Huberman Lab - Dr. Charles Zuker: The Biology of Taste Perception & Sugar Craving

Episode Date: July 18, 2022

My guest this episode is Charles Zuker, Ph.D., Professor of Biochemistry, Molecular Biophysics and Neuroscience at Columbia University and an Investigator with the Howard Hughes Medical Institute. Dr.... Zuker is the world’s leading expert in the biology of taste, thirst and craving. His laboratory explores the mechanisms of taste perception, focusing on how our conscious and unconscious processing of specific foods and nutrients guide our actions and behaviors. We discuss the neural circuits of taste, the “gut-brain axis,” the basis of food cravings and the key difference between wanting (craving) and liking (perceiving) sugar. We also explore how taste perception relates to specific food satiety, thirst, to our emotions, and expectation. We also consider how sugar containing and highly-processed foods can hijack the natural balance of the taste and digestive systems. Dr. Zuker provides a true masterclass in the biology of taste and perception that ought to be of interest to anyone curious about how the brain works, our motivated behaviors and the neural, chemical perceptual aspects of the mind. For the full show notes, visit hubermanlab.com. Thank you to our sponsors AG1 (Athletic Greens): https://athleticgreens.com/huberman LMNT: https://drinklmnt.com/huberman Supplements from Momentous https://www.livemomentous.com/huberman Timestamps (00:00:00) Dr. Charles Zuker & Taste Perception (00:03:22) Sponsors: AG1, LMNT (00:08:35) Sensory Detection vs. Sensory Perception (00:11:48) Individual Variations within Perception, Color (00:16:20) Perceptions & Behaviors (00:20:19) The 5 Taste Modalities (00:26:18) Aversive Taste, Bitter Taste (00:28:00) Survival-Based & Evolutionary Reasons for Taste Modalities, Taste vs. Flavor (00:30:14) Additional Taste Modalities: Fat & Metallic Perception (00:34:02) Tongue “Taste Map,” Taste Buds & Taste Receptors  (00:39:34) Burning Your Tongue & Perception (00:42:54) The “Meaning” of Taste Stimuli, Sweet vs. Bitter, Valence (00:51:55) Positive vs. Negative Neuronal Activation & Behavior (00:56:16) Acquired Tastes, Conditioned Taste Aversion  (01:01:44) Olfaction (Smell) vs. Taste, Changing Tastes over One’s Lifetime (01:09:14) Integration of Odor & Taste, Influence on Behavior & Emotion (01:17:26) Sensitization to Taste, Internal State Modulation, Salt (01:24:05) Taste & Saliva: The Absence of Taste (01:28:10) Sugar & Reward Pleasure Centers; Gut-Brain Axis, Anticipatory Response (01:36:23) Vagus Nerve (01:43:09) Insatiable Sugar Appetite, Liking vs. Wanting, Gut-Brain Axis (01:52:03) Tool: Sugar vs. Artificial Sweeteners, Curbing Appetite (01:54:06) Cravings & Gut-Brain Axis  (01:57:30) Nutrition, Gut-Brain Axis & Changes in Behavior (02:01:53) Fast vs. Slow Signaling & Reinforcement, Highly Processed Foods  (02:10:38) Favorite Foods: Enjoyment, Sensation & Context (02:15:58) Zero-Cost Support, YouTube Feedback, Spotify & Apple Reviews, Sponsors, Momentous Supplements, Instagram, Twitter, Neural Network Newsletter Title Card Photo Credit: Mike Blabac Disclaimer

Transcript
Discussion (0)
Starting point is 00:00:00 Welcome to the Uberman Lab podcast where we discuss science and science-based tools for everyday life. I'm Andrew Uberman and I'm a professor of neurobiology and Ophthalmology at Stanford School of Medicine. Today my guest is Dr. Charles Zucker. Dr. Zucker is a professor of biochemistry and molecular biophysics and of neuroscience at Columbia University School of Medicine. Dr. Zucker is one of the world's leading experts in molecular biophysics and of neuroscience at Columbia University School of Medicine. Dr. Zucker is one of the world's leading experts in perception, that is how the nervous system converts physical stimuli in the world into events within the nervous system
Starting point is 00:00:35 that we come to understand as our sense of smell, our sense of taste, our sense of vision, our sense of touch, and our sense of hearing. Dr. Zucker's lab is responsible for a tremendous amount of pioneering and groundbreaking work in the area of perception. For a long time, his laboratory worked on vision, defining the very receptors that allow for the conversion of light into signals that the rest of the eye in the brain can understand. In recent years, his laboratory has focused mainly on the perception of taste, and indeed
Starting point is 00:01:04 his laboratory is responsible for discovering many of the taste receptors, leading to our perception of things like sweetness, sourness, bitterness, saltiness, and umami that is savouriness in food. Dr. Zucker's laboratory is also responsible for doing groundbreaking work on the sense of thirst, that is, how the nervous system determines whether or not we should ingest more fluid or reject fluids that are offered to us. A key feature of the work from Dr. Zucker's laboratory is that it bridges the brain and body. As you'll soon learn from today's discussion, his laboratory has discovered a unique
Starting point is 00:01:38 set of sugar sensing neurons that exist not just within the brain, but a separate set of neurons that sense sweetness and sugar within the body. And that much of the communication between the brain and body leading to our seeking of sugar is below our conscious detection. Dr. Zucker has received a large number of prestigious awards and appointments as a consequence of his discoveries in neuroscience. He is a member of the National Academy of Sciences, the National Academy of Medicine, and the American Association for the Advancement of Science.
Starting point is 00:02:06 He is also an investigator with the Howard Hughes Medical Institute. For those of you that are not familiar with the so-called HHMI, the Howard Hughes Medical Institute, Howard Hughes Medical Institute investigators are selected on an extremely competitive basis. And indeed, they have to come back every five years and prove themselves worthy of being reappointed as Howard Hughes investigators. Dr. Zucker has been a Howard Hughes
Starting point is 00:02:29 investigator since 1989. What all that means for you as a viewer and or listener of today's podcast is that you are about to learn about the nervous system and its ability to create perceptions, in particular the perception of taste and sugar sensing from the world's expert on perception and taste. I'm certain that by the end of today's podcast, you're not just going to come away with a deeper understanding of our perceptions and our perception of taste in particular, but indeed, you will come away with an understanding of how we create internal representations of
Starting point is 00:03:00 the entire world around us. And in doing so, how we come to understand our life experience. Before we begin, I'd like to emphasize that this podcast is separate from my teaching and research roles at Stanford. It is, however, part of my desire and effort to bring zero cost to consumer information about science and science-related tools to the general public. In keeping with that theme, I'd like to thank the sponsors of today's podcast. And now, for my discussion with Dr. Charles Zucker. Charles, thank you so much for joining me today.
Starting point is 00:03:28 My pleasure. I wanna ask you about many things related to taste. Our first sponsor is Athletic Greens. Athletic Greens is an all-in-one vitamin mineral probiotic drink. I've been taking Athletic Greens since 2012, so I'm delighted that they're sponsoring the podcast. The reason I started taking Athletic Greens and the reason I still take Athletic Greens
Starting point is 00:03:47 once or twice a day is that it helps me cover all of my basic nutritional needs. It makes up for any deficiencies that I might have. In addition, it has probiotics, which are vital for microbiome health. I've done a couple of episodes now on the so-called gut microbiome and the ways in which the microbiome interacts with your immune system, with your brain to regulate mood, and essentially with every biological system relevant to health throughout your brain and body. With athletic greens, I get the vitamins I need, the minerals I need, and the probiotics
Starting point is 00:04:17 to support my microbiome. If you'd like to try athletic greens, you can go to atlettagreens.com slash Huberman and claim a special offer. They'll give you five free travel packs, plus a year supply of vitamin D3 K2. There are a ton of data now showing that vitamin D3 is essential for various aspects of our brain and body health, even if we're getting a lot of sunshine. Many of us are still deficient in vitamin D3.
Starting point is 00:04:39 And K2 is also important because it regulates things like cardiovascular function, calcium in the body, and so on. Again, go to athleticgreens.com slash uberman to claim the special offer of the 5 free travel packs and the year supply of vitamin D3 K2. Today's episode is also brought to us by Element. Element is an electrolyte drink that has everything you need and nothing you don't. That means the exact ratios of electrolytes are an element, and those are sodium, magnesium, and potassium, but it has no sugar.
Starting point is 00:05:07 I've talked many times before on this podcast about the key role of hydration and electrolytes for nerve cell function, neuron function, as well as the function of all the cells and all the tissues and organ systems of the body. If we have sodium, magnesium, and potassium present in the proper ratios, all of those cells function properly, and all our bodily systems can be optimized. If the electrolytes are not present and if hydration is low, we simply can't think as well as we would otherwise. Our mood is off, hormone systems go off, our ability to get into physical action to engage
Starting point is 00:05:40 in endurance and strength, and all sorts of other things is diminished. So with element, you can make sure that you're staying on top of your hydration and that you're getting the proper ratios of electrolytes. If you'd like to try element, you can go to drinkelement. That's L-M-N-T dot com slash huberman, and you'll get a free element sample pack with your purchase. They're all delicious. So again, if you want to try element, you can go to elementlment.com slash huberman.
Starting point is 00:06:04 And gustatory perception, but maybe to start off and because you've worked on a number of different topics in neuroscience, not just taste, how do you think about perception? Or rather, I should say, how should the world and people think about perception, how it's different from sensation, and what leads to our experience of life in terms of vision, hearing, taste, etc.
Starting point is 00:06:29 So, you know, the brain is an extra ordinary organ that weights maybe 2% of your body mass. Yet it consumes anywhere between 25 to 30% of all of your energy and oxygen. And it gets transformed into a mind. And this mind changes the human condition. It changes, it transforms, you know, fear into courage, conformity into creativity, sadness into happiness. How, how, how the hell does that happen? Now, the challenge of the brain faces is that the world is made of real things. You know, this here is a glass, and this is a cord, and this is a microphone. But the brain is only made of neurons that only understand electrical signals. So how do you transform that reality into nothing that electrical signals that now need to represent the world. And that process is what we can operationally define as perception.
Starting point is 00:07:59 In the senses, let's say ol' factory, other taste vision, we can very straightforwardly separate detection from perception. Detection is what happens when you take a sugar molecule, you put it in your tongue, and then a set of specific cells, now sense that sugar molecule. That's detection. You haven't perceived anything yet. That is, yes, your cells, in your tongue, interacting with this chemical.
Starting point is 00:08:32 But now that cell gets activated and sends a signal to the brain, and now detection gets transformed into perception. And is trying to understand how that happens, that's been the maniacal drive of my entire career in neuroscience. How does the brain ultimately transform the detection into perception so that it can guide actions and behaviors? Does that make sense? Absolutely.
Starting point is 00:09:06 And is a very clear and beautiful description. At sort of high level question related to that, and then I think we can get into some of the intermediate steps. Yes. I think many people would like to know whether or not my perception of the color of your shirt is the same as your perception of the color of your shirt is the same as your perception. What an excellent question Am I okay to interrupt you as you're as I'm guessing what you're going all right very interruption is welcome on this
Starting point is 00:09:33 Podcast the audience will always penalize me for interrupting you and we'll never penalize you for interrupting I like the one way penalize now Given what I told you before, that the brain is trying to represent the world based in nothing but the transformation of these signals into electrical languages that now neurons have to encode and decode, it follows that your brain is different than my brain, and therefore it follows that the way that you're perceiving the world
Starting point is 00:10:13 must be different than mine, even when receiving the same sensory cues. And I'll tell you about an experiment. It's a simple experiment yet brilliant that demonstrates why we perceive the world, how we perceive the world different. So in the world of vision, as you know, well, well, no, we have three classes of photoreceptor neurons that sense three basic colors, red, blue and green. It blue, green and red, if we go from sure to long wavelength. And these three are sufficient to accommodate the full visible spectra. I wanna take three light projectors.
Starting point is 00:11:01 And I wanna project with one into a white screen, it red light and the other one green light. I'm going to overlap the two beams and on the screen there's the yellow. Okay, this is the superposition when you have two beams of red and green. And then I'm going to take a third projector and I'm going to put a filter that projects right next to that mix beam, a spectrally pure yellow. And I'm going to ask you to come to the red and green projectors and play with the intensity knobs so that you can match that yellow that you are projecting so that you can match that yellow that you are projecting to the spectrally pure next to it. Is this making sense?
Starting point is 00:11:48 Perfect answer. And I'm going to write down the numbers in those two volume intensity knobs. And then I'm going to ask the next person to do the same. And then I'm going to ask every person around this area of battery park in New York to do the same. And guess what? We're going to end up with thousands of different number combinations. Amazing.
Starting point is 00:12:14 So for all of us, it's yellow enough that we can use a common language. But for every one of us, that yellow is going to be every so slightly differently. And so I think that simple psychological experiment beautifully illustrates how we truly perceive the world differently. I love that example. And yet in that example, we know the basic elements from which color is created. If we migrate into a slightly different sense, we pick a hard one. Like sound, sound or a faction. Very hard then to do an experiment that will allow us to get that degree of granularity
Starting point is 00:13:01 and beautiful causality. We can show that A produces and leads to B. If I give you the smell of a rose, you can describe it to me. If I smell the same rose, I can describe it also. But I have no way whether the two of us are experiencing the same. But it's close enough that we can both pretty much say that it has the following enough features
Starting point is 00:13:32 or other determinants, but no question that your experience is different than mine. The fact that it's good enough for us to both survive, that your perception of yellow and my perception of yellow, at least up until now, is good enough for us both to survive. You got it. Raised as a thought about a statement made by a colleague of ours, Marcus Meister at Keltek. It's never been on this podcast, but in the review that I read, by Marcus at one point, he said,
Starting point is 00:14:06 that the basic function of perception is to divide our behavioral responses into the outcomes downstream of three basic emotional responses. Yum, I like it, yuck, I hate it, or meh, whatever. What do you think about, I'm not looking to establish a debate between you markets without markets here. I understand.
Starting point is 00:14:32 But what I like about that is that it seems like the, we know the brain is a very economical organ in some sense, despite its high metabolic demands. And this variation in perception from one individual to the next, at once seems like a problem, because we're all literally seeing different things. And yet, we function. We function well enough for most of us to avoid death and cliffs and eating poisons and so forth, and to enjoy some aspects of life, one hopes. So is there a general statement that we can make about the brain not just as a organ to generate perception, not just as an organ to keep us alive, but also an organ that is trying to batch our behaviors into general
Starting point is 00:15:20 categories of that. I think so, but again, I think the role of Marcus too. And I think he's right that broadly speaking, you could categorize a lot of behaviors falling into the true categories. And that's 100% likely to be the case for animals in the wild. likely to be the case for animals in the wild. Where, you know, the choices are not necessarily binary, but they're very unique and distinct. Yeah. Do I want to eat this? Do I want to kill that?
Starting point is 00:16:03 Do I want to go there or do I want to go here? We humans deviated from that world long ago and learned to experience life where we do things that we should not be doing. Some of us more than others. Exactly. You know, in my own world of taste. Yeah. The likelihood that an animal in the wild will enjoy eating something bitter. It's inconceivable. Yet we, you know, love Tonic water. We enjoy, We like living on the edge. We love enjoying experiences that makes us human. And that goes beyond that simple set of categories, which is Yammy, Yaki, who cares. And so I think it's not about palette, but I think it's overly reductionist for certainly
Starting point is 00:17:21 what we humans do. I agree, and since we're here in New York, I can say that the many options, the extensive variety of food, flora, and fauna in New York, explains a lot of the more nuanced behaviors that we observe. Let's talk about taste, because,
Starting point is 00:17:43 well, you've done extensive work in the field of vision and it's a topic that I love You could spend all day on taste is fascinating First of all, I'd like to know why you migrated from studying vision to studying taste and Perhaps in that description you could highlight us why we should think about and how we should think about this sense of taste you could highlight to us why we should think about and how we should think about the sense of taste. My goal is always been to understand, as I highlighted before, how the brain does its magic. What part do you wonder?
Starting point is 00:18:15 Ideally, I like to help contribute to understand all of it. How do you encode and decode emotions? How do you encode and decode emotions? How do you encode and decode memories and actions? How do you make decisions? How do you transform the textual inter perception? And the least goes on and on. But one of the key things in science, as you know, is ensuring that you always ask the right question
Starting point is 00:18:47 so that you have a possibility of answering it. Because if the question cannot be tractable or reduced to an experimental path that helps you resolve it, then we end up doing some really fun science, but not necessarily answering the important problem that we want to study. Make sense? All right.
Starting point is 00:19:12 From a first person perspective, yes. The hardest question, the most important question is, what question are you going to try and answer? You guys are right. And so, for example, I would love to understand the neural basis of empathy. It's a big market for that. 100%. But I wouldn't even know, I mean, at the molecular level, that's what we do. How do the circuits in your brain create that sense? I have not clue how to do it. I can come up with ways to think about it, but I like to understand what in your brain
Starting point is 00:19:50 makes someone a great philanthropist. What is the neural basis of love? I wouldn't even know where to begin. So if I want to begin to study these questions about brain function that can cover so many aspects of the brain, I need to choose a problem that affords me that window. But in a way that I can ask questions that give me answers. And among the senses that have the capacity of transforming the detection into perception of being stories memories of creating emolitions of giving you different actions and
Starting point is 00:20:35 perceptions as a function of the internal state. You know when your hungry things taste very differently than when you're sated. How? Why? When you taste something, you now remember this amazing meal you had with your first date. How does that happen? All right. So, if I want to begin to explore all of these things that the brain does, I have to choose a sensory system that affords some degree of simplicity in the way that the
Starting point is 00:21:11 input output relationships are put together. I mean, a way that still can be used to ask every one of these problems that the brain has to ultimately compute and code and decode. And what's remarkable about the taste system at the time that I began working on this is that nothing was known about the molecular basis of taste. We knew that we could taste what has been usually defined as the basic taste qualities. Sweet, sour, bitter, salty, and umami.
Starting point is 00:21:52 Umami is a Japanese word that means yummy, delicious, and that's nearly every animal species, the taste of amino acids. And in humans, it's mostly associated with the taste of MSG, monosodium glutamate, one amino acid in particular. Just by way of example, some foods that are rich in umami evoking stimulation. Seaweed, tomatoes, cheese. And it's a great flavor enhancer. It enriches our sensory experience. And so the beautiful thing of the system is that the lines of input are limited to five. You know, sweets are a bit salty on the mommy and each of them has a predetermined meaning. and each of them has a predetermined meaning. You are born like in sugar and dislike in bitter.
Starting point is 00:22:49 You have no choice. These are hard wire systems. But of course, you can learn to dislike sugar and to like bitters. But in the wild, let's take humans out of the equation. These are 100% predetermined. You're born with that specific valence value for each taste of sweet, umami, and low salt are attractive taste qualities. They evoke a pettity of responses. I wanna consume them. And bitter and sour are innately predetermined
Starting point is 00:23:36 to be aversive. Could I interrupt you just briefly and ask a question about that exact point. For something to be repetitive to and some mother taste to be repetitive to, and some mother tastes to be aversive, and for those to be hardwired, can we assume that the sensation of very bitter,
Starting point is 00:23:55 or of activation of bitter receptors in the mouth, activates a neural circuit that causes closing of the mouth, retraction of the tongue, and retraction of the body, and that the taste of something sweet might actually induce more licking. 100 percent, you got it. The activation of the receptors in the tongue that recognize sweet versus the ones that recognize bitter, activate an entire behavioral program. And that program that we can refer as a petitiveness or a version, it's composed of many different subroutines.
Starting point is 00:24:36 In the case of bitter, it's very easy to actually look at, see them happening in animals, because the first thing you do is you stop leaking, then you put an unhappy face, then you squint your eyes, and then you start guiding. And that entire thing happens by the activation of a bitter molecule in a bitter sense in cell in your tongue. It's incredible. Again, the magic of the brain, you know, how it's able to encode and decode this extraordinary actions and behaviors in response of nothing but a simple, very, you know, unique sensory stimuli. Now, let me say that this palette of five basic tastes accommodates all the dietary needs of the organism.
Starting point is 00:25:26 Sweet to ensure that we get the right amount of energy. Umami to ensure that we get proteins and that is essential nutrient. Salt, the three appetitive ones, to ensure that we maintain our electrolyte balance. Beater to prevent the ingestion of toxic, nauseous chemicals, nearly all beta-tasting things out in the wild are bad for you.
Starting point is 00:25:54 And sour, most likely to prevent the ingestion of spoil acid, fermented foods. And that's it. That is the palate that we deal with. Now, of course, there's a difference between basic taste and flavor. Flavor is the whole experience. Flavor is the combination of multiple tastes coming together, together with smell, with texture, with temperature, with the look of it that gives you what you and I would call the full sense or experience. But we scientists need to reduce the problem into its basic elements so we can begin to break it apart before we put it back together. So when we think about the sense of taste and we try to figure out how these lines of information go from your tongue to your brain
Starting point is 00:26:51 and how they signal and how they can integrate it and how they trigger all these different behaviors, we look at them as individual qualities. So we give the animal sweet or we give them a bitter, we give them sour, we avoid mixes. Because the first stage of discovery is to have that clarity as to what you're trying to extract so that you can hopefully, meaningfully, make a difference
Starting point is 00:27:20 by being able to figure out how easy that A goes to B to C and to D. Does he make sense? Yeah, almost like the primary colors to create the full array of the color spectrum. Exactly. Before I ask you about the first and second and third stages of taste and flavor perception, is there any idea that there may be more than five? There is, for example, what about fat?
Starting point is 00:27:47 I love fat too. And I love the texture of fat, especially if it's slightly burnt, like the in South America, when I visited Buenos Aires, I found that at the end of a meal they would take a steak, the trimming off the edge of the steak, burn it slightly, and then serve it back to me, and I thought that's disgusting, and then I tasted it, burn it slightly, and then serve it back to me. And I thought that's disgusting. And then I tasted it and it's delightful. It is. There's nothing quite like it.
Starting point is 00:28:12 This goes back to this notion before that we like to live on the edge. And we like to do things that we should not be doing, Andrew. But on the other hand, look at those muscles. The... The... I don't suggest anyone eat pure fat. But on the other hand, look at those muscles. The, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, the, problem that exists. Yeah, we'll talk about that in a little bit about the God brain access. I think it'll be important to cover it because it's the other side of the taste system.
Starting point is 00:28:54 And so, so missing taste, you know, one is fat, although like you clearly highlighted a lot of fat taste, in quotation marks, is really the feeling of fat rolling on your tongue. And so there is a compelling argument that a lot of what we call fat taste, it's really mechanosensory. It's somato-sensory cells, cells that are not responding to taste, but they're responding to mechanical stimulation of fat molecules rolling on the tongue that gives you that perception of fat.
Starting point is 00:29:39 I love the idea that there is a perception of fat, regardless of whether or not there's a dedicated receptor for fat. Mostly because it's making evoking sensations and imagery of the taste of slightly burnt fat. For example, and another one, you could argue it's metallic taste. I know exactly what it tastes like. If you ask me to explain it, I will have a hard time. You know, what, what are the palettes of that color that can allow me to define it?
Starting point is 00:30:12 I wouldn't be easy, but I know precisely what it tastes like. You know, take a penny put in your mouth and you know what it tastes like, yeah? Or blood. Or blood, that's another very good example. and you know what it tastes like, yeah? Or blood. Or blood, that's another very good example. And is there really a receptor for metallic taste,
Starting point is 00:30:30 or it's nothing but this magical combination of the activation of the existing lines? Think of it as lines of information, just separate lines. By the keys of a piano, yeah? Sweet sour beat, as old to Miami, you play the key, and you activate a one chord. And that one chord, in the case of a piano, yeah? Sweet sour beat, a salty umami, you play the key and you activate a one chord. And that one chord in the case of a piano leads to a note,
Starting point is 00:30:52 and in the case of taste, leads to an action and a behavior. But you play many of them together and something emerges that it's different than any one of the pieces. And it's possible that metallic, for example, represents the combination of the activity just in the right ratio. Makes sense. And it actually provides a perfect, your example, the piano provides a perfect segue for what I'd like to touch on next, which is if you would describe the sequence of neural events leading to a perceptual event of taste.
Starting point is 00:31:34 And I'm certain that somewhere in there, you will embed an answer to the question of whether or not we indeed have different taste receptors distributed in different locations on our tongue or elsewhere in the mouth. Yes. So let's start by the banking that all tale and myth. Who came up with that? There are many views, but the most prevalent is that there was an original drawing describing the sensitivity of the tongue to different tastes. So imagine I can take a Q-tip. This is a thought experiment, yeah? And I'm going to dip that Q-tip in salt and in quainine or something bitter and glucose as something sweet.
Starting point is 00:32:26 And I'm going to take that Q-tip, ask you to stick your tongue out and start moving around your tongue and ask you what do you feel. And then I'm going to change the concentration of the amount of salt or the amount of bitter, and as can I get some sort of a map of sensitivity to the different tastes? And the argument that has emerged is that there is a good likelihood that the data was simply mistranslated, as it was being drawn. And of course, that led to an entire industry.
Starting point is 00:33:06 This is the way you maximize your wine experience because now we're going to form the vessel that you're going to train from so that it acts maximally on the receptors which happen. All right. Now, there is no tongue map. All right. Now, there is no tongue map. All right? We have taste bats distributed in various parts of the tongue, so there is a map on the distribution of taste bats. But each taste bat has around a hundred taste receptor cells. And those taste receptor cells can be of five types, sweet sour, bittersalted, or umami. And for the most part, all taste buds have the
Starting point is 00:33:57 representation of all five taste qualities. Now, there's no question that there is a slight bias for some taste. Like, bitter is particularly enriched at the very back of your tongue. And there is a teleological basis for that, actually a biological basis for that. That's the last line of defense before you swallow something bad.
Starting point is 00:34:22 And so let's make sure that the very back of your tongue has plenty of these bad news receptors so that if they get activated, you can trigger a gagging reflex and get rid of this that otherwise may kill you. But the notion that all sweeties in the front and soldies on this side it's not real And they're go ahead. Oh, I was just gonna ask are there it's first of all Thank you for dispelling that myth Yes, and we will propagate that information as far and wide as we can because I think that's the number one myth related to taste
Starting point is 00:35:03 The other one is, are there taste receptors anywhere else in the mouth? For instance, on the lips? The palate, the palate, not the lips. So it's in the far range at the very back of the oral cavity, the tongue and the palate. And the palate is very rich in sweet receptors. I'll have to pay attention to this
Starting point is 00:35:24 the next time I eat something sweet. When you pull it up, now, the important thing is that, you know, after the receptors for these five detectors, the molecules that sense sweets our bit as well to mommy, these are receptors proteins found on the surface of taste receptor cells that interact with these chemicals. Once they interact, then they trigger the cascade of events by chemical events inside the cell, that now sends an electrical signal that says,
Starting point is 00:35:59 there is sweet here or there is salt here. Now having these receptors and my laboratory identify the receptors for all five basic taste classes, sweet bit result to mommy and most recently sour, now completing the palette, you can now use these receptors to really map where are they found in the tongue in a very rigorous way. This is no longer about using a q-tip and trying to figure out what you're feeling, but rather what you have in your tongue. This is not a guess, this is now
Starting point is 00:36:37 a physical map that says the sweet receptors are found here, the bitter are found here, and when you do that, you find that in fact every taste bad throughout your oral cavity has receptors for all of the basic taste glasses. Amazing. And as it turns out, and I'm sure you'll tell us important in terms of thinking about how the brain computes in codes, the decodes, this thing we call taste. Yes. I'm going to inject a quick question that I'm sure is on many people's minds before we get back into the biological circuit,
Starting point is 00:37:13 which is many people including myself are familiar with the experience of drinking a sip of tea or coffee that is too hot. And burning my tongue is the way I would describe it. Horrible. Horrible, and then disrupting my sense of taste for some period of time afterward. Yes. When I experience that phenomenon, that unfortunate phenomenon,
Starting point is 00:37:34 have I destroyed taste receptors that regenerate, or have I somehow used temperature to distort the function of the circuit so that I no longer taste the way I did before. Excellent question and the answer is both. It turns out that your taste receptors only leave for around two weeks. And this by the way makes sense because here you have an organ, the tongue, that is continuously exposed to everything you could range from the nicest to the most horrible possible things.
Starting point is 00:38:10 You see your imagination. And so you need to make sure that these cells are always coming back in a way that I can re-experience the world in the right way. And there are other organs that have the same underlying logic. Your intestines are the same way. Amazing. Again, they're receiving everything that you ingest. God forbid what's there. From the spiciest, you know, to the most horrible tastings for the most delicious.
Starting point is 00:38:46 And again, those intestinal cells whose role is to ultimately take all these nutrients and bring them into the body also renewal in a very, very fast cycle. All factory neurons in your nose is the other example. So then A, yes, you're burning a lot of yourselves and it's over for those. The good news is that they're going to come back. But we know that when you burn yourself with tea, they come back within 20 minutes, 30 minutes an hour. And this results are not renewing that in that timeframe, but they're not listening to your needs. They have their own internal clock. And so you are really affecting your damage in them in a
Starting point is 00:39:44 way that they can recover. And then they come back and you also damage your somatosensory cells. These are the cells that feel things, not taste things. And then, you know, you wait half an hour or so, and then my goodness, thank God, it's back to normal. And most of the time, I don't even notice the transition realizing as you tell me. And later I'll ask you about the relationship between odor and taste. But as a next step along the circuit,
Starting point is 00:40:19 let's assume I ingest some, let's keep it simple, a sweet taste. Let's make it even simpler, but at the same time, perhaps more informative. Let's compare and contrast sweet and bitter as we follow their lines from the tongue to the brain. So the first thing is that the tool evoke diametrically opposed behaviors.
Starting point is 00:40:45 If we have to come up with two sensory experience that represent polar opposites, it will be Sweden-Bitter. There are not two colors that represent polar opposites because you can say black and white, they're polar opposites. One detects only one thing,
Starting point is 00:40:58 the other one detects everything. But they don't evoke different behaviors. Even the political parties have some over now. Sweden and Bitter are the two opposite ends of the sensory spectrae. Now a taste can be defined by two features. Again, I'm a reductionist. I'm reducing it in a way that I think it's easier to follow the signal.
Starting point is 00:41:28 And the two features are its quality and its valence. And valence with a little V, that's what we say in Spanish with a V, means the value of that experience, or in this case of that stimuli. And you take sweet, sweet has a quality, an identity, and that's what you and I will refer to as the taste of sweet. We know exactly what it tastes like, but sweet also has a positive valence, which makes it incredibly attractive and repetitive. But it's attractive and repetitive, as I'll tell you in a second, independent of its identity and quality. In fact, we have been able to engineer animals where we completely remove the valence
Starting point is 00:42:27 from the stimuli. So these animals can taste sweet, can recognize it as sweet, but it's no longer attractive. It's just one more chemical stimuli. And that's because the identity and the valence are encoded in two separate parts of the brain. In the case of Beecher, again, it has, on the one hand, its identity, its quality. And you know exactly what Beecher tastes like. I can taste it now even as you describe it. But it also has a valence, and that's a negative valence, because it evokes a variety of behaviors. Are we on?
Starting point is 00:43:11 Absolutely. And it comes to mind, I remember telling some kids recently that we're gonna go get ice cream, and it was interesting, they looked up and they started smacking their lip. Like, you know, they'll actually vote anticipatory response.
Starting point is 00:43:23 Absolutely. When we talk about the Godbrain, maybe we'll get there. So then the signals, if we follow now these two lines, they're really like two separate keys at the two ends of this keyboard. You press one key and you activate this chord, so you activate the sweet cells throughout your oral cavity and the all-converged into a group of sweet neurons in the next station which is still outside the brain is one of the taste ganglia these are the neurons that innervate your tongue and the oral cavity where do they sit approximately? There's one there.
Starting point is 00:44:05 Yeah, right here around there. The lymph nodes more or less. You got it. And there are two main ganglia that innervate the vast majority of all taste the bots in the oral cavity. And then from there, that sweet signal goes onto the brainstem.
Starting point is 00:44:25 The brainstem is the entry of the body into the brain. And there are different areas of the brainstem and there are different groups of neurons in the brainstem. And there's this unique area in a unique topographically defined location in the rostral side of the brain stem that receives all of the taste input. A very dense area of the brain. A very rich area of the brain, exactly. And from there, the switch signal goes to this other area higher up on the brain stem. And then it goes through a number of stations where that switch signal goes from sweet neuron to sweet neuron to sweet neuron to eventually get to your cortex.
Starting point is 00:45:20 And once it gets to your taste cortex, that's where meaning is imposed into that signal. It's then, and only then, this is what the data suggests, that now you can identify this as a sweet stimuli. And how quickly does that all happen? Um, you know, the timescale of the nervous system, it's fast. Yeah. And we're in less than a second. Yeah. Absolutely. Yeah. I rarely mistake bitter for sweet. Yeah. Maybe with respect to people and my own poor judgment, but, but not with respect to taste. Yeah. It's, and in fact, we can demonstrate this because we can stick
Starting point is 00:46:06 electrodes at each of these stations conceptually. And we can stimulate the tongue. And then we can record the signals pretty much time log to stimulus delivery. You deliver the stimuli and within a fraction of a second, you see now their response in these following stations. Now it gets to the cortex, here, and now in there you impose meaning to that taste. There is an area of your brain that represents the taste of sweet in taste cortex and a different area that represents the taste of bitter. In this
Starting point is 00:46:46 sense there is a topographic map of this taste quality this inside your brain. Now we're going to do a thought experiment, all right? Now if this group of neurons in your cortex really represents the sense of sweet. And this added different group of neurons in your brain represents the taste, the perception of bitter. Then we should be able to do two things. First, I should be able to go into your brain somehow silence those neurons, find a way to prevent them from being activated and I can give you all the sweet you want and you'll never know that you're tasting sweet. And conversely, I should be able to go into your brain, come up with a way to
Starting point is 00:47:39 activate those neurons while I'm giving you absolutely nothing. And you're going to think that you're getting that full person. And that's precisely what we have done, and that's precisely what you get. This, of course, is in the brain of mysy, but presumably in humans it would work. Absolutely the same. zero doubt. I have no question. So, this attests to two important things. The first, to the predetermined nature of the sense of taste, because it means I can go to these parts of your brain
Starting point is 00:48:19 in the absence of any stimuli and have you throw the full behavioral experience. In fact, when we activate in your cortex this bitter neurons the animal can start gagging. But it's drinking only water. But the animal thinks that it's getting a bit estimular. It's amazing. And so, and the second, just to finish the line so that it doesn't sound like it teaches two things and then I only give you one lesson. Is that, you know, it substanceiates
Starting point is 00:48:59 this capacity of the brain to segregate, to separate in this notes of action, the representation of these two diametrically opposed pursuits, which is sweet, for example, versus bitter. The reason I say amazing, and that is also amazing, is the following. You told us earlier, and you're absolutely correct, of course, that the end of the day, whether or not it's one group of neurons over here and another group of neurons over there, which is the way it turns out to be. Electrical activity is the generic common language of both sets of neurons.
Starting point is 00:49:39 So that raises the question for me of whether or not those separate sets of neurons are connected to areas of the brain that create this sense of valence, or whether or not they're simply connected, excuse me, to sets of neurons that evoke distinct behaviors of moving towards and inhaling more and licking or aversive. Are we essentially interpreting our behavior and our micro responses, or are micro responses in our behaviors, the consequence of the person. Excellent, excellent question. So first the answer is they go into an area of the brain where valence is imposed and that area is known as the amygdala. The sweet neurons go to a different area than the bitter neurons.
Starting point is 00:50:28 Now, I want to do a thought experiment because I think your audience might appreciate this. Let's say I activate this group of neurons, and the animal increases leaking, and I'm activating the sweet neurons. So that's expected because now it's tasting this water as it was sugar. Now this is Moses transforming water into wine. In this case, we're going to, and today it's Passover. So then it's an appropriate example.
Starting point is 00:50:58 We're transforming it into sweet. Yeah. But how do I know? How do I know that activate in them is evoking a positive feeling inside, a goodness, a satisfaction, I love it. Versus, I'm just increasingly licking, which is the other option because all we're seeing
Starting point is 00:51:21 is that the animal is licking more and we're trying to infer that that means that it's feeling something really good versus you know what? That piano line is going back straight into the tongue and all is doing is forcing it to move faster. Well, we can actually separate this by doing experiments that allow us to fundamentally distinguish them. Imagine the following experiment. I'm going to take the animal and I'm going to put them inside a box that has two sides. And the two sides have features that make them different. One has yellow little toys, the other one has green toys.
Starting point is 00:52:01 One has little, you know, black stripes, the other one has blue stripes. So the animal can tell the two halves. I take the mouse, put them inside this arena, this play arena, and it will explore and pots around both sides with equal frequency. And now what I'm going to do is I'm going to activate this neurons, the sweet neurons, every time the animal is on the side with the yellow stripes. And if that is creating a positive internal state, what would the animal now want to do? It will want to stay on this side with the yellow stripes. There is no leak in here. The animal is not extending its strong every time I'm activating this noise. This is known as a place preference test.
Starting point is 00:53:00 And its generally used is just one form of many different tests to demonstrate that the activation of a group of neurons in the brain is imposing, for example, a positive versus a negative valence. Whereas if I do the same thing by activating the bitter neurons, the animal will actively want now to stay away from the side where these neurons are being activated. And that's precisely what you see. And that's precisely what we see. Many people, including myself, are familiar with the experience of going to a restaurant,
Starting point is 00:53:41 eating a variety of foods, and then fortunately it doesn't happen that often, but then feeling very sick. I learned coming up in neuroscience that this is one strong example of one trial learning that from that point on, it's not the restaurant or the waitress or the waiter or the date, but it's my notion of it had to have been the shrimp. That leads me to then want to avoid shrimp in every context, maybe even shrimp powder. You got it for a very long time. I can imagine all the evolutionsarily adaptive reasons why this such a phenomenon would
Starting point is 00:54:19 exist. Do we have any concept of where in this pathway that exists? We do. We know actually a significant amount at a general level. In fact, more than shrimp oysters are even a more dramatic example, do you? One bad oyster. It's all you need to be driven away for the next six months. I think because the texture alone is something that one learns to overcome. I actually really enjoy the oysters. I despise muscles, despise shrimp, not the animal, but the taste.
Starting point is 00:54:54 And yet oysters for some reason, I'm yet to have a bad experience. It's like, it's like, oony by the way. You know, texture is hard to get over, but once you get over, it's delicious. That's what they tell me. We were both in San Diego at one point and I'll give a plug to Sushi Ota is kind of the famous good also. Oh my goodness. And they have amazing Uni and I've tried it twice and it's a I'm over to it. It's somehow the texture outweighs any kind of the deliciousness that people report. It's a very acquired taste. It's like beer. If you, I grew up in Chile,
Starting point is 00:55:26 that's where the accent comes from in case anyone wonder. And you know what, at the time I came here to graduate school, I was 19, two old, to, you know, over time, I have a Chilean accent. So here I am, 40 years, 50 years late, not quite. 40 plus. And I still sound like I just came off the boat So in Chile you don't drink beer when you're young you drink wine, you know, Chile is a huge wine producer
Starting point is 00:55:56 So when I came to the US all of my you know classmates, you know we're drinking beer Because they you know they had finished college where they were all you know we were drinking and you know, we're drinking beer because they, you know, they had finished college where they were all, you know, we've been drinking and, you know, graduate school, you're working 18 hours a day every day the way they, you know, relax, let's go and have some beers. And beer is cheaper and beer is cheap and we were being clearly underpaid. Yeah, yeah. I couldn't do it. It's an acquired taste. It was too late by then. And here I am, you know, 60 plus. And if you take all the beer I've drunk in my entire life, I would say they add to less than an eight ounce glass of water. Impressive. Well, your health is probably better for it. I'm not sure. Your physical health.
Starting point is 00:56:47 So you know it goes back to you know acquire taste. This is the connection to uni and to oysters. Now going back to to the one trial learning. You know this is the great thing about our brains. Certain things we need to repeat a hundred times to learn them. Hello operator, can I have the phone number for sushi otta please? And then she'll give it to you over the phone, at least in the old days. And then you need to repeat it to yourself over and over and over the next minute so you can dial sushi otta. And five minutes later, it's gone. That's what we call working memory.
Starting point is 00:57:26 Then there is the short-term memory. We park our car, and if we're lucky by the end of the day, we remember where it is. And then there is the long-term memory. We remember the birthdays of every one of our children for the rest of our lives. Well, there are events that a single event is so traumatic, that it activates the circuits in a way that it's a one trial learning.
Starting point is 00:57:53 And the taste system is literally at the top of that food chain. And there is a phenomenon known as condition taste aversion. You can pair an attractive stimuli with a really bad one. And you can make an animal begin to vehemently dislike, for example, sugar. And that's because you've conditioned the animals to now be averse to this otherwise Nice taste because it's been associated with malaise and when you do that now you could begin to ask why does change In the signal as it travels from the tongue to the brain in a normal animal versus an animal where you have now transformed sweet
Starting point is 00:58:46 from being attractive to being aversive. And this is the way now you begin to explore how the brain changes the nature, the quality, the meaning of a stimuli as a function of its state. I have a number of questions related to that, all of which relate to this idea of context. Because you mentioned before that flavor is distinct from taste because flavor involves smell,
Starting point is 00:59:17 texture, temperature, and some other features. Uni, see urchin being a good example. I can sense the texture. It actually, yeah, I won't describe what it reminds me of for various reasons. The ability to place context into a perception or rather to insert a perception into context is so powerful. And there's an element of kind of mystery about it, but if we start to think about some of the more nuanced that we like to live at the edge, as you say, how many different tastes on the taste dial to go back to your analogy earlier, the color
Starting point is 01:00:01 dial, do you think that there could be for something as fixed as bitter? So for instance, I don't think I like bitter taste, but I like some fermented foods that seem to have a little bit of sour and have a little bit of that briny flavor. How much plasticity do you think there is there? And in particular, across the lifespan, because I think one of the most salient examples of this is that kids don't seem to like certain vegetables, but they all are hardwired to like sweet taste.
Starting point is 01:00:31 And yet you could also imagine that one of the reasons why they may eventually grow to incorporate vegetables is because of some knowledge that vegetables might be better for them. So, is there a change in the receptors, the distribution, the number, the sensitivity, et cetera, that can explain the transition from wanting to avoid vegetables to being willing to eat vegetables, simply in childhood to early development.
Starting point is 01:00:57 So I'm gonna take the question slightly differently, but I think it would illustrate the point. And I'm gonna just use the difference between the olfactory system and the taste system to make the point. Taste system, five basic palettes. Switzerland, our bit of salt and umami, each of them has a predetermined identity. We know exactly what the, and valence.
Starting point is 01:01:22 These are attractive, these are aversive. In the olfactory system, what to and valence. These are attractive, these are aversive. Indulphactory system, it's claimed that we can smell millions of different others. Yet, for the most part, none of them have an innate predetermined meaning. Indulphactory system, meaning is imposed by learning and experience. Even the smell of smoke. So I'm going to give you, I'm going to make it differently. There are a handful of the millions of others that were claimed that you could immediately
Starting point is 01:01:57 tell me these are aversive and these are attractive. Vulner. a very simple and this are attractive. Vomit. So vomit, it's not correct because I can assure you that there are cultures and societies where things which are far less appealing than vomit do not evoke an aversive reaction. Really? Really. Solve for would be maybe a universal.
Starting point is 01:02:20 I'm not talking pharaoh months, okay? Pharaoh months are in a different category that trigger innate responses. But nearly every other is afforded meaning by learning and experience. And that's why you like broccoli. And I despise broccoli because I remember my mother forcing me to eat broccoli. So same sensory experience. All right. This, this, a combo that is two important things. In the case of taste, you have neurons at every station that
Starting point is 01:02:54 are for sweet, for sour, for bitter, for salty, and umami. It's only five classes. So it's not going to take a lot of your brain. If we can, in fact, smell a million others, and everyone else of others had to have predetermined meaning, there's not going to be enough brain just to accommodate that one sense. And so evolution in its infinite wisdom evolved a system where you put together a pathway and a cortex, or a factory cortex, where you have the capacity to associate every other in a specific context that now gives it the meaning.
Starting point is 01:03:39 Now let's go back to the original question then. So other than clearly plastic, mega plastic because it's fundamental basis and neural organization. But taste, we just told you that, you know, predetermined hard wire. But predetermined hard wire doesn't mean that it's not modulated by learning or experience, it only means that you are born like in sweet and dislike in bitter. And we have many examples of plasticity, beer being one example. So why do we learn to love beer is in a coffee? It's because it has an associated gain to the system, and that gain to the system, that positive valence that emerges out of that negative signal is sufficient to create that positive association. And in the case of beer, of course, it's alcohol.
Starting point is 01:04:43 The feeling good that we get after is more than sufficient to say, I want to have more of this. In the case of coffee, of course, it's caffeine activating a whole group of neurotransmitter systems that give you that high associated with coffee. So yes, this taste system is changeable, it's malleable, and is subjected to learning and experience. But I like the olfactory system is restricted in what you could do with it because its goal is to allow you to get nutrients and survive.
Starting point is 01:05:20 The goal of the olfactory system is very different. It's being used not in our case, but in every admiral species, to identify friend versus foe, to identify mate, to identify ecological niches that want to be in. So it plays a very broad role. That then requires that it be set up, organized, and function in a very different type of context. Tase is about, can we get the nutrients we need to survive? And can we ensure that we are attracted to the ones we need?
Starting point is 01:05:59 And we are versed to the ones that are going to kill us. And being over this implicit and reductionist, but I think it illustrates a huge difference between these two chemosensoresist. I don't think you've been overly simplistic. I think it illustrates the key and tractable nature of this system and the way you've approached it. And I think it's important for people to hear that. Because everybody, as we are mystified
Starting point is 01:06:25 with empathy and love, et cetera. So in fairness to that, I'm going to ask a sort of high level question or abstract question. So it's based on a conversation I had with a former girlfriend where we're talking about chemistry between individuals. Yes. Very complicated topic on the one hand. But on the other hand, quite simple in that certain people for whatever reason evoke a tremendous sense of arousal for lack of a better word between two people, one would hope.
Starting point is 01:07:01 At least for some period of time. I didn't know at least what least that kind of a podcast. No, well, the reason I... But this has to do with taste because she said something, I think in part to maybe irritate me a bit, but we were commenting not about our own experience of each other, but of someone that she was now very excited about, we're on good terms.
Starting point is 01:07:24 Uh-huh. And she said, what do you think it is of someone that she was now very excited about. We're on good terms. Uh-huh. And she said, what do you think it is this thing of chemistry? So maybe she was trying to, you know. Warren you. Well what's coming. Warren me what's coming. And she said, I have a feeling something about it is in smell.
Starting point is 01:07:40 And something about it is actually in taste. Literally the taste of somebody's breath. That's the way she described it. And I thought that it was a very interesting example for a number of reasons, but in particular because it gets to the merging of odor and taste, but also to the idea that, of course, the context of a new relationship, I'm assuming that, and in fact, they're both attractive. People, et cetera, there's a whole context there, but I've had the experience of the odor of somebody's
Starting point is 01:08:13 breath being aversive, not because I could identify it as aversive. Because you didn't like that. But because it just didn't like it. But that's because you also see it with others. That trigger that negative aversey reaction, by the way. Absolutely. There are certain perfumes to me that are aversive. You got it. And there are other sense in, can recall sense of skin, of foods, et cetera, that are immensely repetitive.
Starting point is 01:08:48 So I've experienced both sides of this equation myself, and she was describing this, and to me, more than tasting wine, which is the typical example, where people inhale it and then they drink it, to me, this seems like something that more people might be able to relate to, that certain things and people smell delicious.
Starting point is 01:09:08 Even mothers describing the smell of their babies. The part where you're, right? I'm mother. Of course. I mean, you know, our own babies when they're next. That's the magic out place. The neck. The back of their neck.
Starting point is 01:09:19 There you go. Oh my goodness. I have a grandchild now, so I know exactly what Rio, that's his name, smells like. Okay, so more beautiful examples, always more fun to think about the beautiful, positive, repetitive examples. The smell of the back of your grandson's neck. Yes.
Starting point is 01:09:36 I mean, you couldn't, you could get more specific than that, but not a lot more. So what is going on in terms of the combination of odor and taste given that these two systems are so different? Yes. And they come together. Ultimately, there is a place in the brain where they come together to integrate the two into what we would call, you know, that sensory experience. And I'll tell you an experiment that you could do, that demonstrates this. I think in, I think it's good for the, you know, for, for your audience, you have to get a sense of how we approach these problems so that we can get in a meaningful scientific answer.
Starting point is 01:10:21 So we know where the olfactory cortex is in the brain. We know where the taste cortex is in the brain. They're in two different places. We can go to each of these two cortices, put color traces, we put green in one, we put red in the added, and we see where the colors go to. That's a reflection of where those neurons are projection,
Starting point is 01:10:45 a projection tool into their next targets. Once they get the signal, where do they send the signal to? And then we reason that if other entities come together somewhere in the brain, we should find an area that now it's getting red and green color. And we found such an area that now it's getting red and green color. And we found such an area. And next we anticipate, we hypothesize that maybe this is the area in the brain of the mouse, corresponding area in the brain of humans, that integrates other and taste. It's known this term normally uses multi-sensory integration.
Starting point is 01:11:28 And if this is true, we could do the following experiment. We can train a mouse to click sweet. And if they guess correctly that that is supposed to be sweet, they should go now to the right port to the right side to get a water reward. If they go to the left when it was sweet, then they are incorrect and they get no reward and they actually get a timeout. Now, the mice are thirsty, so they're very motivated to perform. If you repeat this task 100 times, 100 trials, incredibly, now this animal learn to recognize the suite and execute the right action.
Starting point is 01:12:20 By their action, we now are being told what that animal is tasting. We can make it more interesting and we can give them sweet and bitter and say if it's sweet go to the right and it's bitter go to the left. And after you train them this mice with 90% accuracy will tell you when you randomize now the stimuli, what was sweet and what was bitter? We can now do the same experiment But now mix tastes with other And say if you got
Starting point is 01:12:57 other alone Go to the right or push this lever in Mice if you get taste alone go to this other part or push this lever in mice. If you get taste alone, go to this other part or push this other lever. And if you get the two together, do this something else. And if you train the mice, the mice are able now to report back to you when it's sensing taste alone, order alone, or the mix. Make sense? Yeah, make sense. Now, we can go to the brain of this mice
Starting point is 01:13:29 and go to this area that we now and cover, discover as being the site of multi-sensor integration between taste and other and silence it, prevent it from being activated experimentally. If that area really represented the integration of these two, the animals should still be able to recognize the tasealon,
Starting point is 01:13:55 they still should be able to recognize the other alone, but should be incapable now to recognize the mix. Exactly as predicted, that's exactly what you get. All right? The brain is basically a series of engineering circuits. Complex, you got it. And our task is to figure out how can we extract this amazing architecture of these circuits in a way that we can begin to uncover the mysteries of the brain? And why certain people's breath tastes so good and other people's not so good? So I never answered that, but I told you how we can figure out where in the brain is happening.
Starting point is 01:14:46 As we've been having this discussion, I thought a few times about similarities to the visual system or differences to the visual system. The visual system, there are a couple of phenomena that I wonder if they also exist in the TAS system. In the visual system, we know, for instance, that if you look at something long enough and activate the given receptors long enough, that object will actually disappear. We offset this with little micro-eye movements, et cetera. But the principle is a fundamental one. This habituation or desensization, everyone seems to call it something different. But you get the idea, of course. In the taste system, I'm certainly familiar with eating something very, very sweet for the
Starting point is 01:15:26 first time in a long time, and it tastes very sweet. But a few more licks, a few more bites, and now it tastes not as sweet. With olfaction, I'm familiar with the odor in a room I don't like or I like, and then it disappearing. So, similar phenomena. Where does that occur? And can you imagine a sort of system by which people could leverage that? Because I do think that most people are interested in eating not more sugar, but less sugar. I think we have better ways to approach that. And we can transition from taste into these other circuits that makes sugar so extraordinarily
Starting point is 01:16:10 impossible not to consume. Impossible. Exactly. So where does this sensitize scene happens? That's the term that we use it. the sensitize in happens, that's the term that we use it. And it's, I think, happening at multiple stations. It's happening at the receptor level, i.e. the cells in your tongue that are sensing that sugar. As you activate this receptor and it's trigger and activity after activity, as you activate this receptor and its triggering activity after activity after activity, eventually you exhaust the receptor. Again, I'm using terms which are extraordinarily
Starting point is 01:16:53 loose. Let's say that the discussion, the receptor gets to a point where it undergoes a set of changes, chemical changes, where it now signals far less efficiently, or it even gets removed from the surface of the cell. And now what will happen is that the same amount of sugar will trigger far less of a response. And that is a huge side of this modulation. And then the next, I believe, is the integrated, again, loss of signaling that happens by continuous activation of the circuit at each of these different neural stations. You know, there is from the chunk to the ganglia, from the ganglia to the first station and the brainstem, a second station and the brainstem to the thalamus, then to the cortex. So there are
Starting point is 01:17:53 multiple steps that this signal is traveling. Now, you might say, why, this is a label line, why do you need to have so many stations? And that's because the test system is so important to ensure that you get what you need to survive that it has to be subjected to modulation by the internal state and each of these nodes provides a new side to give it plasticity and modulation not necessarily to change the way that something tastes, but to ensure that you consume more or less or differently of what you need. I'm going to give you one example of how the internal state changes the way the taste system works. Solved is very repetitive at low concentrations, and that's because we need it.
Starting point is 01:18:48 Our electrolyte balance requires solved. Every one of the neurons uses solved as the most important of the ions, you know, with potassium to ensure that you can transfer these electrical signals within and between neurons. But at high concentrations, let's say ocean water is incredibly aberacive. And we all know this because we go into the ocean and then when you get in your mouth, it's not that great. However, if I sold the preview and we can do this in experimental models quite readily. Now this incredibly high concentration of salt, one molar sodium chloride, becomes amazingly repetitive and attractive. What's going on in here? Your tongue is telling you this is horrible, but your brain is telling you, I don't care,
Starting point is 01:19:47 you need it. And this is what we call the modulation of the tail system by the internal state. And presumably, if one is hungry enough, even Uni will taste good. You can't do it. You can't write on the money. No, no, this is exactly correct. Or if you're thirsty and hungry, you suppress hunger so that you don't waste water molecules in digestion food.
Starting point is 01:20:17 Why? Because if you're thirsty and you have no water, you will die within a week or so. But you can go on a week or so. But you can go on a hunger strike as long as you have water for months because you're going to eat up all your energy reserves. Water is a different story. So you could see that there are multiple layers at which the test system that guides our drive and our motivation to consume the nutrients
Starting point is 01:20:49 we need has to be modulated in response to the internal state. And of course, internal state itself has to be modulated by the external world. And so that I think is a reason why what could otherwise would have been an incredibly simple system from the tongue to the cortex in one yes, wire, it's not. Because you have to ensure that, you know, each step you give the system that level of flexibility or what we call neuroscience plasticity. I think we're headed into the gut, but I have a question that has just been on my mind for a bit now, because I was drinking this water, and it has essentially no taste. Is there any kind of signal for the absence of taste, despite having something in the mouth. And here is why I ask. What I'm thinking about is saliva.
Starting point is 01:21:48 And while it's true that if I eat a lot of very highly palatable foods, that does change how I experience more bland foods. I must confess, when I eat a lot of these highly processed foods, I don't particularly like them. I tend to crave healthier foods, but that's probably for contextual reasons about nutrients, etc. But I could imagine an experiment where you see a taste of no taste. Right. There are taste of no taste because in the visual system there is. Right. You close the eyes and you start getting increases in activity in the visual system as opposed to decreases, which often surprises people. But there are reasons for that
Starting point is 01:22:24 because everything is about signal to noise, signal to background. And it's a good question. I can tell you that most of our work is trying to focus on how the taste system works. Not how it doesn't work. Well, but I know you're being playful and I knew when inviting you here today, I was setting myself up for it. I actually, on a different, we're trying to learn things.
Starting point is 01:22:46 Yeah, I know, however. All right, listen, I was weaned in this system of, and I'll say it here for the second, actually I recorded a podcast recently with a very prominent podcast, a Lex Friedman podcast, and I made reference to the so-called New York neuroscience mafia.
Starting point is 01:23:02 I won't say whether or not we are sitting in the presence of the New York neuroscience mafia member, but in any event, I know the sorts of ribbing that they provide. For those listening, this is the kind of hazing that happens, benevolent hazing in academia. I'm the target. Of course, it's a sign of love. He's going to tell me that. And it's always about the science in the end. Right. But it's an interesting question. Look, I don't know the answer.
Starting point is 01:23:31 And I don't even know how I would explore it in a way that you will rigorously teach me. But here, let me tell you why I'm asking. And then I'll offer an experiment. Teach me But Well here let me tell you why I'm asking and then I'll offer an experiment. Yes, that I'd love to see someone In here to do I'm thinking about saliva. Yes, which it's no no, but that we know that we can figure it out But the question is whether or not the saliva in a fed state is Distinct from the saliva in an unfed state such that it
Starting point is 01:24:07 modulates the sensitivity of the research. That experiment has been done. It has been done. And so the answer is no. It's no. And the way you could do the experiment is because we use artificial saliva. There's such a thing. I know there's artificial tears.
Starting point is 01:24:21 No, no, we, I don't mean that you go to Walgreens that you get. I mean, we in my laboratory, we know the composition of saliva. And so you can make such a thing. And, and, and you can take, you know, taste cells in culture or in a tongue where you wash it out of. And then you can apply artificial saliva. And what happens is that the system is being engineered to desensitize to become a agnostic for saliva to become invisible.
Starting point is 01:24:57 And there is no difference on the state of the animal. This is the reason to do experiments. Yeah, absolutely. So it doesn't defeat any grand hypothesis. Yeah. It's just a pure curiosity. state of the animal. This is the reason to do experiments. Yeah, absolutely. So it doesn't defeat any grand hypothesis. It's just a pure curiosity. So do you know that curiosity killed the cat? Yeah, I do.
Starting point is 01:25:14 But saves the career of a scientist. Every single time that drives us absolutely every single story of our lives. Exactly. Okay. So if it's not saliva and apparently it is not, what about internal state? And what aspects of this internal milieu are relevant because there's autonomic, there's a sleep and awake, there's stress. One of the questions that I got from hundreds of people when I solicited questions in advance of this episode was, why do I crave sugar when I'm stressed, for instance? And that could be contextual, but what are the basic?
Starting point is 01:25:49 Because it makes us feel good, by the way, we'll get to that. That's the answer. Sude. It activates what I'm going to generically refer to as reward, pleasure centers, in a way that it dramatically changes our internal state. This is in a why do we eat a galoma ice cream when we're very depressed?
Starting point is 01:26:13 In fact, this is a good way to go into this, an entirely different world of the body telling your brain what you need. And in important things like sugar and fat, okay, but anyways, go ahead. You were going to ask something. Well, no, I would like to discuss the most basic elements of internal state, in particular the ones that are below our conscious detection.
Starting point is 01:26:43 And this is a, of course, as a segue into this incredible landscape, which is the gut brain access, which I think 15 years ago was almost a... Maybe it was a couple of posters that have meeting. And then now I believe you and others, there are, their app, companies, their active research programs, there's a beautiful work. Maybe you could describe some of that work that you and others have been involved in. And a lot of the listeners of this podcast
Starting point is 01:27:16 will have heard of the Gut Brain Access. And there are a lot of misconceptions about the Gut Brain Access. Many people think that this means that we think with our stomach because of the quote the gut brain acts. Many people think that this means that we think with our stomach because of the quote-unquote gut feeling aspect, but I'd love you to talk about the aspects of gut brain signaling that drive our, or change our perceptions and behaviors that are completely beneath our awareness. Yes, excellent. So let me begin maybe by, by stating that, you know, the brain needs to monitor the state of
Starting point is 01:27:51 every one of our organs, organs. It has to do it. This is the only way that the brain can ensure that every one of those organs are working together in a way that we have healthy physiology. Now, this monitoring of the brain has been known for a long time, but I think what hadn't been fully appreciated, that this is a two-way highway where the brain is not only monitoring, but is now modulating back what the body needs to do. And that includes all the way from monitoring the frequency of heartbeats and the way that the inspiration and aspirations in the breathing cycle operate
Starting point is 01:28:42 to what happens when you ingest sugar and fat. Now let me give you an example again of how the brain can take what we would refer to contextual associations and transform into incredible changes in physiology and metabolism. Remember Pavlov? So Pavlov in his classical experiments in associated conditioning, he would take a bell, it would ring the bell, every time he was going to feed the dog. And eventually the dog learned to associate the ringing of the bell with food coming. Now, the first incredible finding he made is the fact that the dog now, in the presence of the bell alone,
Starting point is 01:29:44 will start to celebrate. And we will call that, you know, neurologically speaking, an anticipatory response. Okay, I could understand it. I get it, you know, neurons in the brain that form that association now represent food is coming and they're sending a signal to moron neurons to go into your salivary glands to squeeze them so you release you know in our saliva because you know food is coming But what's even more remarkable is that those animals are also releasing insulin
Starting point is 01:30:25 In response to a bell. Okay. This illustrates one part of this two-way highway, the highway going down. Somehow the brain created these associations and their neurons in your brain now that no food is coming and sent a signal somehow all the way down to your pancreas. Then now it says release instantly because sugar is coming down. All right. This goes back to the magic of the brain. It's a never ending source of both joy and intrigue.
Starting point is 01:31:00 How the hell do they do this? Okay. I mean the neurons. I share it. I share your delight and fascination. There's not a day or a lecture or some talks are better than others or talk where I don't sit back and just think it's absolutely amazing.
Starting point is 01:31:18 Oh, yeah. It's amazing. It's amazing. Now, over the past, I know, a dozen years, and with great force over the last five years. Now, the main highway that is communicating the state of the body with the brain has been and cover, has been what we now refer to as the gut brain axis.
Starting point is 01:31:44 And the highway is a specific bundle of nerves, which emerge from the vagal ganglia, the nodos ganglia. And so it's the vagus nerve that it's innovating the majority of the organs in your body. It's monitoring their function, sending a signal to the brain, and now the brain going back down and saying, this is going all right, do this, or this is not going to well do that. And I should point out, as you well know, every organ, spleen, pancreas, they all must
Starting point is 01:32:23 be monitor. In fact, you know, I now, I have no doubt that the diseases that we have normally associated with metabolism, physiology, and even immunity are likely to emerge as the disease's conditions, states of the brain. I don't think obesity is at the C-Soph metabolism. I believe obesity is at the C-Soph brain circuits. I do as well. Yeah. And so this view that we have been working on for the longest time because the molecules that we're dealing with are in the body, not in the head. You know, led us to view, of course, these issues and problems has been one of metabolism, physiology and so forth.
Starting point is 01:33:19 They remain to be the carriers of the ultimate signal, but the brain ultimately appears to be the conductor of this orchestra of physiology and metabolism. All right. Now, let's go to the Gat Brain and Sugar. Maybe. Please. Please. No, I mean, the vagus nerve has, in popular culture, has been kind of converted into this single
Starting point is 01:33:51 meaning of calming pathways, mostly because I actually have to tip my hat to the yogic community was among the first to talk about Vegas on and on and on, there are calming pathways, you know, so-called parasympathetic pathways within the Vegas. But I think that the more we learn about the Vegas, the more it seems like an entire set of neural connections as opposed to one nerve. You guys- I just wanted to just mention that because I think a lot of people have heard about the Vegas, turns out, experimentally in the laboratory, many neuroscientists will stimulate the Vegas to create states of alertness and arousal
Starting point is 01:34:30 when animals or even people believe it or not are close to dying or going into coma. Stimulation of the Vegas is one of the ways to wake up the brain, counter to the idea that it's just this site way of calming oneself down. I know, of course. I mean, when that has to be cautious there in that.
Starting point is 01:34:47 So the biggest nerve is made out of many thousands of fibers, you know, individual fibers that make this gigantic bundle. And it's likely, as we're speaking, that each of these fibers carries a slightly different meaning. They're not necessarily one by one, maybe five, five or ten, five or twenty do they all right. But they carry meaning that's associated with their specific task. This group of fibers is telling the brain about the state of your heart. This group of fiber is telling the brain about the state of your heart. This group of five are telling the brain about the state of your gut.
Starting point is 01:35:29 This is telling your brain about its nutritional state, this, your pancreas, this, your langues. And they are, again, to make the same simple example, the keys of this piano. Yes, you're right, there is a lot of data showing that activating the entire Vega bundle has very meaningful effects in a wide range of conditions. In fact, it's being used to treat untractable depression, those stimulators, epileptic seizures.
Starting point is 01:36:15 But again, there are thousands of fibers carrying different functions. So to some degree, this is like So to some degree, you know, this is like turning the lights on the stadium because you need to illuminate where you lost your keys under your seat, yet 10,000 balls of a thousand watts each of just come on. Only one of this is pointing to where and so I'm lucky enough that one of them happened to point to my site. So here you activate the bundle of thousands of fibers I'm lucky enough that some of those happen to do something to make a meaningful difference in depression or to make a meaningful difference in epileptics.
Starting point is 01:37:12 But it should not be misconstrued as arguing that this broad activation has any type of selectivity or specificity. We're just lucky enough that among all the things are being done, some of those happen to change the biology of these processes. Now, the reason this is relevant, because the magic of this God brain access is the fact that you have these thousands of fibers really doing different functions.
Starting point is 01:37:47 And our goal, and along with many other great scientists, including Steve Lieberlis, that started a lot of this molecular dissection on this vagal gut brain communication line at Harvard. He's trying to uncover why there are each of those lines doing. Why are each of those keys of this piano playing? What's the latest there? Just as a brief update. I know Stephen Lee released, I think I was there
Starting point is 01:38:25 when he got his Howard Hughes and I did not. So that was fun. Always great to get beat by excellent people. First of all, I'm happy you did him because that way you can focus on these amazing podcasts. That's very gracious of you. It's always feels better. It's not good to get beat out by excellent people. Steven is second to none. And he is defining, as you said, the molecular constituents of different elements of these many, many fibers. Is there an update there? Are they finding multiple parallel pathways? They are. They are. Some say control, hard beat, some that control the respiratory cycle, some that might be involved in a gastric movement, you know, this notion that you are full and you feel full in part because your gut gets
Starting point is 01:39:16 distended, your stomach, for example. And then there are little sensors that are reading that and telling the brain your full. So the text books will soon change on the basis of the liberally is another word. In essence, I think we are learning enough about these lines that could really help put together this holistic view of how the brain is truly changing body physiology, metabolism and immunity. The part that hasn't been yet developed and that it needs a fair amount of work, but it's an exciting thrilling, you know, the journey of discovery is how the signal comes back to now change that biology. You know, the example I gave you before with Pablo's dog, yeah?
Starting point is 01:40:15 All right, I figure out, you know, how the association created this link between the bell, but then how does the brain tell the pancreas to release in the right amount of insulin? Okay, so tell me, tell me, let me tell you about the God Brain axis and our insatiable appetite for sugar and fat. Insatiable for sugar and quenchable for fat. And this is a story about the fundamental difference between liking and wanting. Liking sugar is the function of the taste system. And it's not really liking sugar, it's liking sweet.
Starting point is 01:41:16 One tin sugar. Our never ending appetite for sugar is the story of the God brain access, liking versus wanting. And this work is work of my own laboratory. You know, that began long ago when we discovered the sweet receptors. And you can now engineer mice that lack these receptors. So, in essence, these animals will be unable to taste sweet, a life without sweetness, a horror. And if you give a normal mouse a bottle containing sweet,
Starting point is 01:42:02 and we're going to put either sugar or an artificial sweetener. Alright, they both are sweet. They have slightly different tastes, but that simply because artificial sweetners have some off-tastes. But as far as the sweet receptor is concerned, they both activate the same receptor, trigger the same signal. And if you give an animal an option of a bottle containing sugar or a sweetener versus water, this animal will drink 10 to 1 from the bottle containing sweet. That's the taste system. Animal goes samples, each one leaks a couple of leaks and then says, oh, that's the one
Starting point is 01:42:43 I want because it's a petitive and because I love it. To it prefers sugar to artificial sweetener. No, no, no, no, no, equally artificial sweetener. Equally artificial sweetener. Equally artificial sweetener. Equally artificial sweetener. Equally artificial sweetener. Equally artificial sweetener.
Starting point is 01:42:56 Equally artificial sweetener. Equally artificial sweetener. Equally artificial sweetener. Equally artificial sweetener. Equally artificial sweetener. Equally artificial sweetener. Equally artificial sweetener. Equally artificial sweetener.
Starting point is 01:43:04 Equally artificial sweetener. Equally artificial sweetener. Equally artificial where we separate those two. For now, it's sweet versus water. Okay. And sweet means sweet, not sugar. Sweet means anything that tastes sweet. All right. And sugar is one example and splendor is another example. Aspartane, monk, stevia. All right.
Starting point is 01:43:22 Stevia doesn't matter. Yeah, I mean, there's some that only humans can taste. Mice can not taste because their receptors between humans and mice are different. But we have put the human receptor into mice, we engineer mice, and we completely humanize this mouse's taste world. All right. But for the purpose of this conversation, we're only comparing sweet versus water. An option, my goodness, they will
Starting point is 01:43:54 leak to no end in from the sweet side. 10 to 1 at least versus the water. Make sense? All right. Now we're going to take the mice and we're going to take the mice, and we're going to genetically engineer it to remove the sweet receptors. So this mice no longer have in their oral cavity any sensors that can detect sweetness, be that sugar molecule, be the neurodificial sweetener,
Starting point is 01:44:20 be it anything else that tastes sweet. And if you give this mice an option between sweet versus water, sugar versus water, artificial sweet versus water. It will drink equally well from both because you cannot tell them apart because it doesn't have the receptors for sweet so that sweet bottle tastes just like water. Makes sense? Makes sense. Very good. Now we're going to do the experiment with sugar from now on. Let's focus on sugar. So I'm going to give a mouse a little sugar versus water. Normal mouse will drink from the sugar, sugar, sugar,
Starting point is 01:44:57 sugar, sugar. Very little from the water. No powder sweet receptors. Eliminate them. Mouse can no longer tell them apart and they will drink from both. But if I keep the mouse in that cage for the next 48 hours, something extraordinary happens when I come 48 hours later and I see what the mouse is leaking or drinking from. The mouse is drinking almost exclusively from the sugar bottle. How could this be? He cannot taste it? Doesn't have sweet receptors. During those 48 hours, the mouse learn that there is something in that bottle
Starting point is 01:45:44 that makes me feel good. And that is the bottle I want to consume. Now, how does the mouse identify that bottle? It does so by using other sensory features. The smell of the bottle, the texture of the solution inside, sugar, the high concentration is kind of guppy. The sadness in which the bottle is in the cage. It doesn't matter what, but the mouse realized there is something there that makes me feel
Starting point is 01:46:18 good and that's what I want. And that is the fundamental basis of our unquenchable desire and our craving for sugar and is mediated by the God Brain Access. The first clue is that it takes a long time to develop. Immediately suggesting a post-injustive effect. So, we reason if this is true and is the Godbrain axis that's driving sugar preference, then there should be a group of neurons in the brain that are responding to post-injustive sugar. And lo and behold, we identify a group of neurons in the brain that does this, and these neurons receive their input directly from the gut brain access.
Starting point is 01:47:14 From other neurons. You got it. And so what's happening is that sugar is recognized normally by the tongue, activates an repetitive response, now you ingest it, and now you activate a selective group of cells in your intestines that now send a signal to the brain via the vagal ganglia, that says, I got what I need. The tongue does not say, no, that you get what you need. It only knows that you tasted it. This knows that you got to the point that it's going to be used, which is the gut. And now it sends the signal to now reinforce the consumption of this thing because this is the one that I needed sugar,
Starting point is 01:48:08 source of energy. And are these neurons in the gut? So, these are not neurons in the gut. So, these are gut cells that recognize the sugar molecule. O.S. Send a signal and that signal is received by the vagal neuron directly, got it. And the sense of signal through the gut brain access to the cell bodies of these neurons in the vega ganglia. And from there to the brainstem to now trigger the preference for sugar. Two questions. One, you mentioned that these cells that
Starting point is 01:48:42 detect sugar within the gut are actually within the intestine. You didn't say stomach, which surprised me. I always think gut as stomach, but of course, intestine. They're intestine because that's where all the absorption happens. So you won the signal. You see, you won the brain to know that you had successful ingestion and breakdown of whatever you consume into the building blocks of life. And glucose, amino acids, fat. And so you want to make sure that once they are in the form that intestines can now absorb them, is where you get the signal back saying, this is what I want. this what I want. Okay? Now, let me just take it one step further.
Starting point is 01:49:35 And this now sugar molecules activates this unique got brain circuit that now drives the development of our preference for sugar. Now, a key element of this circuit is that the sensors in the gut that recognize the sugar do not recognize artificial sweeteners at all, because their nutrient value is uncoupled from the taste. Generally speaking, one can make that by this because it's a very different type of receptor. I see. Turns out that it's not very different type of receptor. I see. Turns out that it's not the tongue receptors being used in the gut. It's a completely different molecule that only recognizes the glucose molecule, not artificial sweeteners. This has a profound impact on the effect of ultimately artificial sweeteners
Starting point is 01:50:25 in curbing our appetite, our craving, our insatiable desire for sugar. Since they don't activate the gut brain access, they'll never satisfy the craving for sugar, like sugar does. And the reason I believe that artificial sweeteners have failed in the market to curve our appetite, our need, our desire for sugar,
Starting point is 01:50:58 is because they beautifully work on the tongue, the liking to recognize sweet versus non-sweet, but they felt to activate the key sensors in the cat that now inform the brain, you got sugar, no need to crave anymore. So the issue of wanting, can we relate that to a particular set of neurochemicals upstream? So the pathway is, so glucose is activating these cells in the gut through the vagus that's communicated through the, presumably the nodos ganglion and up into the brainstem. Very good. And from there, where does it go? Yeah, where is it going?
Starting point is 01:51:44 What is the substrate of wanting? I, you know, of course I think molecules like dopamine, craving, there's a boat, even called the molecule of more, et cetera, et cetera. Dopamine is a very diabolical molecule, as you know, because it evokes both a sense of pleasure-ish, but also a sense of desiring more of craving. So if I understand you correctly, artificial sweeteners are, and I agree, are failing
Starting point is 01:52:11 as a means to satisfy sugar craving at the level of nutrient sensing. And yet, if we trigger this true sugar evoked wanting pathway too much, and we've all experienced this, then we eat sugar and we find ourselves wanting more and more sugar. Now that could also be insulin dysregulation, but can we uncouple those? Yeah, I mean, look, we have a mega problem with over-consumption of sugar and fat, we're facing a unique time in our evolution, where the diseases of malnutrition are due to overnutrition. I mean, how nuts is that?
Starting point is 01:53:02 I mean, historically, the diseases of malnutrition have always been linked to undernutrition. And so, we need to come up with strategies that can meaningfully change. The activation of these circuits that control our wanting, certainly in the populations at risk. And this gut brain circuit that ultimately, you know, it's the lines of communication that are informing the brain, the presence of intestinal sugar in this example. It's a very important target in the way we think about.
Starting point is 01:53:54 Is there a way that we can manipulate these circuits? So, I make your brain think that you got satisfied with sugar, even though I'm not giving you sugar. So that immediately raises the question, are the receptors for glucose in these gut cells susceptible to other things that are healthier for us? That's very good. Excellent idea. And I think an important goal will be to come up with a strategy and identify those very means that allow us to modulate the circuits in a way that certainly for all of those, where this is a big issue, it can really have a dramatic impact in improving a human health.
Starting point is 01:54:49 I could be wrong about this and I'm happy to be wrong. I'm often wrong and told I'm wrong that we have cells within our gut that don't just sense to sugar glucose, which to be specific, but also cells within our gut that sense amino acids and fatty acids. I could imagine a scenario where one could train themselves to feel immense amounts of satiety from the consumption of foods that are rich in essential fatty acids, amino acids, perhaps less caloric or less insulin dysregulating than sugar. I'll use myself as an example.
Starting point is 01:55:30 I've always enjoyed sweets, but in the last few years, for some reason, I've started to lose my appetite for them, probably because I just don't eat them anymore. At first, that took some restriction. Now, I just don't even think about it. Yeah, and you're not reinforcing the circuits. And so your innocence are moving yourself.
Starting point is 01:55:49 But you tend to be the exception. You know, we have a huge, a huge, incredible large number of people there. I mean, continuously exposed to highly processed foods. Yeah. And hidden. That's so-called hidden sugars. They don't even have to be hidden. You know, it's right there hiding in plain sight. Yeah, I agree.
Starting point is 01:56:12 So much is made of hidden sugars that we often overlooked that they are. They are also the overt sugars. Yeah, I mean, we can have a long discussion on the importance of coming up with strategies that could meaningfully change public health when it comes to nutrition. But I want to just go back to the notion of these brain centers that are ultimately the ones that are being activated by this essential nutrients. So sugar, fat and amino acids are building blocks of our diets.
Starting point is 01:56:53 And this is across all animal species. So it's not unreasonable then to assume that dedicated brain circuits would have evolved to ensure their recognition, their ingestion, and the reinforcement that that is what I need. And indeed, animals evolve these two systems. One is the taste system that allows you to recognize them and trigger this predetermined hard wire immediate responses. Yes?
Starting point is 01:57:38 Oh my goodness, this is so good, it's so sweet. I personally have a sweet tooth, my eye out. And oh my god, this is so delicious, it's fatty or umami recognizing them in acids. So that's the liking pathway, yeah? But in the wisdom of evolution, that's good, but that's not quite do it. You want to make sure that these things get to the place where they're needed. And they're not needed in your tongue. They're needed in your intestines
Starting point is 01:58:12 where they're going to be absorbed as the nutrients that will support life. And the brain wants to know this. And he wants to know it in a way that he can now form the association between that that I just tasted is what got where it needs to be and he makes me feel good. And so now next time that I have to choose, what should I eat? That association now guides me to, that's the one I want. I want that fruit, not that fruit.
Starting point is 01:58:55 I want those leaves, not those leaves, because these are the ones that activate the right circuits that ensure that the right nutrients got to the right place until the brain. This is what I want and need. Are we on? We're on. One thing that intrigues me and puzzles me is that this effect took a couple of days, at least in mice, and the sensation, sorry, the perception of taste, is immediate. Is immediate. And yet this is a slow system. And so, in a beautiful way, but in a kind of mysterious way,
Starting point is 01:59:34 the brain is able to couple the taste of a sweet drink with the experience of nutrient extraction in the gut. Under a context where the mouse and the human is presumably ingesting other things, smelling other mice, smelling other people. That's incredible. Yeah, but you have to think of it, not as as humans. Remember, we inherited the circuits of our ancestors and they threw evolutionary from their ancestors. And we haven't had that many years to have fundamentally changed in many of these hard wire circuits.
Starting point is 02:00:13 So forget as humans, let's look at animals in the wild. Okay? Which is easier now to comprehend the logic. Why should this take a long time of continuous reinforcement, given that I can taste it in a second? You want to make sure that this source of sugar, for example, in the wild, is the best. It's the riches. It's the one where I get the most energy for the least amount of extraction, the least amount of work.
Starting point is 02:00:52 I want to identify which sources of sugar. And if the system simply responds immediately to the first sugar that gets to your gut. You're gonna form the association with those sources of food, which are not the ones that you should be eaten from. Don't fall in love with the first person you encounter. Oh my goodness, exactly. And so evolutionarily, by having the taste system given you the immediate recognition,
Starting point is 02:01:25 but then by forcing this God brain access to reinforce it only when sustained in a repeated exposure has informed the brain. This is the one you don't want to form the association before. And so, you know, when we remove it from the context of, we just go to the supermarket. We're not hunting there in the wild where I need to form. And so what's happening is that highly processed foods are hijacking, you know, co-opt in the circuits in a way that they would have never happened in nature.
Starting point is 02:02:12 And then we not only find these things repetitive and palatable, but in addition we are continuously reinforcing, you know, the wanting in a way that, oh my god, this is so great. What do I feel like eating? Let me have more of these. You've just forever changed the way that I think about supermarkets and restaurants. They're understanding this fast signaling and this slower signaling and the utility of having both makes me realize that supermarkets and restaurants are about the most unnatural thing for our system ever. Almost the equivalent of living in small villages with very few suitable mates versus online dating, for instance. I'm not going to make a judgment call there
Starting point is 02:02:58 because they do serve an important purpose. I like restaurants too. Yeah, and so do supermarkets think Thank God. I think they're not the culprits. Yeah, I think the culprits, of course, you know, are reliance on foods that are not necessarily healthy. Now, going back to the supermarket, the don't fall in love with the first, they need to work. You take a tangerine, and you take an extract of tangerine that you used to cook,
Starting point is 02:03:44 that spike, let's say, with sugar. And you equalize in both where they both provide the same amount of calories. If you eat them both, they're going to have a very different effect in your gut brain axis and your system. Once you make the extract and you process it and you add it, process sugar, you know, to use it now to cook, to add, to make it really
Starting point is 02:04:14 sweet, tangerine thing. Now you're providing now fully ready to use broken-down source of sugar. broken-down source of sugar. In the tangerine, that sugar is mixed in the complexity of a whole set of other chemical components, fiber, long chains of sugar molecules that need a huge amount of work that need a huge amount of work by your stomach, your gut system, to break it down. So you're using a huge amount of energy to extract energy. And the balance, it's very different than when I take this process highly extracted, tangerine, by the way, I use tangerines because I had a tangerine just before I came in. Delicious. They are delicious.
Starting point is 02:05:13 And so, and so this goes back to the issue of supermarkets. And so to to to some degree, you know, a given a choice, you don't want to eat process, highly processed foods because everything is already been broken down for you. And so your system has no work. And so you're co-opting hijacking the circuits in a way that they're being activated at a time scale that normally wouldn't happen. Well, this is why I often feel that, and I think a lot of data are now starting to support the idea that while indeed the laws of thermodynamics apply, calories ingested versus calories
Starting point is 02:05:53 burn is a very real thing, right, that the appetite for certain foods and the wanting and the liking are phenomena of the nervous system, brain and gut, as you've beautifully described, and that that changes over time, depending on how we are receiving these nutrients. Absolutely. Look, we have a lot of work to do. I'm talking as a society. I'm not talking about you and I. We also have a lot of work to do.
Starting point is 02:06:32 Now, I think understanding the circuits is given as important insights and how ultimately, hopefully, we can improve human health and make a meaningful difference. Now it's very easy to try to, you know, connect the dots, A to B, B to C, C to D. And I think there's a lot more complexity to it, but I do think that the lessons that are emerging out of understanding how these circuits operate can ultimately inform how we deal with our diets in a way that we avoid what we're facing now,
Starting point is 02:07:23 you know, as a society, yeah? I mean, it's nuts that the over-nutrition happens to be such a prevalent problem. And I also think the training of people who are thinking about metabolic science and metabolic disease is largely divorced from the training of the neuroscientist and vice versa. No one field is to blame, but I fully agree
Starting point is 02:07:44 that the brain is the key or the nervous system to be more accurate, is that one of the key overlooked features. Is the arveter, ultimately, is the arveter of many of these pathways. As a final question, and one, which is simply to entertain my curiosity and the curiosity of listeners, what is your absolute favorite food? which is simply to entertain my curiosity and the curiosity of the listeners. What is your absolute favorite food? Oh my goodness. Taste, I should say.
Starting point is 02:08:14 Yes. Taste. To distinguish between taste and the nutritive value or lack that. Yes. Look. We, unlike every animal species, eat for the enjoyment of it. It doesn't happen in the wild. Most animals eat when they need to eat. That's something they don't enjoy it, but it's a completely different story. It's a completely different story. I have too many favorite foods because I enjoy the sensory experience. Rather than the food itself, to me is the whole thing.
Starting point is 02:08:55 It's from the present day. There have been experiments done in psychophysics. I'm going to take a salad made out of 11 components. to take a salad made out of 11 components. And I'm going to mix them all up in a pot puree of greens and other things here. And in the other one, I'm going to present it in a beautiful arrangement. And I'm going to put it behind a glass cabinet. And I'm going to sell them. And I'm going to sell one for two dollars and one for eight dollars. Precisely the same ingredients, exactly the same amount of each. Ultimately, you're gonna mix them, they're all gonna be the same.
Starting point is 02:09:30 And people will pay the $8 because, you know what? Either Vogue's a different person. It gives you the feel that, oh my goodness, I'm gonna enjoy that salad. So going back to what is my favorite food. To me, eating is really a sensory journey. I don't mean the everyday, let me have some, you know, chicken wings because I'm hungry. let me have some chicken wings because I'm hungry.
Starting point is 02:10:10 But every piece, I think, has an important evoking sensory role. And so, you know, in terms of categories of food, you know, I grew up in Chile, so meat is always been. But I eat it so seldom now. That's all right. Here, because, you know, I know that's not necessarily the whole thing. Read me them talking about it. And so, you know, I grew up eating it every day. And so I grew up eating it every day. I'm talking seven days a week, chilling in Argentina. You know, that's the main stay of our diet, eh? Now maybe I have red meat.
Starting point is 02:10:57 I know. Once every four weeks. And you enjoy it. You see, oh, I love it. Part of it is because I haven't had it in four weeks, eh? But,. Oh, I love it. Part of it is because I haven't had it in four weeks. But, you know, I love sushi, but I love the the art of sushi. You know, the whole thing, you know, the way it's presented, it changes the way you taste it. I love ethnic food. I love ethnic food, in particular, you're in the right place. You got it.
Starting point is 02:11:28 That was the main reason I wanted to come to New York. No, I'm just kidding. There's also that Columbia University that's, and I came here because I wanted to be with you know, people that are thinking about the brain the same way that I like to think which you know, can we solve this big problem? This big question. And certainly you're making amazing strides in that direction. And I love your answer because it really brings together
Starting point is 02:11:53 the many features of the circuitries and the phenomena we've been talking about today, which is that while it begins with sensation and perception, ultimately it's the context, and that context is highly individual to person, place, and time, and that context is highly individual to person, place, and time, and many, many other things. On behalf of myself, and certainly on behalf of all the listeners, I want to thank you,
Starting point is 02:12:14 first of all, for the incredible work that you've been doing now for decades in vision, in taste, and in this bigger issue of how we perceive and experience life. It's truly pioneering and incredible work and I feel quite lucky to have been on the sidelines seeing this over the years and hearing the talks and reading the countless beautiful papers. But also for your time today to come down here and talk to us about what drives you and the discoveries you've made. Thank you ever so much. It was great fun. Thank you for having me.
Starting point is 02:12:46 We'll do it again. I like it. We shall. Thank you for joining me today for my discussion about perception, and in particular, the perception of taste with Dr. Charles Zucker. If you're learning from and are enjoying this podcast, please subscribe to our YouTube channel. That's a terrific zero-cost way to support us. In addition, please subscribe to the podcast on Spotify and Apple. And on Spotify and Apple, you can leave us up to a five-star review. If you have
Starting point is 02:13:11 feedback for us in terms of comments related to topics we've covered or questions or topics or guests that you'd like us to cover on future episodes of the Urimin Lab podcast, please put those in the comment section on YouTube. We do read all the comments. On today's episode of the Huberman Lab podcast, we didn't talk about supplements, but on many previous episodes of the Huberman Lab podcast, we talk about supplements that are useful for sleep, for focus, for hormone support, and other aspects of mental health, physical health, and performance. If you're interested in some of those supplements, you can go to livemomentus.com slash Huberman to see the catalog of supplements that we've helped them formulate and that map directly onto specific protocols described on the Huberman Lab
Starting point is 02:13:49 podcast. Again, that's livemomentus.com slash Huberman. If you're not already following us on social media, we are Huberman Lab on Twitter and Huberman Lab on Instagram. And at both Twitter and Instagram, I cover science and science-related tools, some of which overlap with the content of the Huberman Labrelated tools, some of which overlap with the content of the Uberman Lab podcast, but much of which is distinct from the information contained in the Uberman Lab podcast. We also have a newsletter in which we spell out protocols and some summaries from previous
Starting point is 02:14:14 podcast episodes. If you'd like to check those out, you can go to hubermanlab.com, go to the menu, and you'll find the neural network newsletter. You can sign up for that newsletter simply by providing an email. It's completely zero cost. We have a very strict privacy policy. We do not share your email with anybody. Again, that's hubermanlab.com. Go to the menu and look for the neural network newsletter.
Starting point is 02:14:34 And if you'd like examples of previous newsletters, you'll also find those at hubermanlab.com. Once again, thank you for joining me today in my discussion with Dr. Charles Zucker about the biology of perception and the biology of the perception of taste in particular. I hope you found that discussion to be as enriching as I did. And last but certainly not least, thank you for your interest in science.

There aren't comments yet for this episode. Click on any sentence in the transcript to leave a comment.