Huberman Lab - Dr. Karl Deisseroth: Understanding & Healing the Mind
Episode Date: June 28, 2021Dr. Karl Deisseroth, MD, PhD, is a Clinical Psychiatrist and scientist who directs a bioengineering research laboratory at Stanford University School of Medicine. His work aims to understand and devel...op treatments for disorders of the mind such as depression, attention deficit disorders (ADHD & ADD), autism, schizophrenia, anxiety, eating disorders, borderline personality and obsessive-compulsive disorder. We discuss his experience treating his patients and his laboratory’s mission to find and develop cures for mental disease and tools for probing how the brain works. For the full show notes, visit hubermanlab.com. Thank you to our sponsors AG1 (Athletic Greens): https://athleticgreens.com/huberman LMNT: https://drinklmnt.com/huberman Supplements from Momentous https://www.livemomentous.com/huberman Timestamps (00:00:00) Introduction (00:03:47) Sponsors: AG1, LMNT (00:07:41) Using Language to Understand the Mind (00:12:19) Blood Tests For Mental Disease (00:13:38) The Largest Challenges Facing Treatment of Mental Health (00:20:21) Predicting Depression & Suicide (00:22:47) Drugs That Work for Brain Illness (00:27:01) What Would A Cure For the Broken Mind Look Like? (00:32:23) Channelopsins: Tools For Understanding & Treating the Mind (00:39:10) Curing Blindness with Channelopsins (00:41:58) Why Karl Became a Scientist (00:47:10) Vagus Nerve In Depression (00:54:12) Challenges To Overcome for Treating Mental Illness with Channelopsins (00:58:34) Using the Dialogue with Patients to Guide Treatment (01:00:52) How Our Eyes Reveal Our Mental Health (01:06:04) Controlling Structures Deep In the Brain (01:08:23) The Most Effective Drugs Often Have the Most Side Effects (01:09:50) Do Psychiatrists Take the Drugs They Prescribe (01:14:15) Moving From Experimental Tools To Novel Treatments (01:16:00) Brain-Machine Interfaces & Neuralink (01:19:30) ADHD & Dr. Deissroth’s Approach To Focusing His Mind (01:26:36) How Dr. Deisseroth Balances A Career In Medicine, Science & Family (01:35:41) New Ways of Exploring Brains: CLARITY (01:38:49) What Is Special About the Human Brain? (01:46:03) Psychedelics (01:54:12) MDMA (01:57:15) Dr. Deisseroth’s New Book “Projections: A Story of Emotions” (01:59:42) Connecting with Dr. Deisseroth on Twitter Title Card Photo Credit: Mike Blabac Disclaimer
Transcript
Discussion (0)
Welcome to the Huberman Lab podcast where we discuss science and science-based tools for everyday life.
I'm Andrew Huberman and I'm a professor of neurobiology and ophthalmology at Stanford School of Medicine.
Today I have the pleasure of introducing the first guest of the Huberman Lab podcast.
My guest is Dr. Carl Diceroth.
Dr. Carl Diceroth is a medical doctor, he's a psychiatrist,
and a research scientist at Stanford School of Medicine.
In his clinical practice,
he sees patients dealing with a range
of nervous system disorders,
including obsessive-compulsive disorder, autism,
attention deficit disorders, schizophrenia,
mania, anxiety disorders, and eating disorders.
His laboratory develops and explores tools
with which to understand how the nervous system works in the healthy situation as well as
in disorders of the mind. Dr. Diceross laboratory has pioneered the development and use of
what are called channel options, proteins that come from algae, which can now be introduced
to the nervous systems of animals and humans
in order to precisely control the activity of neurons in the brain and body with the use of light.
This is a absolutely transformative technology because whereas certain drug treatments can
often relieve certain symptoms of disorders, they often carry various side effects. And in some
individuals, often many individuals, these drug treatments
simply do not work. The channel opson and their related technologies stand to transform
the way that we treat psychiatric illness and various disorders of movement and perception.
In fact, just recently, the channel opson were applied in a human patient to allow an
adult fully blind human being to see light for the very first time.
We also discuss Dr. Diceross newly released book, which is entitled,
Projections, a Story of Human Emotions. This is an absolutely remarkable book that uses
stories about his interactions with his patients to teach you how the brain works in the healthy
and disease state and also reveals the motivation
for and discovery of these channel opposites and other technologies by Carl's laboratory
that are being used now to treat various disorders of the nervous system, and that in the future
are certain to transform the fields of psychiatry, mental health, and health in general.
I found our conversation to be an absolutely fascinating one about how the brain
functions in the healthy state and why and how it breaks down in disorders of the mind. We also
discuss the current status and future of psychedelic treatments for psychiatric illness, as well as for
understanding how the brain works more generally. We also discuss issues of consciousness, and we even
delve into how somebody like Carl,
who's managing a full-time clinical practice and a 40-plus person laboratory and a family
of five children and is happily married, how he organizes his internal landscape, his
own thinking in order to manage that immense workload and to progress forward for the
sake of medicine and his pursuits and science.
I found this to be an incredible conversation I learned so much.
I also learned through the course of reading Carl's book, Projections,
that not only is he an accomplished psychiatrist and obviously an accomplished
research scientist and a family man, but he's also a phenomenal writer.
Projections is absolutely masterfully written.
It's just beautiful and it's accessible to anybody,
even if you don't have a science background.
So I hope that you'll enjoy my conversation
with Carl Dyseroth as much as I did,
and thank you for tuning in.
Before we begin, I want to point out
that this podcast is separate
from my teaching and research roles at Stanford.
In my desire and effort to bring zero cost
to consumer information about science
and science-related tools to the general public, I'd like to acknowledge the sponsors of today's podcast.
Our first sponsor is Athletic Greens.
Athletic Greens is an all-in-one vitamin mineral probiotic drink.
I've been taking Athletic Greens since 2012, so I'm delighted that they're sponsoring
the podcast.
The reason I started taking Athletic Greens and the reason I still take Athletic Greens
once or twice today is that it helps me cover all of my basic nutritional needs.
It makes up for any deficiencies that I might have.
In addition, it has probiotics, which are vital for microbiome health.
I've done a couple of episodes now on the so-called gut microbiome and the ways in which
the microbiome interacts with your immune system, with your brain to regulate mood, and
essentially with every biological system relevant
to health throughout your brain and body.
With athletic greens, I get the vitamins I need,
the minerals I need, and the probiotics
to support my microbiome.
If you'd like to try athletic greens,
you can go to atlettagreens.com slash Huberman
and claim a special offer.
They'll give you five free travel packs,
plus a year supply of vitamin D3K2.
There are a ton of data now showing that vitamin D3 is essential
for various aspects of our brain and body health,
even if we're getting a lot of sunshine,
many of us are still deficient in vitamin D3.
And K2 is also important because it regulates things
like cardiovascular function, calcium in the body,
and so on.
Again, go to atletagreens.com slash huberman
to claim the special offer of the five free travel packs and the year
supply of vitamin D3 K2.
Today's episode is also brought to us by
element.
Elements is an electrolyte drink that has
everything you need and nothing you don't.
That means the exact ratios of electrolytes
are an element and those are sodium, magnesium,
and potassium, but it has no sugar.
I've talked many times before on this
podcast about the key role of hydration and electrolytes
for nerve cell function, neuron function, as well as the function of all the cells and
all the tissues and organ systems of the body.
If we have sodium, magnesium, and potassium present in the proper ratios, all of those
cells function properly and all our bodily systems can be optimized.
If the electrolytes are not present and if hydration is low, we simply can't think as well
as we would otherwise.
Our mood is off, hormone systems go off, our ability to get into physical action, to engage
an endurance and strength, and all sorts of other things is diminished.
So with element, you can make sure that you're staying on top of your hydration and that
you're getting the proper ratios of electrolytes.
If you'd like to try element, you can go to DrinkElement.
That's LMNT.com slash Huberman, and you'll get a free element sample pack with your purchase.
They're all delicious.
So again, if you want to try element, you can go to elementlmnt.com slash Huberman.
And now, my conversation with Dr. Karl Diceroth.
Well, thanks for being here.
Thanks for having me.
It's been a long time coming for me because you may not know this,
but one of the reasons I started this podcast was actually so I could have this conversation.
It's, it's but one.
There are other reasons, but one of the goals is to be able to hold conversations with
colleagues of mine that are doing incredible work
in the realm of science.
And then here we also have this really special opportunity
because you're also a clinician.
You see patients and they're not for a long time.
So for people that might not be so familiar
with the fields of neuroscience, et cetera,
what is the difference between neurology and psychiatry?
Well, you know, I'm married to a neurologist and I am a psychiatrist and we make fun of each other all the time.
So this is a lot of neuroscientists and a lot of brain clinicians actually think these two should be the same field at some point in the future.
They were in the past, they started together. Psychiatry, though, focuses on disorders
where we can't see something that's physically wrong,
where we don't have a measurable, where there's no blood test
that makes the diagnosis, there's no brain scan that tells us,
this is schizophrenia, this is depression,
for an individual patient.
And so psychiatry is much more mysterious
and the only tools we have are words.
Neurologists are fantastic physicians.
They see the stroke on brain scans, they see the seizure and the pre-seizure activity with
an EEG and they can measure and treat based on those measurables.
In psychiatry, we have a harder job, I think.
We use words.
We have rating scales for symptoms.
We can measure depression and autism
with rating scales, but those are words still.
And ultimately, that's what psychiatry's built around.
It's an odd situation because we've got
the most complex, beautiful, mysterious, incredibly
engineered object in the universe, and yet all we have are words to find our way in.
So do you find that if a patient is very verbal or hyperverbal, that you have an easier
time diagnosing them as opposed to somebody who's more quiet and reserved, or I could
imagine the opposite might be true as well.
Well, because we only have words, you put your finger on a key point.
If they don't speak that much in principle, it's harder.
The lack of speech can be a symptom.
We can see that in depression.
We can see that in the negative symptoms of schizophrenia.
We can see that in autism sometimes by itself.
That is a symptom
reduced speech, but ultimately you do need something. You need some some words to help guide you.
And that in fact, and there's there's challenges that I can tell you about where patients with
depression or so depressed they can't speak. That makes it a bit of a challenge to distinguish
depression from some of the other reasons they might not be speaking.
And this is sort of the art and the science of psychiatry.
Do you find that there are patients that have, well, let's call them comorbidities or conditions where they would land in both psychiatry and neurology, meaning there's damage to a particular area of the brain and therefore they're depressed.
And how do you tease that out as a psychiatrist?
This happens all the time.
Parkinson's disease is a great example.
It can be debilitating in so many ways.
People have trouble moving, they have trouble walking, they have trouble swallowing, and
they can have truly severe depression.
And this is, you might say, oh, well, they've got a life-threatening illness.
But there are plenty of neurological disorders where depression is not a strong,
strongly comorbid symptom.
Like ALS, Lou Gehrig's disease, for example, and depression is not a strong,
strongly comorbid in that disease. but in Parkinson's it is extremely common.
And as you know, in Parkinson's disease we have loss of the dopamine neurons in the midbrain.
And this is a very, you know, specific population of cells that's dying and probably that leads
to both the movement disorder and the depression. There are
many examples of that where these two fields come together and you really need to work as
a team. I've had patients in my clinic that I treat the depression associated with their
Parkinson's and a neurologist treats the movement associated with the Parkinson's and we work together.
Do you think we will ever have a blood test for depression or schizophrenia or
autism? And would that be a good or a bad thing? I think ultimately there will be quantitative tests.
Already efforts are being made to look at certain rhythms in the brain using external EEGs,
to look at brain waves effectively, look at the ratios of certain frequencies to other frequencies.
And there's some progress being made on that front.
It's not as good as it could be.
It doesn't really give you the confidence for the individual patient that you would like.
But ultimately, what's going on in the brain in psychiatric disease is physical
and it's due to the circuits and the connections and the projections and the
brain that are not working as they would in a typical situation.
And I do think we'll have those measurements at some point.
Now, is that good or bad?
You know, I think that will be good.
One of the challenges we have with psychiatry is it is an art as well as a science to elicit these symptoms in a
precise way, it does take some time and it would be great if we could just do a
quick measurement. Could it be abused or misused? Certainly, but that's I think
true for all of medicine. I want to know and I'm sure there are several, but what do
you see as the biggest challenge facing psychiatry and the treatment of mental illness today?
I think we have, we're making progress on what the biggest challenge is, which I think
there's still such a strong stigma for psychiatric disease that patients often don't come to us and they feel that they should be able to
handle this on their own. And that can slow treatment, it can lead to
worsening symptoms. We know, for example, patients who have untreated anxiety
issues, if you go for a year or more with a serious untreated anxiety issue that can convert to depression.
You can add another problem on top of the anxiety.
And so it would be, you know, why do people not come for treatment?
They feel like this is something they should be able to master on their own, which can
be true, but usually some help is a good thing.
That raises a question related to something I heard you say
many years ago at a lecture, which was that
this was a scientific lecture and you said,
you know, we don't know how other people feel.
Most of the time, we don't even really know how we feel.
I mean, we could elaborate on that a little bit
and the dearth of ways that we have to talk about
feelings.
There are so many words, I don't know how many, but I'm guessing there are more than
a dozen words to describe the state that I call sadness, but as far as I understand,
we don't have any way of comparing that in a real objective sense.
So as a psychiatrist, when your job is to use words to diagnose, words
of the patient to diagnose, do you maneuver around that and what is this landscape that we
call feelings or emotions? This is really interesting. People, here we have
it, there's a tension between the words that we've built up in the clinic that mean something
to the physicians. And then there's the colloquial use of words that may not built up in the clinic that mean something to the physicians.
And then there's the colloquial use of words that may not be the same. And so that's the first
level we have to sort out. When someone says, you know, I'm depressed, what exactly do they mean by that?
That may be different from what we're talking about in terms of depression. So part of psychiatry is
to get beyond that word and to get into how they're
actually feeling, get rid of the jargon and get to real world examples of how they're feeling. So
how do you look forward into the future? How much hope do you have? How much planning are you
doing for the future? So these here, now you're getting into actual
things you can talk about that are unambiguous. If someone says, yeah, I can't even, I can't even think about tomorrow. I don't see how I'm going to get to tomorrow. That, that's a nice precise thing that,
you know, it's sad, it's tragic, but it's also, that means something, and we know what that means,
that's the hopelessness symptom of depression.
And that is what I try to do when I do a psychiatric interview. I try to get past the jargon
and get to what's actually happening in a patient's life and in their mind. But as you say,
ultimately, you know, this shows up across, I address this issue every day in my life, whether
it's in the lab where we're looking at
animals, whether fish or mice or rats, and studying their behavior, or when I'm in a conversation with just a friend or a colleague, or when I'm talking to a patient, I never really know what's going on
inside the mind of the other person. I get, I get some feedback, I get words, I get
behaviors, I get actions, but I never really know. And as you said at the very
beginning of the question, you know, often we don't even have the words and the
insight to even understand what's going on in our own mind. I think a lot of
psychiatrists are pretty introspective. That's part of the reason they end up in
that specialty. And so maybe we spend a little more time in the
average person thinking about what's going on within, but it doesn't mean we have answers.
So in this area of trying to figure out what's going on under the hood through words,
it sounds like certain words would relate to this idea of anticipation and hope.
Is it fair to say that that somehow relates
to the dopamine system in the sense that dopamine
is involved in motivated behaviors?
Is that, if I say, for instance, and I
won't ask you to run a session with me here for free?
We'll do that off camera.
OK, right.
If I were to say, I just can't imagine the tomorrow.
I just, I just can't do it. So that's, that's not action based. That's purely based on my,
my internal narrative. But I could imagine things like, you know, I, I have a terrible time
sleeping. I'm not hungry. I'm not eating. So it's statements about physical actions. I'm guessing also have validity. And there are now ways to measure the accuracy
of those statements. Like, for instance, if I gave you permission, you could know if
I slept last night or whether or not I was just saying I had a poor night sleep. So in
moving forward through 2021 and into the next 10 and 100 years of
psychiatry, do you think that the body reporting some of the actions of a human are going to become
useful and and mesh with the words in a way that's going to make your job easier? I do think that's
true. And these the two things you've mentioned eating and sleeping those are additional
Criteria that we use to diagnose depression these are the vegetative signs we call them of depression poor sleep and poor eating
And if you have a baseline for somebody that's the real challenge, so what's different in that person?
Some people would depressed they sleep more some people would who are depressed they sleep less
Some people who are depressed they're more physically agitated and they move around more some people who are depressed, they sleep less. Some people who are depressed, they're more physically agitated and they move around more.
Some people who are depressed, they move less,
even while they're awake.
And so you need, here's the challenge,
is you can't just look at how they are now.
You have to get a baseline and then see how it's changed.
And that can be a challenge that raises ethical issues,
how do you collect that baseline information
from someone healthy?
I don't think that's something we have solved.
Of course, we are with phones and accelerometers and phones.
You could, in principle, collect a lot of baseline information
from people, but that would have to be treated very carefully
for privacy reasons.
And in terms of measuring one's own behavior,
I've heard of work that's going on,
Sam Golden up in the University of Washington
who works on aggression and animal models
was telling me that there's some effort that he's making
and perhaps you're involved in this work as well.
I don't know of devices that would allow people
to detect, for instance, when they're
veering towards a depressive episode for instance, when they're veering towards a
depressive episode for themselves, that they may choose or not choose to report that to their
clinician. Maybe they don't even have a clinician. Maybe this person that you referred to at the
beginning, this person who doesn't feel comfortable coming to talk to you, they maybe something is
measuring changes in the inflection of their voice or the speed at which they get up from a chair.
Do you think that those kind of metrics will eventually inform somebody, hey, you know,
you're in trouble.
This is getting to this question of back to the statement that I heard you make and rung
in my mind now, I think, for more than a decade, which is oftentimes we don't even know how
we feel.
Yeah.
You know, that, I do like because that gives the patient the agency to detect what's going
on.
And even separate from modern technology, this has been part of the art of psychiatry
is to help patients realize that sometimes other people observing them can give them
the earliest warning signs of depression.
We see this very often in family, they'll notice when the patient is changing before the
patient does.
And then there are things the patient may notice, but not correctly.
Ascribe to the onset of depression and a classic example of that is what we call early morning
awakening.
And this is something that can happen very early as people start to slide into depression.
They start to wake up earlier and earlier, you know, just inexplicably their wake up.
This is like 2 a.m. 3 a.m.
Yeah, it could start.
Yeah, it could start at 5am, could go to 4am.
And unable to fall back asleep.
Unable to fall back asleep exactly.
So that's, and that they may not know what to do with that.
It could just be from their perspective,
it's just something that's happening.
But if you put enough of that information together,
that could be a useful warning sign for the patient,
and it could help them seek treatment.
And I think that is something that could be really valuable.
Interesting.
So in this framework of needing words to self-report
or machines to detect how we feel
or maybe inform a psychiatrist how a patient feels,
wanna touch on some of the technologies
that you've been involved in building, but as
a way to march into that, are there any very good treatments for psychiatric disease?
Meaning, are there currently any pills, potions, forms of communication that reliably work
every time or work in most patients?
Could you give a couple examples of great successes
of psychiatry if they exist?
Yes.
Yeah, we are fortunate.
And this coming back to my, you know, the joking
between my wife and myself in terms of neurology
and psychiatry, we actually, in psychiatry,
despite the depths of our, the mystery we struggle with,
many of our treatments are actually,
we may be doing better than some other specialties
in terms of actually causing therapeutic benefit
for patients.
We do help patients, the patients who suffer
from, by the way, both medications and talk therapy
have been shown to be extremely effective in many cases.
For example, people with panic disorder,
cognitive behavioral therapy, just working with words,
helping people identify the early signs
when they're starting to move toward a panic attack.
What are the cognitions that are happening?
You can train people to derail that,
and you can very, potently treat panic disorder that way.
How long does something like that take on average?
For a motivated, insightful patient,
you can have a very cookbook-y series of sessions,
six to 12 sessions, or even less, for someone who's very insightful and motivated, and can have a
very powerful effect that quickly. And that's just with words, there are many psychiatric
medications that are very effective for the conditions that they're treating
anti-psychotic medications, they have side effects, but boy, do they work, they really
can clear up particularly the positive symptoms of schizophrenia, for example, the auditory hallucinations,
the paranoia.
People's lives can be turned around by these.
We should clarify positive symptoms.
You mean not positive in the qualitative sense,
you mean positive meaning that the appearance of something abnormal. Exactly. Yeah, thank you
for that clarification. When we say positive symptoms, we do mean the addition of something that
wasn't there before, like a hallucination or a paranoia. And that stands in contrast to the negative
symptoms where something is taken away. And these are patients who are withdrawn. They have what we call thought
blocking. They can't even progress forward in a sequence of thoughts. Both of those can be
part of schizophrenia. The hallucinations in the paranoia are more effectively treated right now,
but they are effectively treated. And then this is a frustrating and yet heartening aspect of psychiatry.
There are treatments like electroconvulsive therapy, which is, you know,
it's extremely effective for depression. We have patients who nothing else works for them,
or they can't tolerate medications, and you can administer under very safe controlled condition,
where the patient's body is not moving.
They're put into a very safe situation where the body doesn't move or sees.
It's just an internal process that's triggered in the brain.
This is an extraordinarily effective treatment for treatment resistant depression.
At the same time, I find it as hardening as it is to see patients respond to this with with a who have severe depression, I'm also frustrated by it. Why why can't we do
something more precise than than this for these very severe cases and people
have sought for decades to understand how is it that a seizure is leading to
the relief of depression and we don't know the answer yet. We would love to do
that. People are working hard on that.
But that is a treatment that does work too.
All in all of these cases though in psychiatry,
the frustrating thing is that we don't have
the level of understanding that a cardiologist has
and thinking about the heart.
You know, the heart is, we now know it's a pump,
it's pumping blood and so you can look at everything
about how it's working or not working in terms of that frame.
It's clearly a pump. We don't really have that level of what, what is the circuit really there for in psychiatry?
And that's, that's what is missing. That's what we need to find so we can design truly effective and specific treatments.
so we can design truly effective and specific treatments. So, what are the pieces that are gonna be required
to curatism, cure Parkinson's, cure schizophrenia?
I would imagine there are several elements
and bins here, understanding that the natural biology,
understanding what the activity patterns are,
how to modify those.
Maybe you could just tell us what you think.
What is the bento box of the perfect cure?
Yeah.
I think the first thing we need is understanding.
We need almost every psychiatric treatment has been serendipitously identified,
just noting by chance that something that was done for some person also had a side effect.
Like lithium or psych...
Lithium as a good example.
Is it true that it was the urine of guinea pigs given lithium that was given to manic
patients that made them not manic?
Is that true?
I don't have a first hand knowledge of that, but I would defer that.
But it's true for essentially every treatment, you know, that the antidepressants originally
you know, aroses, antide tuberculosis drugs, for example.
I did not know that.
And so this is a classic example for, and this is across all of psychiatry.
And of course, the seizures as well, that was noticed that patients who had epilepsy,
they had a seizure, and also had depression that they became much, at least for a while,
they were improved after the seizure. That's amazing. I don't want to take you off course of the question that,
answering the question I asked, but I've heard before that if autistic children get a fever,
that their symptoms improve, is that true? I've done a fair bit of work with autism.
In my clinical practice, I work with adult autism, and I have heard statements like that and descriptions like that from patients and their families.
That is very hard to study quantitatively because often with the children you have this not as quantitative as you like collection of symptom information from home.
But I have heard that enough that I think there may well be something to that.
And you know, what is, anytime you have a fever, what's going on, what we know, all the
cells in the brain, and I know this as an electrophysiologist, if you just change the
temperature by a few degrees, everything changes about how neurons work, and that's even just
a single neuron. It's even more likely to be complex and different with a circuit of neurons that are all affecting each other, just elevate the temperature a little bit, it's even more likely to be complex and different with a circuit of neurons
that are all affecting each other, just elevate the temperature a little bit, everything's
different. And so it's plausible for sure that things like that could happen and do happen.
Now, but, and yet, when you think about autism to take your example, yes, we see changes, but
what is the element in the brain that's analogous to the pumping
heart?
When we think about the symptoms of depression, that's maybe, you know, we think about motivation
and dopamine neurons.
When we think about autism, it's a little more challenging.
There's a deficit in social interaction and in communication.
And so where is that? Where is that situated? What is the key principle governing the social
interaction? This is where we need the basic science to bring us a step forward. It's
we can say, okay, this is the process that's going on. This is what's needed for the incredibly complex task of social interaction where you've got incredibly rich data streams
of sound and meaning, eye contact, body movement. And that's just for one person, but if there's a group
of people, this is overwhelming for people with autism. What's the unifying thing there? It's a lot of information. And that maybe
is unmatched in any realm of biology, the amount of information coming in through a social
interaction, particularly with words and language. And so then that turns our attention as
neuroscientists, we think, okay, let's think about the parts of the brain that are involved in dealing with emerging complex
data streams that are very high in bit rate that need to be fused together into a unitary
concept.
That starts to guide us, and maybe we can, and we know other animals are social in their
own way, and we can study those animals.
That's how I think about it.
There's hope for the future thinking about the symptoms as an engineer might, and trying
to identify the circuits that are likely working to make this typical behavior happen, and
that will help us understand how it becomes atypical.
So that seems like the first, to me, the first bin of this, what I call a bento box, for
lack of a better analogy, that we need to know the
circuits, we need to know the cells in the various brain regions and portions of the body
and how they connect to one another and what the patterns of activity are under a normal,
quote unquote, healthy interaction.
If we understand that, then it seems that the next step, which of course could be carried
out in parallel, right, that work can be done alongside work where various elements within
those circuits are tweaked, just right, like the tuning of a piano in the subtle way,
or maybe even like the replacement of a whole set of keys if the piano is lacking keys,
so to speak.
You've been very involved in trying to generate those tools.
Tell us about channel options, why you created them, and where they're at now in the laboratory
and perhaps also in the clinic.
This is, first of all, I give nature the credit for creating channel revapsons.
These are beautiful little proteins that are made by algae, single-celled, green algae.
And it's a great story in basic science that our understanding of animal behavior, sensation,
cognition, and action in our brains all the way back to a botanist in the 1850s and 1860s in Russia
is where the story begins. So this was a botanist named Andre Fomensen who worked at St. Petersburg
and he had noticed in the river near his laboratory that there were algae that he could look at
near his laboratory that there were algae that he could look at in a dish in a saucer. He could put them there and we had light shining from the side.
The green tinge in the saucer of water would move to a particular distance from the light
that he was shining from the side, which was an amazing thing.
If he made the light brighter,
the green tinge would back off a little bit
to a more optimal location, so just the right light level.
So this was plant behavior.
It was light driven plant behavior,
and he delved into this a little bit.
He identified that with my microscopy,
he could see that there were little single cell algae
with flagella that were swimming to the right light level.
So behaving plants, and this has been a secret that's helped us unlock so many principles
of animal behavior.
So it turns out, you know, these algae achieve this amazing result with a single gene that encodes a single protein.
What's a protein?
It's just a little biomotule that does a job in a cell.
And these are proteins that sit in the surface of cells in their surface membrane.
And when a photon, a light particle hits them, they open a little pore, a little hole in
the membrane, and charge particles
ions like sodium rush across the pore. Now, why do they do that? They do that to guide
their flagella. That signal coming in, those ions coming in through the pore in response
to light, guide their flagella motor that guides them to a particular spot in the saucer.
Now, that's plant behavior, but it turns out, as you know,
this movement of ions across the membrane,
this happens to also be neural code in our brains for on or off.
Sodom ions rushing into cells turns them on,
makes them fire away, fire action potentials communicate
to the next cell down the chain.
And this is an amazing opportunity because we can borrow these proteins. In fact, we can take the gene
that directs the creation of the protein and we can use genetic tricks, modern genetic tricks to
put that gene into neurons in the brains of mammals and then use light to turn those cells, the specific
cells that we put this gene into, turn them on. There are other options we call them that
you can use to turn cells off. It's all fast real time. You can play in patterns of activity
and real time into cells or kinds of cells, just as a conductor,
it elicits the music from the orchestra,
the strings, and the woodwinds.
And you can see what matters,
what matters for sensation, what matters for cognition,
what matters for action, and we call this optogenetics.
Beautiful, and I must say it was quite an honor
and a privilege to watch optogenetics move
from idea to discovery to the laboratory.
I think we were postdocs at the same time, which is a living proof that people move at
different rates because that's a joke at my expense, by the way.
But it's we end up in the same spot. That's a really, really, really more or less. Physically, if not professionally, but nonetheless,
it's been a marvelous story thus far.
And I'd like to, maybe you could give us,
I'd like to just touch on a couple of examples
of where the technology resides in laboratories now.
So maybe the range of animals that it's being used in,
and some of the phenomenon that channel out,
channel out, and theyopsens and their related genes
and proteins are starting to elicit what you've seen.
And then I'd like to talk about their applicability
to the clinic, which is I think the bigger mission, if you will.
So this is, you know, this whole thing,
you know, it's been about now going on at 17 years that we've
been putting channel red options into neurons. It started just like Andre Femincins work in a dish
by 2000, that was in 2004 and 2007, we were putting these into behaving mice and we were able to
to, with the, the kind of switch cause, cause them to move one direction or another.
By 2009.
So basically, you're controlling the mouse's behavior.
Yeah, exactly.
In real time.
So we could make a mouse that was just sitting there
doing nothing to then turn left very consistently.
In fact, go around in a circle.
And as soon as we turn off the light, it would stop.
That was an eye opening moment.
It took really a few years to make optogenetics work.
There was a lot of putting all the,
there were a lot of problems that had to be solved.
These, these channel redopsens actually don't move many ions.
They have a small current, small conductance, as we say.
And so we had to figure out ways to pack a lot of them
into cells without damaging cells and still make them
targetable. So we don't want them to just be in all the cells because then it
becomes just like an electrode. You're just stimulating all the cells that are
nearby. We had to keep that specificity, make them targetable to just one kind of
cell or another while still packing in large numbers of them into those cells.
And we had to get in the light and safe in specific ways.
And so it took probably about four or five years
to really create optogenetics between 2004 and 2009.
By the end of that time, though, we had all the basic
light delivery, gene delivery principles worked out,
and people started to apply the technology to fish, to rats, to mice, to non-human primates,
like monkeys.
And just a couple months ago, my colleague, Botan Roska in Switzerland, succeeded in putting
Chandler-Dopsins into the eyes of human beings and making a blind person to see.
And so that's pretty cool.
This was a patient with retinal degeneration and he provided a channel redopsin
into the eye of this patient and was able to confer some light sensitivity onto this patient
that wasn't there before.
An amazing paper and discovery.
I realize it was one patient, but it's such an important milestone. Well, it's, as you say, it's a very important milestone. And the history of that
is very deep. Almost 10 years earlier, Botan Roscan, I had published a paper in science in human
retina, but ex-planted, taken from cadavers from someone who had died, that living retina taken out, opsyns put into this retinal tissue and showing that it worked,
recording from the cells showing that in these human neurons, retinal neurons
that you could get light responses.
But then from that moment, you know, almost 10 years of, you know,
how clinical development goes, and this is a gene therapy.
So you've got all the regulations
and concerns and all that.
It took almost 10 years to get to this point now where a living human being has a new
functionality that wasn't there before.
Now that's incredibly inspiring and it's a beautiful thing.
I would say, though, that the broader significance of autogenics is really still understanding,
because once you understand how the circuitry works and which cells actually matter, then
any kind of treatment becomes more grounded and logical and specific and principled.
And whether it's a medication or a talk therapy or brain stimulation treatment with electrical
or magnetic means.
If you actually know what matters, that is incredibly powerful.
And I think no, no, you know, not intended to disparage this beautiful, you know,
retinal work and conferring, you know, vision on someone who couldn't see, of course, that's wonderful.
But that's direct, what you might call direct after genetics and patients.
Indirect is everything that comes from understanding.
Okay, we know these cells matter now for this symptom.
Well, how can we target those cells
and help them work better in patients by any means?
And I think that's the broader
significance of after genetics clinically.
You and I know Botan well,
and you and Botan share this incredible big vision that I think
only a clinician can really understand, you know, being in close contact with and the
suffering of patients as a ultimate motivator of developing technologies, which makes me
have to ask, did you decide to become a scientist to find cures for mental disease?
No, I didn't. It's a really important question to actually look back and see the steps that
brought you to a particular place. And that was not what brought me initially to science.
And it's okay to embrace the twists and turns
that life brings to you.
But I was always interested in the brain.
And so that was something that, for me,
started from a very early age.
I was, you know, we talked about being introspective.
I noticed very early on.
I had a deep love of poetry and stories, and I was a voracious reader.
And I was amazed by how words could make me feel in particular ways, just even separate
from their, of course, dictionary meanings, the rhythm and how they work together, even
separate from meaning.
And I was stunned by poets that could use words in new ways
that were even divorced from their meaning at all
and yet could still trigger specific emotions.
And this was always fascinating to me.
So, you know, I wanted to understand that.
And so I was interested in the brain and I thought well
I'm gonna have to study the human brain
because only human beings can
describe what's going on inside enough
So in college I began to steer myself toward
medicine and with the idea of becoming a neurosurgeon and
so I came here to medical school
and did an MD-PhD program,
planning neurosurgery all the way through.
The first rotation I did at the end of medical school,
as you know, you do rotations.
You go through different specialties
and some of these are required rotations.
Everybody has to do some are elected
where you can pick what you want to do.
I elected to do neurosurgery first even before regular surgery. I was that sure I wanted to do it.
And I loved it. I had a fantastic time. There was an amazing patient who had a
philamic damage and there was a neglect syndrome where the patient was not able to be aware of
something that was right in front of him. Even though the vision was not able to be aware of something that was right
in front of him.
Even though the vision was perfectly fine.
And I loved the operating room, I loved the rhythm of suturing and the precision of
it, and I loved being able to help patients immediately.
But then a required rotation was in psychiatry, which I was not looking forward to
at all. And that completely reset my whole life, that experience in psychiatry, and it was
that at that moment that I saw this is, first of all, the greatest need, the depth of suffering, and the depth of the mystery
together.
And also, it was, you know, I almost feel a little guilty about this.
It's so interesting, too.
You know, yes, yes, there's, yes, we can help.
Yes, there's need.
But as a scientist, this is amazing that someone's reality can be different from my own.
You know, with everything physically,
as far as we can tell, the same, the measures we have, and yet we've got a different reality.
That is an amazing thing, and if we couldn't understand that and help these people, that would be just, you know, more than anybody could ask for.
And so that's how I ended up taking this path, just a required rotation in psychiatry.
It all started with poetry.
And it started with poetry.
At a respect for poetry, are there any favorites
that you spend time with on a regular basis?
I mean, the ones who got me down this path early on,
I remember in childhood and high school,
Borges had an immense influence on me.
I studied Spanish all the way through and reading his work.
He was a great writer.
He wrote both in English and in Spanish and being able to appreciate his poetry both in
English and in Spanish was a pretty amazing thing. Not many poets can do that.
You're bilingual.
I wouldn't say now I became, at one point I was effectively fluent in Spanish and I have
I'm pretty good with medical Spanish still because we use Spanish all the time in the
clinic here. I wouldn't claim full fluency but it's something I can definitely use all
the time.
And it's been very helpful in the clinic. Yeah, Boris is wonderful. As the son of an
Argentine, I grew up hearing about it and I learned that Boris's favorite city was Geneva. So I
spent time in Geneva only for that reason. It also turns out to be an interesting city. Yes.
Yes. So you developed methods to control neurons with these algae proteins using light.
In 2015, there was this, what I thought was a very nice article published in the New Yorker
describing your work and the current state of your work in the laboratory and the clinic and and an interaction with a patient, so as I
recall, a woman who was severely depressed, and you reported in that article some of
the discussion with this patient, and then in real time, increase the activation of the
so-called vagus nerve, this 10th cranial nerve that extends out of the skull and it innervates many of the viscera and body.
What is the potential for channel redopsins or related types of algae engineering to be
used to manipulate the vagus, because I believe in that instance, it wasn't channel
opusin, stimulatio, it was electrical stimulation, right?
Or to manipulate, for instance, a very small localized region of the brain.
Let me frame it a little bit differently,
and light up what we were talking about a couple minutes ago.
My understanding is that if somebody has severe depression
and they take any number of the available
pharmaceutical agents that are out there,
SSRI, serotonin or agents, increased dopamine,
increased whatever, increased
whatever, that sometimes they experience relief, but they're often serious side effects.
Sometimes they don't experience relief, but as I understand it, channel options and
their related technology, in principle, would allow you to turn on or off the specific
regions of the brain that lead to the depressive symptoms,
or maybe you turn up a happiness circuit or a positive anticipation circuit.
Where are we at now in terms of bringing this technology to the nervous system?
And let's start with the body and then move into the skull.
Yeah. So starting with the body is a good example because it highlights the opportunity
and how far we have to go.
So let's take this example of vagus nerve stimulation.
So the vagus nerve, it's the 10th cranial nerve, it comes from the brain, it goes down,
it innervates the heart, it innervates the gut, and by innervate I mean it sends little
connections down to help guide what happens in these organs in the abdomen and chest.
It also collects information back.
And there's information coming back from all those organs that also go through this Vegas
nerve, the 10th cranial nerve back to the brain.
And so this is somewhat of a super highway to the brain then, it was the idea.
And maybe the idea is maybe we could put a little cuff,
a little electrical device around the Vegas nerve itself
and maybe have just like a pacemaker battery,
have a little power source here under the clavicle,
everything under the skin,
and have a little cuff and drive signals,
and maybe they'll get back to the brain.
So a way of getting into the brain
without putting something physical into the brain. And why the Vegas? I mean, it's there, but and it's accessible. back to the brain. So a way of getting into the brain without putting something physical into the brain.
And why the vagus?
I mean, it's there, and it's accessible.
That's the reason.
That's the reason?
That's the reason, yes.
Really?
You're not kidding.
I'm not kidding.
So stimulating the vagus to treat depression
simply because it's accessible.
It started as an epilepsy treatment,
and it can help with epilepsy, but yes.
It's like, have a love medicine.
As a scientist, I get, this is where I get to chuckle and you say, I mean, the field of
medicine, it from that perspective, from, from the perspective of a scientist and outsider,
the field of medicine as a field that goes in and tickles pathways because they're there.
It's, I don't know what to say.
It's a little shocking.
And we, at least in my laboratory, I always say, you never do an experiment because you
can.
You do an experiment to test a specific hypothesis.
Yeah.
Yeah.
I mean, there are stories people tell.
So the, the, the Vegas nerve lands on a particular spot on the brain called the solitary track nucleus
which is just one synapse away from the serotonin and dopamine in the north or in the front.
So there's a link to chemical systems of the brain that make it a rational choice?
Yes, it's not it's not irrational but I can tell you that even if that we're not true the same thing would have been tried.
You got what I done it anyway.
Because it's accessible.
Yeah, see.
Okay.
And why?
Well, it's not, again, not to disparage what's been happening in this branch of medicine.
There's immense suffering, treatments.
Many treatments don't work, and we try things.
And this is how so many advances in medicine happen. You think about kidney dialysis, which is kept many people alive.
That was just started by someone saying, hey, let's try this.
Maybe there's something building up in the blood.
Maybe we can dialize something and help them.
Yeah, it worked.
And it was just sort of a test pilot mentality.
We can access the blood.
Let's run it across the dialysis membrane, put it back in the body.
Oh my God, that actually works.
And sometimes you do need that test pilot mentality, of course, to do it in a rigorous,
safe, controlled way, which is what we do.
And so, anyway, that's how we ended up with, but still with the Vegas Nurse Stimulation.
Okay, so what does it, does it work?
It has, it's FDA approved for depression, this Vegas nurse stimulation, but on the population
level, if you average across all people, the effect sizes are pretty small.
Some patients, it has an amazing effect in, but some patients, it doesn't work at all,
and average across everybody, the effect size is pretty small.
How do you think it's working when it does work?
Is it triggering the activation of neurons that release more serotonin or dopamine?
It could be, but I would say we don't have evidence for that.
And so I just don't know.
But what is clear is that it's dose limited in how high
and strongly we can stimulate.
And why?
It's because it's an electrode, and it's
stimulating everything nearby.
And when you turn on the vagus nerve stimulator, the voice, patient's voice becomes strangulated
and horse.
They can have trouble swallowing, they can have trouble speaking for sure, even some
trouble breathing because everything in the neck, every electrically responsive cell and
projection in the neck is being affected by this electrode.
And so you can go up just so far with the intensity,
and then you have to stop.
So to your initial question, could a more precise stimulation
method, like optogenetics help in this setting?
In principle, it could, because that would,
if you would target the light sensitivity
to just the right kind of cell, let's say cell X that
goes from point A to point B
that you know causes symptom relief of a particular kind.
Then you're in business.
You can have that be the only cell that's light-sensitive.
You're not going to affect any of the other cells,
the larynx and the farings and the projections passing through.
So that's the hope.
That's the opportunity.
The problem is that we don't yet have
that level of specific knowledge.
We don't know. OK, it's the cell starting point problem is that we don't yet have that level of specific knowledge. We don't know
Okay, it's the cell starting and point A going to point B that will release this particular substance. We want to fix this key on the piano
And then I see two other
Steps they're required one is to get the channel ops and gene into the cell in the case of botan roscow and colleagues
rescuing vision in this patient
They did that by an injection of a virus that doesn't damage the neurons.
The virus itself is fairly innocuous, but carries a cargo, and it's a one-time injection.
The cells express, and then they used light to stimulate.
So, let's say I'm depressed, which I don't think I am, although now sitting in front
of a psychiatrist, you probably can see signs that maybe I am or maybe I'm not.
But let's say we put Channel Opsen into my specific branch of the Vegas that we understand
is responsible for mood.
How are we going to get it in there and then how are we going to deliver the light?
Because we're not talking about sunlight or standing in front of a light bulb necessarily.
But what are the mechanisms for the body?
Yeah.
So we had to solve exactly these questions.
You're saying, how do you get the light
and how do you get the gene in a potent and robust
and safe way?
And that's no solved.
And that's not a challenge.
So there are very safe, well-tolerated gene
delivery mechanisms that are called ad-no associated viruses, AAVs, and these are
things that are associated with the common cold they themselves don't cause any
symptoms. They've been engineered and there's been a broad community of viral
engineering that's been going on for decades making these safer, well-tolerated and so on.
We can put the channel redops and gene into these viral vectors that deliver the gene and we can have little bits of additional DNA that govern expression only in one kind of cell, but not another. These are called promoters and enhancers. All genetic tricks built up by a very broad community of great scientists
over the decades. We can put these different bits of DNA and package them into this AAV,
this little virus, and that can be then injected into a particular part of the body. And
sticking with this vagus nerve example. We know that there are
particular clumps of neurons, there's one called the nodos ganglion that has a
clump of cells related to the vagus nerve and you could for example target a
little injection into that ganglion. Would that be an outpatient procedure?
Yeah. So you come in in the morning, get your injection, maybe walk out a few hours
later. Yeah. that's right.
And so that's the gene.
Then the light delivery, this is also something that we've worked on, we've worked on making
very, very light sensitive options.
One challenge and Botan would be the first to state this, in fact, and solving this problem
for the patient.
He had to build goggles that created much brighter light
than the normal ambient light delivery.
Because as I mentioned earlier,
you have to pack a lot of these channeled options in,
they don't have much current.
You have to really make sure that you've got a tense enough light
to activate enough of them to cause a stimulation.
And it has to be the right wavelength. It has to be the right wavelength.
Going back to your example of the algae moving toward or away, the light, it has to
be tuned just right.
So could you, I'm imagining in my mind as a non-engineer, I know you're also a bioengineer.
I'm imagining a little tiny blue light emitting thing object that's a little bigger than a clump
of cells, or maybe about the size of a clump of cells.
And for those who don't know, you know, your credit card is about 200 micron stick on
the side, and micron is stamp that's basically half a millimeter
in size all around.
Each edge half a millimeter in size.
I could imagine that being put under my skin and then I would, what I'd hit an app on
my phone and I'd say, I'd say Dr. Diceroth, I'm not feeling great today.
Can I increase the stimulation and you say go for it?
And then I ramp it up.
Is that how it would go?
I mean, that's effective, but we already do with the vagus nerve stimulation, the doctor,
in this case.
And I have this in some of my patients in the clinic.
I do vagus nerve stimulation.
I talk to them.
I say, I go through the symptoms.
I use the psychiatric interview to elicit their internal states.
And then I have a radio frequency controller
that I can dial in right there in real time.
You're holding the remote control
to essentially to their brain,
although it's remote control.
Through a couple steps, but yeah.
And I can turn up the frequency,
I can turn up the intensity,
all with the radio frequency and control.
And then it's reprogrammed or redost,
and then the patient can then leave at this altered dose.
So this is happening now.
This is happening right now, electrically.
You do this routinely.
I do it routinely in my clinic, electrically.
And you're getting the verbal content,
which as you described earlier,
is the indication of how well something is working
in real time.
So this maybe you could just describe a little bit of the interaction with that particular
patient or another patient.
What's a typical arc of narrative as you go from no stimulation to increased stimulation?
In most patients, the actual therapeutic effects, the benefits actually take many days to weeks.
So what I'm mostly focusing on in the office in real time is making sure I'm in a safe
low side effect regime.
First I talk to the patient who has been on a particular dose of the stimulation for
weeks or longer.
I talked about symptoms, how were things over the past month, how was your whole,
how was your energy level sleep, you know, what is your mood.
And then we talked with the patient, we decided, this is not yet where we'd like to be.
And so then I can turn up the intensity of the stimulation real time in the office.
I don't, in most patients, I don't expect an immediate mood change.
What I do is I increase the dose until a next level
up while asking the patient for side effects. Can you still breathe? Okay. Can you still
swallow? Okay. And I can hear their voice as well. And I can get a sense. And you're looking
at their face. And I'm looking at their face. And so I can get a sense as they're a,
am I in a still on a safe side effect regime? And, and and then you know, I stop at a particular point
That looks safe and then patient goes home
Comes back a month later and I get the report on how things were over that month
I asked if you're looking at their face because in your book
You describe the incredible complexity of social interactions. And at one point, you describe the incredible
amount of information that the eyes inform about the brain and the context of somebody's
inner experience, whether depressed or happier or otherwise. I want to make sure that we
get back to how to maneuver them and manipulate the nervous system for sake of mental health.
But what are you looking for?
So as a vision scientist, I think, you know, pupils dilating is a sign of arousal, but
that could be a positive arousal, positive valence, like excitement, or it could be terror.
You're going to get the same dilation of the pupils.
And I'm always reminding people of these two little goodies are two pieces of brain.
Basically, they're just outside the cranial vault.
So they're not unlike the vagus in that sense, but they're more of a report than a control
knob, although I like to think they could be used as control knobs too.
So without putting you on the spot, again, to diagnose me, not something I would never
ask you to do
with the camera's rolling, but what are you looking for
that the patient might not be aware of?
In other words, can you see depression in somebody's eyes?
And if you know a patient or if you don't,
can you see it in their body posture when they walk in?
Realizing of course that a trained psychiatrist
like yourself
develops an intuitive sense that's aggregating
lots of different features of a patient.
But what about the eyes?
What's going on there?
The eyes are incredibly rich in information.
And as you alluded to, though, it's not
as if any one measurable conveys all the information
you need.
It's what a engineer would say,
joint statistics.
It's many things all at once, whether they're in synchrony or out of synchrony that actually
turns out to matter.
The eye contact question, we all know eye contact is incredibly important.
You don't feel you've connected with somebody unless there's eye contact. But eye contact can go awry too. It can be too intense or it can be
mistimed or if there's someone with autism, it can be barely there at all. And this is one of the
most striking symptoms of autism as the avoidance of eye contact,
as if it's almost as if it's a harmful quantity.
And so there's an immense amount of information you get from the eyes,
but it's the pairing of what's going on in the eyes with everything
else going on, the body language, the verbal content of what's going on in the eyes with everything else going on, the body language, the verbal
content of what's coming out.
All that together is the art of psychiatry and social interaction.
But sometimes you don't have the eye contact.
This is an amazing thing, and I do talk about this in the book as well.
In many cases, in psychiatry, sometimes it's over the phone that you have to make key decisions.
And as a recall, vividly being as a resident very often,
you have to take these phone calls from people who are not
in the hospital, people you can't see.
You can't see their body, anything about them.
Just the sound of their voice, and you can ask them questions, and you have to make,
in some cases, life or death decisions.
You know, is this person truly suicidal?
Something like that, as it comes up all the time.
And so I developed over the course of training, and I think all psychiatrists do this, is
you develop a way to whatever data stream you have,
whether it's the eyes or whether it's just the sound of a voice coming over the phone.
You learn to home in on that data stream you have and focus on it and identify changes.
And it's quite amazing. I found that you can actually, if you know a patient,
you can detect very precise changes
in mood just from the sound of the voice.
And you can have a realization that all of this patient's depression has improved by about
half, just by the tone of their voice.
And same with eyes, you can, with enough practice, you can get enough information
from a single data stream to give you some information. But when you do have the whole
picture, that of course is best.
So so many theories out there about excessive blinking and lying, lack of blinking and
sociopathy. I like to remind people that people have varying degrees of lubrication of the eyes, which also
influence the frequency of blinking and presumably have nothing to do with whether or not what they're
saying is true or not. But incredible nonetheless that it's that the eyes are a portal to
overall arousal state. I'm fascinated by the effects of light on circadian biology, just overall
desire to be awake or asleep, et cetera.
So the eyes are on the outside of the cranial vault.
The vagus is outside the cranial vault, obviously.
What about the goodies in here?
Parkinson's, we know the, at least one of the major sites of degeneration and failure that
lead to those symptoms.
I can name off any number of other things. In your book, you talk about the beautiful work done
with optogenetics of active versus passive coping,
that there are areas of the brain, like the ventula,
that make, when active, make animals and presumably people
passive and unwilling or uninterested in fighting back
against pressures of life,
whereas in other region, the RAFA, you stimulate that and they actively cope.
They get their grit going and they are able to lean into life.
So how does one get to those structures in a focused way?
And what is the next two to five to 10 years look like?
Well, this is the promise on that, and it is on that time scale that I think things may start to play out.
The specificity of outdoor genetics is really only useful if you have
some idea of how to use that specificity.
It's an actually, it's a frustrating aspect of psychiatry
that in many cases, the most effective treatments we have
have the least specificity, electric convulsive therapy
being a great example where you're causing a brainwired.
Which looks barbaric, but as you mentioned, is effective.
I mean, it is.
These days, it's much more clinically, you know,
it doesn't look like one flu, the last scene in one flu over the cookies.
Now it's a very clinically safe and stable procedure,
but what I would say, yeah, it is,
it's got this almost medieval lack of specificity,
even if the procedure is well controlled
and clinically safe and stable.
And it has a, it's not very specific.
You're causing a brain-wide seizure.
How could you be less specific than that?
And we don't know the source of the relief.
We don't, we don't know.
There's only it's a dump of neuromodulators,
like dopamine and serotonin.
But we don't, there certainly is a dump of neuromodulators.
We don't know that that's the cause for the relief.
And likewise with medications,
this is also an interesting thing.
So some of the most effective antidepressants, some of the most effective antistycotics are the ones
that have the most side effects. And many examples of this, for example, the most effective
antistycotic is something called clasapine, which has, some question we have, the most side effects,
terrible, terrible side effects.
The D4 antagonist.
It has basically every receptor.
Does it really?
Yeah, it acts as interesting.
Yeah, it has prominent serotonin, prominent muscarinic, certainly acts on dopamine receptors,
but it causes blood cell counts.
How do people feel?
So if I were schizophrenic and I was getting auditory
hallucinations, et cetera, and I took close a pain,
what could I expect to feel?
Well, so you would notice side effects
and you would notice resolution of symptoms both.
And so the voices would go away
in a good situation, the voices would go away,
but I would feel not good in my body.
You would have, you might have dizziness, you might have drooling, you might have any number
of physical sensations that would be due to these off-target effects, the medication acting
on these other receptors.
And I'm certainly not suggesting this, but what if somebody without schizophrenia
took close-up pain?
You had the same side effects presumably.
And so it would not be something that I would recommend.
Do psychiatrists take the drugs that they prescribe?
I just finished, for the third time,
all of her sacs is autobiography, which is marvelous,
and I highly recommend to people.
He certainly took a lot of drugs,
not as part of his professional role.
But just out of curiosity,
what is the interest or kind of role of drugs
in the field of psychiatry?
Because I would imagine for a group of very curious
introspective people who are making recommendations
about what to take,
there could actually be some benefit for understanding what the experience of those drugs was like for their patients.
I think that's true.
And I will say that probably many or most psychiatrists have sampled a number of these, for
exactly the reason that you're saying is to understand better and to help treat their
patients better.
And I've spoken to people who have, you
know, really been found this very helpful to know, okay, this, this sleep disruption caused
by this medication or the libido disruption caused by this other medication. Wow, that
is, that is a big effect. And it really helps with empathy for the, for the patients to
understand.
I'm not, I'm not suggesting that physicians or anybody experiment with drugs, but I am relieved to
hear that because I think that when you're talking about accessing somebody's mind and
their basic physiology, as you mentioned, relate to appetite, libido, and sleep, you're
really one is acting as a mechanic of their, the person's whole experience.
They walk out of the office and they have a life experience
that extends beyond the script.
Yeah.
And so, and yeah, and so that, so at the same time though,
you can't let that completely guide
your clinical decisions because, as I mentioned,
some of these medications that have the most side effects,
they are also the most effective.
And Clausepin is a great example.
That will work in patients where nothing else works.
And believe me, we don't take the step of clasping
prescription lightly because of all these side effects.
You have to come in for a weekly blood cell,
or every few weeks of blood cell,
check to make sure that the blood counts are not off,
for example.
But there are patients where no other medication works
for the schizophrenia and clasping works.
It's more well.
And so we do it, even though there are the side effects. And so then this comes back to your
question, what if we had better and better specificity? Well, only if we know exactly what we're doing
is the point. And so because as we become more refined, we better be right about where we're
refining to. And you imagine a day where it will be a single, maybe even outpatient neurosurgery
would go in through the skull or the back of the year, deliver a small viral injection
of one of these adenoviruses, a little sticker of light emitting diode. Is that deep in
the brain? Is that how you envision this? That certainly could happen. What I actually
prefer as a vision is still medications because those are minimally invasive.
If we knew what we were doing, we could make them more specific, have fewer side effects.
But optogenetics that will arm us with true causal understanding.
And so we'll know, and we're already moving rapidly toward this point. We'll know, okay, this symptom, the loss of pleasure in life that we call
anendonia, or the loss of motivation or energy to overcome challenges,
active coping.
These are largely subserved, largely controlled by this circuit,
or that circuit, or the cell that inhabits this other circuit.
And we will know that because of the work done with channel ups.
Exactly. Yeah, I agree.
In ways that we never could have the confidence otherwise.
And so we'll know that this is the circuit that that underlies the symptom or its resolution.
And then we'll get to understand these cells very deeply.
Okay, these cells that are causal, that do matter.
Who are they?
What are they?
What's their wiring?
What are the proteins that they make?
What are the little things that are on the surface of the cell that could be receptors
for specific medications or combinations of receptors that would give us the specificity
we need. And then armed with that causal and precise and rigorous knowledge,
then you can imagine medication development becoming totally different,
no longer serendipitous, but truly grounded in causality.
I see. So using channel op-sense as a way to probe the circuitry
and figure out the sites that are disrupted,
what patterns of activity are required, probe the circuitry and figure out the sites that are disrupted.
What patterns of activity are required?
And then by understanding the constituents of those cells, like what they express and
what they make, then developing drugs that could target those cells, not necessarily putting
light inducing diodes into the brain or walking around with wire packs attached to our skull
or something.
That's fantastic. And I realize no one has a crystal ball.
But what do you think the arc of that is, meaning,
are we going to see that in a year, in two years, three years?
Let me reframe that.
If how soon will a pill-based treatment for a psychiatric
disease be available that targets a specific
set of cells that we know are important because of the work done with channel absence?
I think that is in some ways it's already happening at the level of individual patients.
And here it's Stanford. Yep. Yep.
And more broadly in terms of new drugs, new multi-center clinical trials that will play out
over the next few years.
And these could be drugs that are already safe and approved for other purposes, but we
might say, okay, now we know that this medication based on what we know
from causal optogenetics, this could be useful for this other purpose, this psychiatric
symptom.
And so the path to helping patients is could be relatively swift.
That's very exciting.
What are your thoughts about brain machine interface and neural link always comes up, although I do want to point out at
a tremendous respect for the folks at neural link, including someone who came up through my lab is now there as a neurosurgeon, but
that brain machine interface is something that's been happening for a long time now. Some of the some of the work, among the best work being done here at Stanford
and elsewhere too of course.
I always thought what you just described
compatible with or different than brain machine interface
meaning devices, little probes,
they're gonna stimulate different patterns
of activity and ensembles of neurons.
And what are your general thoughts
about brain machine interface as going forward?
Yeah, I mean, this is, first of all, it's an amazing scientific discovery approach.
As you mentioned, we and others here at Stanford are using electrodes collecting information
from tens of thousands of neurons in humans, I should add.
And even, yes, there's quite even separate from the neural link work. As you point out,
many people have been doing this
in humans as well as in non-human primates
and this is pretty
powerful. It's important. This will let us understand what's going on in the brain in
psychiatric disease and neurological disease and we'll give us ideas for treatment.
It is of course, it's still invasive.
You're still talking about putting a device into the brain.
And that has to be treated as a situation that
has some risks and a step that has to be taken carefully.
I see that as something that will be part of psychiatry in the long run.
Already with deep brain stimulation approaches, we can help people with psychiatric disorders,
and that's putting just a single electrode, not even a complex, you know, a closed loop
system where you're both playing in and getting information back, even just a single stimulation electrode in the brain can help people with OCD, for example,
quite powerfully. And that will become much more powerful when we get to a true brain machine
interface, collecting information back, stimulating only when you need to. If we could identify a pathological
activity pattern,
a particular almost like the the pro-drome or the early stage of a seizure,
maybe there are events that happen leading up to on some time scale,
a psychiatric symptom. We could intervene in a closed loop way to
detect what's happening, what's starting to go wrong, feed that back to the brain
stimulation electrode.
Have it be in that way more efficient and more principled.
This is, I think, that's great.
It's something that, of course, will be grounded again and causal understanding.
We'll need to know what is that pathological pattern that we're detecting,
and we need to know that it matters.
And so again, that's where optogenics is helping us.
Helping us know, okay, this pattern of activity in these cells and these circuits, this does mean that there's a
particular kind of symptom that's happening. But armed with that knowledge, absolutely, even the
simple closed loop device detect and stimulate is going to be part of psychiatry in the future.
And then, of course, as you get to more cells, more connections,
the ability that we have to help people will become more powerful.
One of the questions I get asked a lot is about ADHD and attention deficit of various kinds. I
have the hunch that one reason I get asked so often is that people are feeling really
distracted and challenged
in funneling their attention and their behavior.
But there are a number of reasons for that, of course.
But what is true ADHD and what does it look like?
What can be done for it and what if any role for channel options or these downstream technologies that you're developing,
what do they offer for people that suffer from ADHD
or have a family member that suffers from ADHD?
Yeah.
This is a pretty interesting branch of psychiatry.
There's no question that people have been helped
by the treatments.
There's active debate over what fraction of people who have
these symptoms can or should be treated. This is typically aterol or stimulants of some kind.
For example, stimulants, that's right. So ADHD, as its name suggests, it has symptoms of,
it can have either a hyperactive state or an inattentive state.
And those can be completely separate from each other. You could have a patient who
who effectively is not hyperactive at all, but can't remain focused on the what's going on
around them. So their body can be still, but their their mind is darting around. That's right.
Or they can be very hyperactive with their body.
Yeah, it happens both.
Probably rarely if somebody hyperactive with their body,
but their mind is still.
Although I have to say,
and this is a benevolent shout out to Botan Raska,
Botan is an incredibly sharp and focused mind.
And his hand movements are extremely exact also.
So I do sometimes wonder whether or not our body movements
and our head movements are,
whether or not their coordinator or not our head movements, whether or not
their coordinator is a readout
of how directed our attention is.
I notice I have to think complex abstract thoughts.
I notice I have to be very still.
So my body has to be almost completely
unmoving for me to think very abstractly
and deeply.
Other people are different.
Some people, when they're running,
they get their best thoughts.
I can't even imagine that. My brain does not work that way at all. I have to be totally emotional.
Which is kind of interesting. How do you go about that?
I sit much like this. I try to have time in each day where I am literally sitting almost in this position,
but without distraction and thinking. So it's kind of a, it's almost meditative in this position, but without distraction and thinking.
And so it's kind of a, it's almost meditative in some ways, except it's not true meditation,
but I am thinking while not moving in.
You're trying to structure your thoughts in that time.
Yeah, interesting.
So, but everybody, as you say, is very different.
And so with ADHD, you have, the key thing is we want to make sure
that this is present across different domains of life,
so a school and home, to show that it really is a pervasive pattern
and not something specific to the teacher
or the home situation or something.
And then you can help patients.
It's interesting that ADHD is one of those disorders
where people are trying to work on quantitative EEG based diagnoses.
And so there's some progress toward making up a diagnosis with looking at particular,
externally detectable brain wave rhythms.
So skull cap with some electrodes that don't penetrate the skull.
And this can be done in an hour or two hour session.
Has to be done in clinic, right?
Yeah, in the clinic, right.
You have to have the right recording apparatus and so on.
But that's in principle, as increasing confidence comes in exactly which measurements one could
even imagine moving towards home tests, but we're not there yet.
Amazing. I think one of the reasons I get asked about it
so much is a lot of people wonder if they have ADHD.
Do you think that some of the lifestyle factors
that inhabit us all these days
could induce a subclinical or a clinical like ADHD?
Meaning if I look at people's phone use,
including my own, and I don't think of it like addiction
It looks to me and feels to be more like OCD and I'll come clean here by saying when I was younger when I was a kid
I had a grunting tick. I used to hide it and I actually used to hide in the closet because my dad would make me stop and
I used to I couldn't feel any relief of my mind until I
Would do this and actually now if I get very tired if I've been pushing long hours, it'll come back.
I was not treated for it,
but I will confess that I've had the experience
of I always liked sports where I involve a lot of impact,
fortunately not football,
because I went to high school where the football team was terrible.
Maybe that would have avoided more impact,
but things like skateboarding, boxing,
they bring relief. I feel clarity
after a head hit, which I avoid, but I used to say that's the only time I feel truly clear for a lot. And then eventually it dissipated by about age
16, 17 he had just disappeared.
So I have great empathy for those that feel like there's something contained in them that won't allow them to focus on what they want to focus on.
And these days with the phone and all these email, et cetera, I wonder and I empathize a bit when I hear people saying like,
I think I might have ADHD or ADD.
Do you think it's possible that our behaviors and our interaction with the sensory world, which is really what phones and email really are, could induce ADD or reactivate it.
Yeah, this is a great question.
I think about it a lot.
And you mentioned this tick-like behavior in yourself.
It's very common that people who have ticks have this building up of something that can
only be relieved by executing that the tick, which can be a motor movement or a vocalization or even a thought.
And people do, I think, these days do have this, if they haven't checked their phone
in a while, they do have a build up, a build up, a build up until they can check it and
relieve it.
And there's some similarities, you know, there is a little reward that comes with the checking.
But the key question in all of psychiatry, what we do is
we don't diagnose something unless it's disrupting
what we call social or occupational functioning.
Like you could have any number of symptoms,
but literally every psychiatric diagnosis requires that it has
to be disrupting someone's social or occupational functioning.
And these days, checking your phone is pretty adaptive, that pretty much helps your social
and occupational functioning.
And so we can't make it a psychiatric diagnosis.
Interesting.
At least in the world of today.
Yeah.
Opting out of
communication now makes it makes you it's always less adapt to the though. I
would point to you as an example of somebody who is quite good at managing his
interactions at least from from the outsider perspective. I do want to ask you a
little bit about you and first of all and I realize this is only a partial list,
but your clinician, you see patients,
you run a big laboratory.
How many people are in your laboratory now?
That's a huge laboratory from experience.
I can say that's an enormous laboratory.
You have a family of five children
and you're happily married to a wonderful colleague of ours as well who does incredible work
How how do you organize at a kind of conceptual level the day and the week?
And I should say
What stress mitigation practices if any do you incorporate everave emails from you at three in the morning?
I sometimes send emails at three in the morning,
but that's when I wake up, maybe I'm depressed,
but I go back to sleep.
So maybe you just describe the arc
of the blocks of the day, not hour by hour,
necessarily the details of what are in those blocks,
but how do you conceptualize the day?
How do you conceptualize the week? And how do you feel about
how that's lined up with your larger goals of making sure these five young people flourish,
which I hear they are? But how do you go about this? What for most people would just be an overwhelming set of items? Well, it's a...
Of course, sometimes it's just a take a day by day and...
So you bring the horizon into the unit of the day?
I do, I do. It's the unit is the day, that's right.
And what I try to have in each day,
as I mentioned earlier, some, at least an hour of time where I can
think, and that can be when kids are napping, it can be, you know, actually, because while
driving I can do that too, because I'm sitting still.
But that's the one thing I try to preserve.
When I was writing the book, I adapted that time to be my writing time, but it wasn't
enough.
So I had to add in a new block of time, which was sort of midnight to 2 a.m. writing
time.
And so that carving out these even small protected times are very important.
There's of course, you know, obligations will expand a fill the time available and you have to be disciplined.
In my, at least I found I had to be disciplined and truly protecting those times where one can think.
So that means no phone.
That means no phone, no checking of the phone.
I would, you know, when I was writing the book, I would have,
there's a focus mode on the Macbook, which kind of rules the border, and you just have your document, and it's very pure, and you don't have a temptation of distraction.
I'm a big believer in because the vision and the eyes
play such a prominent role in directing our cognition,
something you talk about in the book,
really beautifully, and with a lot of depth and rigor,
using visual tools to harness one's
complete mental attention.
When you do this practice of sitting
and just thinking, sitting still and thinking,
you said your eyes are open.
Are you hearing your own verbal voice,
although in your head?
Yes.
So you're actually in conversation with yourself.
Yes, and hearing literally,
I mean, not quite literally,
I don't actually hear a phonation,
but I'm hearing words.
And so I discovered this about myself, other people, I think, may operate differently,
but I'm extremely verbal in how I think.
That's how all my reasoning is done.
It's with sentences and construction of almost equations with words.
Complete sentences or complete-ish anyway,
mostly complete.
And then, and when writing the book,
everything about the writing,
I would always, every sentence
was always played out in my mind,
listening for rhythm and timing.
And I would obsess over
exact placement of words to get the right rhythm
of the spoken sentence in
my mind.
I don't mean to interrupt your flow, but when you do that and having experienced this process
a bit, although differently, do you experience any kind of welling up of anxiety when you're
hitting the friction points?
And if so, do you have tools or ways that you quell that anxiety in real time?
Because what we're really talking about here is your mind.
But what we're really talking about is this process of converting the activity of neurons
into something physically concrete in the world.
And these intermediate steps are so mysterious to everybody.
We hear, you know, just write the book, just do it, whatever that means.
In fact, the statements like that, to me, just write the book, just do it, whatever that means.
In fact, the statements like that to me are kind of empty and meaningless.
But when you hear your voice and you're trying to find the correct word and you keep hitting,
it doesn't sound quite right. What is the experience in your body?
Yeah, when it's not right, it's definitely, it's aversive. It doesn't feel good, but it's not, but I, but there's also a hope
because I know I can solve it too.
And, and so there's, there's this, it's almost like you're,
you're almost there, you know, you've, there's a path that you know is there.
You don't quite see it, but it's there.
And, and I keep that in mind.
And so there's a propulsive force forward,
because I know that the solution is there.
And that said, there were single words that would occupy,
I would spend days on, because I was just not happy
until I got it right.
And there were some things that I never quite got perfect.
And so I left out of the book entirely because it was so close, but not quite there.
And so I, and I was like, no, I can't put that in.
Everything you just said is entirely consistent with my experience of you.
And the way you go about everything I have to ask are your kids' writers?
Do they like books and
words and poetry? I know one of your children is going on to a career in medicine and science.
Yeah, they're each different, which is amazing. Yet they all, I think, do have some appreciation,
or a lot of appreciation for reading, but some are very musical. Two of the five are extremely musical, very, very talented with guitar and singing
and vocal, you know, impressions, it's just astonishing. And some of them are great with drawing
and artistry, and some are very physical and vigorous and are never happy except when,
you know, leaping about. And so it's just amazing how different they are, honestly. But I think there is a shared appreciation for language.
Do you think the one can train their mind
in using these practices?
I really like your description of the sitting,
staying physically still and learning to grapple
with those challenges.
It's something that, especially in laboratory science,
we aren't really trained to do like many professions.
We're taught to come in and just get into motion.
And I found that very relaxing
and someone who probably has an underlying tick
or something like that.
It felt great to be in motion.
One of the hardest things about becoming a university
professor and running a lab was that I,
no longer working with my hands.
And it felt like I was,
it felt like some big important part of my life
had been amputated.
But what sorts of practices do you incorporate there?
And do you think people can learn to get better
at focusing through a dedicated practice
of the sort that you describe?
I think, I also,
I remember the rhythms of physical work
in the laboratory very well. I, my work, you know, these days as the laboratory leader,
my job has returned mostly to words now again. And so it's kind of coming full circle. I
was, so it's a different mode.
I think you just have to embrace that, that different stages of life come with, with different
modes, but you can definitely train yourself for each mode.
I was not, um, you know, I, I, I, I loved, you know, the, as I mentioned, the, the rhythm
of, of sewing and, and, and suturing and surgery.
And I worked really hard on that
and became good at it.
And now I never do it,
but it's what's the next challenge.
There's all the various experimental techniques,
the deceptions of the brain.
I can't tell you how many thousands of brain deceptions
I've done in my life.
And now I don't do them at all.
And then you developed a method
so that we don't have to dissect brain.
As you mentioned, it maybe tells us for a moment about clarity, and for the, for people who
will probably never set foot into a laboratory, what an incredible yet another incredible discovery
and development clarity is and why it helps us understand how the brain is structured.
Yeah, so this is this is a different technology
to also developed in my lab here,
and it's a part of a broader approach
that we call hydrogel tissue chemistry.
And what this is is it's building a gel,
like a clear gelo substance from within all the cells
of a tissue or even an animal all at once.
So you're building, effectively building a gel inside all the cells at once.
Now that's a odd thing to do.
Well, how do we do it?
Well, we do it to transform the tissue into a more tractable, accessible object.
And the reason that works is we we having built this gel, this new
infrastructure inside the tissue.
We can then use chemical tricks and we can link the molecules we care about like proteins or
RNAs, which are the things as you know right before they become proteins. We can link them physically
anchor them to this gel, which is a scaffold, basically.
It's an interlocking network of polymers.
We can link all these interesting molecules in place, lock them in where they were initially
in the tissue and the cell and all the cells.
And then we can remove very vigorously everything we don't care about.
That's blocking our light, that's blocking our molecules
coming in to exchange information with the tissue.
We can get rid of everything else like the lipids, the fats, we can effectively use detergents
to get them all out.
And then we can see in all the things that we're absorbing, our scattering light are gone.
You can have a brain that's completely transparent.
And yet all the interesting molecules are still locked into place there at the cellular and
subcellular level.
And so this is Hydrogel tissue chemistry.
The first form we described was called clarity.
We use that quite a bit still, but there are many variants now that we and others have developed
on this basic concept of building this gel than the tissue and anchoring molecules into place.
Literally glass clear brains.
I've done this. I've taken a brain clear with this method
and looked at somebody through it,
and although you don't want to get it too close to your eye,
I don't want to touch it to your own eye,
but and you can see all the way through it.
That's incredible for, it raises an important question,
which is again about the human brain.
I mean, as somebody who essentially started out in neuroanatomy and then got into other things,
I always bothered by the fact that we actually know very little about the microstructure of the
human brain compared to the brains of other organisms. And in thinking about understanding the
circuitry and the piano, so to speak,
that and how to manipulate it in order to relieve suffering.
One wonders are the structures in these animal brains
and how they behave and active coping, passive coping,
ADD, et cetera, those models,
how well they translate to the human condition.
Do you think it's fair to say that there are entire regions of the human brain that aren't
just bigger, but that exist only in the brains of humans, especially given that we have this
speech, although I do wonder sometimes if animals are reporting to each other there.
Maybe they have little psychiatric sessions with one another.
other there. Maybe they have a little psychiatric sessions with one another. You know, I'm always careful to not assume we do things better. We certainly
understand what we're doing better than we understand what animals are doing
and they certainly do things better than we do. That said, we do have amazing
wonderful brains and many structures that are very highly developed in our
brains that are not nearly so developed in mice and fish,
for example. Now, that said, when I look at the big picture, you know, what is the mammalian
brain really doing? There are things that you would never have thought we could study
in animals and laboratory mammals like mice,
that it turns out you can actually. And so I would never draw the line and say,
here's something you can't study in mice
or here's something that has no parallel in mice.
I would be very careful before making any statement
like that.
A good example of that is we've been able to study
just in the past year,
it comes to an understanding of dissociation.
And both, we had a paper that came out in late 2020, both mouse and human work in which
we got to the sort of the circuit basis for dissociation.
Now, what is dissociation?
A lot of people might not have experienced it, but it's actually very common.
More than 70% of people who've been through trauma,
experienced dissociation, it shows up in borderline personality,
it shows up in PTSD.
What it is, is a separation of the sense of self
from the body.
And so you can have someone who's,
it's not, as if you're numb, you're not anesthetized,
you can still, you know that something's happening to the body,
but you just don't care because you don't describe it to yourself
Which is very interesting right that is how interesting is that the self-report narrative?
Yeah, almost in your book you touch on this and I I will say is the most
precise and
Meaningful and eloquent description of what might be consciousness, this narrative toward the self or of the self and where it might reside.
So in dissociative conditions, people are feeling as kind of an absence of a merge between mind and body.
Is that one way to describe it?
And is that recall this paper involved an exploration of ketamine. ketamine was a big part of it. Yeah, that's right. And so ketamine is another one of those cases where people can experience dissociation,
ketamine or PCP.
We call these the dissociative drugs.
They cause it just like these other psychiatric conditions can cause it.
And so we were able to manifest this in mice, administering these dissociative agents in
mice.
We could make them still able
to detect stimulus, but not care that it was happening.
All the while we were recording the activity of individual cells in the brain to see what
was going on, what was happening along with this dissociation, and then use optogenetics
to see that it mattered to actually provide that pattern of activity in CO.
That actually causes the dissociation.
So we could do all that in mice,
which was just a, who would have thought
that you could study something like this in mice.
And we were able to go back and forth with a human work
because here in our Stanford Comprehensive Eplepsy Center,
there are a lot of what we call stereo EEG recording patients
who come in and in the course of normal clinical care.
They have electrodes recording in their brain
to identify where the seizure is,
so they can be candidates for removing a little patch
of the brain that's causing the seizure.
This is done for patients who medications
are not helping their seizure disorder.
And there was a patient who had a dissociative state before every seizure. So there was a human being who was really dissociating. He could tell us literally as it was happening. And we could see
this pattern, the same pattern that was happening in the mice, in the same patch of the brain.
We could see that happening in the human being at exactly the right time, in the same patch of the brain, we could see that happening in the human being at exactly
the right time, in the same patch of the brain that's homologous across these immense evolutionary
distances. And we knew that it mattered too, both in Nelson human because in the human
we could cause it to happen.
And I just want to underscore the power of not just that I want to underscore the power
of optogenetics and the ability to not just remove a particular
experience or behavior by lesioning or destroying, but then to go back and actually activate
the same structure or group of structures and see the emergence.
So it's essentially, these days, you hear a lot about gain of function research in the
context of viral manipulation, but gain of function is something that we do in the laboratory and you do in patients to both take away something
and put it back, which gives you causality.
That's right.
And so, and exactly.
And so without the genetics,
we were able to provide in animals without being on any
ketamine or any drug,
and we could cause the dissociative state
by playing in a precise pattern of activity.
And who would have thought you could do that, but there was a combined mouse and human
paper. Likewise, we've been able to play in, you know, visual sensations into the brains
of mice. And by observing which cells in the visual part of the brain, visual cortex
are naturally responsive to, for example, vertical bars instead of horizontal bars in the visual part of the brain visual cortex are naturally responsive to, for example,
vertical bars instead of horizontal bars in the visual world. We could see which cells were normally
reporting on vertical bars and then we could use optogenetics to come and play in
activity just to those cells. To these animals are not viewing anything. Not doing anything at all,
and we could activate just the vertical bar cells and not only
did the animal act as if it was seeing a vertical bar behaviorally, it was trained to do a particular
thing if it saw a vertical bar and it did that just as if it was seeing something visually.
But everything in the brain that we were recording to, the internal representation of this external
world was naturalistic to it, looked like the brain was seeing something visual. So that's
gain a function too, you know, playing in, providing a complex sensation or percept that wasn't
there before. And we can do that across species. So we haven't, you know, and of course,
mice are social and they do amazing acts of information processing. And so I don't,
I try not to disparage our cousins too much. They certainly have helped the field of neuroscience
and medicine I should mention.
And I know that people have various sensitivities
about animal research, but the work that's been carried out
in my, has been absolutely vital and instructional
for treatment of human disease.
That's right. Since we talked about dissociation and instructional for treatment of human disease.
Since we talked about dissociation
and dissociative states rather,
and Kenamine, I'd love your thoughts on psychedelic medicine.
I sort of have joke having grown up in this area
in Northern California when it was much more counter culture
than it is now,
that many of the things that we're hearing about now,
at least from my read of the history books
happened before.
There was a movement aimed at taking
the very same compounds essentially,
putting them into patience,
or people were obviously using them recreationally,
but putting them into patience
and seeing tremendous
positive effects, but also tremendous examples of induced psychiatric illness. In other words,
many people lost their minds as a consequence of overuse of psychedelics. I'll probably lose a few
people out there, but I do want to talk about what is the state of these compounds?
And I realize it's a huge category of compounds.
But LSD and psilocybin, as I understand, trigger activation of particular serotonin receptor
mechanisms may or may not lead to more widespread activation of the brain, more that one wouldn't
see otherwise.
But when you look at the clinical and experimental literature, what is your top contour sense of how
effective these tools are going to be for treating depression? And then if we have at the time,
we could talk about trauma and MDMA and some of that. Well, you're right to highlight both
the opportunity and the parallel that is there. And of course, we want to help patients and of course we want to explore anything that
might be helpful and what we want to do it in a safe and rigorous way. But I do think we should
explore these avenues. These are agents that alter reality and alter the experience of reality,
I should say, in relatively precise ways, they do have problems, they can be
addictive, they can cause lasting change that is not desirable. But we have to see these
as opportunities. We have to, first of all, study in the laboratory and I'm doing this here.
We have big, we have safes with many interesting psychedelics that all very carefully regulated,
we get inspections from the DEA and so on.
Many of them are hoping to find these labs.
They exist in outer space, so you need to be on board one of the SpaceX missions in
order to access them, so don't try and combine them.
No, that's exactly true.
And we're doing exactly this.
We're saying this is an incredible opportunity.
If we could understand how the perception of reality
is altered, we could create new kinds of intervention
that don't have the risks and the problems
of causing lasting change or addiction.
Now, that said, even as these medications exist now, as you know,
there's an impulse to use them and very small doses and to use them as adjunctive treatments
for therapy of various kinds. And I'm also supportive of that, if done, carefully and rigorously.
Of course, there's risk, but there's risk with many other kinds of treatment,
and I'm not sure that the risks for these medications vastly outweigh the risks that we normally
tolerate in other branches of medicine. Why would they work? I mean, the, you know, let's say that,
indeed, their main effect is to create more connectivity, at least in the moment,
between brain areas. So the way I think about the two extremes of my experience,
anyway, is a high degree of stress and focus for whatever reason is going to make
create changes in my visual field and changes in the way that I perceive time,
so that I'm in a microslice time. I'm in a very contracted view of whatever my experience is. Whereas on
the opposite extreme, in a dream or in sleep space in time are very fluid and I'm essentially
relaxed, although it might be a very interesting dream, it might not be. Psychedelic seemed to be a trajectory, not too far off from the dream state where space
and time are essentially not as rigid.
And there is this element of synesthesia, blending of the senses, you know, feeling colors
and hearing light and things of that sort.
You hear these reports anyway. Why would having that dreamlike experience
somehow relieve depression, long-term? Do we have any idea why that might be?
We have some ideas, and no deep understanding. One way I think about the psychedelics is they
deep understanding. One way I think about the psychedelics is they increase our willingness to, they increase the willingness of our brain to accept unlikely ways of constructing
the world, unlikely hypotheses as it were as to what's going on. The brain, in particular
cortex, I think is a hypothesis generation and testing machine. It's coming up with models about everything.
It's got a lot of bits of data coming in and it's making models and updating the models and changing
them theories, hypotheses for what's going on. And some of those never reach our conscious mind.
And this is something I talk about in projections in the book quite a bit is many of these are filtered
out before they get to our conscious mind.
And that's good.
We think how distracted we'd be if we were constantly
having to evaluate all these hypotheses about what kinds
of shapes or objects or processes were out there.
And so a lot of this is handled before it gets to consciousness.
What the psychedelics seem to do is they
change the threshold for us to become
aware of these incomplete hypotheses or wrong hypotheses or concepts that might be noise,
but are just wrong and so are never allowed to get into our conscious mind. Now, that's pretty
interesting and it goes wrong in psychiatric disorders,
I think, in schizophrenia, sometimes the paranoid delusions that people have are examples of
these poor models that escape into the conscious mind and become accepted as reality, and they
never should have gotten out there.
Now how could something like this in the right way
help with something like depression?
Patients with depression often are stuck.
They can't look into the future world of possibilities
as effectively.
There's everything seems
Hopeless and what does that really mean they they discount the value of their own action they discount the value of the world at giving rise to a future that matters
Everything seems to run out like a river just running out into a desert and drying up and
What these agents may do that increase the the flow through circuitry if you will, the percolation of activity through circuitry may end up doing for depression,
is increasing the escape of some tendrils of process, of forward progression through the world.
That's a concept. That's how I think about it. There are ways we can make that rigorous. We can indeed identify in the brain by recording. We can see cells
that represent steps along a path and look into the future and we can rigorously
define these cells and we can see if these are altered on psychedelics. And so
that's one of the reasons that we're working with these agents in the
laboratory to say, is this really the case? Are these opening up new paths or representations
of paths into the future? MDMA, ecstasy, is a unique compound in that it leads to big increases in
brain levels of dopamine and serotonin simultaneously.
And I realized that the neuromodulators, like dopamine serotonin, often work in
concert not alone the way they're commonly described in the, you know, the
more general popular discussions. However, it is a unique compound and it's
different than the serotonergic compounds like LSD and psilocybin. And there are now data still emerging that it might be and in some cases can be useful
for the treatment of trauma, PTSD and similar things.
Why would that work?
And a larger question, perhaps the more important question is, psychedelics,
MDMA, LSD, all those compounds, in my mind, there are two components. There's the experience
you have while you're on them. And then there's the effect they have after. People are generating
variations of these compounds that are non-helicinatory variations. But how crucial do you think it is to have,
let's stay with MDMA, the experience of huge levels
of dopamine, huge levels of serotonin,
atypical levels of dopamine, serotonin released,
having this highly abnormal experience
in order to be normal again.
Yeah.
I think the brain learns from those experiences,
that's the way I see it.
And so, for example, people who have taken MDMA,
they will, as you say, they'll be the acute phase
of being on the drug and experiencing
this extreme connectedness with other people, for example.
And then the drug wears off, but the brain learned from that experience.
And so what people will report is, yeah, I'm not in that state, but I saw what was possible.
You know, I saw it. Yeah, you can, they don't need to be barriers, or at least not as many
barriers, as I thought. I can connect with more people in a way that is helpful.
And so I think it's the learning that happens in that state that actually matters.
And as you described that, that sounds a lot like what I understand to be the hallmark
feature of really good psychoanalysis, that the relationship between patient and therapist
hopefully evolves to the point where these kinds of tests can be run within the context
of that relationship and then exported to other relations.
Exactly right.
And that probably, I'm assuming, is still the goal of really good psychiatry also.
It's a part of intimacy really.
It should be, when we have time, I think all good psychiatrists try to achieve that level of connection and learning,
try to help patients create a new model that is stable, that is learned, and that can help
instruct future behavior.
One of the things that I took from reading your book in addition to learning so much science and the future of psychiatry and brain
science was, um, you know, amidst these very, many, in many cases, very tragic cases and sadness.
And a lot of the, the, the weight that that puts on the clinician on you also, that there's a,
that there's a central cord of optimism that where we're headed is not just possible but very
likely and better. And you know, are you an optimist? I am. And this is by the way
this was a really interesting experience in writing projections because I
had a dual goal. I wanted it to be for everybody, literally everybody in the world who wants to
to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to to when I was speaking about science, when I was speaking about the neurobiology of the brain or psychiatry, I wanted to not have any of my scientific colleagues think, oh, he's
going too far, he's saying too much.
And so I had these two goals, which I kept in my mind the entire time, and a lot of this
trying to find exactly the right word we talked about was on this path of staying excruciatingly
rigorous in the science.
Yet, letting people see the hope, the where things have everybody see that we've come
a long way, we have a long way to goals, but I kept that in my mind the whole way through.
And yes, I am optimistic and I hope that came through in the book. colleague, you did achieve both. And it's a wonderful, it's a masterful book, really,
and one that, as a scientist and somebody who is a fellow brain explorer, hits all the
marks of, of rigor and is incredibly interesting. And there's a ton of storytelling. I don't
want to give away too much about it. But people should definitely check out the book.
Are you active
on social media? People want to follow you and connect with what you're doing now and
going forward.
Yeah, I have a Twitter, that's where I mainly do exchange, you know, tell people about
things that are happening. We'll provide a link to it, but that's Carl Diceroth,
is that recall with a K? That's right.
And so you're on Twitter and people will hear this
definitely check out the book. There are other people in our community that of course are going to
be reaching out on your behalf, but it's incredible that you juggle this enormous number of things.
Perhaps even more important, however, is that it's all in service to this larger thing
of relieving suffering.
So, thank you so much for your time today for the book and the work that went into the
book.
I can't even imagine for the laboratory work and the development of channel ops and clarity
and all the related technologies and for the clinical work you're doing and for sharing
with us.
Well, thank you for all you're doing and reaching out.
I'm very impressed
by it. It's important and it's so valuable. And thank you for taking the time and for all
your gracious words about the book. Thank you. I hope you enjoyed today's discussion with Dr.
Diceroth as much as I did. Be sure to check out his new book, Projections, A Story of Human Emotions.
It's available on Amazon, Audible, and all the other standard places where books are found.
It's available on Amazon, Audible, and all the other standard places where books are found. If you'd like to support this podcast, please subscribe to us on YouTube.
As well, you can subscribe to us on Apple or Spotify.
At Apple, you also have the opportunity to leave us a five-star review and to give us feedback.
Please put any questions you have in the comments section below the YouTube video.
If you'd like us to address certain things in future episodes,
or if you have questions about this particular episode.
In addition, please check out our sponsors.
That's a terrific way to support us.
And as mentioned, at the beginning of today's episode,
we are now partnered with Momentus Supplements
because they make single ingredient formulations
that are of the absolute highest quality
and they ship international.
If you go to livemomentus.com slash Huberman, you will find many of the supplements that
have been discussed on various episodes of the Huberman Lab podcast, and you will find
various protocols related to those supplements.
Last but not least, if you're interested in understanding more about how the brain works
and how it functions and how it breaks down in various conditions, check out the first
episode of the Huberman Lab podcast.
The title of that episode is How Your Nervous System Works and Changes.
If you're watching this right now on YouTube, you can simply click on the title card for
that episode.
And last but not least, thank you for your interest in science.
you