
Sorry, maybe cut out any misinformation I might have said in the end.
No, no, no, you didn't.
You didn't.
I think the point is just that it's very clear that there are certain individuals for whom
high THC consumption can trigger psychotic episodes.
Yeah, and we're seeing this with-
But not everyone.
Yeah.
I mean, we're now interestingly seeing this with psychedelics where it's like all these positive effects of psychedelics
We're being are being brought up
But you know a lot of people remember the negative effects on people like rocky Erickson from the 13th floor elevators or like
Sid Barrett from Pink Floyd who became psychotic after doing large amounts of LSD. What was the first example?
Rocky Erickson from the 13th floor elevators,
great psychedelic band.
Oh, psychedelic, okay, we'll talk more about,
Charne is himself a rock and roll musician
and loves rock and roll,
hence the reference to Rick Rubin earlier.
And there's a photo of Rick here in the studio
that our photographer, Mike Blayback, took.
So we were looking at that together.
So yes, psychedelics have claimed the minds
of certain people, made them, helped contribute
to their preexisting, presumably psychosis.
I should also say in fairness
to the other compounds out there,
methamphetamines have also significantly contributed
to the progression of psychosis in many people.
So it's not just psychedelics.
Yeah, yeah, yeah. Right.
Yeah. And then of course, there are those
who have somehow managed to take psychedelics
and become more sane.
Yeah, I saw some-
Or at least remain as sane as they were before.
Yeah, and of course sanity's in the eye of the beholder too,
but what I'll say is that yeah, and you can see this,
actually there's some new concern
about Adderall and stimulants,
and if you're giving it to people
who might be at high risk
for schizophrenia, it might also promote psychosis.
That makes sense, given the noradrenergic dopaminergic
involvement in schizophrenia and those drugs
are pro-noradrenergic dopaminergic in general.
Yeah, yeah, exactly, exactly.
So it's really a much more complicated interaction
and I think this is where this whole realm
of personalized medicine will be super helpful
But there does seem to be a broad general effect for certain dietary
Interventions berries seem to be good leafy greens seem to be healthy. We talked about the exercise
Yeah, and my understanding I've been looking at this in detail lately, but I'd love your thoughts is that while everybody
We now believe men women women, et cetera, should do both cardiovascular exercise, so to speak,
elevate heart rate for 12 to 60 minutes kind of thing,
depending on the intensity,
as well as resistance training
to maintain neuromuscular function,
offset sarcopenia, et cetera.
To me, the really impressive effects of exercise
on learning capacity and the brain in terms of brain health
seem to come from cardiovascular exercise.
And that could just be because that's what's been
emphasized in the studies.
But even when one looks at something
and compares the best human studies,
it really does seem like getting blood flow up to the brain,
getting a nice release of neuromodulators
into the brain facilitates learning.
And then of course people have to do something
with that learning, right?
So do you make an effort to exercise
for the specific purpose of maintaining
or enhancing brain function?
Yes, yeah, actually.
So I like, when I finished my book,
I limped to the finish line.
I had all sorts of crazy stuff happen.
I won't depress the readers with all the crazy stuff that happened.
I'm sure people would be curious, what does it take to finish a book and how much, do
you mean you took a toll on your body?
It took probably, I mean, I don't know, I probably lost some biological years in that,
but it was really like, I mean, it was great.
I mean, it was really an emotional roller coaster though, but then I had a bunch of...
I'm trying to do science, write this book basically in my spare time, which doesn't
really exist as you know how it goes.
And then I had life happen.
My mom was in the hospital, my cat died on my birthday.
It was just like...
Yeah, see, I didn't want to impress people with all this stuff.
It's real life.
I'm just sorry to hear it.
Yeah, no, no, no.
It's real life. I'm just sorry to hear it. Yeah. No, no, no.
It's okay.
So then I finished my book and I was just thoroughly thrashed and I had a sabbatical
because I wanted to have time to promote the book and educate people about what's in the
book, which I'd never gotten a chance to do before.
It's like doing this.
Fantastic.
I get to talk to people.
So I really wanted to make some changes.
And actually, this gets something we were talking about before we started recording,
which is after I wrote the book, it's all, you know, it's going in the proofing stage.
I was talking to my daughter and just, you know, out of the blue, she said, we're talking
about ADHD. And she's like, Dad, you totally have ADHD. And I'm like, what?
And I'm like, oh, Gen Z, overdiagnosis of ADHD,
whatever, right?
And then I remembered when I was a kid,
my school contacted my parents and said, you have ADHD.
He has ADHD.
And it was interesting because it was like,
I actually was ahead in school by a year
and I got held back because I just
was so socially bad.
I couldn't stop talking in class and I was just like really awkward and
impulsive and, um, um, and so, but the, you know, it was the seventies, nobody
did anything, you know, and, and I had all sorts of behavior issues and so forth.
And there are other factors going on too.
But it really got me thinking, oh my God,
I gotta make some changes.
I'm living this unsustainable life
where I'm jumping from crisis to crisis to crisis.
And I say, I don't have time for blah blah blah.
And so, my, again, this is gonna sound depressing,
but it's got a happier ending.
So my dog had died in 2019 of cancer and that was my first dog.
So I thought I'm never getting a dog again.
And in 2020, in the pandemic, I got another dog.
Good for you.
Yeah.
What kind of dog?
She's a, both of these were shelter dogs.
Great.
Good for you.
They're all mixes.
I'm sure there's some pit bull in her, because every shelter dog either has pit bull or chihuahua. Exactly. But she looks very Belgian Malinois and she moves
like a Belgian Malinois.
Beautiful dogs.
Yeah. And they're so smart and super athletic. I mean, she can jump vertically. So when I'll
come home, she'll jump up and then push herself off me, which is like a very classic thing. And so that's why they can jump. Like she can climb up like seven
feet up a tree to chase a squirrel.
Yeah. They use these for military operations in tier one military. Yeah. Yeah. Yeah. They're
trained to jump out of planes. Yeah. Parachutes. Yeah. Yeah. And she's, but she's a smaller
dog. So our big, our older dog looked like kind of, she wasn't, she was actually a Kelpie
mix I think, but she looked like a Rottweiler.
And so everyone was scared of her even though she's the sweetest dog.
This one, she's like smaller even though she looks kind of shepherd like.
Everyone's like, oh, your dog is so sweet.
She's so cute.
You know.
You're like, but this one's the ninja.
Exactly.
Yeah.
Yeah.
I mean, all sheltered dogs have a little bit of a crazy switch in them, don't they?
It's like, it's tough for them.
They're like feral people, feral dogs.
Yeah.
But they have big hearts.
Yeah, yeah.
Because they're eternally grateful. You give them a home.
Yeah, yeah, exactly. And you know, one of the things that I was thinking is I missed walking the dog.
I missed that activity. And so I make sure to do that every morning.
And this goes back to some of the activity things that we're talking about.
I know I'm totally drifting away from ADHD, which is ironic.
No, I see where you're going with this.
Yeah.
But this is a little bit different, but it's related, which is having a sense of purpose
is very important for healthy brain aging.
There's a Trends of Cognitive Science article I'll send you. It's one of these things that neuroscientists don't talk about because it's not – we
don't understand it.
But it's hugely important as part of this whole phenomenon, what they call cognitive
reserve.
And having this dog that I'm taking care of, especially because my kid had gone to
college, he's grown up living independently, walking this dog every day.
It gives me, obviously I'm married and I love my family and I've got lots of them.
I've got a lot of my students and so forth, but it gave me more and I'm exercising in
a way that's kind of fun.
I'm listening to podcasts and I'm moving.
And so that's something right there.
It's not just the exercise, but it's the whole thing, right?
I'm not doing something that I hate.
So then I'm like, I hate running.
I hate, I have this inertia because I've my ADHD brain doesn't like to do stuff
unless it's shiny and fun, right?
And I could go into work, I could write this book cause it's fun for me, you know?
But so I'm like, how could I do this? And so I ended up shelling out for a personal trainer.
I blew my advance on a personal trainer who's great.
And I go to see her and she tells me what to do.
So I don't have to think about it.
And it's fun.
You're in great shape.
Oh, thank you.
You're a few years older than I am.
And I haven't seen you in a while.
And I always have this slight fear
when I run into a colleague again after a while.
Because there was this joke that we didn't tell professors
until we became professors about the so-called tenured look. I run into a colleague again after a while because there was this joke that we didn't tell professors
until we became professors about the so-called tenured look.
You see someone come in as a postdoc,
you see them as a junior professor,
and then you see them after tenure.
And tenure is a big milestone, right?
It corresponds to academic freedom, et cetera.
So it's a wonderful milestone.
It's a wonderful thing that we both have this.
But you see some people who got tenure and just go,
oh goodness, they look like they're, you know,
aged 25 years in five years.
Yeah.
We also see this with former presidents,
not all of them, but a lot of them.
And so to run into you, well,
I saw you on Lex Friedman's podcast,
but then to see you on my charn's
taking great care of himself, it makes me happy.
It's not a judgment.
It makes me happy because I love my colleagues
and I want to see them live a very long time
because I don't subscribe to the idea
that science progresses one funeral time
of my favorite scientists.
Okay, let's not attach that saying to me.
It was a joke.
No, I'm saying that there's certain scientists.
I love my fellow scientists.
I do too.
If you're out there anywhere, don't hate me.
There are certain scientists
that you'd love to see live forever and you're one of them.
So you said walk in the dog,
which presumably gets you some sunlight,
a lot of sunlight in Davis, even in the winter.
Cloud cover is bright up there,
it gets you on a regular sleep rhythm.
So, but you said this sense of purpose, right?
So, and I'm curious about how you now frame exercise.
You said you don't like working out,
you made an investment in your health by paying a trainer.
So now you train regularly.
And that's also an investment in your brain health.
And if we were to go back to this notion
of sense of purpose,
are you talking about a larger sense of purpose?
Like, okay, I want to contribute to understanding
of how the brain works.
You're a brain explorer after all. And therefore the exercise and the money you put towards
the trainer is linked to the ability to do that.
Are you linking these nodes or are these kind of separate entities?
Like I want to be healthier and here's a way to be healthier and ergo, I'll be around
longer to study the brain.
To me, and again, you know, I'm not a social psychologist of this a little bit off out of my wheelhouse
but to me the sense of purpose is kind of this existential thing of like
You know, I got to take care of this dog and I got a you know
And I am when I look at this dog and she's moping around in the corner. I feel bad
But I feel like it's my responsibility to do something with my students. Yeah, with my students,
I have... I was very, very fortunate to have many people leave my lab after the pandemic,
which destroyed so many careers, and many people left my lab and I got faculty positions. I'm so
happy for them. But it also...
It speaks to your mentoring as well.
Oh, thank you. You do well because you have good people in the lab,
as you know.
That's true.
But what was interesting was I'd finished the book,
and my lab was relatively empty.
And I did feel purposeless.
I felt that absence of that sense of this bigger thing.
And so part of the work is, and this was like a thing
that I felt doing the book promotion is, I feel a sense of purpose in explaining science to people.
I got an email this morning as I was getting on the plane from somebody who was asking
me a question about memory.
And I was just like, this is so cool.
You know, after you spend years lecturing students and some of them are sleeping in
class and you wonder, is anybody really impacted by this?
And it's just been a beautiful thing.
So that gives me a sense of purpose. class and you wonder is anybody really impacted by this? And it's just been a beautiful thing.
So that gives me a sense of purpose. I've come up with, I've really rededicated myself to research and we're doing these
huge computational models of learning and I'm trying to get, we're doing VR stuff
and we're going to be doing all sorts of new things in the research.
And that gives me a sense of purpose.
But a lot of it to me about the connection thing that you bring up, it's super important
because often I find myself again because of the ADHD thing.
For all of these things that I like, there's things that you have to do that suck.
And for me, that suckiness is utterly painful.
I mean – and I know there's a lot of people like – I know ADHD is over-diagnosed right
now for reasons that are interesting
And I know a lot of people it's very kind of fashionable to say stuff something like that
Maybe I don't know people can be judgy about the stuff, but for me it really is painful
I mean, I've actually found that it's hard for me to work with certain people if they talk slowly
So I've really had to think about,
so I actually hooked up with an ADHD coach
who has been phenomenal for me.
And I know coaches are another controversial thing.
I don't know that I'm not in my world.
You wanna learn something, you learn something
from somebody who's skilled in how to improve somebody
at something.
Yeah, yeah, well, you know, I mean,
I didn't know what to expect because it's like an unregulated
world so to speak or minimally regulated.
But the person I found was just amazing.
And the first thing she had me do was she put down, had me write down a sheet of all
my values and order them and rank them and stuff.
I'm thinking this is such a waste of time.
Sorry, Laurie, but I was thinking initially this is such a waste of time.
Why am I doing this? I don't really value any. I don't know. I'm sorry, Laurie, but I was thinking initially this is such a waste of time. Why am I doing this?
I don't really value it.
I don't know.
I just do things.
I don't necessarily value them or whatever.
But then I started writing them down and then later I was talking to her about like this,
some of my troubles with motivation and getting things done that I don't want to do and kind
of infamous for having trouble getting things done that I don't want to do in terms of like administrative tasks and so forth.
You're a real scientist.
Well, that's true, I guess.
ADHD is-
Any scientist that likes administrative stuff,
I think I'm willing to call out as,
what are they doing in science?
Cause that's like,
you're supposed to be focusing on experiments.
So bravo.
So values then motivation.
Yeah, so basically it's like, so then I put the two together and I said,
I'm not giving myself credit for why I'm doing this. So this goes back to the whole, I mean,
in a way it goes back to the prefrontal cortex conversation is what's my goal? If I'm going to
see someone and have a meeting and I don't feel like going to the meeting because I'm tired and
I'm bored or I want to just look at this YouTube video or I want to go on social media
or whatever dumb thing that I would waste my time on, right?
I say to myself, well, why am I doing this?
And I remind myself of that motivation and it kicks everything in gear because now I
have that goal in place because the goal just doesn't pop up for me automatically. And so relating what I do to values is a game changer for me.
But it's a conscious thing that I have to work on to remind myself of those values and
connect them.
And that's part of what I think people lose when they retire, for instance.
I see this in people I'm close to who have
retired, they feel like work is their only purpose. And so afterwards, they feel purposeless.
And then they're just doing things like, you know, doom scrolling, or, you know, being
radicalized on, on the internet, or like, you know, going into like, but it's just like,
whatever captures their attention. And I think so a big part of that sense of purpose for me
has really been to get in touch with what do I really want?
And I mean, this goes back to another thing with memory.
And I know this is a total ADHD pre-association thing,
but it's like, people often ask me, okay, fine,
give me all this stuff for brain aging.
And we didn't even get into hearing aids
and vision testing and oral hygiene and so forth.
We'll talk about those three things.
But what I tell people is, I can tell you lots of strategies for remembering names, for remembering where you've been for trying to remember like to do something in the future. Some of the hardest memory challenges we have, but unless you do them, do those
strategies, I can't help you.
And the problem is, is that, and this speaks to, I really liked what you said.
We do have to talk about some exceptions like retrieval induced forgetting and
some interesting things like the pre-testing effect, but your thing on
study skills, I listened to a podcast and I was like the pre-testing effect. But your thing on study skills, I listened to that podcast
and I was like, the beautiful thing that you did with that,
one of them, was that you said, assume that you will forget.
Because if you go back to the earliest research
on memory by Ebbinghaus, he tested himself
and he actually created these weird words called trigrams
that weren't really words.
He tried to memorize them and what he found was within 20 minutes, he had
forgotten about half of what he memorized within.
And I don't mean just forgotten, like he couldn't bring it to mind.
It took him as long to re memorize them as it was if he had never done it.
Right.
So sometimes we have partial memory where we can't recall it, but we get some
savings and it's easier for us to learn.
He didn't even have that for a lot of the stuff.
So then he waited 24 hours and he had lost two-thirds of what he had memorized, right?
So translate this into the real world.
There are some things that are caveats that we do better in the real world I would say
at the big things, the gist of what we encounter, but the details we lose, most of them, most of the details of your life will be gone. And this is true
for even, I would argue this is even true for people with highly superior autobiographical memory,
you don't know for sure, but I can tell you more about that. This is true for everyone who's been studied as far as I know. And so if that's the case, the
question is not like, why am I so forgetful? It's why do we remember the first one? It's why that
title did for the book. And the question is, what do you want to remember? What are the memories
that you want to take with you? Whether it's memorizing things for a class like in study skills,
or whether it's your kid's birthday party,
which I talk about in chapter one.
These are, it's about intention is what I say.
It's the difference between attention,
which can be grabbed by anything,
versus intention, which is saying,
this is what I want to take with me, right?
Let's hover on that.
Attention versus intention.
We hear these words all the time.
Attention is the directing of one's perception to particular sensations or things in one
environment is the way it loosely defined.
Accurate but not exhaustive.
Intention is understanding why we're having a cognitive sense, maybe a cognitive emotional
sense of why I am directing my perception to particular things.
Is that right?
Yes.
It is directing your attention based on some reason that's an internal goal, right?
And that's where the prefrontal cortex really comes in.
So it's very easy in some ways to pay attention to me if I'm like gesticulating and I'm talking very loudly
because it's grabbing your attention constantly.
I mean, let's face it.
I know lots of people in my life who hate this
because I'm so loud and gesticulatory.
You're not loud.
Who are these people giving you this feedback?
Send me their names and numbers.
I have words for them.
And listen, I would say,
given that you run a world-class laboratory,
you're successful in your family life,
you're successfully raising your second doll,
you've written a spectacular book,
you're going on podcasts, you're educating the public,
I would say you're doing great.
So keep going.
And whoever these people are, we'll have words with them.
I need like, Homan's like words of encouragement
on my phone that I could just open up.
Plus your-
I don't want to take us off track,
but you know, I spend a lot of time each morning.
I first do a non-sleep deep rest
or some sort of meditation.
Rick Rubin taught me this to get into intention.
And there are other people who have come
into my life recently.
This notion of intention,
the reason I said let's hover on it is so important.
Because we are in a world where things will grab
our attention, especially on social media.
It's basically a war for attention.
I don't think it's an attention economy anymore,
or Simon's brilliant, you know,
I'm not trying to take anything away from that,
but it's a war for attention.
And one of the ways that you rob your competitors
is by taking their attention.
I used to joke when I was in a very competitive area
as a postdoc, I was competing against a big lab at Harvard
and this and that, which trying to find genetic markers
for retinal neurons, et cetera.
And I said, if I could just get them excited about The Wire,
remember that show, The Wire?
Because it'll suck like 15 hours of those postdocs time.
So I thought, you know, it'd be really diabolical.
I didn't do it, but you know, telling people like,
you have to see The Wire, they, you know, this and that,
you know, because you get someone
on a really good Netflix program,
and if they're a competitor,
you just got a competitive advantage.
But this is being done all day long, right?
In any case, attention is our ability to,
for our perception to be drawn to whatever is most moving,
most loudest, most salient, intention is different.
Yeah, and this is by the way, this isn't a technical term,
this is just, I like it because it rhymes
and a friend of mine came up with it.
So, but yes, intention is your ability to say,
this is what's important to me right now,
and that's why I need to pay attention to it.
Hence the values list.
Hence the values list,
because if I don't keep that in mind,
so we tend to think of control as being just like willpower
or like you want to do the right thing or whatever.
It's not.
It's really a big, there's so many parts of it really,
but a big part of it is motivation.
And motivation is not a trivial thing.
It's not simply wanting to do the right thing,
but being able to keep that value in mind and retrieve that value. Motivation is not a trivial thing. It's not simply wanting to do the right thing,
but being able to keep that value in mind
and retrieve that value,
because everything has a value associated with, right?
And sometimes things that are, you know,
I'm thirsty and there's water in front of me,
that has a big value and that should grab my attention.
So it's not that having your attention shift,
you want it to be flexible,
but you wanna to keep these
higher order goals in mind.
And so it's this balance between stability and flexibility.
Now the key is, or let's get to your specific example of like technology, right?
So ever since I got a phone, that is a smartphone, my first iPhone.
When was that?
Mine was 2010.
Mine was, well, you did better than me, I think.
Maybe it was around the same.
iPhone 3, I think, was my first.
OK.
We'll have to look up the iPhone.
I think I had the iPhone 3, which
was whenever that came out.
So until then, I would check email
when I was at a computer.
When I'm not, I don't think about it.
Now, it's always there. And you get alerts on your phone, right?
So let's play this out now.
We're having this great conversation.
Let's say we leave, we talk about skateboarding and punk rock and like, yes, why didn't
we talk about this on the podcast?
We're having a great conversation, right?
But now let's say I didn't put my phone on focus mode and I start getting all these
little beeps on my phone.
I know people.
I know I played in a band with somebody who had ADHD and he would – anytime we were
in a conversation, he would just check his text messages.
Sometimes he would text in front of me, right?
So what happens is every time you do that, you're essentially shifting your task.
Your mindset changes.
Your intention is somewhat changed by this new task.
So now I've shifted and there's a cost associated with that.
In fact, actually people who study this, there's like four or five different costs that go
on.
It makes you slower to do the new thing.
Now I go back to the conversation.
Well, now I have another cost associated with that. And so I'm not there where you are in this moment.
I'm several seconds behind you and I'm still catching up while you're talking and that requires even more control
to get caught up and get back up to speed. So I'm straining my natural resources.
I'm straining my natural resources.
I'm straining my cognitive control
by shifting back and forth.
But here's an even worse part of it.
So a memory, we haven't even talked about this,
but it's like a lot of our forgetting happens
because we have these blurry memories.
They're not distinctive.
They don't, you don't get a population of neurons
that shouts out loud, hey, that's this conversation
I had with Andy, right?
It's like, it's just kind of this blurry sense of I talk to someone on a podcast.
Not saying that this will happen now because it won't.
So I have this blurry memory.
Well, why does that happen?
Well, part of it is you have to catch the distinctive moments of these events and you have to associate
them together into this cohesive narrative.
One of the things we found in our research and other people have found is when, let's
say if you're watching a movie and somebody changes the topic of conversation or a character
comes in, something that shifts your attentional focus and shifts your understanding of what's
going on, you see this big peak in activation in the hippocampus.
And what that seems to be related to is encoding a memory for what happened up to that point.
And so we call that an event boundary.
And so once you have an event boundary, it turns out you, like you go on to the next event
and you have trouble remembering the stuff that happened right before the event battery.
It's why people end up in the kitchen and they're like, what was my reason for coming
here?
And it's because they pass through three different rooms and their sense of where they
were was changing, their mental context updated to the point where now they have to work to
figure it out, right?
So this is what's happening when we are shifting between different tasks.
I'm texting and I'm emailing and then I'm talking to you.
Or as you've probably seen in going to conferences,
people, scientists, scientists who know better
are sitting typing emails while someone is giving,
I've done this because I'm attentionally impaired.
Or I'm impaired, actually ADHD is a cognitive
control issue I think, but nonetheless,
it's like I do this, right?
So it's like, I get it nonetheless, it's like I do this, right? So it's like I get it.
But it's like you are now creating this fragments of memories where it's not I have
a cohesive conversation.
I have a little bit.
I got a little bit more.
I got a little bit more.
And those fragments of memory don't play together well in memory.
They can compete with each other.
And that competition is a big part of forgetting.
And so that's why it's super important to just do one thing and then do another.
If you want to do social media, fine, do it.
Then do whatever it is you are supposed to do for work, right?
But it's the shifting that really kills you because it creates saps your cognitive control it
Actually creates these fragmented memories. It also actually increases stress levels. So there's all sorts of you know things
I know there's a lot of tech bros who are just like, oh no, I multitask. I'm great
I hear not well, so I you know
I'm from the Bay Area and spend a lot of time with those folks men and women in tech
I think that the best ones,
like the truly exceptional ones,
are very good at dropping into a trench of attention.
They're very disciplined with their phone use.
And the ones that are doing a lot of task switching
often don't have complete lives.
They really don't.
They're not taking care of their health also,
and they are sort of under the sense
that they're working all the time when they're not.
As a graduate student, I didn't have a smartphone.
I did something recently.
I tweeted about this.
You may have commented about it.
I don't know, but this has helped me a lot.
I took an old phone and I put social media
on the old phone and only social media.
So it doesn't operate as a phone.
I can airdrop things to it.
So I use that for looking at social media and for posting.
That phone is in a box.
And then my main phone is for texting
and other forms of communication.
So I still have that distraction around me,
but social media is now a dedicated thing
that I spend a specific amount of time,
and I have a timer on that phone.
So I allocate myself a certain amount of time each day
So for every moment, I'm gonna start that timer. Yeah, once it hits zero
That's it
and I'm starting to shorten that amount of time the impact on productivity in terms of writing in terms of
Researching in terms of just dropping into conversation. It has been enormous with that
simple switch
And I just find it easier to just segregate
social media from the phone.
Part of the problem is that the phone is,
it's like in a walking office.
It's not even, it's a phone, it's a computer,
it's a, I mean, it's just too much access.
Well, and here's the thing,
and this is really gets back to this idea
of engineering your environment.
And because so much of our lives we're out of control
even though we feel like we're in control, right?
And it's really, if I have a higher order goal, sometimes you have to do exactly what
you did, which is hack your environment to enable you to regain control, right?
So what I mean by this is it's like, I, even though I might not check my phone, I might have alerts off, if I have a habit,
I'm thinking about it.
And every time I think about it, that urge pops into mind, I'm getting a little distracted,
I'm losing a little bit of executive control.
So you don't even have to do it, right?
You can just think about it.
So and multitasking is just one thing.
There's other things.
Like one thing is,
I talk about in the book is taking pictures.
So you've probably been to concerts, I know I have,
where it's like, you know, people are like
just filming the whole things on their phone.
Or like now you see the rise of Instagram walls
where you go into places and there's a wall that exists,
so people can post to Instagram.
Nothing wrong with this, but most,
not all the research, interestingly,
and I can get into why, shows that taking pictures
actually impoverishes people's memory.
Really?
Yeah.
What about looking at pictures of,
I think it was Larry Squire,
when I was down at San Diego,
that said that hanging a few pictures in your office
of things that are really pleasant memories
can really enhance your work environment
because you look at them and go, I remember that.
I think like because of all the context it brings about,
but you're saying that the act of taking pictures
depletes our memory for that experience.
Well, let me be more specific about this, right?
So let's say I'm mindlessly taking pictures.
So I go in, I'm seeing the Grand Canyon,
I'm just like, ch-ch-ch-ch-ch.
A lot of people, like intuitively on average,
will say that they will remember it better
because they took pictures.
But what often happens is they're not really focusing
on the distinctive elements of their experience.
They're just grabbing as much stuff as they can, right?
Let's take the concert as another example.
If you are filming the concert
and you're just trying to grab what's being sung,
you will have a recording of the concert.
But what's the memory you want to have?
Do you want to have a memory of the song
that you already can stream anytime?
Or do you want the memory of how
you felt, the friends you were with, the connection that you had with the artist because you were
there? I know this sounds real hippie-dippie, but it's real in the sense that this is what
you want to remember. I think, at least I don't want to lecture people if they want
to remember taking pictures of things. That's fine. I'm sure everybody remembers taking pictures at these things.
But what did you take a picture of?
And so you can use the phone, and this is where the studies show good effects,
is if I mindfully use the phone and I say like,
there's something here that will be a good cue that will remind me later of this great conversation.
I focus the camera on that.
I take a picture mindfully.
I use the camera not as a way of spreading my attention
and just grabbing everything in this shotgun approach,
but rather use it to find what's distinctive
and what's important and actually focus me on it.
That's a really good thing.
So what I try to do is selectively document, not over document.
I'll take a picture of people laughing or people eating.
I like to go to conferences now.
John Listman used to do this, late John Listman, and so he passed away.
So now I try to do this.
I think more about these things.
So I try to take pictures of people randomly, you know, they're drinking a beer and then
they spit out laughing or something like that.
And these are not things that are like landmarks, they're not things that are tourists, but
they are great retrieval cues.
And so what happens is the next part of it is seeing the picture.
Well, what do you do when you see the picture, right?
Do you scroll, right?
Or do you use it as a cue to effectively test yourself, to recall what happened during that event and integrate it?
Now what's interesting is that act of recollecting the event in itself will change the memory.
And it can make it more accessible, but it can also make it a little bit more abstract and story-like.
And so there is an interesting trade-off where you have these things where you have these memories,
and you can even document it
But if you use the if the more you retrieve it the more accessible it will be
But sometimes it'll be less immersive and more like a story that you've told a hundred times
So is it so it sounds like if we have we go on and
On a vacation or to a show or something that taking a photo as long as it's intentional of something specific
that will look, the key is to look at it later,
not just post it, but to look at it later
and to spend a few moments or more drawing to mind
some of the emotional and cognitive experiences
around that memory.
That, however, changes the memory, right?
Anytime we create a story, we're changing the memory.
But perhaps provided it was a good experience,
that's better than to not access the photo at all.
But I'm struck by, as you are,
the number of people who are taking photos at a concert.
A friend of mine who's a very successful photographer
who shoots a lot of photos of musicians
thinks this is the craziest thing.
As if any one of those photos is going to be meaningful,
right, that they're outside of the experience
of the concert, which is exactly what you're describing.
Maybe you just have a memory of taking a lot of photos.
Yeah, yeah, that's exactly what happens.
People will remember something,
but it's not their feeling,
it's not like the friends they were with
and what they talked about. It's more like the friends they were with and what they talked about. You know, it's more like, you know, I use this example in an interview.
So apologies if people have already seen this.
But it's like I just I got to see the descendants.
I'm guessing since you skateboarded, you probably had heard the band.
The descendants. The band Milo.
Yeah, yeah, yeah. Exactly.
So I grew up listening to them in high school, but I never got to see them
because all the
great bands I got into broke up right before Black Flag and the Minutemen and so forth.
And so when they reunited, I was like – I had an opportunity to see them in a club in
Sacramento.
And so I saw them for the first time and what was – I wasn't one of the kids taking
photos.
I was actually watching.
But then there was a moment where Bill Stevenson, the drummer, who's a super intense guy and
really one of the creative forces behind the band, he gets up and he starts walking towards
the crowd and Milo's like, sit down, go back behind the drums.
And he's like, no, I want to say something.
And I was like, I got to take a picture of this because this is going to be like he's
really connecting with us.
And he talked about how cool it was to just be able to have this moment where he's now
at this age able to appreciate this connection that he has with the audience that he couldn't
appreciate before he was younger.
And I was like, I'm taking a picture of this because that's what I want to remember.
You won't remember everything, but if I look at that photo, technically I haven't
looked at that photo again, but just taking the picture of it forced me to really think
about that.
And that's the biggest takeaway I have.
You know, a lot of the songs they played, they did a really good job on, but what I
really took away from that was this connection that I really wanted
when I was a kid, you know, and re-experiencing that feeling of being a kid and hearing these
songs when they were fresh and new for me.
And so that's, I know I'm sounding like kind of a hippie or something.
That's not my style.
Not a hippie, you sound like a punk rocker.
It's not confusing the two.
That's good.
Yeah, exactly.
I like to agree.
I got a digressing all over the place.
No, no, I have a question that I'm hoping
binds some of this together
related to taking photos and memories.
I keep many photos, I like printed photos,
I have these in a drawer, they mean very much to me.
Some of them are in the studio,
but I keep most of them in a drawer at home.
Polaroids are an interesting example,
I think, of what you're describing.
The act of taking a Polaroid is more than just clicking
or pressing with your thumb on a camera.
There's a waiting process, there's a,
you actually get to see the photo emerge over time.
I would bet, even though I haven't run the study,
I would bet that people keep Polaroids more than they,
and look at Polaroids more than they keep other photos.
Which if you think about it is,
if it's true, if it's true,
is counter logic because, you know,
usually people want to do another photo
because they don't like the way they looked
in the previous photo.
With Polaroids, you can't do too many of those.
It's kind of one and done, maybe two and done.
But I feel like the act of taking the Polaroid,
waiting for the photo to emerge,
kind of stamps it in your memory of the experience itself.
And the act of taking the photo is more involved.
It's more of a process than just a click.
That then you see the photo later.
Now, of course, with digital photography, you see it,
but you can take a hundred,
you can take two two photos like that.
If we were to export this theme
of limiting our task switching
as a way to enhance our memory,
setting up our environment in a way
where we put our phone away perhaps,
and we also are focused on intention,
why we are in something,
do you think that there's something positively reinforcing
about getting into a trench as I call it?
Because I find that conversations like this,
one of the reasons I do this podcast,
the solo episodes and these interviews is that
they provide something that my life prior to it
did not provide, which was depth.
I mean, we're just here.
There's no phones here.
And if there are, they're off, right?
And I feel like anytime we go into these trenches,
could be a video game,
could be an interaction with a loved one of various kinds.
But when we go into these tunnels of attention,
there's something that's so deeply satisfying about it,
especially to those who have attention deficit issues,
that it feels like something real happened
and the rest is just noise.
Is there any relationship of the focus system
to release of dopamine?
I know release of dopamine can drive focus,
but is the reverse also true?
That if you're in a state of focus, do you enhance the release of dopamine can drive focus, but is the reverse also true? That if you're in a state of focus,
do you enhance the release of dopamine?
Correct.
That's the question.
Yeah.
Well, I don't know.
I wish I could give you an answer and say, I don't know.
And it would be really hard to disentangle the chicken
and the egg, right?
Because if you're measuring dopamine activity,
you'd have to, well, okay.
So here's what I can say, is that I think that we often think of, you know, we think
of like, let's say an emotion, right?
Or any other mental state.
And we think of it like it just happens.
But in fact, there's a time scale to these things, right?
So it's like, there is a basic response that you get
when somebody points a gun at you,
but then there's an interpretation that you have
that can take that threat response
and make it into something more.
And then you're like really jacking up
your neuroadrenergic activity, right?
You could test her rising.
Yeah, but it's like, I mean, well, in that case,
it could be a catastrophe.
But if you survive, you averted the catastrophe,
the question is, does the potential catastrophe
live within you or does it die within you?
You only need to live within you sufficiently enough
that you avoid the threat in the future, right?
But that's the double-edged sword
of noradrenergic systems is that they capture lots
of memory and they open up thoughts
about what could have gone wrong.
Yeah.
But if it didn't go wrong, it didn't go wrong.
You're alive.
You only need to remember to avoid
whatever puts you in that circumstance.
Yeah, but that can still be scary, right?
Sure.
And this is where I think it's like this,
we talked about this before,
this appraisal is very important.
In the case of focus, it's a little bit different,
but just to make this very concrete,
the prefrontal cortex has top-down inputs
to many of the neuromodulatory systems.
So on average, people tend to think
of the neuromodulatory systems like dopamine
and norepinephrine as being very bottom-up.
They just send signals everywhere
and set the brain into focus or not focus or whatever.
But the prefrontal cortex has some anatomically at least some capability of regulating those
systems both directly and indirectly.
And so that does I think speak to this idea that if you have a very strong goal focus,
you can in fact regulate the dopamine system. I think it's a and nor a norapinephrine to nor adrenergic system
So I think it's a reasonable thing and I bet you Amy Arnston has done some related work on this topic
I'll check it out earlier
You mentioned and I want to make sure that we return to this notion of taking care of one's vision and one's hearing as
a way to offset memory loss.
Very important concepts.
Could you share with us what's known about that?
This is just starting to be a thing,
but the effect sizes, for instance,
for hearing aids are really strong,
both in reducing AD risk, I believe,
and in Alzheimer's risk and in just good cognitive aging
and risk keeping your memory as you get older.
So don't listen to your headphones too loud, right?
Well, actually, okay, so speaking of things I did
to preserve my brain health,
I'm playing in a band now and we're pretty damn loud.
And so I went to an audiologist and I got a custom earplugs.
Oh yeah, all the top music,
I'm friends with some really amazing musicians. They all wear insert earplugs. Oh yeah. All the top music, I'm friends with some really
amazing musicians, they all wear insert earplugs.
Oh yeah, yeah, but I think these custom ones
will be more effective at both preserving
the spectrum of all the frequencies.
Well, I just mean, protect your hearing.
Yeah, yeah, yeah.
No, but yeah, but they're related because
if you can't hear your frequencies,
you might end up turning up and you get this
paradoxical thing. Sure.
So too much information, I know.
But basically that is, yeah.
So there is this issue with, in fact, actually there's an article that the Shersaie sent
me on in the Lancet that one of their public health recommendations is to get into a preventative
mode for preventing Alzheimer's disease.
And one of the things they say is,
screen for hearing and give people hearing aids
and make people use them if they have,
encourage people to use them.
Vision is starting to be a big one.
People who are older get cataracts, get it treated, you know.
A lot of this preventative healthcare,
which our system is not really equipped for it,
but it can really save so much money,
it can save so much emotional pain for so many people.
It's really amazing.
Another one I mentioned briefly is oral hygiene.
Gum disease, it turns out, increases your risk
for I believe it's Alzheimer's
and also for cognitive brain health in general.
I did an episode on oral health
and the effects on, as you said, on brain health
are amazing because streptococcus mutans,
which is the bacteria that causes cavities,
can funnel its way into the bloodstream
and potentially cross the blood-brain barrier,
which is, I think, why people think it might be detrimental
to brain health.
Yeah, yeah, I mean, I don't know the detailed mechanisms,
but I think that makes sense.
And there's this vague notion of cognitive reserve,
which is basically, some people seem to be quite immune to vague notion of cognitive reserve which is basically some people seem
to be quite immune to the effects of cognitive aging and some people seem to be very protected
against Alzheimer's.
So what is it?
One of the things that seems to come up as far as depleting cognitive reserve or putting
it at high risk seems to be inflammation, right?
Neuroinflammation and so Kim McAllister as know, was doing work on this topic of neuroimmune interactions.
And basically, the immune system expresses itself
in the brain.
You get microglia activation that
can cause these inflammatory responses.
And there's reasonable evidence suggests
that it's interacting with tau and amyloid
in this kind of cascade of stuff that
happens in Alzheimer's disease, right?
And one of the things that we're learning now, and we don't know nearly enough, but
the data out there is quite scary, in fact, is long COVID is associated with significant
cognitive effects.
And brain, is that the explanation for the brain fog that people report?
Well, it's-
Many, many months in-
So they report a subjective brain fog, and you can see this,
you can measure this as a significant cognitive deficit
that they're experiencing, right?
And we've seen in the past, like HIV,
was there was actually a whole variety of dementia
that was associated with HIV from the viral transmission.
We can, like you can see with multiple sclerosis
where you have like autoimmune responses
in the brain affect mental function dramatically.
We're seeing more and more evidence of this.
So this is again another one of those things.
It's like you'll go, oh, COVID, I don't care about it, blah, blah, blah.
I mean this is a health thing that can really affect people and it can – I don't think
anybody – it's not a political issue to get brain fog, it just sucks, right?
Nobody wants this.
So I think there's a lot we're learning about viruses and bacteria.
One of the cool things I was talking about before we started recording is I was at a
conference and I met the coolest guy and I'm blanking on his name but I'll send it to
you after.
But he's, he did this great study and he was studying the effects of nutrition on brain health and
memory, especially cognition.
And so he said, what's the most interesting finding that you've gotten?
I love to ask people this because I'm curious and it stimulates my curiosity and I usually
get a good answer.
So he told me he did this study where he has these rats and he gives them sugary water
During the day about the equivalent. He said to a can of coke a day
So they're getting this sugar when they reach adulthood, you know
These teenage rats they reach adulthood and they have memory problems and they do they have hippocampal atrophy
So you go? Okay. Well the hippocampus is affected, memory is affected, sugar, blah, blah, blah,
no problem.
So then what he does is he takes the gut bacteria from the sugar animals and puts it in an animal
that doesn't get this diet.
And he finds the same kind of pathology and the same kind of memory deficit in these animals.
So there's something about that process of like the gut-brain interaction that also seems
to be playing a part in ways that I don't understand.
I think they're still figuring it out.
But again, this really shows this tight neuroimmune link.
We're seeing this now with pollution.
Air pollution is a big factor.
So even if people don't believe in global warming, there's nothing good about being
in a place with a lot of smoke in the air. It definitely can, and this is one of the risk factors that is noted in the Lancet
Report for Alzheimer's disease. One of my colleagues, Pam Lyon, is doing research on this
at UC Davis showing that you get, like she actually takes real pollution from the Caldecott Tunnel,
which connects Oakland and- Walnut Creek.
Yeah.
Yeah.
And finds that rats exposed to this pollution have hippocampal damage.
And so there's so many of these environmental factors that can
trigger the inflammatory response.
We talked about blood sugar.
Blood sugar also seems to be related to these issues.
So, and diabetes is like so bad in so many ways.
It's associated with those white matter hyperintensities that we talked about.
And so that's bad.
We've done some research on that.
But it also affects – it can cause little – if you get severe diabetic ketoacidosis,
you can actually have hippocampal damage from
that directly and it also dramatically increases Alzheimer's risk.
We have an epidemic of diabetes right now.
Matthew Feeney This probably explains, at least to my mind,
why these lifestyle factors like improved sleep, cardiovascular and resistance training
exercise, but certainly cardiovascular exercise,
eating a lot of leafy foods, et cetera.
We know all of those things offset inflammation to some degree or another, right?
I mean, one of the best ways to inflame your brain
and body is to not get enough sleep
and eat a lot of highly processed foods, for instance.
To date, are there any even semi satisfactory
prescription drugs or other compounds
that can slow the progression of Alzheimer's dementia
once it's started?
There are now some drugs that are,
I think they're targeting amyloid
that are producing some modest effects
in stalling the progression of the disease.
See, the problem with Alzheimer's, as you know, is once you lose neurons,
you're not getting them back, right?
And it's like, yeah, there's neurogenesis and you can run around,
but it's not much.
It's not, you don't want to, if you're depending on that, you're hosed, you know?
So, like, but getting back to the exercise thing, it's neuroprotective.
And so, like, let's say with a drug, right? I mean, everybody wants a drug. If I told you,
I give you this drug, you're 60 years old, and it's going to have some terrible side effects,
you're going to get diarrhea, nausea, all this stuff, but it'll reduce your risk of Alzheimer's
by 40%. A lot of people would be motivated to take it.
Now I tell you, okay, well, here's a lifestyle intervention that's going
to involve what Sarah Madden calls down states.
It doesn't have to, we can actually get into that and memory reactivation
during down states, but, um, uh, involves sleep, diet, exercise, social stimulation.
Right.
And these things, by the way, also reinforce
each other.
Having better sleep makes it easier to exercise.
Having exercise makes it easier, better to sleep.
All of these improve mood, right?
So these will improve your mental function, your
mood, as well as your mental function relatively
soon and reduce your risk by at least 40 percent if not more.
If you go to, I can send you this Lancet article, but it's like the amount, the proportion
of variance, meaning the degree of risk that you can reduce with fully preventable or fully
in our control lifestyle issues is huge.
It's as big or bigger than the genetics.
So-
I think people really need to hear and internalize that
because I think everyone's waiting for this miracle drug
that is unlikely to ever arrive, frankly.
I mean, you know, to date we have some okay treatments
for Parkinson's to try and offset the loss
of dopaminergic neurons, but they can even
transplant essentially dopaminergic neurons into the substantia nigra.
But none of those things, L-DOPA, et cetera, have proved to be cures for Parkinson's.
Not getting hit in the head is helpful.
Oh yeah, yeah.
Traumatic brain injury is another one of the big risk factors.
So there are a lot of don'ts.
I'm grateful that today you're sharing a number of dos and both in the context of offsetting age-related
cognitive decline Alzheimer's,
but also in terms of how to enhance focus
and enhance memory.
I want to make sure that we touch on a few topics
related to memory that are a little bit
off the trajectory we're on now,
but that come up a lot when people start thinking
about memory.
And one that's kind of intriguing,
very intriguing is deja vu.
Do we have any understanding about what deja vu is?
Is it just like a recollection of something similar
that spontaneously gets triggered?
What is deja vu?
Well, it's not fully understood,
but I'll give you my best guess that's science-based
and not just my wildly speculating completely.
But basically, one of the early findings that gave you sense about what deja vu is, is Hewlings-Jackson,
who is this great neurologist who did pioneering work in behavioral neurology, observed that
many patients who get epilepsy would have this aura, it's this mental sensations
right before a seizure where they would get an intense feeling of deja vu.
It doesn't happen in everyone but a certain number.
This is associated with temporal lobe epilepsy and the hippocampus as you know is in the
temporal lobe.
But there's also these areas around it that are super important for memory, including the amygdala, but also really
the perirhinal cortex is a key, key player in this.
And so then you have Wilder Penfield and other people
who started to do these surgeries for epilepsy,
and they said, well, I wanna make sure
I'm not taking out good brain, right?
You don't wanna, so Penfield wasn't responsible for HM
and that was like kind of an irresponsible surgery.
HM is a now dead famous patient
and literally chapter in the history of neuroscience
that somebody who had his hippocampi bilaterally,
one on each side of the brain, removed to treat epilepsy,
it fixed the brain, removed to treat epilepsy, fixed the epilepsy, but he had lost all capacity
to remember prior events.
Yeah.
And so, in fact, he had this dense, dense amnesia, right?
And actually, one of the little known things, even in memory research, is he actually lost,
he had what's called a temporal lobectomy
where they just hack off the front part of the temporal lobe or it might have been cauterized.
I can't remember the – I think he cauterized it.
But anyway, they do that temporal lobectomy and he actually had the posterior one-third
of his hippocampus but he had lost his perirhinal cortex bilaterally.
And that turned out to be Betsy Murray at NIH later and other people turned out to be
a huge thing.
So one of the reasons I think that he became so densely amnestic is that it was bilateral.
So if you think about the brain, like you have a side of the brain that's causing
a seizure, so you kind of got a spare tire on the other side where it's like that other
healthy tissue on the other side can sometimes pick up the slack.
But if you take out both hemispheres now you're in really bad shape.
So Scoville did that.
He actually did it for people who are HM had epilepsy and it was a legit operation in that
sense but he did it for people who had I think like psychosis, too, depression.
I mean back then they just did all kinds of crazy stuff. So,
but Penfield was like, no, I want to make sure I take out only the tissue that needs
to go. So what do you do? You stimulate different parts of the brain and you see, does it produce
anything other than a seizure? And if so, that's not an area you want to remove. And
so he would go into the anterior temporal lobes and stimulate and people would have,
sometimes they would have an intense people would have, sometimes they would
have an intense real memory.
But sometimes they would have this intense sense of deja vu where it's like they feel
this I feel like I've lived out this whole thing that's happening right now.
I've lived it before.
When you know that's not true, right?
So what is this?
Well, a number of people, my lab was heavily involved with this and Andy Anelinas
at UC Davis was really central to a lot of the stuff, found that the perirhinal cortex,
which is this area as I said, it's a big player with the hippocampus, seems to be very
critical for this general sense of familiarity that we have. And so, you know, I use in the
book an example of like, if I say,
have you eaten a rambutan before? Now you being a worldly guy might... Have you
ever eaten a rambutan? No, I don't know what that is. Okay. So how quick was it
that you were able to say no, that you were able to think about it and go, I've never
eaten a rambutan? Less than a second. Okay. So you didn't have to search your
entire memory for whether or not you've eaten a rambutan, you know, because it's so unfamiliar, right?
So
things that are highly familiar, like, you know, maybe I'll ask if you've eaten a banana before, or grapes before, you can say yes,
because, and partly you don't have to remember any instance. It just feels right, you know,
those are very familiar things to you. Have you ever seen a grape before? Yes, of course you have, right?
Apple, very familiar to people. So we just have that general fluency and you can look at this like I've
you go into the grocery store and you see someone and you're like, I know I've seen this person before
Where have I seen them before and then you leave and eventually you're like, oh well
That was somebody who I met
at this conference or something like that.
But you weren't expecting them at this context and no episodic memory is triggered.
But there was something about their features that felt very fluent and natural to you and
triggered that sense of familiarity.
And that seems to be processed and you can see brain activity associated with that in
the perirhinal cortex.
And people with damage to the perirhinal cortex seem to not differentiate between the rambutan
and the banana.
It's all kind of unfamiliar to them.
They might remember I've eaten a banana, but they don't necessarily have that sense of
familiarity.
And Rebecca Burwell at Brown University is the coolest
experiment that doesn't nearly get you know how in science you get these
unsung hero experiment well what if this was one of them where she stimulated in
rats the perironal cortex at this frequency called the beta frequency
which is kind of a relatively low frequency oscillation, and
basically put two objects in front of the animal.
And so like typically if there's a new object, the animal will spend more time like exploring
it, right?
And depending on how she timed the stimulation, she could make the animal think that a familiar
object was novel.
She stimulates at a different frequency, I think it was gamma,
and the animal now thinks of, or actually it was like,
yeah, so she thinks, now the animal thinks that a familiar thing is novel.
With beta, it was that it thought a novel thing was familiar.
Wow.
So, it could literally use this stimulation to change the way the animal is interacting with and presumably a
memory driven way with this object. So for those looking for novelty in different domains of life,
maybe this is the solution. Maybe. Yeah. And so, Anne Cleary, just to close the loop here,
who's a great researcher at Colorado State, developed this beautiful paradigm where what she
does is she said, okay, well, does that relate to deja vu well let's see so what she did was use virtual reality and so in
virtual reality you can create these environments and you know put objects in particular places
and so she creates these virtual environments where there are particular objects in particular
places and let's say one's a museum right so a person can go through passively and watch
a movie or they actively navigate through passively and watch a movie
or they actively navigate through these spaces.
And then what she does is she has them go through,
let's say, a video arcade.
But unbeknownst to the subject,
the objects that are in the room
are in exactly the same positions
as the objects in the museum.
But it's a video arcade, so it looks different.
But the room shape,
the spatial layout, you know, everything is identical. It's got a different skin
on it so to speak for video gamers. So what happens is people are much more
likely, very likely, to produce a deja vu sensation when they're in these places,
these virtual environments that look very much like where they've been,
but they're mismatching in some critical way.
So it's like you've got enough to trigger
the strong sense of familiarity,
but the mismatch is suppressing recollection.
And so that seems to be a crucial part
of why you get this uncanny feeling of remembering
is the strong familiarity you get.
And by the way, I've watched these movies and I cannot for the life of me see that the
museum is the same as the arcade. It just feels so different cognitively.
But, you know, I can imagine being like if I really did it immersively, having that sense
of familiarity. So you're really pitting these things in opposition to each other.
So what likely happens with deja vu
is something uncanny that triggers a little bit
of memory retrieval or a strong fluency,
but then there's a mismatch that suppresses it
and prevents that context from coming up.
I'd like to talk about the relationship
between memory and mental health for the following reason.
I'm very struck by the fact that in experiments
such as the work that Carl Dyseroff,
who was actually the first guest on this podcast,
brilliant neuroengineer, of course, and psychiatrist,
described in which he's talking to a patient
who's depressed.
This patient has a stimulator for the vagus nerve.
They can crank up stimulation of the vagus nerve.
And essentially the narrative goes from this patient,
I believe it was a woman in this case,
talking about being suicidally depressed.
She can't anticipate doing anything of any interest
or excitement to her, increasing vagal stimulation,
which by the way, folks, does not just calm people down.
Vagal stimulation actually creates a lot of alertness.
So this is a vast misconception out there
that vagal stimulation is all about calming.
In any case, as the vagal stimulation goes up,
her narrative literally changes in real time to,
yeah, I could see myself going out and applying for a job.
I'm kind of excited about the future, et cetera.
So complete transformation of one's outlook,
but also in some instances, memory of prior events.
So how we cast prior events is so interesting.
And the bridge I'd like to build right now conceptually
is that there are two papers that intrigue me.
One is a paper from Lombardo Maffei's lab in Pisa,
which had a paradigm for exploring learned helplessness
in rodents, which is a sort of a model for depression,
how long a rodent is willing to swim in water
to save its life, right?
Before it gives up.
And there's a learned helplessness
that eventually arrives these yes or not kind experiments.
But at some point they give up
and then they've essentially learned they're helpless.
And of course they save the animal before it dies.
But these animals given a essentially an SSRI
like Prozac can restore some sense of hope,
meaning they'll swim longer
after having learned to be helpless.
Okay, is it recovery of depression?
We don't know, but in humans,
you see some of the same thing
when SSRIs have been effective
and they're not always effective.
You also see this in some of the psilocybin trials
where people have done the psilocybin therapies.
Yeah, we got to talk about that.
In the correct context.
And now all of a sudden people have this
completely different emotional version of the same events.
Like, yep, a bunch of terrible things happened
or with the MDMA trials for PTSD,
controversial right now, FDA didn't approve it,
but a good number of patients described saying,
yeah, this really terrible set of things that happened,
those happened, but I accepted and I'm taking the lessons
and I'm moving on and then there's maybe even forgiveness, et cetera, but I accept it and I'm taking the lessons and I'm moving on.
And then there's maybe even forgiveness, et cetera, et
cetera.
So to me, this is a, this is a shift in memory brought
about by a dramatic shift in neuromodulators SSRIs,
of course, increased serotonin, psilocybin increases
serotonin.
And it's interesting to me that MDMA while it increases
dopamine, most dramatically increases serotonin,
seven X or more in terms of the,
now I'm not suggesting anyone do these drugs at all.
You could blow out the serotonergic system
with too much of it.
With too much of.
Too much MDA, MA.
Although the studies on this is interesting
because the study that claimed that MDMA did that
actually was retracted.
Oh, is that right?
It turns out they had inadvertently used methamphetamine. Oh, jeez. Keep in mind folks that MDMA did that actually was retracted. Oh, is that right? It turns out they had inadvertently used methamphetamine.
Oh, geez.
Keep in mind folks that MDMA
is methylene dioxymethamphetamine.
So I'm not suggesting anyone do these drugs.
I'm using this as a conceptual template.
Yeah, yeah, yeah.
I mean, this is abuse we're talking about too.
Yeah, right, right, right.
It's not like-
Right, right.
I mean, so in clinical trials,
it's clearly been shown both for SSRIs
as well as for psilocybin.
These are still emerging clinical trials,
and MDMA that in a significant percentage of individuals,
especially when combined with therapy,
people can now feel differently about the same memory.
So feeling different about the same memory
and feeling different about therefore
the sense of possibility going forward.
This to me is incredible.
And it speaks to the fact that much of depression,
the lack of a positive anticipation about the future,
et cetera, is based on memories about failures of past
or harms of past.
Yeah, rumination basically.
Right.
So what is the relationship
between the serotonin system and memory?
Or what is the relationship more broadly
of these neuromodulatory systems or the vagal system
that can create these incredible reversals
of what we previously thought of as terrible,
as manageable, and therefore we're willing
to lean into life again.
What is that?
Again, serotonin's like a neuromodulator.
It enhances plasticity.
And what I mean by that is that if you have like a transient learning event, you will
get a change in the connections between neurons that were active during that event.
And super interesting work right now going on in behavioral time scale plasticity and
all that stuff.
So it's not just cells that wire together, it's more interesting actually,
or fire together, wire together, it's more interesting.
But those changes can often be transient
and what people, so like Eric Kandel, for instance,
one who studied serotonin in particular,
and emphasized this, but basically many neuromodulators,
if you give a little bath, bathe these neurons in serotonin or other neuromodulators, if you give a little bath, bathe these neurons in serotonin or other neuromodulators,
you stabilize that plasticity and that allows increases in receptor density between these neurons
that allow them to communicate more effectively. Now you can get weakening in LTD too, we won't get into that,
but serotonin definitely promotes plasticity, right?
And so one of the things I talk about in my book is that memories are, we all have plasticity.
As I said, retrieving a memory can allow us to change the memory in certain ways.
When you get into the details of it, it becomes complicated in interesting ways.
But the short version is you can change it.
We get a small part of what happened when we remember.
But there's that feeling of the context.
There's that emotional response that we have that's both kind of a basic,
raw motivational, my heart's racing or something like that.
That's why people often say, well, emotional memories are stored in the body.
Well, it's just part of the memory.
It's a retrieval cue, so to speak.
And it can be also be part of the retrieved experience.
But you have all of these factors going on that are part of this emotional memory.
And then you have a story that you create, a narrative that you use to make sense of it.
And that affects all these physiological systems too, right?
So every time I use, I talk in the book about an example
of how group therapy is so powerful
as a means of memory updating, social interactions,
where it's like people can change the narrative.
They say, well, you know, I gave you this narrative
about how I'm loud, and you told me,
well, I remember hanging out with you,
and you weren't loud then, and you're not loud now,
and so now I can update these memories maybe.
Vigacious. Yeah, exactly, I can update these memories maybe. Visacious.
Yeah, exactly. I could reframe it, right? And these framing effects are huge. So in theory,
people can take an experience that was traumatic, and many people do, and say,
this made me who I am, and I'm happy with who I am now, even though it's a horrible thing, I'm stronger for it, or I'm a survivor, or, you know,
I couldn't have done anything about it and it's not my fault.
Or you can have these narratives of shame and so forth,
and guilt, and anger, and so forth.
I'm not judging anybody's reaction to trauma,
but what I am saying is that's part of the emotional response
It's part of the memory that people construct
The problem is is that with traumatic memories when they do stick it's hard to change
Because there's so much plasticity driven by the neuromodulators during that event with PTSD
Even we could talk about that as a whole nother thing, but just let's take traumatic memories. It's so intense and the amygdala response drives the physiology in many cases of that
arousal which makes you feel like this immediacy of it, right?
And in fact sleep and nightmares, all of a sudden.
But anyway, stay out of PTSD for a moment because that's a whole other thing.
But those memories are very resistant because of that intensity and often the more you retrieve
them we re-traumatize ourselves.
So reframing in a cognitive therapy sense is very difficult because they feel this and
their brain is telling them I'm under threat or I'm ashamed because
they've reinforced this narrative so many times and you can work through the logic but
sometimes you need to create some big prediction error to generate some error driven learning
or something we can talk about or you need some kind of help.
So if you're driving neuromodulatory systems, like that theoretically could give you a broader
window of plasticity.
In fact, actually, we're trying Prozac on my dog.
One of the things that I've seen …
Why?
She's very anxious and very like … and it's to the point where she'll not exercise,
even though she's a very active dog.
She'll stop on a walk if she hears a garbage truck anywhere.
And so it's very low dose and I'm not necessarily saying go drug your dogs.
But I'm just saying that the story that I've heard is that you get this period of plasticity
where you can kind of rewrite some of these behavioral patterns and make them more open
to training and so forth.
So it doesn't even have to be a permanent thing, right?
And I think a lot of these things like you're talking about with learned helplessness studies
probably transient effects of not being on it for years or something.
But it's not very effective in terms of SSRIs.
But psilocybin and psychedelics have shown a lot of promises being bigger effects, right?
And these produce massive plasticity.
There's two things I think that are really interesting about it. of promises being bigger effects, right? And these produce massive plasticity.
There's two things I think that are really interesting about it.
So my neighbor, Dave, by the way, Davis, I will just say, I'm biased, but it's one of
the top three places in the world for learning and memory research.
So next door to David Olson, it turns out, is studying psychedelic effects on plasticity. And so he really emphasizes that there's these massive neurotrophic factors,
BDNF, like all these factors that are going on that are promoting plasticity.
And for people who've taken them, that's what they report,
is that there's this period of integration afterwards where your brain's just like,
you can feel it. Everything's good.
You change.
You change, yeah.
You change if the change, yeah. You change.
If the integration is guided properly.
One thing that I do want to make sure I highlight
and it's not just for, you know, public safety reasons,
although that as well is that people are so intrigued
by the idea of quote unquote, opening plasticity.
Plasticity is just an opportunity
for learning new contingencies.
Yeah.
Right. Just taking psychedelics is an experience,
but certainly, but the learning of new contingencies occurs
in the integration phase, as well as within the session.
That's why the clinical trials that showed some efficacy
for some people were guided intensely by therapists.
The mere act of having plasticity,
plasticity is an opportunity for learning.
It's not the actual learning.
That's right. It opens up like significant opportunities for reshaping.
But the second part of it, which maybe I think is really interesting is there is also a dissociative element of these drugs.
So ketamine isn't a psychedelic, but I think there are some interesting plasticity effects and definitely produces this dissociative element too. But
with psychedelics, there's often a major perspective shift. And perspective is hugely
important for memory because a lot of our sense of the emotional impact of our memory
is based on a perspective that we adopt when we remember.
And research has shown that you can take the same encoding event, meaning like I tell you a story,
let's just take a very simple thing, a very, you know, I give you a story and it's like I tell you
now viewed from the perspective of someone else and you're trying to remember it, you can remember
things that you didn't remember
the first time around.
Changing your perspective can literally
change what you remember.
It can also change the narrative that you produce.
So now let's say you pull up this traumatic memory
but you're viewing it from the outside
and you're feeling your heart's racing,
your eyes are dilating,
there's these crazy effects from psychedelic,
but you're seeing it and it's not you.
There's some deeper self you're feeling,
whether it's true or not.
Or you have a sense of agency in there.
Yeah.
Some of the psychedelics, I've never tried this one,
but there are interesting studies of Ibogaine, Ebola,
where the universal experiences I understand
is that it's 22 hours long.
It's actually a cardiovascular risk,
there's some things that need to be offset there,
so don't run out and do this folks,
but people I'm told get a high definition movie
of specific events in their life that actually happened,
only when they close their eyes,
so no hallucinating with eyes open, interesting,
and then they have agency within those movies,
and once they exact the change they wanted to have it rotates like a cube very interesting perhaps to a memory researcher why this would be and
Then they get another event of past where they have agency in that event
Incredible it's I mean, you know, there's so much that we don't know and I think it's you know
And I will say that some of the psychedelic stuff
is overhyped and there's not, some of the science
is quite bad in that field right now.
A number of papers are being retracted now.
Yeah.
Yeah, I do think that some of the concerns they had in the FDA
I take issue with.
But what I will say is that you have
a drug that dramatically increases plasticity,
but it changes dramatically the mental context that you have
when you pull up the memory.
So you have a real opportunity for memory updating.
Now there's a phenomenon in the animal literature
called reconsolidation, where essentially the act
of retrieving a memory opens it up to requiring some kinds of neuromodulators again
to really promote resealing of the memory, so to speak.
But it can also, if you interrupt it, you can erase the memories.
And theoretically, if you can do that, you could also change the memories pretty significantly.
So if I can vividly access some neural population that's giving
me this, normally gives me this physiological response that sets off this train of thoughts
and it's associated with this physical and mental context, and I can dramatically, I
can access those neurons but dramatically reshape the context and dramatically reshape
the narrative, I've created the opportunity for massive change.
I don't know if that's true, but it sure makes sense to me that that's the case.
Now having said that, if you and me share our traumatic elements of our childhood, I'm
sure we could go out for drinks and do this for quite a few hours, right?
That in and of itself will also produce some change
in our memories. It's very powerful. And I saw this in the clinic where I was doing group
therapy with Vietnam vets. And I'm like, I'm a total fraud. I'm like, I don't know, 27
years old, 28 years old. And I'm like in with these like, you know, Vietnam vets in their 50s who've really seen stuff,
you know, and they live in combat zones now in Chicago.
And what I realized was everyone was telling their story, but they're hearing reflections
of their story from me, but also from other members of the group who they can relate to
that are different from their narrative.
And now all of a sudden, what happens is
the memory is no longer theirs.
It's a collective memory that's shared by all these people
because the memory now incorporates elements
of their reactions as well.
It allows people to remember it in a new context, right? And I mean, and we can
just take a much more watered down version of this where how many times have you had
a terrible experience and it became a great story? I basically say that there's no point
in having a bad experience in life if you don't get a great story out of it, right?
So I talk in the book about a near-death experience I had paddle boarding and everything about this was stupid.
It's like the degree, I could send you pictures of it
and you'd be like, oh my God, what was he thinking?
I've made foolish errors in outdoor adventures in my past
where afterwards I thought that was a really dumb move
to even go to that location to dive,
let alone what we did when we did there.
Like, you know, I mean, some of the stupid stuff
that we did, even as kids, like bridge jumps
and without testing water depth,
I mean, stupid, stupid stuff
that I don't recommend anyone repeat.
But you're right, the surviving stories,
you carry those forward.
Yeah, yeah, exactly, exactly.
The problem is the other stories we can talk about,
people who are paralyzed, dead, et cetera,
those stories exist too.
Yeah, yeah, and well, so, I mean, to be clear,
it's like I felt horrible during that experience,
and it was one of the most immediately
fear-inducing experiences I've had.
But later, me and Randy O'Reilly, the computational
neuroscience, we're friends.
We both did the stupid thing together.
We would tell the story, tag team it.
And, you know, we told it to students and friends and so forth.
And it just became funny.
And so each time we told it, it just became kind of funnier and funnier.
And you start to embellish things and so forth.
And so that change of perspective was really drawn out by sharing and seeing
people laugh and see people like, what were you thinking?
You're such a smart person and you did this and, and it becomes part of the
narrative and you know, keep in mind, there are people who do this, that they
say like, I've had a really traumatic experience, but I've learned from them.
I've had a horrible, emotionally abusive relationship, but I've learned from it, right?
And I don't mean to trivialize anybody's experience who didn't have that thing and they're just
traumatized by it and they carry it with them.
But what I am saying is that the memory, so to speak, I think in neuroscience right now
there's a big hot topic about N-gram grams as if a bunch of neurons is the memory. But every time we have a memory, we're painting a new picture. We're creating
a new novel.
Well, and I think this is where people have to be very careful not to cowboy post-traumatic
stress disorder treatment in a way that allows the narrative to make it worse because people, previous guest on this
podcast has this notion of, in describing this,
of story fondling where people can go further and further
into the description of how terrible something is,
reinforced by others and then the memory changes
to become much worse than either the real events were
or just simply worse within their body and mind,
and then they have to live that forward.
So it can go both ways, which is really points to the key,
which is to do this with really trained professionals.
Yeah, yeah, and it's all about,
because you can re-traumatize yourself,
and this is also why rumination is so bad in depression,
is because you recall a negative memory,
and that gets you in a negative mood
because you pull up the contacts and then that makes it easier to recall more negative
memories and then every time you recall them, now they're getting more power because they're
associated with these negative moods.
Trevor Burrus So recollection is really a double-edged blade.
David Kopel Oh, yeah.
Yeah.
I mean look at like – so if you take something like reminiscence or nostalgia.
So the original term nostalgia,
I credit Felipe de Brigard for,
he's a philosopher neuroscientist.
He told me that nostalgia used to be a term for disease
that was coined by a Swiss physician.
And he used it to talk about a kind of post-traumatic
experience that soldiers had,
where they would get so wistful about their home
that it just made them miserable in the places that they were at. It's been referred to as the pain
of an old wound. Yeah, yeah, yeah, exactly. I didn't say that someone else. Yeah, yeah. And so it can,
the research shows it can have very positive effects on mental health, right?
And it can have positive effects on mental health if you use that as a way of saying,
hey, this is just a great thing that's happened. I'm grateful for that.
But it can become toxic if you're like, my life used to be so great and now it's terrible.
Even if it's positive reminiscence, if you come into it with the wrong attributions,
it can become negative, toxic, and it can contaminate your present, right? I mean,
the past has got good and it's got bad in it, just like the present has got good and it's got bad in
it. And it's really all about what are the narratives you're constructing for it in many
ways. And that plays a big part in the dynamic, malleable,
constantly shape-shifting nature of memory.
Incredible.
I want to make sure that we talk about something
at first glance very different than all of this,
but it lands squarely in the conversation
we've had until now, which is your love for
and your participation in rock and roll.
You have a band, right?
Pavlov's Dogs.
Yep, actually I have a couple bands now, I think, please.
Okay, what are the other ones called?
Well, so Pavlov's Dogs, I'll say,
versus like a band of neuroscientists
and psychology, neuroscientists and psychologists,
neuroscientists who met actually,
most of us met at a memory meeting, actually.
And so we get together at conferences
and we'll rent out a club and we'll play basically,
Brad calls it skinny tie music.
It's like-
Is it original music or covers?
Covers, that one's a cover band.
Covers, okay.
And so we'll play like the Ramones, the Clash Gang of Four.
Great.
It's a lot of, we added Blondie and...
She's still touring.
Yeah, she's amazing, isn't she?
A friend of mine went on tour with her.
Yeah, there's a band called Surfboard.
Surfboard was on tour with Blondie.
Oh, sir, I know them.
Yeah.
Oh, that's a friend of yours.
Yeah, Danny from Surfboard, yeah, great band, and she toured with Blondie. Oh sir, I know them. Yeah. Oh, that's a friend of yours. Yeah, Danny from Surfboard. Yeah, that great band.
And she toured with Blondie.
Blondie's amazing.
She's still super vivacious as a surfboard
and Danny.
What instrument do you play?
I play guitar and I also do vocals.
Lead vocals?
Yes, or yeah.
And-
Reluctant, because sometimes it's talking
or being off key, but yeah.
Can people find links to any Pavlov dog live shows
or recordings online?
I think we have some recordings on YouTube.
And if you look on our Facebook page,
maybe our Instagram too, we have live recordings.
Okay, and you have a show coming up.
We'll put a link to that,
because people will listen to this long after that,
presumably, that's coming up in Chicago on October.
I think it's the seventh, is it a month?
It's the Monday.
We'll put a link to that.
Yeah, yeah, yeah, sorry.
But for those that can't make it,
because most of us aren't in Chicago, including me,
unfortunately, we'll keep an eye out for Pavlov's dogs.
I love that you play music,
and I just have, for sake of time,
one question about your love for rock and roll
and playing music.
When you're playing rock and roll live,
are you thinking about anything else?
Sometimes and that's when I suck.
Actually, you know, one thing, one hack that I came up with
cause it's like with the cover band, we practice like, we cram.
It's the worst thing.
It's not space learning.
It's like we go through the songs
and we keep adding and taking away songs,
but it's like we'll cram for like, I don't know,
about eight hours of practices over three days or something.
And so for me, it's like a constant memory thing,
because my brain doesn't want to play covers.
It wants to play the songs that I've written.
And so there's always this very thing.
And then I get nervous.
I get really nervous.
And so I move a lot, and then I'll see friends.
And that kills me, because then I
start thinking, what are they thinking?
And so I started my last show I did with Pavlov's dogs.
I wore sunglasses, and it was great.
Because then I wasn't attuned to them and I was,
it just goes back to what we're talking about kind of with the camera and stuff. I was feeling it
and I was thinking, I was, I was not, I was in the flow in the zone and feeling it and doing it and
not thinking about it. And there's a whole interesting literature choking under pressure
that actually relates to this idea of like, you know, sometimes having too much cognitive control going on is like really bad
When you're under stress, and if you know something fairly well, you're gonna be better off if you just go into an automatic state
I love that when I do live shows. I like to have the
House lights relatively dim. I don't want to see anybody at first and then as they get more comfortable
I'm happy to have the house lights come. Oh, what do you play? I didn't know you play. I don't want to see anybody at first. And then as they get more comfortable, I'm happy to have the house lights come up.
Oh, what do you play?
I didn't know you play.
Oh, I don't play anything.
I do live events where I talk about science.
Oh, yeah, yeah.
Where I tell stories about science and scientists.
Okay, a couple of things are right in the front of my mind
and I'd be remiss if I didn't say them right now.
First of all, it's absolutely clear
that we need to get you back here
for more discussion about memory and learning.
There's just so much that we didn't have the opportunity
to cover in this conversation,
but we most certainly will in a future conversation.
We didn't bring up the turkeys of Davis.
Not the turkeys, no we didn't.
Second of all, I want to thank you
for writing your book, Why We Remember,
because it's a fantastic exploration
of the modern understanding of memory.
Still some of the mysteries that remain,
but this is a field that's evolved a lot
and you capture so much of the incredible findings there
over the years in a very pleasurable way.
So it's a pleasure to read.
And then I also want to thank you for coming here today
to share with us your understanding about memory
and also your sharing of your experience with ADHD
and some of the tools you use, some of the struggles.
I think all too often people hear about these, you know,
scientists or physicians or people who are authorities
on a topic and they don't hear about the challenges
they face and I assure you that a great, great many people
will appreciate the fact that you yourself
have struggled with certain issues related to attention,
but that you've overcome them at least as well
to be able to be a functional parent and family man,
professor, author, now public educator, dog owner,
second time around.
And for that and for a great many other reasons,
you've educated us and you've given us
a great many practical tools.
It's also great to see you as a fellow punk rocker
and old friend.
And I even let you call me Andy.
So thank you.
So thanks for coming here today
and please do come back again, Charn.
Oh, I would love to.
Thank you, Andrew.
It's just been great to be here.
Thank you for joining me for today's discussion
about memory and ADHD with Dr. Charan Ranganath.
To learn more about his work and to find a link to his book
as well as social media accounts,
please see the links in the show note captions.
If you're learning from and or enjoying this podcast,
please subscribe to our YouTube channel.
That's a terrific zero cost way to support us.
In addition, please subscribe to the podcast
on both Spotify and Apple.
And on both Spotify and Apple,
you can leave us up to a five-star review.
Please check out the sponsors mentioned at the beginning
and throughout today's episode.
That's the best way to support this podcast.
If you have questions for me or comments about the podcast
or guests or topics that you'd like me to consider
for the Huberman Lab podcast,
please put those in the comment section on YouTube.
I do read all the comments.
For those of you that haven't heard,
I have a new book coming out.
It's my very first book.
It's entitled, protocols,
an operating manual for the human body.
This is a book that I've been working on
for more than five years,
and that's based on more than 30 years
of research and experience.
And it covers protocols for everything from sleep
to exercise, to stress control,
protocols related to focus and motivation.
And of course I provide the scientific substantiation
for the protocols that are included.
The book is now available by presale at protocolsbook.com.
There you can find links to various vendors.
You can pick the one that you like best.
Again, the book is called Protocols,
an operating manual for the human body.
If you're not already following me on social media,
I am Huberman Lab on all social media platforms.
So that's Instagram, X, formerly known as Twitter,
threads, Facebook, and LinkedIn.
And on all those platforms,
I discuss science and science related tools,
some of which overlaps with the content
of the Huberman Lab podcast, but much of which is distinct from the content on the Huberman
Lab podcast. Again, that's Huberman Lab on all social media channels. If you haven't already
subscribed to our Neural Network Newsletter, our Neural Network Newsletter is a zero cost monthly
newsletter that includes podcast summaries, as well as protocols in the form of brief one to
three page PDFs. Those protocol PDFs are on things like neuroplasticity and learning,
optimizing dopamine, improving your sleep, deliberate cold exposure,
deliberate heat exposure. We have a foundational fitness protocol
that describes a template routine that includes cardiovascular training
and resistance training with sets and reps all backed by science.
And all of which again is completely zero cost.
To subscribe, simply go to hubermanlab.com,
go to the menu tab up in the upper right corner,
scroll down a newsletter and provide your email.
And I should emphasize that we do not share your email
with anybody.
Thank you once again for joining me for today's discussion
with Dr. Charan Ranganath.
And last, but certainly not least,
thank you for your interest in science.