Huberman Lab - Dr. David Berson: Understanding Your Brain's Logic & Function

Episode Date: December 13, 2021

In this episode, my guest is Dr. David Berson, Professor & Chairman of Neuroscience at Brown University. Dr. Berson discovered the neurons in your eye that set your biological rhythms for sleep, wakef...ulness, mood and appetite. He is also a world-renowned teacher of basic and advanced neuroscience, having taught thousands of university lectures on this topic. Many of his students have become world-leading neuroscientists and teachers themselves.  Here Dr. Berson takes us on a structured journey into and around the nervous system, explaining: how we perceive the world and our internal landscape, how we balance, see, and remember. Also, how we learn and perform reflexive and deliberate actions, how we visualize and imagine in our mind, and how the various circuits of the brain coordinate all these incredible feats.  We discuss practical and real-life examples of neural circuit function across the lifespan. Dr. Berson gives us a masterclass in the nervous system—one that, in just less than two hours, will teach you an entire course's worth about the brain and how yours works. For the full show notes, visit hubermanlab.com. Thank you to our sponsors AG1 (Athletic Greens): https://athleticgreens.com/huberman LMNT: https://drinklmnt.com/huberman Supplements from Momentous https://www.livemomentous.com/huberman Timestamps (00:00:00) Dr. David Berson  (00:03:11) Sponsors: AG1, LMNT (00:08:02) How We See  (00:10:02) Color Vision  (00:13:47) “Strange” Vision (00:16:56) How You Orient In Time (00:25:45) Body Rhythms, Pineal function, Light & Melatonin, Blueblockers (00:34:45) Spending Times Outdoors Improves Eyesight  (00:36:20) Sensation, Mood, & Self-Image (00:41:03) Sense of Balance (00:50:43) Why Pigeons Bob Their Heads, Motion Sickness  (01:00:03) Popping Ears (01:02:35) Midbrain & Blindsight  (01:10:44) Why Tilted Motion Feels Good  (01:13:24) Reflexes vs. Deliberate Actions (01:16:35) Basal Ganglia & the “2 Marshmallow Test” (01:24:40) Suppressing Reflexes: Cortex  (01:33:33) Neuroplasticity  (01:36:27) What is a Connectome? (01:45:20) How to Learn (More About the Brain) (01:49:04) Book Suggestion, my Berson Appreciation (01:50:20) Zero-Cost ways to Support the HLP, Guest Suggestions, Sponsors, Supplements Title Card Photo Credit: Mike Blabac Disclaimer

Transcript
Discussion (0)
Starting point is 00:00:00 Welcome to the Huberman Lab podcast where we discuss science and science-based tools for everyday life. I'm Andrew Huberman and I'm a professor of neurobiology and Ophthalmology at Stanford School of Medicine. Today my guest is Dr. David Berson, professor of medical science, neurobiology and ophthalmology at Brown University. Dr. Berson's laboratory is credited with discovering the cells in the eye that set your circadian rhythms. These are the so-called intrinsically photosensitive melanopsin cells, and while that's a mouthful, all you need to know for a sake of this introduction is that those are the cells that inform your brain and body about the time of day.
Starting point is 00:00:39 Dr. Bersen's laboratory has also made a number of other important discoveries about how we convert our perceptions of the outside world into motor action. More personally, Dr. Bersen has been my go-to resource for all things neuroscience for nearly two decades. I knew of his reputation as a spectacular researcher for a long period of time, and then many years ago, I cold-called him out of the blue. I basically corralled him into a long conversation over the phone after which he invited me out to Brown,
Starting point is 00:01:11 and we've been discussing neuroscience and how the brain works and the emerging new technologies and the emerging new concepts in neuroscience for a very long time now. You're going to realize today why Dr. Berson is my go-to source. He has an exceptionally clear and organized view of how the nervous system works. There are many, many parts of the nervous system, different nuclei and
Starting point is 00:01:34 connections and circuits and chemicals and so forth, but it takes a special kind of person to be able to organize that information into a structured and logical framework that can allow us to make sense of how we function in terms of what we feel, what we experience, how we move through the world. Dr. Berson is truly one of a kind and his ability to synthesize and organize and communicate that information, and I give him credit as one of my mentors and one of the people that I respect most in the field of science and medical science generally.
Starting point is 00:02:05 Today, Dr. Berson takes us on a journey from the periphery of the nervous system, meaning from the outside, deep into the nervous system, layer by layer, structure by structure, circuit by circuit, making clear to us how each of these individual circuits work and how they work together as a whole. It's a really magnificent description that you simply cannot get from any textbook, from any popular book, and frankly, as far as I know, from any podcast that currently exists out there. So it's a real gift to have this opportunity
Starting point is 00:02:35 to learn from Dr. Berson. Again, I consider him my mentor in the field of learning and teaching neuroscience, and I'm excited for you to learn from him. One thing is for certain, by the end of this podcast, you will know far more about how your nervous system works than the vast majority of people out there, including many expert biologists and neuroscientists. Before we begin, I'd like to emphasize that this podcast is separate from my teaching and
Starting point is 00:02:59 research roles at Stanford. It is however part of my desire and effort to bring zero cost to consumer information about science and science-related tools to the general public. In keeping with that theme, I'd like to thank the sponsors of today's podcast. Our first sponsor is Athletic Greens. Athletic Greens is an all-in-one vitamin mineral probiotic drink. I've been taking Athletic Greens since 2012, so I'm delighted that they're sponsoring the podcast. The reason I started taking Athletic Greens and the reason I started taking athletic greens and the reason I still take athletic greens once or twice a day
Starting point is 00:03:27 is that it helps me cover all of my basic nutritional needs. It makes up for any deficiencies that I might have. In addition, it has probiotics, which are vital for microbiome health. I've done a couple of episodes now on the so-called gut microbiome and the ways in which the microbiome interacts with your immune system, with your brain to regulate mood, and essentially with every biological system relevant to health
Starting point is 00:03:50 throughout your brain and body. With athletic greens, I get the vitamins I need, the minerals I need, and the probiotics to support my microbiome. If you'd like to try athletic greens, you can go to atlettagreens.com slash Huberman and claim a special offer. They'll give you five free travel packs plus a year supply of vitamin D3 K2. There are a ton of data now showing that vitamin D3 is essential for various aspects of our brain and body health, even if we're getting a lot of sunshine.
Starting point is 00:04:16 Many of us are still deficient in vitamin D3. And K2 is also important because it regulates things like cardiovascular function, calcium in the body, and so on. Again, go to athleticgreens.com slash Huberman to claim the special offer of the 5 free travel packs and the year supply of vitamin D3 K2. Today's episode is also brought to us by Element. Element is an electrolyte drink that has everything you need and nothing you don't. That means the exact ratios of electrolytes are an element, and those are sodium, magnesium,
Starting point is 00:04:43 and potassium, but it has no sugar. I've talked many times before on this podcast about the key role of hydration and electrolytes for nerve cell function, neuron function, as well as the function of all the cells and all the tissues and organ systems of the body. If we have sodium, magnesium, and potassium present in the proper ratios, all of those cells function properly properly and all our bodily systems can be optimized. If the electrolytes are not present and if hydration is low, we simply can't think as well as we would otherwise. Our mood is off, hormone systems go off, our ability to get into physical action, to
Starting point is 00:05:19 engage in endurance and strength, and all sorts of other things is diminished. So with element, you can make sure that you're staying on top of your hydration and that you're getting the proper ratios of electrolytes. If you'd like to try element, you can go to drinkelement.com slash huberman, and you'll get a free element sample pack with your purchase. They're all delicious. Again, if you want to try element, you can go to element element.com slash huberman. And now, for my discussion with Dr. David Berson.
Starting point is 00:05:46 Welcome. Thank you. So nice to be here. Great to have you. For more than 20 years, you've been my go-to source for all things, nervous system, how it works, how it's structured. So today I want to ask you some questions about that. I think people would gain a lot of insight into this machine that makes them think and feel and see, etc.
Starting point is 00:06:09 If you would, could you tell us how we see, you know, a photon of light enters the eye? What happens? Right. I mean, how is it that I look outside, I see a truck drive by or I look on the wall, I see a photo of my dog. How does that work? Right. So this is an old question, obviously. And clearly, in the end, the reason you have a visual experience is that your brain is got some pattern of activity that associates with the input from the periphery. But you can have a visual experience with no input from the periphery as well. When you're dreaming, you're seeing things that aren't coming through your eyes. Are those memories? I would say in a sense they may reflect your visual experience.
Starting point is 00:06:54 They're not necessarily specific visual memories, but of course they can be. But the point is that the experience of seeing is actually a brain phenomenon. But of course, under normal circumstances, we see the world because we're looking at it and we're using our eyes to look at it. And fundamentally, when we're looking at the exterior world, it's what the retina is telling the brain that matters. So there are cells called ganglion cells. These are neurons that are the key cells for communicating between eye and brain.
Starting point is 00:07:24 The eye is like the camera. It's detecting the initial image, doing some initial processing, and then that signal gets sent back to the brain proper. And of course, it's there at the level of the cortex that we have this conscious visual experience. There are many other places in the brain that get visual input as well,
Starting point is 00:07:40 doing other things with that kind of information. So I get a lot of questions about color vision if you would. Could you explain how is it that we can perceive reds and greens and blues and things in that sort? Right. So the first thing to understand about light is that it's just a form of electromagnetic radiation. It's vibrating.
Starting point is 00:08:03 It's oscillating. Well, I'd say it's vibrating, it's oscillating. But light if say it's vibrating, it's oscillating, you mean that photons are actually moving. Well, in a sense, photons are, they're certainly moving through space. We think about photons as particles, and that's one way of thinking about light, but we can also think of it as a wave, like a radio wave. Either way is acceptable, and the radio waves have frequencies like the frequencies on your radio dial, and certain frequencies in the electromagnetic spectrum can be detected by neurons in the retina, those are the things we see, but there are still different wavelengths within the light that can be seen by the eye,
Starting point is 00:08:38 and those different wavelengths are unpacked in a sense or decoded by the nervous system to lead to our experience of color. Essentially different wavelengths give us the sensation of different colors through the auspices of different neurons that are tuned to different wavelengths of light. So when a little bit of light hits my eye, goes in, the photoreceptors convert that into electrical signal. Right. How is it that a given photon of light gives me the perception, eventually leaves the perception of red versus green versus blue?
Starting point is 00:09:16 Right. So, if you imagine that in the first layer of the retina where this transformation occurs from electromagnetic radiation into neural signals, that you have different kinds of sensitive cells that are expressing their making different molecules within themselves, forth as express purpose of absorbing photons, which is the first step in the process of seeing.
Starting point is 00:09:42 Now, it turns out that that altogether there are about five proteins like this that we need to think about in the typical retina, but for seeing color really, it's three of them. So there are three different proteins. Each absorbs light with a different, you know, preferred frequency, and then the nervous system keeps track of those signals, compares and contrast them to extract some understanding of the wavelength composition of light. So you can see, despite looking at a landscape, it must be late in the day because things are looking golden. That's all a function of our absorbing the light that's coming from the world and interpreting that with our brain, because of the different composition of the light
Starting point is 00:10:25 that's reaching our eyes. Is it fair to assume that my perception of red is the same as your perception of red? Well, that's a great question. And that mine is better. I'm just kidding. I'm just kidding. It's a great question.
Starting point is 00:10:37 It's a deep philosophical question. It's a question that really probably can't even ultimately be answered by the usual empirical scientific processes, because it's really about an individual's experience. What we can say is that the biological mechanisms that we think are important for seeing color, for example, seem to be very highly similar
Starting point is 00:11:00 from one individual to the next, whether it be human beings or other animals. And so we think that the physiological process looks very similar on the front end. But once you're at the level of perception or understanding or experience, that's something that's a little bit tougher to nail down with the sort of scientific approaches that we approach biological vision, let's say. You mentioned that there are five different cone types, essentially, cones being the cells that absorb light of different wavelengths.
Starting point is 00:11:35 I often wondered when I had my dog what he saw and how his vision differs from our vision and certainly there are animals that can see things that we can't see. What are some of the more outrageous examples of that? Of seeing things in the extreme. You know, dogs, I'm guessing, see reds more as oranges. Is that right? Because they don't have these,
Starting point is 00:12:00 the same array of neurons that we have for seeing color. Right. So the first thing is, it we have for seeing color. Right. So the first thing is, it's not really five types of columns. There are really three types of columns. And if you look at the way that color vision is thought to work, you can sort of see that it has to be three different signals. There are a couple of other types of pigments. One is really mostly for dim light vision.
Starting point is 00:12:22 When you're walking around in a moonless night and you're seeing things with very low light. That's the rod cell that uses its own pigment. And then there's another class of pigments, we'll probably talk about a little bit later, the smell and obs in pigment. I thought you were referring to like ultraviolet and infrared and... Right, right. ...and that sort of... ...right.
Starting point is 00:12:41 So in the case of a typical, well, let's put it this way, in human beings, most of us have three corn types, and we can see colors that stem from that. In most mammals, including your dog or your cat, there really are only two corn types, and that limits the kind of vision that they can have in the domain of wavelength or color, as you would say. So really, a dog sees the world kind of like a particular kind of colorblind human might see the world. Because instead of having three channels to compare and contrast, they only have two channels,
Starting point is 00:13:17 and that makes it much more difficult to figure out exactly which wavelength you're looking at. Do colorblind people suffer much as a consequence of being colorblind? Well, you know, it's like so many other disabilities. We are, you know, the world is built for people of the most common type. So in some cases, the expectation can be there that somebody can see something that they won't be able to if they're missing one of their cone types, let's say. So in those moments, that can be a real problem. You know, if there's a lack of contrast to their visual system, they will be blind to that.
Starting point is 00:13:55 In general, it's a fairly modest visual limitation as things go. You know, for example, if not being able to see acutely, it can be much more damaging, not being able to find print, for example. Yeah, I suppose if I had to give up the ability to see certain colors or give up the ability to see clearly, I certainly trade out color for clarity. Right. Of course, color is very meaningful to us as human beings. So we would hate to give it up. But obviously dogs and cats and all kinds of other mammals do perfectly well in the world.
Starting point is 00:14:29 Yeah, because we take care of them. I spend most of my time doing care of that dog. He took care of me too. Let's talk about that odd photopigment. Photopigment, of course, being the thing that absorbs light of a particular wavelength. And let's talk about these specialized ganglion cells that communicate certain types of information from the eye to the brain that are so important for so many things. What I'm referring to here,
Starting point is 00:14:58 of course, is your co-discovery of the so-called intrinsically photosensitive cells, the neurons in the eye, that do so many of the things that don't actually have to do with perception, but have to do with important biological functions. What I would love for you to do is explain to me why, once I heard you say, we have a bit of fly eye in an hour eye. And you showed this slide of a giant fly from a horror movie, trying to attack this woman. And maybe it was an eye also. So what does it mean that we have a bit of a fly eye in our eye? Yeah, so this is this last pigment is a really peculiar one.
Starting point is 00:15:42 One can think about it as really the initial sensitive element in a system that's designed to tell your brain about how bright things are in your world. And the thing that's really peculiar about this pigment is that it's in the wrong place in a sense. When you think about the structure of the retina, you think about a layer of cake essentially. You've got this thin membrane at the back of your eye, but it's actually a stack of thin layers. And the outermost of those layers
Starting point is 00:16:10 is where these photo receptors you were talking about earlier are sitting. That's where the film of your camera is, essentially. That's where the photons do their magic with the photopigments and turn it into a neural signal. I like that. Never really thought of the photo receptors as the film of the camera, but that makes sense.
Starting point is 00:16:24 Or like a sensitive chip on, you know, CCT chip in your cell phone. It's the surface on which the light pattern is imaged by the optics of the eye. And now you've got an array of sensors that's capturing that information and creating a bitmap, essentially. But now it's in neural signals distributed across the surface of the retina. So all of that was known to be going on 150 years ago. A couple of types of photo receptors, cones and rods, if you look a little bit more closely, three types of cones. That's where the transformation from electromagnetic radiation to to neural signals was thought to take place. But it turns out that this last photopigment
Starting point is 00:17:05 is in the other end of the red, the innermost part of the red. Now that's where the so-called ganglion cells are. Those are the cells that talk to the brain, the ones that actually can communicate directly, what information comes to them from the photoreceptors. And here you've got a case where actually some of the output neurons that we didn't think had any business being directly sensitive to light or actually making this photo pigment absorbing
Starting point is 00:17:32 light and converting that to neural signals and sending to the brain. So that made it pretty surprising and unexpected, but there are many surprising things about these cells. What is the relationship to the fly eye? Right. The link there is that if you ask how the photo pigment now communicates downstream from the initial absorption event to get to the electrical signal, that's a complex cellular process. It involves many chemical steps. If you look at how photoors in our eyes work, you can see what that cascade is, how that chain works. If you look in the eyes of flies or other insects or other invertebrates, there's a very similar kind of chain, but the
Starting point is 00:18:20 specifics of how the signal gets from the absorption event by the pigment to the electrical response that the nervous system can understand are characteristically different between fuzzy furry creatures like us and insects, for example, like the fly, I say. So these funny, extra photoreceptors that are in the wrong layer doing something completely different are actually using a chemical cascade that looks much more like what you would see in a fly voter receptor than what you would see in a human voter receptor, a rod or a cone, for example.
Starting point is 00:18:55 So it sounds like it's a very primitive part of primitive aspect of biology that we maintain. Exactly right. Exactly. And despite the fact that dogs can't see as many colors as we can, and cats can see as many colors as we can, we have all this extravagant stuff for seeing color. And then you've got this other pigment sitting in the wrong, not wrong, but in a different part of the eye, processing light very differently, and sending
Starting point is 00:19:22 that information into the brain. So what do these cells do? I mean, presumably they're there for a reason. There are. And the interesting thing is that one cell type like this, carrying one kind of signal, which I would call a brightness signal essentially, can do many things in the brain. When you say brightness signal, you mean that it, like right now, I have these cells, I do. Of course not. I mean, I'm joking. I hope I have these cells in my eye,
Starting point is 00:19:51 and they're paying attention to how bright it is overall, but they're not paying attention, for instance, to the edge of your ear or what else is going on in the room. Right, so it's the difference between knowing what the objects are on the table and knowing whether it's bright enough to be daylight right now. So what is your nervous system to need to know whether it's daylight right now?
Starting point is 00:20:12 Well, one thing that needs to know that is your circadian clock. If you travel across time zones to Europe, now your internal clock thinks it's California time, but the rotation of the earth is for a different part for, you know, a different part of the planet. You're the rising and setting of the sun is not at all what your body is anticipating. So you've got an internal representation of the rotation of the earth in your own brain. That's your circadian system. It's keeping time. But now you've played a trick on your nervous system. You put yourself into different place where the sun is rising at the quote,
Starting point is 00:20:45 wrong time. Well, that's not good for you, right? So you got to get back on track. One of the things this system does is sends a, oh, it's daylight now, a signal to the brain, which compares with its internal clock. And if that's not right,
Starting point is 00:20:59 it tweaks the clock gradually until you get over your jet lag and you feel back on track again. So, the jet lag case makes a lot of sense to me, but presumably these elements didn't evolve for jet lag. Right. So, what are they doing on a day-to-day basis? Right.
Starting point is 00:21:18 Well, one way to think about this is that the clock that you have in not just your brain, in all the cells, we're almost all of the cells of your body. They're all oscillating. They're all, you know, they got little clocks in them. Little clocks in themselves. They're all clocks. They need to be synchronized appropriately.
Starting point is 00:21:41 And the whole thing has to be built in biological machinery. This is actually a beautiful story about how gene expression can control gene expression. And if you set it upright, you can set up a little thing that just sort of hums along at a particular frequency. In our case, it's humming along at 24 hours because that's how our Earth rotates and it's all built into our biology. So this is great. But the reality is that the clock can only be so good.
Starting point is 00:22:08 I mean, we're talking about biology here. It's not precision engineering, and so it can be a little bit off. Well, also it doesn't, it's in our brain, so it doesn't have access to any regular unerring signal. Well, if in the absence of the rising and setting of the sun, it doesn't. If you put someone in a cave, their biological clock will keep time to within a handful of minutes of 24 hours. That's no problem for one day, but if this went on without any correction, eventually you'd be out of phase. And this is actually one of the things that blind patients often complain about. If they've got retinal blindness is insomnia and or sleep away from the normal night.
Starting point is 00:22:51 Exactly. They're not synchronized. Their clock is there, but they're drifting out of phase because their clock is only good to, you know, 24.2 hours or 23.8 hours, though by little they're drifting. So you need a synchronization signal. So even if you never across time and so on, and of course we didn't back on the Savannah we stayed within walking distance of where we were, you still need a synchronizer. Because otherwise you have nothing to actually confirm when the rising and the setting of the Sun
Starting point is 00:23:22 is. That's what you're trying to synchronize yourself to. I'm fascinated by the circadian clock and the fact that all the cells of our body have essentially a 24 hour-ish clock in them. Right. We hear a lot about these circadian rhythms and circadian clocks, the fact that we need light input from these special neurons in order to set the clock,
Starting point is 00:23:43 but I've never really heard it describe how the clock itself works and how the clock signals to all the rest of the body when the liver should be doing one thing and the stomach should be doing another. I know you've done some work on the clock. So if you would just maybe briefly describe where the clock is, what it does, and some of the top contour of how it tells the cells
Starting point is 00:24:09 of the body what to do. So the first thing to say is that as you said, the clock is all over the place. Most of the tissues in your body have clocks. We probably have what millions of clocks in our body. I would say that's probably fair. You have millions of cell types. You probably may have millions of clocks. Yeah, I would say that's that's probably fair. You have millions of cell types. You might be have millions of clocks. The role of the central pacemaker for the circadian system
Starting point is 00:24:31 is to coordinate all of these. And this is there's a little nucleus, a little collection of nerve cells in your brain. It's called the superchiasmatic nucleus, the SCN. And it is sitting in a funny place for the rest of the structures in the nervous system that get direct retinal input. It's sitting in the hypothalamus, which you can think about as sort of the great coordinator of drives and the source of all our pleasures and all our problems. Right. Or most our problems. Yes, it really is. But it's sort of, you know,
Starting point is 00:25:06 deep in your brain, things that drive you to do things. If you're freezing cold, you put on a coat, you shivery, all these things are coordinated by the hypothalamus. So this pathway, we're talking about from the retina and from these peculiar cells that are encoding light intensity, are sending signals directly into a center that's surrounded by all of these centers that control autonomic nervous system and your hormonal systems. So this is a part of your visual system that doesn't really reach the level of consciousness. It's not something you think about. It's happening under the radar kind of all the time. And the signal is working its way into this central clock coordinating center.
Starting point is 00:25:54 Now, what happens then is not that well understood, but it's clear that this is a neural center that has the same ability to communicate with other parts of your brain is any other neural center. And clearly there are circuits that involve connections between neurons that are conventional, but in addition it's quite clear that there are also sort of humoral effects that things are oozing out of the cells in the center and maybe into the circulation or just diffusing through the brain to some extent, they can also affect neurons all swear. But hypothalamus uses everything to control the rest of the bodies, and that's true, the superchismatic nucleus, this circadian center as well, it can get its fingers into the autonomic nervous system, the human role system, and of course, up to the centers of the brain that organized coordinated
Starting point is 00:26:50 rational behavior. So, if I understand correctly, we have this group of cells, the Supercars Mac nucleus, it's got a 24-hour rhythm. That rhythm is more or less matched to what's going on in our external world by the specialized set of neurons in our eye. But then the master clock itself, the SCN, releases things in the blood, humoral signals that go out various places in the body.
Starting point is 00:27:19 And then you said to the autonomic system, which is regulating more or less how alert or calm we are, as well as our thinking and our cognition. So I'd love to talk to you about the autonomic part. Presumably that's through melatonin, it's through adrenaline. How is it that this clock is impacting how the autonomic system, how alert or calm we feel? Right. the autonomic system, how alert or calm we feel. Right, so there are pathways by which the superchismatic nucleus can access
Starting point is 00:27:50 both the parasympathetic and sympathetic nervous system. Just so people know that sympathetic nervous system is the one that tends to make us more alert. And the parasympathetic nervous system is the portion of the autonomic nervous system makes us feel more calm. Right, in broad. First, yeah, to first approximation. portion of the autonomic nervous system makes us feel more calm. Right. And broad.
Starting point is 00:28:05 First, yeah. Yeah. To first approximation. Right. So, this is both of these systems are within the grasp of the circadian system through hypothalamic circuits. One of the circuits that will be, I think, a particular interest to some of your listeners is a pathway that involves this sympathetic branch of the autonomic nervous system,
Starting point is 00:28:26 the fighter flight system, that is actually through a very circuitous route, innovating the pineal gland, which is sitting in the middle of your brain. This is called third eye. Right. So this is... We'll have to get back to why it's called the third eye, because... That's an interesting idea. You can't call something the third eye and not and just, you know, just leave it there.
Starting point is 00:28:47 Just leave it there. Right. Right. Anyway, this is the major source of melatonin in your body. So light comes into my eye. Yes. Past off to the superchisementic nucleus, essentially not the light itself, but the signal representing the light.
Starting point is 00:29:02 Sure. Then the SCN, the super-cars night nucleus, can impact the melatonin system via the pineal. The way this is seen is that if you were to measure your melatonin level over the course of the day, if you could do this hour by hour, you'd see that it's really low during the day, very high at night.
Starting point is 00:29:23 But if you get up in the middle of the night and go to the bathroom and turn on the break full fluorescent light, your melatonin level is slammed to the floor. Light is directly impacting your hormonal levels through this mechanism that we just describe. So this is one of the routes by which light can act on your hormonal status through pathways that are completely beyond what you normally would think about, right? You're thinking about the things in the bathroom. Oh, there's the toothbrush. There's the tube of toothpaste. But meanwhile, this other system is just counting photons and saying, oh, wow, there's a lot of photons right now. Let's shut down the melatonin release. This is one of the main reasons why I've encouraged people to avoid bright light exposure in
Starting point is 00:30:08 the middle of the night, not just blue light, but bright light of any wavelength, because there's this myth out there that blue light, because it's the optimal signal for activating this pathway and shutting down melatonin, is the only wavelength of light that can shut it down. But am I correct in thinking that if a light is bright enough, it doesn't matter if it's blue light, green light, purple light, even red light, you're going to slam melatonin down to the ground, which is not a good thing to happen in the middle of the night. Right.
Starting point is 00:30:40 Right. Yeah. I mean, any light will affect the system to some extent. The blue light is somewhat more effective, but don't fool yourself into thinking that if you use red light, that means you're avoiding the effect. It's certainly still there, and certainly if it's very bright, it'll be more effective in driving the system
Starting point is 00:30:59 than dim blue light would be. Interesting. A lot of people wear blue blockers, and in a kind of odd twist of misinformation out there, a lot of people wear blue blockers during the middle of the day, which basically makes no sense because during the middle of the day is when you want to get a lot of bright light and including blue light into your eyes, correct? Absolutely. And not just for the reasons we've been talking about in terms of circadian effects, there are major effects of light on mood and seasonal effect of disorder apparently is essentially a reflection of the same system in reverse. If you're living in the northern
Starting point is 00:31:39 climbs and you know you're not getting that much light during the middle of the winter in Stockholm, and you're not getting that much light during the middle of the winter in Stockholm, you might be prone to depression and photo therapy might be just the ticket for you. And that's because there's a direct effect of light on mood, there's an example where if you don't have enough light, it's a problem. So I think you're exactly right.
Starting point is 00:32:00 It's not about it's like good or bad for you. It's about what kind of light and when, that makes the difference. Yeah, the general of thumb that I've been living by is to get as much bright light in my eyes, ideally from sunlight. Anytime I want to be alert. And doing exactly the opposite
Starting point is 00:32:15 when I want to be asleep. Yeah. We're getting drowsy. And there are aspects of this that spin out way beyond the conversation we're having now to things like this. It turns out that the incidence of myopia, near-sightedness, near-sightedness, right,
Starting point is 00:32:32 is strongly related to the amount of time that kids spend outdoors. And in what direction of effect? The more they spend time outdoors, the less near-sightedness they have. So this is all about, because they're viewing things at a distance or because they're getting a lot of blue light.
Starting point is 00:32:47 Sunlight. It's a great question. It is not fully resolved what the epidemiological... What the basis of that epidemiological finding is. One possibility is the amount of light, which would make me think about this melanopsyns system again. But it might very well be a question of accommodation that is the process by which you focus on near or far things. If you're never outdoors, everything is nearby. If you're
Starting point is 00:33:09 outdoors, you're focusing far. So this is your on your phone. Right. Exactly. Which there's a tremendous amount of interest these days in watches and things that count steps. Beginning to realize that we should probably have a device that can count photons during the day. Right. And can also count photons at night and tell us, hey, you're getting too many photons, you're gonna shut down your melatonin at night or you're not getting enough photons. Today, you didn't get enough bright light, whether or not it's from artificial light or from sunlight. I guess that where would you put it? I guess you'd put it on top of your head or so. You probably want it someplace outward facing. Right.
Starting point is 00:33:45 Probably what you need is as many photons over as much of the retina as possible to recruit as much of this system as possible. In thinking about other effects of this non-image forming pathway that involves these special cells in the eye and the SCN, you had a paper a few years ago looking at retinal input to an area of the brain, which has a fancy name, the Perry Hubanula, but names don't necessarily matter. That had some important effects on mood and other aspects of light. What maybe you could tell us a little bit about what is the perihambenula? Oh, wow. So, you know, that's a fancy term, but I think the way to think about this is, is a chunk of the brain that is sitting as part of a bigger chunk that's really the linker between peripheral sensory input of all kinds, or virtually all kinds, whether it's auditory input or tactile input or visual input to the region of your brain,
Starting point is 00:34:49 the cortex that allows you to think about these things and make plans around them and to integrate them and that kind of thing. So, you know, we've known about a pathway that gets from the retina through this sort of linker center, it's called the Thalamus, and then on on on to the train station. Exactly. But you want to arrive at the destination, right? Now you're at Grand Central and now you can do your thing, because you're up at the cortex. So this is the standard pattern.
Starting point is 00:35:21 You have the sensory input coming from the periphery. You've got these peripheral elements that are doing the initial stages of the eye, the eye, the skin of your fingertips, right? You know, the taste buds on your tongue. They're taking this raw information in, and they're doing some pre-processing, maybe, or, you know, the early circuits are. But eventually, most of these signals have to pass through the gateway to the cortex, which is the phalamous. And we've known for years, for decades, many decades, what the major throughput pathway
Starting point is 00:35:52 from the retina to the cortex is, and where it ends up, it ends up in the visual cortex. You know, you pat the back of your head. That's where the receiving center is for the main pathway from retina to cortex. But wait a minute, there's more. There's this little side pathway that goes through a different part of that linking phalamous center. The gateway to the cortex is like a local train from Grand Central to a weird part of the neighborhood, right? It's a completely different, it's like a little trunk line that branches off and goes out into the hinterlands and it's going to the part of this linker center
Starting point is 00:36:27 that's talking to a completely different part of cortex way up front, frontal lobe, which is much more involved in things like planning or self image or self image. I'm going to literally how one thinks about this. A few of us oneself. You know, do you feel good about yourself? Or, you know, what's your plan for next Thursday?
Starting point is 00:36:49 It's a very high level center in the highest level of your nervous system. This is the region that is getting input from this pathway, which is mostly worked out in this function by Sam or Hitars lab. I know you had him on the podcast. We didn't talk about this pathway at all, right? So, Diego Fernandez and Samar and the folks that work with them, we're able to show that this pathway doesn't exist and get you to a weird place.
Starting point is 00:37:18 But if you activate it at kind of the wrong time of day, animals can become depressed. And if you silence it under the right circumstances, then weird lighting cycles that would normally make them act depressed no longer have that effect. So it sounds to me like there's this pathway from I to this unusual train route through the structure we call the thalamus, then up to the front of the brain that relates to things of self-perception,
Starting point is 00:37:52 kind of higher level functions. I find that really interesting because most of what I think about when I think about these fancy, well, or these primitive, rather, neurons that don't pay attention to the shapes of things, but instead to brightness, I think of, well, it that don't pay attention to the shapes of things, but instead to brightness, I think, well, it regulates melatonin, circadian clock, mood, hunger,
Starting point is 00:38:10 the really kind of vegetative stuff, if you will. And this is interesting because I think a lot of people experienced depression, not just people that live in Scandinavia and the middle of winter. And we are very much divorced from our normal interactions with light. It also makes me realize that these intrinsically photosensitive cells that set the clock, et cetera,
Starting point is 00:38:35 are involved in a lot of things. I mean, they seem to regulate a dozen or more different basic functions. I wanna ask you about a different aspect of the visual system now, which is the one that relates to our sense of balance. So I love boats, but I hate being on them. I love the ocean from shore
Starting point is 00:38:56 because I get incredibly seasick. It's just, it's awful. I think I'm gonna get seasick if I think about it too much. And once I went on a boat trip, I came back and I actually got motion sick or wasn't seasick because I was rafting. So there's a system that somehow gets messed up. They always tell us if you're feeling sick to look at the horizon, et cetera, et cetera.
Starting point is 00:39:18 So what is the link between our visual system and our balance system? And why does it make it snosh sometimes when the world is moving in a way that we're not accustomed to? I realize this is a big question because it involves eye movement, et cetera. But let's maybe just walk in at the simplest layers of vision vestibular or so-called balance system and then maybe we can piece the system together
Starting point is 00:39:45 for people so that they can understand. And then also we should give them some tools for adjusting their nausea when their vestibular system is out of whack. Cool. So, the first thing to think about is that the vestibular system is designed to allow you to see how you're or detect sense, how you're moving in the world through the world. It's a funny one because it's about your movement in relationship to the world in a sense, and yet it's sort of interoceptive in the sense that it is really in the end sensing the movement of your own body. So in inter in interception we should probably delineate for people is when you're focusing on your
Starting point is 00:40:30 internal state as if it's something outside you. Right, so it is a gravity sensing system. Well, it's partly a gravity sensing system. In the sense that gravity is a force that's acting on you as if you were moving through the world in the opposite direction. All right, now you got to explain that. You got to explain that one to me. OK, so basically the idea is that if we leave gravity aside, we're just sitting in a car in the passenger seat
Starting point is 00:41:03 and the driver hits the accelerator and you start moving forward. You sense that. If your eyes were closed, you'd sense it. If your ears were plugged in, your eyes were closed, you'd still know it. Yeah, many people take off on the plane like this. They're dreading the flight, and they know when the plane is taking off. Sure. That's your vestibular system talking because anything that jostles you out of the current
Starting point is 00:41:23 position you're in right now will be detected by the distributor system pretty much. So this is a complicated system, but it's basically in your inner, you know, ear, very close to where you're hearing. I think I put it there. And I don't know who they are. I really know. There's already arrived. I'm just getting this to steal our friend, Russ Van Gelder's explanation. We weren't consulted the design phase and no one knew. That's a great line.
Starting point is 00:41:48 That's a great line. But it's interesting it's in the ear. Yeah, it's deep in there and it's served by the same nerve, actually, that serves the hearing system. One way to think about it is both the hearing system and this vestibular self-motion sensing system are really detecting the signal in the same way. They're hairy cells, and they're excited.
Starting point is 00:42:13 Yeah, sort of, they got a little silly sticking up off the surfaces. And depending on which way you bend those, the cells will either be inhibited or excited. They're not even neurons, But then they talk to neurons with a neuron-like process and off you go. Now, you've got an auditory signal. If you're sensing things bouncing around in your cochlea, which is, you know, sympathetically, the bouncing of your ear drum, which is, you know, sympathetically, the sound waves in the world. But in the case of
Starting point is 00:42:40 the vestibular apparatus, evolution is built a system that detects the motion of, say, fluid going by those hairs. And if you put a sensor like that in a tube that's fluid filled, now you've got a sensor that will be activated when you rotate that tube around the axis that passes through the middle of it. Those, you know, we're just listening. Well, one people are in some way. You know, I think any sense, I was thinking of it as three hula hoops.
Starting point is 00:43:06 Right. Three hula hoops. One standing up, one lying down on the ground, that, you know, the other way. Three three directions. No. You know, the people who fly will talk about roll, pitch, and y'all, that kind of thing. So three axes of encoding, just like in the call.
Starting point is 00:43:21 Sort of the rest. No. No. And then I always say it's, and then the puppy head told. Yeah, the puppy head tells us that's the other one. So the point is that your brain is eventually going to be able to unpack what these sensors are telling you about how you just rotated your head in very much the way that the three types of cones we were talking about before
Starting point is 00:43:42 are reading the incoming photons in the wavelength domain differently. And if you agree here, you think we're in trust, you get red, blue. So it's the same basic idea. If you have three sensors and you array them properly, now you can tell if you're rotating your head left or right, up or down, that's the sensory signal coming back into your brain confirming that you've just made a movement that you will.
Starting point is 00:44:07 But what about on the plane? Because when I'm on the plane, I'm completely stationary. The plane's moving. Right. But my head hasn't moved. Right. So I'm just moving forward. Gravity is constant.
Starting point is 00:44:17 Exactly. So how do I know I'm accelerating? So what's happening now is your brain is sensing the motion. And the brain is smart enough also to ask itself, did I will that movement or did that come from the outside? So now in terms of sort of understanding what the the distributor signal means, it's got to be embedded in the context of what you tried to do
Starting point is 00:44:39 or what your other sensory systems are telling you about what's happening. I see. So it's very interesting, but it's not conscious. other sensory systems are telling you about what's happening. I see. So, that's very interesting, but it's not conscious. Or at least if it's conscious, it's very, it's not conscious. It's definitely very fast, right? The moment that plane starts moving, I know that I didn't get up out of my chair and run
Starting point is 00:44:56 forward. Right. But I'm not really thinking about getting up out of my chair. I just know. I guess the way I think about it is that the nervous system is, quote, aware at many levels. When it gets all the way up to the cortex and we're thinking about it, you're talking about it. You know, that's cortical, but the lower levels of the brain that don't
Starting point is 00:45:19 require you to actually actively think about it, there does doing their thing are also made aware, right? A lot of this is happening under the surface of what you're thinking. These are reflexes. Okay. So we've got this gravity sensing system for, I'm nodding for those that are listening, for a yes movement of the head, a no movement of the head, or the tilting of the head from side to side. Right. And then you said that knowledge about whether or not activation of that system comes from my own movements, or something acting upon me, like the plane moving, has to be combined with other signals.
Starting point is 00:45:57 And so how is the visual information, or information about the visual world combined with balance information? Right. So, yeah, I mean, I guess maybe the best way to think about how these two systems work together is to think about what happens when you suddenly rotate your head to the left. When you suddenly rotate your head to the left, your eyes are actually rotating to the right. Automatically, you do this in complete darkness. If you had an infrared camera and watched yourself in complete darkness, you can't see anything. Rotating your head to the left, your
Starting point is 00:46:32 eyes would rotate to the right. That's your vestibular system saying, I'm going to try to compensate for the head rotation, so my eyes are still looking in the same place. Why is that useful? Well, if it's always doing that, then the image of the world on your retina will be pretty stable most of the time. And that actually helps vision. Have they built this into cameras for image stabilization? Because when I move, when I take a picture with my phone, it's blurry.
Starting point is 00:47:02 It's not clear. Well, actually, you might want to get a better phone because now what they have is software in the better apps that will do a kind of image stabilization post-hawk by doing a registration of the images that are bouncing around. They say this the edge of the house was here. So let's get that aligned in each of your images. So you may not be aware if you're using a good new phone, that if you walk around a landscape and hold your phone, that there's all this image stabilization going on. But it's built into standard cinematic technology now, because we tried to do a hand-held camera, things would be bouncing around, things would be unwatchable, you wouldn't be able to really understand what's going on in the scene.
Starting point is 00:47:46 So the brain works really hard to mostly stabilize the image of the world under it. And of course, you're moving through the world so you can't stabilize everything. But the more you can stabilize most of the time, the better you can see. And that's why when we're scanning a scene, looking around at things, we're making very rapid eye movements for very short periods of time, and then we just rest. But we're not the only ones that do that. If you ever watch a hummingbird, it does exactly the same thing at a feeder, right? But it's like this body, each old thing.
Starting point is 00:48:18 It's going to make a quick movement. And then it's going to be stable. And when you watch a pigeon walking on the sidewalk, it does this funny head-bopping thing. What it's really doing is racking his head back on its neck while its body goes forward so that the image of the visual world stays static. Is that why they're doing it? Yes. And you've seen the funny chicken videos on YouTube, right?
Starting point is 00:48:40 You've taken chicken, move it up and down the head, stays in one place. It's all the same thing. All of these animals are trying hard to keep the image of the world stable on their retina as much of the time as they possibly can. And then when they've got a move, make it fast, make it quick, and then stabilize again. That's all the pigeons of their head back. It is. Yeah. Wow. Yeah. I mean, I just need to pause there for a second and digest that. Amazing. In case people aren't, well, there's no reason why people would know what we're doing here. But essentially what we're doing is we're building up from sensory, you know, light onto
Starting point is 00:49:16 the eye, make color to what the brain does with that, the integration of that, you know, circadian clock, melatonin, et cetera. And now what we're doing is we're talking about multi-sensory or multimodal combining one sense vision with another sense, balance. Right. And it turns out that pigeons know more about this than I do because pigeons know to keep their head back as they walk forward.
Starting point is 00:49:38 Right. All right, so that gets us to this issue of motion sickness. Right. And if you don't have to go out on a boat, anytime I go to New York, I sit in an Uber or in a cab in the back. And if I'm looking at my phone while the car is driving, I feel nauseous by time I arrive at my destination. I always try and look out the front of the windshield because I'm told that helps, but
Starting point is 00:50:02 it's a little tiny window. And I end up feeling slightly less sick if I do that. So what's going on with the vision and the balance system that causes a kind of a nausea? And actually, if I keep talking about the sitting, I don't throw up easily, but for some reason motion sickness is a real thing for me. It's a problem for a lot of people. I mean, I think the fundamental problem, typically, when you get motion sickness, is what they call visual vestibular conflict.
Starting point is 00:50:34 That is, you have two sensory systems that are talking to your brain about how you're moving through the world. And as long as they agree, you're fine. So if you're driving, you know, your body senses that you're moving forward, your vestibular system is picking up this acceleration of the car, and your visual system is seeing the consequences of forward motion in the sweeping of the scene past you. Everything is honky-dory, right? No problem.
Starting point is 00:51:03 But when you are headed forward, but you're looking at your cell phone, what is your retinasing? You're retinasing the stable image of the screen. There's absolutely no motion in that screen. Or the motion is just or some other motion. Or it's a movie or a game. If you're playing a game or you're watching a video, a football game, you know, the motion is uncoupled with what's actually happening to your body. Your brain doesn't like that. Your brain likes everything to be, you know, aligned. And if it's not, it's going to complain to you. So making me feel nauseous and maybe you'll change your behavior so you're getting... I'm getting punished.
Starting point is 00:51:37 Yeah. For setting it up so you're... You're a signal song. Right. By the vestibular... You'll learn. Visuals. In time. I love it. I love the idea of reward signals.
Starting point is 00:51:49 And we've done a lot of discussion about this on this podcast of things like dopamine reward and things, but also punishment signals. And I love this example. Well, maybe marching a little bit further along this pathway, visual input is combined with balance input. Where does that occur?
Starting point is 00:52:09 Maybe because I have some hint of where it occurs. You could tell us a little bit about this mysterious little mini brain that they call the cerebellum. The way I try to describe the cerebellum to my students Is that it serves sort of like the air traffic control system functions in air travel So that it's a system is very complicated and it's really dependent on great Information so it's taking in the information about everything that's happening everywhere, not only through your sensory systems, but it's listening into all the little centers elsewhere in your brain that are computing what you're going
Starting point is 00:52:53 to be doing next and so forth. So it's just roughness for that kind of information. So it really is like a little mini brain. It is. It's got access to all those signals. And it really has an important role in coordinating and shaping movements. But if you suddenly eliminated the aircraft control system, planes could still take off and land, but you might have some unhappy accidents in the process. So the cerebellum is gonna like that. It's not that you would be paralyzed if your cerebellum was gone
Starting point is 00:53:31 because you still have motor neurons, you still have ways to talk to your muscles. You still have reflex centers. And it's not that you would have any sensory loss because you still have your cortex getting all of those beautiful signals that you can think about. But you wouldn't be coordinating things so well anymore.
Starting point is 00:53:50 The timing between input and output might be off. Or if you were trying to practice a new athletic move like an overhead serve in tennis, you'd be just terrible at learning. You'd be just terrible at learning. But all of the sequences of muscle movements and the feedback from your sensory apparatus that would let you really hit that ball exactly where you wanted to after the nth rep, right now, the thousandth rep or something, you'd get much better at it. So the cerebellum's all involved in things like motor learning and refining the precision of movements so that they
Starting point is 00:54:26 get you where you want to go. If you reach for a glass of champagne, that you don't knock it over or stop short. You know, that's what you're good at. People who have selective damage to the cerebellum. Absolutely. And what I come familiar with, well,
Starting point is 00:54:43 Corsicaf is different, right? that a B-vitamine deficiency from in chronic alcoholics? Right. And they tend to walk kind of bowlegged and they can't coordinate their movements. Is that, that has some, that memory about it is also a cerebellum. I'm not sure about the cerebellum involvement there, but you know, the typical thing would be a patient who has a cerebral or stroke or a tumor, for example, might be not that steady on their feet. If the dynamics of the situation is standing on a street car with no hole to hold on to,
Starting point is 00:55:22 they might not be as good at adjusting all the little movements of the car. There's a kind of tremor that can occur as they're reaching for things because they reach a little too far and then they over-correct and come back, things like that. So it's very common neurological phenolinon, actually, cerebellular ataxia, this is what the neurologists call it, and it can happen not just with cerebellar damage, but damage to the tracks that feed the information
Starting point is 00:55:52 into the cerebellum, the private structure. Exactly, or output from the cerebellum. And so the cerebellum is where a lot of visual and balance information is combined. In a very key place in the cerebellum, which is, it's really one of the oldest parts in terms of that flocculus. In terms of that flocculus.
Starting point is 00:56:09 The flocculus, right. This is a critical place in the cerebellum where visual and distributed information comes together, recording just the kinds of movements we were talking about, this image stabilizing network. It's all happening there. And there's learning happening there as well, so that if your distributor apparatus is a little bit damaged somehow, your visual system is actually talking to your cerebellum, saying there's a problem here, there's an error,
Starting point is 00:56:36 and your cerebellum is learning to do better by increasing the output of the distributor system to compensate for whatever that loss was. So it's a little error correction system, that's sort of typical of a cerebellar function. And it can happen in many, many different domains. This is just one of the domains of sensory motor integration that takes place there. So I should stay off my phone in the ubers.
Starting point is 00:56:59 If I'm on a boat, I should essentially look and as much as possible act as if I'm driving the machine. Right. That'd be weird if I was in the passenger seat pretending I was driving the machine, but I do always feel better if I'm sitting in the front seat passenger. Right.
Starting point is 00:57:14 Some more of the visual world that you can see as if you were actually the one doing the motion. I would think, let's stay in the inner ear for a minute as we continue to march around the nervous system. When you take off in the plane or when you land or sometimes in the middle of there, your ears get clogged, or at least my ears get clogged. That's because of pressure build up in the various tubes of the inner ear, etc. We'll get into this. But years ago, our good friend, Harvey Carton,
Starting point is 00:57:46 is another world-class neuronatomist, gave a lecture and talked about how plugging your nose and blowing out versus plugging your nose and sucking in can, should be done at different times, depending on whether or not you're taking off or landing. And I always see people trying to unpop their ears. And when you do scuba diving, you learn how to do this without necessarily,
Starting point is 00:58:11 I can do it by just kind of moving my jaw now because it's done a little bit of diving. But what's the story there? We don't have to get into all the differences, atmospheric pressure, et cetera. But if I'm taking off and my ears are plugged, where I've recently ascended, planted off, my ears are plugged, do I plug my nose and blow out, or do I plug my nose and suck in? Right. So the basic idea is that if your ears feel bad, because you're going into an area
Starting point is 00:58:38 of higher pressure, so if they pressurize the cabin more than the pressure that you have on the surface of the planet, your eardrums will be bending in and they don't like that. If you push a more, they'll hurt even more. That's a good description that the pressure goes up and they're going to bend in. Bend in. And then the reverse would be true if you go into an area of low pressure. So if you started to drive up the mountain side, the pressure is getting lower and lower outside.
Starting point is 00:59:03 Now the air behind your drum is ballooning out. So it's just a question of, you're trying to get more pressure or less pressure behind the eardrum. And there's a little tube that does that and comes down to your, you know, back of your throat there. And if you force pressure up that tube, you're going to be putting more air pressure into the compartment to counter it. If it's not enough, and if you're sucking, you're going the other way. In reality, I think as long as you open the passageway,
Starting point is 00:59:30 I think the different pressure differential is gonna solve your problem. So I think you could actually blow in when you're not, quote, supposed to. Okay, so you could just blow your nose and blow air out or hold your nose and suck in the effect either way is fine. I think so. I mean, so you just I just won a hundred dollars from Harry.
Starting point is 00:59:49 Thank you very much. This is a lot. We Harvey nice to teach you anatomy together. And I'll say, I don't think it matters, but thank you, verse. I'll split. I'll split that with you. Okay. This is this is important stuff.
Starting point is 01:00:02 But but it's true. You hear this, you know, so So it doesn't matter either way. I'm no expert in this area. Don't quote me. He's not going to quote you. Okay, so we've talked about the inner ear and we've talked about the cerebellum. I want to talk about an area of the brain that is rarely discussed, which is the midbrain. And for those that don't know, the midbrain is an area beneath the cortex.
Starting point is 01:00:26 I guess we never really defined cortex. It was kind of the outer layers or are the outer layers of the least mammalian brain or human brain. But the midbrain is super interesting because it controls a lot of unconscious stuff, reflexes, etc. And then there's this phenomenon even called blind sight. So could you please tell us about the midbrain, about what it does and what in the world is blind sight? Yeah, so this is a, there's a lot of pieces there. I think the first thing to say is if you imagine
Starting point is 01:01:04 the nervous system in your mind's eye, you see this big honking brain and then there's this little one that dangles down into your vertebral column, the spinal cord, and that's kind of your visual impression. What you have to imagine is starting in the spinal cord and working your way up into this big magnificent brain. And what you would do as you enter the skull is get into a little place where the spinal cord kind of thickens out. It still has that sort of long, skinny trunk-like feeling. Sort of like a paddle or a spoon shake.
Starting point is 01:01:38 Right. It starts to spread out a little bit. And that's because your evolution is packed more interesting goodies in there for processing information and generating movement. because your evolution is packed more interesting goodies in there for processing information and generating movement. So beyond that is this tween brain we were talking about. So this link or brain, what Dianne's stuff really means the between brain?
Starting point is 01:01:56 Oh, I thought you said tween. Well, yes, yes. No, no, between, between. As I said, tween, tween. Yeah, it's the between brain. Yeah, the between brain is what the name means. It's the linker from the spinal cord in the periphery up to these grand centers of the cortex.
Starting point is 01:02:11 But this midbrain you're talking about is the last bit of this enlarged sort of spinal cordy thing in your skull, which is really the brain stem is what we call it. The last bit of that before you get to this relay up to the cortex is the midbrain. And there's a really important visual center there. It's called the superior colliculus. There's a similar center in the brains of other vertebrate animals, a frog, for example, or a lizard would have this. It's called the optic tectum there. But it's a center this is called the optic tectum there, but it's a center then in these non-mammalian vertebrates is really the main visual center. They don't really have what we would call a visual cortex, although there's something sort of like that, but this is where most of the action is in terms of
Starting point is 01:02:59 interpreting visual input and organizing behavior around that. You can sort of think about this region of the brainstem as a reflex center that can reorient the animal's gaze or body or maybe even attention to particular regions of space out there around the animal. And that could be all for all kinds of reasons. I mean, it might be a predator just showed up in one corner of the forest and you pick that up and you're trying to avoid it.
Starting point is 01:03:31 Or just any movement. Many movement, right? It might be, you know, that's suddenly, you know, something splats on the page when you're reading a novel and your eye reflexly looks at it. You don't have to think about that. That's a reflex. What if you throw me a ball, but Ily looks at it. You don't have to think about that. That's a reflex. What if you throw me a ball,
Starting point is 01:03:47 but I'm not expecting it, and I just reach up and try and grab it, catch it or not? Is that handled by the midbrain? Well, that's probably not the midbrain, although by itself, because it's gonna involve all these limb movements, this movement of your arm and body,
Starting point is 01:04:04 and it's probably- And probably throw them ahead. Sure, right. Things like that will certainly have a brain stem limb movements, this movement of your arm and body, and what about ducking, and something suddenly thrown in the head? Yeah, sure, right. Things like that will certainly have a brain stem component, a mid-brain component, you know, something looms and you duck. It may not be the superior colliculus we're talking about now. It might be another part of the visual mid-brain, but these are centers that emerged early in the evolution of brains like ours to handle
Starting point is 01:04:25 complicated visual events that have significance for the animal in terms of space, where is it in space? And in fact, this same center actually gets input from all kinds of other sensory systems that take information from the external world, from particular locations, and where you might want to either avoid or approach things according to their significance to you. So you get input from the touch system, you get input from the auditory system. I worked for a while in rattlesnakes, they get input from a part of their warm sensors on their face. They're in these little pits on the face. They work on baby rattlesnakes, right? Well, they were, they were adult sex.
Starting point is 01:05:05 Oh, I wasn't trying to diminish the danger. I thought for some reason they were little ones. No. Why in the world would you work on rattlesnakes? Well, because they have a version of an extra-acceptive sensory system that is they're looking out into the world using a completely different set of sensors. They're using the same sensors that would feel the warmth on your face if you stood in front of a bond fire,
Starting point is 01:05:27 except evolution has given them this very nice specialized system that lets them image where the heat's coming from. You can sort of do that anyway, right? If you walk around the fire, you can feel where the fire is from the heat hitting your face. Is that the primary way in which they detect prey? It's one of one of the major ways. And in fact, they use vision as well.
Starting point is 01:05:50 And they bring these two systems together in the same place, in this tactile regions, brain stem, membrane, what's all the tongue judding about when it snakes? That I don't know. That may be old factory. There may be. They're sniffing the air with their tongue.
Starting point is 01:06:03 Yeah, there may be a early year in our drive. You told me that flies actually taste things with their factory. They're sniffing the air with their tongue. Yeah, there may be a little dirt. Because we are in our drive, you told me that flies actually taste things with their feet. They do, yeah. That's so weird. Yeah, they have taste receptors in lots of funny places. I want to pause here just for one second before we get back into the midbrain. I think what's so interesting in all seriousness about taste receptors on feet, heat sensors, tongue shutting out of snakes,
Starting point is 01:06:29 and vision and all this integration is that it really speaks to the fact that all these sensory neurons are trying to gather information and stuff it into a system that can make meaningful decisions and actions, and that it really doesn't matter whether or not it's coming from eyes or ears or nose or bottoms of feet, because in the end it's just electricity flowing in.
Starting point is 01:06:53 So it sounds like it's placed on each animal. It's always feels weird to call fly an animal, but they are features. They are animals. It's placed in different locations on different animals, depending on the particular needs of that animal. Right. But how much more powerful if the nervous systems can also cross-quarrelate across sensory systems. So if you've got a weak signal from one sensory system, you're not quite sure there's something there, and a weak signal from another sensory system that's telling you the same locations is a little bit interesting. There might be something there. If you've got those
Starting point is 01:07:29 two together, you've got corroboration. Your brain now says it's much more likely that that's going to, you know, be something worth paying attention to. Right, so maybe I'm feeling some heat on one side of my face, and I also smell something baking in the oven. Right. So now, neither is particularly strong, but as you said, there's some corroboration. And that corroboration is occurring in the midbrain. Right. And then if you throw things into conflict, now the brain is confused, and that may be where
Starting point is 01:07:59 your motion sickness comes from. So it's great to have, you know, as a brain, it's great to have as many sources of information as you can have, just like if you're, you know, you're a spy or a journalist, you know, one is much information as you can get about what's out there. But if things conflict, that's problematic, right? Your sources are giving you different information about what's what's going on. Now you've got a problem on your hands. What do you publish? The midbrain is so fascinating. I don't want to eject us from the midbrain and go back to the vestibular system, but I do have a question
Starting point is 01:08:30 that I forgot to ask about the vestibular system, which is, why is it that for many people, including me, there's, despite my motion sickness and caps, that there's a sense of pleasure in moving through space and getting tilted relative to the gravitational pull of the earth. For me growing up it was skateboarding, but people like to corner in cars, corner on bikes.
Starting point is 01:08:51 It may be for some people it's done running or dance, but what is it about moving through space and getting tilted? A lot of surfers around here. Getting tilted that can tap into some of the pleasure centers. So do we have any idea why that would feel like? I have no clue. Is there dopamine energy can put to this system?
Starting point is 01:09:12 Well, the dopamine energy system gets a lot of places. It's pretty much, to some extent, everywhere in the cortex, a lot more in the front of the lobe, of course. But that's just for starters. There's basically dopamine-merging innovation in most places in the central nervous system. There's the potential for dopamine-merging involvement, but I really have no clue about the tilting phenomenon. I mean, people pay money to go on roller coasters.
Starting point is 01:09:39 Right. Well, I think that maybe is much about the thrill, is anything. Sure. And falling reflexes are very robust in all of us. Right. When the visual world is going up very fast, it usually means that we're falling. Right. Right. But in some people like that, some people don't.
Starting point is 01:09:51 Right. And kids tolerate a lot more, you know, sort of a distributor craziness spinning around until they drop. And I've friends, it always, you know, worries me a little bit that will they throw their kids. I'm not recommending anyone do this when they're low kids, you know, like throwing the kids really far back and forth. Some kids seem to love it. Yeah. Yeah, our son loved being shaken up and down very, very vigorously. That's the only thing that would calm him down sometimes. Interesting. Yeah, so I'm guessing we can guess that maybe there's some activation
Starting point is 01:10:27 of the reward systems from being moving through space. Well, I mean, if you think about how rewarding it is to be able to move through space and how unhappy people are who are used to that, who suddenly aren't able to do that, there is a sense of agency, right? If you can choose to move through the world and to tell, that's not only you're moving through the world, but you're doing it with a certain
Starting point is 01:10:48 amount of finesse, maybe that's what it is. You can feel like you're the master of your own movement in a way that you wouldn't if you're going straight. I'm just blowing smoke here, right? Yeah, well, we can speculate. That's fine. I couldn't help but ask the question. Okay, so if we move ourselves, pun intended, back into the midbrain, the midbrain's combining all these different signals for reflexive action. At what point does this become deliberate action? Because if I look at something I want and I want to pursue it, I'm going to go toward it and many times that's a deliberate decision. Right. So this gets very slippery, I think, because what you have to try to imagine is all these
Starting point is 01:11:31 different parts of the brain working on the problem of staying alive, you know, and surviving in the world. They're working on the problem simultaneously, and there's not one right answer to how to do that. But the one way to think about it is that you have high levels of your nervous system that are very well designed to override an otherwise automatic movement if it's inappropriate. So if you imagine you've been invited to tea with the queen and she hands you very fancy wedge wood, you know, teacup, very thin wedge wood teacup. Yes, with very hot tea in
Starting point is 01:12:14 it and you're burning your hand, you probably will try to find a way to put that back down on the saucer rather than just dropping it on the floor because you're with the queen. You're trying to be appropriate to that. So you have ways of raining in automatic behaviors if they're going to be maladaptive. But you also want the reflex to work quickly if it's the only thing that's going to save you, the looming object coming at your head. You don't have time to think about that. So this is the interplay in these hierarchically organized centers of the nervous system. At the lowest level, you've got the automatic sensors and centers and reflex, you know, arcs that will keep you safe, even if you don't have time to think about it. And then you've got the higher center saying, well, maybe we could do this as well, or
Starting point is 01:13:02 maybe we shouldn't do that at all, right? So you have all of these different levels operating simultaneously, and you need bidirectional communication between high level cognitive centers, decision making on the one hand, and these low level, very helpful reflexive centers, but they're a little bit rigid, a little hardwired, so they need some nuance. So there's, they're both of these things are operating in tandem in real time, all the time in our brains, and sometimes we listen more to one than the other. You've for people in sports talking about messing up at the play because they overthought it, you know, thinking too hard about it.
Starting point is 01:13:40 That's partly, you've already trained your cerebellum out of you to fastball right on the middle. Right. And if you start looking for something new or different, you're going to mess up your your reflexive swing. Right. If you're trying to think about the physics of the ball as it's coming at you, you've already missed, right? Because you know, you're not using your, all those reps have built a kind of knowledge is what you want to rely on. What do you know, have enough time to contemplate? This is important and a great segue for what I'd like to discuss next, which is the basal ganglia, this really interesting area of the brain that's involved in go type commands
Starting point is 01:14:23 and behaviors, instructing us to do things and no go, preventing us from doing things. Because so much of motor learning and skill execution and not saying the wrong thing or sitting still in class when or as you use with the, you know, T with the queen example, feeling discomfort involves suppressing behavior and sometimes it's activating behavior. A tremendous amount of online attention is devoted to trying to get people motivated. This isn't the main focus of our podcast. We touch on some of the underlying neural circuits of motivation, dopamine, and so forth,
Starting point is 01:14:58 but so much of what people struggle with out there are elements around failure to pay attention or challenges in paying attention, which is essentially putting the blinders on, you know, getting a soda straw view of the world and maintaining that for a bout of work or something of that sort, and trying to get into action. So of course, this is carried out by many neural circuits, not just the basal ganglia, but what are the basal ganglia and what are their primary roles in controlling go-type behavior and no-go-type behavior? Yeah, so I mean, the basal ganglia are sitting deep in what you would call the four brains, or the highest levels of the brain. They are sort of cousins to the cerebral cortex, which we talked about
Starting point is 01:15:47 is sort of the highest level of your brain that thing you're thinking with. The cerebral cortex being the refined cousins, and then you've got the, you know, the brute. Yeah. I mean, that's probably totally unfair, but that's all right. I like the basal ganglia. I can relate to the brudish parts of the brain. Well, a little bit of hypothalamus, a little bit of basal ganglia, sure.
Starting point is 01:16:07 We need it all. We need it all. And, you know, this area of the brain has gotten a lot bigger as the cortex has gotten bigger. And it's deeply intertwined with cortical function. The cortex can't really do what it needs to do without the help of the basal ganglia and vice versa. So they're really intertwined.
Starting point is 01:16:30 And in a way, you can think about this logically, is saying, you know, if you have the ability to withhold behavior or to execute it, how do you decide which to do? Well, the cortex is going to have to do that thinking for you. You have to be looking at all the contingencies of your situation and decide, is this a crazy move or is this a really smart investment right now or what? I don't want to go out for a run in the morning, but I'm going to make myself go out for a run. Or I'm adding a great time out on a run, and I know I need to get back, but I kind of want
Starting point is 01:17:01 to go another mile. Another great example is that the marshmallow test for the little kids, they can get two marshmallows if they hold off, just 30 seconds initially. They can have one right away, but if they can wait 30 seconds, they got two. So that's the no go, because our cortex is saying, I really like to have two more than having one,
Starting point is 01:17:22 but they're not gonna get the two unless they can not reach for the one. So they've got to hold off the action. And that has to result from a cognitive process. So the cortex is involved in this in a major way. Yes, I recall in that experiment, the kids used a variety of tools to somewhat distract themselves, particularly related to the kid that would just put himself right next to the marshmallows and then would, and then some of the kids covered their eyes, some of them would count or sing. Yeah, so that's all very cortical, right?
Starting point is 01:17:54 Coming up with a novel strategy, simple example that we're using here, but of course this is at play anytime someone decides they want to go watch a motivational speech or something just, you know, Steve Jobs commencement speech just to get motivated to engage in their day. Sure, I take this new job. You know, it's got great benefits, but it's an allows you part of the country. Why do you think that some people have a harder time running these go-no-go circuits and other people seem to have very low activation energy, we would say. They can just, you know, you have a task, they just lean into the task, whereas some people
Starting point is 01:18:28 getting into task completion or things that sort is very challenging for them. Yeah, I mean, I think it's really just another, it's a special case of a very general phenomenon, which is brains are complicated, and brains, you know, the brains we have are the result of genetics and experience. And my genes are different from your genes and my experiences are different from your experiences. So the things that are easier hard for us won't necessarily be aligned.
Starting point is 01:18:56 They might just happen to be just because they are. But the point is that you know, you're dealt with a certain set of cards, you're delta certain set of cards, you have certain set of genes, you are handed a brain, you don't choose your brain, it's handed to you. But then there's all this stuff you can do with it. You can learn to have new skills or to act differently or to show more restraint, which is kind of relevant to what we're talking about here.
Starting point is 01:19:25 Or maybe show less restraint if your problem is you're so buttoned down, you never have any fun in life and you should loosen up a little bit. Thank you. I appreciate the answer. Yeah. Yeah. David's always encouraged me to have a little more fun in life. So Basil Ganglia are kind of the disciplian, or they're sort of the instructor
Starting point is 01:19:47 or conductor of sorts, right? Go, no go, you know, you be quiet, you start now. I wish I knew more about the Basil Ganglia than I do. My sense is that, you know, this system is key for implementing the plans that get cooked up in the cortex, but they also influence the plans that the, you know, cortex is dishing out because this is a major source of information to the cortex. So it becomes almost impossible to figure out where the computation begins and where it ends and who's doing what because these things are all interacting in a complex network.
Starting point is 01:20:27 And it's all of it. It's the whole network. It's not, you know, one is the leader and the other is the follower. Of course, yeah, these are all the structures that we're discussing are working in parallel. Right. And there's a lot of changing crosstalk. I have this somewhat sick habit, David, every day I try and do 21 no-goes.
Starting point is 01:20:47 So if I want to reach for my phone, I try and not do it, just to see if I can prevent myself from engaging in that behavior. If it was reflexive, if it's something I want to do, deliberate choice, then I certainly allow myself to do it. I don't tend to have too much trouble with motivation, with go- type functions,
Starting point is 01:21:06 mostly because I'm so busy that I wish I had more time for more goes, but so to speak. But do you think these circuits have genuine plasticity? Absolutely. I mean, everybody knows how they've learned over time to wait for the two marshmallows, right? You don't have to have instant gratification all the time. You're willing to do a job sometimes,
Starting point is 01:21:29 it isn't your favorite job because it comes with a territory and you want the salary that comes at the end of the week or the end of the month, right? So we can defer gratification. We can choose not to say the thing that we know is going to inflame our partner and create a meltdown for the next week. We learn this control, but I think these are skills like any other. You can get better
Starting point is 01:21:53 at them if you practice them. So I think you're choosing to do that dyspontaneously. It's kind of a, you know, it's a mental practice. It's a discipline. It's a way of building a skill that you want to have. Yeah, I find it to be something that when I engage in a no-go type situation, then the next time, and the next time that I find myself about to move reflexively, there's a little gap in consciousness that I can make a decision whether or not this is really the best use of my time. Because I sometimes wonder whether or not all this business around attention, certainly there's the case of ADHD and clinical diagnosed ADHD, but all these, the issue around focus
Starting point is 01:22:31 and attention is really that people just have not really learned how to short-circuit a reflex. And so much of what makes us different than rattlesnakes or actually they could be deliberate, but from the other animals and is our ability to suppress reflex. Yeah. deliberate, but from the other animals is our ability to suppress reflex. Yeah. Well, that's the cortex. I mean, let's say the forebrain.
Starting point is 01:22:51 Cortex and basil ganglia working together, sitting on top of this lizard brain that's giving you all these great adaptive reflexes that help you survive. You just hope you don't get the surprising case where the thing that your reflex is telling you is actually exactly the wrong thing and you make a mistake, right? That's right. So that's what the cortex is for. It's adding nuance and context and experience, past association and in human beings, obviously learning from others through, you know, communication. Well, I was, you went right to it, and it was where I was gonna go. So let's talk about the cortex. We've worked our way up the so-called NIR axis as the Fisiinados will know.
Starting point is 01:23:33 So we're in the cortex. This is the seat of our higher consciousness, self-image, planning, and action. But as you mentioned, the cortex isn't just about that. It's got other regions that are involved in other things. So maybe we should, staying with vision, let's talk a little bit about visual cortex. You told me a story, an amazing story about visual cortex, and it was somewhat of a sad story, unfortunately, about someone who had a stroke to visual cortex.
Starting point is 01:23:59 Maybe you would share that story because I think it illustrates many important principles about what the cortex does. Right. So, you know, the visual cortex is, you could say the projection screen, the first, you know, place where this information streaming from the retina through this valamus, you know, connecting linker gets played out for the highest level of your brain to see. I mean, it's a representation. It's a map of things going on in the visual world that's in your brain. And when we describe a scene to a friend, we're using this chunk of our brain to be able to put words which are coming from a different part of our cortex to the objects and movements and colors that we see in the world. So you know, that's a key part of your visual experience when you when you can describe the things you're seeing you're looking at your visual cortex. And this is said I just had a quick question. So right now because I'm looking at your face. Right. As we're seeing, you're looking at your visual cortex. And this is, could I just ask a quick question?
Starting point is 01:25:06 So right now, because I'm looking at your face, as we're talking, there are neurons in my brain, more or less in the configuration of your face that are active as you move about. And what if I were to close my eyes and just imagine, I do this all the time by the way, David, close my eyes, and I imagine David Bersen's face. I don't tend to do that as often, maybe I should. But you get the point, I'm now using visualization of what you look like by way of memory. If we were to image the neurons in my brain,
Starting point is 01:25:41 would the activity of neurons resemble the activity of neurons that's present when I open my eyes and look at your actual face? This is a deep question. We don't really have a full account. It seems like an experience to do that. Yes, except you're talking about looking in detail at the activity of neurons in a human brain, and that's not as easy to do as it would be in a, you know, in some kind of animal model. But, you know, the bottom line is that you,
Starting point is 01:26:11 you have a spatial representation of the visual world, late as a map of the visual world, laid out on the surface of your cortex. The thing that's surprising is that it's not one map. It's actually dozens of maps. What do each of those maps do? Well, we don't really have a full accounting there either, but it looks a little bit like the diversification of the output neurons of the retina, the ganglion cells we were talking about before. There are different types of ganglion cells that are encoding different kinds of information
Starting point is 01:26:42 about the visual world. We talk about the ones that are encoding the brightness, but other ones are encoding motion or color, these kinds of things. The same kinds of specializations in different representations of the visual world and the cortex seem to be true. It's a complex story. We don't have the whole picture yet, but it does look as if some parts of the brain are much more important for things
Starting point is 01:27:04 like reaching for things in the space around you and other parts of the cortex are really important for making associations between particular visual things you're looking at now and their significance. What is that object? What can it do for me? How can I use it? What about the really specialized areas of cortex, like neurons that respond to particular faces, or neurons that can help me understand where I am relative to some other specific
Starting point is 01:27:35 object? Right. So, these are properties of neurons that are extracted from, detected by, recording the activity of single neurons in some experimental system. What's going on when you actually perceive your grandmother's face is a much more complicated question. It clearly involves hundreds and thousands and probably millions of neurons acting in a cooperative way.
Starting point is 01:28:02 So you can pick out any one little element in this very complicated system and see that it's responding differentially to certain kinds of visual patterns and you think you're seeing a glimpse of some part of the process by which you recognize your grandmother's face. But that's a long way from a complete description and it certainly isn't going to be at the level of a magic single neuron that has the special stuff to recognize your grandmother. It's going to be in some pattern of activity across many, many cells resonating in some kind of special way that will represent the internal
Starting point is 01:28:36 You know memory of your of your mother. So it was really incredible. Yeah, I mean that every time we do this deep dive Which we do from time to time you and I we kind of let march into the nervous system and Explore how different aspects of our of our life experiences is handled there and how it's organized the it After so many decades of doing this it still boggles my mind that that the collection of neurons one through seven, active in a particular sequence, gives the memory of a particular face and run backwards seven through to one, it gives
Starting point is 01:29:14 you a complete, you know, could be, you know, rattlesnake, pitviper, heat sensing organs to were talking about earlier. So it sounds, is it true that there's a lot of multi-purposing of the circuitry? Like we can't say one area of the brain does A and another area of the brain does B. So, you know, areas can multitask or have multiple jobs. They can moonlight. Right. But I think in my career, the hard problem has been to square that with the fact that things are specialized, that there are specific genes expressed in specific neurons that make them make synaptic
Starting point is 01:29:58 connections with only certain other neurons. And that particular synaptic arrangement actually results in the processing of information that's useful to the animal, to survive. So it's not as if it's either a big undifferentiated network of cells and looking at anyone is never going to tell you anything. That's too extreme on the one hand, nor is it the case that everything is hardwired, and every neuron has one function, and this all happens in one place in the brain. It's way more complicated and interactive and interconnected than that. So we're not hardwired or softwired. We're sort of, I don't know what the analogy should be,
Starting point is 01:30:36 what substance would work best, David? No idea there, but the idea is that it's always network activity. There's always many, many neurons involved, and yet there's tremendous specificity in the neurons that might or might not be participating in any distributed function like that. So you have to get your mind around the fact
Starting point is 01:30:56 that it's both very specific and very non-specific at the same time. It's a little tricky to do, but I think that's kind of where the truth lies. Yeah, and so this example that you mentioned to me once before about a woman who had a stroke in visual cortex, I think speaks to some of this. Right. Could you share with us that story? Sure. So the point is that you all, those of us who see have representations of the visual world and our visual cortex. What happens to somebody when they become blind because of problems in the eye, the retina
Starting point is 01:31:33 perhaps? You have a big chunk of the cortex, this really valuable real estate for neural processing, that has come to expect input from the visual system. And there isn't any anymore. So you might think about that as fallow land, right? It's just, it's unused by the nervous system. And that would be a pity, but it turns out that it isn't fact-used.
Starting point is 01:31:59 And the case that you're talking about is of a woman who was blind from very early in her life and who had risen through the ranks to a very high level executive, secretaryal position in a major corporation. And she was extremely good at rail reading and she had a braille typewriter and that's how everything was done. And apparently she had a stroke and was discovered at work, collapsed, and they brought it to the hospital.
Starting point is 01:32:30 And apparently the neurologist who saw her when she finally came to said, you know, I've got good news and bad news. Bad news is he had a stroke. The good news is that it was in an area of your brain you're not even using. It's your visual cortex, and I know you're blind from birth, so there shouldn't be any issue here. The problem was she lost her ability to read braille. So what appears to have been the case,
Starting point is 01:32:54 and this has been confirmed in other ways by imaging experiments in humans, is that in people who are blind from very early in birth, the visual cortex gets repurposed as a center for processing tactile information. And especially if you drain to be a good braille reader, you're actually reallocating somehow that real estate to your fingertips,
Starting point is 01:33:18 a part of the cortex that should be listening to the eyes. So that's an extreme level of plasticity, but what it shows is the visual cortex is kind of a general purpose processing machine. It's good at spatial information and the skin of your fingers is just another spatial sense. And deprived of any other input, the brain seems smart enough if you want to put it that way to rewire itself, to use that real estate for something useful in this case reading Braille.
Starting point is 01:33:49 Incredible. Somewhat tragic, but incredible. At least you're not in that case tragic. Very informative. Very informative. And of course, it can go the other way too, where people can gain function in particular modalities like improved hearing or tactile function in the absence of vision. in particular modalities, like improved hearing or tactile function in the absence of vision. Right.
Starting point is 01:34:05 Tell us about connectomes. We hear about genomes, proteomes, microbiomes, ohms, ohms, ohms these days. What's a connectome and why is it valuable? Yeah, so connectome actually now has two meanings. So I've only referred to the one that is my passion right now. And that is really trying to understand the structure of nervous tissue. At a scale that's very, very fine. Smaller than a millimeter way smaller than a millimeter, a nanometer or less, as that's a thousand times smaller, or essentially, a million times smaller. So really, really tiny.
Starting point is 01:34:54 On the scale of individual synapses between individual neurons or even smaller, like the individual synaptic vesicles containing a little packets of neurotransmitter that are going to get it released to allow one neuron to communicate to the next. So very, very fine. But the notion here is that you're doing this section after section at very fine scale. So in theory, what you have is a complete description of a chunk of nervous tissue that is so complete
Starting point is 01:35:26 that if you took enough time to identify where the boundaries are, all the cells are, you could come up with a complete description of the synaptic wiring of that chunk of nervous tissue because you have a complete description of where all the cells are and where all the synapses between where all the cells are. So now you essentially have a wiring diagram of this complicated piece of tissue. So the omics part is the exhaustiveness of it, rather than looking at a couple of synapses that are interesting to you from two different cell types, you're looking at all the synapses of all of the cell types, which of course is this massive avalanche of data, right? So in genetics, you have genetics and then you've genomics, which is the idea of getting the whole genome. All of it. And we don't really have an
Starting point is 01:36:10 analogous word for genetics, but it would be connectivity and kinomics. Right. I can't excuse me. I'm connic, Tomic. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure.
Starting point is 01:36:18 I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. I'm sure. It's wanting it all. And of course, it's crazy ambitious, but that's where it gets fun.
Starting point is 01:36:26 It's really a use of electron microscopy, a very high-resolution microscopic imaging system on a new scale with way more payoff in terms of understanding the connectivity of the nervous system. And it's just emerging, but I really think it's gonna revolutionize the field because we're to be able to query these circuits. How did they actually do it? Look at the hardware in a way that's never been possible before.
Starting point is 01:36:53 The way that I describe this to people is if you were to take a chunk of cold, cooked, but cold spaghetti, and slice it up very thin. You're trying to connect up spaghetti and slice it up very thin. You're trying to connect up each image of each slice of the edge of the spaghetti as figure out which ropes of spaghetti belong to which. And have a complete description of where this piece of spaghetti touches, that piece of spaghetti, there's something special there. Obviously, meat sauce is in all the other cell types and the pesto, you know, where it all is around the spaghetti, because those are the other cells, the blood vessels and the glesto, and all where it all is around the spaghetti because those are the other cell, the blood vessels and the glial cells.
Starting point is 01:37:28 So what's it good for? I mean, maps are great. I always think of connectomics and genomics and proteomics, et cetera, as necessary, but not sufficient. Right, right. So I mean, in many cases, what you do is you go out and probe the function and you understand how the brain does the function by finding neurons that seem to be
Starting point is 01:37:51 firing in association with this function that you're observing and little by little you work your way in and now you want to know what the connectivity is, maybe the anatomy could help you. But this connectomics approach, or at least the serial electron microscopy reconstruction of neurons, approach, really is allowing us to frame questions starting from the anatomy and saying, I see a synaptic circuit here. My prediction would be that these cell types would interact in a particular way. Is that right? And then you can go and probe the physiology and you might be right or you might be wrong. But more often than not, it looks like the structure is pointing us in the right direction. So in my case, I'm using this to try to understand a circuit that is involved in this image stabilization
Starting point is 01:38:36 network we're talking about, keeping things stable on the retina. And this thing will only respond at certain speeds of motion. These cells in the circuit, like slow motion, they won't respond to fast motion. How does that come about? Well, I was able to probe the circuitry. I knew what my cells looked like. I could see which other cells were talking to it. I could categorize all the cells that might be the players here that are involved in this mechanism of tuning the thing for slow speeds. And then we said, it looks like it's that cell type and we went and looked and the data bore that up.
Starting point is 01:39:12 But the anatomy drove the search for the particular cell type because we could see it connected in the right place to the right cells. So that creates the hypothesis that you go query the physiology, but it can go the other way as well. So it's always the synergy between these functional and structural approaches. It gives you the most lift. But, you know, in many cases, the anatomy has been a little bit the weak sister in this. The structure trying to work out the diagram because we haven't had the methods.
Starting point is 01:39:42 Now the methods exist. And this whole field is expanding very quickly, because people want these circuit diagrams for the particular part of the nervous system that they're working on. If you don't know the cell types and the connections, how do you really understand how the machine works? Yeah, what I love about is we don't know what we don't know. Right.
Starting point is 01:40:02 And scientists, we don't ask questions. We pose hypotheses, hypotheses don't ask questions, we pose hypotheses, hypotheses being, of course, some prediction that you wager your time on, basically. And it either turns out to be true or not true. But if you don't know that a particular cell type is there, you could never, in any configuration of life or a career or exploration of a nervous system wager a hypothesis because you didn't know it was there. So this allows you to say, ah, there's a little interesting little connection between this cell that I know is interesting and another cell that's a little mysterious but interesting. I'm going to hypothesize that it's doing blank, blank
Starting point is 01:40:41 and blank and go test that. And in the absence of these connect-tombs, you would never know that that cell was lurking there in the shadows. Right. Right. Yeah. And if you're just trying to understand how information flows through this biological machine, you want to know where things are. Neurotransmitters are dumped out of the terminals of one cell,
Starting point is 01:41:01 and they diffuse across the space between the two cells, which is kind of a liquid-y space. And they hit some receptors on the post-Naptic cell and they have some impact. Sometimes that's not through a regular synapse. Sometimes it's through a neuromodulator, like you often talk about on your podcast that are sort of losing, doping into the space between the cells, and it may be acting at some distance far from where it is released, right? But if you don't know where the releases happening and where other things are that might respond to that release, you're groping around in the dark.
Starting point is 01:41:32 Well, I love that you are doing this, and I have to share with the listeners that the first time I ever met David, and every time I've ever met with him in person, at least at his laboratory at Brown, he was in his office door closed, drawing neurons and their connections. And this is somewhat unusual for somebody who's a, you know, endowed full professor of chairman the department, et cetera, for many years to be doing the hands-on work. Typically, that's the stuff that's done by technicians or graduate students or postdocs. But I think it's fair to say that you really love looking at nervous systems and drawing the accurate renditions
Starting point is 01:42:14 of how those nervous systems are organized and thinking about how they work. Yeah, it's pure joy for me. I mean, I'm a very visual person. My wife is an artist. We look a lot of art together. Just the forms of things are gorgeous in their own right. But to know that the form is, in a sense, the function,
Starting point is 01:42:32 that the architecture of the connectivity is how the computation happens in the brain at some level, even though we don't fully understand that in most contexts. It gives me great joy, because I'm working on something that's both visually beautiful, but also deeply beautiful in it, sort of a higher sort of knowledge context. What is it? What is it all about? Love it. Well, as a final question, I get asked very often about how people should learn about neuroscience, or how they should go about pursuing maybe an education in neuroscience if they're at
Starting point is 01:43:10 that stage of their life or that's appropriate for their current trajectory. Do you have any advice to young people, old people, anything in between about how to learn about the nervous system, maybe in a more formal way? I mean, obviously obviously we have our podcasts that are other sources of neuroscience information out there. But for the young person who thinks they want to understand the brain, they want to learn about the brain, what should we tell them?
Starting point is 01:43:36 Well, that's a great question. And there's so many sources out there. It's almost a question of, you know, how do you deal with this avalanche of information out there? I mean, I think your podcast is a great way for people to learn more about the nervous system in an accessible way. But there's so much stuff out there, and it's not just that. I mean, the resources are becoming more and more available for average folks to participate in neuroscience research on some level. There's this famous iWire project of Syberspace.
Starting point is 01:44:02 Yeah, maybe tell us about iWire. Yeah, so that's connectomics. And that's a situation where a very clever scientist realized that the physical work of doing all this reconstruction of neurons from these electron micrographs is a lot of time involved. Many, many person hours have to go into that to come up with the map that you want of where the cells are. And he was very clever about setting up a context in which he could crowdsource this and people who are interested in getting a little experience looking at nervous tissue and participating in a research project could learn how to do this and do a little bit. From their living room. From their living room. They're like, well, we'll put a link to iWire.
Starting point is 01:44:44 That's it also is a great bridge between what we were just talking about, connectomics, and actually participating in research. And you don't need a graduate mentor or anything like that. Right. So more of this is coming. And I'm actually interested in building more of this.
Starting point is 01:44:59 So that people who are interested want to participate at some level don't necessarily have the time or resources to get involved in laboratory research, can get exposed to it and participate and actually contribute. So I think that's one thing. I mean, just asking questions of the people around you who know a little bit more and have them point you in the right direction, here's a book you might like to read. There's lots of great popular books out there that are accessible that will give you some more sense of the full range of what's out there in the neurosciences and how we can put some links to a few of those that we like on basic neuroscience. A good friend Dick Masland,
Starting point is 01:45:38 the late Richard people who called him Dick Masland had a good book. I forget the title at the moment. I had said sitting behind me somewhere over there on the shelf, but about vision and how nervous systems work. A pretty accessible book for the general public. Right. Right. So, you know, that, and, you know, there's so many sources out there. I mean, Wikipedia is a great way. If you had a particular question about visual function, I would say, by all me, it's, you know, you had the audience and get the first look and follow the references from there or go to your library or, you know, there's so many ways to get into it. It's such an exciting field now.
Starting point is 01:46:15 There's so many, I mean, any particular realm that's special to you, your experience, your, you know, your strengths, your passions, there's a field of neuroscience devoted to that. If you've got, if you know somebody who's got a neurological problem or a psychiatric problem, there's a branch of neuroscience that is devoted to trying to understand that and to solve these kinds of problems down the line. So, feel the buzz. It's an exciting time to get involved. Great. Those are great resources that people can access from anywhere, zero cost, as you need an internet connection, but aside from that, we'll put the links to some. And I'm remembering Dick's book is called We Know It When We See It.
Starting point is 01:46:56 Right. One of my heroes. Yeah. A wonderful colleague. Unfortunately, we lost a few years ago. But listen, David, this has been wonderful. It's been a blast. We really appreciate you taking the time to do this. People probably realize by now, you're an incredible wealth of knowledge about the entire nervous system. Today, we just hit this top contour of a number of different areas to give a flavor of the
Starting point is 01:47:19 different ways that the nervous system works and is organized and how that's put together, how these areas are talking to one another. What I love about you is that you're such an incredible educator and I've taught so many students over the years, but also for me personally as friends, but also any time that I want to touch into the beauty of the nervous system, I rarely lose touch with it, but any time I want to touch into it and start thinking about new problems and ways that the nervous system is doing things that I hadn't thought about, I call you, so I please forgive me for the calls past, present, and future, unless you change
Starting point is 01:47:54 your number. And even if you do, I'll be calling. I've been such a blast, Andy. This has been a great session, and it's always fun talking to you. It's always gets my brain racing. So thank you. Thank you. Thank you for joining me today for my discussion with Dr. David Berson. By now, you should have a much clearer understanding of how the brain is organized and how it works to do all the incredible things that it does. If you're enjoying and or
Starting point is 01:48:23 learning from this podcast, please subscribe to our YouTube channel. That's a terrific zero-cost way to support us. In addition, please subscribe to our podcast on Apple and Spotify. And on Apple, you have the opportunity to leave us up to a five-star review. As well, if you'd like to make suggestions for future podcast episode topics or future podcast episode guests, please put those in the comment section on our YouTube channel. Please also check out our sponsors mentioned at the beginning of each podcast. That's the best way to support us. And as mentioned at the beginning of today's episode, we are now partnered with Momentus
Starting point is 01:48:56 supplements because they make single ingredient formulations that are of the absolute highest quality and they ship international. If you go to livemomentus.com slash huberman, you will find many of the supplements that have been discussed on various episodes of the huberman lab podcast and you will find various protocols related to those supplements. If you're not already following huberman lab on Instagram and Twitter, feel free to do so. Both places I regularly post short video posts or text posts that give tools related to health and neuroscience and so forth.
Starting point is 01:49:26 And most of the time, that information is not overlapping with the information on the podcast. Again, it's just Huberman Lab on Instagram and Twitter. And last but not least, thank you for your interesting science. you

There aren't comments yet for this episode. Click on any sentence in the transcript to leave a comment.