Huberman Lab - The Science of Hearing, Balance & Accelerated Learning

Episode Date: July 5, 2021

This episode I describe how our ears and nervous system decode sound waves and gravity to allow us to hear and make sense of sounds. I also describe protocols for rapid learning of sound and other typ...es of information. I discuss sound localization, doppler effects (sound motion), pitch perception and how we isolate sounds in noisy environments. I also review the scientific findings on binaural beats and white noise and how they can improve learning. Other topics and protocols include tinnitus, sea sickness, ear movement, ear growth and the science-supported ways we can all accelerate learning using "gap effects". For the full show notes, visit hubermanlab.com. Thank you to our sponsors AG1 (Athletic Greens): https://athleticgreens.com/huberman LMNT: https://drinklmnt.com/huberman Supplements from Momentous https://www.livemomentous.com/huberman Timestamps (00:00:00) Overview of Topics (00:02:20) Protocol: New Data for Rapid Learning  (00:09:10) Introduction: Hearing & Balance  (00:09:30) Sponsors: AG1, LMNT (00:13:53) How We Perceive Sounds (00:21:56) Your Hearing Brain (Areas)   (00:23:48) Localizing Sounds (00:28:00) Ear Movement: What It Means  (00:33:00) Your Ears (Likely) Make Sounds: Role of Hormones, Sexual Orientation  (00:35:30) Binaural Beats: Do They Work?  (00:43:54) White Noise Can Enhance Learning & Dopamine (00:51:00) Headphones (00:55:51) White Noise During Development: Possibly Harmful (01:03:25) Remembering Information, & The Cocktail Party Effect (01:12:55) How to Learn Information You Hear (01:18:10) Doppler (01:22:43) Tinnitus: What Has Been Found To Help? (01:30:40) Aging: How Big Are Your Ears? (01:35:00) Balance: Semi-Circular Canals (01:40:35) A Vestibular Experiment (01:43:15) Improve Your Sense of Balance (01:48:55) Accelerating Balance (01:51:55) Self-Generated Forward Motion (01:56:25) Dizzy versus Light-Headed (01:58:38) Motion Sickness Solution (02:01:23) Synthesis Title Card Photo Credit: Mike Blabac Disclaimer

Transcript
Discussion (0)
Starting point is 00:00:00 Welcome to the UBREM and Lab podcast where we discuss science and science-based tools for everyday life. I'm Andrew UBREM and I'm a professor of neurobiology and ophthalmology at Stanford School of Medicine. Today we're going to talk all about hearing and balance and how you can use your ability to hear specific things and your balance system in order to learn anything faster. The auditory system, meaning the hearing system and your balance system, which is called the vestibular system, interact with all the other systems of the brain and body and used properly can allow you to learn information more quickly, remember that information longer and with more ease.
Starting point is 00:00:45 And you can also improve the way you can hear. You can improve your balance. We're going to talk about tools for all of that. This is one area of science where we understand a lot about the cells and the mechanisms and the ear and in the brain and so forth. So we're going to talk about that a little bit and then we're going to get directly into protocols, meaning tools. We're also going to talk about ways in which the auditory and balanced system suffer. We're going to talk about tinnitus, which is this ringing of the ears that,
Starting point is 00:01:15 unfortunately, for people that suffer from it, they really suffer. It's very intrusive for them. We're going to talk about some treatments that can work in some circumstances and some of the more recent emerging treatments that I think many people aren't aware of. We're also gonna talk about this, what seems like kind of a weird fact, which is that 70% of people, all people, make what are called autoacoustic emissions.
Starting point is 00:01:42 Their ears actually make noises. Chances are your ears are making noises right now, but you can't perceive them. And yet, those can have an influence on other people and animals in your environment. It's a fascinating aspect to your biology. You're going to learn a lot about how your biology and brain and ears and the so-called inner ear that's associated with balance. You're going to learn a lot about how all those work, you're gonna learn a lot of neuroscience.
Starting point is 00:02:07 I'll even tell you what type of music to listen to. And if you listen to me, you can leverage that in order to learn faster. Before we begin talking about the science of hearing imbalance and tools that leverage hearing imbalance for learning faster, I want to provide some information about another way to learn much faster.
Starting point is 00:02:25 There's a paper that was published recently. This is a paper that was published in the cell reports, an excellent journal. It's a peer-reviewed paper from a really excellent group looking at skill learning. Now, previously, I've talked about how in the attempt to learn skills, the vital thing to do is to get lots of repetitions. You've heard of the 10,000 hours thing, you've heard of lots of different strategies for learning faster, 80, 20 rule and all that. The bottom line is you need to generate many, many repetitions of something that you're trying to learn. And the errors that you generate are also very important for learning. It also turns out that taking rest within the learning episode is very important. I want to be really clear what I'm referring to here.
Starting point is 00:03:17 In earlier episodes, I've discussed how when you're trying to learn something, it's beneficial, it's been shown in scientific studies, that if you take a 20 minute shallow nap or you simply do nothing after a period of learning, that it enhances the rates of learning and the depth of learning, your ability to learn and remember that information. What I'm about to describe are new data that say
Starting point is 00:03:41 that you actually should be injecting rest within the learning episode. Now, not talking about going to sleep while learning. This is the way that the study was done. The study involved having people learn sequences of numbers or keys on a piano. So let's use the keys on a piano example. I'm not a musician, but I think I'll get this correct. They asked people to practice a sequence of keys, G-D-F-E-G, G-D-F-E-G, G-D-F-E-G,
Starting point is 00:04:15 and they would practice that either continually for a given amount of time, or they would just do that for 10 seconds. They would play G-D-F-E-G, G-D-F-E-G, G-D-F-E-G, G-D-F-E-G for 10 seconds, and would play GDFEG, GDFEG, GDFEG, GDFEG, GDFEG for 10 seconds. Then they would take a 10 second pause, arrest. They would just take a space or a period of time where they do nothing for 10 seconds. Then they would go back to GDFEG, GDFEG. The two conditions, essentially, where to have people practice continually, lots of repetitions,
Starting point is 00:04:46 or to inject or insert these periods of 10 seconds idle time, where they're not doing anything, they're not looking at their phone, they're not focusing on anything, they're just letting their mind drift wherever it wants to go, and they are not touching the keys on the keyboard. What they found was that the rates of learning, the skill acquisition and the retention of the skill was significantly faster when they injected these short periods of rest, these 10 second rest periods.
Starting point is 00:05:14 And the rates of learning were, when I say significantly faster, were much, much faster. I'll reveal what that was in just a moment. But you might ask, why would this work? Why would it be that injecting these 10 second rest periods would enhance rates of learning? What they called them was micro offline gains because they're sort of taking their brain offline from the learning task for a moment. Well, it turns out the brain isn't going offline at all. You've probably heard of the hippocampus, the area of the brain involved in memory, and the neocortex, the area of the brain that's involved in processing sensory information.
Starting point is 00:05:48 Well, it turns out that during these brief periods of rest, these 10 second rest periods, the hippocampus and the cortex are active in ways such that you get a 20 times repeat of the GDFEG. It's a temporal compression, as they say. So basically the rehearsal continues while you rest, but at 20 times the speed. So if you were normally getting just, let's just say five repetitions of GDFEG, GDFEG,
Starting point is 00:06:19 GDFEG per 10 seconds, now you multiply that times 20. In the rest periods, you've practiced it a hundred times. Your brain has practiced it. We know this because they were doing brain imaging, functional imaging of these people, with brain scanners while they were doing this. This is an absolutely staggering effect. And it's one that, believe it or not, has been hypothesized or thought to exist for a very
Starting point is 00:06:43 long time. This effect is called the spacing effect and it was actually first proposed by Ebington in 1885. And since then, it's been demonstrated for a huge number of different what they call domains in the cognitive domain, so for learning languages, for in the physical domains, so for learning skills that involve a motor sequence. It's been demonstrated for a huge number
Starting point is 00:07:06 of different categories of learning. If you want to learn all about the spacing effect and the categories of learning that it can impact, there's a wonderful review article. I'll provide a link to it. The title of the review article is parallels between spacing effects during behavioral and cellular learning. What that review really does is it ties the behavioral learning and the improvement of skill to the underlying changes in neurons that can
Starting point is 00:07:33 explain that learning. I should mention that the paper that I'm referring to, the more recent paper that injects these 10-second little micro offline games, rest periods, is the work of the laboratory of Leonard Cohen, not the musician Leonard Cohen. He passed away. He was not a neuroscientist, a wonderful poet and musician, but not a neuroscientist. Again, the paper was published in cell reports and we will provide a link to the
Starting point is 00:07:58 full paper as well. So the takeaway is if you're trying to learn something, you need to get those reps in but one way that you can get 20 times the number of reps in is by injecting these little 10 second periods of doing nothing. Again, during those rest periods you really don't want to attend to anything else as much as possible. You could close your eyes if you want or you can just simply wait and then get right back
Starting point is 00:08:22 into generating repetitions. I find these papers that so reports and other journals have been publishing recently to be fascinating because they're really helping us understand what are the best protocols for learning anything. And they really leverage the fact that the brain is willing to generate repetitions for us, provided that we give it the rest that it needs. So inject rest throughout the learning period and if you can, based on the scientific data,
Starting point is 00:08:48 you would also wanna take a 20 minute nap or a 20 minute decompress period where you're not doing anything after a period of learning. I think those could both synergize in order to enhance learning even further, although that hasn't been looked at yet. Before we begin talking about hearing and balance, I just want to mention that this podcast is separate from my teaching and research roles at Stanford.
Starting point is 00:09:10 It is, however, part of my desire and effort to bring zero cost to consumer information about science and science-related tools to the general public. In keeping with that theme, I want to thank the sponsors of today's podcast and make it clear that we only work with sponsors whose products we absolutely love and that we think you will benefit from as well. Our first sponsor is Athletic Greens. Athletic Greens is an all-in-one vitamin mineral probiotic drink. I've been taking Athletic Greens since 2012, so I'm delighted that they're sponsoring
Starting point is 00:09:40 the podcast. The reason I started taking Athletic Greens and the reason I still take Athletic Greens once or twice a day is that it helps me cover all of my basic nutritional needs. It makes up for any deficiencies that I might have. In addition, it has probiotics, which are vital for microbiome health. I've done a couple of episodes now on the so-called gut microbiome
Starting point is 00:09:59 and the ways in which the microbiome interacts with your immune system, with your brain to regulate mood, and essentially with every biological system relevant to health throughout your brain and body. With athletic greens, I get the vitamins I need, the minerals I need, and the probiotics to support my microbiome. If you'd like to try athletic greens, you can go to atlettagreens.com slash Huberman and claim a special offer. They'll give you five free travel packs plus a year supply of vitamin D3K2.
Starting point is 00:10:26 There are a ton of data now showing that vitamin D3 is essential for various aspects of our brain and body health, even if we're getting a lot of sunshine. Many of us are still deficient in vitamin D3. And K2 is also important because it regulates things like cardiovascular function, calcium in the body, and so on. Again, go to at letitgreens.com slash Uberman to claim the special offer of the
Starting point is 00:10:46 five free travel packs and the year supply of vitamin D3 K2. Today's episode is also brought to us by Element. Element is an electrolyte drink that has everything you need and nothing you don't. That means the exact ratios of electrolytes are an element and those are sodium, magnesium, and potassium, but it has no sugar.
Starting point is 00:11:04 I've talked many times before on this podcast about the key role of hydration and electrolytes for nerve cell function, neuron function, as well as the function of all the cells and all the tissues and organ systems of the body. If we have sodium, magnesium, and potassium present in the proper ratios, all of those cells function properly and all our bodily systems can be optimized. If the electrolytes are not present and if hydration is low, we simply can't think as well as we would otherwise. Our mood is off, hormone systems go off,
Starting point is 00:11:35 our ability to get into physical action, to engage in endurance and strength, and all sorts of other things is diminished. So with element, you can make sure that you're staying on top of your hydration and that you're getting the proper ratios of electrolytes. If you'd like to try element, you can go to drink element that's LMNT.com slash Huberman and you'll get a free element sample pack with your purchase. They're all delicious. So again, if you want to try element, you can
Starting point is 00:11:57 go to element LMNT.com slash Huberman. Can you hear me? Can you hear me? Okay, well, if you can hear me, that's amazing because what it means is that my voice is causing little tiny changes in the air waves wherever you happen to be and that your ears and whatever's contained in those ears and in your brain can take those sound waves and make sense of them. And that is an absolutely fantastic and staggering feat of biology. And yet we understand a lot about how that process works. So I'm going to teach it to you now in simple terms over the next few minutes. So what we call ears have a technical name.
Starting point is 00:12:40 That technical name is oracles but more often they're called pinna. The pinna is P-I-N-N-A, pinna. And the pinna of your ears, this outer part that is made of cartilage and stuff, is a range such that it can capture sound in the best way for your head size. We're going to talk about ear size also because it turns out that your ears change size across the lifespan and That how big your ears are or rather how fast your ears are changing size is a pretty good indication of how fast you're aging So we'll get to that in a few minutes But I want to talk about these things that we call ears and some of the stuff contained within them that allow us to hear
Starting point is 00:13:25 So the shape of these ears that we call ears and some of the stuff contained within them that allow us to hear. So the shape of these ears that we have is such that it amplifies high frequency sounds. High frequency sounds is the name suggests or the squeakier stuff, right? So low frequency sound, cost yellow snoring in the background, that's a low frequency sound, or high frequency sound. Okay. So we have low frequency sounds and high frequency sounds and high frequency sound. Okay, so we have low frequency sounds and high frequency sounds and everything in between. Now those sound waves get captured by our ears and those sound waves, for those of you that don't maybe fully conceptualize sound waves,
Starting point is 00:13:56 are literally just fluctuations or shifts in the way that air is moving toward your ear and through space. In the same way that water can have waves, the air can have waves, okay? So it's reverberation of air. Those come in through your ears and you have what's called your ear drum. And on the inside of your ear drum, there's a little bony thing that's shaped like a little hammer. So attached to that ear drum, which can move back and forth like a drum, it's like a little
Starting point is 00:14:30 membrane, you've got this hammer attached to it. And that hammer has three parts. For those of you that want to know, those three parts are called the Malius, Incus, and Stapis. It's like it, but basically you can just think about it as a hammer. So you've got this earardrum and then a hammer. And then that hammer has to hammer on something. And what it does is it hammers on a little coiled piece of tissue that we call the cochlea,
Starting point is 00:14:54 sometimes called the cochlea, depending on where somebody lives in the country. So typically in the Midwest on the East Coast, they call them cochlea. And on the west coast we call them Cochlear. Same thing. Okay? So this snail-shaped structure in your inner ear is where sound gets converted into electrical signals that the brain can understand. But I want to just bring your attention to that little hammer because that little hammer is really, really cool. What it means is that sound waves come in through your ears, that's what's happening right now. That ear drum that you have is like a, it's like the top of a
Starting point is 00:15:28 drum. It's like a membrane or it can move back and forth. It's not super rigid and it moves that little hammer and then the hammer goes, do do do do do and hits this coil shaped thing that we're calling the cochlea. Okay. Now the cochlea at one end is more rigid than the other. So one part can move really easily, and the other part doesn't move very easily. And that turns out to be very important for decoding or separating sounds that are low frequency, like cost-ello-storing, and sounds
Starting point is 00:16:03 that are of high frequency, like a shriek or a shrill. And that's because within that little coiled thing we call the cochlea, you have all these tiny little, what are called hair cells. Now, they look like hairs, but they're not at all related to the hairs on your head or elsewhere on your body. There's your shaped-like hairs. We call them hair cells. Those hair cells, if they move, send signals into the brain that a particular sound is in our environment. And if those hair cells don't move, it means that particular sound is not in our environment. Okay? So, just to give you the mental picture of this, sound waves are coming in because there's stuff out there making noises like my voice. It's changing the patterns of air
Starting point is 00:16:52 around you in very, very subtle ways. That information is getting funneled into your ears because your pinnas are shaped in a particular way. The eardrum then moves this little hammer and the hammer bangs on this little snail-shaped thing. And because that snail-shaped thing at one end is very rigid, it doesn't want to move. And at the other end, it's very flexible. It can separate out high frequency and low frequency sounds. And the fact that this thing in your inner ear that we call the cochlea is coiled is actually really important to understand. Because along its length, it varies in how rigid or flexible it is. I already mentioned that before. And at the base, it's very rigid.
Starting point is 00:17:37 And that's where the hair cells, if they move, will make high frequency sounds. And at the top, what's called the apex, it's very flexible, and it's more like a bass drum. So basically, what happens is sound waves come into your ears, and then at one end of this thing that we call the cochlea, at the top, it's essentially encoding or only responding to sounds are like, boom, boom, boom, boom, boom, whereas at the bottom, it responds to high frequency sounds, like a symbol, and everywhere in between, we have other frequencies, medium frequencies.
Starting point is 00:18:17 Now, this should stagger your mind. If it doesn't already, it should. Because what this means is that everything that's happening around us, whether or not it's music or voices or crying or screaming or screaming of delight from small children who are excited because they're playing or because they get cake, all of that is being broken down into its component parts. And then your brain is making sense of what it means. These things that I've been talking about, like the pin of your ears and this little hammer
Starting point is 00:18:49 and the cochlea, that's all purely mechanical. It has no mind of its own. It's just breaking things down into high frequencies, medium frequencies, and low frequencies. And if you don't understand sound frequency, it's really simple to understand. Just imagine ripples on a pond, and if those ripples are very close together, that's really simple to understand. Just imagine ripples on a pond and if those ripples are very close together, that's high frequency. They are a correct high frequency. If those ripples are further apart, it's low frequency and obviously medium frequency is in between. So just like you can have waves in water, you can have waves in the air.
Starting point is 00:19:19 So that's really how it works. Now, we're all familiar with light and how if you take a prism and put it in front of light, it will split that light into its different wavelengths, it's different colors, red, green, blue, etc. Right? Sort of like the pink, Floyd dark side of the moon album I think has a prism and it's converting white light into all the colors, all the wavelengths that are contained in white light. Your cochlea essentially acts as a prism. It takes all the sound in your environment and it splits up those sounds into different frequencies. So you can think of the cochlea of your ear
Starting point is 00:19:58 sort of like a prism. And then the brain takes that information and puts it back together and makes sense of it. So those hair cells in each of your two cochlea, because you have two ears, you also have two cochlea, send little wires, what we call axons, that convey their patterns of activity into the brain. And there are a number of different stations within the brain that information arrives at. Before it gets up to the parts of your brain,
Starting point is 00:20:26 where you are consciously aware. And because some of you have asked for more names and no mink lecture, I'll give that to you. If you don't want a lot of detailed names, you can just ignore what I'm about to say. But basically the cochlea, send information to what's called the spiral ganglion. The spiral ganglion, a ganglion, by the way,
Starting point is 00:20:43 if you're going to learn any neuroscience, just know that anytime you hear ganglion, a ganglion, by the way, if you're going to learn any neuroscience, just know that anytime you hear ganglion, a ganglion is just a clump. So it means a bunch of neurons. So a clump of cells. So the spiral ganglion is a bunch of neurons that the information then goes off to what are called the cochlear nuclei in the brainstem. Brainstem is kind of down near your neck. Then up to a structure that has a really cool name called the superior olive because you have one on each side of your brain. And if I were to bring you to my lab and show you the superior olives in your brain or anyone else's brain, they look like little olives. They even have a little dividend in them that
Starting point is 00:21:17 to me looks like a pimento, but they just called them the superior olive. And then the neurons in the superior olive, then they send information up to what's called the inferior calliculus, only called inferior because it sits below a structure called the superior calliculus. And then the information goes up to what's called the medial geniculate nucleus and then up to your neocortex where you make sense of it all.
Starting point is 00:21:40 Now you don't have to remember all that, but you should know that there are a lot of stations in which auditory information is processed before it gets up to our conscious detection. And there is a good reason for that, which is that more important than knowing what you're hearing, you need to know where it's coming from. It's vital to our survival that if something for instance is falling toward us, that we know if it's coming to our right side, if it's going to hit us from behind, we have to know for instance of a car is coming at us from our left or from our right.
Starting point is 00:22:16 And our visual system can help with that, but our auditory and our visual system collaborate to help us find and locate the position of things in space. That should come as no surprise. If you hear somebody talking off to your right, you tend to turn to your right, not to your left. If you see somebody's mouth moving in front of you, you tend to assume that the sound is going to come from right in front of you. Disruptions in this auditory hearing and visual matching are actually the basis of what's called the ventral-equism effect, which we'll talk about in a few minutes in more depth, but the ventral-equism effect can basically be described in simple terms as when you essentially think that a sound is coming from a location that it's not actually
Starting point is 00:23:04 coming from. We'll talk about that in a moment, but what I'd like you to realize is that one of these stations, deep in your brainstem, is responsible for helping you identify where sounds are coming from through a process that's called interoral time differences, and that sounds fancy, but really the way you know where things are coming from, what direction a car or a bus or a person is coming from is because the sound lands in one ear before the other. And you have stations in your brain, meaning you have neurons in your brain, that calculate the difference in time of arrival for those sound waves in your right versus your left
Starting point is 00:23:43 ear. And if they arrive at the same time, you assume that thing is making noise right in front of you. If it's off to your right, you assume it's over on your right. And if the sound arrives first to your left ear, you assume quite correctly that the thing is coming toward your left ear. So it's a very simple and kind of mechanical system at the level of sound localization. But what about up and down? If you think about it, a sound coming from above is going to land on your right ear
Starting point is 00:24:11 and your left ear at the same time. A sound from below is going to land on your right ear and your left ear at the same time. So the way that we know where things are in terms of what's called elevation, where they are in the up and down plane is by the frequencies. The shape of your ears actually modifies the sound depending on whether or not it's coming
Starting point is 00:24:35 straight at you from the floor or from high above. And so already at the level of your ears, you are taking information about the outside world and determining where that information is coming from. Now this all happens very, very fast and subconscious, but now you know why, if people really wanna hear something, they make a cup around their ear. They essentially make their ear into more of a phoenix-fox type ear, if you've ever seen those cute little fennec fox things,
Starting point is 00:25:07 they have these big, spiky ears. They kind of look like a French bulldog, although they're kind of the fox version of the French bulldog, these big, tall ears, and they have excellent sound localization. And so when people lean in with their ear, like with their hand like this, if you're listening to this,
Starting point is 00:25:22 I'm just cupping my hand at my ear, I'm giving myself a bigger pinna. Oh yeah, and if I do it on the left side, I do this side. And if I really want to hear something, I do it on both sides. Okay, so this isn't just gesturing. This actually serves a mechanical role. And actually, if you want to hear where things are coming from with a much greater degree of accuracy, this can actually help because you're capturing sound waves
Starting point is 00:25:46 and funneling them better. It's really remarkable this whole system. So you've got these two ears, and because of the differences in the timing of when things arrive in those two years, as well as these differences in the frequencies of that certain things sound, or I should say, the differences in the frequencies that
Starting point is 00:26:05 arrive at your ears depending on whether or not the thing is above you or right in front of you or below you, you're able to make out where things are in space pretty well. So now you're probably starting to realize that these two things on the side of our head that we call ears are there for a lot more than hanging earrings on or for other aesthetic purposes or for putting sunglasses on top of, there are very powerful devices for allowing us to capture sound waves from our environment. Now I have a question for you, which is, can you move your ears? Turns out that unlike other animals, humans are not
Starting point is 00:26:45 terrifically good at moving their ears. Other animals can move their ears even independently. So Costello is pretty good at raising his ears, the two of them together. He can't really move his ears separately. Some dogs can do that really well. In fact, sight hounds and some scent hounds do that exquisitely well. Some animals like deer and other animals that really have a very acute hearing will put one ear down to a very particular angle and will tilt the other one and they will actually capture information about two distant sound making organisms.
Starting point is 00:27:24 Those could be hunters coming after them or other animals coming after them, they are very good at doing this. We're not so good at it, but about 60% of people it's thought can move their ears consciously without having to touch their ears. So can you do that?
Starting point is 00:27:39 Maybe you should try it. Ask someone to look at you and see whether or not you can do it. The typical distances that people can move it is usually no more than two or three millimeters. It's subtle, but can you flap your pinna with just using mental control? If you can, or if you can't, try looking all the way to your right or all the way to your left. Obviously, if you're driving a car or doing something or exercising, don't put yourself in danger right now. But if you move
Starting point is 00:28:07 your eyes all the way to your left, which I'm doing now, or all the way to my right, you might feel a little bit of a contraction of the muscles. It's a that control your movement. All right. Now I want to ask you this, can you raise one eyebrow? I'm not very good at it. I can do a little bit, but it's mostly by like cramping down my face on one side and I certainly can't raise my right eyebrow. I can only do my left eyebrow, trying to talk while I'm doing this,
Starting point is 00:28:34 so this is why it looks strange. People who can raise one eyebrow very easily, almost always can move their ears without having to touch them. It's Control by the same motor pathway and there does seem to be a small but statistically significant sex difference in the ability to move one's ears Typically men can do this more than women can although plenty of women can move their ears as well
Starting point is 00:29:08 Now if you think that is all a little strange or off topic, it's not because what we're really talking about here is a system of the brain, but also of the body of the musculature for localizing things in space. And so you might find it interesting to note that one of the things that we share very closely with other primates, with non-human primates, like macaque monkeys and chimpanzees, if you look at their ears, their ears are remarkably similar to our ears, or rather our ears are remarkably similar to their ears. The eyes of certain monkeys, like macaque monkeys, are remarkably similar to human eyes. This is one of the reasons why
Starting point is 00:29:47 if you look at a baby macaque monkey, it sort of has this unbelievably human element to it. But the ears of these primates is very similar to our ears, our ears, similar to their ears. If you're interested in ear movements and what they could mean mean and some of the things that ear movements correlate with in other aspects of our biology, there's a nice paper actually, a scientific paper.
Starting point is 00:30:13 The author's last name is Code, C-O-D-E. It was published in 1995. I'll give a reference to that. It's a review article that discusses some of the sex differences in ear movement control, as well as the relationship between ear movements and eye movements. And it's a pretty accessible paper. It's the one that I think any of you who are interested in this topic could parse fairly easily.
Starting point is 00:30:36 And there's some very interesting underlying biology and some theories as to why humans would have this so-called vestigial or ancient carryover of a system for moving our ears. Now, if ear movement seems strange, next I wanna talk about a different feature of your hearing and ears that's even stranger, but that has some really interesting implications for your biology. And I'm guessing that you've not heard of this.
Starting point is 00:31:10 What I'm about to describe are called autoacoustic emissions. And autoacoustic emissions, as the name suggests, are sounds that your ears make. Believe it or not, 70% of people Believe it or not, 70% of people make noises with their ears, but they don't actually detect them. Like I said, you've never heard of this. That's not what I mean. But what I do mean is that 70% of people's ears are making noise that's cast out of the ear. And these autoacoustic emissions actuallyhing and be detected by microphones. Sometimes
Starting point is 00:31:45 they can be detected by other people in the room if they have very good hearing. Now, it turns out that women, or I should be technical here, females who report as themselves as heterosexual have a higher frequency, not frequency of sound, but a higher frequency of autoacoustic emissions than do men who report themselves as heterosexual. Women who report themselves as homosexual or bisexual make fewer autoacoustic emissions than heterosexual women. These are data that come from Dennis McFadden's lab at the University of Texas, Austin. He actually discovered these what are called sexual dimorphisms and differences based on sexual orientation without looking for them.
Starting point is 00:32:32 He was studying hearing. He's an auditory scientist and people were coming into his laboratory and they were detecting these autoacoustic emissions and they started to notice the group differences in autoacoustic emissions. So they started asking people about their sex and about their sexual orientation. And these differences fell out of the data, as we say. And it's interesting because autoacoustic emissions are not something that we associate with sex
Starting point is 00:32:59 or sexual dimorphism, but what these data really underscore is, first of all, a lot of us are making noises with our ears, some of us more than others, and that exposure to certain combinations of hormones during development are very likely shaping the way that are hearing aparaty, meaning the cochlea and the pina and all sorts of things, how those develop and how those function throughout the lifespan. We did do an episode on hormones and sexual development, which gets much deeper into the other effects that hormones have on the developing brain and body. If you want to check out that episode, we will put a link to it in the captions.
Starting point is 00:33:39 So now I want to shift to talking about ways to leverage your hearing system, your auditory system, so that you can learn anything, not just auditory information, but anything faster. I get a lot of questions about so-called BINORAL BEATS. BINORAL BEATS, as their name suggests, involve playing one frequency of sound to one ear and a different frequency of sound to the other ear. So it might be, do-do-do-do-do-do to your right ear and it might be, do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do-do- that bring information from the ears into the brain, eventually crossover. They actually share that information with both sides of the brain. That the brain will average that information and come up with a sort of intermediate frequency. And the rationale is that those intermediate frequencies place the brain into a state that is better for learning.
Starting point is 00:34:41 When I say better for learning, I want to be precise about what I mean. That could mean more focus for encoding or bringing the information in. As you have heard me say before, we have to be alert and focused in order to learn. There is no passive learning unless we're little tiny infants. So can Binaral Beats make us more focused? Can Binaral Beats allow us to relax more if we're anxious? I know some people, they go to the dentist and the dentist offers binaural beats as they drill into your teeth and give root canals and things of that sort, probably causing some anxiety just describing those things right now.
Starting point is 00:35:18 But those are available in several dentist, many dental practices. Their binaural beats have been thought to increase creativity, or at least have been proposed to increase creativity. So what are the scientific data say about binaural beats? There are a number of different apps out there that offer binaural beats, there are a number of different programs. I think you can also even just find these on YouTube and on the internet, but typically it's an app
Starting point is 00:35:42 and you'll program in a particular outcome that you want more focused, more creative, fall asleep, less anxious, etc. So what are the scientific data say? So believe it or not, the science on binarital beats is actually quite extensive and very precise. So sound waves are measured typically in hertz or kilohertz. I know many of you aren't familiar with thinking about things in hertz or kilohertz, but again, just remember those waves on a pond, those ripples on a pond, if they're close together, then they are of high frequency. And if they're far apart, then they are low frequency. So when you hear more hertz, what you're essentially hearing is higher frequency, right? And so if it's many more killer hurts,
Starting point is 00:36:25 then it's much higher frequency than if it's fewer hurts or killer hurts. And so you may have heard of these things as delta waves or theta waves or alpha waves or beta waves, et cetera. Delta waves would be big, slow waves, a low frequency. And indeed, there's quality evidence
Starting point is 00:36:42 from peer reviewed studies that are not sponsored by companies that make BINORLB apps that tell us that Delta waves like one to four hertz, so very low frequency sounds. Think Costello snoring can help in the transition to sleep and for staying asleep. And that theta rhythms, which are more like four to eight hertz, can bring the brain into a state of subtle sleep or meditation. So deeply relaxed, but not fully asleep. And then you can sort of ascend the staircase of findings here
Starting point is 00:37:16 so to speak and you'll find evidence that alpha waves eight to thirteen hertz can increase alertness to a moderate level. That's a great state for the brain to be in for recall of existing information. And that beta waves, 15 to 20 hertz, are great for bringing the brain into focused states for sustained thought or for incorporating new information, and especially gamma waves, the highest frequency, the most frequent ripples of sound. So to speak, 32 to 100 hertz for learning and problem solving. Now all of this matches where I should say maps onto what I've said before about learning
Starting point is 00:37:57 really nicely, which is that you need to be in a highly alert state in order to bring new information in in order to access a state of mind in which you can tell your brain or the brain is telling itself, okay, I need to learn this. This is why stress and unfortunate circumstances are so memorable is because our brain gets into a really high alert system. Here we're talking about the use of binaural beats in order to increase our level of alertness or our level of calmness. Now that's important to underscore
Starting point is 00:38:25 because it's not that there's something fundamentally important about the binaural beats. They are yet another way of bringing the brain into states of deep relaxation through low frequency sound, or highly alert states for focused learning with more high frequency sound. So they are effective, and I'll review a little
Starting point is 00:38:45 bit of the data in detail. They're effective, but it's not that they're uniquely special for learning. It's just that they can help some people bring their brain into the state that allows them to learn better. So there are a lot of studies that allowed us to arrive, or I should say allowed the field to arrive, on these parameters of slow, low frequency waves are going to bring you into relaxed states, high frequency waves, and to more alert states. There's very good evidence for anxiety reduction from the use of binaural beats. And what's interesting is the anxiety reduction seems to be most effective when the binaural beats are bringing the brain into delta,
Starting point is 00:39:34 so those slow big waves, like sleep, theta, and alpha states. And I'll link to a couple of these studies, although I will probably link more to the list that really segregates them out one by one, so you can see them all next to one another. There's good evidence that binaural beats can be used to treat pain, chronic pain. There's three studies in peer-reviewed journals which I took a look at and they seem to be of good quality, not sponsored research, as we say, not paid for by any specific company. Binary obese have been shown to modestly improve cognition, attention, working memory,
Starting point is 00:40:10 and even creativity. But the real boost from binary obese appears to be for anxiety reduction and pain reduction. Some people might find these beneficial for these oral surgeries, right? Believe it or not, there are people who would rather have the entire root canal or cavity drilled without Novakaine. And that's because they sometimes have a syringe phobia or something of that. Or they just don't like being numb from the Novakaine or maybe there's an underlying medical reason. But I think most people don't enjoy getting their teeth drilled, even if they have novocaine in there or a root canal. And so it seems that binaural beats can be effective
Starting point is 00:40:49 in that environment. And you don't have to go into that sort of extreme environment to benefit from binaural beats. Binaural beats are either relatively inexpensive thing to access. Most of the apps are pretty inexpensive. I don't have a favorite binaural beats app to recommend to access. Most of the apps are pretty inexpensive. I don't have a favorite BINORL BEATS app to recommend to you. I confess I did use BINORL BEATS a few years ago. I shifted over to other what I call NSTR non-sleep deep-rest protocols in favor of those. But many people like BINORL BEATS and say that they benefit from them, especially while studying or learning. I think part of the reason for that relates to the ability to channel our focus when we have some background noise.
Starting point is 00:41:30 And this is something I also get asked about a lot. Is it better to listen to music and have background noise when studying or is it better to have complete silence? Well, there's actually a quite good literature on this as well, but not so much as it relates to Bynoral Beats, but rather whether or not people are listening to music, so-called white noise, brown noise, believe it or not, there's white noise and there's brown noise, there's even pink noise, and how that impacts brain states that allow us to learn information better or not.
Starting point is 00:42:01 So now I'd like to talk about white noise. And I want to be very clear that white noise has been shown to really enhance brain states for learning in certain individuals in particular in adults. But white noise actually can have a detrimental effect on auditory learning and maybe even the development of the auditory system in very young children in particular in infants. So first I'd like to talk about the beneficial effects of white noise on learning. There are some really excellent studies on this.
Starting point is 00:42:34 The first one that I'd like to just highlight is one that's entitled, Low Intensity White Noise Improves Performance in Auditory Working Memory Task and FMRI study. This is a study that explored whether or not learning could be enhanced by playing white noise in the background, but the strength of the study is that they looked at some of the underlying neural circuitry and the activation of the neural circuitry and these people as they did the learning task. And what it essentially illustrates is that white noise provided that white noise is of low enough intensity, meaning not super loud, right?
Starting point is 00:43:14 Not imperceptible, so not so quiet that you can't hear it, but not super loud either. It actually could enhance learning to a significant degree. And this has been shown now for a huge number of different types of learning. There's a terrific article as well, is somewhat obscure, journal at least obscure to me, which is the effects of noise exposure on cognitive performance and brain activity patterns.
Starting point is 00:43:39 That's a study involving 54 subjects. They basically were evaluated for mental workload and attention under different levels of noise exposure, background noise, and different essentially loudness of noise. And the reason I like this study is that they looked at different levels of noise and types of noise, and they varied a number of different things as opposed to just doing a kind of two-condition either white noise or no white noise type thing. What they found again is that provided the white noise is not extremely loud, it could really enhance brain function for sake of learning any number of different kinds of information.
Starting point is 00:44:16 Now that's all great, but it really doesn't get to the kind of deeper guts of mechanism. As a neuroscientist, what I really want to see is not just that something has an effect, that's always nice, it's always nice to see in a nice peer reviewed study without any kind of commercial biases that there's an effect, okay, Bynorobites can enhance learning or listening to white noise, not too loud, can enhance learning, but you really want to understand mechanism
Starting point is 00:44:40 because once you understand mechanism, not only does it start to make sense, but you can also imagine ways in which you could develop better tools and protocols. So I was very relieved to find, or I should say excited to find, this study published in the Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. This is a 2014 paper, white noise improves learning by modulating activity in dopamine and ergic midbrain regions and the right superior temporal sulcus. Okay, I don't expect you to know what the right
Starting point is 00:45:09 superior temporal sulcus is. I don't expect you to know what the dopamine midbrain region is. But if you're like me, you probably took highlighted notice of the word dopamine, dopamine is a neuromodulator, meaning it's a chemical that's released in our brain and body, but mostly in our brain, that modulates, meaning controls the likelihood that certain brain areas will be active and other brain areas won't be active. Dopamine is associated with motivation, dopamine is associated with craving, motivation is
Starting point is 00:45:40 associated with all sorts of different things, including movement. But what this study so nicely shows is that white noise can really enhance the activity of neurons in what's called the substantioneigra VTA. The substantioneigra VTA is a very rich source of dopamine, and that's because it's very chalk-a-block full of dopamine neurons. It's an area of the brain that is perhaps the richest source of dopamine neurons. And you actually can see this brain region under the microscope. If you take a slice of brain or you look at a brain without even staining it for any proteins or dopamine or anything, it's two very dark regions at the kind of bottom of the brain.
Starting point is 00:46:24 And the reason it's called substantial nigra, nigra meaning dark, is because the dopamine neurons actually make something that makes those neurons dark. You've got these two regions down there that contain dopamine and can release dopamine and essentially activate other brain regions and activate our sense of motivation and activate our sense of desire to continue focusing and learning. But you can't just snap your fingers and make them release dopamine.
Starting point is 00:46:51 You actually have to trigger dopamine release from them. Now that trigger can be caused by being very excited about something or the fact that that thing gave you a lot of pleasure in the past or you're highly motivated by fear or desire. But what's so interesting to me is that it appears that white noise itself can raise what we call the basal, the baseline levels of dopamine that are being released from this area, the substantioneigra. So now we're starting to get a more full picture of how particular sounds in our environment can increase learning and that's in part, I believe, through the release of dopamine from substantial nigro.
Starting point is 00:47:33 So I'm not trying to shift you away from binaural beats if that's your thing, but it does appear that turning on white noise at a low level, not too loud. You may say, well, how loud, and I'll tell you in a moment, but not too loud can allow you to learn better because of the ways that it's modulating your brain chemistry. So how loud or how soft should that white noise be while you learn? Well, in these studies, it seemed that white noise
Starting point is 00:48:01 that could be heard by the person, so it wasn't imperceptible to them, so it was loud enough that they could hear, but not so loud that they felt it was intrusive or irritating to them. Okay, so that's going to differ from person to person because people have different levels of auditory sensitivity. It's going to depend on age, going to depend on a number of different factors. So I can't tell you, you know, it turns to level two on your volume controller. That's just not going to work.
Starting point is 00:48:28 Also, I don't know how far you are from a given speaker in the room, or if you've got earphones in your head, or you've got speakers in the room, or if it's coming out of your computer, I don't know those things. So what you're going to have to do is adjust that white noise to a place where it's not interfering with your ability to focus, but rather it's enhancing your ability to focus. I think a good rule of thumb is going to be to put it probably in the lower third of any kind of volume dial, as opposed to in the upper, you know, upper third where it would really be blasting. And really blasting any noise, frankly, is not good, but that's especially not good,
Starting point is 00:49:07 meaning it's especially bad if you have headphones in. I do want to mention something about headphones before I talk about white noise in the developmental context and why it can be dangerous there. When you put headphones in your ears, it has this incredible effect of making the sounds like they come from inside your head, not from out in the room. And that might seem like kind of a duh, but that's actually really amazing, right? Your brain assumes that the sounds are coming from inside your head, as opposed from the environment that you're in, the moment you put headphones in.
Starting point is 00:49:42 So if you're listening to an audio book or maybe you're listening to this podcast with headphones, that's very different than when you're listening to something out in the room and there are other sounds, other sound waves, especially if you use these noise cancellation headphones. So if you're going to use white noise to enhance studying or learning of any kind, or this also could be for skill learning, motor skill learning while you're exercising, my suggestion would be that if you're using headphones to keep it quite low, right? This is an effect on the mid-brain dopamine neurons. That's a background effect of raising the baseline of dopamine release.
Starting point is 00:50:15 The way that dopamine neurons fires, they're always firing. Yours are firing right now, so are mine. When something exciting happens, they fire a lot. When something disappointing happens, that firing, the release of dopamine goes down below baseline. What you're talking about here is raising your overall levels of attention and motivation, which translates to better learning,
Starting point is 00:50:32 by just tickling those neurons a little bit, raising the baseline firing. Okay, so this isn't, you're not turning up the white noise to the point where you're feeling amazing. This isn't like turning on your favorite song. This is actually the opposite. This is about getting that baseline up just a bit. Okay. So I recommend turning it, the volume up just a bit so that you can focus entirely on the task that you're trying to do.
Starting point is 00:50:56 And of course you've turned on white noise so your attention might drift to that for a moment. Is it too loud? Is it too soft? If you can disappear into the work, so to speak, if your attention can disappear into the work, so to speak, if your attention can disappear into the work, then that's probably sufficiently quiet. And for those of you that say, while I like really loud music, and if I just blast the music, then I forget about the music.
Starting point is 00:51:15 I don't suggest blasting music. And this is coming from somebody who really likes loud music. You know, I grew up with kind of a loud fast rules mentality, and if you don't know what loud fast rules means, um, then I can't help you. But you, it, there's a time in a place, perhaps to listen to music loud, but especially with headphones, you can trigger head, you can trigger, excuse me, hearing loss quite rapidly. And unfortunately, because these hair cells that we talked about earlier,
Starting point is 00:51:43 our central nervous system neurons, they do not regenerate. They do not come back. Now along the lines of hearing loss, I should just say that the best way to blow out your hearing for good, to eliminate your hearing, is to have very loud sounds superimposed on a loud environment. So loud environments can cause hearing loss over time. So if you work at a construction site, clanging really loud, or if you work the sound board
Starting point is 00:52:11 at a club or something, you are headed towards hearing loss unless you protect your hearing with ear plugs and headphones. Nowadays, some of the ear plugs are very low profile, meaning you can't see them. So that's kind of nice. So you're not like when I was like, you didn't want to be the door to go to the concert with the ear plugs, but turns out those dorks were smarter than everybody else because they're not the ones who are,
Starting point is 00:52:32 you know, craning their neck to try and hear trivial things. At the age of, you know, 30 or so, because they blew out their hearing. So if you are in working in a loud environment or you expose yourselves to a loud environment, you really want to avoid big inflections and sound above that. So loud environment plus fireworks, loud environment plus gunshot, loud and loud environments, plus very high frequency and tensile sound, that's what we call the two-hit model. When you, this is also true for concussion, that you can take a kind of a stimulus that normally would be below the threshold of injury. You add some another stimulus at the same time that would be below the threshold of injury
Starting point is 00:53:15 and then suddenly you killed the neurons. So I don't want to make people paranoid, but you do want to protect your hearing. It's no fun to lose your hearing. If you're going to use headphones and you feel like you want to crank it up all the way, just remember that the more that you can get out of a lower volume, meaning the longer that you can go listening to things that at lower volume, the longer you'll be able to hear that music or that thing. So again, I'm not the hearing cop that's not my job, but as somebody who's lost some of his high frequency hearing, I can tell you, it's not a pleasure.
Starting point is 00:53:51 The old argument that it helps you not have to hear or listen to people that you don't want to listen to, that doesn't really work. They just send you text messages instead. So what about white noise and hearing loss in development? I know a lot of people with children have these noise machines like sound waves and things like that that help the kids sleep. And look, I think kids getting good sleep and parents getting good sleep is vital to physical and mental health and family health.
Starting point is 00:54:18 So I certainly sympathize with those needs. However, there are data that indicate that white noise during development can be detrimental to the auditory system. I don't want to frighten any parents. If you played white noise to your kids, this doesn't mean that their auditory system or their speech patterns are going to be disrupted or that their interpretation of speech is going to be disrupted forever.
Starting point is 00:54:42 But there are data published in the journal Science, the science being one of the three Apex journal science nature cell, the most stringent journals. Data published in the journal Science some years ago, actually by a scientist who I know quite well, his name is Edward Chang, he's a medical doctor now, he's a neurosurgeon, he's actually the chair of neurosurgery at UCSF. And he runs a laboratory
Starting point is 00:55:05 where they study auditory learning, neuroplasticity, et cetera. He and his mentor at the time, Mike Merzenick, published a paper showing that if young animals, and this was in animal models, were exposed to white noise. So the very type of noise that I'm saying as a older person, so, and when I say older, I mean somebody who's in their late teens early 20s and older, could benefit from listening to that at a low level in the background for sake of learning. Well, they exposed very young animals to this white noise.
Starting point is 00:55:41 It actually disrupted the maps of the auditory world within the brain. Now, we haven't talked about these maps yet, but I want to take a moment and talk about them and explain this effect and what it might mean for you if you have kids or if you were exposed to a lot of white noise early on. So auditory information goes up into our cortex into the, essentially, the outside portion of our brain that's responsible for all of our, all of our higher level cognition, our planning, our decision making, et cetera, creativity. And up there we have what are called tonotopic maps.
Starting point is 00:56:16 What's a tonotopic map? Well, remember the cochlea? How it's coiled and at one end it responds to high frequencies and the other end it responds to low frequencies and the other end it responds to low frequencies. Sort of like a piano. The keys sound different as you extend down and up the piano keys and it's organized in a very systematic way, right? It's not all intermixed high frequencies and low frequencies.
Starting point is 00:56:35 It's organized in a very systematic way from one end to the other. Your visual system is in what's called a retinatopic map. So neighboring points in space off to my right, like my two fingers off to my right, are mapped to neighboring points in space in my brain. And the space right in front of me is mapped to a different location in my brain, but it's systematic. It's regular.
Starting point is 00:56:58 It's not random. It's not salt and pepper. It goes from high to lower, from right to center to left. In the auditory system, we have what are called tonatopic maps. We're frequency, high frequency, to low frequency, and everything in between is organized in a very systematic way. Now, our experience of life from the time we're a baby until the time that we die is not systematic. We don't hear low frequencies at one part of the room or at one part of the day, and high frequencies is another part of the room and another part of the day.
Starting point is 00:57:28 They're all intermixed. But if you remember, the cochlea separates them out just like a prism of light separates out the different wavelengths of light, the cochlea separates out the different frequencies. And the developing brain takes those separated out frequencies and learns this relationship between itself, meaning the child and the outside world. White noise essentially contains no tonotopic information. The frequencies are all intermixed. It's just noise. Whereas when I speak, my voice voice has now I'm getting technical
Starting point is 00:58:05 But it has what's called a certain envelope meaning it has some low frequencies and some slightly high frequencies I can make my have voice higher although I'm not very good at that. My voice starts to crack and I can make my voice lower Although not as low as costello snore. So it has an envelope has a container White noise has no container. It's like all the colors of the rainbow whiter noise has no container. It's like all the colors of the rainbow spread out together, which is actually what you get when you get white light. White noise is analogous to white light. So one of the reasons why hearing a lot of white noise during development for long periods of time can be detrimental to the development of the auditory system is that these tonotopic maps don't form normally, at least they don't in experimental animals.
Starting point is 00:58:48 Now, the reason I'm raising this is that many people I know, in particular friends who have small children, they say, I want to use a white noise machine while I sleep, but is it okay for my baby to use a white noise machine? And I consulted with various people, scientists about this, and they said, well, you know, the baby is also hearing the parents voices and is hearing music and is hearing the dog bark. So it's not the only thing they're hearing,
Starting point is 00:59:17 however, every single person that I consulted with said, but you know, there's neuroplasticity during sleep. That's when the kid is sleeping. And I don't know that you'd want to expose a child to white noise the entire night because it might degrade that tonotopic map. It might not destroy it. It might not eliminate it, but it could make it a little less clear. Like sort of taking the keys on the piano and taping a few of them together, right?
Starting point is 00:59:46 So you still got the highs and lows in the appropriate order and everything in between, but when you tap the keys together, you don't get the same fidelity, you don't get the same precision of the noise that comes out of that piano. So I'm, I don't, again, I don't want to scare anybody, but I would say if you are in a position to make the choice
Starting point is 01:00:04 of either using white noise or something similar, pink noise is just a kind of variation. It's got a little bit more of a certain frequency, just like pink light, has a little bit more of a certain wavelength and white light. If you are in a position to make choices about things to put in a young, especially very young child sleeping environment, white
Starting point is 01:00:25 noise might be something to consider avoiding. Again, I'm not telling you what to do, but it's something to perhaps consider avoiding. I don't think most pediatricians are going to be aware of these data, but if you talk to any auditory physiologist or an audiologist or somebody who studies auditory development, I'm fairly certain that they would have opinions about that. Now, whether or not their opinions agree with mine and these folks that I consulted with or not is a separate matter. I don't know because I don't know them.
Starting point is 01:00:54 But it's something that I felt was important enough to cue you to, especially since I've highlighted, excuse me, the opposite effect is true in adulthood. Once your auditory system has formed, once it's established these tonotopic maps, then the presence of background white noise should not be a problem at all. In fact, it shouldn't be a problem at all because you're also not attending to it.
Starting point is 01:01:16 The idea is that it's playing at a low enough volume that you kind of forget it in the background and that it's supporting learning by bringing your brain into a heightened state of alertness and especially this heightened state of dopamine, dopamine-ergic activation of the brain, which will make it easier to learn faster and easier to learn the information. So now I want to talk about auditory learning and actually how you can get better at learning information that you hear, not just information that you see on a page or motor skill learning.
Starting point is 01:01:44 There are a lot of reasons to want to do this. not just information that you see on a page or motor skill learning. There are a lot of reasons to want to do this. A lot of classroom teaching, whether or not it's by Zoom or in person, is auditory in nature. Not everything is necessarily written down for us. It's also good to get better at listening, or so I'm told. So there's a phenomenon called the cocktail party effect. Now, even if you've never phenomenon called the cocktail party effect. Now even if you've never been to a cocktail party, you've experienced and participated in
Starting point is 01:02:09 what's called the cocktail party effect. The cocktail party effect is where you are in an environment that's rich with sound, many sound waves coming from many different sources, many different things. So in a city, in a classroom, in a car that contains people having various conversations. You somehow need to be able to attend to specific components of those sound waves, meaning you need to hear certain people and not others. The reason it's called the cocktail party effect is that you and meaning your brain are exquisitely good at creating a cone of auditory attention, a narrow band of attention
Starting point is 01:02:48 with which you can extract the information you care about and wipe away or erase all the rest. Now this takes work, it takes attention. One of the reasons why you might come home from a loud gathering, maybe a stadium, a sports event or a cocktail party for that matter. And feel just exhausted is because if you are listening to conversations there or trying to listen to those conversations while watching the game and people moving past you and hearing
Starting point is 01:03:15 all this noise, clinking of glasses, et cetera, it takes attentional effort. And the brain uses up a lot of energy just at rest, but it uses up even more energy when you are paying strong attention to something. Literally, caloric energy, burning up things like glucose, et cetera, even if you're ketogenic, it's burning up energy. So the cocktail party effect has been studied extensively in the field of neuroscience, and we now know at a mechanistic level how one accomplishes this feat of attending to certain sounds, despite the fact that we are being bombarded with all sorts of other
Starting point is 01:03:53 sounds. So there are a couple ways that we do this. First of all, much as with our visual system, we can expand or contract our visual field of view. So we can go from panoramic vision, see the entire scene that we are in by dilating our gaze. Talked a lot about this on this podcast and elsewhere. We can, for instance, keep our head and eyes stationary or mostly stationary. You don't have to be rigid about it. And you can expand your field of view so you can see the walls and ceiling and floor. You can see yourself in the environment.
Starting point is 01:04:24 That's panoramic view. That's what you would accomplish without having to try it all. If you went to a horizon, for instance, or we can contract our field of view, I can bring my focus to a particular location, what we call a virgins point directly in front of me. Now I'm pointing at the camera directly in front of me. Okay. We can do that. We can expand and contract our visual field of view.
Starting point is 01:04:43 Well, we can expand and contract our auditory field of view, so to speak, or our auditory window. You can try this next time you are in an environment that's rich with noise, meaning lots of different sounds. You can just tune out all the noise to a background chatter. You kind of just, you try not focus on any one particular sound and you get the background kind of chatter of noise. And you'll find that it's actually very relaxing in comparison to trying to listen to somebody
Starting point is 01:05:15 at a cocktail party and you're shouting back and forth. Now if you're very, very interested in that person, we're getting to know them better, or what they're telling you, or some combination of those things, then you'll be very motivated to do it, but nonetheless, it requires energy and effort and attention. How do we do this? Well, it's actually quite simple or at least it's simple in essence, although the underlying mechanisms are complex. Here I have to credit the laboratory of a guy named Mike Weir, W-E-H-R, up at the University of Oregon, who essentially figured out that we are able to accomplish this extraction
Starting point is 01:05:55 of particular sounds. We can really hear one person or a small number of people amidst a huge background of chatter because we pay attention to the onset of words, but also to the offset of words. Now, the way to visualize this is if the background noise is just like a bunch of waves of noise, it's literally just sound waves coming every frequency, low frequency, high frequency, glasses clinking together. If you had a game, people are shouting, people are talking on their phone. There's the crack of the ball. If somebody actually manages to hit the ball, the announcer, et cetera.
Starting point is 01:06:33 But whatever we're paying attention to, we set up a cone of auditory attention, a kind of a tunnel of auditory attention, where we are listening, although we don't realize that we are listening for the onset and the offset of those words. Now, this is powerful for a couple of reasons. First of all, it's a call to arm, so to speak, to disengage your auditory system when you don't need to focus your attention on something particular. So if you are somebody you're coming home from work, you've had a very long day,
Starting point is 01:07:06 and you're trying to make out a particular conversation on the background noise, you might consider just not having that conversation, just letting your auditory landscape be very broad, almost like panoramic vision. If you're trying to learn how to extract sound information, it could be notes of music, it could be scales of music, it could be words spoken by somebody else. Maybe somebody is telling you what you need to say for a particular speech, or the information
Starting point is 01:07:39 that you need to learn for a particular topic, and they're telling it to you. Deliberately paying attention both to the onset and to the offset of those words can be beneficial because it is exactly the way that the auditory system likes to bring in information. So one of the more common phenomena that I think we all experience is you go to a party and or you meet somebody new and you say, hi, I would say hi, I'm Andrew and they'd say hi, I'm Jeff, for instance, great, great to meet you. And then a minute later, I can't remember the guy's name.
Starting point is 01:08:13 Now is it because I don't care what his name is? No, somehow the presence of other auditorium information interfered. It's not that my mind was necessarily someplace else. It's that the signal to noise, as we say, wasn't high enough. Somehow, the way he said it, or the way it landed on my ears, which is really all that matters, right, when it comes down to learning, is such that it just didn't achieve high enough signal to noise. The noise was too high, or the signal was too low, or some combination of those. So the next time you ask somebody's name, remember, listen to the onset of what they say and the
Starting point is 01:08:49 offset. So it would be paying attention to the Jeff in Jeff and it would be paying attention to that in F in Jeff. Excuse me. All right. And chances are you'll be able to remember that name. Now, I don't know if people who are super learners of names do this naturally or not. I don't have access to their brains. I don't think they're going to give me access to their brains either. But it's a very interesting way to take the natural biology of auditory attention and learning and apply it to scenarios where you're trying to remember either people's names or specific information. Now, I do acknowledge that trying to learn every word and ascentence by paying attention to its onset and
Starting point is 01:09:30 offset could actually be kind of disruptive to the learning process. So this would be more for specific attention, like you're asking directions in a city and somebody says, okay, you know, you say you're lost and they say, okay, you're going to go two blocks down, you're going to turn left, and then you're going to see a landmark on your right, and then you're going to go in the third door on your left. That's a lot of information, at least for me.
Starting point is 01:09:54 Okay? So, the way you would want to listen to that is you're going to go down the road. I see, I already forgot. You're going to go left, and you're just going to program. And instead of just hearing the word left, you're going to think the L at the front of left and the T. You're going to left. Okay. And then, so you're coding in specific words.
Starting point is 01:10:14 And what this does is this kind of hijacks these naturally occurring attention mechanisms that the auditory system likes to use. It's a little bit of data that for auditory encoding, this kind of thing can be beneficial. There are a lot of data that attention for auditory coding is beneficial. There are a little bit of data showing that deliberately encoding auditory information this way, meaning trying to learn auditory information this way
Starting point is 01:10:41 can be beneficial or can accelerate learning. And some of these features of what I'm describing here map onto some of the work of Mike Merzenick and others that have been designed to try and overcome things like stutter and to treat various forms of auditory learning disorders. But more importantly and perhaps more powerful orders. But more importantly and perhaps more powerful is the work of Mike Merzenick that was done with his then graduate student Greg Reckon's own that showed that using the attentional system, we can actually learn much faster and we can actually activate neuroplasticity in the adult brain, something that's very challenging to do, and that the auditory system is one of the main ways in which we can access neuroplasticity
Starting point is 01:11:30 more broadly. So I just want to take a couple of minutes and describe the work of Reconzone and Merzenick because it's absolutely fantastic and fascinating. What they did is they had subjects try to learn auditory information, except that they told them to pay attention to particular frequencies. So now you know what frequencies are. So essentially, high pitch sounds or low pitch sounds. What they found was just passively listening to a bunch of stuff does not allow the brain
Starting point is 01:12:04 to change and for that stuff to be remembered at all That's not a surprise. We've all experienced the you know the phenomenon of having someone talk and we see their mouth moving We're like yeah, this is really important. This is really important. We're listening We're trying to listen and then they walk away and we think I didn't get any of that and you wonder whether or not it was them Maybe this is happening you right now. May or you wonder whether or not it was them, maybe this is happening to you right now. You wonder whether or not it was you, you wonder whether or not you have trouble with learning or you have a tension deficit. It could be any number of different things.
Starting point is 01:12:32 But what Reckonsone and Merzenick discovered was that if you instruct subjects to listen for particular cues within speech or within sounds, that not only can you learn those things more quickly, but that you can remap these tonotopic maps in the cortex that I referred to earlier. You actually get changes in the neural architecture, the neural circuitry in the brain, and this can occur not only very rapidly, but they can occur in the adult brain, which prior to their work was not thought to be amenable to change.
Starting point is 01:13:06 It was long thought that neuroplasticity could only occur in the developing brain, but the work of Reckon's own and Merzenick in the auditory system actually was some of the first that really opened up everybody's eyes and ears to the idea that the brain can change in adulthood. So here's how this sort of process would work and how you might apply it. If you are trying to learn music or you're trying to learn information that you're going to then recite, you can decide to highlight certain words
Starting point is 01:13:39 or certain frequencies of sound or certain scales or certain keys on the piano and to only focus on those for certain learning bouts. Okay, so I'll give an example that sort of real time for me, meaning it's happening right now. I know generally what I want to say when I arrive here. I even know specifically certain things that I want to make sure get across to you. But I don't think about every single word that I'm going to say and the precise order in which I'm going to say those things. That would be actually very disruptive because it wouldn't match my normal patterns of
Starting point is 01:14:16 speech. You'd probably think I was sounding rather robotic if I were to do that. So one way that we can remember information is, as we write out, for instance, something that we want to say, we can highlight particular words. We can underline those. If we're listening to somebody and they are telling us information, we can decide just to highlight particular words that they said to us and write those down. Now, of course, we're listening to all the information, but the work of Reconzone and Merzenick and the work of others, in addition to them, is
Starting point is 01:14:51 former student or former postdoc. I don't know which Michael Kilgard, who's now got his own lab down in Texas or others, has shown that the queuing of attention to particular features of speech, particular components of speech, the way in which it increases our level of attention overall allows us to capture more of the information overall. And so I don't want this to be abstract at all. What this means is when you're listening, you don't have to listen to every word. You're already listening to every word. All the information is coming in through your ears.
Starting point is 01:15:23 What you're trying to extract is particular things or themes within the content. So maybe you decide, if you're listening to me, that you're only going to listen to the word tools, or you're only going to listen to when my voice kind of goes above background. You get to decide what you decide to listen to or not. And what you decide to focus on isn't necessarily as important as the fact that you're focusing. So I hope that's clear. The auditory system does this all the time with the cocktail party effect.
Starting point is 01:15:53 What I'm talking about is exporting certain elements of the mechanisms of the cocktail party effect, paying attention to the onset and offset words, or particular notes within music, or particular scales, or you can make it even broader and particular motifs of music or particular sentences of words or particular phrases. And in doing that, you extract more of the information overall even though you're not paying attention to all the information at once. Now I'd like to talk about a phenomenon that you all experienced before, which is
Starting point is 01:16:25 called Doppler. So the Doppler effect is the way that we experience sound when the thing that's making that sound is moving. The simplest way to explain this is to translate the sound into the visual world once again. So if you've ever seen a duck or a goose sitting in a pond or a lake, and it's kind of bobbing up and down, what you'll notice is that the ripples of water that extend out from that duck or goose are fairly regularly spaced in all directions. And that's because that duck or goose is stationary. It's moving up and down, but it's not moving forward or backward or to the side. Now, if that ducker goose were to swim forward by paddling its little web feet under the surface, you would immediately notice that the
Starting point is 01:17:16 ripples of water that are close to and in front of that ducker goose would be closer together than the ones that trailed it that were behind. And that is essentially what happens with sound as well. With the Doppler effect, we experience sounds that are closer to us at higher frequency. The ripples are closer together and sounds that are further away at lower frequency, especially when they're moving past us.
Starting point is 01:17:44 So if you've ever, for instance, heard a siren in the distance, that's essentially my rendition of a siren. I don't know what ambulance or police or what passing you on a street. That is the Doppler effect. The Doppler effect is one of the main ways that we make out the direction that things are arriving from and their speeds and trajectories. And we get very good from a very young age at discerning what direction things are arriving from and the direction that they
Starting point is 01:18:25 are going to pass us in. And the Doppler, in fact, is probably saved your life many, many times. In this way, you just don't realize it because you'll step off the curb or you're driving your car and you pull to the side so that the ambulance or fire truck can go by because you heard that siren off in the distance. And then you pull away from the curb and you get back on the road in part because you don't see it any longer, but also you don't hear any other sirens in the distance. Now, some animals, such as bats, are exquisitely good and navigating their environments according to sound. Now, we've all heard that
Starting point is 01:19:03 bats don't see. That's actually not true. They actually have vision, but they just rely more heavily on their auditory system. And the way that bats navigate in the dark and the way that bats navigate using sound is through Doppler. Now they don't simply listen to whether or not things are coming at them or moving away from them and pay attention to the Doppler, like the siren example I gave for you. What they do is they generate their own sounds. So a bat as it flies around is sending out clicks. I think that's my best bat sound or maybe is. And they're clicking. They're actually propelling
Starting point is 01:19:43 sound out at a particular frequency that they know. Now, whether or not they're clicking, they're actually propelling sound out at a particular frequency that they know. Now, whether or not they're conscious of it, I don't know, I've never asked them and if I did ask them, I don't think they could answer and if they could answer, they couldn't answer in a language that I could understand. But the bat is essentially flying around, sending out sound waves, pinging its environment with sound waves of a particular frequency. And then depending on the frequency of sound waves that come back, they know if they're getting closer
Starting point is 01:20:08 to an object or further away from it. So if they send out sounds at a frequency of, this was much slower than it would actually occur, but let's say one every half second, and it's coming back even faster, then they know they're getting closer, right, because of the Doppler effect. And if it comes back even faster than they know they're getting closer, right, because of the Doppler effect.
Starting point is 01:20:27 And if it comes back more slowly, they know that there's nothing in front of them. So the bat is essentially navigating its world by creating these auras of sound that bounce back onto them from the various objects, trees, et cetera, buildings and people. It's going to e you to think about, but yes, they see you with the experience you with their sound, they sense you, and they're using Doppler to accomplish it. Now I'd like to talk about ringing in the ears. This is something that I get asked about a lot,
Starting point is 01:21:00 and speaking of signal to noise, I don't know if I get asked about it a lot because many people suffer from ringing in their ears or because the people who suffer from ringing in their ears suffer so much that they are more prone to ask. So it could be a sampling bias, I don't know, but I've been asked enough times and some of the experiences of discomfort that people have expressed about having this ringing of the ears really motivate me to go deep into this literature. So the ringing of the ears that one experiences is called tinnitus, not tinnitus, but tinnitus.
Starting point is 01:21:39 And tinnitus can vary in intensity and it can vary according to stress levels. It can vary across the lifespan or even time of day. So it's very subject to kind of background effects and contextual effects. So I think, you know, we all know that we should do our best to maximize healthy sleep. We did a number of episodes on that, essentially the first four episodes of the Huberman Lab podcast. We're all about sleep and how to get better sleep. We all know that we should try and limit our stress and we had an episode
Starting point is 01:22:10 about stress in ways to mitigate stress as well. However, there are people, it seems that are suffering from tinnitus for which stress or lack of sleep just can't explain the presence of the tinnitus. Tinnitus. Tinnitus can be caused by disruption to these hair cells that we talked about earlier or damage to the hair cells. So, that's another reason why, even if you have good hearing now, that you want to protect that hearing and really avoid putting yourself into these kind of two-hit environments,
Starting point is 01:22:43 environments where there's a lot of background noise, and then you add another really loud auditory stimulus. This also can happen at different times, I should mention. If you go to a concert or you listen to loud music with your headphones, and then you go to a concert or you go into a very loud work environment, the hair cells can still be vulnerable. And once those hair cells are knocked out, currently we don't have the technology to put them back. Although many groups, including some excellent groups at Stanford and elsewhere, too, of course, are working on ways to replenish those hair cells and restore hearing. There are treatments for tinnitus that involve taking certain substances, there are medications for tentatives. In the non-prescription landscape, which is typically what we discuss on this podcast, when we discuss taking anything, there are essentially four compounds for which there are quality peer review data, where there does not appear to be any overtvert commercial bias meaning that nothing is reported in the papers as you know funding from a particular company and those are
Starting point is 01:23:51 Melatonin Ginkgo Bilboa zinc and magnesium now I've talked about Melatonin before I'm personally not a fan of Melatonin as a sleep aid but there are four studies Melatonin as a sleep aid, but there are four studies. First one entitled the effects of melatonin on tinnitus. Tinnitus, excuse me, and sleep. Second one, treatment of central and sensory neural tinnitus with oral aid minister, melatonin. And then the title goes on much longer, but it's a randomized study.
Starting point is 01:24:20 I'm not gonna read out all of these melatonin, can it stop the ringing, which is an interesting article, double-blinded study, and the effects of melatonin on tinnitus. Each one of these studies has anywhere from 30 to more than 100 subjects, one case 102 subjects, both genders, as they list them out. Typically, it's listed as sex, not gender in studies, so it should say both sexes, but nonetheless. An age range anywhere from 30 years old all the way up to 65 plus.
Starting point is 01:24:53 I didn't see any studies of people younger than 30. All three focused on melatonin, not surprisingly, because of the titles. Looking at a range of dosages anywhere from three milligrams per day, which is sort of typical of many supplements for melatonin, still much higher than one would manufacture indulgiously through your own pineal gland, but three milligrams in these studies for a duration of anywhere from 30 days to much longer in some cases, six months. And all four of these studies found modest, yet still statistically significant effects
Starting point is 01:25:35 of taking melatonin by mouth, so it's orally administered melatonin, in reducing the severity of tinnitus. So that's compelling, at least to me. I, you know, it doesn't sound like a cure. And of course, as always, I'm not a physician, I'm a scientist, so I don't prescribe anything. I only profess things. I report to you the science. You have to decide if melatonin is right for you
Starting point is 01:26:00 if you have tinnitus. And certainly I say that both to protect myself, but also protect you. You're responsible for your health and well-being. And I'm not telling anyone to run out and start taking melatonin for tinnitus, but it does seem that it can have some effects in reducing its symptoms. Ginkgo boboa is an interesting compound. It's been prescribed for or recommended for many, many things. But there are a few studies. Again, double-blinded studies lasting
Starting point is 01:26:32 one to six months. Any one that has an impressive number of subjects, 978 subjects ranging from age 18 all the way up to 65, so on and so forth, that show not huge effects of Ginkgo, but as they quote, limited evidence suggests that if Tinnitus is a side effect of something else in particular cognitive decline, so age-related Tinnitus might be helped by Ginkgo Bilbo. I won't go through all the details of the zinc studies, but it seems that zinc supplementation at higher levels than are typical of most people's intake, so 50 milligrams per day, do appear to be able to reduce subjective symptoms of tinnitus in most of the people that took the supplemented zinc. There aren't a lot of studies on that, so I could only find one double-blinded study.
Starting point is 01:27:25 It lasted anywhere from one to six months, 41 subjects, both. Genders listed out again here, 45 to 64, and they saw a decrease in the severity of tinnus symptoms with 50 milligrams of elemental zinc supplementation. And then last but not least is the magnesium study. Again, only a single study. It's a phase two study looking at a fairly limited number of subjects, so only 19 subjects, taking 532 milligrams of elemental magnesium.
Starting point is 01:27:55 For those of you that take magnesium, there's magnesium and elemental magnesium and it's always translated on the bottle. But it was associated with a lessening of symptoms related to tinnitus. So for you, tinnitus suffers out there. You may already be aware of this. You may already be taking these things
Starting point is 01:28:12 and had no positive effects, meaning they didn't help, maybe not. I hope that you'll at least consider these, talk to your doctor about them. I do realize that tinnitus is extremely disruptive. I can't say I empathize because I don't from a place of experience that is because I don't have tinnitus, but for those of you that don't, including myself, you can imagine that hearing sounds of things that aren't there and the ringing in one's ears can be very disruptive.
Starting point is 01:28:39 And I think would be very disruptive and explains why people with tinnitus reach out so often with questions about how to alleviate that. And I hope this information was useful to you. I'd like to now talk about balance and our sense of balance, which is controlled by, believe it or not, our ears and things in our ears, as well as by our brain and elements of our spinal cord. But before I do that, I want to ask you another question, or rather I'd like to ask you to ask yourself a question and answer it, which is, how big are your ears? It turns out that the ears grow our entire life. In the early stage of our life, they grow more slowly, and then as we age, they grow more quickly.
Starting point is 01:29:27 You may have noticed if you have family members who are well into their 70s and 80s, and if you're fortunate into their 90s and maybe even hundreds, is that the ears of some of these individuals get very, very big relative to their previous ear sizes. It turns out that biological age can actually be measured according to ear size. Now, you have to take a few measurements, but there's a, believe it or not, there is a formula in the scientific literature. specific literature, if you know your ear circumference, so the distance around your ear, ears plural, presumably you have two, most people do, in millimeters.
Starting point is 01:30:14 So you're going to take the circumference of your ears and millimeters. How would you do this? All right. How would you do this? Maybe you take a string and you put it around your ear and then you measure the string. That's probably going to be easier than marching around your ear or somebody else's ear with a ruler and measuring a millimeter. So what's your ear circumference?
Starting point is 01:30:32 On the outside, don't go in on the divot or anything. You're just going around as if you're going to trace the closest fitting oval, assuming your ears are oval. Closest fitting oval that matches your ear circumference. Take that number in millimeters, subtract from it. Oh, excuse me, I should do this correctly. Do that for both ears. Add them together.
Starting point is 01:30:55 Add those numbers together, divide by two. Get the average for your two ears. Get your average ear circumference for the cluster of two ears. Then take that number in millimeters, subtract 88.1. And then whatever value that is, multiply it times 1.96. And that will tell you your biological age. Now, why in the world would this be accurate?
Starting point is 01:31:19 Well, as we age, there are changes in number of different biological pathways. One of those pathways is the pathways related to collagen synthesis. So, not only are our ears growing, but our noses are growing too. My nose seems to be growing a lot, but then again, I did sports where I get my nose broken, something I don't recommend. It's always point out you don't get a nose like mine doing yoga. But nonetheless my nose is still growing and my ears are still growing and I suspect as I get older, if I have the good fortune of living into my 80s and 90s, my ears are
Starting point is 01:31:54 going to continue to grow. A comparison between chronological age and biological age is something that's of really deep interest these days and the work of David Sinclair at Harvard Medical School and others, so-called Horvath clocks that people have developed have tapped into how the epigenome and the genome can give us some insight into our biological age and how that compares to our chronological age. Most of us know our chronological age because we know when we were born and we know where we are relative to that now.
Starting point is 01:32:26 But you can start to make a little chart if you like about your rates of your growth, your rates of your growth actually correlate pretty well with your rates of biological progression through this thing that we call life. So it's not something that we think about too often, but just like our DNA and our epichinome and some other markers of metabolic health and hormone health relate to our age. So does our collagen synthesis and one of the places that shows up the most is in these two little goodies on the sides of our heads, which are our ears. So even though it's a little bit of a bizarre metric, it makes perfect sense in the biological context.
Starting point is 01:33:06 So let's talk about balance and how to get better at balancing. The reason why we're talking about balance and how to get better at balancing in the episode about hearing is that all the goodies that are going to allow you to do that are in your ears. They're also in your brain, but they're mostly in your ears. So as you recall from the beginning of this episode, you have two cochlea, cochleas, that are one on each side of your head, and that's a little spiral snail-shaped thing that converts sound waves into electrical signals that the rest of your brain can understand. Right next to those, you have what are called semi-circular canals. The semi-circular canals can be best visualized
Starting point is 01:33:50 as thinking about three hula hoops with marbles in them. So imagine that you have a hula hoop and it's not filled with marbles all the way around. It's just got some marbles down there at the base. Okay, so if you were to move that hoop around, the marbles will move around. Shhh, shhh, shhh, shhh. Okay, you've got three of those,
Starting point is 01:34:11 and each one of those hula hoops has these marbles that can move around. One of those hula hoops is positioned vertically with respect to gravity. So it's basically parallel to your nose. It sits like this, if you're watching on a video, but basically it's upright. Another one of those hula hoops is basically
Starting point is 01:34:32 at a 90 degree angle to your nose. It's basically parallel to the floor if you're standing up right now if you're seated. Okay? And the other one is kind of tilted about 45 degrees in between those. Now why is the system there? Well, those marbles within each one of those hula hoops
Starting point is 01:34:54 can move around, but they'll only move around if your head moves in a particular way. And there are three planes or three ways that your head can move. Your head can move up and down like I'm nodding right now. So that's called pitch. It's pitching forward or pitching back. Okay. So it's a nod up and down. Or I can shake my head no side to side. That's called yaw. You pilots will be very familiar with this. Yaw, not yaw on yaw. And then there's roll tilting the head from side to side,
Starting point is 01:35:31 the way that a cute puppy might look at you from side to side, okay? Or that somebody doesn't really understand or believe what you're saying, they might tilt their head. Very common phenomenon. I mean, no, it does that to me, but they do that to each other. So, Pichyan roll are the movements of the head in each of the three major planes of motion, as we say. And each one of those causes those marbles
Starting point is 01:35:58 to move in one or two of the various hoops. Okay? So, if I move my head up and down when I nod, one of those hoolahoops, literally right now, the marbles are moving back and forth. They aren't actually marbles, by the way. These are little, little, kind of like little stones basically,
Starting point is 01:36:17 little calcium-like deposits. And when they roll back and forth, they deflect little hairs, little hair cells that aren't like the hair cells that we use for measuring sound waves. They're not too different, but they are different from them, not like the hairs on our heads, but they're basically rolling past these little hair cells and causing them to deflect. And when they deflect downward, the neurons, because hair cells are neurons, send information up to the brain. So if I move my head like this, there's a physical movement of these little stones
Starting point is 01:36:54 in this hula hoop, as I'm referring to it, but they deflect these hairs, send those hairs, which are neurons, those hair cells send information off to the brain. If I move ahead from side to side, different little stones move. If I roll my head, different stones move. This is an exquisite system that exists in all animals that have a jaw. So any fish that has a jaw has this system.
Starting point is 01:37:19 A puppy has this system. Any animal has a jaw has this so called balance system, which we call the vestibular system. Any animal has a jaw, has this so-called balance system, which we call the vestibular system. One of the more important things to know about the vestibular, the balance system, is that it works together with the visual system. Let's say I hear something off to my left and I swing my head over to the left to see what it is. There are two sources of information about where my head is relative to my body, and I need to know that. First of all, when I quickly move my head to the side, those little stones as I'm referring to them, I realize they're not actually stones, but as I'm referring to them, they quickly activate those hair cells in that one semi-circular canal,
Starting point is 01:38:03 and send a signal off to my brain that my head just moved to the side like this. Not that it went like this and pitched back or not, that it tilted, but it just moved to the side. But also visual information slid past my field of view. I didn't have to think about it, but just slid past my field of view. And when those two signals combine, my eyes then lock to a particular location. Now, if this is at all complicated, you can actually uncouple these things. It's very easy to do. You can do this right now. In fact, I'd like you to do this experiment if you're
Starting point is 01:38:37 not already doing something else that requires your attention. And certainly don't do this if you're driving. Okay. You're going to sit down and you're going to move your head to the left very slowly. You're going with your eyes open. So you're going to move it very, very slowly. The whole thing should take about five, six, maybe even 10 seconds to complete. Okay. I just did it. Now I'm going to do it very quickly. I'd like you to do it very quickly as well. Now do it slowly again. Okay, what you probably noticed is that it's very uncomfortable to do it slowly, but you can do it very quickly without much discomfort at all. You just move your head to the side.
Starting point is 01:39:24 The reason is when you move your head very slowly, those little stones at the base of that hulu, they don't get enough momentum to move. So you're actually not generating this signal to the brain that your head is moving. And what you'll notice is that your eyes have to go boom, boom, boom, jumping over step by step. Whereas if you move your head really quickly, the signal gets off to your brain and your eyes just go boom, right to the location you want to look at. So moving your body slowly is actually very disruptive to the, to the vestibular system. And it's very disruptive to your visual system.
Starting point is 01:40:01 Now, if you've ever had them as fortunate of being on a boat and you're going through big oscillations on the boat, for those of you C-sick, folks that get C-sick, this can actually make certain people C-sick just to hear about it. That was big oscillations going up and down and up and down. Those are very disruptive. I'll talk about nausea in a minute
Starting point is 01:40:19 and how to offset that kind of nausea. I get pretty C-sick, but there are ways that you can, you can deal with this. But this is incredible, because what it means is it's a purely physical system of these little stones rolling around in there and directing where your eyes should go. So you can do this also just by looking up. So let's just say you're sitting in a chair, you're going to look up towards the ceiling and your eyes will just go there. You're doing this eyes opening. You look down guys. Now try doing it really, really slowly. Some people even get motion sick doing this, which I, if you do, then just stop.
Starting point is 01:40:55 Okay. So you can do this also to the side, although it works best if you're moving your head from from side to side. It were nodding up and down. So what we're doing here is we're uncoupling these two mechanisms. We're pulling them apart, the visual part and the vestibular part, just to illustrate to you that normally these mechanisms in your inner ear
Starting point is 01:41:15 tell your eyes where to go. But as well, your eyes tell your balance system, your vestibular system, how to function. So I'd like you to do a different experiment. I'm not going to do it right now, but basically stand up. If you get the opportunity, you can do this safely wherever you are. You're going to stand up and you're going to look forward about 10, 12 feet. You know, pick a point on a wall or you can, you know, anywhere that you like.
Starting point is 01:41:40 If you're out in public, you know, just do it anyway, you know, just tell them you're listening to, to, to, to, to urban lab podcastsberman lab podcast and someone's telling you to do it. Anyway, if you don't want to do it, don't do it, but basically do it. Stand on one leg and lift up the other leg. You can bend your knee if you like and just look off into the distance about 10, 12 feet. If you can do that, if you can stand on one leg, now close your eyes. Chances are you're going to suddenly feel what scientists call postural sway. You're going to start swaying around a lot. It is very hard to balance with your eyes closed.
Starting point is 01:42:15 And I think, and if you think about that, it's like, why is that? That's crazy. Why would it be that it's hard to balance with your eyes closed? Well, information about the visual world also feeds back onto this vestibular system. So the vestibular system informs your vision and tells you where to move your eyes. And your eyes in their positioning tell your balance system, your vestibular system,
Starting point is 01:42:36 how it should function. So there's a really cool way that you can learn to optimize balance. You're not gonna try and do this by learning to balance with your eyes closed. What you can do is you can raise one leg and you can look at a short distance, maybe off to just the distance that your thumb would be if you were to reach your arm out in front of you. Although, you don't necessarily have to put your thumb in front of you. So maybe just about two feet in front of you. Then while still
Starting point is 01:43:01 balancing, you're going to step your vision out a further distance and then a further distance. As far as you can possibly see in the environment that you're in, and then you're going to march it back to you. Now, what the literature shows is that this kind of balance training where you incorporate the visual system and extending out and then marching back in the point at which you direct your visual focus, sends robust information about the relationship between your visual world and your balance
Starting point is 01:43:32 system. Of course, the balance system includes not just these hula hoops, these semi-circular canals, but they communicate with the cerebellum, this little so-called mini brain actually means mini brain, the back of your brain, combines that with visual information and your map of the body surface. That pattern of training is very beneficial for enhancing your ability to balance because the ability to balance is in part the activation of particular postural muscles,
Starting point is 01:44:02 but just as much, perhaps even more so, it's about being able to adjust those postural muscles, excuse me, it's about the ability to adjust those postural muscles as you experience changes in your visual world. And one of the most robust ways that you can engage changes in your visual world is through your own movement. And so most people are not trying to balance in place, right? They're not just trying to stand there like a statue on one leg. Most of what we think about when we think about balance is for sake of sport or dynamic balance of being able to break ourselves and when we're lunging in one particular direction to stop ourselves, that is, and then to move in another direction or for skateboarding
Starting point is 01:44:44 or surfing or cycling or any number of different things, gymnastics. So the visual system is the primary input by which you develop balance, but you can't do it just with the visual system. So what I'm recommending is if you're interested in cultivating a sense of balance, understand the relationship between these
Starting point is 01:45:01 semicircular canals, understand that they are both driving eye movements and they are driven by eye movements. It's a reciprocal relationship. And then even just two or three minutes a day, or every once in a while, even three times a week, maybe five minutes, maybe 10 minutes, you pick. But if you want to enhance balance, you have to combine changes in your visual environment
Starting point is 01:45:27 with a static posture standing on one leg and shifting your visual environment or static visual view, looking at one thing and changing your body posture. Okay, so those two things we now know from the scientific literature combine in order to give an enhanced sense of balance. And there's a really nice paper that was published in 2015 called Effects of Balance Training on Balance Performance. This was in healthy adults. It's a systematic review and a meta analysis. A meta analysis is when you combine a lot of literature from a lot of different papers and extract the really robust and the less robust statistical effects.
Starting point is 01:46:08 So it's a really nice paper as well. There are some papers out there, for instance, a comparison of static balance and the role of vision in the elite athletes. This is essentially the paper that I've extracted most of the information that I just gave you from. And that paper and
Starting point is 01:46:25 there are some others as well. But basically I distilled them down into their core components. The core components are move your vision around while staying static still, but in a balanced position like standing on one leg could be something more complicated if you're somebody who can do more complicated movements. Unilateral movement seem to be important. So standing on one leg is opposed to both, right? Or trying to generate some tilt is another way to go about it, or imbalance, meaning one limb asymmetrically
Starting point is 01:46:56 being activated compared to the other limb. And then the other way to encourage or to cultivate and build up this vestibular system in your sense of balance actually involves movement itself, acceleration. So that's what we're going to talk about now. So up until now, I've been talking about balance only in the static sense, like standing on one leg, for instance, but that's a very artificial situation. Even though you can train balance that way, most people who want to enhance their sense
Starting point is 01:47:23 of balance for sport or dance or some other endeavor want to engage balance in a dynamic way, meaning moving through lots of different planes of movement, maybe even sometimes while squatting down low or jumping and landing or making trajectories that are different angles. For that, we need to consider that the vestibular system also cares about acceleration. So it cares about head position, it cares about eye position and where the eyes are and where you're looking, but it also cares about what direction you're moving and how fast. And one of the best things that you can do to enhance your sense of balance is to start to bring together your visual system, the semicircular canals of the inner ear, and what we call linear
Starting point is 01:48:12 acceleration. So if I move forward in space rigidly upright, it's a vastly different situation than if I'm leaning to the to the side. One of the best ways to cultivate a better sense of balance literally within the sense organs and the neurons and the biology of the brain is to get into modes where we are accelerating forward, typically it's forward, while also tilted with respect to gravity. Now, this would be the carve on a skateboard or on a surfboard or a snowboard. This would be the taking a corner on a bike while being able to
Starting point is 01:48:54 lean safely, of course, lean into the turn so that your head is actually tilted with respect to the earth. So, anytime that we are rigidly upright, we aren't really exercising the vestibular system in balance. This is why you see people in the gym on these, one of those bouncy balls, bunchy balls, the one that the guys roll in the park, right? Bonsy balls where they're balancing back and forth. That won't work the small stabilizing muscles. What I'm talking about is getting into modes where you actually tilt the body and the head with respect to earth.
Starting point is 01:49:29 What I mean is with respect to earth's gravitational pull. Now the cerebellum is a very interesting structure because not only is it involved in balance, but it's also involved in skill learning and in generating timing of movements. It's a fascinating structure deserving of an entire episode or several episodes all on its own. involved in skill learning and in generating timing of movements. It's a fascinating structure deserving of an entire episode or several episodes all on its own. But some of the outputs of the cerebellum, meaning the neurons in the cell cerebellum get inputs, but they also send information out.
Starting point is 01:49:59 The outputs of the cerebellum are strongly linked to areas of the brain that release neuromodulators that make us feel really good, in particular serotonin and dopamine. And this is an early emerging subfield within neuroscience, but a lot of what are called the non-motor outputs of the cerebellum. Have a profound influence, not just on our ability to learn how to balance better, but also how we feel overall. So for you, exercises out there, I do hope people are getting regular, healthy amounts of exercise. We've talked about what that means in previous episodes, so at least 150 minutes a week of endurance work, some strength training, a minimum of five sets per body part to maintain musculature, even if you don't want to grow muscles, you want to do that to order, maintain healthy strength and bones, et cetera. If you're doing that, but you're only
Starting point is 01:50:50 doing things like curls in the gym, squats in the gym, riding the peloton, or even if you're outside running and you're getting forward acceleration, but you're never actually getting tilted, right? You're never actually getting tilted with respect to earth's gravitational pull. right? You're never actually getting tilted with respect to Earth's gravitational pull. You're not really exercising and getting the most out of your nervous system. Activation of the cerebellum in this way of being tilted or the head being tilted and the body being tilted while in acceleration, typically forward acceleration, but sometimes side to side has a profound and positive effect on our sense of mood and well-being. And as I talked about in previous episode, it can also enhance our ability to learn information
Starting point is 01:51:35 in the period after generating those tilts and the acceleration. And that's because the cerebellum has these outputs to these areas of the brain that release these neuromodulators like serotonin and dopamine and they make us feel really good. I think this is one of the reasons why growing up I had some friends, some of whom it might have been, you know, the world heavyweight champions of laziness for essentially everything except they would wake up at 4.30 in the morning to go surf. They would like drive, they would get up so early to go surf. I, it's not just surfers. And some surfers, by the way, I should point out are not lazy humans.
Starting point is 01:52:13 They do a lot of other things. But I knew people that couldn't be motivated to do anything, but they were highly driven to get into these experiences of forward acceleration while tilted with respect to gravity. Likewise with snowboarding or skiing or cycling, those modes of exercise seem to have an outsized effect both on our well-being and our ability to translate the vestibular balance that we achieve in those endeavors to our ability to balance while doing other things. So, and I don't mean psychological balance, and necessarily I mean physical balance.
Starting point is 01:52:50 So, for those of you that don't think of yourselves as very coordinated or with very good balance, getting into these modes of acceleration, forward movement or lateral movement while getting tilted, even if you have to do it slowly, could be beneficial. I do believe, and the scientific literature points to the fact that it will be beneficial for cultivating better sense of physical balance. It can really build up the circuits of this vestibular system. And then, of course, the feel good components of acceleration while tilted or while getting the head into different orientations relative to gravity.
Starting point is 01:53:24 Well, that's the explanation for roller coasters. Some people hate roller coasters. They make them feel nauseous. Many people love roller coasters. And one of the reasons they love roller coasters is because of the way that when you get the body, even if you're not generating the movement, you get the body into forward acceleration and you're going upside down and tilted to the side as the tracks go from side to side and tilt, et cetera. You're getting activation of these deeper brain nuclei that trigger the release of neuromodulators
Starting point is 01:53:54 that just make us feel really, really good. In fact, some people get a long arc, a long duration kind of buzz from having gone through those experiences. Some people who hate roller coasters are probably getting nauseous, just hearing about that. So I encourage people to get into modes of acceleration while tilted every once in a while provided you can do it safely. It's an immensely powerful way to build up your skills in the realm of balance. And it's also, for most people, very, very pleasing.
Starting point is 01:54:23 It feels really good because of the chemical relationship between forward acceleration and head tilt and body tilt. Now speaking of feeling nauseous, some people suffer from vertigo, some people feel dizzy, some people get lightheaded. An important question to ask yourself always, if you're feeling quote unquote dizzy or lightheaded is, are you dizzy or are you lightheaded? Now, we're not going to diagnose anything here because there's just no way we can do that. This is essentially me shouting into a tunnel.
Starting point is 01:54:54 So we don't know what's going on with each and every one of you. But if ever you feel that your world is spinning, but that you can focus on your thumb, for instance, but the rest of the world is spinning and your thumb is stationary. That's called being dizzy. Now if you feel like you're falling or that you feel like you need to get down onto the ground because you feel light headed, that's being light headed. And oftentimes with language, we don't distinguish between being dizzy and being light-headed. Now there are a lot of ways that dizziness and light-headedness can occur, and I don't
Starting point is 01:55:30 even want to begin to guess at the number of different things and ways that it could happen for those of you that suffer from it because it could be any number of them. But oftentimes if people are light-headed, yes, it could be low blood sugar, it could also be that you're dehydrated. It could also be that you're dehydrated. It could also be that you are low in electrolytes. We talked about this in a previous episode, but we will talk about it more in a future episode. Many people have too little sodium in their system salt, and that's why they feel light-headed. I have family members who for years thought they had
Starting point is 01:56:00 disrupted blood sugar. They would get shaky, jittery, light-headed, I feel like they were nauseous, et cetera. And simply the addition of a little sea salt to their water remedy the problem entirely. I don't think it's going to remedy every issue of light-headedness out there by any stretch, but just the addition of salt in this particular case helped the person. And they are not alone. Many people who think that they have low blood sugar actually are light headed because of low electrolytes and because of the way that salt carries water into the system and creates changes in blood volume, etc. Low sodium can often be a source of lightheadedness. As can low blood
Starting point is 01:56:40 sugar and of course other things as well. Now for dizziness or seasickness, we were all taught that you need to pick a point on the horizon and focus on it. But actually, that's not correct. It is true that if you are down in the cabin of a boat or you're on the lower deck and all you can see are things up close to you, that getting sloshed around like so or the boat going up and down like so. I think I'm getting a little seasick even as I do this and I describe it. Focusing on things close to you can be problematic. And in that case, the advice to go up on deck and get fresh air and to look off into the horizon, that part is correct. But focusing your eyes on a particular location on the horizon is effectively like trying
Starting point is 01:57:32 to move very slowly as I had you do before where you're trying to move your head very slowly while fixating on one location. Your eyes and your balance system were designed to move together. So really what you want to do is allow your visual system to track with your vestibular system. This is why sitting in the back of an uber or a taxi and being on your phone can make you suddenly feel very nauseous. Sometimes the cabs, particularly in New York City, they have a lot of occluders.
Starting point is 01:58:00 They have a lot of stuff blocking your field of view. It's usually a little portal out that where you can see out to the front of the front windshield. But there's all this stuff now, televisions in the backseat and you're watching that television and the cab is moving. You're in linear acceleration and sometimes you're taking corners, you're breaking. So then your your your vestibular system has to adjust to that. If you're looking at your phone or a book or even if you're talking to somebody, actually I'm starting to feel a little nauseous. I'm, I promise, I'm not going to finish this episode by vomiting at the end, at least not here. But what you, what can happen is that you're
Starting point is 01:58:35 uncoupling your, the visual information from your motion, from your vestibular information. You want those to be coupled. This is why a lot of people have to drive. They can't be in the passenger seat, because when you drive, you also get what's called proper receptive feedback. Your body is sending signals also to the vestibular system about where you are in space. When you're the passenger,
Starting point is 01:58:57 you're just getting jolted around as the person is driving. And if you're looking at your phone, it's even worse. And if you're looking at the occluder between you and the two front seats, it's even worse. And if you're looking at the occluder between you and the two front seats, that's even worse. So this is why staring out the front windshield is great, but you don't want to fixate. Okay, so hopefully I spared a few people and hopefully a few cab drivers of having people get sick in their cars or ubers. Let your visual system and your vestibular system work together.
Starting point is 01:59:24 If appropriate, get into linear acceleration, and you'll improve your sense of balance. Once again, we've covered a tremendous amount of information. Now you know how you hear, how you make sense of the sounds in your environment, how those come in your ears and how your brain processes them. In addition, we talked about things like low-level white noise and even binaural beats, which can be used to enhance certain brain states, certain rhythms within the brain,
Starting point is 01:59:51 and even dopamine release in ways that allow you to learn better. And we talked about the balance system and this incredible relationship between your vestibular apparatus, meaning the portions of your inner ear that are responsible for balance and your visual system and gravity. And you can use those to enhance your learning as well, as well as just to enhance your sense of balance. If you're learning from this podcast,
Starting point is 02:00:16 please subscribe on YouTube. That really helps us. In addition, please leave us any comments or feedback or suggestions for future episode content on YouTube in the comment section. If you haven't already subscribed on Apple and Spotify, please do that as well. And on Apple, you have the opportunity to leave us up to a five star review. At Apple, you can also leave us comments and feedback. In addition, if you'd like to support the podcast, please check out our sponsors mentioned at the beginning of the episode. That is absolutely the best way to support us. Last but not least, I'd like to thank you for your time and attention and desire and
Starting point is 02:00:50 willingness to learn about vision and balance. And of course, thank you for your interest in science. And as mentioned at the beginning of today's episode, we are now partnered with Momentous Supplements because they make single ingredient formulations that are of the absolute highest quality and they ship international. If you go to livemomentus.com slash huberman, you will find many of the supplements that have been discussed on various episodes of the huberman lab podcast, and you will find various protocols related to those supplements.
Starting point is 02:01:15 supplements.

There aren't comments yet for this episode. Click on any sentence in the transcript to leave a comment.