Huberman Lab - The Science of Love, Desire and Attachment
Episode Date: February 14, 2022In this episode, I discuss the psychology and biology of desire, love and attachment. I explain how childhood attachment types are thought to inform adult attachment styles to romantic partners, and I... describe some of the major theories of human mate selection, relationships and infidelity. Additionally, I explore the neurobiology and proposed subconscious processing underlying desire, love and attachment, including the roles of empathy and “positive delusion." I outline how self-awareness can shift one’s relationship attachment style towards securely bonded partnerships. Finally, I describe specific tools and supplements that have been researched to increase libido and sex drive. Throughout the episode, I explain the science and key mechanisms underlying romantic love and outline tools for those seeking to find a strong, healthy relationship, or for those wanting to strengthen an existing relationship. For the full show notes, visit hubermanlab.com. Thank you to our sponsors AG1 (Athletic Greens): https://athleticgreens.com/huberman LMNT: https://drinklmnt.com/huberman Supplements from Momentous https://www.livemomentous.com/huberman Timestamps (00:00:00) Desire, Love & Attachment (00:02:59) Odor, Perceived Attractiveness & Birth Control (00:08:21) Sponsors: AG1, LMNT (00:14:13) Romance: Balancing Love & Desire (00:19:00) Animal Studies, Vasopressin & Monogamy (00:22:06) Strange Situation Task, Childhood Attachment Styles (00:32:52) Adult Attachment Styles (00:38:50) Secure Attachment (00:41:23) Autonomic Arousal: The “See-Saw” (00:50:39) Tool: Self-Awareness, Healthy Interdependence (00:53:11) Neurobiology of Desire, Love & Attachment (00:58:02) Empathy & Mating & the Autonomic Nervous System (01:10:02) Positive Delusion, Touch (01:15:20) Relationship Stability (01:21:22) Selecting Mates, Recognition of Autonomic Tone (01:38:28) Neural Mechanisms of Romantic Attachment (01:47:43) Autonomic Coordination in Relationships (01:56:13) Infidelity & Cheating (02:08:56) “Chemistry”, Subconscious Processes (02:12:44) Tools: Libido & Sex Drive (02:20:20) Maca (Maca root) (02:25:58) Tongkat Ali (Longjack) (02:28:56) Tribulus terrestris (02:33:14) Zero-Cost Support, YouTube, Spotify/Apple Reviews, Sponsors, Instagram, Twitter, Supplements Title Card Photo Credit: Mike Blabac Disclaimer
Transcript
Discussion (0)
Welcome to the Huberman Lab podcast where we discuss science and science-based tools for everyday life.
I'm Andrew Huberman and I'm a professor of neurobiology and ophthalmology at Stanford School of Medicine.
Today we are going to talk about the psychology and the biology of desire, love, and attachment.
Today happens to be Valentine's Day 2022. However, the themes we are going to discuss
pertain to desire, love, and attachment on any given day. And indeed, the mechanisms we are
going to discuss almost certainly were at play thousands of years ago, hundreds of years ago,
and no doubt will still be at play in our minds and in our bodies and in our
psychologies for the decade centuries and thousands of years to come. Indeed, today I want
to focus on core mechanisms that
lead individuals to seek out
other individuals with whom to
mate with, with whom to have
children with or not, with whom
to enter short or long term
relationships with, and perhaps
to end those relationships or
to seek relationships on the
side, so called
infidelity.
I'm certainly not going to encourage or discourage any of these behaviors.
I'm simply going to cover the peer-reviewed scientific data on all these aspects of desire,
love and attachment.
I'm going to discuss how our childhood attachment styles, as they're called, influence our
adult attachment styles.
Yes, you heard that right.
How we attached or did not attach to primary caregivers in our childhood has much to do with how we
attach or fail to attach to romantic partners as adults.
Because the same neural circuits, the neurons and their connections in the brain and body, the underlying attachment between infant and caregiver,
between toddler and parent or other caregiver,
and during adolescence and in our teenage years,
are repurposed for adult romantic attachments.
I know that might be a little eerie to think about,
but indeed that is true.
Now, the fortunate thing is that regardless of our childhood attachment styles and experiences, the neural circuits for desire, love and attachment are quite
plastic. They are amenable to change in response to both what we think and what we feel, as
well as what we do. However, all three aspects that we're discussing today, desire, love
and attachment are also strongly
biologically driven.
We're going to talk about biological mechanisms such as hormones, biological mechanisms
such as neurochemicals, things like dopamine, oxytocin, and serotonin, and neural circuits,
brain areas, and indeed areas of the body that interact with the brain that control whether
or not we desire somebody or not,
whether or not we lose or increase our desire
for somebody over time, whether or not we fall in love,
what love means, and whether or not the relationships
we form continue to include the elements of desire
and love over time or not.
In order to illustrate just how powerfully our biology
can shape our perception of the attractiveness
of other people, I wanna share with you the results of a couple of studies.
Both studies explore how people rate other people's attractiveness.
In both studies, the major variable is that women are at different stages of their menstrual
cycle.
In the first study, men are rating the attractiveness of women according to the smell of those women.
Now they're not smelling them directly, they're smelling clothing that women wore for a couple
of days at different phases of their menstrual cycle.
What they find is that men will rate the odors of women as most attractive if those women
wore those shirts that clothing in the pre-avulatory phase of their cycle. Okay, so this is not to say that men do not find women attractive at other stages of their cycle.
It is to say that men find women's odors particularly attractive
if those odors were worn by women that are in the pre-avulatory phase of their menstrual cycle.
Okay, now there was also a study that was done where women at different stages of their menstrual cycle. Now there was also a study that was done where women at different stages of their menstrual
cycle are rating the odors of men.
And a similar but mirror symmetric result was found such that women who are in the pre-avulatory
phase of their menstrual cycle will rate men's odors as more attractive than at other stages
of their cycle.
So the simple way to put this is that there seems to be something special about the pre-avulatory
phase of a woman's menstrual cycle that makes men rate them as more attractive during that
time and women rate men as more attractive during that particular time as well.
So this is a bidirectional effect.
The way that the second study was done where women are rating men was not just to
smell the odors of those men on t-shirts. They did that, but they correlated that with whether
or not the shirts were worn by men that were particularly physically symmetrical. They
actually had these men divided into groups. It was more of a continuum, rather, rated
according to body symmetry and face symmetry, and women preferred more
symmetrical men when they were doing the preference test during the pre-avulatory
phase of their cycle. So again, the point is that that pre-avulatory phase of the
cycle seems to create a bidirectional mutual attractiveness. Now also extremely
interesting is that this effect does really seem to have something to do with
ovulation.
Because in both studies, they had women that were taking oral contraception or not.
What they found was if a woman is taking oral contraception, it prevented that peak and
perceived attractiveness by the men, meaning men no longer perceived a woman to be more
attractive at a particular phase of their cycle.
And also, women taking oral contraception no longer prefer the odors of more symmetrical
men during the pre-avulatory phase of their cycle.
Now, I want to make sure that it's especially clear that it is not the case that oral contraception
reduced the perception of a woman as attractive. That did not happen in these studies.
It reduced the further increase in a male's perception
of her as attractive.
And if women took oral contraception,
it prevented them from preferring more symmetrical men
based on the odors of those men.
Now, I realize there are a lot of variables here.
We've got odors, we've got symmetry,
we've got menstrual cycle, pre-obitory, non-pre-obitory,
and we have whether or not people are taking contraception or not.
But the basic finding is that depending on where women are in their menstrual cycle influences
both men's perception of them as attractive and their perception of men as attractive and
oral contraception eliminates that effect.
So I share with you those data to illustrate
that we often think that somebody is attractive
or not based on, I don't know how they look,
their skin, their hair, et cetera,
but it also illustrates that their odor is a powerful cue
for some people more than others.
You know, some of us tend to be more ol' factory-driven
than others, although if you watched the
Cuban MenLab podcast episode that I did with Professor David Bus from the University of Texas Austin,
who's a luminary in the field of evolutionary psychology and has studied mate choice and
mate selection bias over decades.
He's really one of the founders of that field.
He emphasized findings that odor for many people is a maker or a deal breaker, meaning there are some
people that even if somebody has all the characteristics that they're looking for in terms
of kindness and attractiveness and values and other features that would, would and should
be a very high priority in selecting a mate, that if they, if someone does not like the way
that person smells, they're innate body odor, independent
of colognes and perfumes and soaps, etc.
That's often a complete and total deal breaker.
I'm sure there are some of you that can relate to that and there are some of you, perhaps,
for which that is not the case.
You can't even imagine that being such a powerful variable.
Yet, the data suggested, indeed, it is a powerful variable for many people out there.
Before we begin, I'd like to emphasize that this podcast is separate from my teaching
and research roles at Stanford.
It is, however, part of my desire and effort to bring zero cost to consumer information
about science and science-related tools to the general public.
In keeping with that theme, I'd like to thank the sponsors of today's podcast.
Our first sponsor is Athletic Greens.
Athletic Greens is an all-in-one vitamin mineral
probiotic drink. I've been taking Athletic Greens since 2012, so I'm delighted that they're sponsoring
the podcast. The reason I started taking Athletic Greens and the reason I still take Athletic Greens
once or twice today is that it helps me cover all of my basic nutritional needs. It makes up for
any deficiencies that I might have. In addition, it has probiotics, which are vital for microbiome health.
I've done a couple of episodes now on the so-called gut microbiome and the ways in which
the microbiome interacts with your immune system, with your brain to regulate mood, and
essentially with every biological system relevant to health throughout your brain and body.
With athletic greens, I get the vitamins I need, the minerals I need, and the probiotics
to support my microbiome.
If you'd like to try athletic greens, you can go to athleticgreens.com slash Huberman and
claim a special offer.
They'll give you five free travel packs plus a year supply of vitamin D3K2.
There are a ton of data now showing that vitamin D3 is essential for various aspects of our
brain and body health.
Even if we're getting a lot of sunshine, many of us are still deficient in vitamin D3 is essential for various aspects of our brain and body health, even if we're getting a lot of sunshine.
Many of us are still deficient in vitamin D3.
And K2 is also important because it regulates things like cardiovascular function, calcium
and the body, and so on.
Again, go to athleticgreens.com slash uberman to claim the special offer of the 5 free travel
packs and the year supply of vitamin D3 K2.
Today's episode is also brought to us by Element.
Element is an electrolyte drink that has everything you need
and nothing you don't.
That means the exact ratios of electrolytes are an element
and those are sodium, magnesium, and potassium,
but it has no sugar.
I've talked many times before on this podcast
about the key role of hydration and electrolytes
for nerve cell function, neuron function,
as well as the function of all the cells and all the tissues and organ systems of the body.
If we have sodium, magnesium, and potassium present in the proper ratios, all of those cells function properly and all our bodily systems can be optimized.
If the electrolytes are not present and if hydration is low, we simply can't think as well as we would otherwise. Our mood is off, hormone systems go off, our ability to get into physical action to engage
in endurance and strength, and all sorts of other things is diminished.
So with element, you can make sure that you're staying on top of your hydration and that
you're getting the proper ratios of electrolytes.
If you'd like to try element, you can go to drink element that's elementy.com slash
huberman, and you'll get a free element sample pack with your purchase.
They're all delicious.
Again, if you want to try element, you can go to elementlment.com slash Huberman.
Let's talk about desire, love, and attachment.
Of course, these are topics that grab tremendous interest, so it's worth us defining our terms
a little bit before going any further.
Of course, we can have many different kinds of
loves. There's romantic love. There's love of family, so-called familial love. There's
love of pets. We can even love objects, where we can feel as if we love objects. We can love
certain activities. We can have friends that we love, and so on and so forth. The word love is used
to encompass a lot of different types of relationships. Today, we are mainly going to be focused on romantic love and the neural mechanisms of romantic love.
I want to acknowledge here at the outset that most of the studies of romantic love have focused on monogamous heterosexual love.
And also, when we talk about studies focused on desire and attractiveness and attachment. That's also the case.
That simply reflects the general bias of the literature over the last 50 to 100 years.
It does, of course, not rule out that similar or different mechanisms could be at play in
non-monogamous relationships, in homosexual relationships, or in relationships of any
kinder variation.
It's also worth us defining our terms around desire. It can mean lust.
It can mean the desire for long-term partnership. So we need to define our terms. And throughout,
I will do my best to very carefully define what I mean by desire, what I mean by love, and what I
mean by attachment. The formal study of love and desire and attachment goes back to the early 1900s.
One of the classic studies on this is entitled Love and Desire.
It was published in 1912 and really focused on two opposing themes within romance.
One is love, which in that paper was really meant to include attachment and dependence
or interdependence between individuals. Right? And the other end of the spectrum being
desire or the sexual desire for another. And romance was meant to encapsulate both those
things, love and desire. And for much of the 1900s, it was thought that love and desire were on sort of
opposing ends or kind of a push pull. And it was the dynamic push and pull between love and desire
that one could define romance. And that actually led to much of what's out there in the psychological
literature. Today we are going to explore some neurobiological studies,
some studies of the endocrine system,
meaning the hormone system,
that actually support that general model.
And I'll point you toward what I think
is a very useful book in thinking about how relationships
can both form and last over long periods of time,
and how those relationships can include both desire and interdependence.
I'll also talk about some studies that have really focused on why relationships succeed
and why they fail and how that relates to whether or not there is sufficient amounts of attachment
and desire.
So today we're going to talk about the science and indeed you'll also get some tools.
Those tools should be useful to you whether or not you happen to be in a relationship or not,
whether or not you're seeking a relationship or not.
I'd like to begin with an anecdote,
and this is not an anecdote about my relationship history.
It's an anecdote about my scientific history.
When I started graduate school,
the chairman of the department I was in at the time
said to me, you know, most PhDs last longer than most
marriages. And indeed, he was right. And also, most marriages in this country end in
divorce. I think it's about 50% with a slight skew toward more ending in divorce than persist
until death do them part. But nonetheless, it's about half and most marriages end before the eight-year period
is up. Most PhDs take anywhere from four to nine years. So there was a bit of smearing
of averages there, but the point he was trying to make really landed home for me, it did
not scare me out of relationships. Nor did it scare me out of a PhD, obviously. What
it did illustrate was that there's something about our attachment machinery that can
be very, very compelling such that people take on tremendous levels of commitment.
I have to imagine that most people enter marriages, assuming that they're going to stay in those
marriages.
I don't think most people enter marriages thinking they're going to get divorced, but that
if 50% of those commitments end in divorce, there must also
be mechanisms by which our attachments can break.
And today, we're going to talk about both the forming of attachments and the breaking of
attachments, what can prevent those breaks and attachments, and indeed, what can lead to
reattachments. There are biological mechanisms to desire, love, and attachment.
That's abundantly clear.
Now there's a robust and very large literature in animal models.
What I mean by that are field studies and laboratory studies in primates of different kinds,
such as macaque monkeys or bonobos.
People have looked at these sorts of things, believe it or not, in ducks, in laboratory mice,
in different types of birds, et cetera.
And if you look at that literature,
you can essentially find biological examples
in the animal kingdom for just about any behavior
that you can easily map to human behavior.
So for instance, there's a species of animal
called the Prairie Vole.
In one portion of the United States, this Prairie Vole species is monogamous.
They only mate with one other Prairie Vole, only raise young with one other Prairie Vole
for their entire life.
And in another region of the United States, the same species of animal, the Prairie Vole,
will mate with many individuals, they're non-monogamous.
And the major difference, at least as far as we know, between the peripherals in one location
and another location, is the levels of a molecule called vasopressin in the brain and body.
V vasopressin is present in humans.
It has numerous biological roles.
It's responsible, for instance, for controlling the amount of urine that you had scraped, the
amount of water that you retain, and for sexual desire, as well as mate seeking.
Levels of vasopressin and prairie voles
are strongly determinant of whether or not a prairie vol
is going to be a monogamous or non-monogamous.
Why do I raise this?
Well, I raise this because the literature on prairie voles
is quite beautiful and has been discussed
quite a lot in the popular press.
You can look it up with an easily just web edge and search.
You'll find lots of information about this, lots of news articles about this, and lots
of interpretations as to how vasopressin might be involved in similar different mechanisms
and humans.
Now, I don't have a problem with mapping animal studies to humans.
I think there's certainly a place for that.
But if you just sort of lean back and look at the giant mass of studies and animals and
their mating behavior and their mate selection behavior, you can essentially find examples
of anything.
You can find examples of polygamy.
You can find examples of cheating, of infidelity.
You can find examples of all sorts of different behaviors that in your own mind you can map
to human behavior.
But it's really hard to make the leap from animal models to humans in any kind of direct
way.
And so thankfully, there's been tremendous work done in the last, mainly 20 years or so,
looking at human mate selection, human desire, human love, and human attachment.
So we're mainly going to focus on those studies today. Looking at human mate selection, human desire, human love, and human attachment.
So we're mainly gonna focus on those studies today
and we're appropriate, we will map those findings
back to the findings and animals
to see if there's some universal truths
or some universal principles
about how the neural circuits and biological mechanisms work.
But by and large, we're gonna focus on human studies today.
So unless I say otherwise,
the data that I'm referring to today
are entirely from human beings. So let's talk about attachment
and attachment styles. And this will offer you the opportunity to answer some important
questions for yourself, such as what is my, meaning your attachment style in relationship.
One of the most robust findings in the field of psychology is this notion of attachment
styles.
And this was something that was discovered through a beautiful set of studies that were
done by Mary Ainsworth in the 1980s, in which she developed a laboratory condition called
the Strange Situation Task.
Now the Strange Situation Task has been studied over and over again in different cultures,
in different locations throughout the world.
And in preparing for this episode, I actually spoke to three different psychologists.
I spoke to a psychoanalyst.
I spoke to a cognitive behavioral psychologist.
And I actually spoke to a psychiatrist, excuse me not a psychologist, but a psychiatrist
with a medical degree and asked, is the strange situation task and the various attachment
styles that emerge from that task
are those still considered valid?
And indeed, all three of them said, if ever there was a literature in psychology that is
absolutely tamped down and has a firm basis in both data and real world principles and
real world examples, it's this notion of attachment styles.
So what is the strange situation task?
The strange situation task involves a parent, typically a mother, in the studies that
we're done, but a parent or other caregiver, bringing their child, their actual child,
into a laboratory, and there's a room with a stranger.
And the mother enters the room with the child,
and there's some toys in the room,
and typically the mother and the stranger will talk.
Obviously, the stranger is part of the experiment.
It's not just some random person off the street.
And the child is allowed to move about the room.
They can observe the mother interacting
with the other person or not.
They can play with toys or not.
But then at some point, the mother leaves. And then at some point later, designated by the experimenter, the mother comes back.
And what is measured in these studies is both how the child, the toddler reacts to the
mother leaving and how the child reacts to the mother returning at the end of the experiment.
And oftentimes this will have two or three different phases where the mother will bring the
child in, then leave, then come back in, and leave.
There are also studies in which the behavior of the child with the stranger is also examined.
So there are a lot of variations of this, but the basic findings are that
toddlers, children fall into four different categories of attachment style,
and that these attachment styles can predict many features of adolescent, teen, young adult,
and even adult attachment styles, not in strange situations of the sort that
I just described, but in romantic attachments.
I should mention also that attachment style is plastic, meaning it can change across
the lifespan.
So as I described the results, I described the different attachment styles that are out
there.
And if any of those resonate with you
or bring to mind certain people in your life,
please do not assume that those attachment styles
are rigid and fixed for the entire lifespan.
There are also terrific data that indicate
that through specific processes, both psychological
and some biological adjustments,
that people can change their attachment style.
And that indeed, people who have different attachment styles can change the attachment
styles of others.
But just to make very clear what the results of the study were, I want to review what the
four different attachment styles are.
And typically, people fall into one group or another, but not several.
So the four patterns of attachment that were revealed by these studies, again,
were revealed by examining the behavior of the child in response to the mother leaving
the and the mother returning and the child's response to the stranger that is in the room
with them. The first style is the so-called secure attachment style. In the nomenclature
of this kind of study, these are the so-called B babies as in the
letter B bulldog, B, not for bulldogs, but just to designate this category. The secure attachment
style is one in which the child will engage with the stranger, with the experimenter, while
the parent is present in the room, but that when the parent, typically it's a mother, but when the parent
or other caregiver leaves, the child does get visibly upset.
They might whine, they might cry, they might even tantrum a bit.
However, when the caregiver, meaning the mother or father or other caregiver returns, the
child visibly expresses happiness that the caregiver has returned.
Okay, so that's the hallmark of the secure attachment style.
And again, this is all pre-verbal.
This is happening long before the child can express how they feel with words.
And the interpretation of this is that the secure child feels confident that the caregiver
is available and will be responsive to their needs and their communications.
So that when the child winds or is distressed,
the parent doesn't come right back into the room, but at some point they do and they seem
to have a sense of trust that if the parent or caregiver leaves, that the parent will
come back and that they're happy that they do.
These children are also very good at exploring novel environments after the parent is gone, and while the parent is there,
and almost always when the parent is there,
they will explore more broadly, literally in space,
they'll venture out further than they will
when the parent is gone.
They also tend to engage with the caregiver in a way
that's not immediately and completely trusting,
but that over time seems to evolve
from one in which they're suspicious of this person to one in which they're at least somewhat trusting.
Okay, so these are the general contours of the secure attachment style.
And fortunately nowadays there are physiological studies measuring things like heart rate
and breathing and other measures that correlate with the subjective assessment of what these
children are feeling.
Okay, so first category is secure attached.
The second category is a so-called anxious avoidant or insecurely attached, which are the
category A babies.
The children with anxious avoidant insecure attachment patterns generally tend to avoid
or ignore the caregiver, meaning the parent, and show very little
emotion when the parent leaves or returns.
So this is the reason they call them avoidant or anxious avoidant and kind of insecure.
There isn't this happiness or joy that the parent is back.
They don't seem to express that.
They do not exhibit distress on separation, and they generally tend to have some tendency
to approach the caregiver when they return, but there doesn't seem to be a general expression
of joy. And again, physiological measures support that as well. Things like changes in heart
rate tend to be less dramatic in the anxious avoidant insecure attachment style than in the anxious avoidant insecure attachment style then in the secure attachment style.
Okay, so that's the second one.
The third category is the so-called
anxious ambivalent slash resistant insecure category.
I didn't name these categories,
so you have to blame others in this one instance.
For everything else blame me,
but in this instance, you have to blame
the psychologist that named this category.
The anxious ambivalent slash resistant, insecure category, also called the C-babies for the
letter C, just as a categorization.
The anxious, ambivalent, resistant, insecure toddlers, really, show distress even before
separation from their mother or other caregiver, and they tend to be very clingy and difficult
to comfort when the caregiver returns. Okay, so they're distressed even before the mother
leaves the room and they tend to be very clingy and really hard to calm down when the mother
returns. They tend to show either what seems to be resentment in response to the parents'
absence. We don't really know what they're feeling or some sort of helpless passivity.
And there's actually subcategorizations that the psychologists have come up with with
C1 subtypes and C2 subtypes.
We don't have to get bogged down in that.
But just know that there isn't one absolute measure that says, oh, well, this person is anxious
and bivalent resistant and secure.
They could be somewhat passive or they could be somewhat angry at the
caregiver. But the basic ideas that before and after the separation, they are clingy and
difficult to comfort. They just can't seem to calm themselves down. And physiological
measures of heart rate and hormone measurements, such as cortisol, also support that statement.
And the third category of attachment style is the so-called disorganized or disoriented
or D for the letter D babies.
This is a categorization that was added later to this strange situation task that is a
real hallmark of developmental psychology studies.
It was developed by Mary Ainsworth graduate student Mary Main who actually had the great
fortune of taking a course from and learning from when I wasworth graduate student, Mary Main, who actually had the great fortune of taking
a course from and learning from when I was a graduate student at Berkeley many years ago.
And this fourth categorization was controversial for a while, but now is generally accepted.
The key feature of the disorganized, disoriented category is that the toddlers tend to be tense
and they tend to encompass a lot of
kind of odd physical postures.
They tend to hunch their shoulders.
They'll put their hands behind their neck.
They'll cock their head to the side.
For those of you listening,
I'm doing this on the video version.
It's not where you don't have to go see that,
but for those of you that are watching this on video,
they tend to kind of constrain their body size a bit
and go into odd postures
that they normally wouldn't do anywhere else.
So this is why it's called the disorganized or disoriented category.
It seems like these children just don't really know how to react to a separation.
And they just start to manifest behaviors and emotional tones that aren't observed in
other situations.
Okay, so we've got our four categories. to manifest behaviors and emotional tones that aren't observed in other situations.
Okay, so we've got our four categories. I'll try and use the shortest possible names for each category. We've got category one, which is securely attached. We've got category two, which is
insecurely attached, also sometimes called anxious avoidant. Then we've got category three, which is
the resistant insecure category, which is anxious ambivalent. And then there's this
fourth category, the disorganized disoriented category, or is anxious and bivalent, and then there's this fourth category,
the disorganized disoriented category,
or the so-called D babies.
Now, what's interesting about this
from the perspective of desire, love, and attachment,
is that the categorizations of children
into one of these four different categories,
as toddlers, is strongly predictive of their
attachment style in romantic partnerships later in life, which is to me, both amazing and
surprising and not surprising all at the same time.
Amazing because it means that, first of all, we are relatively hardwired for attachment.
I think that that's incredible and beautiful, that we have designated neurons, nerve cells
and hormonal systems that are there to ensure that we have some sort of response to a caregiver
being there or not being there or returning or leaving, but also that the same neural
circuitries, the same hormonal responses are at least in some way repurposed
for entirely different types of attachments later in life.
So when we hear the psychologists talk about how,
you know, we formed a template early in life
based on experiences that were even pre-verbal
before we had language.
And those templates are superimposed on our relationships
or we should say our later relationships are superimposed on our relationships
or we should say our later relationships
are superimposed on those templates.
There really is a basis for that.
We now have neuroimaging studies to support,
for instance, the work of Alan Shore from UCLA
showing that when a mother and child interact,
either through very soothing interactions
like bottle feeding or breastfeeding
or singing
to one's baby or putting them to sleep, that the brain of the child and the brain of the
mother are entering a coordinated state of relaxation.
And it's not one direction mother to child.
The child is also calming the mother.
Typically these studies were done with mothers again, sometimes with fathers, but typically
with mothers again, sometimes with fathers, but typically with mothers. And in addition to that, when the mother or other caregiver acts very excited and raises their
voice or puts a lilt in their voice or widens their eyes, that the child will do the same.
And again, there's a bidirectional interaction in that case of excitement. And there's the
release of neurochemicals like dopamine into the bloodstream whereas in the relaxation scenario and the soothing scenario
There's we know the release of things like serotonin and oxytocin so the neural systems for attachment and the neural systems for what we call
autonomic arousal for being alert and calm
Don't act in a vacuum
They are tethered to other people in our environment. And of
course, we know this, right? We sometimes hear the statement, no one can make you feel anything.
I've always had a little bit of a problem with that statement. I don't think I'm
contradicting anyone in particular, but you hear that a lot. No one can make you feel anything.
Indeed, they can, right? A physical injury can make you feel something. If somebody says something
that you very much like, it can make you feel something. If somebody says something that you very much like,
it can make you feel something.
And if somebody says something that you very much dislike,
it will make you feel something.
So the idea that no one can make us feel anything
isn't actually true.
Our nervous system is tethered to the nervous systems
of others, and that is true from the very earliest stages
of our lives.
And in this case, we're talking about how our templates
for attachment in romantic relationships,
how we find them, how we maintain them, and indeed, how we break them and reform them,
is based on a template that was established through an entirely different set of priorities,
which was how we feel safe and secure in novel environments, depending on whether or
not our primary caregiver is there or not.
Neuroimaging supports that.
When I say neuroimaging, I mean brain scans support that, measures of hormones in the body
and brain support that, measures of neurochemical support that.
There's simply no way around this truth that we have a set of road maps in our mind that
are reused for entirely different purposes later in life.
That is vitally important to understand because if one is successful in forming
romantic attachments, maintaining them, et cetera, or not, does in fact reflect the earlier
templates that you've created. But as I've mentioned before, the good news is that these
templates can shift over time. And one of the more powerful ways to shift those templates over time is purely by the
knowledge that they exist and the understanding that those templates are malleable.
They can change through the process of neuroplasticity.
Again, neuroplasticity is just a rewiring of nerve connections that is very much present
in childhood, but also very much present in adulthood.
So if you're somebody who you think falls in the category
one, two, three, or four, or you know somebody or involved with somebody who
falls in the category one, two, three, and four, the mere knowledge of that can be very useful.
But you might ask, well, what do I do with that knowledge? Well, fortunately, both
psychologists and biologists have started to leverage that knowledge toward establishing
better, more secure bonds
in adult romantic relationships.
And there's a book that has really tapped into this.
I think it's the first book that has really addressed this head on.
And that book comes from two Columbia professors.
And the title of the book is Attached.
The new science of adult attachment and how it can help you find and keep love.
The authors of this book are Amir Levine and Rachel Heller.
Again, both of them are skilled academics and researchers who have really taken the literature
that I described on the strain situation task and mapped it to adult attachment styles.
And also, they've mapped out ways that they've observed in their clinical practice and that is laboratory supported for, for instance, people that have an anxious and bivalent or what we would call an insecure attachment style or for people that fall into the disorganized or disoriented attachment style, how they can modify that attachment style in or out of relationships in order to establish what I think everybody wants,
which is secure attachment.
Why does everybody want that?
Well, secure attachment allows people to be both
in relationship or if they choose to be on their own
or to be in relationship but physically separated
from somebody else or even emotionally separated
from somebody else and maintain what we call
a stable autonomic equilibrium. The ability to remain calm, clear headed.
You might not like what's happening, but you're able to navigate that with some
sense of clarity and not excessive discomfort.
So is there a goal in all of this stuff about love, desire and attachment?
Indeed, there is the the secure attachment style is the one that leads to the
most stable and predictable
long-term relationships.
Put differently babies, toddlers, adolescents, teens, and young adults that have a secure
attachment style are more likely to find and form long-term relationships than are people
in the other categories.
But people in other categories can learn and eventually migrate into the secure attachment
style.
And I think that book attached, I have no affiliation to the authors or the book itself,
I should just mention that, attached the new science of adult attachment and how it
can help you find and keep love.
Really, it sounds very pop psychology-esque, but it is really grounded in the research
psychology literature and there's also some interesting biology there.
Another point to make about attachment styles is that it is possible.
And some of you may be familiar with circumstances whereby people who are securely attached,
either because they grew up in an environment where secure attachment was cultivated, or
because they developed that on their own, can also migrate
out of the securely attached category into insecurity attached or into avoidant types
of attachment styles as teens or as young adults or as adults of at any age or any stage
of life, by virtue of being with somebody who has a different perhaps less adaptive
attachment style, right?
What this means is that if you have or you develop a secure attachment style
It's vitally important to protect that attachment style because it is possible to become anxiously attached
even if you grew up in a stable attachment framework and
Again, this can happen at any stage
So if you're interested in attachment styles
and how they influence adult romantic attachments, and certainly if you are a parent, I would
encourage you to check out the book attached. Again, it's quite good. And I think that it
offers a number of actionable tools to both form and hold on to secure attachment styles.
So I mentioned that the neural circuits for child parent
or child caregiver attachment are repurposed
for romantic attachment later in life.
But what are these neural circuits?
What do they do?
It's so attractive, if you will, to think about a brain area
that controls love or a brain area that controls desire
or a brain area that controls attachment. but it simply doesn't work that way.
As I've talked about before in this podcast and I will say again and again because it will
persist to be true long after I'm gone, is that no one brain area can give rise to anything
as complex as desire, love, or attachment.
Instead there are multiple brain areas that, through their coordinated action, create
a sort of a song that we call desire, or a song that we call love, or a song that we
call attachment, not a literal song, although there are songs about desire, love and attachment,
of course, many songs, some good, some not so good.
But rather, different brain areas being active in different sequences, and with different
intensities, can make us feel as if we are in the mode that we call desire, in the mode of love or in the
mode of attachment.
But beneath all of that is this element of autonomic arousal.
And I want to focus on autonomic arousal just for a bit longer because it really is one
of the three core elements by which we form and
maintain loving attachments and by which we break loving attachments. The autonomic nervous system,
as the name suggests, is automatic. In fact, that's what autonomic means. Now, it's actually the case
that we can control our autonomic nervous system to some degree or another. But the autonomic nervous system controls things like digestion, our breathing, whether
not we're conscious of that breathing or not.
It controls things like how alert we are or how sleepy we are.
And the autonomic nervous system, as I just briefly described earlier, is really something
that we come into the world with.
It's hardwired.
All the elements are there, but through interactions with our parent, either soothing interactions
or fun, playful interactions, or dare I say scary interactions, our autonomic nervous system
gets tuned up, meaning we each develop a tendency to either be more alert and anxious or
more calm or kind of a balance
of alert and calm.
Now, of course, this changes across each day.
And depending how tired we are, late in the day, if we've been awake for a while, we tend
to get sleepy early in the day.
If we're very rested, we tend to wake up and feel very alert.
So the way to think about the autonomic nervous system is it's kind of a seesaw.
We go back and forth between being very alert.
We can be alert and calm or we can be very, very alert.
We can be in a state of panic, we can be fast asleep, so we can be extremely calm,
or we can just be kind of sleepy, semi-com, but still also alert. So think about it like a seesaw,
and that seesaw has a hinge, and that hinge defines how tight or loose that seesaw is,
how readily it can tilt back and forth.
Our autonomic tone is how tight that hinge is, and there are biological mechanisms to explain
this, but here I just want to stay with the analogy of the seesaw for now.
The interactions between child and caregiver early in life take the child and the caregiver
from one end of the seesaw to the other,
from being very alert in a state of play, for instance, to being nursed and being very
sued until we go to sleep. And of course, we each have a seesaw. The parent and the child has a
seesaw. And they're interacting. What do I mean by that? Well, there are beautiful studies,
and beautiful, not in the sense that they focused on a pleasant topic, but beautiful because they were done so beautifully well, that looked at, for
instance, the response of mothers and their physiologies and the response of children and
their physiologies during the bombing of cities during World War II.
So an unpleasant situation, but what was revealed during the course of these studies was that if the mothers
were very stressed during an onslaught of bombing of the city, the children's physiologies
tended to be stressed also and persisted in being stressed long after that stressful
episode was done.
They actually followed that these children well out for many decades afterwards.
Conversely, if the parent, and in this case, again, it was mothers that were explored
in these studies, had turned this whole business of going into the bomb shelters into somewhat
of a game, all right, taking it seriously, but essentially telling the children, okay,
it's time to go but not expressing much stress or distress.
The children also didn't develop much stress or distress or trauma. Now, there were exceptions to this, of course, but in general, that was the rule that the autonomic nervous systems of
children tend to mimic the autonomic nervous systems of the primary caregiver.
And the mechanisms by which this occurs has been explored. And again, I just refer to the
beautiful work of Alan Shore at University of California Los Angeles. I mean, again, his name is Shore spelled
SC-ORI. I'm looking down briefly at the floor here because I'll just reach for the book.
He has a wonderful book called Right Brain Psychotherapy. It's a little bit technical, but if
you're interested in some of the studies, this book, right, being psychotherapy, details
how everything from nursing of children to play time behavior to a strange situation type
task behavior, we talked about before, which of course occurs when children get dropped
off at daycare or nursery school or with babysitters, et cetera.
And indeed, all types of caregiver child interactions tune up that autonomic nervous system so that
the child ends up with an autonomic nervous system that either tends to lean more towards
alert and anxious, or can be very alert but calm, or can be very calm and hard to budge.
Again, it's the tightness of that hinge that really underlies these attachment styles that we were
talking about earlier. And not on this episode of the Hubertman Lab podcast,
but on many other previous episodes,
such as the Master Stress episode
or some of the Optimize Health episodes,
you can find these if you want at HubertmanLab.com.
A lot of the tools and techniques
that are recommended there have to do
with readjusting the autonomic nervous system
in deliberate ways as an adult.
Again, I won't go into the specific tools, but for instance, the physiological sigh, this
tool that I've talked about extensively of two inhales through the nose, as deeply as
you can on the first one, sneaking in a little bit more air on the second one, and then
a long exhale through the mouth, is a way of adjusting that autonomic seesaw.
It tends to make us more calm.
It activates what we call the parasympathetic arm of the autonomic nervous system, which is
just fancy nerd speak for it's a quick way to calm yourself down, right?
Things like ice baths or cold showers or cold immersion or hyperventil, deliberate
hyperventilation by contrast or ways in which we can deliberately increase
the level of our so-called sympathetic arm
of our autonomic nervousness to make ourselves more alert.
Why would you want to do that?
Well, you can do that to be more alert,
to be more awake if you like,
or as a form of self-induced stress inoculation,
to be able to tolerate higher levels of adrenaline
by making it a practice that you self-direct.
The reason those tools are out there is because many of us, for whatever reason,
we don't have to blame anyone, but because of our childhood templates, because of things that
happened and didn't happen in terms of our interactions with caregivers, have autonomic nervous
systems that are tilted to one side or the other more than we would like, or in which the hinge that I'm talking about in this analogy is to loose or that is too tight,
and we're sort of stuck in a mode of anxiousness or stuck in a mode of lack of energy. That's what
those tools are really about. But at a deeper level, the autonomic nervous system is really the system that governs how we
will react in response to a romantic partner being present or leaving.
And I don't necessarily mean leaving the relationship entirely, although it could mean that, right?
We know people, I'm sure you know people, that upon the end of a relationship that they
wanted very much are absolutely crushed.
And actually in researching this episode there, I discovered there's an extensive literature
finding that the feelings that one has after a breakup are very much like a clinical depression
in many cases.
But there are individuals that can look at a breakup as a transient event that they don't
interpret as going to mean so much for all aspects of
their life or reshaping their view of themselves.
Why?
Well, we have different levels of autonomic function.
And depending on where our seesaw is, if you will, some of us become extremely distraught
and can't recalibrate ourselves, can't adjust ourselves down from stress to calm, or can't take
ourselves from exhausted to more alert if we need to do that on our own.
And so that's why tools for doing that exist.
But attachment itself is about where our autonomic nervous system resides.
So if I were to offer a set of tools around these topics of desire, love, and attachment,
I would say first of all, you might want to think about whether or not you fall into
the security and secure or other attachment styles.
Second, I think it is vitally important for all of us, but certainly for people that are
in relationships or seeking relationships, to be able to at least have some recognition
of where our autonomic nervous system tends to reside
both in terms of when we are with somebody and when they leave.
When we are apart for long periods of time, can we calm ourselves, can we self-sooth,
or are we very much dependent on the presence of another in order to feel sooth?
Now I absolutely want to emphasize that there is nothing wrong.
In fact, there's everything right with feeling great in the presence of somebody else.
That is actually a whole mark of strong and quality attachments.
These days, we hear the term codependent a lot.
This was a, I believe the term was first coined by Pia Melody.
And it actually does occupy an important role in the world of trauma,
trauma healing, so-called trauma bonding, topics of another episode. I actually did an episode on world of trauma, trauma healing, so-called trauma bonding topics of another episode.
I actually did an episode on fear and trauma and we will do one all about trauma bonding
with an expert at some point in the future.
But codependence and codependency, the term can sometimes be misinterpreted as any dependence
on another isn't good.
Interdependence, healthy interdependence, of course, is good.
It is the hallmark of healthy child parent relations, sibling relations, and romantic relations.
But a key element of healthy interdependence is that yes, our autonomic nervous system is
adjusted by the presence of another, but that also that we can adjust our own autonomic nervous system
even in the absence of that person that if the person goes away
temporarily or permanently that we can still regulate our own autonomic nervous system both from states of stress to states of calm both from states of
exhaustion to states of more
alertness and of course we all need sleep to go from exhaustion to alertness. But what I'm referring to here is the ability to regulate when distraught or regulate when
fatigued or feeling depressed.
And that is and is all about the autonomic nervous system.
So as we talk about attachment styles, when we talk about infant and toddler and adult
attachment styles, what we are really talking about is a complex set of neural circuitries and one of those neural circuitries, which is absolutely crucial, is this
autonomic nervous system. So if the autonomic nervous system is one key component of desire,
love, and attachment, what are the other two? And what I'm going to tell you next is largely the
pioneering work of Helen Fisher, who is really an anthropologist
who's become a bit of a neuroscientist and is collaborated with neuroscientists to
establish brain areas and neural circuits that are associated with different aspects of
attachment, love, and desire.
I think the first really high quality study of neural circuits associated with these themes
was a paper published in 2005 in a very fine anatomical journal,
perhaps the best neuro anatomical journal, which is the Journal of Comparative Neurology.
The Journal of Comparative Neurology has been around for more than 100 years
and is considered the archival location for placing really high quality anatomy. They have
tremendously high standards. And the study that I'm referring to
is entitled Romantic Love, an FMI, meaning functional magnetic resonance imaging study of a neural
mechanism for mate choice. And Dr. Fisher is an author on this paper as is Arthur Aaron and Lucy Brown.
So all very fine researchers. And this study, as well as several other studies using magnetic resonance imaging, things
like EEG, neuroantomical tracing, et cetera, have identified a large number of brain areas
that are associated with different aspects of desire, love, and attachment.
And I'll just throw out a few names of those brain areas and what they control.
And then I'll tell you how those anchor to the other two categories of neural circuits
essential for desire, love and attachment.
So not surprisingly, the dopamine system in the brain is associated with desire, love and
attachment and mainly with desire, although to some extent love dopamine is a neurochemical,
sometimes associated with reward.
But as some of you have heard me say before, it is mainly a molecule
of motivation, craving, and pursuit. And that motivation craving and pursuit that relates
to dopamine is not unique to attachment or love or sex or mating, et cetera. It is a universal
generic currency in the brain for pursuing something, food when you're hungry, a mate when you want one, to mate when
you want to, warmth when you're cold, et cetera, et cetera.
Okay.
So it's not for one specific purpose, but the brain area is associated with dopamine involved,
for instance, the ventral tegmental area, the substantioneigra, areas of that sort, the
basil ganglia.
You don't need to know these names.
Just understand that these are networks of neurons
that tend to put the person you into a state of forward action and pursuit and craving
and motivation.
They are not about being quiescent, relaxed, et cetera.
The neural circuits for quiescent and relaxation are most associated with love and attachment,
not surprisingly.
And they're the neurochemical serotonin, and to some extent oxytocin are the predominant neurochemicals
involved. And those are released from brain areas such as the Raffa nucleus in the back of the brain.
You may have heard that the majority of serotonin in your body is made in your gut, and indeed that's
true. But I hate to break it to you. The serotonin in your gut is not responsible for your feelings of love and attachment, at
least not to a high degree.
That's mainly going to be the reflection of neurons in your brain that make serotonin.
There are other areas of the brain that make serotonin as well and oxytocin as well,
but they tend to be associated with the kind of warmth and calm and the soothing that we
feel in the presence of
another. And again, these are not strictly divided circuits. We can have dopamine and serotonin
present in our brain and body at the same time to equal or different degrees. And we were
returning a little bit to what happens when levels of dopamine are very high and levels of serotonin
are low and vice versa and so on, including in states of neurochemically
modified states as it were in when we talk about things like MDMA, so-called ecstasy.
But in the meantime, I want to just discuss the two neural circuits that used dopamine,
that used serotonin and oxytocin, and that collaborate with the autonomic nervous system
to drive what we call desire, love, and attachment. And the three circuits are autonomic nervous system,
we talked about that one. Then there's the nervous system components or the neural circuits
for empathy, for being able to see and respond to, indeed match the emotional tone or the autonomic tone
of another.
And then there's the third category, and this might surprise some of you.
It certainly surprised me.
But the data point to the fact that the third neural circuit that's very important for establishing
bonds is one associated with positive delusions.
So given that the neural circuits for empathy are absolutely crucial for falling in love and maintaining stable attachments, I'd like to talk about
those neural circuits and what they are. Now, often when we hear empathy, we think, oh,
empathy is really about listening to and really understanding what somebody else is feeling,
maybe even feeling what they're feeling. And that's the case, but what do we mean by that, right?
What is it to feel what another feels?
Well, what it means is that their seesaw is driving your seesaw, or your seesaw is somehow
driving their seesaw, that there's a match in terms of the tilt of those seesaws.
Now, it doesn't have to be an exact match, right?
If someone that you really care about is very, very stressed,
you could also become very stressed.
That's a form of empathic matching,
and there are indeed neural circuits for that.
I'll describe what those neural circuits are in a moment.
But sometimes the best role for us to take
is actually one in which we are calm
when the person that we care about
or that we were romantically involved with is very, very anxious.
And in a few minutes, I'll talk about how matching of emotional tone
can be good or bad for the stability of a relationship.
And complementarity of autonomic matching can be good or bad.
In other words, sometimes it's beneficial for a relationship
to go into the same state as the
other. And sometimes it's more beneficial for us to not go into the same state as the other.
But the important feature here is that when we talk about emotional matching or empathy or going
into the same state or not going into the same state, what we're really talking about is whether or not the
autonomic seesaw of one individual is driving the autonomic seesaw of the other individual.
And this is a vital principle for how we fall in love and form attachments. And it's actually
part of the desire and mating process itself. I would go so far as to say that one of the prerequisites to the propagation and expansion
of our species is this notion of autonomic regulation and to some extent matching of
autonomic nervous systems.
Let me explain what I mean.
Last I checked, the only way that new humans can be created is by way of sperm-meeting egg,
either in body or in dish, but sperm meets egg, and then typically nine months later, we
have a human baby.
The process of bringing sperm to egg, right, mating behavior, sex behavior in humans, is
one of autonomic regulation.
And what I mean by that is the process of finding a mate. And in this case, I mean actually
someone to mate with typically. While scenario is very typically, is one of elevated autonomic
arousal, meaning increased activation of the so-called sympathetic nervous system.
This is related to dopamine release and it's related to epinephrine release. There has to be
a pursuit or at least there has to be a mobilization to arrive in the same location whereby one can
mate, right? That almost always is the case. However, the sexual arousal itself is in both males and females,
is actually driven primarily by the parasympathetic arm
of the autonomic nervous system.
So while pursuit is one of alertness and sympathetic drive,
as we say.
Again, sympathy is not really what's at play here.
The word sympathy means together, and the activation of the autonomic nervous system toward
more alert state is because of a sympathetic nervous system, mean the co-activation together
of many neurons in the brain and spinal cord.
But then the actual physiological arousal state that we call sexual arousal is predominantly
parasympathetically driven.
To be quite direct about this, if the sympathetic nervous
system activation is too high, the sexual arousal response
cannot happen in either males or in females.
It's inhibited.
However, the orgasm and ejaculation
response, which, if you think about it, is required
for sperm to meet egg, is sympathetic driven.
And then after orgasm and ejaculation, the parasympathetic nervous system kicks back
in, and there's a calming and relaxation. So the arc of mating involves sympathetic arousal,
okay, not sympathy, but alertness and arousal for pursuit.
Then a tilt of the seesaw, at least to some degree,
for arousal of the sort that we typically
here of, of sexual arousal.
Then at the point of orgasm and ejaculation
is back to a sympathetic response.
How can I say that?
How do I know that?
The sympathetic nervous system, meaning neurons within the sympathetic arm of the autonomic
nervous system are what drive ejaculation and orgasm.
Then afterward, there's a return to increased parasympathetic activation. And we don't know for sure why that happens,
but it's thought that in species that parabond,
humans generally parabond, not always,
they're the return to more parasympathetic activation
after orgasm and ejaculation,
is thought to increase the exchange of
pheromonal orders, odors, excuse me,
and to increase pillow talk and pair bonding of different
kinds, okay?
So that's the the the seesaw going back and forth is actually built into the process
by which our entire species propagates.
So in some ways, every human is required to navigate that process if they want their
offspring to persist.
And of course nowadays there are technologies like in vitro fertilization and intra-yudarin
insemination. There are a variety of ways that technology is allowed people to
circumvent the actual physical mating process in the way that I described.
But by and large, that's the way it's done and certainly that's the way it was
done historically for, if not tens of thousands or hundreds of thousands of years. That process is also what happens in all
mammalian species that mate. Okay, so I'm overlooking an entire literature of animal studies
that classic studies of this were done by two individuals. I'll just briefly mention them in
case you want to look at the literature. There's a guy at the Rockefeller University named Donald Fawf, P-F-A-F-F, who has
done beautiful studies identifying the neural circuitry, what's called the lordosis response.
Unlike in humans, the mating behavior of animals is rather stereotyped in terms of the
positions that they occupy, and the lordosis response is a kind of a ushaping
or a bending up of the hindquarters of typically of rodents,
but of other animals as well.
The male mounting is almost always from behind,
except in some species of primates,
and that Lordosis response is only going to occur
during particular phases of the estrus cycle.
The estrus cycle is sort of the analog to the menstrual cycle,
but it's not 28 days, it's four days, or some other duration in other animals, depending
on the animal. The lordosis response is strongly regulated by odors, by contact, and is estrogen
and testosterone controlled. And then the male portion of the mating sequence in animals, the mounting and thrusting and ejaculation
as they're called, or mounting thrusting,
intramission and ejaculation.
Those are the four scientific categories
that have been described.
That's Presence in Rodents and also in dogs,
where it was primarily studied by Frank Beach,
who was at University of California Berkeley
for a long time.
And the entire literature around the neural circuitry for sexual and mating behavior in animals
largely stemmed from the work of Donald Fawf and Frank Beach and their scientific offspring,
not their actual offspring. You can look at that literature if you like.
There have been human neuroimaging studies of the process that I described a few minutes ago, believe it
or not, of people in brain scanners not necessarily mating with other people, but going through
that arc of arousal sympathetic activation during orgasm or ejaculation and then the post
ejaculatory or orgasmic phase in both men and women. And the brain areas associated with those
have all been mapped out now.
The spinal cord areas that control things like
erection, vaginal lubrication, ejaculation,
and orgasm, those have also been mapped out.
And this has all been explored from the perspective
of both basic science, just to get an understanding
of how our species has
sexual interactions and reproduces, but also from the perspective of, for instance, trying
to repair sexual function after spinal cord injury, which is a prominent concern for a lot
of people depending on where they have their injury, but in the number of people that
have spinal cord injuries.
So this is both vital, biological, and clinical data.
The neural circuits for everything that I just described
reside in the autonomic nervous system
and are coordinated with the neural circuits
that are associated with empathy.
The neural circuits for empathy, again, there are many,
but mainly two structures that you should know about,
the prefrontal cortex, which is how we perceive things outside of us and make decisions on the basis of those
perceptions, how we organize those decisions.
And an area of the brain called the insula, INSU LA.
The insula is a really interesting brain area that allows us to interocept, to pay attention
to what's going on inside our body, and to split some of our attention to extraocept to pay attention to what's going on inside our body and to split some of our
attention to extraocept.
And the mating dance, whether or not it's the dinner and date portion of the mating dance
or the actual physical dance part of the mating dance or actual mating and sexual behavior,
kissing or otherwise, that is a coordinated activity of two bodies, typically it's two.
I realize sometimes it's more, sometimes it's only one, but typically it's two bodies
and at least in the framework we're using here.
That coordinated dance is one in which the autonomic nervous system of one individual,
in general, is coordinating with the autonomic nervous system of the other individual and the insula is essentially
splitting one's attention between how we feel ourselves, how our body feels, what we're
thinking, with the thinking and the body's bodily sensations of the other.
And that can be communicated obviously through words.
I can be communicated through sounds.
It can be communicated through touch.
And it can be communicated through a number of kind of more subtle cues, like pupil size or whether
or not.
Certainly in cases where we recognize the person and we kind of know their responses, their
autonomic responses under different conditions, we can assess it as the person comfortable,
are they uncomfortable?
Are they more focused on me or on themselves?
This is the coordinated silent dance that if we look at in neurobiological terms, we can
really see is all about the autonomic nervous system.
Whether or not it's time to tip the seesaw to one side or the other, depending on whether
or not the other person's seesaw is tilted higher or lower than the other.
Okay, so we have the autonomic nervous system and then we have this thing that we're calling empathy,
which is really about autonomic matching.
And again, the insula and the prefrontal cortex are
neural circuits that are crucial for autonomic matching,
because they allow us to say,
what's out there and do I want to match to it or not?
Okay?
And then the third category of neural circuit
that Helen Fisher and others have found to be important for desire, love and attachment is the Neural Circuit associated with self-delusion.
What do we mean by that? Well, first of all, self-delusion implies a kind of cynicism about love and attachment.
And I think it was George Bernard Shaw that said, love is really about overestimating the differences between individuals
um... actually when i hear that and as i say it i really don't like that quote
uh... i've no bone to pick with george brinard shaw but what it
suggests and i think what he meant was that uh... you know in love and attachment
we tend to put so much value in the other that we forget that
many of the processes that are going on in our brain and body actually could be evoked
by many other people too.
But I think it somewhat overlooks the enormous power of attachment and the ways in which somebody
smells, somebody's voice, somebody's everything or somebody's particular thing or things can
really become so vital for our
autonomic nervous system to feel soothed and to feel elated, etc.
So I think that while the quote is accurate in the one sense, I think it does overlook
the neural circuits for attachment and just how deeply wired those can become for us.
So I will balance that quote with an enormous number of other quotes that I won't mention,
but that you can find out there, they're really point to how incredible the person is that
one tends to be attached to, that there's really only one or several of people that could
ever exist, that could evoke those feelings from us.
And of course, you can read your Ner you know, Neruda poetry and you're, you can find these
things all over the place in music and poetry and writing.
So for every cynical quote about these neural circuits being generic and could be activated
by anybody, I think you'll find an ample number of opposing quotes that these neural circuits
can only be activated by that special someone
or that particular person or maybe just a small set of those people.
So what of delusion?
Well, the work of Helen Fisher and others has really pointed to the fact that desire,
love and attachment are three separate phases of what we call romantic relationships that typically, not always,
but typically desire tends to come first or falls in the early phase, that the process
of romantic slash sexual interactions, it doesn't necessarily have to be sex itself, but
certainly something that involves intimacy of some kind, right, and generally touch of some kind,
eventually transitions into what we call love, which eventually transitions into what we
call attachment.
And I should just mention touch because touch is a fundamental aspect of this whole process.
There's an article, a research article, I should say.
A title of it is, relationshiphip-specific encoding of social touch
in somatosensory and insular courtesies,
courtesies being cortex.
Cortex is plural, courtesies.
And again, there's our friend, the insula.
So this is a study that explored what brain areas
and what body areas are activated
by specific forms of attachment and social touch.
What they found, not surprisingly, is that the areas of the brain they're associated with
touch, the somatosensory areas, but more interestingly, the insula cortex are strongly
activated by touch.
Touch in the amount of touching and proximity and skin contact not surprisingly activates
brain areas associated
with somatosensory touch, but also the insular cortex, which again is this brain area that
links the internal or feelings about what's going on inside us and at the surface of our skin
with events external. And they found activation of number of brain areas, the amygdala,
orbital frontal cortex, and so on and so on.
That's not as essential as just understanding that the insula is the place in which we start to
take our experience of our internal landscape, attach that to our perceptions of the external landscape, and then assign that a value, or assign that some sort of interpretation. And positive
delusion is predictive of long-term attachment. What do we mean And positive delusion is predictive of long term attachment.
What do we mean by positive delusion? Positive delusion is the contradiction of that George
Bernard shock quote. It's the belief that only this person can make me feel this way.
This other person holds the capacity to make me feel this way physically or emotionally or both.
And so, as we move from desire to love to attachment, our brain circuitry is literally
getting tuned up such that that individual that we happen to be attached to.
Again, here thinking about monogamous relationships, but I guess for non-monogamous relationships, you'll be individuals,
is an R the way in which our autonomic nervous system can be regulated.
They actually get access to our control panel.
So it's our autonomic nervous system, empathy, and this positive delusion.
Now positive delusion is critical.
If you look at the stability of relationships over time, something that's
been extensively studied mainly by psychologists, but now also by neurobiologists, what you
find is that there are some key features of interactions between individuals that predict
that a relationship will last. And those are many, but mainly fall under this category
of positive delusions. I'll return to those and what those exactly are many, but mainly fall under this category of positive delusions.
I'll return to those and what those exactly look like. But there are also just a handful of things
that predict that relationship will fail over time. This is largely the work of the Gatmans. It's
actually a husband and wife team up at the University of Washington in Seattle. The Gatmans have
run a laboratory
in the Department of Psychology for a long time.
They've also done a lot of public facing work
around relationships.
And they've talked about the various aspects
of relationships and interactions between people
that predict either staying together or breaking up.
So much so that they've established a method
by which they can look at video interactions
between couples and with a very high degree of certainty predict whether or not those couples
will stay together or break up over time. And they've identified what they call the four
horsemen of relationships. These are things that essentially almost always predict that a couple will break up. And I think the current number on this is that
Gottman can predict divorce with 94% accuracy,
which if you think about it, is pretty remarkable.
So even though these are purely psychological studies,
I'm not aware of any analysis of underlying physiology.
There are some things that they can observe between couples
that can lead them
to predict whether or not a couple will stay together or break up with 94% accuracy.
So what are those things?
Those four behaviors, what they call the four horsemen of the apocalypse, of four relationships,
are one criticism, two defensiveness, three stonewalling, and four contempt, with contempt being the most powerful predictor of breaking up.
Criticism, of course, does not mean that there's no place for criticism in stable relationships.
Of course, there is. I have to do with how frequent and how intensely that criticism is voiced.
Defensiveness, of course, is defensiveness. We know as
the sort of lack of ability to hear another or to adopt their stance. So lack of empathy, I think is a one way to interpret defensiveness.
Stonewalling, which is actually another form of lack of empathy. It's a turning off of this neural circuit that's so critical for desire, love, and attachment.
The stonewalling essentially means the emotional response or the request of another is completely cut off.
So it's, I don't think there's been brain imaging of this, but I think we can reasonably imagine that it involves
untethering your insula response from the other and what they're
dealing with and focusing your insular response, no pun intended, on your own internal state
or perhaps the state of someone else entirely.
I'll talk about infidelity in a moment.
And then contempt.
And contempt has actually been referred to as the sulfuric acid of relationship, I didn't
say that, but gotman and colleagues have,
that it is such a powerful predictor
of divorce and breakups in the future.
And contempt, of course, by definition,
is the feeling that a person or thing
is beneath consideration, worthlessness,
or deserving scorn.
And apparently they can identify this in the videos
of couples having discussions and interacting by very elaborate eye roles, by expressions of anger in one individual when
their partner is actually expressing enthusiasm or excitement about something.
It's the, oh yeah, you would say that, or deep-seated resentment toward the other.
So much so that it's apparent that one kind
of actively dislikes the other partner.
So contempt, disregard for something that should be taken into account is the other way to
think about it.
It runs counter to all of the neural circuits, all three of the neural circuits that we talked
about before.
It certainly is the antithesis of empathy. It is anything but a positive delusion.
It's really looking at the other individual,
either accurately or inaccurately,
as somebody that you kind of despise.
And then it is an absolute inversion
of the autonomic seesaw matching
that I was talking about before.
It's a dissociating of your seesaw from their seesaw.
They're very excited about something you're unexcited by it. In fact, it's anating of your seesaw from their seesaw. They're very excited about something. You're
unexcited by it. In fact, it's an inversion of their seesaw where they're excited, you're down.
They're down, you're up. Okay, so it's a basically an inversion of all of the neural circuits that
Helen Fisher and others have identified as critical for desire, love, and attachment, and therefore it's not surprising that it is so strongly predictive of breakups and in the case of married couples of divorce.
For those of you that are interested in the work of the Gopmans and similar work,
they've written several popular books. They're fairly easy to find. We can link to one of those in the caption, but they've also developed some quite interesting online resources
in their so-called love lab.
I guess it's fortunate that they didn't call it the hate lab
or the breakup lab because they focused a lot on what predicts breakups,
but they've also written extensively and researched extensively
in peer-reviewed studies what makes people find appropriate partners for them
and to maintain those partnerships over time.
So you can go, you can search for LoveLab,
University of Washington, Gotman,
or any number of their various books.
I think you'll find some useful resources there.
I want to shift back to the work of Helen Fisher. She's made some
very interesting statements and some very interesting observations that at least to my mind
map very well onto the knowledge of neural circuitry both in humans and in nonhuman primates
and in other species. I realized that she's not the only name in the game, but she's made some observations
that I think are very, as we say, parsimonious, meaning they allow us to organize a lot of this stuff
into some distinct frameworks. She's also done some really beautiful studies that involve data from
millions or even tens of millions of individuals on dating sites. So I'm going to share those with you in a moment.
But before I do that, I just want to paraphrase Dr. Fisher,
who said that sex drive or desire,
the pursuit of someone to mate with,
meaning to mate the verb,
not necessarily to find a mate,
may be, she didn't say definitively, but may be a way to forage
for potential love partners.
That the arc of this whole business is really the order that we're describing it, that
it's desire, then love, and then attachment.
And that oftentimes people can get confused.
You may know some of these people.
You may be one of these individuals who might confuse desire for attachment or might confuse
love for attachment, but that there's a sequence of recruitment of these neural circuits
that's established first from the pursuit of someone to mate with.
And she's places in the context of kind of more modern dating themes where,
depending on culture, people might explore several, maybe many, many, individuals before, quote,
unquote, settling down with somebody, at least for some period of time. I think that's an interesting framework because it circumvents a lot of the frankly unanswerable
questions about whether or not humans were meant to be monogamous or whether or not they weren't.
Those are conversations that hold cultural context, that hold all sorts of contexts,
that really can't be addressed in a laboratory setting. but this idea that sex drive is a way to
forage for potential love partners and that love is a kind of a litmus test for whether
or not longer or term or deeper attachments can and will form is one that at least makes
sense to me.
Later in the episode, we'll talk about this notion of sex drive and desire.
I'll actually talk about some tools that have very strong data really to
support them in terms of things that people can do or take to increase libido, both men and women,
because there's actually quite good data on that now. But in the meantime, I want to talk about
some of the work that Dr. Fisher has done in terms of categorizing people into, again, we have four groups.
These are distinct from the ABC and D attachment styles described earlier, although as I described
them, you might be able to map them somewhat onto those.
These four groups are groups that were defined through her studies of people that were or are, I don't know if they were or if they are still on
Match.com, but a very extensive data set. So again, millions if not tens of millions of individuals the number
I heard her quote and I
Forgive me if this is not accurate is that in upwards of 40 million individuals
In terms of whether or not their neurochemical and hormone systems
are tuned toward particular types of behaviors.
What do I mean by that?
Well, both men and women, males and females have both testosterone and estrogen.
Typically, again, these are averages, but typically men have more testosterone than they do estrogen
and they have more testosterone than do women, unless estrogen than do women.
Typically women have more estrogen than they do testosterone.
Again, averages, and they have less testosterone than men, more estrogen than men, and so
on and so forth.
But both hormone systems are active in both sets of individuals.
And, of course, all humans as far as we know manufacture both dopamine and serotonin.
Dopamine, as I mentioned earlier, has a number of effects in the brain and body, but one
of the primary effects is that it places us into states of motivation and pursuit for
various things. There is a somewhat close relationship between the dopamine system and the testosterone system
in the hypothalamus, this brain area above the roof of your mouth, and the pituitary gland,
which is responsible for making hormones that make the ovaries and or testes secrete testosterone
or estrogen.
So there's a lot of signaling that occurs, such that dopamine and testosterone tend to operate
as kind of close cousins in a system of pursuit.
And conversely, the serotonin system tends to, on average, collaborate with the estrogen
system to impart certain physiological functions and behaviors.
So these aren't hard and fast, or I guess better stated,
these aren't strict black and white categorizations,
but I think those general themes hold
when you look at the animal and human data.
So Dr. Fisher has taken some liberties,
but I think they are what I would call logically and scientifically
and neurobiologically grounded liberties in
classifying individuals who are on these dating sites according to the types of things they report about themselves and the type of people
they tend to match with on these dating sites and
created these four categories
the four categories are, she calls one, the dopamine category. So these
are people who would have high dopamine. And again, that's just a name. It doesn't mean
they have low anything else, but they are high on the dopamine scale. People who rate
high on the dopamine scale tend to be what the scientists and psychologists call
high sensation seeking, novel seeking. They like new things, they like spontaneity, they tend
to be very adventurous.
And I think that's largely true.
If you look at conditions where dopamine is super physiological, it's elevated beyond
normal levels, things like mania, or when people take certain drugs of abuse, like
cocaine or infetamine, that really raise dopamine levels up very, very high for some period
of time.
They do tend to increase energy motivation and novelty seeking.
And of course, drugs like infetamine and cocaine have all sorts of deleterious effects that
I don't need to go into here, but it's worth pointing out. But they don't tend to make people calm and relaxed and seek soothing interactions.
Conversely, the group that Dr. Fisher calls the serotonin group tend to be more grounded
in soothing activities, quiescent type activities.
They actually tend to be on average, they tend
to like rules and follow rules, they tend to be home bodies, this sort of thing.
They're really, you can imagine them the sort of stable types, but they really like stability.
They're not really into spontaneity as much.
Again, averages.
And then she created two other categories, the testosterone category of high testosterone. This again could be males or females.
And then the estrogen category again could be males or females.
And she gave these different names that I won't go into here.
You can look up her work online, but she, you know,
names like the director and the follower and things like that.
But I don't really want to use those as much as I want to stick to the biological terms.
So we have dopamine serotonin, testosterone and estrogen.
Now that might seem like an unfair kind of over-generalization, but what's interesting
is not necessarily the name or the neurochemical system, right?
Those could have just been called category one, two, three, and four for all that matters
here.
What is interesting is seeing how those different groups of individuals that she absolutely
can categorize based on their self-reported preferences about behaviors and certain kinds
of interactions, how those groups tend to pair up with people in the same or opposite
categories.
So what her studies reveal is that people that fall into the high sensation seeking novelty,
seeking spontaneity category, the one that she calls the high dopamine category, tend
to pair up with, at least in the short term, tend to pair up with people who are also in
that dopamine-inergic category.
So these would be people that would spontaneously take a trip or explore
something new or new restaurant.
They tend to be creative and explorative types.
So that group on average tends to date and
mate and potentially form long-term relationships within category.
Again, averages.
Individuals that she placed into the Saratone group or the what she hypothesized would be a
high Saratone group.
Again, they didn't measure Saratone, but people that tend to place value on stability,
on rules, on certain forms of kind of traditional organization at home and in relationships. Those people also tended to
pair up with select date we presume mate with and form stable relationships with people in the same
category. Now individuals in the other two categories, the high testosterone group, and again,
testosterone wasn't measured, but she called it the high testosterone group, but these are people that
tend to be
very directive. They tend to know what they want and are comfortable telling other people
what they want and from them. These are individuals that
in her studies and in other studies
tend to be a little bit challenging, meaning they
not necessarily challenging to be around, but they tend to be a little bit challenging, meaning they not
necessarily challenging to be around, but they tend to challenge other people, kind of push
them in order to expand their boundaries, either for sake of the relationship or just in general.
And the people they tend to push are the people that they pair up with, which are the people
in the estrogen category, what she called high estrogen.
Again, they didn't measure estrogen, but the people in the estrogen category were the ones that describe themselves and their choices in life
and their preferences. As being nurturing, they actually seem to like it when someone else
is making the major decisions, not every decision. They certainly like to be heard, of course,
in terms of their preferences, but that those two types, what she called
the testosterone and the estrogen type, tend to pair up.
So why are these categorizations and these averages interesting to me, at least interesting
enough to convey to you?
The reason they're interesting to me is, again, not because of their names, these molecules
were not measured in these individuals, but that they once again bring us to the themes
that we addressed before, which are the autonomic nervous system and whether or not it tends to be
shifted more towards alertness in action or more towards kind of a stable equilibrium or more
towards kind of calm and whether or not individuals are selecting for people who have autonomic nervous systems that are more
less like theirs before they even meet. So again, going back to this seesaw analogy, it's almost
like people who have the kind of flat seesaw alert but calm but not extremely alert, not extremely
overly calm in situations, but kind of in the middle seem to be seeking out people
that are also at that kind of autonomic equilibrium.
People in the what she called the dopamine category, which really can just be described as
high sensation, seeking novelty, seeking, they seem to want to pair with one another.
So there's a selection for similar into of the groups, autonomic tone. I find that very interesting because
in that decision or that preference for similar autonomic tone, it essentially eliminates
a lot of the requirement for figuring out how to match one's autonomic nervous system
to another. They simply find someone with a similar tendency, okay?
Whereas in the other two groups that she called testosterone
and estrogen, the director type and the nurturing
kind of somewhat follower type,
there's an establishment of balance,
but not in the between two individuals as a match
but rather on the whole in the relationship.
One person is kind of driving the novelty seeking and the course of decisions and actions,
and the other person is essentially agreeing to those, now assuming that those decisions
are good for both people.
And I emphasize good for both people because one of the themes that Dr. Fisher underscores
that I'd like to underscore here as well is that it need not be the case that people pair up
exactly according to these categorizations
that I've described, dopamine with dopamine,
serotonin with serotonin, testosterone with estrogen
and so on.
What is important is that there be a recognition
and a respect for the other types,
or a recognition and a respect for the fact that both are of the same type.
You can actually imagine, for instance, that two people of this high-sensiation-seeking novelty-seeking
could have a terrifically exciting relationship, but that it actually might be a relationship in which the financial stability isn't quite there,
or in which the basic stability isn't quite there, or in which the basic stability isn't there.
You could imagine, for instance, a situation in which a relationship between two people of what
she called the high serotonin preference would have a relationship that was actually kind of dull,
in which both of them found themselves kind of bored at some point, or in which there wasn't enough of the dynamic tension that sometimes is required in order to keep this cycle of desire,
love, and attachment going, something that we will talk about in a moment.
So the point here is not that one should necessarily pair up according to these arrangements that
I described.
The point is that on average, that's what tends
to happen and that through a recognition that these categorizations exist, similar to
the recognition that the type A, B, C, and D infant and toddler type attachments exist,
that we can gain better self-awareness of who we are and how we tend to show up in romantic attachments.
And thereby navigate healthier, mate seeking healthier breakups if the case dictates it.
And in some cases, healthy long-term relationships by understanding that the other person can either
be similar or complementary to us, One is neither better than the other.
It's simply the case that in all romantic attachments, from the initial inception of the
romantic attachment, desire, love, and attachment, there is an autonomic coordination.
And of course, there is coordination of all sorts of other things like, you know, food
sex and sleep and finances and where people are going to live and many other features.
But that at the core of all that is a seeking of either autonomic likeness or autonomic
differences.
And I think that recognition can be extremely valuable in thinking about tools to enter and
maintain relationships.
If one thinks about their autonomic nervous system, not simply as something that is driven
by external people and events, but that we can actually gain some control over.
Through techniques of the sort that I talked about earlier and on previous podcasts, but
also generally, if we are able to adjust our autonomic nervous
system in order to at least appreciate or get some empathy into what someone else is experiencing,
then we gain actual cognitive empathy.
And this episode isn't about empathy per se, but the theme keeps coming up again and
again.
And I think it's worth mentioning that when you talk to psychologists, whether or not
they're psychoanalysts or from another source of training, what you find is that they don't
talk about empathy as a general term.
They will talk about emotional empathy.
They'll talk about cognitive empathy.
And what I'm talking about here today is that yet a third category that is very strongly
determinant of relationship dynamics. and that's autonomic
empathy.
I'm a biologist, I'm not a psychologist, so I love mechanism.
And fortunately, there are studies that have been done recently using modern techniques
to look at neural mechanisms of romantic attachment.
I mentioned earlier some of the brain imaging studies that have been done on child and mother,
literally imaging the activity of neurons in the brain
as child is nursing or as a mother is soothing baby
and as you learned earlier, baby is soothing mother as well.
Those are remarkable studies.
You may have seen some of these pictures online.
You can see the kind of silhouette of the infant
and mother and their brains and even some of the brain activation patterns.
Really, really beautiful studies.
Similar studies have been done in romantic couples
with those couples either touching one another,
touching and kissing or in kind of clever,
I think control experiments of the person
just touching a pillow or something or kissing a pillow in order to try and create the most reasonable control for what are actually
pretty complicated interpersonal dynamics to do in a brain imaging scanner.
But some of the other studies that have been done recently involved, so-called EEG, so
these are electrical recordings that are done noninvasively, putting a bunch
of electrodes on the outside of the scalp.
EEG is useful in that you can do it noninvasively.
You can do it while people are moving and doing things, kissing, touching, etc.
It doesn't allow one to image or to evaluate neural activity very deeply in the brain,
so you can miss out on a lot of things.
It's sort of like looking at the wave structure on the ocean without actually looking into the depths of
the ocean. So you can miss certain things, but if you see things generally you trust they are there,
but you can't see what you don't see. Nonetheless, there are some studies that I'll just point you to
and that formed the segue for what I'm going to discuss
in a moment, which is a study published in scientific reports
in 2021 entitled Investigating Real Life Emotions
in Romantic Couples, Mobile EEG Studies.
So this is as the title suggests,
having people wear these EEG caps of electrodes,
get engaged in very passionate emotional kisses,
emotional speech toward one another,
standing at different distances.
So a lot of cool stuff that you can do,
that you really couldn't do in a brain scanner.
Because in a brain scanner, people have to be there.
And usually in a bite bar,
there are actually a draw herricks like this.
I've been in one of these things.
There's not a lot of moving around to be had,
at least not using the current technology.
In any case, what they found was there is a shift
in brain waves, brain states, things like alpha waves,
which is a particular frequency of brain waves
in the neocortex, the kind of outer shell of the brain
just beneath the skull.
And in people that are kissing
or in people that are engaged in romantic speech,
or I didn't actually hear what they said to one another,
but what the couple seems exciting, romantic,
and arousing to them,
they see more alpha wave activity
compared to the control conditions.
And there was some what we call lateralization,
where the left hemisphere was more active than the right
and so forth.
And these studies are important because we know that the autonomic nervous systems of individuals tend to start to collaborate and actually synchronize at the level of heartbeats, at the level of breathing during romantic interactions of different kinds.
But these studies are some of the first of their kind
to start looking at neural synchronization between individuals.
Now, the simple version of looking at this,
and the way I would have got this would all go,
was, okay, two people start kissing,
they start talking about what they find particularly romantic
and arousing for them, and their brainwaves will just match to one another,
and that's really the basis of romantic attachment and romantic engagement in that sort of thing.
But it turns out that the opposite is true. So a really nice study published in a really
fine journal, cerebral cortex is a journal that I've known about for many years, they published
strong anatomy, physiology, and neuroimaging.
There's a study that was published first author, Kajimura, in, and this paper really points,
again, this is 2021.
And the title of this paper is Brain Nose, who is on the same wavelength.
Resting state connectivity can predict compatibility of female male relationship.
Now, what this study did was a little bit different. They looked at the resting or default
mode activity of the brain. So rather than evoked activity as it's called where people are kissing
or are engaged in some sort of activity, this was a neuroimaging study, not EEG, but FMRI,
functional magnetic resonance imaging, which is similar
to EEG in principle, but allows you to look deep into the brain.
And it has a very good resolution in time and space.
So fast events can be monitored, and the precise location of those events can be monitored
somewhat better than EEG.
There are exceptions to this.
So for you EEG is out better than EEG. There are exceptions to this, so for EEG or is out there EEG, don't come after me with
electrodes.
Just understand that FMRI gives you a fuller picture of what's going on.
What Kachimura at all found was that contrary to what your reflexive prediction might be, people tend to select people that have resting brain states
that are different than theirs,
or sometimes they found that are actually opposite
to their own resting brain state.
And you might say, well, that doesn't make any sense.
I thought this is all about autonomic coordination.
But actually, if we go back to Helen Fisher's categorizations of the dopamine types, the sensation seeking types, that is serotonin, the kind of stable rule following types, testosterone and estrogen types.
Remember that the two categories that she called testosterone estrogen type, the director and the follower, the nurture, I guess it would be the more accurate way, the director and the nurture. Those tend to pair up across categories, not within category.
And so I think what's really needed for this field, which to my knowledge hasn't happened
yet, is to really start to map the Neurionatomical and Nerophysiological findings of, in this
case, that resting brain state is in one form, in one individual, and they tend to seek out
people whose resting brain state is different than theirs, not similar. That needs to be mapped onto
the more subjective psychological categorizations that Helen Fisher and indeed the gotmins and others
have created. That's sort of the state of the field now. And I mention this, not to confuse you,
but to the contrary, to illustrate that it's not just about finding someone just like you, and it's not
just about finding someone who's opposite to you. This is actually the reason that I decided to
become a biologist at some point in my life, which is that we can find verbal sayings and stories and
examples to support just about
anything. This is not a knock on the field of psychology, as you can probably tell from
today's episode, I have great respect for in reverence for the field of psychology, especially
its collaboration with North Science and vice versa. But in the popular culture, we can
find examples and sayings that support essentially anything as it relates to a relationship.
For instance, I've heard, and you've probably heard, absence makes the heart grow fonder,
and indeed I've experienced that, and I believe it's true.
But I also have experienced, and I believe to be true, that out of sight, out of mind
also exists, and that there will be a biological mechanism for that.
The point here is that matching of same to same or same to different can both be
effective in creating the desire, love, attachment process. It's a matter of who is looking for
same and who is looking for different. And there I think Dr. Fisher and the work of these
neurophysiologists and brain imagers really does point in a direction whereby there is
not one form of attachment that is going to be wholly above all else and will predict
good outcomes.
There is not going to be a case in which opposites attract and that's always the best rule
to follow.
Sometimes it will.
Sometimes it won't.
There is also not the case that people tend to pair up with similar. Sometimes it will be the case. Sometimes it won't. There is also not the case that people tend to pair up with similar. Sometimes it will
be the case, sometimes it won't. Now, there are certain statistics that support that statement.
For instance, people on average, people pair up with individuals of similar educational
background, income, and attractiveness. That is true on average, but it's not always the case.
And again, a knowledge of and a respect for the different categorizations of attachment,
the different categorizations of mate seeking described by Fisher and others, and the recognition
that matching of autonomic nervous systems but also mismatching of resting state brain
networks are all at play in driving what we are calling desire, love, and attachment.
So in keeping with the exploration of the fact that there's a saying or a book or a song
or an example of pretty much any relationship dynamic, I want to now talk about an article
that came out a little over 10 years ago that talked about the universality of love
and the ability to fall in love. So this would be very much in line with the George Bernard Shaw
quote that I mentioned earlier that love is really
overestimating the differences between individuals.
And again, I should say that is not something
that I personally believe,
although maybe I'm just deluding myself.
I like to think that the people that we fall in love with
are really special for us,
that they could not easily be replaced with anybody else.
That's simply my stance.
I'm not basing that on any hardcore neurobiological mechanism, but nonetheless, an article was
published in The New York Times in 2015 that related to some psychological studies that
were done as well as some clinical work work as well as some what I would call
Pop psychology or things that fall outside the domains of academic science and the
the whole basis of this article was
36 questions that lead to love and
It involved a listing out indeed of 36 questions
Set divided into set one set set two, and set three,
that progress from somewhat ordinary questions
about life experience and self-report to more,
let's call them deep questions about people's values
and things that are emotionally close to them.
And I'll just give an example of a few of these.
You can find this easily online by just putting
into your search engine 36 questions that lead to love.
Some of the questions in set number one
were, for instance, what would constitute a perfect day
for you, for what in your life do you
feel most grateful, kind of standard questionnaire stuff?
In set two, what is your most treasured memory?
Was your most terrible memory?
So these are, as you can tell, are drilling a little bit deeper into one's personal experience and emotional system. And then set three questions
25 through 36 are things, you know, what is a very embarrassing moment in your life? When did you
last cry in front of another person and by yourself? What is something that's too serious to be joked about?
So it's going deeper into one's emotional system.
And even questions like all the people in your family,
whose death would you find most disturbing and why?
So pretty, pretty heavy stuff there at the end.
Now, the reason this article got so much traction and the reason I'm bringing up
today is that there was a
statement that was made in and around this article that if two people went on a date
or simply sat down and asked each other these questions and each answered these questions
and the other was paying attention carefully and at some level emotionally responding or not responding,
but certainly paying attention to the answers of the other person, that by the end of that
exchange where one person asks 36 questions and the other person answers all 36 and then
the other person asks all 36 and the other person answers all 36, that they would fall in
love, right? Which
seems like I don't have a ridiculous thing. And yet, it is the case that people who go
through this exercise report feeling as if they know the other person quite well and feeling
certain levels of attachment or even love and desire for the other person that they would not have predicted,
excuse me, would not have predicted had they not gone through that process. So what's going on in
this exchange of questions and answers of a progressively more emotional and deep level?
Well, what I predict is going on is that inside of that exchange, people are creating a sort of delusional story about the nature
of the exchange being a reflection of some deeper attachment.
And so even though people are just exchanging words, they're not physically touching, they
are not at the point where they're running these kind of questionnaire studies, they may
touch afterwards for all I know
and probably did in some cases,
but they're not exchanging life experience in an immediate way.
They're not actually going off into the world
and doing things together yet.
They are simply exchanging narrative,
but we know based on recent studies,
and I've covered this before on this podcast,
but I'll mention again, there was a study
published in Cell Reports, a cell press journal, excellent journal showing
that when individuals listen to the same narrative, their heart rates tend to synchronize or at
least follow a very similar pattern, even if they're not in the same room listening to
a given narrative. Whereas in this case, people are facing one another, listening to the narratives
of each other, certainly they are having autonomic responses. And it stands to reason that
their autonomic nervous systems are synchronizing much in the same way that the cell report
study found that people will synchronize their autonomic nervous systems to a shared herd
story from another. In other words, whether or not we hear a story, watch a movie,
listen to a song, or exchange our own individual stories, our autonomic nervous systems have the
potential to map on to one another. So I'm not all that surprise that people find that they fall
in love in quotes after answering these questions to one another, because essentially the way these
questions are laid out is they establish a narrative.
They establish a very personal narrative, and the other person is listening very closely,
and we don't have physiological or brain imaging studies to support what I'm about to say,
but the reasonable interpretation is that that's causing some sort of autonomic synchronization. So, if you want to try this on a date or even it's actually been hypothesized that this
could be useful for existing couples, even if they already know the answers to some of
these questions, and that doesn't surprise me either.
I think the autonomic coordination is present during mating behavior, it's present during
shared experience of the outside world, movies, concerts, watching
one's children with somebody else, etc.
And it's established by sharing one's own narrative of their own personal experience.
So I don't want to seem overly reductionist.
I'll never propose that all of our sensation, perception, action, and experience in life
boils down to us just being bags of
chemicals and the action of those chemicals or any aspect of our nervous system.
And yet, in looking across the psychological literature of development of attachment, in
the psychological literature of adult and romantic attachment, and what makes and breaks
those attachments, it's very clear to me, and I think courses through the literature at multiple levels
that autonomic coordination is absolutely key for the establishment of desire, love,
and attachment.
In fact, I talked earlier about how our actual conception is born out of autonomic coordination
of one sort or another.
So again, it doesn't necessarily mean that autonomic nervous systems
always be synchronized. In the case of the two categorizations that Fisher proposed of the
director slash testosterone type and the nurturing follower slash estrogen type, it was actually
the coordination, but in opposite directions of individuals that fall into each
of those categories that led to more stable attachments or the seeking out of those attachments,
I should say.
But nonetheless, it's at least to my mind very clear that autonomic coordination is a
hallmark feature of desire, a hallmark feature of what we call love, and a hallmark feature of what we call love and a hallmark feature of what we call
attachment and that the breaking of attachments were the failures of desire,
the failures of love and the failures of attachment over time in line with the
work of Gottmann and others and even just simply what's required for mating
behavior is also reflected in the autonomic nervous system.
But in that case, a failure to coordinate the autonomic nervous systems in some sort of
concerted way.
Any discussion about desire, love, and attachment would be incomplete if we didn't talk about
the dreaded infidelity and cheating.
Much has been made of infidelity and cheating and whether or not people who are higher on
dopamine and sensation seeking tend to cheat more or less.
Frankly, I don't think there's any solid evidence for that.
I think there are a lot of examples that we can draw from in our own lives and in the
lives of others that would generally support one or the other model, but I'm not aware of
any decent physiological studies or psychological studies that really point to that.
For instance, I would never say that the serotonergic phenotype as described in my Fisher is less
prone to cheat, or that the people who have an insecure attachment are more likely to cheat
at, for instance.
I don't think those correlations have been drawn in any kind of meaningful way yet.
So I would be cautious about assigning them without that evidence.
However, there are some interesting studies involving, again, neuroimaging and some subjective
measures in humans, meaning asking them questions that there are good ways to tease out lies
from truths in these sorts of studies.
And whether or not people tend to find their partner
or others more or less attractive,
depending on how people feel about themselves.
And I think this is a very interesting aspect
to desire love and attachment for the following reason.
You hear a lot out there that in order to form a really strong relationship, you have to
have a good relationship with yourself, or you have to love yourself.
You often hear, for instance, that it's exactly when you're not looking for a relationship
that you're going to find what you hear this stuff, right?
But none of that is really grounded in any studies.
Again, that's like out of sight out of mind
or absence makes the heart grow fonder.
There are many life examples to support those statements
and there are many life examples to support
statements to the opposite.
There's a particular study that I found,
this was published in Frontiers in Psychology,
but it's an experimental study
that involves neuroimaging.
The title of this study is Manipulation of Self-Expansion, Alters Responses to Attractive
Alternative Partners.
And I love the design of this study.
What they did in this study is they took couples and they evaluated members of that relationship
for what's called Self-expansion.
Now self-expansion is a metric that involves
one's perception of self as seen through
the relationship to the other.
And this is something that was developed by
the authors are Aaron and Aaron,
so they have the same last name.
So I'm assuming this was either a sibling team or a somehow related team or a romantic couple
team, ARON and ARON.
Aaron and Aaron, in 1986, proposed the self-expansion model of close relationships.
They proposed that people are motivated to enter relationships, I'm reading here, in
order to enhance the self and increase self-efficacy.
In other words, that one of the reasons why many people enter relationships is that it makes us feel good about ourselves
and more capable. And I would see that as a healthy interdependence, not necessarily co-dependence.
This is especially strong at the beginning of a relationship. It turns out when people are forming pair bonds. And it's the case that pleasure, arousal, and excitement, again, all hallmark features
of autonomic nervous system function, pleasure, arousal, and excitement, give rise to self-expansion,
meaning to self-efficacy.
So what this self-expansion model is really about is how great other people that we are
close to and romantically
attached to can potentially make us feel in terms of what they say, in terms of what
they do, in terms of the way in which we believe they feel about us.
So it doesn't necessarily have to involve explicit statements of them telling us how great
we are or them doing great gestures for
us, but how we actually feel they feel about us.
Turns out to be a very strong parameter in terms of how we feel about ourselves and the relationship
overall.
Now, some of you out there are probably thinking, oh, yeah, it isn't there.
This thing, the love languages, right?
I don't have any neuroscience to support that.
I think the love languages, I'm not super familiar with this.
I didn't list it out, but that some think the love languages I'm not super familiar with this I didn't listed out but that some people are
Their autonomic nervous system if you will tends to be very responsive to gifts or to
quality time or to physical touch or acts of kindness
I think I've got a few of these right. I probably have a few wrong anyway
They're easy to find online and people do tend to have a to have a bias toward two or three of these things that are especially
meaningful for them.
And when I hear meaningful I hear, they tend to push the autonomic nervous system and
neurochemical systems of the brain embody in a direction that makes us feel good as
opposed to lousy or neutral.
In any event, this study looked at whether or not people have high levels of self expansion through the actions or statements
of their significant other, and how that influences their perception of people outside the relationship,
meaning how attractive they perceive people outside the relationship to be, turns out to be strongly influenced by A, whether or not their self-expansion is
very strongly driven by the other person that they are involved with, that they're in
the romantic relationship with, and whether or not that's being expressed to them.
So here's how the study went.
First of all, they rated or categorized individuals on the basis of the self-expansion metric.
Some people have more of a potential to experience self-expansion through others, right?
Some of us feel great about ourselves and we're kind of topped off at the others.
Don't feel so great about themselves, but they can feel much better in response to praise
and particular praise or self-expansion type behaviors or statements from people that we
really care about.
And still other people are a mixture of the two,
the kind of moderate levels of both.
So they rated them on this scale.
And then they had people experience self-expansion narratives.
They heard their significant others say
really terrific things about them
and about the relationship in particular,
that the relationship that they have
was exciting, novel, and challenging. So that was one form of self-expansion. them and about the relationship in particular, that the relationship that they have was
exciting, novel, and challenging.
So that was one form of self-expansion.
And they went into some detail as to why that was the case in their particular relationship.
Or they heard a narrative from us, from their significant other, about strong feelings
of love between the two that had been experienced previously in the relationship.
So in the one case, it sort of directed more towards them.
And in the other case, it's more about the relationship itself.
And then they did brain imaging of one person in the relationship, while that person assessed
the attractiveness of people outside the relationship.
And what they found was that people who were primed for this self expansion had lower
activation of brain areas associated with assessing others attractiveness, then did
the people who experienced a lot of self expansion.
Now the takeaway from that, at least the way I read this study, is if you're with somebody
who really benefits from or experiences a lot of self-expansion,
unless you really want them to pay attention to the attractiveness of other people, it
stands to reason that they would benefit from more self-expansion type gestures or statements.
Not so much centered on the relationship.
We have such a great relationship.
There's so much love.
It's so great.
That too, but in the context of this study and these findings, that the person is really
terrific, that the relationship that they've created together is really exciting, novel
and challenging, that there's a narrative around the relationship that really has a lot
to do with the dynamics between the individuals in particular, that the person who really
likes self-expansion
is vital to that dynamic.
Okay, so it's not looking down at the relationship as a set of equals.
There is sort of this bias written into this of that this person is really essential for
the relationship.
I'm not saying this is something that anyone has to do.
I'm not saying this is right or wrong.
This is just what the data say.
But what's remarkable is that in the absence of those statements, people who have or that rate high on
this scale of self-expansion rate attractive alternative partners as more attractive. Now, that's
interesting to me because it means that their actual perception of others is changing. It's not
that their opportunity to see others is changing, right? This is not
a matter of them somehow getting access or no access to attractive alternative partners.
Again, attractive alternative partners, literally the language in the title of this paper.
They're still seeing all these attractive people. It's just that if they're feeling filled
up in air quotes, psychologically filled up, emotionally filled up, autonomically filled, enhanced in
the language that we're using today, by the self-expansion narrative, will then the same
set of attractive faces appear less attractive to a given individual.
Now whether or not this predicts cheating or loyalty, I certainly can't say that would
be very hard to assess in it in neuroimaging.
And there, of course, people rarely, if ever, report accurately their cheating behavior.
There are some studies in which confidentiality is assured to the point where people seem
to be more trusting and willing to reveal cheating behavior. But if you look at the statistics
on cheating behavior, it's very hard to track because people lie all the time about their cheating in and outside of the context of
psychological and neuroimaging studies. But I find this study, again, the title,
manipulation of self-expansion, alters responses to attractive alternative partners to be absolutely
fascinating because, again, it points to the fact that the interactions with our significant others,
shapes are autonomic arousal, shapes are perception of self, and thereby shapes are perception of
other potential partners in the outside world, or shuts us down to the potential of other people
in the outside world. So when I hear statements such as it's important that you love yourself in order to really fall in love with somebody else. Or it is when one is
not looking for a relationship that they're most likely to fall in love and
form a stable relationship. I can filter that through these findings to say that
it's really the person who needs a lot of self-expansion, stimulating statements or actions coming from
other people that is most prone to seeing other potential partners out in the world as attractive.
And in this sense, we can return to the autonomic nervous system as kind of a glass that can
be filled up through various contexts.
It can be filled up through our own ability to regulate it.
It can be filled up through other people's ability to enhance our sense of well-being.
And in some sense, this points to an idea where it is true that the better that we can
feel about ourselves in the absence of any self-expansion type input from somebody else, really
does place us on more stable
ground such that when we do receive that praise or we do receive those acts of
kindness or service or physical touch or whatever they are that we are able to
to further enhance the way that we feel but that we don't necessarily tether all
of our feelings of self-worth or self-expansion to that one individual.
So you might think that if Person A can only receive the self-expansion from the statements
from the action of the person they're involved with, Person B, that will form a very stable
bond.
But what this study points to is the fact that that's a very unstable bond.
That Person A is actually very susceptible to the attractiveness of others because they're
so desperately attached to this notion of self-expansion, even if they don't realize it.
This really does point to the idea that while it is important to link our autonomic nervous
systems to establish desire, love, and attachment, that we want to have a stable internal representation of ourselves,
a stable autonomic nervous system to some degree or another so that we can be in stable romantic
partnership with another individual if that's what we're really trying to do.
So until now, I've been weaving together studies from the field of experimental psychology and
the fields of neuroscience in particular neuroimaging. But if you recall back to the very beginning of the episode, when I was discussing how
odors and how hormones and how even birth control can shape people's ratings of attractiveness
of others, you'll realize that there's a deeper layer to all this, which is that our biology that resides
below our conscious awareness, things like our hormones, things like pheromones even, are shaping
the way that we choose, interpret, and act with other potential romantic partners or the romantic partners that we already have.
Now, this cannot be overemphasized.
No matter how much we would like to create a sort of top down description, meaning from the cortex and our understanding of things,
onto what we find attractive, who we find attractive, what we enjoy, what we don't enjoy in the pursuit and
romantic interactions with others.
There always seems to be and indeed there always is a
deeper layer in which our subconscious processing
drives us to find a particular person to be particular attractive or
in which we have chemistry with somebody or in which
we lack chemistry with somebody.
And I would say that one of the more exciting, fascinating, and indeed mysterious aspects
of desire, love, and attachment are those subconscious processes, those things that we call
chemistry.
I mean, people will report, for instance,
that somebody's smell is just absolutely positively
intoxicating for them, or that somebody's smell
is absolutely repulsive to them, and they don't know why.
That the taste of someone's breath,
and I don't mean that in any kind of poetic sense,
I literally mean the taste of somebody's breath,
in some cases, can be very exciting to somebody. And believe it or not, we can taste of somebody's breath in some cases can be very exciting to somebody.
And believe it or not, we can taste each other's breath.
I talked about this in the chemical sensing episode some months back, but we actually have
receptors for taste and smell that engage in coordinated action such that we can't really
separate taste and smell at some level.
And this is especially true when it comes to the formation of romantic relationships and
what we call chemistry.
Now, is chemistry absolutely required for forming stable attachments for love and for desire?
No, of course they're not.
But in general, these are primitive mechanisms that exist in all animals. They exist in special forms in humans, but that they drive us toward behaviors that will,
as the theory goes, lead to love and attachment.
Not always, as Dr. Fisher pointed out, that sex and sex drive is one way to explore potential
love relationships and to explore potential attachments, which
of course are major investments that extend well beyond, you know, one night or a week
or a vacation or even a year.
When we talk about stable attachments, in general, that means long-term attachments in humans.
Now there is a biology to all of that chemistry stuff.
And the studies of oral contraception and
men finding women more attractive at certain phases of their menstrual cycle,
and women finding men more attractive at certain phases of the woman's menstrual cycle,
point to the incredible power of those deeper biological mechanisms.
In the Hubertman Lab podcast, I discuss both science and science-based tools, and so I'd
be remiss if I didn't actually cover some of the tools that relate to those deeper biological
mechanisms.
The hormones testosterone and estrogen are almost always the first biological chemicals and hormones that are mentioned and described and explored when
thinking about desire and love and attachment to for that matter since love and attachment
stem from desire.
I did an entire episode about the biology of testosterone and estrogen and ways to optimize
testosterone and estrogen.
You can easily find that episode at hubermanlab.com.
It's timestamped.
There you can find all sorts of information about how certain behaviors or absence of behaviors
drive up or down testosterone in estrogen.
I also dispel some myths about sexual behavior and things like masturbation and how they relate
to testosterone and estrogen,
as well as some myths about how those hormones change across the lifespan.
I also talk about the role of exercise.
I talk about supplementation, and I also talk a little bit about hormone replacement therapy
although that will be the topic for a future episode.
So if you're interested in the biology of testosterone and estrogen, two hormones that
absolutely influence things like libido and desire, two hormones that absolutely influence things
like libido and desire.
Please check out that episode as well as what I'm going to talk about in just a moment
here.
The simple stereotype version of the hormones, testosterone and estrogen, are that testosterone
drives libido or increases it, aka sex drive, and that estrogen somehow blunts it or is
not involved in libido and sex drive, and that estrogen somehow blunts it or is not involved in libido and
sex drive, and that is simply not the case.
As I describe in that testosterone and estrogen optimization episode, and as I'll tell you
now, yes, testosterone and some of its other forms like the hydro testosterone are strongly
related to libido and sex drive in the pursuit and ability to mate. However,
the hormone estrogen is also strongly associated with libido and mating behavior. So much so that
for people that either chemically or for some other reason have very low estrogen,
libido can severely suffer. So it's a coordinated dance of estrogen and testosterone
in both males and females that leads to libido or sex drive.
So I absolutely want to make clear that it's not
a simple relationship between testosterone and sex drive,
or estrogen and sex drive, both are required
at appropriate ratios.
Now, with that said, there are things that can shift libido
in both men and women in the direction of more desire, or more desire to mate, either
to seek mates or to mate with existing partners. And there's a quite solid literature around
a few of those substances.
Now a common misconception is that because dopamine is involved in motivation and drive,
that simply increasing dopamine through any number of different mechanisms or tools will
increase libido and sex drive.
And that's simply not the case either. It is true that some level of dopamine or increase in dopamine
is required for increases in libido. However, because of dopamine's relationship to the
autonomic nervous system, and because the autonomic nervous system is so intimately involved,
no pun intended, in sexual activity, in seeking and actual mating behavior, as I described earlier.
It's actually the case that if people drive their dopamine system too high, they will be in states of arousal that are high enough such that they seek and want sexual activity, but they can't actually engage the
parasympathetic arm of the autonomic nervous system sufficient to become physically aroused.
Now, there's a whole description of this that awaits us in a future episode, but I'll
summarize now by saying, for people that are taking substances just simply to increase
dopamine, in order to increase libido, that can be a potentially hazardous route to follow because
depending on whether or not that dopamine level is high enough that it puts them into a
mode of seeking mates or mating, but they can't adjust their autonomic nervous system during
actual mating behavior.
What essentially is I'm saying is it can place people into a chronic pursuit, but an inability
to perform sexually.
And this is true for men and women.
So I would just caution people against just thinking, oh, a lack of libido is simply a
lack of dopamine.
That is not the case.
It could be from lower levels of dopamine, but it could also be for other reasons.
These systems, these signaling systems, then these neurochemicals are very intricate.
Just simply ramping up dopamine has actually been found, for instance, in infetamine and
cocaine users.
There is a phenomenon in which they become hyperarous, but can't perform sexually.
This is also true for people who take elevated levels of other recreational drugs or who
take antidepressants that increase the dopamine system too much, right?
Dosage has to be worked out with your physician, with your psychiatrist, such that mood is enhanced
and the various aspects of a healthy well-being, mind and body are enhanced, but not
so much so that what we call the arousal arc is locked with the seesaw in the sympathetic
drive position such that sexual arousal can't occur.
So this is an important point to make because I think that a lot of people are under the impression
that if they just drive up testosterone,
increased dopamine and generally get themselves into high states of autonomic arousal that that's
going to increase the libido.
That's simply not the way the system works.
It's that seesaw and that seesawing back and forth that is the arc of arousal that we
talked about earlier.
Now there are substances legal over the counter substances that fall under the
categorization of supplements that do indeed increase libido and arousal. And so I'm going
to talk about some of those in the context of peer reviewed literature now. I want to
be clear, however, that these are by no means required. Many people have healthy libidos
or have libidos that are healthy for their
life and what they need and want. And as always, in any discussion about supplementation,
you absolutely have to check with your physician. I don't just say that to protect us. I say
that to protect you. Your health and well being is dependent on you doing certain things and
not doing others. And everybody is different. Nonetheless, there are studies that point
to specific substances that are sold over the
counter, that at least in the United States are legal, and that have been shown to be statistically
significant in increasing measures of libido.
There are many such substances, but three that in particular have good peer-reviewed research to support them are Maka, M-A-C-A, which is actually a root,
Tongat Ali also sometimes called Long Jack. I didn't name them, forgive me, and Tribulus,
or Tribulus, it's sometimes called. I'm going to talk about each of these in sequence, but on the whole,
But on the whole, the studies on Maka are quite convincing that consumption of 2 to 3 grams per day of Maka, which generally is sold as a powder or a capsule, typically consumed early in the day because it can be somewhat of a stimulant,
meaning it can increase alertness, and you wouldn't want it to interfere with sleep by taking it too late in the day.
But in studies that include both men and women of durations anywhere from eight to 12 weeks
of athletes and non-athletes and different variations of Maka.
Turns out there's black Maka, red Maka, yellow Maka.
There are a bunch of different forms of Maka, but that they can increase subjective reports
of sexual desire, independent of hormone systems, meaning it does not seem, at least based
on the existing literature, that Maka increases testosterone or changes estrogen, at least
not on the time scales that these studies were done or with the measures that were performed in these studies.
But that Maka again consumed it in doses of anywhere from 2 to 3 grams per day has been
shown to significantly increase libido.
And in fact, those dosages of Maka have been shown to offset so-called SSRI-induced sexual dysfunction.
So there are various routes to sexual dysfunction.
The SSRIs are selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors.
They go by name brands like Prozac and Zoloft
and there are many others now on generic forms and so forth.
Those don't always, I should point out,
lead to sexual dysfunction.
There's a dose dependence.
Some people do quite well on SSRIs and don't have any issues with sexual function.
Other people suffer quite a lot.
From sexual dysfunction, while taking SSRIs, highly variable.
You need to work with a physician, a qualified psychiatrist.
But nonetheless, everything I've been saying about Maka thus far has also been explored
in the context of SSRI and duesectual dysfunction.
The paper that I'm referring to here is a double-blind randomized pilot dose finding study of Maka
root.
It goes by the name L, Mayenny.
These always have fancy names and the Latin names in biology are always more
complicated, but it's Maka root for the management of SSRI and duosectual dysfunction.
First author is Dording D-O-R-D-I-N-G.
This was a study done at Mass General, which is one of the satellite locations around Harvard
Med, associated with Harvard Med, that found significant improvements in libido when people were
taking a pretty low dose.
It was actually, in this case, just 1.5 grams per day, up to a high dose, three grams per day
of maca, and they were doing this in 20 remitted depressed outpatients.
These are people that had depression.
Their depression was successfully treated with S.O.
Ferries, but they were suffering from some of these S.O.
Thorais related sexual effects and Mocca seemed to offset some of those effects significantly
in this population.
The other studies exploring the lack of effect on serum testosterone in adult healthy men
was a 12-week study again consuming anywhere from 1.5 to 3 milligrams, excuse me,
1500 milligrams to 3,000 milligrams or placebo.
So again, this is 1.5 up to 3 grams of macar or placebo and they rated sexual desire,
depression and other measures just testosterone in the blood, again, no change in testosterone or estrogen,
estradiol levels in men treated with maca
and those treated with placebo.
But nonetheless, there was a significant
and positive effect on libido with this dosage
of 1.5 to 3 grams per day of maca.
And there are several other studies
that also show this again in people that are
taking SRIs and people that are not taking SRIs in chronically over trained athletes.
This was also found to be the case.
So it seems like across the board Maka is a fairly useful supplement for those that are
seeking to increase the libido and And there are fewer studies involving women, but there are a few such studies that also
point to the same general positive effect on libido in women taking Maca at equivalent
doses to those I just described.
I think it's noteworthy that Maca supplementation does not seem to adjust testosterone or estrogen
levels to any significant degree, but it does
change libido. I think that points to the fact that there are multiple systems in the brain
and body that influence libido, not just testosterone and estrogen. Indeed, we know that to be the case,
things like PEA, which is a substance found in chocolate and is a substance that some people
supplement is known, for instance, to increase sexual
desire, but also the perception of sexual experiences as more stimulating, for instance.
So there are a lot of pathways in the brain, in particular in the hypothalamus, this ancient
area of our brain that harbors neurons and hormone secreting cells, including neurons, that can shape our perceptions of our, even
just our tactile experience of others and their attractiveness, and indeed can shift levels
of desire, independent of changing levels of circulating hormones.
Another substance that has been shown to increase libido across a range of human populations
is so-calledat ali. I've talked a little
bit about this before on the Huberman Lab podcast in reference to testosterone. And I've
talked about it extensively as a guest on other podcasts. Tonga ali goes by a number of
different names. One of them is exceedingly difficult for me to pronounce. It's Yuri Koma Longifolia, also called Longjack.
But Tonga Ali is the typical name.
This is an herb.
There's a Malaysian version and an Indonesian version.
My understanding is that the Indonesian variety of Tonga Ali
is the one that is most potent for its effects on libido.
Previously, I've talked about Tonga Ali taken in 400 milligram per day capsules as a means
to increase the amount of free, meaning unbound testosterone.
So testosterone has a both bound form and an unbound form.
Very briefly the bound form is bound to albumin in the blood or to so-called sex hormone
binding globulin. When it's bound, it can't be
biologically active at many cells. It is important that some of it be bound
in order to get a sort of time release and proper distribution of testosterone
through the body, but is the unbound free testosterone
that can really have its most potent effects. And there's some evidence
that Tonga Ali
can increase the amount of unbound, so-called free-test
austroan, by lowering sex hormone binding globulant,
although it is almost certain that it has other routes
of mechanism as well.
Nonetheless, there are some reports of Tonga Ali
increasing libido one particular article
Last author or I should say last name of first author excuse me is male is male. This was
published in an evidence-based complimentary and alternative medicine this from
2012 reports a significant
Increase in libido and sexual function. There are other such studies, not a lot of them, not as many robust, controlled, quality-peer
-reused studies as there are from Maka, nonetheless, a number of people, men and women that I know
do take Tonga Ali and it seems to work well for them.
The question always comes up around discussion of supplements.
Do you need to cycle these things? The only way to determine that is really to do your blood work, monitor
liver enzymes, monitor hormone levels, and so forth. So I simply can't say whether or
not you need to or you don't need to cycle them. Typically, Tonga Ali and Maka are not
cycled in any regular kind of way that I'm aware of. But again, you really need to check with your doctor
if you're going to initiate taking any of these things.
And you certainly should do your best to monitor
your blood work as well as subjective measures
in evaluating whether or not they're working for you,
safe for you, and so forth.
The third and final substance slash supplement
that I want to touch on as it relates to libido
is called Tribulus Tarestis.
So that's TRI, B-U-L-U-E-R-R-E-S-T-R-I-S.
This is a commonly sold over-the-counter supplement
for increasing testosterone for fitness purposes
and so on, whether or not it actually does that
to a meaningful degree
is in clear.
But I'm aware of four peer reviewed studies that were focused on both males and females,
ranging anywhere from 18 years old all the way up to 65 plus, they say 65 plus, I guess
it could be 70, it could be 80, I don't know, but fairly broad age range,
where people took anywhere from 750 milligrams per day,
divided into three equal doses.
So 750 total per day, divide into three equal doses
of tribulus or placebo for 120 days. This particular study was focused on females.
And according to the female sexual function index questionnaire, no significant difference
between any of the groups. However, free and bioavailable testosterone increased in the
group taking tribulus terrestis, total testosterone, did not reach statistical significance.
So this is sort of the inverse of what we see with Maka where there do seem to be increases
in testosterone, which would predict that there would be increase in libido.
In this case, in this was postmenopausal women, there was no increase in libido.
There wasn't increase in testosterone.
I mentioned it only because there might be instances in which people want to increase their testosterone. I mentioned it only because there might be instances in which people want
to increase their testosterone. It does seem that tribulus, at least in that population,
is capable of doing that. Now, there's a separate study that was done
a double-blind study lasting anywhere from one to six months that had a clear and significant
increase in libido. Now, this was taking 6 grams, so that's 6,000 milligrams,
of tribulus root for 60 days, and it did seem to increase various aspects of sexual function.
And there was a, what appeared to be a substantial 16.3% increase in testosterone, but in this
particular study because of the variability across individuals that did not actually arrive at statistical significance.
Now there were a number of other studies that explored the role of tribulus in particular
in females.
And one of those studies was a study that was actually quite short.
It was two to four weeks.
It involved 67 subjects.
These were subjects that had experienced a loss of libido and took tribulus divided into
two equal doses compared that to placebo.
They did see a significant improvement in these measures of sexual desire and function
on this female sexual function index.
There is some evidence that tribulus can be effective in increasing testosterone in
certain populations, in increasing sexual desire and function in certain populations, particular
in females.
I think more studies are certainly needed, but these three substances, a slash supplements,
maka, tonga ali, in particular, Indonesian tonga ali, and Tribulus can indeed create significant increases in
sexual desire and in some cases by adjusting the testosterone and estrogen system in some
cases, not by adjusting the testosterone and estrogen system.
Again, pointing to the complexity of neurochemicals and features that adjust things like libido
aka desire.
So we covered a lot of material today related to desire, love and attachment.
And yet I acknowledge that it is not exhaustive of the vast landscape that is the psychology
and biology of desire, love and attachment.
Nonetheless, I hope that you found the information interesting and hopefully actionable in some cases toward
the relationships of your past, of present, and potentially for the relationships of your
future.
If you're enjoying and are learning from this podcast, please subscribe to our YouTube
channel.
That's a very straightforward zero cost way to support us and it really does help us.
In addition, please subscribe to our podcast on Apple and Spotify.
And on Apple, you have the opportunity to leave us up to a five-star review. In addition,
please leave us comments, feedback, and suggestions for future guests in the comment section on YouTube.
We do read all of those comments. Please also check out the sponsors mentioned at the beginning of
today's episode. That is the best way to support the Huberman Lab podcast.
Please also follow us on Instagram and Twitter.
I teach neuroscience and neuroscience related tools on both Instagram and Twitter.
Some of that material overlaps with material covered on the podcast.
Some of it is unique material only covered on Instagram and Twitter.
And as mentioned at the beginning of today's episode, we are now partnered with momentous
supplements because they make single ingredient formulations
that are of the absolute highest quality
and they ship international.
If you go to livemomentus.com slash huberman,
you will find many of the supplements
that have been discussed on various episodes
of the huberman lab podcast,
and you will find various protocols related
to those supplements.
Thank you for joining me for today's discussion
about desire, love, and attachment, and last but certainly not least, thank you for your interest in science.