Huberman Lab - Dr. Gina Poe: Use Sleep to Enhance Learning, Memory & Emotional State

Episode Date: February 13, 2023

My guest this episode is Gina Poe, PhD, a professor in the department of integrative biology & physiology at the University of California, Los Angeles (UCLA). We discuss her research exploring how sle...ep impacts learning, memory, hormones and emotions. She discusses tools to enhance your quality of sleep, increase deep sleep, rapid eye movement sleep and growth hormone release-- a key hormone for health, immune function and vitality. Dr. Poe explains how a specific brain area, the locus coeruleus, facilitates the processing of emotions, helps relieve traumas and how to maximize locus coeruleus function. She also explains sleep’s vital role in opiate addiction recovery and how anyone can determine their optimal sleep timing and duration. This episode is rich with basic science information and zero-cost tools to enhance quality and effectiveness of sleep for sake of mental health, physical health and performance. For the full show notes, visit hubermanlab.com. Thank you to our sponsors AG1: https://athleticgreens.com/huberman LMNT: https://drinklmnt.com/huberman Supplements from Momentous https://www.livemomentous.com/huberman Timestamps (00:00:00) Dr. Gina Poe (00:02:52) Sponsor: LMNT (00:06:58) Sleep Phases, Perfect Night’s Sleep (00:10:32) Can You Oversleep? (00:14:50) Sleep Cycles, Sleep Spindles, “Falling” Asleep, Dreams & Memories (00:19:01) Tool: Growth Hormone Release & Sleep (00:22:05) Adolescence; Early Sleep, Alcohol & Sleep Spindles (00:24:55) Middle Sleep States & REM, Schema, Waking at Night (00:30:33) Deep Sleep, Dreams & Senses (00:33:22) Sponsor: AG1 (00:34:37) Later Sleep, Paralysis, Sleepwalking, Sleep Talking (00:36:47) Alarm Clock & Grogginess; Sleep Trackers, Brain & Sleep (00:43:19) Early Slow Wave Sleep & “Washout”, Normal Sleep Cycle & Night Owls (00:54:30) Locus Coeruleus, Learning & REM Sleep (01:01:46) Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD), Locus Coeruleus & Sleep (01:07:31) Locus Coeruleus, Trauma & Sleep, Antidepressants, Norepinephrine (01:12:29) Locus Coeruleus, Bedtime & Novelty, Estrogen & Trauma (01:16:22)Sex Differences & Sleep (01:19:12) Tool: Non-Sleep Deep Rest (NSDR), Insomnia, Meditation, Prayer (01:27:42) Sleep Spindles, Learning & Creativity, P Waves & Dreaming (01:34:51) Lucid Dreams, Reoccurring Dreams, Trauma (01:44:11) Trauma Recovery, Locus Coeruleus & Norepinephrine, REM Sleep  (01:52:15) Opiates, Addiction, Relapse & Sleep (02:02:45) Zero-Cost Support, Spotify & Apple Reviews, YouTube Feedback, Sponsors, Momentous, Neural Network Newsletter, Social Media Title Card Photo Credit: Mike Blabac Disclaimer

Transcript
Discussion (0)
Starting point is 00:00:00 Welcome to the Huberman Lab podcast where we discuss science and science-based tools for everyday life. I'm Andrew Huberman and I'm a professor of neurobiology and Ophthalmology at Stanford School of Medicine. Today my guest is Dr. Gina Poe. Dr. Gina Poe is a professor in the department of Integrative Biology and Physiology at the University of California Los Angeles. Her laboratory and research focuses on the relationship between sleep and learning. In particular, how specific patterns of brain activity that are present during specific phases of sleep impact our ability to learn and remember specific types of information. For instance, procedural information, that is how to perform specific cognitive or physical tasks, as well as encoding of emotional
Starting point is 00:00:45 memories and discarding emotional memories. Indeed, her research focuses on how specific phases of sleep can act as its own form of trauma therapy, discarding the emotional tones of memories. In addition, her laboratory focuses on how specific phases of sleep impact things like the release of growth hormone. Growth hormone, of course, plays critical roles in metabolism and tissue repair, including brain tissue repair, and therefore has critical roles in vitality and longevity. Today, you will learn many things about the relationship between sleep, learning, emotionality, and growth hormone. One basic but very important takeaway that you'll learn about today, which was news to me,
Starting point is 00:01:23 is that it's not just the duration and depth of your sleep that matter, but actually getting to sleep at relatively the same time each night ensures that you get adequate growth hormone release in the first hours of sleep. In fact, if you require, let's say, eight hours of sleep per night,
Starting point is 00:01:40 but you go to sleep two hours later than your typical bedtime on any given night. You actually miss the window for growth hormone release. That's right. Getting growth hormone release in sleep, which is absolutely critical to our immediate and long-term health, is not a prerequisite of getting sleep, even if we are getting enough sleep. As Dr. Poe explains, there are critical brain circuits and endocrine that is hormone circuits
Starting point is 00:02:02 that regulate not just the duration and depth and quality and timing of sleep, but when we place our bout of sleep, that is when we go to sleep each night, plus or minus about a half hour or so, strongly dictates whether or not we will experience all the health promoting, including mind promoting benefits of sleep. Today's episode covers that and a lot more in substantial detail. You will learn, for instance, how to use sleep in order to optimize learning as well as forgetting for those things that you would like to forget. So during today's episode, Dr. Gina Poe shares critical information about not just neuroscience, but physiology and the hormone systems of the brain and body that strongly inform mental
Starting point is 00:02:38 health, physical health, and performance. So by the end of today's episode, you'll be far more informed about sleep and how it works, the different roles it performs, and you'll have several new actionable steps that you can take in order to improve your mental health, physical health, and performance. Before we begin, I'd like to emphasize that this podcast is separate from my teaching and research roles at Stanford. It is, however, part of my desire and effort to bring zero cost to consumer information about science and science-related tools to the general public. In keeping with that theme, I'd like to thank the sponsors of today's podcast. Our first sponsor is Element.
Starting point is 00:03:11 Element is an electrolyte drink with everything you need and nothing you don't. That means plenty of salt, magnesium, and potassium, the so-called electrolytes, and no sugar. Salt, magnesium, and potassium are critical to the function of all the cells in your body, in particular to the function of your nerve cells, also called neurons. In fact, in order for your neurons to function properly, all three electrolytes need to be present in the proper ratios. And we now know that even slight reductions in electrolyte concentrations or dehydration of the body can lead to deficits in cognitive and physical performance.
Starting point is 00:03:45 Element contains a science-backed electrolyte ratio of 1000 mg, that's 1g of sodium, 200 mg of potassium and 60 mg of magnesium. I typically drink element first thing in the morning when I wake up in order to hydrate my body and make sure I have enough electrolytes. And while I do any kind of physical training and after physical training as well, especially if I've been sweating a lot If you'd like to try element, you can go to drink element that's lmnt.com slash Huberman to claim a free element sample pack with your purchase again that's drink element lmnt.com slash Huberman And now for my discussion with Dr. Gina Poe. Dr. Gina Poe, welcome. Thank you. I've really been looking forward to this conversation
Starting point is 00:04:26 because I'm familiar with your work. And I know that many people are going to be excited to learn about your work as it relates to sleep, as it relates to problem solving, creativity, addiction and craving, relapse, and a number of other important topics. So to start things off, I would love for you to educate us a bit about this thing that we are all familiar with, and yet very few of us understand, which is sleep.
Starting point is 00:04:51 And if you would, could you describe the various phases of sleep that exist, what distinguish them, and perhaps frame this within the context of what would a perfect night sleep look like? Okay. How long would it last, more or less, and a perfect night sleep look like? Okay. How long would it last? More or less. And what would the biology look like?
Starting point is 00:05:10 What is a perfect night sleep? Oh yeah, that's a great question. All right, so sleep is really different from wakefulness. And in fact, can't be replaced by any state of wakefulness that we've been able to come up with so far. Our brain chemistry is completely different, and in the different stages of sleep, which there is non-REM and REM,
Starting point is 00:05:29 are the two major states of sleep, and every animal we've studied so far seems to have both of those states. Anyway, those two states are entirely different from one another, too. And even within non-REM, there are three states, stage one, which is what you slip into, when you first falling asleep, it's dozing.
Starting point is 00:05:47 There's kind of an interesting rhythm that goes on in the brain. It's kind of a fast gamma rhythm. And then there's stage two, which is a really cool state. We sort of used to ignore sleep researchers because it was a transient state between wakefulness and the deep stage three slow-wave sleep, which is the most impressively different. And then, and between that and REM sleep. So stage two, I'll talk a little bit more about, and then the deep slow-wave sleep state, which is when big slow-wave sweep through our brain, and now we realize that it cleans our brain. One of the things that those big slow-waves do is
Starting point is 00:06:20 cleans our brain and does other really important things to restore us from a day of wakefulness. And then REM sleep, which is the most popular because that's where we have the most active dreams. And when you wake up someone out of REM sleep, they'll almost always report having dreams, something really bizarre. That's called REM sleep, rapid eye movement sleep. So those are the four states of sleep, of human sleep, and we cycle through them every 90 minutes or so. When we go to sleep, say 10, 10, 30, 11 o'clock, our first REM sleep period comes about 105 minutes
Starting point is 00:06:56 after we fall asleep and lasts about 20 minutes. Actually, it comes about 95 minutes in last 10 or 15 minutes. And then we start over again. And we have about five of those per night for a perfect night's sleep, four or five, something like that. So a perfect night's sleep is seven and a half, eight hours. There was a really great study that put people in a semi-darkened room with nothing but the bed for 12 hours every day for a month. And what people did initially is because we're in a sleep deprived nation is that they slept a lot more than usual, like 10 or 11 hours of the 12.
Starting point is 00:07:35 And then they leveled off after a week or two to about 8 hours and 15 minutes of sleep. So you actually can't over sleep. I mean, they had nothing else to do but sleep and they would round off to on average eight hours and 15 minutes a night and then spend the rest of the time twiddling their thumbs, humming tunes. Interesting. I want to get back to the contour of a perfect night sleep, but I'm intrigued by this idea that people can't over sleep. I'm often asked whether or not we can get too much sleep and whether or not sleeping too long, excuse me, can make us groggy the next day. Is there anything to that? And how does one determine how long they should sleep on average? On average. Yeah. Well, that's interesting, because different people need, seem to need different amounts of sleep, But we don't really even know exactly what sleep is for. So what they need is kind of... it's... you know, murky, murky. So we do know a lot of things that sleep does now for us, but we don't know how long those things take. So how long we need to
Starting point is 00:08:41 sleep is also just a big question mark. But some people don't feel rested until they've slept nine hours. Some people don't feel rested after three or four and a half. But most people, if they consistently deprive themselves of sleep so that they're only sleeping four, four and a half hours a night, build up cognitive deficit that just builds up over time the more nights you have with sleep deprivation, the more cognitive deficit you have. And so you need sleep again to sleep more to recover. Now the question you had about... Can you over sleep? Can you sleep to the point where it's too much, you know,
Starting point is 00:09:20 that we... I growing up when I was in high school my girlfriend's dad had this belief that no one should sleep in past 6 a.m. So he would he'd wake all the There were two children in that home. He would wake up the kids in that house He had to sing against over sleep and regardless of when people went went to sleep And I always thought that was an interesting mentality. Yeah It's not terrible actually because what that will do is it'll put you make you sleep your the next night to get to bed on time. So it'll build up your homeostatic need if you wake up too early. But so I don't think you can over sleep, but people who sleep a lot like people who sleep
Starting point is 00:09:58 over nine hours, it's probably indicative of some other problem because in fact, if you have a lot of different conditions, it will cause you to sleep a lot more probably because of what it does as it interferes with your efficient sleep, the efficiency of your sleep. So if you find yourself sleeping consistently nine hours plus every night, then you might want to consult a doctor about maybe what else it might be. It could be cancer. It could be sleep apnea that affects a lot of people. It could be that your sleep is super inefficient because
Starting point is 00:10:31 you're snoring a lot more than you know or you're waking up a lot more than you know every night. So you might want to sleep study just to see how your sleep is and then see what else might be causing you to sleep so much. And that wouldn't be if somebody is sleeping nine or ten hours, you know, every once in a while. You mean if they're consistently sleeping from more than nine hours? If they feel like they needed an ordered function cognitively the next day, it might be that your sleep is just not efficient and you might want to look into why that's the case. Interesting. Forgive me for the anecdote, but I can't resist.
Starting point is 00:11:05 Years ago, I went to an acupuncturist and he gave me these red pills, of which I don't know what they contained. But I took them because he told me they would help with my sleep. And I would fall asleep about 30 minutes after taking them. And I would have incredibly, excuse me, vivid dreams and I'd wake up four or five hours after having gone to sleep, feeling completely rested.
Starting point is 00:11:30 Something that I've never really experienced on a consistent basis. I want to do mass spec on these pills. I still have no idea what was in them whatsoever. I want to do mass spec on these pills exactly. Some people have thought that perhaps they had GHB, Gamahydroxybutyrate, which is by the way, an illegal drug, it can kill you. It's not, which is, by the way, an illegal drug.
Starting point is 00:11:45 It can kill you. It's not something you want to take. No, that's not. But anyway, if ever someone can figure out what the red pills were, I'll be very... That's really great. And this is not a red pill of the other sort, red pill. This is just the red sleep pills.
Starting point is 00:11:58 Interesting. I mean, it could have been even a placebo effect, because placebo is extremely strong. So... Although, I don't know, there was really something to these red pills. So, shout out to the acupuncturists and the Eastern medicine. But to return this idea of the architecture
Starting point is 00:12:16 of a perfect night's sleep. So, you said, we fall asleep, the first 90 minutes of sleep, REM sleep, rapid eye movement sleep, will arrive at about 95 minutes in. Does that mean that the rest of that 90 minutes is consumed with slow wave sleep? Yeah, non-rem sleep. Okay, and what about the sleep where we are lightly asleep?
Starting point is 00:12:38 And we might have a dream that has us somehow thinking about movement or that we jolt ourselves awake. That often happens early in the night. Yeah, yeah, that's the first stage, stage one that has us somehow thinking about movement or that we jolt ourselves awake. That often happens early in the night. Yeah, that's the first stage one and stage two asleep. And stage two sleep is really cool because that has something called
Starting point is 00:12:55 sleep spindles and K complexes. And what sleep spindles are a little bit of activity that's 10 to 15 hurts in frequency. It's a conversation between the thalamus and the cortex. The gap thalamus is the gateway to consciousness and the neocortex, you know, processes, all our cognition. And so it's these spindles, they're called sleep spindles. And if you wake up out of that state, you will often report a dream, like a hallucination-style
Starting point is 00:13:27 dream. It won't be a long dream report, like you have out of REM sleep, but it will be some hallucination state. And during, while we're falling asleep, one of the reasons we call it falling asleep is because in stage one and stage two, our muscles are relaxing. And if there's part of our brain that's conscious enough to sort of recognize that relaxation, we'll feel like we're falling and we'll jerk away. So we'll often that hallucination, it's called hypnagogic hallucination, we'll feel like it'll include some falling aspect,
Starting point is 00:13:58 but we'll wake up out of. That's really interesting to me. a long felt that sensation of almost like dropping back into my head, so much so that if I elevate my feet just slightly and I tilt my head back just slightly in order to go to sleep, I find that I fall asleep much faster. But it does feel as if I'm going to fall like almost going to do a backward, somersault. That's fast, really. Actually, really like the sensation. And usually because it proceeds falling deeply. Yeah, that's really interesting.
Starting point is 00:14:25 I'm somebody has to do a study of elevated feet. And yeah, there's a little bit on body position and sleep and some of the washout that we'll talk about. So early in the night, you got these lighter stages of sleep, less rapid eye movement sleep. What can we say about the dreams that occur during the say first and second, you know, 90-minute cycles of sleep? Are they quite different than the patterns of sleep and dreaming that
Starting point is 00:14:52 occur later in the night or toward morning? Well, okay, that's an interesting question. There's a lot of facets to it. There is some evidence that the first four hours of sleep are very important for memory processing. And in fact, if you've learned something new that day or have experienced a new sensory motor experience, then your early sleep dreams will incorporate that experience much more than the later sleep dreams. Later is that memory gets consolidated from the early structures, which are the hippocampus deep and the temporal lobe to the cortex in a distributed fashion. That memory seems to move from that hippocampus to the cortex and also the dreams that incorporate that, also move later in the night. So nobody knows why,
Starting point is 00:15:46 but there was a great study by Siddhartha Rubeiro, who studied the consolidation of memories from the hippocampus to the cortex in a rat across the period of a full day sleep because rat sleep in the daytime. And he found that each subsequent REM sleep period moved that memory from the hippocampus to the first area that projects to it and then the second area and then the third area and you can see
Starting point is 00:16:16 the memory moving through throughout the sleep. Very cool. So I have to read that study. So there's a number of different hormones associated with the different stages of sleep. We know that melatonin is a hormone of nighttime. That makes us sleepy. What about growth hormone release? When does that occur during sleep? So growth hormone release happens all day long and all night long. But the deep, slow-wave sleep that you get the very first long and all night long. But the deep slow-a-sleep that you get the very first sleep cycle is when you get a big bolus of growth hormone release and in men and women equally. And if you miss that first deep slow-a-sleep period, you also miss that big bolus of growth hormone release. And you might get ultimately across the day just as much overall growth
Starting point is 00:17:05 hormone release. But endocrinologists will tell you that big boluses do different things than a little bit eaked out over time. So that is, well we know there's also a big push to synthesize proteins. So that's when the protein synthesis part that builds memories, for example, in our brain happens in that first cycle of sleep. So you don't want to miss that, especially if you've learned something really big and needs more synaptic space to encode it. How would somebody miss that first 90 minutes?
Starting point is 00:17:40 They're striving themselves. Yeah, so. So let's say I normally go to sleep at 10 p.m. And then from 10 to 1130 would be this first phase of sleep, and that's when the growth hormone, big bolus of growth hormone would be released. Does that mean that if I go to sleep instead at 1130 or midnight that I missed that first phase of sleep? Why is it not the case that I get that first phase of sleep just simply starting later. It is a beautiful clock that we have in our body that knows when things should happen. And every cell in our body has a clock and all those clocks are normally synchronized and those circadian clocks are synchronized.
Starting point is 00:18:18 And so our cells are ready to respond to that growth hormone release at a particular time. And if we miss it, and it's a time in relation to the melatonin also. So if you miss it, yeah, you might get some growth hormone release, but it's occurring at a time when that your clock is already moved to the next phase. And so it's, it's just a clock thing. Yeah, I don't think we can overstate the importance of what you just described. And to be honest, despite knowing a bit about the sleep research in circadian biology,
Starting point is 00:18:49 this is the very first time that I've ever heard this, that if you normally go to sleep at a particular time and growth hormone is released in that first phase of sleep, that you can't simply initiate your sleep about later and expect to capture that first phase of sleep. Yeah. That's incredible and I think important.
Starting point is 00:19:07 And as many listeners are probably realizing, also highly actionable. So what this means is that we should have fairly consistent bed times. In addition to fairly consistent wake times, is that right? Exactly. And in fact, one of the best markers of good neurological health when we get older is consistent bed times. Wow. Okay. I don't want to backtrack, but I did write down something that I think is important for me to resolve, or for you to resolve. So I'm going to ask this, people that sleep nine hours or more, perhaps that reflecting an issue, some underlying issue, perhaps, is being a teenager or an adolescent
Starting point is 00:19:47 and undergoing a stage of development where there's a lot of bodily and brain growth and exception to that because, yes. I don't recall sleeping a ton when I was a teenager. I had a ton of energy, but I know a few teenagers and they sleep a lot. They'll just sleep and sleep and sleep and sleep.
Starting point is 00:20:05 Yeah. Should we let them sleep and sleep and sleep? Yes. Okay. So that's the one exception. Just like babies. Okay. When you're developing something in your brain or the rest of your body, you really need sleep
Starting point is 00:20:16 to help organize that. I mean, sleep is doing really hard work in organizing our brains and making it develop right. And if we deprive ourselves of sleep, we will actually also, just like I said, we have a daily clock. We also have a developmental clock. And we can miss a developmental window if we don't let ourselves sleep extra like we need to.
Starting point is 00:20:38 What other things inhibit growth hormone release or other components of this first stage of sleep? In other words, if I go to sleep religiously every night at 10 pm, are there things that I perhaps do in the preceding hours of the preceding day like in just caffeine or alcohol that can make that first stage of sleep less effective even if I'm going to sleep at the same time? Alcohol definitely will do that because alcohol is a REM sleep suppressant and it even suppresses some of that stage two transition to REM with those sleep spindles. And those sleep spindles, we didn't talk about their function yet, but they're really
Starting point is 00:21:11 important for moving memories to our cortex. It's a unique time when our hippocampus, the sort of like the ram of our brains, writes it to a hard disk, which is the cortex. And it's a unique time when they're connected. If you don't want to miss that, you don't want to miss REM sleep when it is also a part of a consolidation process and schema-changing process. Alcohol in there, before we go to sleep, we'll do that. Until we've metabolized alcohol and put it out of our bodies, it will affect our sleep badly.
Starting point is 00:21:46 So probably fair to say no ingestion of alcohol within the four to six hours proceeding sleep given the half-life or at all or at all would be better. But I know some people refuse to go that way. Maybe a little bit is okay. I don't know what those dose responses, but there are studies out that you can look at. Great. So, we're still in the first stage of sleep, and I apologize for slowing us down, but it sounds like it's an incredibly important first phase of sleep. What about the second and third 90 minute blocks of sleep?
Starting point is 00:22:19 Is there anything that makes those unique? What is their signature besides the fact that they come second and third in the night? There's more and more REM sleep the later the night we get. There's also a change in hormones, you know, the growth hormone and melatonin levels are starting to decline, but other hormones are picking up. So it is a really different stage that you also don't want to short-change yourself on. And I think that's the stage. Many studies are showing that those are the
Starting point is 00:22:50 times in sleep when the most creativity can happen. That's when our dreams can incorporate and put together old and new things together into a new way. And our schema are built during that time. So yeah, we can change our minds best during those phases of sleep. Could you elaborate a little bit more on schema? No one, I don't think anyone on this podcast has ever discussed schema. I'm a little bit familiar with schema from my courses on psychology, but it's been a while. So maybe you could just refresh mine and everyone ever knows it's still a concept. Sure maybe you could just refresh mine and everyone ever drinks this room. Well, it's still a concept. Sure. How do you define schema?
Starting point is 00:23:28 Right. I think of schema as, like, we have a schema of Christmas. We have all kinds of ideas that we so together and call Christmas, a holiday season in the Northern Hemisphere. It's cold. We have Santa Claus and reindeer and jingle bells and even things that are false,
Starting point is 00:23:51 but we normally associate with Christmas presents, family gathering, when it is, all of this stuff is sewn together until one, there's a thread linking them all and we can just give ourselves a list of words and none of them contain the word Christmas linking them all and we can just give ourselves a list of words and And none of them contain the word Christmas and then ask people later You know give them another list of words and include the word Christmas and they'll say oh, yeah that word was there because in their minds They brought up that word Christmas because it's part of that whole schema. So that's what it's sort of a related
Starting point is 00:24:24 Lot of related concepts, I guess. I think about sort of like the desktop of my computer would scare some people, but it's just a ton of folders. But each of the folder names means something very clear and specific to me. Right. And inside of those folders are collections of things that make sense in terms of how they're batch. Is that one way to think about? Exactly. No, that's a great way to think of it. And when you're in REM sleep in the later parts of the night and that transition to REM, that's when your computer of your brain is opening folders and comparing documents. Seeing if there is anything the same, these two documents look very much the same, but there's a little bit of difference and it can link those conceptually so that that's probably one of the origins
Starting point is 00:25:06 of creativity is finding things that are related, maybe just linked a little bit and you can find that link and strengthen it if it makes your scheme interesting and different. Very interesting. Many people including myself tend to wake up maybe once during the middle of the night to use the restroom. I've tried to drink less fluid before going to sleep. I've heard also that the impulse to urinate, forgive the topic, but a lot of people deal with this. So the impulse to urinate is also dictated
Starting point is 00:25:38 by how quickly you drink fluid, not just the total volume. So I've switched to sipping fluids more slowly for my last beverage of the day, which seems to help. But the point here is that I think a lot of people wake up once in the middle of the night, oftentimes to use the restroom, but oftentimes just around 3 a.m. and might be up for a few minutes, hopefully not on their phone or viewing any bright light, which can cause more wakefulness. But then go back to sleep. Is there any known detriment to this middle of the night waking? Or should we consider it a normal feature
Starting point is 00:26:07 for some people's sleep architecture? Yeah, I think we shouldn't worry about it. Actually, I think sleep is really incredibly well-homestatically regulated. And so, really don't worry about how much you're sleeping as long as you're not intentionally depriving yourself of sleep by doing something really rewarding and exciting because even that is stressful to your body and deprives you of a lot of things we're
Starting point is 00:26:28 talking about. So, don't worry about it. It's absolutely normal to wake up at least once in the middle of the night to go to the bathroom and as long as you can get back to sleep in a reasonable amount of time, you know, or even if it takes you an hour. Don't worry about it as long as you have a lifestyle that allows you to then make up that sleep either the next morning or the next night, or going to bed a little earlier. So if I understand correctly, there's a little bit of asymmetry to sleep that catching that
Starting point is 00:26:57 first phase of sleep, it's like you either get it or you don't and you have to get it by going to sleep. Essentially the same time each time, maybe plus or minus 15 minutes or so. But then if I wake up in the middle of the night and go back to sleep, I can not catch up, but I can gather all the sleep that I would have gotten had I just slept the whole way through the night. Is that right? Yeah, yeah. And we don't know, actually the answer to whether or not the sleep in the middle between
Starting point is 00:27:22 that early sleep and the late sleep is, in fact, different for another reason. And whether depriving yourself of sleep from, say, one to two thirty in the morning is bad in a different way. We don't know. Well, I suppose I am the experiment in that case, because I do tend to wake up once per night, and I've sort of come to recognize it as part of my normal sleep architecture. I don't obsess over it. I do notice that when I go back to sleep and especially toward morning
Starting point is 00:27:48 That my sleep is incredibly deep my dreams are incredibly vivid. I don't always remember them But what is unique perhaps about the architecture of dreams and sleep in the let's say the last third of the night Or this or the second half of the night right? Yeah, the second third of the night or the second half of the night. Right. Yeah. And the second half of the night, we have longer REM sleep periods. And those are considered the deepest sleep, even though slow wave sleep, big slow wave is considered deep. It is deep. Yeah. They call slow wave sleep deep sleep and REM sleep rapid. I mean, but now you're telling me that REM sleep is actually the deeper sleep. Okay. There needs to be a new nomenclature sleep researchers. I really should call it deeper. No, no, please. The reason why you call slow-wave sleep deep sleep is because it's difficult to arouse people out of that state. And when you do arouse them out of
Starting point is 00:28:34 that state, they're most often confused and just want to go back into sleep and can go back pretty easily. If you arouse someone out of REM sleep, they're more likely to report something that was really kind of almost like wakefulness. It was so vivid. But in fact, if you give someone a non-threatening kind of stimulation, like somebody dropping keys or a ping or something like that, instead of waking that same volume will wake someone up out of non-rim sleep, but out of REM sleep and instead lengthen the amount of time or make it even more dense and we're rapid, I move with more dense, and often people will incorporate that sound
Starting point is 00:29:15 into their dreams. So the body and brain are somehow conscious of the sound, and I've heard also smells, can even make it into our dreams and REM sleep, but that we, it doesn't rouse us. It doesn't rouse us as often. Yeah. And maybe one of the reasons why REM sleep is deeper, especially in adults and older people that deep slow-wave sleep goes away. So it's not as deep, it's not as big the slow waves aren't as large, which is probably problematic, but we are not sure. And so then REM sleep becomes the deepest stage.
Starting point is 00:29:51 Actually, in children, it's kind of a toss-up because they, it's really hard to wake them up out of that deep slow-wave sleep. And in fact, fire alarms don't wake them up, even really loud fire alarms out of that state of sleep. So that's why they're trying to change fire alarms so that instead of something that the kids don't associate with anything like the rrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrrr maybe less loud but more salient to them and we'll wake them up. I don't know, having carried sleeping children in from the car. I don't know that I want children to start waking up from sleep because that's one of the best things when you get home and the kids are asleep in the back seat.
Starting point is 00:30:33 You can literally throw them over your shoulders, gently, of course, and put them to sleep in it and they are completely out. Yeah, it's wonderful. It is wonderful. One of nature's gifts. I'd like to take a quick break and acknowledge one of our sponsors, Athletic Greens. Athletic Greens, now called AG1, is a vitamin mineral probiotic drink that covers all of your
Starting point is 00:30:54 foundational nutritional needs. I've been taking Athletic Greens since 2012, so I'm delighted that they're sponsoring the podcast. The reason I started taking Athletic Greens and the reason I still take Athletic Greens once or usually twice a day is that it gets to be the probiotics that I need for gut health. Our gut is very important. It's populated by gut microbiota that communicate with the brain, the immune system, and basically all the biological systems of our body to strongly impact our immediate and long-term health. and those probiotics and athletic greens are optimal
Starting point is 00:31:25 and vital for microbiotic health. In addition, athletic greens contains a number of adaptogens, vitamins, and minerals that make sure that all of my foundational nutritional needs are met, and it tastes great. If you'd like to try athletic greens, you can go to athleticgreens.com slash Huberman, and they'll give you five free travel packs that make it really easy to mix up athletic greens while you're on the road in the car on the plane, etc. And they'll give you a year supply of vitamin D3 K2 again, that's at lettagreens.com slash Huberman to get the five free travel packs and the year supply of vitamin D3 K2. So this enhanced volume or proportion of rapid eye movement sleep in the second half of the night relates to more elaborate dreams. We are paralyzed during REM sleep, correct?
Starting point is 00:32:11 Yes, normally paralyzed and that's really good because that's the time when we're actively dreaming storyline dreams and we could hurt ourselves. We were actually really cut off from the outside world in terms of, you know, responding to say this table or window or door. And so different from sleepwalking, which is out of slow-way sleep. Out of slow-way sleep, that sleepwalking is a mixture between sleep and wakefulness. So you actually will respond to the door. You can cook a full meal, drive your car while you're in deep slow asleep. It's scary because you never know what you're going to do.
Starting point is 00:32:49 You don't have voluntary control over it. You don't have no conscious control over it. But you can actually safely navigate some situations in sleepwalking and actually have a conversation, although it may not make much sense when you're sleep talking. In REM sleep, you're not processing the outside world. And instead, when you're acting out your dreams, you could be doing things like walking through a plate
Starting point is 00:33:15 class window or falling off of down the stairs, things like that. So you really want your muscles to be inactivated during REM sleep. Otherwise, you will act out those dreams and really hurt yourself or your bed partner. What about sleep talking or talking in sleep? I don't know how many relationships have been saved by sleep talking, but I'm guessing a few have been destroyed. And I'm guessing that talking in sleep could have meaning or perhaps has
Starting point is 00:33:45 no meaning just as dreams could have meaning or no meaning as we recall them. Yeah, I do not take sleep talking seriously no matter what people say it doesn't necessarily reflect truth. So it's not like you're being more truthful when you're sleep talking. You just saved a number of relationships. I'm not directing this at anyone in particular, but I guarantee you just did noted. So as people start to approach morning or the time when they normally would wake up, I've heard that it's important to, if possible, complete one of these 90-minute cycles prior to waking up. That is, if you set your alarm for halfway through one of these 90-minute cycles prior to waking up. That is, if you set your alarm for halfway through one of these 90-minute cycles that come late in the night of sleep, that it can
Starting point is 00:34:31 lead to rather groggy patterns of waking. So, I'll just ask you directly, do you use an alarm clock? I do not. Thankfully, I'm in a line of work that doesn't require me normally to do anything at any particular time. I do it when I do it. Unless I have to catch a plane and then I always set my alarm just in case. Well as a fellow academic, I can tell you there are plenty of punishing features about being an academic scientist that offset the fact that you don't have to use an alarm clock but it is nice that you can often set your own schedule. So would you recommend that if possible that people not use an alarm clock?
Starting point is 00:35:06 Yeah, absolutely. If you can just listen to your body and wake up when you need to wake up, that would be great. But one of the reasons why we have such a groggyness, it's called sleep inertia when we wake up out of the wrong state, which is deep, slow wave sleep, is because I liken it to like a washing machine cycle. This 90-minute cycle is like a washing machine cycle and the first part is to add water, right?
Starting point is 00:35:34 Then your clothes are soaking wet. You don't want to open the washing machine and try and function, put them on and wear them around while they're soaking wet and full of soap. So you have to wait until the cycle is through before you can, well actually, let's put it in the dryer too before you want to wear them. So yeah, you can function, it just takes a little while for those clothes that brain to dry out so you can actually function well, but it's better to wait through the whole cycle is complete. And so that's why you want to set that 90 minute alarm clock. And again, that's around 90 minutes, because the first stage of sleep, the first cycle of sleep is actually a little longer, more like 105
Starting point is 00:36:20 and 110 minutes, but then the second ones and third ones, they get sort of shorter and shorter as the night goes on. And in the last few cycles, you're just doing the N2 REM sleep cycle, which takes less time. And if you wake up out of REM sleep, there's usually no problem cognitively. You're good to go. Are you a fan of sleep trackers? Sure. Yeah.
Starting point is 00:36:43 Do you use one? I have one on. I don't take, I don't, I don't live my life by them because they are the best ones right now are about 70% effective at staging your sleep. So 70% is okay. It's okay, but take it with a grain of salt is what I'm saying.
Starting point is 00:37:03 Yeah, I've tried various ones and I compare the mattress-based one to actually wear it on my ankle instead of my wrist, but and I do find it informative, but a colleague of mine at Stanford, Ali Crumb, who works on mindset and belief effects, talked to me about a study they did where people often will bias their sense of daytime wakefulness based on their sleep score more than their subjective score. In other words, if they were told they got a poor night's sleep, even if they got a great night's sleep, and this was, of course, measured in the sleep labs, they were able to compare. People will report feeling more groggy, and the opposite is also true, that if it says 100% or
Starting point is 00:37:40 90% on your sleep score, then people will go, I feel great even though they might not have slept well. So this speaks to the, I don't want to say placebo effect, but the sort of belief effects that are woven in with a score. Yeah. So it seems to me that combining subjective and objective data is probably best. And I do believe that you should trust your own physiology and the way that your body is telling you to feel
Starting point is 00:38:03 because in fact, it used to be that people within Somnia were not, we're often not believed because you put them in a sleep lab and they look like they slept great and you wake them up in the morning and they say, oh, I didn't sleep very well at all. And that's because probably we just came out with a paper that shows that sub-cortical structures can be in a completely different sleep state than cortical structures, which is what we measure in the sleep lab, what the cortex is doing. So it might be that people who say, I did not sleep all night long, even though the cortex is saying, oh no, you had great sleep, was because they're monitoring their sub-cortical hypothalamus, hippampus thalamus. Other structures that the sleep lab just can't access unless you have depth
Starting point is 00:38:50 electrons, which nobody really wants. Right, because that requires holes in the skulls and wires. Yeah. Wow. So does that mean that the last 50 plus years of sleep science, it's potentially flawed in some way, because they're only recording from, I guess this would be the analogy would be, it's like recording from the surface of the ocean as opposed to the depth of the ocean. Right.
Starting point is 00:39:13 And trying to ascertain the life moving down deep in the depth of the ocean. Brace yourselves colleagues at Stanford Sleep Lab and elsewhere. But please just tell us because I think scientists want to know the truth. Yeah. I mean, it's not for nothing that you want to know what the cortex is doing. I mean, the cortex is really important for a lot of things, but it doesn't necessarily tell you what a lot of other really important parts of the brain are doing in terms of sleep. But there's hope, because in fact, it would be great. I think that's possible from the paper
Starting point is 00:39:46 if you look at it, it's in PNAS this year, that you could detect subtle changes in the cortical EEG that might be able to tell you what the sort of sub-cortical structures are doing, things like the absolute power in that sleep spindle band, that sigma band would change if the hippocampus is in REM sleep and the cortex is in that sleep spindle state and vice versa. So there is some hope that we can gain from people with depth electrodes or animals with depth electrodes that we could backwards machine learn what the cortex might be able to tell us about sub-cortical structures from the cortical EEG. Interesting. This is going to be a stimulus for development of new technology, which is always going to assist in scientific discovery. There's one more thing I wanted to ask about the architecture of the night sleep in terms of early part of the night. Earlier you mentioned the wash out of debris and the so-called glimphatic system. I think is what you're referring to. Could you tell us a little bit more about the wash out that occurs in the brain during sleep.
Starting point is 00:40:59 What that is and what roles it's thought to serve and perhaps if there are any ways to ensure that it happens or to ensure that it doesn't happen and obviously we want this to happen. Yeah. Yeah. All right. Great question. We talked about the circadian clock and how certain things happen at certain times. Well, one of the things that happens when we're awake and talking to each other is that there's a lot of plasticity. There's something that I'm learning from you today and you're learning from me. And that changes our synapses and it changes the way our proteins are going to be folded and changed during sleep. It unfolds.
Starting point is 00:41:39 This process actually uses a lot of ATP, the power structure, the fuel of the brain, and it unfolds also proteins while we're doing this while we're using them. And so during that first part of the night, when we first fall asleep in the first 20 minutes or so, we're building that adenosine back into ATP, and that's probably by power naps, they're called power naps, because we're actually rebuilding the power. And then we're also cleaning out through the deep slow waves of slow waves sleep, cleaning out all those misfolded proteins and folded proteins and other things that get broken down and need to be rebuilt when we're asleep because of its use during
Starting point is 00:42:28 wakefulness. So I liken that to having a big party during wakefulness and you need all those party goers to leave in order to do the cleanup. And so what I think the mechanism is, and this is still something to be tested, is actually slow waves themselves, which is bad news for us as we get older and those slow waves get smaller and slow as sleep goes away. So, what happens when a neuron is firing is that it expands. The membrane expands a little bit, it becomes more translucent, that's how we know, one of the rays we know that neurons expand when they fire.
Starting point is 00:43:04 And so every action potential, the membrane expands a little bit as sodium brings water into their cell. And then when they're silent, they contract. And so during slow waves, the cool thing is that the reason why you can measure them is that all the neurons at the same time, not all of them, but a good portion of them, are firing at the same time and silent at the same time. And so you think about that as contracting and expanding all at the same time. It's kind of like a bilge pump of the brain. So that can pump out. Glee are also really important for this in terms of cleaning up debris and transferring it to where it needs to go. So I think of it
Starting point is 00:43:44 actually as a bilge pump cleaning out our brain. Interesting. I've heard about the glymphatic system and the lymphatic washout. I've never thought about the mechanical aspects of it before. I always thought that for some reason that now it's obvious to me that there had to be something mechanical, but only now that you've educated me about this. I thought that for some reason the cerebral spinal fluid just starts washing through, but here you're talking about literally an expansion and a contraction of the neurons in unison and pushing the fluid
Starting point is 00:44:14 through cleaning out any misfolded proteins or debris that might occur on the basis of these metabolic pathways. And the consequence of that is to what? To leave the brain in a state of more pristine action for the next day? Is that right? Yeah, you think of it again like a party. And if you don't clean up after that party, you try and hold another one the next day.
Starting point is 00:44:39 It's gonna get more clogged. You know, it's people have a harder time moving around and enjoying themselves. If that builds up day after day, it's going to be cognition. That would be the party, goers, moving around becomes hard. And so this build pump that you describe is associated with the big slow waves of slow wave sleep. So this is going to occur more or less in the first third of the night. Is that right? That's right. And are there things that inhibit
Starting point is 00:45:12 this process? And are there things that facilitate this process? Yeah. So well, one thing to inhibit is not to get it. But right and here too sorry to interrupt but and is this similar to the case with growth hormone where if you go to sleep later than you would normally you miss the wash out. It's not you don't delay it. You miss you miss the wash out. That's right. That's right. So if you go to sleep at one or two in the morning your sleep is still going to be dominated by N2 and REM sleep not by slow wave sleep. So you need to REM sleep, not by slow-wave sleep. So, you need to get that first bit of sleep.
Starting point is 00:45:49 Would a caveat be if somebody normally goes to sleep at 1 or 2 a.m. and wakes up at 10 a.m. If that's their normal sleep cycle. That should be okay. It should be okay. You would probably want to do... Somebody would want to do a sleep study with people who do that normally and see if also the melatonin release is later and the corticosterone rise that happens normally in the morning also happens later. So if everything shifted, good.
Starting point is 00:46:19 Okay. Yeah. There are a few studies I've come across that really do argue for the fact that waking up certain sun rise, that doesn't mean at sunrise, but within an hour or two, maybe three hours of sunrise and going to sleep within four hours after sunset or so, is actually better for the health of all human beings than is being a night owl.
Starting point is 00:46:44 And the night owl, there's almost like a night owl posse out there, especially on social media. They get very upset when you say that you should see morning sunlight that after 10 a.m., you kind of miss the boat. And they get very upset because I think there are about 20 or 30% of people, perhaps, who who really feel like they function better, staying up late and waking up late, and they function much less well waking up early and going to bed early. But the data on health metrics suggests that sorry night owls,
Starting point is 00:47:13 that they are wrong. Yeah, sorry me, because I'm a night owl. Oh boy, okay, well then I'm apologize and directly. And here, I'm not a really early morning person. I'm kind of more typical. If I wake up naturally around 6.30, somewhere between 6.30 and 7.30 a.m., go to sleep somewhere between 10 and 11 p.m.,
Starting point is 00:47:31 these are averages. But I do notice that when I force myself to get up a little earlier and go to sleep a little earlier, that my mood and alertness and just overall productivity is much higher. And there could be other variables there too. You're absolutely right. I'm a night owl.
Starting point is 00:47:48 I love staying up late at night doing, you know, writing grants, writing papers, watching movies, whatever it is. I love it. But I like you and like every human being on earth that do better if I go to bed earlier and wake up earlier. So one good thing for a night out is to have a child because they will wake you up. Their circadian rhythms are so strong. They will wake up.
Starting point is 00:48:14 And even if you deprived them of sleep in the first half of the night, they will still wake up like clockwork because their circadian rhythms are so strong at 6am. And so what you haven't done anything good for your kid, you haven't moved their cycle to later and be more in line with yours. In fact, you've just sleep deprived them and made them misalindo and made them cranky the next day and made your life more miserable.
Starting point is 00:48:34 So go to bed soon after your kids go to bed and wake up with them, that's the way to do it. Great, the child alarm clock. Another reason I have children. I got a dog, a puppy, and then that became a dog specifically, well, for many reasons. But one reason was I wanted to be one of those early morning, you know, 5.30 a.m. every morning, but I ended up getting a bulldog that would literally sleep 16 hours if he could. A nuclear bomb could go off and he wouldn't wake up.
Starting point is 00:49:02 But what I started to learn was that bulldogs actually have sleep apnea. As far as I know, they're the only species that has a genetically, they're essentially an in-bred sleep defect. And so I actually don't encourage people to get bulldogs because it's kind of a cruel breed. They suffer a lot in that body that they're born into.
Starting point is 00:49:21 Anyway, a dog can accomplish some of this, but get the breed of dog that is going to wake up early. So in other words, don't get a bulldog or a master. Well, interestingly, all predatory animals like dogs and cats and lions and us, well, more dogs, cats and lions, then us will can sleep, you know, 16 hours a day. Ferrets are predatory. They have to prefer it. Yeah.
Starting point is 00:49:48 I still, and sadly, I also used to work on ferrets, publishing a number of papers, delightful animals. Yeah. And great because you can study development. It's really cool because they're born very altrucial like we are with brains that are not very well developed. And so you can see what happens through development and how important these different phases of development really are.
Starting point is 00:50:10 But yes, maybe we're not as predator as much predators as we think because in fact, we are sleep is somewhere between the prey and the predators in terms of the amount of sleep that we usually need a night. But those predators can sleep 16 hours, napping all day long, and they're more crepuscular, perhaps, like their prey are more active. So dawn and dusk active. Yeah, dawn and dusk active.
Starting point is 00:50:38 Yeah. But anyway, yes, children and dogs, actually, if there was a poll done by the National Sleep Foundation, to see what the number one thing is that wakes people up at night and number two is going to the bathroom. Number three is children because, you know, when you're children are young, but that only lasts a few years, that they'll wake you up when they're babies. But the number one thing is pets and pets needing to go out or cats wanting to curl up with you or whatever it is Pets needs to wake you up more in the middle of the night than anything else another reason to not get a nocturnal pet
Starting point is 00:51:14 People who get hamsters pretty quickly realize that they are nocturnal They want to run on their wheel and around yeah, you got to put them in the living room away from where you sleep I vote fish tank folks Fresh water fish tank. They're all sorts of reasons sleep. I vote fish tank folks. Fresh water fish tank. There are all sorts of reasons to not get us all over. I think fresh water fish tank or a child. I appreciate that vote. And I appreciate you mentioning ferrets.
Starting point is 00:51:34 And by the way, folks, they are carnivores. They are not rodents. And they have very elaborate brain structures. They're very smart. Yeah. In the same family as the honey badgers and the other muskullets. Anyway, I shouldn't geek out too much on the muskullets or else I'll take the remainder of all our time. I'd love for you to tell us about REM sleep and the sleep
Starting point is 00:51:57 later in the night as it relates to dreams and emotionality. And this is probably the appropriate time for you to introduce us to this incredible structure in the brain Which is the locus sorulius a difficult Structure to spell but a beautiful a beautifully named structure I find locus released to be just fascinating and I know I you know a small fraction of what it does And I'm hoping you're gonna educate me and our audience about more about what it does, and hopefully tell us a little bit about its relationship to epinephrine A.K.A. adrenaline.
Starting point is 00:52:31 Yeah, I'm so glad you brought this up because I can totally geek out on the locus surrealist. Please do. Locus meaning spot or place and surrealist meaning blues, so you could just call it the blue spot. That's the easiest every animal with a brain has a blue spot. And yeah, and I mean every other animal with a brain because, of course, there are animals with nervous systems that are not
Starting point is 00:52:55 centralized like jellyfish. But anyway, we're digressing there. So the locusts to release is filled with neurons that have in them norupinephrine, which is the brain's version of epinephrine or adrenaline, it's also called noradrenaline. And what it does is it just like adrenaline in the rest of our bodies, it helps primus to respond to our environment. So when locusturalists neurons fire and fire in a burst,
Starting point is 00:53:23 we can switch our attention and fire and fire in a burst, we can switch our attention. They will fire in a burst if, for example, a loud noise happens in the middle of your concentrating on something. So it helps, it fires and it helps you switch your attention to that thing and then learn quickly from it. So, it's really important in a stress response. It helps us do a quick one trial learning. And then, the tonic activity during the day when you're just doing normal,
Starting point is 00:53:49 going about your normal concentration, kind of activities is really good for sustained attention. It works with the colonergic system of our basal forebrain, which is really important for learning and memory also to help us learn about things and put things together. But just tonic levels are signature of wakefulness and alertness. So too much is panic with the locus-realis activity.
Starting point is 00:54:18 A burst is switching attention, and then tonic levels are sustained constant attention. And then when we go to sleep, the locust surrealist slows and goes from about on average two hurts to about one hurts, one cycle per second, tonically, and then when we go into REM sleep, it's the only time when it shuts off completely. And it appears that that complete silence is really, really important for a number of things. And the main thing that I think it's important for is the ability to erase and break down synapses that are no longer working for us.
Starting point is 00:54:57 So they encode things that are false now, or they are encoding things that we learned in the novelty encoding pathway of our brain that have now been consolidated to other pathways. And so we need to now erase them from the novelty encoding pathway. And that is really, really important for being able to continue to learn things all of our lives. So like erasing that RAM or that, you know, what do you call those
Starting point is 00:55:25 disks that you stick into computers that? Hard drive. I know. Thumb drives. Yeah, you're racing your thumb drives. That thumb drive is what you carry around all day long. And then during sleep, you write that thumb drive to the cortex, to the long term memory structures. And you need to refresh that thumb drive. And that's what happens during REM sleep when the locus realises off because whenever it's on and neurodrenel is there, it helps us to put things together. It helps us to learn and strengthen synapses, but it does not allow us to actually weaken synapses that are also
Starting point is 00:55:57 a really important part, a part part of lifelong learning. Yeah, there's so much more I could say about that. Yeah, Locusturulius sounds fascinating. So it's connected to the basal four brain called energy system. The neurons in Locusturulius, if I'm not mistaken, release more epinephrine, perhaps epinephrine as well. Well, no, the brain's version of epinephrine is norepinephrine. The other thing it also, the precursor to norepinephrine is dopamine. And so the source of dopamine in the hippocampus seems to be the locuserles, and it's still a mystery as under what conditions the locuserles also releases dopamine, but it's really important
Starting point is 00:56:41 when we're learning something new to also release dopamine or to at least activate the dopaminergic receptors in our hippocampus. So, yeah, so dopamine, norepinephrine, and then there's also gallinine, which is important for releasing when we're stressed, and it helps also without rapid learning. It works in concert with Norapineffren and in doing what it needs to do to strengthen synapses so that we learn really quickly. I love that there are multiple molecules involved because that signals us to a principle, which is that even if people can't remember all the names,
Starting point is 00:57:21 that rarely in biology is something handled by just one molecular pathway. That redundancy is the rule because signaling attention to specific events is so important. So that I'm going to use that as a just-so story. I always say, you know, I wasn't consulted at the design phase, but it makes sense to me as to why redundancy would exist in the system. Absolutely. And in fact, when we form hypotheses about the brain, we're always wrong. And the reason why we're always wrong
Starting point is 00:57:49 is because it's more complicated than we'd like to think. And because in our brains, when we're forming hypotheses, we fail to account for all of the factors that are involved, the galea, the neuropeptides, the neurotransmitters, the physical structure of synapses. And so when I was going through grad school 35 years ago, the dogmo was that every neuron contains one neurotransmitter, and releases one neurotransmitter,
Starting point is 00:58:15 and you had excitatory neurotransmitters, and inhibitory neurotransmitters, and neuromodulatory neurotransmitters. But that's as complicated as God. And then we started talking about neuropeptides and people said, oh, no, please don't, don't complicate. And then we started talking about how neurons contain both neuropeptides and neurotransmitters and maybe more than one
Starting point is 00:58:33 neurotransmitter. Maybe even hormones too. And hormones and oh, Lord, it's just so complicated. But I mean, God admit, that's why it works, right? And every time the brain teaches us something new about itself that we didn't hypothesize, we say, oh, of course, that wouldn't work if the way I hypothesized it with it, you know, we actually need redundancy. We need all these systems to work together. Yeah, it's daunting sometimes, but it also ensures many, many careers in science and
Starting point is 00:59:00 neuroscience in particular. So, So, note that aspiring scientists, there's plenty of room for discovery. Do you want me to talk about Norepinephrine? Yeah, so what I've... Yes, well, what I'd love for you to tell us about is, you know, what role this lack of Norepinephrine release during rapid eye movement sleep is thought to achieve. And maybe you could also review some of your work
Starting point is 00:59:27 describing conditions under which Nora Puneferin invades. Yeah, invades sleep. Rapid eye movement sleep and other patterns of sleep and how that can be detrimental. Yeah, so a lot of this is hypothetical that based on a lot of good evidence that we're sowing together into a schema
Starting point is 00:59:42 from which these hypotheses come. So a model schema from which these hypotheses come, so a model schema from which the hypotheses come. But one thing that happens to people with post-traumatic stress disorder is that there is a lot of evidence that the locusarilis doesn't stop firing in REM sleep. So whereas there are levels of norepinephrine might be similar to people without PTSD There are levels of norepinephrine might be similar to people without PTSD during the day and even during the first part of the night. During the wee hours of the morning and when you measure norepinephrine levels from metabolites in the blood or the cerebral spinal fluid, you see that people with PTSD, it's during
Starting point is 01:00:20 the wee hours of the morning when you have the most REM sleep that they have. There are norepinephrine levels differentiate most from those that don't have PTSD. And so that's evidence that the locusturalist is not shutting down during REM sleep like it should. Other evidence is heart rate variability. When our locusturalist is firing, our heart rates are generally a little higher and they don't vary as much as they do when the locusarillus is not firing. So during slow-wave sleep normally we have this big
Starting point is 01:00:49 juicy variability in heart rate with every breath in and breath out because our neurodinergic levels, our norepinephrine levels are lower during REM sleep that goes away entirely and our heart rate is dominated by parasympathetic rather than sympathetic activity and also what our brain is driving, you know, what our dreaming about. For example, if we're dreaming, we're running our heart rates will go up. But norapeneference levels still should be low or off.
Starting point is 01:01:19 So people with PTSD that Nora Dinergic's, we're studying these in rats too, is it true that our locustralis doesn't shut off when we have post-traumatic stress disorder and the preliminary evidence is, yes, it's true that it doesn't shut off. So what that would do is, Nora Peneffrin would act at synapses
Starting point is 01:01:40 to prevent that weakening that you really need, for example, of novelty and coating structures. And it keeps memories in that novelty and coating structure. Even once, it's consolidated to the rest of the brain. So in the hippocampus, which is important for remembering things throughout our lives, and that's that thumb drive, we need it to be erased so that we can learn new things. Once it's been consolidated to the hard drive of our cortex. And so if we're not able to do that, we fill up that RAM really quickly,
Starting point is 01:02:15 or that thumb drive really quickly, and we're not able to learn new things. So for example, after trauma, I talked to all the local surrealists responding in stressful situations. That's great. It's very adaptive. you're not able to learn new things. So for example, after a trauma, I talked to all the local surrealists responding in stressful situations, that's great, it's very adaptive, but then you need it to stop. Once you've learned what you need to learn from it and you wanna go to sleep, you need the local surrealist to calm down
Starting point is 01:02:36 and during REM sleep you want it to stop because then when you consolidate that traumatic memory to the cortex, you need to erase it from the novelty and coding structures, for example, in the hippocampus. So that then, when you're in the context of safety, you can learn those new things, those new contexts, and stop responding to those same stimuli
Starting point is 01:02:57 as though you're in that original situation. So if you're not able to erase that thumb drive, you will always feel like that trauma happened that same day, like earlier that same day, and respond as you would to an early, a recent trauma, which is with never able to downscale that novelty in coding structure and you purge it from that traumatic memory, it will stay fresh and new and then become maladactive. What approaches are you aware of that can turn down the output of locus rulius during these phases of sleep. And for that matter, what
Starting point is 01:03:46 things can cause ramping up of locus rulius during this phase of sleep. We've had a couple podcast episodes, solo episodes, and with guests talking about trauma, we had Dr. Paul Conti, who's a Stanford trained, Harvard trained psychiatrist, who talked a lot about trauma wrote an excellent book on trauma. And certainly sleep was emphasized as a key thing like get enough sleep But here you're saying even if somebody with trauma gets enough sleep if locus roulius is hyperactive during sleep Those traumas are gonna persist and most of the trauma treatments that I'm aware of are everything ranging from cognitive behavioral therapy talk therapy drug therapy EMDR hypnosis Nowadays, there's a lot of interest and attention on clinical studies on exploring psychedelics,
Starting point is 01:04:31 hydrocybid and MDMA. So it's a vast landscape, none of which as far as I know is really focused on sleep specifically. No, they're not. And they should be because actually psychedelics is a sleep-like state in it's a REM sleep-like state, although of course there are some major differences. So yeah, so much to talk about here. So antidepressants are often neurodinergic or serotonergic reuptake inhibitors. So they leave neuropinephrine, actually, out there in the synapses, and what that does is it inhibits REM sleep.
Starting point is 01:05:08 And if you're able to get REM sleep, it would probably be REM sleep with some neurodinergic activity. So actually, I think, anyway, I'm not a physician that antidepressants are counterinticated. You don't want to take them if you've experienced a trauma and you're experiencing PTSD. Because if anything anything it's going to make it worse or at least prevent the type of adaptive REM sleep that you really need in order to resolve those emotions and move on. Is that statement specific to antidepressants that tickled the noradrogenergic pathway? So the one that comes to mind is group peri-per, I can never pronounce it, group of pyron, which is,
Starting point is 01:05:50 I think, brand name is well-butron. It's a dopamine, anurgic, and noradrinurgic agonist. That's the net effect, as opposed to the prozaxyl-off variety, which are SSRIs. Yes, yes, but SSRIs themselves also are problematic, because we didn't talk about it yet, but the Dorsal Raffae nucleus, which produces serotonin, which is the specific serotonin, specific serotonin reuptake inhibitors block from being reuptaken, leaves too much serotonin out there. And what serotonin also is another neurotransmitter that's down-regulated during REM sleep, that's specifically off during REM sleep. And what Saratonein does is it waits all of our cognition to being able to recognize novelty again.
Starting point is 01:06:33 So it sort of waits our brain away from a sense of familiarity and toward novelty. And it might be one reason why it's an effective magic-depressant because it makes the world feel fresh and new again, right? But you, when you have too much, you're holding a novel, traumatic memory in your novelty and coding structure too strongly already, you don't want to again wait things toward novelty. You need that absence of serotonin also to help you get that sense of familiarity and to
Starting point is 01:07:12 start erasing the novelty and coding structures. So you need both to be absent. It's really interesting. We hear a lot about serotonin and it's not often discussed in terms of its features related to novelty enough, I think. And what you just described, amcuse me to something that Dr. Paul Conti and others have said in terms of trauma. And here I'm paraphrasing some of my apologies to them for not getting this exactly right. That an effective treatment for trauma does not erase the traumatic memory, but it causes a transition of what once was disturbing
Starting point is 01:07:46 and invasive and maladaptive to eventually just become kind of a boring old story that has kind of a fuzzy texture to it as opposed to this kind of sharp, high friction texture that invades our thinking and obviously our sleeping states as well. So again, and I appreciate the disclaimer, the caveats around, you know, not being a clinician et cetera, but I do think that there's a lot of interest now in whether or not antidepressants
Starting point is 01:08:16 are effective for trauma or not. I think these aspects of neuromodulation as they relate to, let's call it erasing traumas or changing the emotional load of traumas during sleep is something important to take note. We also have a lot of clinicians that listen to this podcast, so they should also take note. Yeah. Please.
Starting point is 01:08:34 So if I want to reduce the amount of Norepinephrine released from Locust Ceruleus during rap and eye movement sleep to eliminate the troubling or maybe even traumatic memories and allow late stages of sleep each night to have their maximum positive effect. Is there anything that I can do besides avoiding traumas, avoiding serotonergic or noraginurgic compounds? Well, I would also avoid anything just prior to going to sleep that might excite those systems. So a lot of novelty, a lot of exciting stress inducing video games, try and enter sleep
Starting point is 01:09:19 with as much calm as you can. So maybe deep breathing exercises. That's a beautiful way to calm your sympathetic fight or flight system is deep breathing. And we haven't been able to test this with rats because we can't ask them to do a deep breathing exercise. There might be a way we can do that, but I haven't found out or figured it out yet. But if there's a way you can make your sympathetic system,
Starting point is 01:09:47 nervous system calm down before you go to sleep, might free, free you meditation or deep breathing exercises, might be for some warm bath or comforting book, nothing too exciting, also nothing too boring perhaps. Just something right in the middle which makes you feel happy and calm, is what you should do. And if you instead go to sleep while you're anxious or you're hyped up, then your sleep could become maladaptive. Another thing that happens in rats that we have yet to know
Starting point is 01:10:21 if it happens in women, is that female rats have three phases of their ester cycle that their locus-realis doesn't seem to calm down during REM sleep as much. And we don't know why, but during the high estrogen phases of their ester cycle, the locus-realis shuts down just like it does in male rats. But in the other three phases, it doesn't.
Starting point is 01:10:44 So one thing that might work, and in fact, there are a few studies that show it, it could work really well, is giving women after a trauma event, something that contains estrogen, because estrogen somehow is protective against PTSD, and they know that through retrospective studies where they gave women an emergency room,
Starting point is 01:11:09 either a pill with estrogen or without, and those that had pill with estrogen in it were much less likely to get PTSD from that trauma as measured a year later than those that had the pill without. So there's really good studies by Bronwyn Graham. She's out of Australia to really hone in on how much estrogen do you need and also testosterone just so you know gets converted to estrogen in the brain so testosterone also can be protective because it gets converted to estrogen. But there's something
Starting point is 01:11:40 about estrogen that's really helpful and protective about that from the high-local surrealist firing. And this is, again, preliminary data that we don't have full, we don't have all the answers yet and we are looking into it actively right now, but it's really important. The other thing about women is that we are two to four times more susceptible to anxiety-related mental health disorders, including post-traumatic stress disorder. So if we could figure out what's happening to the locusturus during sleep in women, and then figure out a way to normalize that. So the locusturus is silent, and it needs to be silent. I think
Starting point is 01:12:19 we could go a long way in helping women be more resilient to stress-related disorders. What are some other sex differences as they relate to sleep? Yeah, that's a really good question. There have been very few studies, unfortunately, of women in sleep, women in an ester cycle or menstrual cycle in sleep. women and ester cycle or menstrual cycle in sleep. But what we have found, which actually largely replicated the study in 1960, is that women or females, rather at high estrogen, high hormonal phases of their ester cycle or menstrual cycle
Starting point is 01:13:01 sleep a lot less, but that sleep is more efficient. So that sleep is more dense in those sleep spindles, which I haven't gone into what they might do, except this connection between the hippocampus and cortex, but those sleep spindles are more dense and more coherent across the brain areas. The theta cycle, which is 5 to 10 hertz in the hippocampus, important for one year learning and also important during REM sleep is also bigger in GCR during the high hormonal phases. So even though there's less sleep,
Starting point is 01:13:32 it's more efficient and better. But so all of that efficiency seems to be reduced in those other hormonal phases. So even though you might sleep a little more, you might need more sleep, in fact, in order to accomplish the same thing that you can get with that short, very efficient sleep of high hormonal phases.
Starting point is 01:13:58 Very interesting. I think there is a growing trend at least among NIH-funded grants to require that, as they refer to it, in the grants, biological sex as a variable. And then here we're talking about not him about sex, the variable, though I'm sure there's studies about that too, but biological sex is a variable because there is a dearth of studies exploring sex differences in most everything. There's all sorts of reasons for that, but more importantly, fortunately, the trend is shifting. And even when you study males versus females, a lot of people just
Starting point is 01:14:36 include females in their studies, but then don't track the ester cycle, or menstrual cycle, and hormones have huge effects on our behavior. I mean, just think of it when you say sex. You know, before hormones come in, we're not interested in it. And suddenly, you know, that's kind of a main driver of behaviors. Hormones can definitely change who we are and what we do.
Starting point is 01:14:59 So we should be studying hormones, not just sex. I always say that puberty is perhaps the most massive transformation and rate of aging that any of us go through in a short amount of time. And individual, their cognition changes, their worldview changes, and that's largely hormonal driven and obviously neural architecture has changed too.
Starting point is 01:15:17 I'm very happy that you mentioned, I'm trying to get into calmer states prior to sleep, and some ways to do that. I'm a big fan and I've talked a lot before in this podcast about things like yoga nidra, which is a non-movement-based practice. Sometimes called non-sleep deep rest, where people actually take some time each day to practice how to go into a more parasympathetic, a.k.a. relaxed state deliberately. Because it's a bit of a skill.
Starting point is 01:15:41 Yeah, it's, and there's some good data really, mostly out of a laboratory in Scandinavia showing huge increases in nitro stride, all dopamine, when people go, basically engage in a practice of deliberate non-movement, and that the brain actually enters states of a very shallow sleep. So sort of nap-ish, but the idea is to actually stay awake, but motionless, and it does seem to restore a certain number of features of neurochemistry, but perhaps more importantly, it teaches people to relax, which is something that most people are not very good at.
Starting point is 01:16:17 But in any event, and people who listen to this podcast have heard me say this over and over again, so I sound like a broken record, but this practice as a zero cost practice, that doesn't require any pharmacology, does seem to really enhance people's ability to fall asleep more quickly and to fall back asleep if they wake up in the middle of the night. So in any event, another plug for NSDR, Yoganidra.
Starting point is 01:16:38 Well, I just also want to add to that. That's one of the reasons why insomnia is so insidious is because when people feel like they haven't gotten enough sleep and they aren't getting enough sleep and become anxious about getting enough sleep and then you're anxious before going to sleep like I'm not going to fall asleep is going to be about it and then practice this relaxation to say, hey, it's all okay, it's going to be all right. And then concentrate on things that relax you, whether it's concentrating or not, concentrating, whatever it is. You mentioned yoga, Nidra, and that reminded me of transcendental meditation, which is something that also hasn't been studied well, are largely because we can't ask non-human animals to do it.
Starting point is 01:17:31 And so we don't know what's happening with our neurochemistry and our brain activity in a deep and meaningful way. But one thing that has been shown in those that can do it really well is that that theta activity that I said happens when you're learning something or when you're in REM sleep, it's well established and increases during the trans-ondental meditations. So it might be that some states of meditation could in some ways replace or mimic some functions of,
Starting point is 01:18:07 for example, REM sleep. But again, we don't know if all the neurochemistry is right to do, for example, the thing that I was talking about, which is erasing the novelty and coding structures of the brain. That needs an absence of neuroopinephanen and serotonin, which we don't know that goes away with transcendental meditation. We just don't know if that goes away with transcendental meditation. We just don't know. The answer to that, yeah. Yeah, the studies on yoga knee-dra and sleep replacement are kind of interesting.
Starting point is 01:18:32 It does seem to be the case that nothing can really replace sleep except sleep, but that if one is sleep deprived or is having trouble falling back asleep, that these things like, and I hear it's, I acknowledge this is essentially like Yogan-Ejra, but we now call it non-sleep deep breast or NSDR because oftentimes for names like Yogan-Ejra act as a kind of a barrier for what would otherwise be people willing to try a practice. It sounds mystical. It's an addiction in yoga.
Starting point is 01:18:59 It sounds like flying carpets and you know, it sounds like you have to go to Esselam by the way, Esselam is a beautiful place, but it sounds like you have to go there or live in the west coast to believe in this stuff, but it's simply not the case. These are practices that are really just self-directed relaxation as a practice that allows people to get better and better at directing their brain states towards more relaxation. And most people have an asymmetry, Like for instance, most people can force themselves to stay up later, but they have a hard time going to sleep earlier.
Starting point is 01:19:29 And that just speaks to the asymmetry that's probably adaptive and survival base that we can ramp ourselves up far more easily than we can tend to calm ourselves down. Yeah, and actually, to appeal to other Christians like me, prayer can be a wonderful way to calm yourself down because through prayer, you're giving your cares to God and saying,
Starting point is 01:19:52 and then you are relaxed, more relaxed. And I just wanna say that because the same reason that yoga might put some people off, it might put some people off to talk about prayer, but it's the same process of being able to relax. And yeah. And get outside our own experience a little bit.
Starting point is 01:20:14 Get outside our experience. Back out, get a world view that might actually also help us to relax. Well, you might be surprised at how many clinicians and scientists who've come on this podcast have mentioned things like prayer from various perspectives, Christianity, Judaism, Muslim traditions, and others, that as a parallel to all of these things, and I think what it speaks to is the fact that ultimately the biological architectures that we're all contending with are going to be identical, right? And so different ways to tap into them and ones that are congruent with people's beliefs,
Starting point is 01:20:47 I think, are great. Yeah. Yeah, because anything non-congruing with your beliefs is also stressful. Right. And feels forced. And that's why this idea of calling it non-sleep deep rest in addition to Yoganidra was not to detract from the naming or the history around Yoganidra. But I was finding that it was a barrier, you know, likewise, eoghanidra tends to include things
Starting point is 01:21:07 like intentions, whereas NSTR scripts, and by the way, we will provide links to some NSTR and eoghanidra scripts, but NSTR has no intentions. It's simply a body-scan-deep relaxation base, so it's sort of the scientific version of all of this stuff, and actually we study it in the laboratory, and some of the brain states that people go into, but that's a discussion for another time.
Starting point is 01:21:26 Well, another thing, this is hard not. My mother used to tell me when I said, what do we could play? I can't go to sleep, she'd say, well, start with your toes and relax. So you would clench your muscles around your toes and you relax them and do that all the way from your toes all the way to your head.
Starting point is 01:21:42 And I don't know where she got this. It might have been her own common sense. So she might have gotten it from this NPR shows called The Mind Can Keep You Well. She still listen to. But that's another intentional relaxation that focuses on the body rather than on your own mental processes.
Starting point is 01:22:00 But I do a little bit of work with the military. And there's a method within certain communities of special operations in the US military where if they can't sleep or they're having challenges sleeping, they will deliberately try and relax their facial muscles in particular like sort of drape the facial muscles and use long or exhale emphasized breathing, does seem to increase the probability of transitioning back into sleep. And those are hallmarks of Yoganidra, non-sleep deep rest, body scans. And so I think all of these things converge on a common theme, you know, as neurobiologist, we can say, all of the things that we are describing,
Starting point is 01:22:40 certainly move the needle away from locus to relius activation. And we haven't done the experiment to really look at that. But it seems all these things are counter to noradrenaline release. Right. Another one is yawning.
Starting point is 01:22:52 Yawning in itself is with that kind of sort of tensing of all the muscles in your face and then relaxing them. So it might be why we yawning. We don't know why we yawning yet. But it might also have, it would be really great. Actually, animals yawn too, you know. Oh, my bulldog was a perpetual, if he wasn't sleeping, he was yawning.
Starting point is 01:23:11 And it would be interesting to see what yawning does to the locus roost. Does that also calm and switch locus roost activity? Because it's an interesting that facial nerve, like trigeminal nerve, you know, through the vagus connects indirectly to the locusarillus and has a powerful effect on that. Interesting. I think friend of ours and direct colleague, Vier's Jack Feldman was a guest on this podcast telling us about all the amazing structures he and others have discovered in respiration and breathing.
Starting point is 01:23:41 Sounds like we have a collaboration brewing that the three of us should definitely carry out. I'd love for you to share with us a little bit more about the spindles that have come up a few times. And I don't know if it's relevant to this, so if it's not, let's separate it out. But I'd love for you to tell us a little bit about the role of sleep in problem solving and creativity. And if spindles are involved, then I'll consider myself lucky for batching them in the same question, and if they're not involved, simply feel free to separate them out. I think they could be involved.
Starting point is 01:24:10 And the reason why I think they could be involved, because we now know a lot more about spindles. First of all, the first thing that we knew, first of all, we ignored them. Then we thought they had something to do with keeping us asleep, and that was their function is when an external stimulus came, they would keep us asleep because they would arise.
Starting point is 01:24:25 But now we know that the density of our sleep spindles, the number that we produce per minute is well correlated with our intelligence in the first place and that no matter what your intelligence is, and no matter what your sleep spindle density is, if you're learning something during the day and increase your sleep spindle density, it's really almost perfectly correlated with our ability to consolidate that information and incorporate it into the schema that we already have in our brain. So if you try and learn something new, even if your sleep spindle density at baseline is great, if you don't increase
Starting point is 01:24:58 your sleep spindles that night, you're not going to, you know, use sleep to really incorporate it. Interestingly, sleep spindles are poor in those with schizophrenia. It's one of them characteristic signatures of sleep. Is that sleep spindles are very few and far between, which might mean that, that people with schizophrenia might not be able to incorporate new information into already existing schema, and instead it sort of flaps in the breeze out there and can be accessed erroneously at times when you don't want it to be involved.
Starting point is 01:25:33 So I digress. So sleep spindles and creativity. So one of the things we now know through some great studies by Julie Seap and Anita Luthi is that sleep spindles are accompanied by an incredible plasticity out in the distal dendrites, the listening branches of our neurons, that listen to other cortical areas. So there are proximal dendrites in our neurons that listen to the external world
Starting point is 01:26:00 and are conducted through the phalamus. And then there are distal dendrites, which listen to an internal kind of conversation that's happening in our brains. It's kind of our internal state, really. And during sleep spindles, that's when those distal dendrites are able to best learn from other cortical areas and from the hippocampus,
Starting point is 01:26:22 it is during sleep spindles that the hippocampus and the cortex are best connected and when that class, incredible plasticity can happen. When I talk about schema, that's a cortical, cortical thing. That's when the image of Santa Claus in presents comes together. It's not through some external thing. Once we learn those things together, it's our cortex that encodes and brings those images back up together. And that's during sleep spindles, when that's happening, when there's big surges of calcium into those distilled dendrites and where plasticity happens in just two
Starting point is 01:26:58 amounts. During that sleep spindle stage of sleep, which is N2 stage, there's also another excitatory event that comes all the way from the brainstem and projects everywhere in our cortex, which is called PGO waves, which is P for pons, G for injeaniculate, nucleosophalamus, which is where they're first discovered, and O for occipital area, which is a visual area, which is again where they're first discovered. But in fact, it's now been shown that PGO waves, which we should generalize to P waves,
Starting point is 01:27:30 because they come from the ponds and go to the phalamis, and then the cortex happens all over the brains. And that is where glutamate, which is a major excitatory neurotransmitter involved in learning and plasticity, is being released in big amounts, also in those distal dendrites. So, P waves and spindles work together to cause plasticity and so are schema together, which could be the origins for Insighting creativity. Now, when PTO waves or P waves are first discovered, it was
Starting point is 01:28:00 thought to be random because this small area that generates p-waves all over the brain projects all over the thalamus and causes p-waves all over. And you don't measure p-waves all over the brain at the same time. In fact, it's just, it seems sporadic and random. So that's probably, and p-waves also happening even more during REM sleep, rapid eye movement sleep. So that's probably, that's why people think that REM dreams are so random is because these P waves are random and they could generate dreams because they're an internal source of excitation that kind of replaces the outside world during our dream state.
Starting point is 01:28:43 And so these P waves, if they are random, could function, could be the underlying reason why REM sleep dreams are random. And it might also be why creativity can happen there is because we're randomly activating, co-activating, different things in our brain that we can then sew together. But it might not be as random as we think. So that's a caveat there. I just learned a lot from you because I teach brainstem to medical students and I talk
Starting point is 01:29:13 about the ponds. And the ponds is like this dense collection of all these different nuclei involved in a bunch of different things. And it's close by a bunch of interesting things. And it's still kind of a mysterious brain area. But when I learned about PGO waves, I thought ponds, geniculate, occipital, because occipital was most commonly associated with visual cortex. I thought it was the origin of the visual component of dreams.
Starting point is 01:29:37 I'm very happy to learn that they should be called P waves because they include lots of different areas of the brain. And it makes really good sense to me why the Kind of pseudo-randomness of dreams, especially these late night and early morning Later in sleep, I should say and early morning dreams seem to be cobbled together from disparate experiences, you know, you walk through a door and suddenly it's a completely different context in landscape. Yes, it's beautiful. Yeah, I like this idea.
Starting point is 01:30:09 It makes intuitive sense, it makes biological sense. It also gives me something to talk about to the medical students next quarter when I talk about ponds. You want to talk about where in the ponds? It's right below the locusarulus. It's called the subsarulus. They're glutamatergic. It's also called SLD sub lateral dorsal nucleus.
Starting point is 01:30:27 Well, so note to any aspiring neurologist, there's a vast landscape of yet to be undiscovered structure and functions in the pond. You want to work on something that is sure to reveal something novel, work on the pond. Right. Because it's in every textbook, it's a clinically very important structure. Sadly, gliomas and other cancers of the brain can sometimes, can often surface in the ponds, but we still know very little about it. I read a paper this last year, and I think it was covered in a bit of popular press, that
Starting point is 01:31:01 during rapid eye movement sleep, people can solve problems or respond to external stimuli. Like, for instance, they would give them math problems. They'd whisper in their ear while they were in REM sleep. You know, what's two plus two? And people would say, even though they were paralyzed, apparently they could still move their mouth because they'd say, four, or something like that. Or they'd say, you know, what's your name and people could respond. And so that in REM sleep perhaps people some elements of cognition are still Active I'm glad you brought that up that what do you think and I don't know the authors of that study and
Starting point is 01:31:35 and Listen if ever I say something wrong It's great on this podcast because someone will tell us in the YouTube comments It's one of the great uses of YouTube comments, But I'd love to know your thoughts on that study. I mean, is that just kind of a, an odd feature that, or does this have meaning? Should we actually care about this result? There is no just about it.
Starting point is 01:31:55 It's really actually intriguing and interesting and might relate to this paper that I talked about where we said different areas of the brain can be in different states at the same time. So lucid dreaming is another thing we can't ask animals to do or can't ask them if they've done it. But we can certainly ask humans to do it and some people can do it really well. And it would be really interesting to see in those people who could lucid dream really well, whether they spend more or less time in this asymmetrical state where one area, the
Starting point is 01:32:26 brain is in one state and another area, the brain is in another. And it might be that those people can respond to questions during REM sleep. Best are those that have the most asymmetry or dissimilarity or dissociation between subcortical and cortical structures. Or it might be that they're the ones with the most symmetry. We don't know. I do worry a little bit about lucid dreaming, because people are, it's a fad,
Starting point is 01:32:50 people are really excited about it, and to be able to remember when streams is fun, often, unless they're nightmares. And, but it's really interesting, or to be able to direct when streams, if they are a nightmare, this is really wonderful power to have, to be able to redirect a nightmare
Starting point is 01:33:06 that has been repeated to something else and then kick yourself out of that repetitive nightmare. It's really nice. But I worry a little bit about because we know so little about what's actually going on in the brain. And if this lucid dreaming state is preventing us from, for example, from the locus surrealists from calming down or the serotonergic system from silencing like it should. And maybe what we're doing during this state is, yeah, we're activating the learning and memory structures, but in a way that's maladaptive in terms of the erasure
Starting point is 01:33:39 that we need to do. So maybe one of the reasons why most people don't remember, most of their dreams is for good reason. Your hippocampus is in a state where it's not writing new memories. In fact, it's writing out. It's the memories it learned during the day to the cortex and it's immune from incoming new information. So maybe lucid dreaming is bad because you're activating the hippocampus in a way that's writing new memories and it might be really maladaptive for things like, you know, PTSD. On the other hand, let me just argue myself right
Starting point is 01:34:12 out of this. When I used to have a repeated nightmare when I was a kid, my mother was so wise, would tell me, well, listen, just next time you're in that dream, you dream, say, hey, I'm in a dream and then change something about it. So she and I rehearsed what the horrible dream that it was. It was a big monster, you know, running after me and my legs were like mud and I couldn't run away and it was just terrifying. And that was a dream I would have, you know, time and time again. She said, okay, next time what are you going to do and that monster comes after you.
Starting point is 01:34:44 I'm going to run away. No, that's what you do every time. And it's always the same outcome you can't run. So let's do something different. Like, what could you do that's different? So I came up with, I could turn around and punch it. And then I was just, yeah, that's great. So the next time I had that dream, I did recognize.
Starting point is 01:35:01 This is that same old dream, which means that there's part of my brain that's conscious enough to know that this that I'm in a dreaming state and Then I didn't have the courage in my dream because I was still terrified to punch You know or touch the a monster in any way But I did have the courage to turn around and look at in the eye and say no That was enough. I said no
Starting point is 01:35:20 And that was enough to knock me out of that ret of that dream so that I never had it again. I never had that same dream again. And in fact, it gave me peace about dreaming because I knew that if ever there was a nightmare that was just too scary, I could probably do something to change it and knock myself out of it. So even though I don't recommend lucid dreaming on a normal day-to-day basis, if it's enough that can knock you out of a red, one thing that happens with people with PTSD is they have the same repeated horrible nightmare, which is often a reliving of the day's trauma
Starting point is 01:35:58 that they had. So maybe lucid dreaming can be used on occasion to be a powerful tool because there's so much plasticity that happens during REM sleep to knock you out of that ret of reliving that event and just change it. And you could probably practice that during wakefulness, rehearse the event that happened that was so traumatic, and then just introduce a new element, like, now I'm safe. Now, the sound that was associated with that really traumatic thing, I should now associate with something else, and next time I have that dream, I'm going to change
Starting point is 01:36:37 it, so that sound is now this new thing that should be associated with safety. And that might be enough, maybe, I hope, to knock you out of that repeated nightmare and maybe even start you on the path to recovery because if you can calm down about those nightmare states of sleep, then maybe your local surrealists, which is involved in stress, can also relax. And you can do the erasure parts that need to be done.
Starting point is 01:37:01 I love it. I seem to recall a paper and I'll have to find the reference and send it to you. We will also put in the show no captions. They described a protocol. Essentially matches this idea. I think what they had people do is either cue themselves to a particular smell or tone and wakefulness. Then to try and recall a recurring nightmare, then during the night sleep, they had the tone playing in the background, which would then cue them to the wakeful state. They're still asleep, mind you, but in the pseudo lucid or lucid state, and then try and change some variable as you're describing.
Starting point is 01:37:37 Some either look the predator in the eyes or do something different, and then in the waking state, take a little bit of time to try and script out a different narrative altogether. And it took several nights, as I recall, or more, but that they were able to escape this recurring nightmare. It was like a week or something. So you're familiar with the study. Yeah, it was a beautiful study.
Starting point is 01:37:57 I loved it. Yeah, we will put a reference to that. I need to revisit that study. It's pretty recent, but I need to dive into it again, because I think I didn't go as deep into it as I should have. No, no, but the one thing that you said many, right, things, but one of the things you said is that they were able to cue the dreamer when they knew it, and they were going to REM sleep,
Starting point is 01:38:19 and then they played the sound or had the odor. Now, when you're normally asleep alone in your bed, you're not going to be able to cue yourself. But it might be that rehearsal enough before you go to sleep is enough to help cue you to that repeated nightmare. Remembering what the nightmare is and then figuring out how to cue yourself to do something different. For years I had the same recurring nightmare over and over and over again and it was so
Starting point is 01:38:46 salient and so clear and I'm not going to share what it is because it's it's not that it's that disturbing it was just I think it was the emotional load of it and just how salient certain features were like one person in who's a real life person had a particular clothing on and it's like and that just served as this queue. And I don't know if I ever did any direct work to try and deal with it, but now it almost seems silly to describe it. Oh, yeah.
Starting point is 01:39:11 Well, dreams are usually silly to describe it. Yeah, it's pretty silly, but those are pretty violent dreams. Yeah, and your emotional system is so geared up during REM sleep, which is another thing we could talk about. Yeah, please. I would love to. Yeah, so Lucas Arulius is ideally suppressed, so we can't release nor epinephrine. We can't act out our dreams.
Starting point is 01:39:31 This during these very emotionally laid in thoughts and storylines during sleep. This is almost like, starts to sound like a little bit of a built-in while sleeping trauma therapy, because most trauma therapies involve trying to get people into states of, um, sound like a little bit of a built-in while sleeping trauma therapy. Because most trauma therapies involve trying to get people into states of counter to what most people think you actually want to get close to the trauma in terms of the narrative. That's right. So, try and suppress the emotional activity of it. Or, I guess that's the motivation for ketamine-based therapies for trauma.
Starting point is 01:40:01 Or I've also heard, and this is still perplexing to me, that other waking based trauma therapies involve taking people the other way, making it very cathartic, take them to the peak of the emotional response, but then allow that to finally cycle down into a more relaxed response. So please, if there's anything about locustsarulias and dreams that can help people basically extinguish traumas or traumatic features to real life events. We definitely want to know about them.
Starting point is 01:40:30 Yeah, well, I think one of the things that people might help after a trauma, like a school shooting or whatever, you know, car accident is to talk about it. And in fact, that ended up being counterproductive. And I think one of the reasons why it was counterproductive is because it didn't take them back down. It brought them up and continued to reactivate the emotions of it, but then didn't emphasize the safety of fact that it's over or help them work through
Starting point is 01:41:00 how they might avoid it again in the future to calm the sympathetic nervous system down again before they went to sleep. And none of these studies has sleep ever been considered. But to me, that's the key part. It's bringing down your sympathetic nervous system before you go to sleep so that your sleep can be adaptive, your locustarulus can shut off like it normally does or should do, and then able to erase the
Starting point is 01:41:26 novelty of it. The other thing that I just mentioned a minute ago was that the emotional system is highly activated in REM sleep, and that's definitely true. And that might seem counterproductive in terms of the nightmares and how to help REM sleep be a therapeutic thing rather than reinforcing the emotionality of the trauma. And I think the key to that again is the absence of a norepinephrine. So even though the emotional system is in high gear without npeneferin, you can actually divorce those highly activated emotions from the cognitive parts of the memory
Starting point is 01:42:14 that you have just written out in that end-to-stage of sleep when the sleep spindles are going. So you've just now consolidated the information that you'll need to survive and to make that adaptive. And now you need to divorce from that schema and from that semantic parts of memory, the emotional part. Because whenever you remember something, it's fine if you remember being emotional at the time.
Starting point is 01:42:42 But you don't want to bring back and so into that memory all of the same emotional systems. You don't want to bring back, you know, the heart rate changes and the sweating and all of that. You want to be able to remember all the parts of it and even remember that you were traumatized and that you did cry and that you did have, you know, your heart was racing. But when you're talking about it, years later, you don't want to have to relive all that. Otherwise, who would ever want to recall a dramatic
Starting point is 01:43:10 memory because you're basically putting yourself through the same trauma, which is what people with PTSD have. They don't want to recall this traumatic memory because it's reliving it like it's just happening again. So that's what we're thinking is that the emotional parts are not long, are not able to be divorced because the North-Brennerfin system is not downscaled during REM sleep. And so that REM sleep serves to instead reinforce and in fact amplify the emotions because your emotional system is up. Look, aialysis, hi. Re-sowing in every night the emotionality of those memories in with the memory itself. Told us a lot about locustorialias
Starting point is 01:43:55 and norapinephrine from locustorialias. Is there any role for norapinephrine, epinephrine and cortisol released from the adrenals? My understanding is that norapineanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanephanep in the brainwaring or epinephrine is released from, or is it just Locus released? So there are seven, nine different adonurgic, yes, there's nine different adonurgic extractions. I should know this. No, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, is like the one main. Yeah, that's the one that goes to the cortex. And the locus rilles is also the one that goes to the cortex. But there are other adonurgic sources,
Starting point is 01:44:50 some that from the brain stem that descend and help us to ignore pain, for example, when we're stressed and needing to run away from the tiger. We don't want to be thinking, oh my ankle hurts. You want to just be able to ignore it and do what you need to do. So yeah, so there are lots of other neurodinergic nuclei, but the locustoros is the main one that projects all over the brain.
Starting point is 01:45:13 The actually the only place that doesn't project is the dorsal stridum. You talked about ventral stridum and addiction. The dorsal stridum is the only place the locustoros doesn't project to. And that's involved in procedural learning, motor learning, the kinds of learning that take over when your hippocampus, for example, is compromised, a bilateral, if you don't have a good hippocampus, you can still
Starting point is 01:45:35 do procedural learning and it's great, it's a redundant system. And so if your locustorialis is not working, if you don't have it anymore, you can still do, if you don't have a good help account, as you can still do learning through this dorsal straight-on structure. So it might be for those kinds of learning functions, sleep deprivation where you never let the locusturalist stop firing is okay because it doesn't have any
Starting point is 01:46:02 receptors for North and Epirin anyway. So yeah. And what about bodily adrenals? Yeah. You know, I often, in my people, there's no such thing as adrenal burnout per se, that adrenals don't actually burn out. But some people have adrenal insufficiency syndrome. Other people have adrenals that are just chronically cranking out epinephrine
Starting point is 01:46:26 or epinephrine and cortisol at the wrong times in particular. Yeah, yeah. So that, there's a great questions and I think the answers to them have yet to be discovered. The connections between our periphery and our central nervous system. But we know that there are beautiful connections and it's untapped source of being able to manipulate our brains is to work through our bodies. And so, our juveniles do great things. They constrict our blood vessels,
Starting point is 01:46:55 causing higher blood pressure, which help blood rush out to all the extremities that need blood, or muscles, for example, for running away from the lion or the tiger or when we're eating a grant deadline or catching a train. Yeah. The dreamtals help our hearts bump faster. Our muscles get perfused with the blood it needs. It diverts blood and everything away from our parasympathetic system, which is rest and digest.
Starting point is 01:47:27 We don't really need to digest that croissant when we're running for a train, and we can do that later. So it's doing really important things. What we don't know because it doesn't cross the blood brain barrier is how that affects the brain, and whether or if we can independently activate our adrenals, when a time when our brain thinks that we should be fine and calm in asleep, how our brain detects that. Is it a feedback through, your heart is racing and then our brain stents as what's going
Starting point is 01:47:59 on, my heart is racing and then wakes us up and then our hearts are racing together with our brain racing. We just don't know the answers to these questions yet. There are some good good studies, old studies, but we need a lot more. I will another nod to the fact that there's lots of great work ongoing and still to do. I'd love for you to tell us about some of the work that you're doing more recently on the relationship between sleep and opiate use, withdrawal, relapse, and craving, just addiction generally. I get a lot of questions about people trying to come off benzodiazepines or people's challenges with benzodiazepine and other types of addiction.
Starting point is 01:48:45 Yeah, what is the role of sleep in addiction and recovery from addiction and opiates in particular? Yeah, well this is a very young area and in fact my laboratory has just started. I have a graduate student who's been in my lab for just one year. She's done amazing work already, but completely groundbreaking work. And what she has discovered already, we don't have the paper out yet, but we're working on it, is that when animals withdraw from opiates, and this has been sort of replicated in other ways with other types of things,
Starting point is 01:49:19 our sleep is disturbed, our sleep is terribly disturbed, and the amount of sleep disturbance predicts relapse behaviors. And you might think, well, of course, you're going to relapse if you can't sleep because opiates calm you down. Well, the reason why opiates calm you down is because the locus are realists again. LuSpot is covered with opiate receptors that are normally really responsive to our endogenous
Starting point is 01:49:42 opiates. And so what happens when we're pleased, for example, or laughing, or whatever, endogenous opiates activate those receptors in the locus-realis and calm it down. And it actually suppresses locus-realis activity, makes us happy and relaxed. One of the things reasons why opiates are so addictive is because it also calms us down and makes us relaxed. One of the things reasons why opiates are so addictive is because it also calms us down and makes us relaxed. But the problem with
Starting point is 01:50:09 exogenous opiates is that they really strongly bind these receptors on our locusturalis and if you take exogenous opiates again and again like you're recovering from surgery, for example, take these pain medications is that our locusturalis struggles to do what it's supposed to do, which is keep us awake and learning and concentrating on things. So it will down-regulate. It will internalize these receptors
Starting point is 01:50:34 that are normally only occupied by endogenous opiates. And it will change our genes that are associated with producing these receptors. So you actually have very many fewer receptors. So the locus rules, at least during wakefulness, can fire and help us to do these things like learn about our environment. And so if you long-term reduce the number of receptors out there, then when you withdraw the exogenous opiates, not enough of your endogenous opiates to be able to occupy those few receptors that are there, and our locustrails has nothing to calm it down anymore, no pacifier, and it just fires and fires,
Starting point is 01:51:12 and fires, and that phasic and tonic high activity stresses us out, because it's normally associated with stress. And so any exogenous stressor that adds to that and also activates our locus aryls, there's nothing to comb it down again. And so it just keeps firing, it disturbs our sleep. And that's why maybe sleep disturbance is an indicator of a hyperactive locus arylis. And such a good predictor of relapse behaviors because nobody likes to live in that high stress state. And they'll do anything to get back to normal. So the problem with taking these drugs
Starting point is 01:51:57 is that it leaves you excited and sorry, excited, relaxed and happy. But then when you come off of it, you're worse than when you wear it baseline. You take it again. It only brings you up this far because you have fewer receptors. When you come off it, you're down even more depressed and anxious. And I depressed as a word I use loosely. And that's not what I say.
Starting point is 01:52:22 Certainly central nervous system depression. I mean, sleepier, less motivated, lower mood. Yeah, I mean our locustarilis is actually, it's the anxiety kind of depression, actually the anxiety related depression. So yeah, so we don't know yet what, and there's some good research going on right now. What could restore our own endogenous receptors so that
Starting point is 01:52:45 our endogenous opiates can properly calm our locustarilis once that they have been tamped down by exogenous opiates, but that would be really one way that you can access the sleep disturbance. So we talked about sleep and the importance of sleep in terms of learning and memory, the importance of the structure of the 90-minute cycle for all of that. So you can imagine if your sleep is disturbed by too much locustal illness activity, the structure and the function of those sleep spindles and that theta during REM sleep,
Starting point is 01:53:16 and the lack of neuropinephrine, all of those structures, all those functions for learning, something new like a new behavior that as as an involved the drug becomes compromised. And so that's something that Tanya Lugo's in collaboration with Pamela Kennedy at UCLA that we're looking at. How is learning and memory affected by the sleep disturbance?
Starting point is 01:53:41 If they're way we can, in animals that are coming off of opiates, can we restore their sleep to normal so that then they are less likely to do relapse kinds of behaviors? Fascinating and I will certainly have to have you back on to tell us the results of those studies. Meanwhile, I think for anyone who's trying to come off opiates exogenous opiates and restore these systems, what I'm hearing is that it's going to take some time, but that any and all things that people can do to buffer their healthy, normal sleep architecture, like morning and daytime sunlight,
Starting point is 01:54:17 limiting bright light exposure, lowering the temperature at night, a number of things that we've talked about in this podcast. All in yourself, do you think we're asleep? Do you think extra sunsets is meditation, whatever it is that helps you calm yourself before sleep. Yeah. Right. Would facilitate not just sleep, but perhaps even accelerate the recovery and shorten this period of withdrawal, which from the questions I get and from what I hear can be
Starting point is 01:54:40 absolutely brutal. Yeah. Oh, I can imagine. I had to take opiates for I only took it for three days after giving birth to my first son, I think, second son, one of them. And just I just said after three days, this is enough, I'm just going to try Tylenol. And so I weaned myself, not weaned, I just did a sudden sharp cut off. And even though I felt I didn't get the high of opiates when I was taking the Tylenol coating, when I went off at boy, it was like PMS times
Starting point is 01:55:14 100. I was so anxious and upset at little things. And thankfully only lasted a few hours. But if I had taken it for a week or two weeks, who knows if my endogenous opiate receptors would have been permanently downregulated and I would have been an addict. An addict, I would have been addicted, I shouldn't say, an addict. There's negative connotations. It's just a very physiological state, so no judgments at all associated with it. Yeah, they're powerful, powerful painkillers, but can also alter your entire brain and rewire it.
Starting point is 01:55:49 Yeah. Well, all the more reason why I and many others are grateful that you're doing this work to figure out ways that people can recover more quickly and more thoroughly. I must say you taught us a tremendous amount in a relatively short amount of time about the architecture of sleep, the different phases, the relationship between sleep and dreaming
Starting point is 01:56:10 in this incredible structure, Locust Rulius. And I'm so happy we also got into the ponds that just delights me because we rarely talk about the ponds on this podcast, but such an interesting structure. Sex differences that are important in creativity and problem solving and trauma sleep spindles, just such a wealth of information and much of it that's actionable for people. So first of all, I want to say thank you for taking the time to sit down and have this conversation that so many people are sure to benefit from. I also want to thank you for doing the work you do, even though I'm a fellow neurobiologist.
Starting point is 01:56:42 I think that it's not often that we take a step back and realize that it's really the work of hard thinking, hard, strongly motivated PIs, and it stands for principal investigator, by the way, PIs like yourself, graduate students and postdocs that really drive the discovery forward and that lead to these new therapeutics. Physicians are wonderful, clinicians are absolutely wonderful, but clinicians don't develop new treatments. They only implement the ones that researchers discover. So thank you for being a brain explorer with a focus on growing the good in the world.
Starting point is 01:57:21 I know I speak for everybody when I say thank you so much. Thank you so much Andrew, thank you for being an amazing interviewer. You brought a lot out of me in a coherent, forklift fashion that normally I can't do when I'm speaking in public. I don't know about that. I've heard your lectures and they're superb. We'll direct people to some of the other ones. Well, thank you. And I also want to put a plug in for graduate students in general and the key and amazing role that they play in research.
Starting point is 01:57:46 I'm a PI, as you said. I used to be a graduate student and a postdoc trainee myself doing all of this on the ground, hands on experimentation. It's so hard to do. It's so hard to do right. It's so hard to think through all of that. Now, I'm a PI. I get to be an idea person and just say, hey, why don't you do this? And, hey, you know, what do you think about that? And they, of course, intellectually contribute so much to these planned experiments, but they also do the really hard work. And so I just want to say thank you graduate students, thank you to my graduate students and all graduate students out there.
Starting point is 01:58:21 Thank you post office. You're all underpaid and listen to the major institutions, Stanford, UCLA, and all other major institutions pay them more, please. We need them and they need to have a standard of living. Yeah. Not, I'm not afraid to say that, despite my primary employer, pay them more, they need it, they deserve it. They deserve it.
Starting point is 01:58:38 Absolutely. Great. Well, we will absolutely have you back again, if you'll be willing. And meanwhile, we will direct people to where they can learn more about you and your exciting work. And once again, thanks so much. Thank you so much. Thank you for joining me today for my discussion about sleep, mental health, physical health,
Starting point is 01:58:55 and performance with Dr. Gina Poe. I hope you found it to be as informative and as actionable as I did. In fact, I'm already implementing the regularity of bedtime plus or minus half an hour in order to get that growth hormone release. And I can already see both my sleep scores improving and my feelings of daytime vigor and focus and other markers of sleep health improving as well. If you're learning from Endor and enjoying this podcast, please subscribe to our YouTube channel.
Starting point is 01:59:20 That's a terrific zero-cost way to support us. In addition, please subscribe to the podcast on both Spotify and Apple. And on both Spotify and Apple, you can also leave us up to a five-star review. If you have questions for us or comments or suggestions about guests you'd like me to include on the podcast, please put those in the comments section on YouTube. I do read all the comments.
Starting point is 01:59:38 In addition, please check out the sponsors mentioned at the beginning and throughout today's episode. That's the best way to support this podcast. On the Hubert Mennel podcast, we often discuss supplements. While supplements aren't necessary for everybody, many people derive tremendous benefit from them for things like enhancing the quality and duration of sleep, for enhancing focus, and for hormone support.
Starting point is 01:59:57 The Hubert-Millab podcast is proud to announce that we've partnered with Momentus Supplements. We've done that for several reasons. First of all, Momentus Supplements are of the very highest quality. Second of all, they mainly focus on single ingredient formulations, which is absolutely key if you want to develop a supplement regimen that's most biologically and cost effective. It, for instance, allows you to alternate dosages across days to change the dosages of individual ingredients, so on and so forth. In addition, momentous supplements ship internationally, which we know is important because many of you reside
Starting point is 02:00:26 outside of the United States. If you'd like to see the supplements discussed on the Hubertman Lab podcast, please go to LiveMomentus spelled O-U-S, so that's livemomentus.com slash Hubertman, and you can get a 20% discount on any of those supplements. Again, it's livemomentus.com slash Hubertman. The Hubertman Lab podcast has a zero-cost newsletter that includes summaries of podcast episodes and toolkits. to Just put in your email, we do not share your email with anybody, and you'll get our monthly newsletter. There are also some examples of previous newsletters also there at HubermanLab.com. If you're not already following me on social media, it is HubermanLab on all platforms,
Starting point is 02:01:13 Instagram, Twitter, Facebook, and LinkedIn. At all of those places, I cover science and science-related tools, some of which overlap with the content of the HubermanLab podcast, but much of which is distinct from the Huberman Lab podcast content. Again, that's Huberman Lab podcast, but much of which is distinct from the Uberman Lab podcast content. Again, that's Uberman Lab on all social media platforms. Thank you again for joining me for today's discussion with Dr. Gina Poe, all about sleep, and its relationship to mental health, physical health,
Starting point is 02:01:35 and performance, and last, but certainly not least, thank you for your interest in science. Thanks.

There aren't comments yet for this episode. Click on any sentence in the transcript to leave a comment.