Huberman Lab - Dr. Nolan Williams: Psychedelics & Neurostimulation for Brain Rewiring

Episode Date: October 10, 2022

In this episode, my guest is Nolan Williams, M.D., a triple board-certified psychiatrist, neurologist and professor of psychiatry and behavioral sciences at Stanford School of Medicine. He is also the... Director of the Stanford Brain Stimulation Lab. We discuss clinical applications for brain stimulation, behavioral protocols and novel drug treatments to halt and reverse mental health disorders, including depression and post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). We first discuss the neural circuits for self-identity and mood and stress control. We discuss Dr. Williams’ work using transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) to depression, trauma, PTSD, and other mood disorders. We then dive deep into the history, biology, modern use, and safety margins of the various psychedelics, including MDMA, LSD, ketamine, ibogaine, ayahuasca, and psilocybin, as well as cannabis and the use of SSRIs in both adults and children. Finally, we discuss behavioral treatments for mental health disorders, including sleep and sleep deprivation, light exposure, exercise, and training to control the brain-heart-rate pathways. Regardless of age, all those interested in mental health should benefit from the incredible breadth and depth of Dr. Williams’ knowledge and the clarity with which he conveys that information. For the full show notes, visit hubermanlab.com. Thank you to our sponsors AG1 (Athletic Greens): https://athleticgreens.com/huberman LMNT: https://drinklmnt.com/huberman Supplements from Momentous https://www.livemomentous.com/huberman Timestamps (00:00:00) Dr. Nolan Williams, Brain Stimulation & Depression Treatment (00:03:48) Sponsors: LMNT (00:09:16) Depression, Risk Factors, Emergency Psychiatric Treatments (00:15:11) The Brain-Heart Connection, Vagus Nerve, Prefrontal Cortex (00:17:51) Right vs. Left Brain Hemispheres & Mood Balance, Connectome (00:22:34) Heart Rate & Depression, Behavioral Interventions, Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (TMS) (00:33:02) Prefrontal Cortex & Cognitive Control, TMS (00:35:30) Sponsor: AG1 (00:39:00) Belief/Identity “Rules”, Re-scripting, TMS & Talk Therapy (00:45:49) Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex, TMS & Depression Treatment (00:48:36) Cingulate Cortex & Emotion, Dissociation & Catatonia (00:54:27) Ketamine, the Opioid System & Depression; Psychedelic Experience or Biology? (01:03:42) SSRIs, Serotonin & Depression; Childhood, Chemical Imbalance or Circuit? (01:13:58) Memories & “Rule” Creation; Psilocybin & “Rule” Resolution (01:21:00) MDMA & Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) Treatment, Psilocybin & Depression Treatment (01:24:12) Is MDMA Neurotoxic?, Drug Purity, Dopamine Surges, Post-MDMA Prolactin (01:30:38) Psilocybin, Brain Connectivity & Depression Treatment (01:34:53) Exposure Response Prevention: “Letting Go” & Depression Treatment (01:41:23) Normal Spectrums for Mental Health Disorders (01:45:35) Ibogaine & “Life Review”; PTSD, Depression & Clinical Trials (01:57:16) Clinical Use of Psychedelics (02:01:59) Ayahuasca, Brazilian Prisoner Study (02:06:55) Cannabis: THC, CBD & Psychosis, Clinical Uses (02:14:52) Personal Relative Drug Risk & Alcohol (02:20:42) Circadian Reset for Depression, Sleep Deprivation, Light (02:28:43) Stanford Neuromodulation Therapy (SNT) Study (02:34:25) Space Learning Theory & TMS Stimulation (02:45:35) Zero-Cost Support, YouTube Feedback, Spotify & Apple Reviews, Sponsors, Huberman Lab Premium, Neural Network Newsletter, Social Media Title Card Photo Credit: Mike Blabac Disclaimer

Transcript
Discussion (0)
Starting point is 00:00:00 Welcome to the Huberman Lab podcast where we discuss science and science-based tools for everyday life. I'm Andrew Huberman and I'm a professor of neurobiology and Ophthalmology at Stanford School of Medicine. Today my guest is Dr. Nolan Williams. Dr. Williams is a medical doctor and professor of psychiatry and behavioral sciences at Stanford University School of Medicine. His laboratory and clinic focus on depression and other mood disorders. They focus specifically on the use of transcranial magnetic stimulation, which is a brain stimulation technique that can either activate or quiet specific brain circuits, as well as circuits within the body, in order to treat depression and other mood disorders.
Starting point is 00:00:44 Other laboratories and clinics use TMS. What sets apart the work of Nolan Williams and colleagues is that they combine TMS with other treatments. Some of those treatments are among the more cutting edge that you've probably heard about these days, including IBIGAN, Silasibon, MDMA, Cannabis, DMT, and other drugs that at this point in time are experimental in terms of clinical trials, but that at least the preliminary data show hold a great promise for the treatment of depression and other mood disorders. In the course of my discussion with Dr. Williams, we covered things
Starting point is 00:01:16 such as the history of each of these drugs, how they can to be and their current status in terms of their clinical use and legality. We also talk about their safety profiles, both in children and in adults. And we talk about what the future of psychedelic research and clinical use really looks like. For instance, we discuss how a number of laboratories and clinics are modifying psychedelics to remove some of their hallucinogenic properties while maintaining some of their antidepressant or antitroma properties. You'll also learn about some fascinating research in Dr. William's laboratory, Focus on ketamine, which is a drug that is increasingly being used to treat depression.
Starting point is 00:01:54 And contrary to common belief, the effects of ketamine in terms of relieving depression may not actually arise from its dissociative effects. One thing that you'll find extraordinary about Dr. Williams is that not only does he have vast knowledge of the various treatments for depression, but that he and his laboratory are really combining these treatments in the most potent way. That is, combining psychedelic treatments
Starting point is 00:02:15 with brain machine interface, or combining brain machine interface with particular learning protocols, that is, neural plasticity protocols, which can directly change the brain in specific ways. So today you're going to learn a tremendous amount about the neural circuitry underlying depression as well as positive moods. You'll also learn about all the various drugs that I described, and you're really going
Starting point is 00:02:35 to learn about the current status and future of the treatment of mood disorders. Today you'll also learn about a number of ongoing studies in Dr. Williams' laboratory. I should mention that they are recruiting subjects for these studies. If you go to BSL, which stands for Brain Stimulation Laboratory, so that's BSL.stanford.edu, you have the opportunity to apply for one of these clinical trials
Starting point is 00:02:55 for the treatment of depression and other mood disorders. I confess that the conversation with Dr. Williams was for me one of the more stimulating and informative conversations I've ever had about psychedelics, which is simply to say that his breadth and depth of knowledge on that topic is incredible, and his breadth and depth of knowledge in terms of the underlying brain science and how it can all be combined with clinical applications is also extraordinary. I'm sure that by the end of today's episode you're going to come away with a tremendous
Starting point is 00:03:23 amount of knowledge about the clinical and non-clinical uses of those substances, and you're going to understand a lot more about how the healthy and diseased brain work. Before we begin, I'd like to emphasize that this podcast is separate from my teaching and research roles at Stanford. It is, however, part of my desire and effort to bring zero cost to consumer information about science and science-related tools to the general public. In keeping with that theme, I'd like to thank the sponsors of today's podcast. Our first sponsor is Element.
Starting point is 00:03:50 Element is an electrolyte drink with everything you need and nothing you don't. That means plenty of salt, magnesium, and potassium, the so-called electrolytes, and no sugar. Salt, magnesium, and potassium are critical to the function of all the cells in your body in particular to the function of your nerve cells, also called neurons. In fact, in order for your neurons to function properly, all three electrolytes need to be present in the proper ratios. And we now know that even slight reductions in electrolyte concentrations or dehydration
Starting point is 00:04:19 of the body can lead to deficits in cognitive and physical performance. Element contains a science-backed electrolyte ratio of 1000 milligrams, that's 1 gram of sodium, 200 milligrams of potassium, and 60 milligrams of magnesium. I typically drink element first thing in the morning when I wake up in order to hydrate my body and make sure I have enough electrolytes. And while I do any kind of physical training and after physical training as well, especially if I've been sweating a lot. If you'd like to try element,
Starting point is 00:04:46 you can go to drinkelement. That's LMNT.com slash Huberman to claim a free element sample pack with your purchase. Again, that's drinkelementlment.com slash Huberman. And now for my discussion with Dr. Nolan Williams. Thanks for joining today. I'm really excited to have this conversation. It's been a long time coming
Starting point is 00:05:06 and I have a lot of questions about different compounds, psychedelics in particular. But before we get into that discussion, I wanna ask you about depression, broadly speaking, intractable depression, how common depression is or isn't. I heard you say in a wonderful talk that you gave that depression is perhaps the most debilitating condition worldwide.
Starting point is 00:05:33 And yet in contrast to other medical conditions like cancer, we actually have a fairly limited number of tools to approach depression, and yet number of tools in the potency of those tools is growing. So if you could educate us on depression, I'd really appreciate it. Yeah absolutely. So depression is a condition that it has a lot of manifestations, you know, so you can have kind of a depression that's primarily loss of interest. You can have folks who feel very anxious and they're kind of overactive. You can have people who don't have any anxiety at all. And they're very underactive and they have low motivation to do anything.
Starting point is 00:06:12 So you have this huge range of symptoms that are in that umbrella of depression. And some of our work is to actually work with folks that Connor listed in Cornell and try to actually get biotypes based off of neuroimaging to see if we can kind of parse out the different depression kind of presentations and see that clinically and also see that in the brain. Depression is the most disabling condition worldwide. What's interesting about depression is it's both a risk factor for other illnesses and it makes
Starting point is 00:06:45 other medical and psychiatric illnesses worse. So recently the American Heart Association added depression is the fourth major risk factor for coronary artery disease. So alongside the risk factors that we know, hypertension, high blood pressure, hyperlipidemia, high cholesterol and diabetes, high blood pressure, hyperlipidemia, high cholesterol, and diabetes. High blood sugar, those three have been on the list for a long time and depression, and being at it, the list is the fourth one.
Starting point is 00:07:14 And really interesting, right? So in addition to taking medications to address those other three risk factors, we really have to be thinking about how do you treat folks with depression to reduce the risk of having a heart attack in the future. And you know, there's some of that's being worked on now, but we don't have a complete solution to thinking about that at this time. And then the other thing that's interesting is once you have a heart attack in the
Starting point is 00:07:42 individuals in having a heart attack, The risk of having depression after the heart attack is higher than the normal population, right? And so a lot of what we're doing in the lab actually is measuring kind of brain heart connections and we can actually with transcranial magnetic stimulation and form of brain stimulation, we can actually decelerate the heart rate and capture that heart rate deceleration over the mood
Starting point is 00:08:07 regulatory regions, and so actually a direct probe of that connection. So it's interesting, and so, as you said a second ago, it's a very disabling condition, moderate depressions about is disabling, is having a heart attack, acutely having a heart attack, severe depressions as disabling, is having a heart attack, acutely having a heart attack, severe depressions as disabling is having cancer without treatment, you know, and dying from a cancer without treatment.
Starting point is 00:08:31 And so, you know, it's kind of underappreciated just how disabling depression is in that way. And I think important as stigma is consistently kind of being reduced over the years for mental illness, for mental illness, for mental illnesses, then the idea that we can start really putting more funding and putting more focus to the federal level, you know, private foundation level, whatever it is, at a given university to thinking about developing treatments. We've been very interested in a very particular clinical set of problems around the most severe
Starting point is 00:09:07 and the most high-equality settings that folks with depression end up being in, and that's in emergency settings where they go into inpatient units. And the rest of medicine, it's talking about heart attacks, if I start having chest pain right now and you bring me to a primary care doctor's office they're going to have a certain number of tests and treatments, right? But very limited because it's an outpatient facility if you bring me to the emergency room after that. There are more tests and more treatments if you put me in the ICU or in the cath lab where they do invasive procedures to the heart, there are more tests and more treatments.
Starting point is 00:09:47 In psychiatry, as we elevate the acuity of an individual, you go from being just depressed to being depressed and now thinking about into your life, the number of treatments actually go down on average. I mean, in some scenarios they go up, but on average they go down and there are no tests. Right? And so we've been very focused on that particular problem. Somebody that maybe was doing, you know, fairly okay with a pretty moderate depression and then their depression gets worse and then they end up in an emergency setting. And the field really hasn't developed a way of, you know, consistently being able to
Starting point is 00:10:24 treat that problem and folks end up getting the same standard oral antidepressants that they've been getting outpatient. I came to this because a dual trained as a neurologist and psychiatrist went back and forth between neurology and psychiatry saw that in neurology we have all of these ways of treating acute brain-based problems and really wanted to emulate that in psychiatry we have all of these ways of treating acute brain-based problems and really wanted to emulate that in psychiatry and find ways to develop an engineer new brain-based solutions.
Starting point is 00:10:53 There's a lot to unpack there. One thing that you said I'd like to focus on a bit more because I think we hear that the brain and the heart are connected but you described I believe a direct relationship between areas of the brain associated with emotion and heart rate. Yes, and that makes perfect logical sense to me, but I think at the same time Many people out there probably think of the relationship between the the heart and the mind as kind of woo or kind of a soft biology. But here you're talking about an actual physical connection between what area
Starting point is 00:11:32 of the brain is it? The first place where the stimulation goes is called the Dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex. It's kind of the sense of control kind of governor of the brain. And then what we know is that when you use a magnet, use kind of what we call Faraday's law, this idea of using a magnetic pulse to induce an electrical current in electrically conducting substances. So in this case, brain tissue, but not skull or scalp or any of that or hair, you avoid all that, just the brain tissue. Then you have a direct depolarization of cortical neurons, the surface of the brain's neurons in this dorsal lateral prefrontal. And if you do that in the actual scanner which we can do, you can see that that distributes
Starting point is 00:12:19 down into the enterosingulate in the insula and the amygdala. And ultimately, the tract goes into something called the nucleus tract of solitarius and ultimately into the vagus nerve and to the heart. So the heart very consistently seems to be the end organ of the dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex. If you measure heart rate in standard ways that cardiologists measure heart rate and you stimulate over this left or a lateral, you get a deceleration of the heart rate. It's very time-locked to the stimulation. So it's a two-second train of stimulation. At one second you see the deceleration. It goes down about 10 beats per minute and then it'll drift back up. There's a break for eight seconds
Starting point is 00:13:01 on the stimulation. It drifts back up and the stimulation goes back in and then the heart rate goes back down. So you see the heart rate just do this. Ten beats per minute, every train. And so we know, if you do that over visual cortex, you don't get that or motor cortex, you don't get any of those findings. It's really specific to this kind of control region of the brain. And so, yeah, it seems to, you know, it's our work, other folks work Martin Arns in Europe, and other ones work showing the same connections.
Starting point is 00:13:33 I think it's been replicated like four or five times. So you mentioned left dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex. Anytime I hear about lateralization of function, I get particularly curious because obviously we have two mirror symmetric sides of the brain. There are you know rare exceptions to this like the pineal and things of that sort that are only there is only one pineal What is special about the left dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex? Does this have anything to do with handidness, right hand, or left hand?
Starting point is 00:14:07 Because we know right hand and left handidness has a lot to do with lateralization of function for language, a topic for another time. But why do you think that left door-salato prefrontal cortex would be connected to the heart in this way? Yeah, I think so left or a lateral, you know, is thought to be the side that when you excite it, when you kind of do
Starting point is 00:14:32 excitatory stimulation, potentiating sort of stimulation that you can reduce depressive symptoms and a guy by the name of Mike Fox at Harvard demonstrated that if you have strokes in the brain that caused depression, and you have strokes in the brain that cause depression, you put them on the human connectome 100,000 patient map and you ask the question
Starting point is 00:14:52 what they're all functionally connected to left dorsal lateral. If you take lesions that cause mania in individuals and you put those all on the human connectome map and ask what they're all, the one common area they're all connected to, it's the right dorsal lateral. And so there seems to be hemispheric balancing of mood between these two brain regions. And we know this from an experimental standpoint too, because you can take individuals with depression and you can excite the left or you can inhibit the right and they're both anti-depressant. You can excite the right and that's anti-manic in some studies. And so this idea that there is this hemispheric balancing of mood is quite interesting, right? It's incredibly interesting.
Starting point is 00:15:40 And just to people know if you're curious what the connectome is, a connectome is a term that was built out of this notion of genomes, of being large collections of sequencing and mapping of genes, their proteomes of proteins, of connectomes as so-called connectomics of connections between neurons. So the human connectome project is ongoing. And I find that incredible that within the connectome project they can identify these regularities of right versus left or salato-prefrontal cortex, especially since I've looked at a fair number of brains from humans,
Starting point is 00:16:17 not certainly not as many as you have. And if you look at the architecture, the layers, the cell types, and even the neurochemicals of which cells are expressing say dopamine or serotonin or receiving input from areas that make dopamine or serotonin, they don't look that different on the right and left side. And yet here we're talking about a kind of an accelerator and a break, if you will, on depression and mania, using what, at least by my eye, and I think other people's eye look to be basically the same set of bits, the same parts list, or less. So what gives these
Starting point is 00:16:54 properties to the right and left or saline or prefrontal cortex is that the inputs they receive? Is this something that we learn during development or do you think that we come into the world with these Is it something that we learn during development or do you think that we come into the world with these hemispheric biases? Yeah, that's a great question. And you know, it hasn't been worked out, which your original question was around. And a left-handed individual, which is, you know, 25% of those folks end up having a right-brain dominance, or 1% of right-handed people have a right-brain brain dominance if it's flipped, right? And that, unfortunately, that study still hasn't been done at the level because that would be probably pretty helpful for teasing some of this out. But it's still being sorted out, right?
Starting point is 00:17:37 We know enough to know this phenomenon exists because we can use TMS as a probe and do this sort of these sorts of manipulations. But to my knowledge, there hasn't been anybody that's gotten so interested in it that they've been able to get a mechanism of why that is. But it's empirically true in the sense that you can push and pull on those systems or in the case of strokes that folks have and then you kind of get their brain images and look at where the strokes landed, those kind of causal bits of information point to this asymmetry.
Starting point is 00:18:17 Interesting. Well, in that case, going with what we do know, that stimulation of Dorsalato prefrontal cortex slows the heart rate down transiently, but slows it down and seems to alleviate at least some symptoms of depression leads me to the question of why would that be the case? Does it tell us anything fundamental about depression that anxiety is inherent to depression? I think of faster heart rate as you know,
Starting point is 00:18:42 part and parcel with anxiety. In my laboratory, we've studied fear a bit in animals and in humans, and we often observe brachycardia where somebody or an animal is afraid of something, and rather than the heart rate speeding up, it actually slows down something that most people don't think about or recognize. it actually slows down something that most people don't think about or recognize. But given that stimulation, Dorsalato or prefrontal cortex slows the heart rate down and can alleviate depressive symptoms, and that there are other ways to slow the heart down. I have two questions. What do you think this tells us about the basic architecture of depression and its physiology at the level of the heart.
Starting point is 00:19:25 Does the circuit run in the opposite direction, too? If one were to have, we'll find other ways to slow the heart rate down, say with a beta blocker. Does that help alleviate depression? Yeah, that's a great question. I'll answer the second question first. We know that there are ongoing trials of this. If you stimulate in theus nerve and an implanted
Starting point is 00:19:47 vagus nerve stimulator, you can actually have this, the a-farent parts of the vagus project ultimately up to the DLPFC through the singular through these anterior insula. So that's obviously the same tract, right? And you can stimulate there and alleviate depression, which seems very unusual, right? You're stimulating a cranial nerve down on the neck, but if you can get up into the brain, you actually can improve depressive symptoms. And so, you know, more evidence that this is kind of a whole track and system. And if you stimulate in part of that system system it appears that you can improve mood.
Starting point is 00:20:28 What if I were somebody who did not have a stimulating electrode in my vagus nerve and I was dealing with minor depression and I decided I wanted to take some other approach to slow my heart rate by the vagus. For instance exhale, emphasize breathing or deliberately slow cadence breathing. Things of that sort. Is there any evidence that behavioral interventions of those kinds can alleviate depression or some symptoms of depression? And is there any evidence that it does indeed feed back to the Dorsal Adore prefrontal cortex to achieve some of that alleviation?
Starting point is 00:21:03 Absolutely. There's a number of studies implicating the dorsal lateral and say meditation, mindfulness, that sort of thing. There are small studies, but pretty well designed studies suggesting that behavioral interventions in mild depression actually work quite well. There seems to be a volitional threshold for depression where at some point you start losing, you go from being completely in total volition to having kind of semi-volution. You have thoughts that you really have a hard time controlling and that sort of thing. And you go through that threshold at some point it gets harder and
Starting point is 00:21:44 harder for those sorts of things to kind of kick in and work. And the extreme form of that is cationia, right, where people in a very severe form of depression get kind of stuck, motorically, right? And they obviously can't, they have no control. And so, or very limited control. And so, you know, I think there's a threshold
Starting point is 00:22:03 in which these sorts of interventions will work. Exercise seems to really be a good treatment for mild depression, and it may work through the mechanism you're describing, right? As we all know, you know, athletes hold a lower resting heart rate than folks that aren't, you know, if you're an athlete, you had a lower resting heart rate, you stopped, you know, exercising and a couple years later, you're resting heart rate. In many cases, goes up, right? And so maybe that's, maybe that's part of the process. I'm not aware of any studies specifically looking at door-cellateral prefrontal physiology, pre-post exercise, but it would be a great study. I think that would be really
Starting point is 00:22:45 helpful to understanding this, especially if you had a correlation of changes in lowering and say heart rate with mood improvements. There's been a lot of work with heart rate variability and depression. Studies are kind of point towards it. It's not, not every study is, you know, positive for this, but quite a few studies say basically that lower heart rate variability is associated with moderate to severe depression. And that may be part of that mechanism of that heart brain risk. So I'm both intrigued and a little bit perplexed by this relationship between heart rate and depression. On the face of it, I would think of depression as depressed.
Starting point is 00:23:33 So lower heart rate might make somebody more depressed. You've mentioned catatonia or somebody that just doesn't seem motivated or excited to do anything. I think of mania as elevated heart rate and being excited. On the other hand, I realized that anxiety, which brings about ideas as elevated heart rate, is also built into depression, which brings me back to what you said earlier, which is that when we say depression, are we really talking about four or five different disorders? Yeah, that's right.
Starting point is 00:24:02 For lack of a better word. And for what percentage of people that have depression does some approach to reducing heart rate work, whether or not it's stimulation of the dose, left dose lateral prefrontal cortex by way of trans cranial minor stimulation or by taking a beta blocker or by stimulating the vagus, can we throw out a number, a rough number? Does that help 30%, 50% how long lasting is that relief? Yeah, and to be clear, the acceleration of the heart rate is in the moment when the stimulation is happening,
Starting point is 00:24:37 but it's not something that's necessarily maintained chronically. It's more of an indicator that you're in the right network, more than it appears to be itself central to mechanism. The heart rate variability piece may be, and there's some studies that link the tube. But the actual deceleration seems to be much more of a marker that you're in the right system,
Starting point is 00:25:00 but it very well could be that the heart rate system and the mood system just sit next to each other and the stimulation hits both. If you look at how much of the variance and the mood is explained by the heart rate deceleration, it's not a huge amount, right? So it only explains a small percentage. So it's unlikely that simply reducing the heart rate. And in fact, for many years, perpranolol and these sorts of drugs actually were implicating causing depression.
Starting point is 00:25:31 And so that's been kind of debunked. But it's unlikely that simply decelerating the heart rate is going to improve depression. But what it does tell you is that if you're in that area that is the mood regulatory area, there's some parasympathetic cortical kind of process that's going on that gets in and causes this to happen.
Starting point is 00:25:52 And it's independent of mood. You can take a normal healthy individual and you can do this and they're going to decelerate their heart rate. I'm so glad you mentioned the parasympathetic nervous system, which of course is the most people think of as the rest and digest or the kind of calming side of the autonomic nervous system. As I'm hearing you say all of this and in particular what you just told me which is that it's not as if having a lower heart rate protects you against depression or a higher heart rate is associated with depression, although I think streams that might be true. But rather, it's something about the regulatory network,
Starting point is 00:26:27 the ability to control your own nervous system to some extent. And when I think about the autonomic nervous system, I like to think about as a seesaw of, you know, alertness and calmness, and when you're asleep, it's a lot of calmness. And when you're panicking, it's a lot of alertness to the, but that, and I don't think this has ever been defined and when I
Starting point is 00:26:45 Teach met the medical students at Stanford, Neuronatomy. I my wishes that someday I'll be able to explain What the hinge in that process would be right not the ends of the seesaw We know what the sympathetic nervous system is and what it's to wake us up and make us panic or make us feel Nicely alert and calm. We know what puts someone into sleep or a coma or makes them feel relaxed. But what shifts from one side to the seesaw to the other and the tightness of that hinge seems to be what you're describing that depression is sort of a lack of control over in or state so that when I'm stressed I can't get myself out of it. When I'm feeling completely collapsed with exhaustion, I can't get out of bed and it's motivated to do the very things that would help me get out of depression, like a workout
Starting point is 00:27:28 or social connection or eat a quality meal, these kinds of things. So this is perhaps the first time that I've ever heard about a potential circuit for the hinge as I'm referring to. Does it make sense at all? Absolutely. Absolutely. I just want to make sure that I'm framing this correctly in my mind. Yeah, absolutely. And in some studies, if you do the same identical stimulation on the right door,
Starting point is 00:27:50 Saladro, you can get an acceleration. Just further confirming this idea of lateralization, right? It appears that even the prefrontal cortical areas seem to be lateralized in this way. And it's less, the right finding is more variable, depending upon the study that left's very consistent in this way. So we've talked about churns, cranial man, neted stimulation for getting into these networks.
Starting point is 00:28:20 And I also just wanted to take a brief tangent say, I've heard you say this before, I think it's so vital what you're saying that it's really not about stimulation of areas, it's or any specific brain area or vagus nerve being important per se, it's really about a network, a series of connections. I think that's really important for people to understand. And is it kind of a new emerging theme really? Yeah.
Starting point is 00:28:44 The other thing that to me seems extremely important for people to understand. And as a kind of a new emerging theme really, the other thing that to me seems extremely important for us to consider is what are these lateral prefrontal courtesies doing? Are they involved, for instance, in sensation, sensing the heart rate? Are they involved in thinking and planning? And this gets down to a very simple question that I know a lot of people have, which is Can we talk ourselves out of depression if it's mild? Can we talk ourselves into a manic state or an excited state, a positively excited state that doesn't qualify as mania? You know other areas of the brain I think of they is responsible for perception or for motor control
Starting point is 00:29:23 But here we are in this mysterious frontal cortex area, which people say executive function, planning, et cetera. Are we talking about thoughts? Are we talking about structured thoughts? Are we talking about dreamlike thoughts? What in the world is going on in the prefrontal cortex? Yeah, no. And here I've spent my career in neuroscience,
Starting point is 00:29:41 and I still can't really understand what it's doing and maybe it's doing 50 things. Yeah, that's a great question. So one of the studies that we've been working on in addition to the depression work is actually trying to change trait hypnotizability. So David Spiegel and I have been working on this. He's found and published this 10 years ago that a different part of the left-door sladdle is functionally connected with the door slantier single it with a lot of functional connectivity and high hypnotizables and not much and low hypnotizables.
Starting point is 00:30:20 And that's a different sub region withinion within this bigger brain region. We call left-door slateral prefrontal cortex. Then the part that seems to be important for regulating mood. And so the left-door slateral seems to have connections that are location specific within the overall named brain region that connect to various parts of the singular and seem to regulate it. Right? And so if you knock out the left or a salateral prefrontal cortex and you have people do the strup task, for instance, which is a task where you have, it's a simple task.
Starting point is 00:30:58 But I know this, you have people name the color of words. And so if I look at what, if I look at one of the cards that they'll show you, it'll have the word red and red, and that's very easy, and that's a called a congruent. And then the incongruent is red in the color blue. And you have to say the word, you don't name the color. Do you have to suppress a response?
Starting point is 00:31:27 Yeah, exactly. And so, I'm sorry, you name the color and you see the word written in a different way. And so basically, if you stimulate in a way that inhibits the left or a lateral prefrontal cortex or either one, you can actually knock out the ability to do that well, and it'll take longer for people in the incongruent cards to be able to name it.
Starting point is 00:31:52 And so they have a time delay that's greater than they had before they got stimulated. So that's a part of the prefrontal cortex that's different than the part of the prefrontal cortex that's involved in mood regulation. The nice thing about TMS is that you can go through and you can find these areas that are functionally defined through brain imaging and you can perturb them and answer the question you're talking about.
Starting point is 00:32:18 How do I understand this part of the prefrontal cortex and its function this part. And so we were able to stimulate in a inhibitory way within the left dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex that's involved with this sort of cognitive control area. And we were able to knock that area out and increase trait hypnotizability. So people had greater hypnotizability after they got active stimulation versus when they got sham. And so it suggests that that brain circuit is involved in the process of what hypnotherapy to hypnosis ends up being. But it's a very different region within the left or salateral than say we do when we do these very intensive stimulation approaches to treat severe depression and we're able to get people out of depression. You know, with the part of the door salateral it seems to be lower in the, you know, kind of more lateral and inferior on the DLPFC and connected with the subgenual anterior singulate,
Starting point is 00:33:27 so the part of the anterior singulate that processes emotion. I'd like to take a quick break and acknowledge one of our sponsors, Athletic Greens. Athletic Greens, now called AG1, is a vitamin mineral probiotic drink that covers all of your foundational nutritional needs. I've been taking athletic green since 2012, so I'm delighted that they're sponsoring the podcast. The reason I started taking athletic greens and the reason I still take athletic greens
Starting point is 00:33:51 once or usually twice a day, is that it gets to be the probiotics that I need for gut health. Our gut is very important. It's populated by gut microbiota that communicate with the brain, the immune system, and basically all the biological systems of our body to strongly impact our immediate and long-term health. And those probiotics and athletic greens are optimal and vital for microbiotic health. In addition, athletic greens contains a number of adaptogens, vitamins, and minerals that
Starting point is 00:34:18 make sure that all of my foundational nutritional needs are met. And it tastes great. If you'd like to try athletic greens, you can go to atlettegreens.com slash huberman. And they'll give you five free travel packs that make it really easy to mix up athletic greens while you're on the road and the car on the plane, et cetera. And they'll give you a year supply of vitamin D3K2. Again, that's atlettegreens.com slash huberman to get the five free travel packs and the year supply of vitamin D3K2.
Starting point is 00:34:45 Based on what you told us about the strup task and the role of the prefrontal cortex and strup task. To me, the strup task is a rule switching game. Yep. In one moment, the rule is you read whatever the word says, and then you switch and then you say the rule now is you tell me what color the word is written in and you suppress whatever it is the word says. Okay, that's right.
Starting point is 00:35:08 Okay, a rule in some sense, like that, is a transiently adopted belief system. So I could imagine that in depression, which has all sorts of backstory to it, that of course the psychiatrist or psychologist or friend can pull on that thread. Like for instance somebody might believe that they are bad or that they don't deserve love. I'm trying to bring this into the typical language that people talk about or that they will never
Starting point is 00:35:35 succeed or that even if they keep succeeding it's just going to get harder and harder and it will never feel good. These are sort of rules like the stupe task at some level. They're rules that are more pervasive over time, unfortunately. But I could imagine that if the PFC is also contains some sort of maps or algorithms related to rules of emotionality or self-representation or things that we've heard, I think there must be data out there that's saying that whatever we heard in middle school and someone made fun of us, we can remember that because I can remember things that people said about a jacket I wore one day or something
Starting point is 00:36:11 in the fourth grade, crazy. I didn't even like the jacket. Now I think it was kind of cool, but anyway, the point being that we have an intense memory for these things, is to set up a sort of rule or a question like maybe I don't really know how to dress. For instance, maybe that's why I always wear the same black shirt.
Starting point is 00:36:29 But in all seriousness, it seems like the Dorsal Outpreferental Cortex is in this amazing position to access rules which are beliefs and beliefs are rules and then for moments or longer to switch those rules. And so for somebody who's depressed, to just simply look themselves in the mirror and say, you are great, you are fantastic, that's, it feels like a lie if you feel like garbage, to say that.
Starting point is 00:36:52 It doesn't fit with the rule. It's like saying, that card is not red. That card is green when your eyes tell you that it's red. And it seems like there's something about prefrontal cortex that in principle gives flexibility to rules based on what we know on this troop task. So given its connectivity, can we assume that the talk therapy that occurs in a the psychiatrist's office or with a friend
Starting point is 00:37:20 or through journaling out something? Because we do know that reporting things about trauma or difficult circumstances or the rules that we contain and tend to hide inside of us about how we feel miserable, about ourselves or anything really, that in re-scripting that, that somehow it allows us to do a sort of strup task on our beliefs, is that a tremendous leap? I'm just trying to frame this in the context of what I, and most people think of as depression, because the network components are vitally important. But I guess what I'm trying to figure out is, what are the algorithms that govern prefrontal cortex? Yeah, absolutely. So in a standard cognitive behavioral therapy session, what the therapist is trying to do
Starting point is 00:38:07 is identify those beliefs and determine how fixed they are, if they're flexible as you're saying, and then help folks to find another explanation for them and to kind of reintegrate that potential other explanation into their memory system, right? Where I think TMS is really interesting, actually, we had a lot of patients who've told me like, my therapist told me that I wasn't trying hard enough in therapy and I really am trying hard, but these are modern, pretty severe depressed patients. And as soon as we get them well with the TMS approach, it's kind of rapid five-day approach and the next week we come in and see them, and they'll say, you know what I did all weekend as I looked at my therapy books and now I can understand it.
Starting point is 00:39:00 And so, I actually see TMS as a way of having exogenous sorts of cognitive functions that in milder forms of depression, we can pull off with psychotherapy. This idea of being able to turn that prefrontal cortex on and have it govern these deeper regions. In depression, the deeper regions govern the prefrontal cortex, they precede the prefrontal cortex timing-wise. We've got some data in review now, we're seeing that in depressed individuals that are responsive to our rapid TMS approach, we call Stanford accelerated intelligent neuromodulation therapy or S&T or ST. if you look at the brain before people get this they will have a temporal delay where the single is in front of the DLPFC and in people that are normal
Starting point is 00:39:58 healthy controls, no depression, the Dorsal Adderal Prefrontal cortex is temporarily in front of the anterior singulate. With effective treatment, we can flip the timing of things. So the door-solateral is in front of the anterior singulate just like in a normal person. So you're not talking about, obviously, physically moving these structures, talking about in time, their activation. So in one case, it's like the coach telling the player what to do.
Starting point is 00:40:26 Exactly. And then it's like a player telling the coach what to do. And you restore order to the game. You restore order to the game. And what it looks like is depression, to your point, is a bunch of kind of spontaneous content that's semi-evolutional, that's being kind of generated out of this conflict detection system, the singular that seems to to to sense conflict and kind of feed that information. It's overactive in depression and
Starting point is 00:41:02 And then in depression it looks like the left-door lateral does not sufficiently clamp down on it and what Therapy appears to do is to kind of restore that. What we see with TMS over that region is that we just exogenously do the same sort of thing. We restore the governance of the left-door-slateral over the singular area, and that is correlated with treatment improvement. So the degree in which you can re-time, re-regulate in time, the left-door salateral over the singular, the more of an antidepressant effect you have. Can we therefore say in crude terms that the door salateral, prefrontal cortex really is the governor of how we interpret physiological signals
Starting point is 00:41:45 and spontaneous thoughts. It is, it, it places a lens that the rest of the brain sees things through and you can, you can do these experiments where you, you can put a normal healthy control person in the, in the scanner and you can make them feel like they have a loss of control and then you can see that region come offline. So you can experimentally manipulate the system, and so kind of buffing it up, and it's like almost, TMS is almost like exercise
Starting point is 00:42:15 for the brain, right? You're kind of exercising this region over and over again with a physiologically relevant signal and kind of turning that system on. And what's interesting, I think really interesting for this show is to, we had a couple of folks, probably five or six folks that have actually told me this where if they remit early enough in the week,
Starting point is 00:42:36 we have this very dense stimulation approach where we can stimulate people really rapidly over a five day block. We don't discriminate when they get better to when they stop. So if they get better on day one, we still give them the other four days because it's in the protocol to do that and we can't. We're getting to a point where we can tell how long it's going to take, but we're not there yet. And so, you know, every time somebody gets better at day one or two, at the beginning, when we first started doing this, we'd say, you know, we're not sure. You know, we think this is safe to keep going, but, you know, what do you want to do?
Starting point is 00:43:07 And everybody was like, no, I want to keep going. And so, you know, by Wednesday, they're like totally zeroed out on the depression scales, you know, even better than most people walking around, like really no anxiety, no depression, or anything. By Thursday, the first guy that told me this, he came in and he said, you know, I was driving back to my hotel and I decided to go to the beach and I just sat there and I was totally present in the present moment for an hour and he's like, I read about this in my mindfulness books
Starting point is 00:43:35 but I experienced it last night and I've never experienced anything like this before and I was like, hmm, that's interesting but kind of wasn't sure. And then I didn't tell any, you know, obviously any more patients about that. And then about five over the last couple of years when they get, they were met early in the week. By the end of the week, they're like,
Starting point is 00:43:52 going to the beach and they're like, totally having what people describe as a pretty mindful, present moment sort of experience. Which is really interesting, you know, what that is. I mean, I don't have full on scientific data to tell you, but it's just, it's an interesting anecdote, right, that folks, when you push them through this point of feeling kind of clinically well, that some people
Starting point is 00:44:16 end up reporting this additional set of features. So. You mentioned the singular and the anterior singular in particular. Because now I feel like for the first time in my career I have some sense of what prefrontal cortex would actually be doing. Besides providing a bumper for the rest of the brain, the singulate, it seems, is a more primitive structure in the sense that it's it's under the, ideally it's under the regulation of this top-down control from prefrontal cortex. But what's mapped in the singular? And for the non-narassigned, it's out there when I say mapped,
Starting point is 00:44:51 if we were to put someone in a scanner and focus in on Singulate or put an electrode in there, what makes the neurons in their fire? What sorts of things in the body and in the mind and out in the world light up for lack of a better phrase, the singular. What does the singular like? Yeah, so that's true task. Those incongruent word-color associations, the dorsal part of that. For obsessive-compulsive disorder patients, certain triggers, you'll see some of their imaging studies will point to anterior singulate.
Starting point is 00:45:25 In the kind of very crude psychosourgery world 50 years ago, the anterior singulotomy was a way of treating obsessive compulsive disorder, right? Because that area seems to be overactive in people who are experiencing obsessive compulsive disorder. You can kind of walk the singulate wraps around, this white matter track, like bundles that wraps around that. And so there's a part that's above that, around that, and below
Starting point is 00:45:50 that. And depending upon how much of the, the conflict task subgenual that activation is, so the dorsal part of the anterior single, it seems to be kind of more of a pure cognitive, maybe obsessive, compulsive disorder sort of area, whereas when you start getting into mood sorts of triggers, like facial expression conflicts where you're supposed to, you know, there's an emotional strup task where you show the word happy and then you have a face of a person that looks mad, then that's another way of having the same sort of strup conflict. That seems to be more periginual, sub-genual areas, right? So you can kind of, you can trigger the singular based off the level of emotional valence from none down to a lot. And that seems to be how the, how it's distributed. There are, you know,
Starting point is 00:46:53 heart rate kind of components to an autonomic components in there too. There's something called a kinetic mutism, you know, you know, I'm a, a board certified neurosurcuitive behavioral neurologist. And I've seen, you know, a lot of these, what we call zebra cases and I'm a board-certified neuropsychiatrist behavioral neurologist. And I've seen a lot of these, what we call zebra cases in neurology, where people have these unusual neurological presentations and one of them is a kinetic mutism. And so you have a glioma sitting in the interhemisophoric
Starting point is 00:47:19 fissure and having pressure on the singulate, people can get into an almost catatonic-looking state where they kind of get stuck and they don't speak. And so that tells you something about how the singular works as well, right? It's like if it's not functioning, then people have a hard time kind of connecting with reality.
Starting point is 00:47:43 It seems to need to be constantly on, you know, online to be able to interact with the exterior world. Is it involved in some of the dissociative states that sometimes people who are very stressed or depressed experience, you said, Katatonia being an extreme one, but I know someone, for instance, that when they get really stressed
Starting point is 00:48:03 and it can even be if someone yells at them or even if someone's angry with them or they perceive someone's angry with them, there's a developmental backstory to why they likely feel this way. They just kind of can't, this is a high, high functioning individual normally and they just sort of can't function. They can't complete simple things like email or groceries or things for a short while. It's almost like a catatonia and they refer to as a dissociative state. Do you see that in depression? I mean, we're speculating here as to whether or not that involves a singular, but what you're saying has a whole lot of salience for me in thinking about this example.
Starting point is 00:48:43 Yeah, yeah. There's, so you see, you see Catoetonia is an extreme outcome of depression and sometimes it gets a frenzy and other illnesses. Dissociation is an extreme outcome, or even in some cases a less extreme outcome of PTSD and trauma. And, you know, and it's also a phenomenon that happens naturally in some people that are highly hypnotizable. And so if you ask David Spiegel, I'd say that some of the work that he's been working on is around posterior singulate and the capacity to dissociate. But yeah, with our stimulation approach to DLPFC, Dorsal Antirio Singulate, one of the subscales that moved the most
Starting point is 00:49:26 was the dissociative subscale for hypnotizability. So even in a normal individual, you see that change in that kind of experience of dissociation. I am highly hypnotizable. But David's hypnotized me a number of times. I mean, we have a clip of that on our human life channel. I've always, well always, starting my early teens, I started exploring hypnosis. I'm extremely hypnotizable. And self hypnosis or assisted hypnosis. I don't
Starting point is 00:49:58 know that I ever go into the sociostates. I'll try and avoid forcing you into running a clinical session right now, but to assess anything like that. But this brings about something really interesting, I think, which is I'm aware that some of the more popular emerging treatments for depression include things like ketamine, which is a dissociative anesthetic, is that right? And yet, my assumption is that as a dissociative anesthetic that it leads to dissociative states, where people can sort of third person themselves and be, so feel somewhat distanced from their
Starting point is 00:50:33 emotions. Yes. I've also been hearing that there are emerging treatments, psilocybin being one of them, but some other treatments, MDMA, etc., that will parse each of these in detail, that lead to the exact opposite state during the effect of the drug, which is a highly engaged emotionality and heart rate and sense of self, and can also lead to relief of depression. Now whether or not this, again, reflects that depression has many conditions, as opposed
Starting point is 00:51:05 to just one, or whether or not somehow tickling, or in some cases pushing really hard on the opposite ends of the scale really matter. I am absolutely fascinated, and again, also perplexed by this. Why would it be that a drug that induces dissociative states and a drug taken separately that induces hyper-associative states would lead to relief of the same condition. Yeah, that's a great question. Yeah, so for ketamine, the level of dissociation appears to be correlated with the therapeutic
Starting point is 00:51:39 effect. It appears to be necessary, but not sufficient to produce an antidepressant effect. And so, folks that don't have any psychological change from the ketamine or don't experience any dissociation typically tend to have less potent antidepressant effects from ketamine. We did a study a couple of years ago. It was really interesting. So we gave folks NELF Trexone, which is an opiate antagonist, mu and capo opiate receptor antagonist. And we gave folks the same individuals, a pill of that or a pill of placebo, and they
Starting point is 00:52:16 had no idea which one they were getting. Was this low dose Naltrexone? 50 milligrams, so it's pretty high dose. Okay. And so we gave a typical ketamine therapeutic dose, and then we gave 50 milligrams of naltricks on or placebo, and then in the same individuals, we gave two infusions, one with each of those conditions, and if they had an antidepressant effect, we waited until they relapsed, and then we gave them the other condition.
Starting point is 00:52:41 And then we looked to see what effect of blocking the opioid receptor, what effect would you see on the antidepressant effect of blocking the opioid receptor? Well, the idea that if ketamine works the way that a lot of researchers at the time thought that it completely worked in, which is the glutamate system, then you would have no effect of naltrexone. Because naltrexon just interacts with the opiate system. It doesn't do anything with any other systems. Ketamine has a lot of effects over, it has clear opiate effects in mice and various ways of looking at that.
Starting point is 00:53:16 And NMDA receptor antagonism and glutamate effects. So if it's just that the glutamate part is the part driving the antidepressant effect, you shouldn't have any difference in the antidepressant effect between the two conditions. If however, the antidepressant effect is primarily, the opioid properties of ketamine are necessary for the antidepressant effect, then you should have a loss of antidepressant effect during the ketamine plus naltrexone condition that you observed in the ketamine plus placebo condition. And what we saw was that there was a dramatic blockade of the antidepressant effect when
Starting point is 00:54:00 naltrexone was present in the people that had an antidepressant effect with ketamine plus CBO alone. Then, some friends of mine did a TMS study with pain, and they stimulated over the left or a slat or prefrontal cortex, and they gave Naloxin, which works basically the same way as Naltrexone, and they were able to block the anti-pain effects of TMS with an opiate blocker. So this idea that another kind of convergent point, right, this idea that the opioid receptor may have a role in mood regulation. What's also interesting is if you look at people that are getting a total knee operation,
Starting point is 00:54:43 it's a very painful operation, right? Total knee replacement. And you age sex, you know, everything match the individuals that are going through that, but you have a group of people that don't have depression and a group of people that do have depression. The presence of depression triples the oral opioid dose by day four. That's required. That's required to cover the pain. But what may be happening is it's not just treating physical pain, maybe treating emotional pain as well, right? At least transiently it seems to have a pro an antidepressant effect. Chronically it seems to have a very pro-depressant effect to make people treatment resistant. But you know it's an interesting phenomenon.
Starting point is 00:55:24 But yeah, the opioid system seems to be pretty pretty involved. But it's an interesting phenomenon. But yeah, the opioid system seems to be pretty involved. But what's interesting there with the ketamine trial is that we didn't see any effect on the dissociation. And so the dissociation was the same each time. So the psychological effect of what we call the trip or the kind of dissociative effect where people are having a psychological Phenomenon from ketamine, that was identical both times and so it kind of
Starting point is 00:55:52 It also challenged this idea that the psychological experience of the psychedelic effect may be all that's necessary to produce an effect and that the Pharmacology doesn't matter as long as you can achieve that state. And so, you know, we think we pretty clearly debunked that idea, that the underlying pharmacology and the state, you know, seem to be important. We don't know for sure if you can, a lot of people are working on this, if you can take out, you know, essentially the psychological effect and still have a drug that works to treat the illness that you're trying to target. And a lot of, there was a mouse study out this week where they had an LSD analog and they were able to see some animal level data to suggest that could be true.
Starting point is 00:56:40 But until we figure that out in humans, it's kind of to be determined. But it is curious, right? could be true, but until we figure that out in humans, it's kind of to be determined, but it is curious, right? Being able to kind of use experimental manipulations to try to separate, you know, some of these phenomenon apart and really understand what's doing what? It's so critical, and it's so critical to the other conversation that we'll surely get to, which is the progression of psychedelics
Starting point is 00:57:05 from illicitly legal drugs to clinically validated and presumably at some point either decriminalized or legal drugs, which has not yet happened, at least not in the U.S. But just to make sure that people are getting this and how crucial this is. What we're really talking about here is the fact that, you know, somebody takes a multi-gram dose of psilocybin or somebody takes MDMA or they take ketamine and they experience relief from their trauma, their depression, their addiction, or any number of the other things that indeed those compounds have been shown to be useful for in certain contexts, clinically supported, etc. There's this gravitational pull to the idea that certain contexts, clinically supported, et cetera. There's this gravitational pull to the idea
Starting point is 00:57:48 that, oh, it was the hallucinations. It was the dissociative state. It was the feeling of connectedness. And what we're really saying is that, while that certainly could be true, it may be the case that a major source of the positive shift that occurs after the effect of the drug is some underlying
Starting point is 00:58:06 biology like shifts in the muoopioid receptor, a lot of your experiments with naltrexone, or a change in the underlying neuromodulation that had anywhere from nothing to something to do with the real shift. And I know there's a group up at UC Davis that published a paper in nature about a year ago, also looking at these are, is a chemistry lab essentially modifying psychedelics to remove the hallucinogenic properties, the mood altering properties, and actually seeing some pretty impressive effects in shifts in mood after the drug wears off. And I know this, this gets people upset when they hear it. A lot of people, this gets people upset when they hear it.
Starting point is 00:58:45 A lot of people, this gets a lot of people upset really, because people think, oh no, it's the intense experience that matters. But in fact, that may not be the case at all. In fact, it's so powerful for people that sometimes I like in my mind to, you know, it's like somebody, it's like the birth of a new child, and it's such an incredible experience,
Starting point is 00:59:05 and then people feel so much connection, and then they sort of connect the experience of the actual birth to the connection. When in fact, that's true, it turns out, but there are a bunch of other things happening too, that are simply the reflection of the fact that you're holding a child and the hormonal effects, et cetera. So anyway, I think it's very important
Starting point is 00:59:23 that these different variables be figured out. Along those lines, I want to make sure that before we dive a bit deeper into ketamine and psilocybin, that we do touch on a really important topic that has been in the press a lot lately, which is SSRI, selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors, because we can't really have a discussion about depression without talking about SSRIs. Then I want to circle back to ketamine and psilocybin. It seems that there are now data that essentially say state that there's no direct link between serotonin levels and depression, although my understanding is that the SSRIs are powerfully effective
Starting point is 01:00:08 for certain forms of obsessive compulsive disorder and may also be effective for treatment of depression, but it may again be through some effect unrelated to serotonin itself. Is that right? And how should we think about SSRIs? Are they useful? Are they not useful? What's going on with SSRIs? Are your patients and other people as well? Yeah, the experiment that I described a bit ago around the Naltrexone
Starting point is 01:00:33 and ketamine. It was the first time I'm aware of where we were able to essentially eliminate an antidepressants effect by using a second drug as a kind of a blockade. And it highlights a bigger issue, right? The issue that we haven't had a good way of really understanding how these drugs work. And so it's the difference, I think a lot of the controversy there is that it's been difficult, I think, for folks to see that something can, in one hand, work, and in the other hand, we don't know how it works, right? And so, SSRIs clearly work.
Starting point is 01:01:15 You know, many, many meta-analyses kind of proving that out, right? That in a sub-population of individuals, they achieve great benefit. From depression, you know, for depression, for obsessive-compulsive disorder, for generalizing anxiety disorder panic, you know, all these things, you can see an improvement in those symptoms with what we call SSRIs or selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors. The issue there is that these selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors, the issue there is that these selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors end up blocking the reuptake of serotonin, leaving the serotonin in this kind of in between two neurons for a while and allowing for more serotonin to kind of be there.
Starting point is 01:02:01 The issue is that they don't work immediately, right? So they don't work like the same day you start taking them. And that suggests that probably it's not exactly the serotonin being in there that's directly driving it, that it's much more likely that it may have some say, plasticity effects, right? We know that things like brand-derived neurotrophic factor Get up regulated with chronic oral antidepressant use and so that's that's kind of the idea is that Is that these things work?
Starting point is 01:02:37 But what's powerful and I think what the authors of this paper was extremely controversial patient paper were in part trying to say was that there's not a deficit of serotonin. You're not born with what people call a chemical imbalance. And psychiatry is known this. This is not actually new information. Anybody, you know, it's kind of rehashing of a bunch of information we've known for a while now. But in the lay press, it's kind of hit in a of a bunch of information we've known for a while now, but in the lay press, it's kind of hit in a way that it didn't seem to grab attention before with previous publications. But this idea that this chemical imbalance idea is wrong, I really think that part's important
Starting point is 01:03:21 because I think that for a while, I think psychiatry, you know, what I'll call psychiatry 1.0, right? This kind of idea of Freud and psychotherapy and its origins, it was a lot around, you know, your family and those experiences and psychotherapy kind of going and correcting or helping you to figure out or and you know show you being able to see or people hear you so that you can eventually come to the conclusion of certain cognitions that aren't helping you right and there's a huge you know there's a huge importance there but there's a history where you know things like the schizophrenia, genetic mother and all of that
Starting point is 01:04:03 you know that was a concept at some point, right? And so we've transitioned from that to the, you know, for a long time, the chemical imbalance, which I'll call psychiatry 2.0, you know, this idea that there's something chemically missing. And I think that the trouble there for a patient who's not a physician, who's not someone who's, you know, who's, who's steeped in these sorts of ideas, who's, you know, more of, you know, kind of, kind of a person, kind of average American out there, right?
Starting point is 01:04:41 Is that it's telling, it's sending a message of, there's something missing with me, whether it be my experiences, I had no control over when I was a child or a chemical in my brain. What I think is really powerful with TMS, really powerful TMS, and a lovely, powerful, the psychedelic story, is it saying something different? You know, TMS works and there's no serotonin coming in or out of the brain, right? And we're doing a rapid form of TMS that works in one to five days. There's no, it's very unlikely that there's some long-term kind of upregulation of serotonin that's driving that. So, our work actually kind of pushes back on this serotonin hypothesis is being
Starting point is 01:05:26 kind of the center of depression because it says, look, we're not giving anybody any serotonin, we're simply turning these brain regions on and we're focused on the circuitry. And that's psychiatry 3.0. It's not just like neuromodulation. Neuromodulations are really nice use case for psychiatry 3.0 because it's a way to folkally and directly perturb brain regions and whatever modality you're using. But there are a lot of groups that are actually doing their imaging before and after and they're able to see circuit level changes for something like psilocybin or
Starting point is 01:05:59 ketamine long after the drug is gone, suggesting in those same brain regions converge. So the subgenual default mode network connection that we see is changing with our Stanford Neuromodulation Therapy technique. It's that same set of brain regions that ketamine and psilocybin seem to act on these connections between brain networks that seem to shift. And so it refocuses the story on something that's highly correctable, and it's basically electrophysiology, and it's basically kind of recalibrating a circuit that is recalibratable
Starting point is 01:06:42 instead of, I have something missing or I have some set of experiences early in life that are gonna forever trap me in these psychiatric diagnoses and so it kind of challenges that idea. And I think that's what's so powerful about psychiatry 3.0, this idea of focusing on the circuit because it gets us into thinking about psychiatry and psychiatric illnesses is something that are recoverable.
Starting point is 01:07:10 People can get better. We've seen with our TMS techniques, we've seen with some of the psychedelic work that we've done where people are actually in normal levels of mood for sustained periods of time or within five days. Within five or less days, and in the case of the psychedelics, within a few days, right? So we can get people out of these states. They're totally well.
Starting point is 01:07:31 There's no drug in their system at that point, in the case of psychedelics, it was never a drug in their system, in the case of TMS. And it just tells us that it's fixable. It's just like the heart. It's just like, you know, it's just like an arrhythmia in the heart. It's just like, you know, it's just like an arrhythmia in the heart. It's just like, you know, these other illnesses that,
Starting point is 01:07:48 it's like a broken leg, we can go in and do something and we can get somebody better. Then I think what's empowering and what a lot of patients have told me is they say, you know, I've gotten to, you know, some people will relapse and need more stimulation or need more psychedelics or whatever it is, but they'll tell me,
Starting point is 01:08:05 I've relapsed and I'm depressed again, but I'll never think about killing myself again, because I know that if I go get stimulated again, it improves, it gets better, it will be able to re-achieve it, and I don't fear that I'm chronically broken, I don't fear that the chemical imbalance is still imbalanced, I don't fear that I'm chronically broken. I don't fear that the chemical imbalance is still imbalanced. I don't fear that these things that I couldn't control in my childhood are going to be there and drive this problem forever.
Starting point is 01:08:34 And I think that's what's so powerful about this. The sense of control. The sense of control. The sense of, they're not doing the stimulation themselves. They're not administering the drug in these trials themselves, they probably never will, these will probably be medical treatments, but they are choosing to do it. And in that sense, they are in control. Yeah, I have a good friend.
Starting point is 01:08:54 I won't out him for reasons that will become clear in a moment who was quite obese and lost a lot of weight and was really proud of himself. And then, I guess we could say he sort of relapsed in the sense, not all the way, but far along, but his tone around it was very different. He knew he had accomplished what his goal once before. He was disappointed in himself, but he knew exactly why he had relapsed.
Starting point is 01:09:21 It was very clear. He had essentially relapsed to the previous set of eating behaviors and lack of exercise behaviors and has now brought himself back again. And it just resonates with your story that, you know, once somebody understands they can do it because they've been there before, this idea again of considering new rules that there's. And that brings me to this question about psychedelics and the frankly, the altered thinking and perception that occurs in in high dose psilocybin clinical sessions. It seems that the disorder thinking, even though it could be random, right, hearing,
Starting point is 01:10:01 hearing colors and and seeing sounds is always the cliché statement of the Timothy Leary area. Also, right there, that's a strup task of sorts. It's a synesthesia, it's a combining of perceptions, but it's sort of strup task-ish in that it's a new set of rules for the same stuff, right? And many people do report improvements in trauma-related symptomology and depression as I understand it from my read of the clinical
Starting point is 01:10:31 trials after taking psilocybin because during those sessions something comes to mind spontaneously. As you and I were talking about earlier, they will report for instance a new way of seeing the old problem. And the old problem could be the voice that they're no good. They'll never, nothing will ever work out or could be even more subtle than that. So that raises two questions. One is about the basic functioning of the human brain, which is, why do you think the brain would ever hold on to rules that don't serve us well.
Starting point is 01:11:07 That's one question. And then the second question is, what is it about psilocybin and related molecules in terms of their neurochemistry, in terms of the ways they disrupt thinking and feeling, et cetera, during the session that allow this novel rule consideration phenomenon.
Starting point is 01:11:28 Yeah. So the first question, I think it's an evolutionary neurobiology answer, right? I think that at the individual person level, you know, it doesn't make a whole lot of sense that when we're really stressed out, some of us want to eat more, right? At the individual person level, because it's like, that's not particularly that good for my health in the long term. But if you think about it like, you know, in some 500 years ago, a thousand years ago,
Starting point is 01:11:56 if I'm highly stressed out, it's most likely that I'm about to not have food at some point, and I should eat a bunch of food that is high fat, high sugar, high carb food, to put on weight for that, you know, next phase where in this stress, I may be in battle and I don't have food and I have enough fuel on board, right? And so we end up being, you know, we end up being a result of probably a lot of biology that's not that useful in the modern era. And I think in brain for, let's say PTSD, a lot of veterans come back and they experience these PTSD symptoms and they're not. It all useful back home, right? They hear some loud noise and all of a sudden they're behind a car or they're behind a,
Starting point is 01:12:40 I've heard of folks jumping, run behind a trash can or whatever in the middle of San Francisco when they hear a loud noise. But if you put them back in the battlefield, you're that highly adaptive. That's highly adaptive, right? And so I think what's interesting is that we, in the absence of using substances like psychedelics, end up having these very persistent memories that are attached to negatively-valanced emotion predominantly, as you were saying earlier, the jacket and in elementary school, you know, various things like that for me too, right? You remember these things. And we hold on to those things from it, I think, an evolutionary neurobiology standpoint. But what seems to, for whatever reason,
Starting point is 01:13:34 kind of alleviate that, are these substances some new, like MDMA, some that have been around for thousands of years like psilocybin and used in kind of sacramental, as a sacrament in traditions, seem to have a therapeutic effect. It seems to be pretty long lasting for these phenomenon. And so it's just curious, right? It's curious that in the absence of that, these things will keep going on and on, but in the presence of that exposure, then all of a sudden you see a resolution of the problem. And we have some work now where treating folks with Navy SEALs and the data still being
Starting point is 01:14:21 analyzed, but the anecdotes that we're getting right are folks are coming back and they're saying, finally, it's finally gone, right? This kind of, these set of PTSD symptoms are finally gone. And so this idea that for whatever reason, going into what's probably a highly plastic state, like we were talking about earlier, up regulation of brain drive neurotrophic factor in the case of IBegin, Gleald-derived neurotrophic factor, this highly plastic state and the ability to re-experience memories. And then, as you know, we always reconsolidate a memory.
Starting point is 01:14:57 When we bring it back up, we always reconsolidate. But reconsolidating it in that state for whatever reason may drive a therapeutic effect. And, you know, the jury is still out. I would say that I'm kind of an agnostic to what tool I'm using kind of guy. Like, my business is to find treatments that help people. And so I'm much more like pragmatic about it, you know, if this sort of thing, which has a lot of
Starting point is 01:15:31 cultural baggage, but if this sort of thing ultimately ends up being therapeutic, if we can design trials that convince me and others that it is, then we should absolutely use it, you know, and if it doesn't, then we clearly shouldn't use it. And I think that's a big question. The field's going to have to work out. We have a hard time blinding these trials because the placebo condition is not easy to pull off. A placebo for a psilocybin journey is hard to imagine.
Starting point is 01:16:03 We've been thinking about this. and maybe that ketamine study that I was talking about earlier, if we could give people naltrexone and ketamine, maybe that's a good, you know, a good sort of placebo condition, right? We know that we can block any of the actual antidepressant effects of ketamine. They still have an experience, you know? And so that's one way of doing it, but thinking about ways to do that and really kind of proving this out. And that's been, yeah, I think that's been kind of central
Starting point is 01:16:32 the way I've been thinking about this. But yeah, I think there's the work that's been done so far, the first psilocybin trial, the first MDMA trial is published in Asia Medicine recently. And what do those generally say? I mean, that they are effective for a number of people after one session, two sessions. Yeah. What's the general contour?
Starting point is 01:16:52 Let's start with psilocybin in MDMA. Yeah. So MDMA appears to, and one to a few MDMA sessions have an anti-PTSD effect that seems to be outside of the standard assumed levels of PTSD improvement that you can observe in individuals with this level of PTSD. What we call the effect size, which is essentially like a co-instee effect size, a measure that allows for you to compare different treatments to each other for different conditions that are, you know, agnostic to what the actual illness is. You know, the effect sizes there, you know, approach effect sizes, the things that are pretty
Starting point is 01:17:36 effective, like antacids, for heartburn, right? And you see that with, with, with, um, with MDMA treatment. So does that mean that for people that have trauma who do a and again we're talking about in a clinical setting they take a one or two doses of of MDM I think the standard maps dose is 150 to 175 milligrams again doing this with a position etc. control clinical trial legal. Yeah exactly. They do it once or twice. And broadly speaking, what percentage of people who had trauma report feeling significant relief from their trauma afterward? It's about two-thirds of people had had a clinically significant change in their PTSD. That's impressive. Which is impressive, right? And how long lasting was that? I mean, these trials were ended pretty recently
Starting point is 01:18:25 So it appears the last for a while in the earlier trials where they followed people out It seemed to last for kind of in the years range for some people and so it's you know, it's pretty it's pretty compelling psilocybin You know in contrast that with ketamine which only on average lasts about a week and a half for a single Infusion so it's a much shorter... So they have to get repeated infusions, accounting every 10 days or so? Yeah, or are you...
Starting point is 01:18:50 Forever? For some people, or they end up getting like, like a bunch of doses for a couple of weeks, and then for some people that seems the last a while. You know, that's where I think the psilocybin story for depression and the, in the MDMA story for PTSD seemed more interesting to me. So for psilocybin, what is the rough percentages on, and this would be relief not from trauma but from depression? Yeah, exactly.
Starting point is 01:19:17 So it's an open label study, as it's closer to like half to two thirds of people end up getting better, depending upon their level of treatment resistance. In the blinded trials, it was more like a third or so of people experienced relief. And this is a press release of the data. And so it hasn't, to my knowledge, it hasn't been published yet. And so I'm looking forward to seeing the full paper on that one.
Starting point is 01:19:40 But separated from placebo and looks pretty good as well. It looks like it's the first of two trials need to be done to get this thing approved for treatment resistant depression. So that stuff looks good. In terms of MDMA, for many years, it was reported in the popular press and there was a paper published in Science that MDMA was neurotoxic, that it would kill serotonin neurons. This was what was always said.
Starting point is 01:20:09 Then I saw another paper published in science that wasn't a retraction of the previous paper, but rather was a second paper in the same group that essentially admitted that the first time around they had injected these monkeys because of the, with not MDMA, but with methamphetamine, which is known to be neurotoxia. So it was kind of a public admittance of oops
Starting point is 01:20:33 or big, like really big screw up, so oops, but never attraction and then never really a publicly acknowledged correction in the popular press. So it seems that in the appropriate dosage range and with these one or two sessions, my assumption, and this is again as an assumption, tell me if I'm right or wrong here, is that MDMA is not neurotoxic for serotonergic neurons at appropriate doses and with appropriate sourcing, et cetera. So it was an interesting study that I think guys name is Halpern. Last name is Halpern. Not Casey Halpern. Not Cape.
Starting point is 01:21:12 A different now. I think Josh will help her. I'm playing on his first name, but Casey Halpern was a guest on this podcast and as a former colleague of ours at Stanford who unfortunately we lost to University of Pennsylvania and maybe someday we'll bring him back. Yeah, that's right. So this individual received some NIH funding to actually NIDA, National Institute for Drug Abuse funding to explore individuals of the Mormon faith in Utah
Starting point is 01:21:42 who partake in only MDMA. So the way this works is that MDMA happened kind of after a lot of the religious documents were developed. So MDMA isn't on the prohibited drug list. The band subs, the bonus list. The band subs, the list. I have some good friends who are LDS. Yeah, great, great people. I do as well, just to kind of set effects. And so these folks only use MDMA,
Starting point is 01:22:18 but they're not the problem with some people using drugs. They're poly subs and scusers, right? So you can't say it's the MDMA, if they've also taken other psychedelics and they've taken opiates and they've taken cocaine and you have this picture where you can't really tease out that problem but but with this right it was just individuals that were part of the more than faith and so they they were kind of purest in the sense they only used MDMA and he confirmed all of that. And it was an brilliant study, right? Because then he was able to go in and look at their cognitive profiles versus individuals
Starting point is 01:22:54 of the same geography, the same faith, all of that that happened to not take MDMA and found there were no neurocognitive differences. So does that mean that it was not damaging? It was not damaging. It's hard to know because to really do this study well, you'd have to track these folks down before they ever took MDMA and do a pre-post and compare to people that didn't.
Starting point is 01:23:19 But this is about as good of a study as you can do and given the situation to be able to check this out. Additionally, when I was back in Charleston and working in the Medical University of South Carolina, one of my mentors there, Dr. Wagner, was a neuropsychologist at MUSC. He was also the neuropsychologist for the early MDMA trials.
Starting point is 01:23:44 And so he did all the neuropsychologist for the early MDMA trials. He did all the neurocognitive batteries for individuals pre-posts, and similarly did not see any changes in neurocognitive profiles in a negative way. There's data from experimental patients receiving this. There's data from people that are chronic users who only take MDMA and that combination of data suggests that there's certainly no apparent risk in the kind of one to two to three dose range. It's probably unlikely that at least modest dose exposure over a lifetime doesn't appear to have a profound or cognitive
Starting point is 01:24:28 damaging effect. Interesting. Yeah, I know that sourcing is key. And we're here. We're talking about clinical trials where purity is assured. And years ago when so-called raves were really popular, maybe they're still popular. They've never been to one. So I would know if they're happening or not.
Starting point is 01:24:47 That's how in the know I am. But it was clear that testing for purity was important because sometimes the drugs are made such that there are contaminants like methamphetamine, which we know is highly neurotoxic. I think that one reason why people think that MDMA might be neurotoxic is the reported drop in energy or sort of feeling fatigued for a few days afterward. I spoke to a physician colleague of ours who said that that very likely has something to do with the surgeon prolactin that arrived subsequent to the big dopamine surge that occurs in MDMA. And I mentioned that because I know a number of people talk about serotonin depletion
Starting point is 01:25:32 after taking MDMA. He has it in mind that while that could be true, it's likely that anytime somebody takes something or does something where there's a huge lift in dopamine, that there's very likely a huge compensatory increase in prolactin that follows and prolactin has a kind of sedative effect, numbing effect on mood and libido, et cetera, that eventually also wears off. Does that make sense to you as a physician? Yeah, it makes sense.
Starting point is 01:25:56 The difference between, say, MDMA and psilocybin is that MDMA is kind of an infetamine of sorts, so it has effects in dopamine and the in psilocybin's pretty neutral and maybe a little bit of dopamine effects, but much more of a serotonergic focus drug. And so, yeah, I think you're going to see a different profile after. And I haven't heard that story, but that makes sense to me, too. Since you mentioned psilocybin, let's talk a little bit about the neurochemistry of psilocybin. As a serotonergic agent, my understanding is it operates on these, is it the 5HT serotonin 2C receptor, 2A, excuse me, 2A receptors, and that I've seen a bunch of different reports
Starting point is 01:26:39 in terms of what it's actually doing to the brain while people are under the effects of the drug. This is important for us to segment out because there are the effects that happen while people are under the influence and then the more long lasting effects. But some of the effects I've heard about our, for instance, and tell me again, if these are right or wrong,
Starting point is 01:26:57 that there is increased kind of activation of lateral connection, sort of broader areas of the brain being coactive than would normally occur. Maybe that explains areas of the brain being co-active than would normally occur. Maybe that explains some of the synesthesia's, you know, seeing sounds and hearing colors and that as the trivial example, but rule-breaking within the mind. But then I've also heard that perhaps it's lack of gating of sensory input. So normally if I'm looking at something,
Starting point is 01:27:25 I'm not thinking about the sensation in my right toe unless it's relevant. But if I'm thinking about the sensation in my right toe, I'm generally not thinking about the truck around the corner. So we have these attentional spotlights, but that somehow it creates a more, it adds spotlights. Yeah, it digates the thalamus. It digates the thalamus, right?
Starting point is 01:27:43 Through the particular thilamic structure. So what is the evidence that any of that is true? And are there other phenomena? Is there involvement of Dorsal Adolfo frontal cortex that we are aware of? And what I'm really headed here in a few minutes is, you know, is there a place for combining directed stimulation of the brain with psychedelics so that the effects of serotonin could be primarily within the structures that you know from your work to be relevant to depression?
Starting point is 01:28:14 But to simplify it first, what's going on when one takes psilocybin and why is it interesting in light of depression? Yeah, definitely. So, David Nut and Robin Carter and Harris's work around neuroimaging psychedelics are kind of the some of the first folks to do that work. And, you know, and to their great surprise, they thought there was going to be an increase in activity on psychedelics and what they found is the opposite, right? There was kind of an overall decrease in the level of activity in the brain with psychedelics, but they've also looked
Starting point is 01:28:46 at connectivity and there's this small world, large world connectivity that you think about. Small world meaning there's a much more focused, cortical function or sub-cortical function or whatever it is. What you see is a difference in that level of engagement of brain regions, the connectivity, kind of global connectivity to your point kind of increases. And so, you know, it's interesting, you know, I think to kind of have a convergent theory on this, it's still, you know, to be determined,
Starting point is 01:29:23 there's still a lot of work I think that needs to be done, but it's certainly suggestive that there's pretty profound changes in brain activity and brain connectivity after, and what we've found to be really interesting is the the antidepressant effects of psilocybin have a particular connectivity change that we also see with our TMS approaches, right? And it's this connectivity between the subgenual anterior singulate and the default mode network. And so when we do this effective Stanford neuromodulation therapy stimulation, we see a down regulation that connectivity between the negatively balanced mood state in the case of depressed individuals in the self-representation
Starting point is 01:30:09 of the brain, and you see that same connectivity change occur post-silocybin. Suggesting there's a convergent mechanism, and it makes sense, right? You've kind of got an over-connected negatively-balancedanced conflict system that's kind of attached onto the self-representation and people feel stuck, right? And then, when you do whatever you do that's effective, it unpares those two systems. I want to ask you about this phenomenon I've heard about during psilocybin journeys. I heard about this from Dr. Matthew Johnson Johnson who's running a lot of the clinical trials at Johns Hopkins and has been a guest on this podcast. He said that there's something seems to be important about the patient
Starting point is 01:30:57 who's depressed or who's and is under the influence of psilocybin or the patient who's trying to get over smoking or needing disorder who's taking psilocybin and is in the clinic, that there's something important to this notion of letting go, that people will feel as if their thoughts and their feelings and maybe even their body aren't under their control and that the clinician's job under those circumstances is of course to make sure that they're physically safe, that they don't jump out a window or try to actually give an example of a patient who thought that I think it was a she could move into the painting in the wall. And obviously that wasn't true in the real world, although it was true in her mind. So they prevented her from doing that. But that letting go, that
Starting point is 01:31:37 somehow untethering from the autonomic arousal that's occurring is important. Which brings us back to this idea, or me back to this idea of a like a seesaw where you're sort of letting go of the hinge and just sort of your heart rate's going up, like just go with it and trust, you know. Your heart rate's going down, just go with it and trust. You're thinking about something very powerful and depressing related to your childhood, you're just supposed to go there without fear. You're thinking about what's possible in terms of what could happen. So anyway, you get the
Starting point is 01:32:10 picture. Can we think of that as just the willingness to do a million different variations on the emotional strup task? You will entertain the full array of rules within your head and consider them. Where is there something more to it, you know, and again, we're, we're in the, the outer margins of understanding here, but what are your thoughts on, on this notion of letting go as such a key variable for relief from depression during the psychedelic journey? from depression during the psychedelic journey. Yeah, so I'll talk a little bit about something called exposure and response prevention therapy. That's a typical kind of gold standard treatment for OCD and I'll help to kind of help this a little bit conceptually. And so what that really is, it's a letting go therapy.
Starting point is 01:32:58 And so exposure response prevention, the idea is that you have to expose the individual to something that, you know, something that triggers an obsession that they then want to do whatever the compulsion is, right? And so I'll give you my first exposure response prevention patient when I was a resident. He was very concerned about leaving the lights on this car. And so what we did is we went out and we turned the lights on in his car and locked his door. So it was lights were on. And he was super worried this was going to kill his battery.
Starting point is 01:33:39 And we went and we spent an hour talking about things. And we went back out to his car and his battery was fine, and his lights were on, and he cranked the car, and we did it maybe one other time, and then all of a sudden that was gone. Right, and that's the idea is that, you know, you're essentially exposing, and you want to do it at levels that are from an anxiety standpoint tolerable,
Starting point is 01:34:04 but exposing the person to something and then letting them see that that exposure ends up being fine, right? It ends up not causing the thing that they end up being worried about. And so, you know, in some sense, being in the psychedelic state, and we are all taught at a level to retain some level of control. People have more or less of that, but we are all effectively retaining some level of control. We all wake up in the morning and put clothes on to go into society. We all try to say, most people try to say the right things. They don't try to do things that are outside of cultural
Starting point is 01:34:45 norms when they're in conversation. And so we're constantly at some level controlling the situation that we're in. And so it's not, it makes a lot of sense that in that state, part of the therapeutic effect that maybe linked to the neural circuitry is this idea of letting go and essentially letting the system, you know, the network configuration, maybe whatever it is assume a state that you've essentially been fighting the whole time. The same way that my OCD patient was fighting this need to click the off button on the lights of his car 50 times before he would go and do whatever he needed to do. And in some level, letting go there,
Starting point is 01:35:34 letting us just turn the lights on and him not do anything or letting go, meaning in the psychedelic state, you're just letting go of whatever it is you're holding on to, negatively balanced. Think thoughts about yourself in the setting of having depression or re-experiencing a trauma, memory, and allowing that to just happen and re-seeing it again through a different light. It feels the same in the sense that that's allowing for whatever is going on with these psychedelic states to do whatever they do.
Starting point is 01:36:08 Fascinating. You said exposure response therapies that you're usually doing. Exposure response prevention prevention therapy done outside of this psychedelic journey. It's done outside the psychedelic journey, but that idea of letting go is present in both of those. Psychothera kind of straight up, totally sober, non-psychedelic, non-anything, psychomanualized, psychotherapy that we know works really well for OCD, and then in that psychedelic state. So people have done studies with psilocybin and now there's some studies with MDMA trying to look at you treating OCD you know with the same sort of idea of letting go right and and how do you how do you have an OCD patient kind of let go maybe even letting go of not washing their hands anymore
Starting point is 01:36:58 you know kind of accepting the idea they're not going to get germs on their hands or whatever it is you know and so it's kind of part of that part and parcel that same sort of thinking. When I was in college, I developed a compulsive superstition. I'm not afraid to admit this. I somehow developed a knock on wood superstition. And I was actually kind of shamed by it of it because it rationally made no sense. I don't consider myself a superstitious person. Never was a superstitious kid.
Starting point is 01:37:27 I'd step on the sidewalk crack, I'd walk under ladders, I'd probably even try to walk under a ladder, even though I don't suggest it. But somehow I picked this thing up and I used to sneak it at times. I told my girlfriend at the time that I had it and hopes that that would prevent me from doing it.
Starting point is 01:37:45 And it's tricky. Sometimes it actually comes back. I think, gosh, I didn't say, you know, knock on wood. I didn't knock on wood. I hope that doesn't actually happen. And it's quote unquote crazy. Right. But I crazy in the sense that it makes no sense rationally why the events would be linked. And yet I think a lot of people out there do have internal superstitions. Maybe by talking about it now, it'll go away. I just say, clearly I just need to challenge it. Anyway, I mentioned it because I consider myself generally rational person, but it's interesting how these motor patterns get activated and this notion of letting go because I don't actually know what consequence I fear. As I was hearing the example, you gave the fear of the car running battery running down.
Starting point is 01:38:30 I was about to say, well, what if the battery actually did run out, then the therapy would be undermined. And yet, that could also be interesting, too, because it's not that big of a deal. You jump the car. But in my case, I need to think about what the ultimate fear is. Yeah, and I think a lot of people, so it's interesting if you look at, say, the OCD scale or the depression scale or whatever, we don't define normal as zero.
Starting point is 01:38:53 We define normal as some number range above so zero to, in the case of the Montgomery Asperg depression rating scale, one of the depression scales, we use 10, right? That's the normal range. And so people can have some sadness and still be considered normal in the case of the OCD scale. It's about the same 10, right? Where we say it kind of starts to be, you know, mildly abnormal or something. And I always tell the medical students, look, my friends that are surf instructors, they're
Starting point is 01:39:25 more like a zero on the Y-box, people that are professionals, they're non-zero, but it's still within the normal range. And especially in the case that you're talking about, it doesn't sound like it got in your way. It doesn't sound like you're obviously highly successful, tenured professor at Stanford and do all the great things that you do. And so it's very much kind of within the normal range. And I think totally assumed that a lot of people have these sorts of things.
Starting point is 01:39:56 And I think something is a psychiatric diagnosis when it severely impairs your ability to function. And that's when we when we cross that threshold. But I think that a lot of people, and it's great that you're bringing this up, but it's very anti-stigmatizing that you're bringing up, because I think a lot of people hold that stuff in, and they don't want to talk about it, because they're worried that somebody else may think something,
Starting point is 01:40:21 but the reality is, as a psychiatrist, I talk to a lot of patients, a lot of people that are, you know, family members, you know, folks that are just going through it, a death in the family, whatever it is. And what you figure out is like everybody's got a little something here and there. Everybody has the knock in some way, if that makes sense. And it's just, and we're just all, we're all just kind of more predisposed not to talk about it, but I think it's important to talk about it because I think that when we start all talking
Starting point is 01:40:49 about it, then we realize that we're all kind of in this together in a way and then some folks that have, have to knock 100 times, we call that OCD, and they have all, you know, they're worried about germs and all these other things we call that OCD. And then in that all, you know, they're worried about germs and all these other things we call that OCD. And then in that circumstance, you know, they need treatment, right? But it is really on just like blood sugar
Starting point is 01:41:12 or just like blood pressure, it's on a range, you know, and it's not just these discrete diagnoses you have them or you don't. That's good to know. I actually feel some relief just sharing this because I am slightly, I wouldn't say ashamed, as I've embarrassed by, but I offer it as a, you know, that, you know, it is what it is as they say.
Starting point is 01:41:32 And it certainly doesn't seem to hinder my life much, knock on wood. Nice. So if we could talk a bit about I began, I don't know much about I began, although anytime I hear the you know AI any you know lidocaine eye-begin I think of an anesthetic And going to the dentist which is an unpleasant experience for me generally
Starting point is 01:41:56 What's what is eye-begin Does this have anything to do with the so-called toad? You know people talk about smoking, frog, skin, and toad skin. What is it used for clinically? Is it legal in the US in terms of a clinical tool? Who's using it and for what purposes? If you could educate me on IBegin, I truly know nothing about it, except I think I know how to spell it correctly. That's fair. So Ibo gain is one of the alkaloids that you can extract from a Iboga tree root bark that's
Starting point is 01:42:37 typically growing in the country of Gabon, Africa. So, Gabon is one of the West African countries can middle of Africa on the West Coast. And Gabon has a group of folks, you know, called the Buiti. It's a religious kind of sacramental group that sacramental uses. Iboga root bark is part of that, the sacrament, and they've been using Iboga root bark for a very long time. And it's part of the tradition. There's a whole set of ceremony around it.
Starting point is 01:43:30 If you're interested in this, there's a book called Breaking Open the Head by Daniel Pinchback that goes through and talks about this whole process. But essentially, the Gabonism has been using this for a long time. And it's a kind of an atypical psychedelic. It's not a psychedelic that we normally think about with psilocybin and LSD where their visual perceptual changes, right?
Starting point is 01:43:57 So if you take psilocybin or LSD, what you experience as you experience these kind of visual perceptual differences in the external world, right? And on enough LSD or psilocybin, an individual can actually perceive something visually in the external world that isn't there, as we talked about earlier. I began doesn't do that. I began does something different. It's kind of like, have you ever seen a minority report with you know, a movie with Tom Cruise, I think 15 or 20 years ago, or something, so it dates us a little
Starting point is 01:44:29 bit, but it was this it was this movie where he would be able to go and see these kind of pre-crumbs and he had this big, you know, this big screen where he could look at scenes from from time and like kind of go through that scene and see it. And so what what individuals take I began will say is it open eyes they don't see anything but close eyes they'll go back through and re-experience earlier life memories and they will be able to experience it from a place of empathy not only for themselves but from others and kind of detached empathy and being able to from a place of empathy, not only for themselves, but from others and kind of detached empathy
Starting point is 01:45:09 and being able to see this as almost a third party, even though they were there, but they're able to see it as a third party. So Claudio Narono, a psychiatrist from Argentina, described this a lot of books that he wrote, and then I think then 80s and 90s around this. I began to spend a long time, Howard Lotsoff, American guy that brought it over from Africa, was a polysubstance user, used every drug that he had his hands on, took Ibogaine, and including a lot of other psychedelics, by the way, took Ibogaine and then never did
Starting point is 01:45:43 another drug again supposedly, because he had such a profound Ibogaine experience. Ibogaine is in no way a recreational substance. It's not a recreational substance if you want it to be a recreational substance because you're essentially having this what they call life review. They also call it 10 years of psychotherapy and a night. So these are the terminology that people talk about. The issue, does it last? Is it truly one night? It's usually, you know, we can go depending upon if you get redost or anything, go sometimes, depending upon how fast you metabolize it, sometimes 24, sometimes 36 hours, sometimes it can be shorter, but it is a long time. It's a very long time.
Starting point is 01:46:26 time. It's a very long time. So it's definitely the longest acting psychedelic substance I know of. And so people, people, you will take this and they'll have this re-evaluation of the given memory. And then we were talking about early reconsolidate that memory again, and then it seems to have an effect of that reconsolidation process. And so about four or five years ago, I was tapped by Robert Molinca, one of the senior neuroscientists we both know in the university, he says, well, there's an unnamed donor that's very interested in funding a scientific, kind of open label study of these Navy seals that have been going down to Mexico and taking I've again and also on a five-a-me-odm-t, which I'll talk about in a second, to treat PTSD, you know, the claim to traumatic brain injury, depression, you know, that whole constellation of symptoms.
Starting point is 01:47:25 As it was described to me by various people that had done this by their spouses and whatnot, John, we'll just say John, John couldn't screw a light into a light bulb into a light fixture. They were just so debilitated, they couldn't do simple tasks, what we call activities of daily living. And they were coming back and having these really dramatic improvements in all aspects of life. And so we have over the last couple of years been able to do this first in human, kind of full neurobiological, clinical,
Starting point is 01:48:06 neurocognitive evaluation of what I began is doing. In this case, in special operations, special forces, individuals, former Navy SEALs, former Army Rangers, that kind of crew folks, and look at the preposed changes that we, that their experience to be able to totally quantitate all of that. And so we've been able to capture all the clinical scales, depression scales, PTSD scales,
Starting point is 01:48:30 all that standard stuff, neurocognitive batteries. So how does your executive function work specifically? How does your verbal memory, all of that? And then neuroimaging and EEG. So this will be the first human study of IBugane for those. And the reason why is because Iogaine is kind of the both seemingly the most potent and most seemingly to me at least most powerful psychedelic, but the one that has the most risk too because it has a cardiac effect.
Starting point is 01:49:01 It seems to be that you can screen people out that have risk off of their electrocardiogram and reduce the risk quite a bit. And that's what we all did, but that's why people haven't really studied it as much. And it isn't as, in addition, there's nobody goes to a rave on Ibogaine, there's no recreation at all with that. It's not fun.
Starting point is 01:49:22 It's, people say that it's relieving, but it's hard work, right? Because, yeah, you're re-examining things. And, you know, and so then, so then we see these folks after and I'll tell you, you know, we haven't fully analyzed the data yet, but I'll tell you that, you know, from what my folks are telling me,
Starting point is 01:49:43 it's pretty dramatic, you know, people come back and they're doing a lot better. They're doing a lot better. And nobody, I'm not going to wouldn't, nobody's had any sort of cardiac issue at all. In the cohort that we've studied, and they look a lot better, and they feel a lot better too. And they describe these experiences
Starting point is 01:50:06 of being able to go back through. And soldiers experience something called moral injury, right? Where they maybe they accidentally blew something up and had a kid in it or something like that. If they're in Afghanistan or Iraq, maybe a child died on accident or maybe a civilian died or whatever it was, and they've suffered these moral injuries as part of the job, but it's almost one of the vocational risks. They come back and say that they've forgiven themselves, which is huge.
Starting point is 01:50:39 And in part of that is being able to see themselves in a different light and having empathy finally for themselves and being able to kind of have that experience of forgiving. And so very cool. The study, you know, what was happening was they were taking I'm again, and then taking something called 5MEO DMT, people call the TOD. It's the Sonoran River Tode.
Starting point is 01:51:04 I think it's like you can find these in Mexico, find them in Arizona. And in the back of the toad produces something called five MEO DMT, which is a flavor of DMT that produces a particular psychedelic effect, also used as a sacrament. Is it a dimethyl-triptamine? It is a five-meo dimethyl-triptamine. It's a dimethyl-triptamine with a addition to it. The deal there is that it lasts longer than traditional DMT. It's like 20 minutes at five, three, or whatever kind of thing. And so these guys were taking, I began, and then they'd take the five of me ODMT after we had to kind of divorce those two things, be able to do the study and just understand
Starting point is 01:51:53 what I began was doing. And they go back down a month later and they'll do the five of me ODMT. So two completely separate sessions? Two completely separate sessions. And one quick question about I began before a bit a bit more on 5MEO DMT. Is the IBGain journey guided or the person just closes their eyes and they just start falling into the back catalog of memories? They have a bunch of preparatory sessions and then they have a bunch of sessions after
Starting point is 01:52:18 that they kind of, they're able to kind of rehash things. During, there's a sitter that sits there and kind of sits with them and helps them out, but it's not, it's pretty, the phenomenon of the drug seems to drive a lot of this, right? And so a lot of it ends up being what we call supportive psychotherapies, you're just kind of being there and, you know, maybe you're holding the person's hand, maybe you're just saying, I'm here, maybe whatever it is, but you're making sure they know you're around, but you're not really, there's not really an interaction per se, and then the whole kind of goal there is just to get folks to kind of go back through
Starting point is 01:52:53 and re-examine these memories, then ultimately look like they re-consolidate them. You know, it's very interesting. I mean, there's this kind of, as you said earlier, Timothy Lerie, kind of social cultural construct that ends up being overlaid over psychedelics. And what I think is that if you rid yourself of all of those preconceived notions of what it is and isn't and the counter-culture movement,
Starting point is 01:53:23 all that stuff that neither of us were ever involved in, neither of us were ever partaken. You know, it's kind of straight scientists looking at this, right? If you can kind of rid yourself of all those social, cultural constructions and then re-examine this, these, if we just discovered these today, we would say that these sorts of drugs are a huge breakthrough in psychiatry because they allow for us to do a lot of the sorts of things
Starting point is 01:53:47 we've been thinking about with SSRIs, with psychotherapy, but kind of combined, right? Psychotherapy plus drugs in a substance that kind of allows you to re-examine these things. And so it's interesting, there's a lot to do to try to figure out if that's true. and I can say that as it stands right now, we don't know if that statement is true, right? There's a lot more work that needs to happen for that statement to be proven to be true. But the hypothesis is, if it is true, then it's very likely that this will be seen as a breakthrough
Starting point is 01:54:24 because it allows you to do these sorts of things that you can't do with normal waking consciousness. But also why we have to really think about this and you know these drugs can't be recreational drugs. They really shouldn't be recreational drugs right? They're really too powerful to be used in the context of recreation because they can put you into these states. And this generation of psychedelic researchers are really clear about that.
Starting point is 01:54:56 I think the 60s folks were not clear about that and they felt like there was a whole kind of cultural thing that was going on there. But I think this cohort of individuals really understands that in order to really make this happen, we have to understand that if you need a prescription for an SSRI, which doesn't change your consciousness a whole lot, and we were very worried about that, and the doctor has to evaluate you for that every week, the idea that some of these substances would go outside of very strict medical supervision
Starting point is 01:55:28 is kind of preposterous. Actually, it's kind of a dumb moment, I think, for all of medicine to say, look, if we're going to do this right, we've got to do it in such a way that's so protected, that's so safe, that we make sure people know these things are not recreational and they're really for the pure purposes of really powerfully
Starting point is 01:55:50 changing cognition for a while and letting people have these what seem to be relatively therapeutic states. I think it's great that you're doing this study and along the lines of the sort of the early iterations of psychedelics and the counterculture of the 60s and early iterations of psychedelics and the counter culture of the 60s and 70s, some of which took place like one floor of the Kukuznesse, I think, is actually based on the Menlo Park VA, which is, you know, in our neighborhood of Stanford.
Starting point is 01:56:17 And things are quite a bit different now. I know you and I have spent some time with the operators and former operators at an event in and last veterans day. In fact, the so-called veteran solutions group that's pioneering a lot of these psychedelic treatments for former special operators and current special operators. And what's interesting to me about that is in contrast to the counterculture movement of the 60s and 70s, that room was filled with people that are very much of a structure, the military,
Starting point is 01:56:45 right? So it's no longer considered left wing, right wing, anti-military, pro-military. Here, this is just about one group of people who's exploring psychedelics as a treatment for trauma and PTSD and other things. And of course, you also have other domains of society looking at this. And in fact, it was really interesting because they were both far left and far right politicians at that event. Up on stage together, talking about in kind of a lighter terms, heart medicine, but also talking about neurobiology and talking, it was just fascinating from a, from the perspective of somebody who's trying to learn about this
Starting point is 01:57:20 stuff that psychedelic therapies no longer sit within the anti-establishment realm. It's both... it's independent of all that. Certainly when people in the military are adopting it as a potential treatment. Again, still under exploration, but also under exploration at universities like Stanford and Johns Hopkins and UCSF and of University College London and on and on. Along the lines of tree barks and toadskins, tell me about Iooska and as a plant, you know, it's intriguing. And is it a pro-cerotonergic drug like psilocybin? And is it useful for the same sorts of conditions that we talked about thus far? And if you could perhaps tell me a little bit also about the Brazilian prisoner study. Yeah, yeah, definitely. Iowaska is another psychedelic.
Starting point is 01:58:14 It's used as a sacrament in Brazil and Peru and Ecuador and Colombia. So a lot of the South American countries. And what they do is they combine two plants together where one plant of the two plant combination would effectively do nothing. But the two plant combination together is capable of producing this very profound psychedelic effect. And what's really kind of curious is that there, as I understand it, 10 to 20,000 plant species in the Amazon. And somehow somebody combined these two plants together
Starting point is 01:59:03 in certain proportionality and cook this for five, ten hours to the point where you cook out the dimethyl triptomine out of one of the plants and cook out the reversible monomine oxidase inhibitor out of the other plant. It's such a way that the reversible monomine oxidase inhibitor prevents the GI breakdown of the dimethyl triptomine in such a way that it's then allowed to cross the blood brain barrier and get into the brain. If you didn't add the reversible monomine oxidase inhibitor plant derived into this combination, then it would never cross the brain.
Starting point is 01:59:42 If you put people on a standard psychiatry prescribed monomy and oxidase inhibitor that wasn't reversible, you'd throw them in a serotonin syndrome. So this kind of sweet spot that somehow Iowaska practitioner is a found of being able to get DMT in the brain from an oral source with this combination of a monomy and oxidase inhibitor. It's curious. And so that substance has been explored as an antidepressant agent in some studies have looked at that.
Starting point is 02:00:18 It also seems to be very safe. There was a psychiatrist down at UCLA Harbor who's done a lot of work with this where he's looked at children even that have been exposed to just kind of small doses of ayahuasca as a kind of a sacrament within Amazonian tribes and found no cognitive effects, no neurocognitive effects and adults. And so it appears to be safe. It's part of, it's kind of part and brought into various religions, including kind of merged with Catholicism in South America, which is kind of very interesting. And so, you know, in some sex of Catholicism in Brazil, it's used as sacrament during religious ceremonies.
Starting point is 02:01:06 And so it became interesting to Brazilian researchers as to whether or not they could affect recidivism rates for prisoners in Brazilian prisons, right? So they gave half of the prisoners, you know, some sort of inert substance, and half of the prisoners on aahuasca session. And the recidivism rate of the return to prison rate in the ayahuasca exposed individuals was statistically significantly lower than the recidivism rate in the control group, suggesting that, you know, whatever is going on there seems to have an effect on whatever drives criminal behavior, whatever criminal behavior that happened to be. And I don't have the details on the exact
Starting point is 02:01:50 nature of the crime. You know, I am also in no way saying that we should just be giving psychic deluxe to folks. In prison and all of that, I think that that is a very edgy thing to do and probably not something that anybody should try, but it does kind of bring up this curious question of what is it about that that would drive people to change those behaviors and why do people make those behavioral decisions? And in a lot of times, if you look at prisons
Starting point is 02:02:22 in the United States, you know, people say this, what's the biggest mental health facility in the United States, you know, you said people say this, what's the biggest mental health facility in the United States? It's a prison. Yeah, there's a lot of impact there for sure. You know, the homeless issue, the prison issue. It does lead to something that I heard recently, which is related to all this, which is cannabis. We hear a lot nowadays about people will say, well, it's safer than alcohol.
Starting point is 02:02:50 And we did an episode on alcohol that, at least by my read of the literature, indeed, alcohol does seem to be quite bad for our health. Be on your feet. I think it's pretty clear that not drinking is better for your health than drinking at all. And here I'm not trying to tell people what to do, but those are what the data say. And forget the studies on red wine.
Starting point is 02:03:08 You'd have to drink so much red wine to get enough for us to wear a trowel. It's not even clear with red trowel. It doesn't even use for any way, etc, etc. Nonetheless, cannabis is now available in a lot of very high potency forms. People are vaping cannabis. People are smoking cannabis. I certainly am not saying that cannabis is bad for people necessarily, although I think children, I would like hope that their brain development would be completed first.
Starting point is 02:03:33 Get to age 25. I know that sounds late for a lot of people, but the THC obviously taps into some endogenous systems of endocannabinoid systems and is powerful. And I've seen this report that was in Lancet Psychiatry this last year that said that early use of potent cannabis, meaning age 14 to 20 or so, can potentially lead to an exacerbation of psychosis later in life. And I actually put this out on social media and it sort of exploded. I didn't expect it to. People were saying, well, that's not causal and obviously it's not causal.
Starting point is 02:04:14 Because people say, well, maybe people with psychotic tendencies are seeking out cannabis. Although that's sort of a weak argument in the sense that there's at least a four times 4x increase in these psychotic episodes for people later in life. But what are your thoughts about cannabis? Because I do want to acknowledge that it does have medical benefits for certain things. A pain, chemotherapy. So by no means trying to knock on cannabis and its appropriate medicinal use, but what do we think about cannabis in terms of this finding that can
Starting point is 02:04:47 they exacerbate psychosis in certain individuals? Yeah, so I think there's a couple of things, right? So cannabis is multiple cannabinoids, right? T-H-C-C-B-D, CBN, Sativas, and Indicas. Yeah, there's a lot there, Tonpack. There's a lot, but there are two main kind of chemicals you think about and kind of how things are essentially bred, right? And so there's a lot of cannabis that's really bred to be very high, very potent THC.
Starting point is 02:05:22 And there's cannabis where the THC's bread completely out. So there's stories, you know, from Colorado, right? This strain of cannabis that's THC free. There's no THC at all. And it's all CBD. And they, it's called Charlotte's Web. And a bunch of kids' parents, one kid, and then kind of a string of parents after that moved to Colorado when cannabis was legal, legalized because CBD is anti-epileptic. So CBD is also anti-psychotic. And so there have been a number of studies that if you give CBD at high doses, it's anti-psychotic and gets established, gets schizophrenia establish schizophrenia patients.
Starting point is 02:06:09 The issue is that we've bred CBD out of marijuana selectively over time. We've gotten very good at figuring out how to do that, right? Conversely, THC is pro-psychotic and pro-ptic. Right? And so when you talk about this cannabis cause psychosis or this cannabis treat psychosis, it appears to be more related to the proportions of CBD to THC than it does to the kind of idea of cannabis. So for me, there's a kind of no stock of this or anything like that, but there's's a company called GW pharmaceuticals and I haven't looked into them in a while, but they They they have a lot of clinical trials for something called Dravez syndrome Which is a seizure disorder or kids sees a whole lot Linux gasto syndrome
Starting point is 02:06:58 Which is a seizure disorder kids are seizing 300 times a day both of these are like kids are seizing so much They they're basically in a seizure or in the post-ictal phase constantly and and they've failed everything they failed barbituates they failed bromides which we We just don't use anymore except into these cases because of the side effects and they'll give kids CBD And I think CBD is a pretty safe drug compared bromide. And so this idea that CBD in a kid is actually safe. It's a cannibanoid, but it's CBD and it's safe. And so that, to me, is totally fine. Also giving CBD as an adjunctive treatment for schizophrenia, there have been some positive
Starting point is 02:07:42 trials and negative trials in that, but there seems to be no negative side effects. It seems to reduce some of the metabolic syndrome issues in folks with schizophrenia who are having side effects from the primary anti-psychotic. The converse is, there's clearly cases where people that are taking very high doses of THC become psychotic, they get put into the psychiatric unit. Nothing happens other than they kind of get the THC out of their system and then they resolve their psychosis, right? And so, that, and, you know, a handful of people who have had, you know, seizures related
Starting point is 02:08:19 to the high doses of THC and syncopate and all sorts of things. And so this idea that THC, high doses of THC can be pro-psychotic, is also not taking a shot at people that think that cannabis overall is a good thing. It's just, it's just is what it is. And the kind of pure, if you, I think if you zoom back and you say, you're a true naturalist, you're thinking about natural medicines in the world. You should think, well, probably marijuana was balanced THC CBD at some point and then we just, we humans messed with it, right? And that most likely, that was probably okay at some level and then we pushed it one
Starting point is 02:08:59 way or another. And what I mean by okay is in a 45 year old it's okay kind of thing. Now what I think is going on with the kids with the teenagers is you've got prefrontal maturation right and then you're exposing them to a whole lot of high THC load and while it's unclear if it's if it's cause or effect it it's certainly in the picture. And if I were a parent, I wouldn't want my 16 year old smoking marijuana. If I were a parent in my 30 year old, otherwise healthy, totally fine, you know, whatever, banker, lawyer, kid, decided to try marijuana for the first time, I wouldn't scold them
Starting point is 02:09:45 about it, right? So, I think it's kind of a different thing, right? I would never want my up to 25 year old, just like you're saying, before prefrontal maturation, I would never want my kid to be exposed at all. But it looks like, except in, you know, susceptible individuals that are susceptible to drug induced psychosis, it looks like, you know, it's a relatively safe thing past prefrontal maturation. You know, again, I can't, I'm not going to comment of cause and effect, but I would say that, you know, it doesn't, if you're a parent, it doesn't make much sense, right? You know, you want to, you never know what's ultimately going to hurt your kid.
Starting point is 02:10:25 I mean, my wife's probably talking about it earlier. My wife's pregnant now. She kind of avoids everything, right? And rightfully so, right? This idea that we just, we want to be careful when our children's brains are developing. And I think that's really what you were saying. And I think it actually important.
Starting point is 02:10:39 The bigger question that you asked, which is relative risks of drugs drugs is an interesting one. So David not published and I think it was in the Lancet, I'll have to look it up, I think in the Lancet, an article about relative drug risks for the person and for society. And this was like, he was on the UK's like British drug policy group, where essentially what he showed was, if you look at societal risk plus personal risk and you combine those two, you know what, drug is the most dangerous drug in the world. I'm going to guess it's alcohol.
Starting point is 02:11:18 It's alcohol. Right behind heroin and cocaine and did it and somewhere in the middle is marijuana and right on the tail end on the other on the exact other end of this psilocybin Is caffeine did usually doesn't act a list I may have been on the list It was probably it was probably pretty close to psilocybin, but somewhere in the middle was ketamine somewhere in the middle was was Infatimine somewhere in the you know a little closer to psilocybin I think it was MDMA you know but but if it's it's it's this combined personal you know kind of world risk of these things and so alcohol makes it because there's a huge
Starting point is 02:11:57 amount of personal risk and there's a huge amount of societal risk right drunk drivers kill X amount of kid you of people in the world. Fight, sexual assault. All that, all that. Yeah, and then all the cancer and all that stuff. And so it beats out cocaine, it beats out heroin, it beats out all of these things. And yet, we don't, we don't as a culture for whatever reason.
Starting point is 02:12:21 We don't as a culture culture see it as a drug. And that's the part that really baffles me, you know, and I mean, they serve it. I mean, this is no knock on Stanford at all. Of course, I wouldn't do that. This is every institution I've been to, they serve alcohol at the graduate student events. That's right. They serve alcohol. They do a happy hour. I've never been a drinker.
Starting point is 02:12:44 I can take it or leave it. And I realize that some people they really enjoy alcohol. You know, my former partner, I mean, she just was in that, you know, 10% or so people who have a glass of wine and just feel great, and the second one feel great. I just want to take a nap after I have a bit of alcohol. So it never does much for me. I always feel poisoned. I feel lucky in that sense, but it's unbelievable that it is so prevalent and it's just baked into the medical, even medical institutions,
Starting point is 02:13:13 they'll pop a bottle of champagne to celebrate the opening of a hospital. That's right, that's right. That's pretty crazy. Yeah, no, you're absolutely right. I think what's gonna happen, but this is me looking at crystal ball a little bit, but I think what's going to happen is what happened with doctors and smoking.
Starting point is 02:13:29 So if you look at the 50s and 60s, there are all these pictures of doctors smoking cigarettes with patients, psychiatrists doing psychotherapy and smoking a cigarette with the patient sitting on a couch, surgeon smoking a cigarette in between cases. There are all these pictures of that. Now all of a sudden sudden smoking is totally banned. I think it's totally banned for most of Stanford campus. My suspicion is, as you're suggesting, right? You know, this is everywhere, and it's all kind of ubiquitous. It's some critical point, some tipping point. Everybody's going to realize that just like with smoking, we've got a red hospital systems
Starting point is 02:14:06 in universities of alcohol. And at some point in 50 years, it's my view that we'll look it back at these scenarios that you're talking about and be like, you know what, we were foolish about this. We can't believe that we gave people alcohol when they graduated from whatever. I think we'll have a different take on it. But it's going to take a longer time. I think people did a really good job tying smoking a lung cancer. It's like a very simplistic, smoking lung cancer.
Starting point is 02:14:43 As you know, alcohol increases the risk of a lot of different cancers, not so clear which one. I mean, there's like, you know, the kind of oral, like the throat, tongue cancer, that's one. Rest cancer. Yeah, breast cancer, you know, and so it's, it's kind of just, it's a harder story to tell, you know, and I think that's why, and everybody, you know, and then there's this whole, it's, you know, my mom says this. It's like, I drink my glass of wine because my doctor told me it was heart healthy and we were talking about this and I try to, no, no, but doctor so and so said it's heart healthy
Starting point is 02:15:14 and so it ends up being this thing where like, she's drinking alcohol because she thinks that it's good for her heart. And, you know, and it's hard, I've had those conversations that are it's hard to untie that. And I think that, yeah, at some point, we're gonna hit some threshold moment. And it'll be interesting if we really look at the data
Starting point is 02:15:34 and we really look at what's safe and not safe from purely from this analysis, it kind of points to the right direction. It's really interesting. And also, it's saying nothing of poor judgment under the influence of alcohol. I mean, I would venture that if we were to remove alcohol
Starting point is 02:15:50 from university campuses, watch the students are going to lobby against me if I said this. But if you were to remove alcohol from campuses, I mean, just think about what I suspect would be the improvement in good decision-making. And that would occur. Or, I've got stories from graduate school and it was very different 10 years ago.
Starting point is 02:16:14 There was a lot more alcohol consumption. Again, that was never my thing, but I know people who made really bad decisions. In any case, there's a whole landscape there, emergency, I think you got your finger right on the pulse of it. I want to touch on something slightly different than what we've been talking about, but definitely related to depression.
Starting point is 02:16:32 And this again is one of these intriguing but perplexing things, which is that sleep deprivation can improve symptoms of depression. And yet, I'm personally very familiar with the fact that if I don't sleep well for one night or don't sleep at all, in fact, I do have an ability to function pretty well the next day. I'll do this non-sleep deep rest practice that I blabber a lot about on the Puberman Lab podcast, which is for me is tremendously restorative. But I like
Starting point is 02:16:58 a good night's sleep. I think everybody understands now, thanks to the great work of Matthew Walker and others that have really gotten out into the world saying, look, the foundation of mental health, physical health and high performance, if that's your thing, being a functional human being is to try and get enough quality deep sleep, at least 80% of the nights of your life, if you can. That's something to focus on,
Starting point is 02:17:21 just like good nutrition, just like exercise and social connection, et cetera. So sleep deprivation, we know, in particular, I think rapid eye movement components of sleep deprivation can improve the symptoms of depression. And yet, being sleep deprived can also really dysregulate our control over the autonomic system. I notice on night two or night three,
Starting point is 02:17:43 of course, sleep if I'm going through a stressful phase and that's happening. All of a sudden, my heart rate is chronically elevated. My thought patterns become really disrupted. I can't then exercise, my decision making has thrown off. My emotionality is more like the hinge as we were referring to it earlier.
Starting point is 02:17:59 Feels less in control under my control. And maybe I wonder sometimes if I enter that state that you referred to earlier where the Dorsal Adel prefrontal cortex is no longer leading the singular, but the singular is now in charge. The players are in charge of the coach. Not a good situation. So I know you've done some work on sleep deprivation
Starting point is 02:18:18 and light and effects. Please tell us about that. And please tell us about this triple therapy. Yeah, yeah. So, friend of mine, Greg Salem, one of the professors at Stanford, was very interested in sleep. He did a bunch of training and sleep before he went to medical school and got very interested in this idea that, as you're saying, if you sleep deprived somebody that, as you're saying, if you sleep deprived somebody one night in just kind of an isolated single night, at the end of that sleep deprivation, they will have an antidepressant effect, but as soon as they fall asleep, they lose it.
Starting point is 02:18:54 So, if it's a depressed individual, you can get them to be less depressed acutely. As soon as they fall asleep, they wake up eight hours later, then they come back into the same level of depression. The idea is that you needed to do some sort of circadian reset. Part of what depression is, is that it's a dysregulated circadian system. Mentors of mine say, if you can just get the sleep better, that's half the battle of dealing with depression because so many people have insomnia around depression and have a whole host of types of insomnia. Having a hard time falling asleep, waking up in the middle of the night and waking up early are all symptoms of depression.
Starting point is 02:19:33 And so what this does is it sleep deprives the individual and then there's a certain calculation of shifting their phase and simultaneously exposing them to bright light. So that's the triple, the phase shift, the sleep deprivation and the bright light to try to get their circadian rhythm. Essentially, the theory is re-entrained. And so in the trials that we've done and other trials, prior to ours and after, you know, it looked like there was a pretty, pretty profound anti-depressant effect from this triple therapy,
Starting point is 02:20:12 this triple therapy that seemed to be durable, meaning durability is this term we use to say that not only can you get kind of point relief, but that the relief ends up, you know, lasting. What's important to know about this is like, you shouldn't do this at home for sure. This is what you would need to do. This is with a professional, because it's complicated. It's not just one thing.
Starting point is 02:20:32 And it, in sleep deprivation, while it seems to be antidepressant, it's pro-anxiety. So if you take a highly anxious person, that's not depressed, and you sleep deprived them, they get profoundly anxious. And so that's the other thing that, that you have to really realize is that this is like everything
Starting point is 02:20:48 else that I've talked about today, all things that you have to do under medical supervision, but curious, right? And I think, you know, the question that always comes up is why isn't this used more? And I think the reason is that there's not really a mechanism for, you know, ultimately in medicine as sad as it is, you have to have a code to do a thing. There has to be a code associated with a treatment, and it's hard to figure out how to make a code for this. And so I think that's part of it.
Starting point is 02:21:18 And so if there's a way, and somebody's got to kind of take that baton on that, but if there's a way to make a code for this, I think you could actually turn it into something that was more widely utilized. And you know, we probably dream up ways of how to integrate AI, passive sensing, all that stuff to really make that work. But I think that would be the idea
Starting point is 02:21:44 that would be the trajectory I'd see. So, yeah. Having a billable to insurance code is fundamental. And a lot of listeners of this podcast, I think, have a background in engineering science. And we will put a link to that manuscript that talks about the triple therapy because here we're talking about one night's sleep deprivation, some time, the light exposure to the eyes and then shifting in the circadian clock, things central to the themes of this podcast that come up often.
Starting point is 02:22:10 I think for the typical person, can we say that trying to get a regular light dark cycle at sleep rhythm would be beneficial for overall mood regulation? Yeah, I think for the typical person, really kind of re-regulating your sleep and trying to get a good night's sleep in which you fall asleep, stay asleep, wake up in a set time every morning is going to be pretty crucial. In mild depression, I think that one has a lot of control over that as we were talking about earlier. I think when you hit some threshold and depression where things become kind of semi-volutional,
Starting point is 02:22:46 that's harder to kind of will yourself into that. There are therapies like, there's a CBT for insomnia, for instance, where you can do cognitive behavioral therapy to help with insomnia. Sometimes people, and I'm no sleep expert, that kind of passes to Greg to fully talk about this, but some of what goes on that people that kind of passed this to Greg to fully talk about this, but some
Starting point is 02:23:06 of what goes on that people with kind of milder insomnia experiences like blue light out of their computer and things like that, that they, so you can use like blue light blockers to trick your brain, as you know better than me, a trick your brain to think that it's still light outside and so people will still have insomnia because their brain still thinks that it's light outside and then people will you know the kind of strip CBT for sleep. You know therapists will say there are only two things that you should do in your bed and if you're under a certain age and what not, it's really one thing that you should do in your bed, which is to sleep and be with your partner, right? So those are kind of the two the two things that you should do in a bedroom and that's really the only things that you should
Starting point is 02:23:57 do in a bedroom. If you're having sleep problems, you shouldn't watch TV in a bedroom, shouldn't eat in a bedroom, shouldn't hang out. Keep the phone out of the bedroom. Keep the phone out of the bedroom. Yeah. We should get Grick Salem on the podcast we the I'll just mention for people that want to regulate their sleep we have a sleep toolkit that's available as a downloadable PDF at hubram lab.com just go to the menu and a lot of the things in that toolkit are based on work from Stanford sleep laboratories including Jamie Zyzer's and other lab, not aimed at depression specifically. Listen, Nolan, Dr. Williams, this has been an amazing voyage through the circuitry of
Starting point is 02:24:37 autonomic control. This landscape of the prefrontal cortex is, I find incredibly fascinating and I just, when I start off by saying, please do come back again and teach us more about that and your TMS work. I, before we wrap, however, I do want to give you the opportunity to talk about the St. Study. Yeah.
Starting point is 02:24:57 Is it St. or St. Splerall? Yeah, it's St. So, so, so, um, St. or what we're calling it S&T now, um, St. has, you know, the intent was not to kind of connect it to religion, but we may have accidentally done so. And so we abbreviated it to S&T for the subsequent trials, which was initially Stanford accelerated intelligent neuromodulation therapy, or now we're calling Stanford neuromodulation therapy, but it's the idea there, which is a cool idea, is that
Starting point is 02:25:28 TMS is a device that delivers a treatment. And the treatment is the protocol, and the protocol is the stimulation parameter set in a specific brain region for a specific condition. And so what's cool about neuromodulation, whether it be transcranial magnetic stimulation or transcranial direct current stimulation or deep brain stimulation, like what Casey Halpern talked about on another podcast,
Starting point is 02:25:59 is this idea that in all of those cases, the device itself is a physical layer conduit of a stimulation protocol that's therapeutic for a given condition and a given brain region. And so in the case of depression, which we know the most about with TMS, we've been doing TMS studies for depression for, since 1995, right, in a clearance in 2008, 2009. And in that, in that time frame,
Starting point is 02:26:30 we were able to go from really knowing very little at all about how to do something like this to getting an FDA clearance. And the way that it went down was that there were two groups studying that there were two groups studying different components at NIH. The first group was studying mood neuroanatomy on functional imaging that was kind of the first generation of functional imaging back then. So PET scans, which are kind of metabolic scans and then spect scans. And the idea there was looking at activity and metabolism in prefrontal cortex and what they found in these kind of more crude scans is a just general hypoe activity, hypometabolism. The other group, right upstairs at the National Institute for Neurological Diseases and Stroke
Starting point is 02:27:17 in INDS, they were looking at using TMS, which had been around for 10 years and repetitively stimulating in motor cortex. What they found was, gosh, we can get a readout in thumb muscle movement amplitude that's really reproducible across people. It's like universally reproducible. And if we do certain stimulation approaches, they are biologically active to either increase excitability, i.e. the thumb, motion, and a set intensity goes up, the amount of amplitude goes up, or inhibitory
Starting point is 02:27:51 deep-potentiating, it goes down with other biological stimulation approaches, and then a third outcome, which is important, that it's inert, it doesn't do either. So you can have stimulation approaches that do one, you know, increase activity, decrease activity, or, you know, or, or inert. And so what they found was, oh, we can excite certain brain regions. And my mentor, Mark George, said, had this kind of aha moment where he said, wow, there's under activity and prefrontal cortex and depression. And we can increase activity using this thing that we know we can increase activity in motor cortex. We just need to put it in the left or a salateral pre-frontal cortex, and then they combined the two and started stimulating.
Starting point is 02:28:34 Once a day in this kind of very abbreviated fashion and lo and behold, some of those depression patients resolve their depression and back then, and still today, you can go and as a psychiatric patient stay at the National Institute of Mental Health and go through clinical trials to try to get treated. There were patients who had been there for months and they were able to be discharged because their mood was better. It was just this very crude approach where they were using ruler measurements or a DLP of CWAS and they were stimulating with devices that you needed to physically dunk the coil in an ice bath. And with that, they still were able to, the kind of genius of this, Mark, and others,
Starting point is 02:29:12 they're still able to create a purely engineered stimulation approach. What's cool about that is that they kind of found two things, right? They found this one stimulation protocol. It does have some antidepressant effect. It's limited. It doesn't treat everybody. It does have some antidepressant effect. And this bigger concept that a neuromodulation device
Starting point is 02:29:36 is kind of like a pharmaceutical company for you, right? That in a given individual, a TMS device, or whatever neuromodulation device is able to generate, to, to, you can create a stimulation approach that is specific to a given condition and specific to an individual. And so the physical layer is just how you exert that, similarly to how we make pharmaceutical drugs in a pharmaceutical company, but the actual therapy itself is what you do where you do it. And so what we learned from another 20, 30 years of this is that you can modify the stimulation protocol
Starting point is 02:30:16 in such a way where you can create a whole new treatment and put it through the same TMS device, or thank God, an evolved version of it where you don't have to dunk it in ice baths and they can actually really handle much more aggressive stimulation approaches. And so in 2005, a group published a neuron, a paper demonstrating that if you stimulate with the hippocampal rhythms through a TMS coil,
Starting point is 02:30:43 you can excite the brain with memory rhythms and it'll last an hour. So you can change cortical excitability in this thumbtwich for an hour, sending three minutes of excitatory or 40 seconds in the case of inhibitory stimulation that mimics the hippocampal rhythms. So much more efficient than the original TMS approaches. And so after that, group tried to do it in this kind of six week schedule.
Starting point is 02:31:10 And after that, and while they were doing that, we decided, gosh, this problem I talked about at the beginning of the show where you have this problem that we don't have a treatment for people who are in these high acuity psychiatric emergency states, this idea that we're going to have a treatment for people who are in these high acuity psychiatric emergency states, right? This idea that we're going to engineer a treatment where we can reorganize the stimulation approach in time to be much more efficient by utilizing something called space learning
Starting point is 02:31:37 theory. And so you probably know about the space learning theory. It's the idea for the viewers is it's a simple psychological thing but we've also seen it in hippocampal slice sort of physiology too where if I'm cramming for a test what I do is I write out 60 note cards and I read each one for a minute until I get to the first note card and again and that's about an hour later right and we just we just intuitively do this. We all automatically do that. And we intuit that because we know that what doesn't work is writing out one note card and looking at it over and over again. Nobody ever does that, right? We've all been in graduate school,
Starting point is 02:32:17 medical school, and we have these big stacks of note cards. That's space learning theory. It's this idea that you need to see it about every hour to an hour and a half and that Optimizes learning if you take the same stimulation approach that I'm talking about this theta-burst stimulation approach and you take a hippocampal slice of a mouse and you stimulate You you enlarge some dendritic spines and you prime some and then if you stimulate right after that you don't get any change. It's called en masse stimulation. But if you wait about an hour to an hour and a half, you get more dendritic spines enlarged and more primed, which by the way, also is what ketamine does. It causes a dendritic spine enlargement. And so, you know, what we found was that the old way of doing TMS, this idea of just doing it once a day, every day, five days a week for six weeks, didn't utilize the space learning theory.
Starting point is 02:33:09 It's like studying for a month or two, just a little bit once a day. You remember some of that stuff, but it's not as potent as that week where you're cramming, right? And what we realized is that if we could reorganize the stimulation in times that we took the whole six week course, we actually figured out a way to do it in a day. And then what we also figured out is that people are under-dosing TMS, because if you just keep going after six weeks out to month three, four, five, more and more people got better.
Starting point is 02:33:38 So we figured out it's not just one day, we're going to give five times the normal dose, we're going to give seven and a half months worth in five days, using space learning theory. So every hour? Every hour for 10 hours. For five days. For five days. So it's a 50 hour block. It's 90 minutes of actual stimulation, but spread out through the day in the same way
Starting point is 02:33:56 of learning, which is perfect for an impatient psychiatric unit, right? Five days is manageable. Yeah, you can get stimulation. Nobody's ever dropped out by the schedule. They're, you know, folks that want to do this, want to do it. So they'll do their nine minutes. They'll go get breakfast. They'll do their nine minutes. They'll go see their therapist or whatever it is. And so what we found with this reorganization and time of the stimulation, dose, and then the third component is we do resting state functional connectivity scans on everybody, and we have ways now in the last five to ten years of
Starting point is 02:34:29 picking out that specific subgenual DLPFC sub-circuit that I was talking about earlier, that single DLPFC, we can pick that out in every single one. If you want to come to lab, we can find your DLPFC subgenual, it's even more robust and non-different. And we can still rely too just while we're in there. Yeah, if you want to, we can move around your hypnotizability. And we can find that spot in each person, instead of finding the same spot on the skull, we find the same spot on the brain, and we can stimulate, we do that every hour and the hour, and what we've found is that folks will within one to five days,
Starting point is 02:35:06 in more cases than not, and depending upon if you're looking at the soap and label or in trials somewhere between 60 and 90% of the time, they will go into full-on remission in the sense they're totally normal from a mood standpoint at the end of this. And like I said, with variable durability. So that's the part we have to figure out now about dosing and how to keep people well. But for some people,
Starting point is 02:35:29 you know, we've had four years of remission, you know, year of remission. And it's really that cramming of the test. It's really that idea that you're laying in that information to the exact right spot. And the signal is a simple signal signal but it's a profound one which is turn on, stay on, remember to stay on. You know that idea that you're sending this memory signal into the brain and you're doing it in such a way that you're telling the system. You're kind of taking it out of the hippocampuses, your own hippocampuses hand is you're sending the same signal, the hippocampus normally signals out. Now you're sending that signal into the prefrontal cortex and kind of utilizing the brain's own communication style to get it to get out of the state.
Starting point is 02:36:15 And what's very cool about this is that is that people when they when they kind of exit out of that, they end up, they end up saying, they don't have any side effects from it, and they feel back to normal. Like some people, not everybody, but there's a subsection of people with SSRIs where they'll say, I kind of feel numb or I have GI side effects or I can't,
Starting point is 02:36:39 I can't, I don't have the sexual interest that I used to have and that sort of thing. Not anything gets SSRIized as I said earlier, life saving, you know, for a subsection of people, these things really work. But with this, what you see is that people don't talk about any of that stuff. And I think it's likely because you're tapping into that
Starting point is 02:36:58 core circuitry and you're reversing it and you're doing it with a magnet that because it's a very profound electromagnet, it's the same field strength as an MRI scanner, it's able to induce a current in the brain in this focal targeted way without getting into the rest of the brain, without getting into the rest of the body at all, and just really kind of acting only on that circuitry that's involved. Incredible. Is the St. Studies still ongoing? And if people are interested in potentially being patients
Starting point is 02:37:30 or subjects in the study, can we provide them a portal link? Absolutely. So we have now the treatment. Some of my students went over to a company called Magnus Medical and they've been working on this. They've gotten FDA clearance now. And now folks can get it through trials over the next couple of years because it's going to take some time for that company
Starting point is 02:37:52 to kind of get up and running and get a device and get the whole thing set up nationally. But while that's all going on, there's still about a thousand patients that need to be recruited across a bunch of different trials all over the country. We'll take people from anywhere in the country. We also have partners in New York and San Diego and other, and soon to be South Carolina and other places where we can actually kind of, you know, my lab can help to kind of let people know where to go, where they, you know, based off of where they're at in the US and get them access to being able to be in a trial.
Starting point is 02:38:26 And what we've tried to do is make it so that even if you get the 50-50 chance, you're gonna get the real deal, or you're gonna get the non-real deal, but what we figured out is a way to let everyone have access if they got the not-real deal version, the kind of sham version, or the of sham version or the fake version for the first part of the trial, there are other trials where they can have access to the real version.
Starting point is 02:38:52 So, essentially, everybody eventually gets access to having the real version. And so, that's been a big thing for me. I want everybody that comes through one of our trials to be able to have access. I think it's important while the company is doing what they're doing and what the lab is doing and kind of nationally what other partner labs are doing. Well, I can assure you you're going to get some interest. I'm happy to have it. Thank you. And listen, thank you so much for taking us on this incredible voyage through
Starting point is 02:39:24 the neurosurcatory underlying certain aspects of depression, the coverage of the different types of depression, the various therapeutic compounds, how they work. We've talked about a lot of things that you've shared so much knowledge. And even as I say that, I very much want to have you back to talk about.
Starting point is 02:39:41 Many other things as well that we didn't have time to cover, but I also just really want to thank you for to talk about. Cool. Many other things as well that we didn't have time to cover, but I also just really want to thank you for the work that you do. I know we are colleagues, but you run an enormous laboratory. It's enormous in my, but 40 people is a big group, a very big group. Plus you're in the clinic. You also have a life of your own outside of work. And to take the time to sit down with us and share all this knowledge that really is in service to mental health and human feeling better and in fact avoiding often
Starting point is 02:40:11 suicidal depression. It's just incredible work and an incredible generosity and just thank you so much. Well, thank you, man. I mean, I, you know, similarly I want to thank you for what you're doing. I mean, I think that what you know, I've got a lot of friends, folks that are not in the medical profession, friends of mine, you know, one of my buddies who's a real estate agent who works with us, who's a big fan of your show, and you know, I told a couple of people like that, I was coming on and they were like super stoked, they're like, you know, we watch every show,
Starting point is 02:40:41 and you know, super excited to watch mine. And they said something very important to me that you make this complicated neuroscience and brain body science accessible. And a way that few have a gift to do. And I think that that's so important and this show is doing so much to help with science literacy and yeah, appreciate you.
Starting point is 02:41:11 Well, thank you. I'm gratified and honored by your statement and I look forward to more. Thank you. Absolutely. Thank you. Thank you for joining me today for my discussion with Dr. Nolan Williams. I hope you found our discussion about psychedelics and other compounds about transcranial magnetic stimulation and about the treatments for depression in general
Starting point is 02:41:32 to be as stimulating as I did. If you'd like to learn more about the work being done in Dr. Williams' laboratory, you can go to the Brain Stimulation Laboratory website, which is BSL.Stanford.edu. And there you have the opportunity to apply to be in one of the clinical trials for depression or other studies, as well, if you like to support the work being done in Dr. Williams' laboratory for the treatment of depression and other psychiatric disorders. If you're learning from and are enjoying the Hubertman Lab podcast, please subscribe to our YouTube
Starting point is 02:42:00 channel. That's a terrific zero-cost way to support us. In addition, please subscribe to the Hubertman Lab podcast on Spotify and Apple. And on both Spotify and Apple, you also have the opportunity to leave us up to a five-star review. If you have questions for us or comments about the information we've covered or suggestions about future guests, please put those in the comment section on YouTube. We do read all the comments. Please also check out the sponsors mentioned at the beginning of today's episode. That's the best way to support the Hubertman Lab podcast. Not so much today, but in many previous episodes of the Hubertman Lab podcast, we talk about supplements. While supplements aren't necessary for everybody, many people derive tremendous benefit from them for things like enhancing sleep and focus and hormone optimization.
Starting point is 02:42:40 The Hubertman Lab podcast is partnered with Momentus Supplements. If you'd like to see the supplements of the Hubertman Lab podcast that's partnered with Momentus Supplements. If you'd like to see the supplements of the Huberman Lab podcast has partnered with Momentus on, you can go to LiveMomentus spelled O-U-S, so livemomentus.com slash Huberman. And there you'll see a number of the supplements that we talk about regularly on the podcast. I should just mention that that catalog of supplements is constantly being updated. As mentioned at the beginning of today's episode, the Huberman Lab podcast has now launched a premium channel. That premium channel will feature monthly AMAs or ask me anything where I answer your
Starting point is 02:43:08 questions in depth, as well as other premium resources. If you'd like to subscribe to the premium channel, you can simply go to HubertmanLab.com slash premium. As you mentioned, that the proceeds from the premium channel go to support the standard Hubertman Lab Podcast, which will continue to be released every Monday per usual, as well as supporting various research projects done on humans to create the sorts of tools for mental health, physical health, and performance that you hear about on the Huberman Lab podcast. Again, it's HubermanLab.com slash premium to subscribe.
Starting point is 02:43:37 It's $10 a month or $100 per year. If you haven't already subscribed to our zero cost newsletter, we have what is called the Neural Network newsletter. You can subscribe by going to HubertmanLab.com, go to the menu and click on newsletter. Those newsletters include summaries of podcast episodes, lists of tools from the Hubertman Lab podcast, and if you'd like to see previous newsletters we've released, you can also just go to HubertmanLab.com, click on newsletter in the menu, and you'll see various downloadable PDFs.
Starting point is 02:44:04 If you want to sign up for the newsletter, we just ask for your email, we do not share your email with anybody, and again, it's completely zero cost. If you're not already following me on social media, it's Huberman Lab on Twitter, on Facebook, and on Instagram. And at all three of those places, I cover topics and subject matter that are sometimes overlapping with the information covered on the Huberman Lab podcast, but that's often distinct from information
Starting point is 02:44:24 on the Huberman Lab podcast. Again,'s often distinct from information on the Huberman Lab podcast. Again, it's Huberman Lab on all social media channels. And last, but certainly not least, thank you for your interest in science.

There aren't comments yet for this episode. Click on any sentence in the transcript to leave a comment.