Huberman Lab - Time Perception & Entrainment by Dopamine, Serotonin & Hormones
Episode Date: November 15, 2021In this episode, I discuss how our brain and body track time and the role that neurochemicals, in particular dopamine and serotonin, but also hormones such as melatonin, allow us to orient ourselves i...n time. I review the three types of time perception: of the past, of the present, and the future, and how dopamine and serotonin adjust both our perception of the speed of the passage of time and our memory of how long previous experiences lasted. I also discuss circannual entrainment, which is the process by which our brain and body are matched to the seasons, and circadian (24 hours) entrainment, both of which subconsciously adjust our perceived measurement of time. I explain the mechanisms of that subconscious control. And I cover the ultradian (90 minutes) rhythms that govern our ability to focus, including how to track when these 90-minute rhythms begin and end for the sake of work and productivity. I include ten tools based on the science of time perception that you can apply to enhance productivity, creativity, and relationships in various contexts. For the full show notes, visit hubermanlab.com. Thank you to our sponsors AG1 (Athletic Greens): https://athleticgreens.com/huberman LMNT: https://drinklmnt.com/huberman Supplements from Momentous https://www.livemomentous.com/huberman Timestamps (00:00:00) Introducing Time Perception, Note on Fasting & Supplements (00:05:28) Sponsors: AG1, LMNT (00:09:25) Entrainment, Circannual Entrainment, Melatonin (00:13:20) Seasonal Oscillations in Testosterone & Estrogen, Tool 1 (00:16:06) Circadian Timing, Tools 1, 2, 3 (for Circadian Entrainment) (00:21:13) Tool 4: Timing Physical Activity; Tool 5: Timing Eating Window (00:23:00) When Circadian Entrainment is Disrupted, Time Perception Suffers (00:25:00) Tool 6: Ultradian (90min) Cycles & Focus (00:31:42) Our Sense of the Passage of Time: Present, Prospective, Retrospective (00:34:40) Dopamine (& Nor/epinephrine) Lead to Time Overestimation; Frame Rate (00:37:18) Serotonin & Time Underestimation; Decreased Frame Rate (00:39:10) Dopamine vs. Serotonin Across the Day; Tool 7: When to Do Rigid vs. Creative Work (00:42:38) Example of Tool 7 (00:43:38) How Sleep Deprivation Degrades Performance (00:44:38) Trauma, “Over-clocking” & Memories; Adjusting Rates of Experience (00:50:04) Why Trauma Involves Dopamine & Epinephrine, Arousal (00:51:03) Dopamine, Spontaneous Blinking & Time Perception; Tool 8 (00:53:38) Deliberate Cold Exposure, Dopamine, Tool 9: Adjusting Frame Rate in Discomfort (00:56:30) Fun “Feels Fast” BUT Is Remembered as Slow; Boring Stuff “Feels Slow,” Recall As Fast (01:00:54) Retrospective Time, Context Variation & Enhanced Bonding with Places & People (01:03:00) Dopamine Release Resets the Start of Each Time Bin on Our Experience (01:07:40) Habits & Time Perception; Tool 10 (Setting Functional Units of Each Day) (01:11:58) Synthesis & Book Suggestion (Your Brain Is a Time Machine by D. Buonomano) (01:12:27) Supporting the HLP: Subscribe, Instagram, Supplements Title Card Photo Credit: Mike Blabac Disclaimer
Transcript
Discussion (0)
Welcome to the Huberman Lab podcast where we discuss science and science-based tools for everyday life.
I'm Andrew Huberman and I'm a professor of neurobiology and
Ophthalmology at Stanford School of Medicine. Today, we are talking about time perception.
Our perception of time is perhaps the most important factor in
how we gauge our life. That is whether or not we think we are being successful,
whether or not we are failing, whether or not we live in fear,
whether or not we live in relation to things
in a way that's positive.
And the reason for that is that our perception of time
is directly linked to the neurochemical states
that control mood, stress, happiness, excitement.
And of course, it frames the way in which we evaluate
our past. So whether or not we think of our past as successful or unsuccessful, it frames our
present. Whether or not we think we are on track or off track. And it frames our sense of the future.
Whether or not we think we have a bright future, a dim future, or whether or not the future is very uncertain or not.
Today we are going to talk about the science of time perception, and we are going to talk
about tools and protocols that you can use that can enhance your ability to dilate and
contract time.
What do we mean by dilate and contract time?
We can control the speed at which we experience life.
We can slow things down or we can speed our experience of life up.
And we can do that in a very direct and dynamic way.
It's actually not that hard.
Once you understand how time perception works.
So that's where we're headed.
I think you're going to come away from today's episode with a lot of new knowledge and certainly
with many tools that you can try in your daily life,
whether or not that's work, sport, relational, emotional, and so on.
Before we begin our discussion about time perception, I'd like to answer some questions that
I received related to the episode on fasting and time restricted feeding.
If you haven't seen that episode, this information should still be of use to you.
Time restricted feeding involves eating for a particular period of time in each 24-hour
cycle.
That's fairly regular.
This would be an eight-hour, most often, or a 10-hour block.
Some people do shorter feeding windows.
But regardless, that feeding window is supposed to fall at more or less the same period within
each 24-hour day.
This has a number of positive effects on gene expression that regulate a number of positive
effects on the different issues of the body. And for some people, not all, but for some people,
makes weight loss easier because of the way that they are not eating for large periods of each
24 hour cycle. In any event, one of the major questions I got after that episode was, do supplements
break a fast? And during that episode, I talked about what breaks a fast is highly contextual.
It basically boils down to whether or not something you ingest, whether it be liquid or food,
increases your resting blood glucose, how much it increases that resting blood glucose,
and how long that increase lasts.
So you can check out the episode for more about what breaks are fast, but to address this issue about supplements and whether or not
supplements, a particular break of fast, many of the questions were about
athletic greens. Athletic greens is a sponsor of this podcast. It is also a
terrific supplement that I had been taking for more than a decade before this
podcast launched. And many people have been using and continue to use
athletic greens does athletic greens break a fast. Well, that will somewhat depend on
whether or not your resting blood glucose tends to run high or low. But for most people,
including me, because I've measured it, ingesting athletic greens does not break a fast. And
if it happens to break a fast, it would be a very transient break in fast. So without
knowing your resting blood glucose levels on an individual basis, there's no way I can say for sure
that it doesn't break a fast, but chances are it does not because it doesn't contain much carbohydrate or sugar
and it doesn't tend to therefore pull you out of the molecular milieu associated with low blood glucose states.
The other question I get is whether or not things like
fish oil break a fast.
And once again, this will be contextual, but because
fish oil is a fat, mainly if it's central fatty acids,
in particular EPA and DHA, those don't tend to raise
blood glucose very much.
In my case, having measured, using it continue with glucose
monitor, my resting
blood glucose, fischoil does not in any way change my resting blood glucose. Chances are,
it won't do that for most people as well. So does fischoil break a fast? Chances are,
it does not. And of course, people wanted to know about pill type supplements, you know,
caffeine and things that raise dopamine and their vitamins and minerals. In general, if something doesn't contain sugar or much carbohydrate of any kind, it's not
going to raise blood glucose very much.
Now, of course, protein can raise blood glucose and fat can too as well, although to a lesser
extent.
So, again, this is all contextual, but at least by the logic that I just spelled out,
athletic greens, fish oil, and most forms of supplements
provided they don't have any sugar or protein content
should not, quote unquote, break a fast.
Before we begin, I'd like to emphasize that this podcast
is separate from my teaching and research roles at Stanford.
It is, however, part of my desire and effort
to bring zero cost to consumer information about science
and science-related tools to the general public.
In keeping with that theme,
I'd like to thank the sponsors of today's podcast.
Our first sponsor is Athletic Greens.
Athletic Greens is an all-in-one
vitamin mineral probiotic drink.
I've been taking Athletic Greens since 2012,
so I'm delighted that they're sponsoring the podcast.
The reason I started taking Athletic Greens
and the reason I still take Athletic Greens once or twice a day
is that it helps me cover all of my basic nutritional needs.
It makes up for any deficiencies that I might have.
In addition, it has probiotics, which are vital for microbiome health.
I've done a couple of episodes now on the so-called gut microbiome and the ways in which
the microbiome interacts with your immune system, with your brain to regulate mood, and
essentially with every biological system relevant to health throughout your brain and body.
With athletic greens, I get the vitamins I need, the minerals I need, and the probiotics
to support my microbiome.
If you'd like to try athletic greens, you can go to atlettagreens.com slash Huberman and
claim a special offer.
They'll give you five free travel packs plus a year supply of vitamin D3K2.
There are a ton of data now showing that vitamin D3 is essential for various aspects of our
brain and body health, even if we're getting a lot of sunshine.
Many of us are still deficient in vitamin D3.
And K2 is also important because it regulates things like cardiovascular function, calcium
in the body, and so on.
Again, go to athleticgreens.com slash uberman to claim the special offer of the 5 free travel
packs and the year supply of vitamin D3 K2.
Today's episode is also brought to us by Element.
Element is an electrolyte drink that has everything you need and nothing you don't.
That means the exact ratios of electrolytes are an element and those are sodium, magnesium
and potassium, but it has no sugar.
I've talked many times before on this podcast
about the key role of hydration and electrolytes for nerve cell function, neuron function,
as well as the function of all the cells and all the tissues and organ systems of the
body. If we have sodium, magnesium, and potassium present in the proper ratios, all of those
cells function properly and all our bodily systems can be optimized. If the electrolytes are not present and if hydration is low, we simply can't think as well
as we would otherwise.
Our mood is off, hormone systems go off, our ability to get into physical action, to engage
an endurance and strength, and all sorts of other things is diminished.
So with element, you can make sure that you're staying on top of your hydration and that
you're getting the proper ratios of electrolytes.
If you'd like to try element, you can go to drinkelement.
That's lmnt.com slash huberman, and you'll get a free element sample pack with your purchase.
They're all delicious.
Again, if you want to try element, you can go to elementlmnt.com slash huberman.
So, let's talk about time perception.
The most fundamental aspect of time perception is something called entrainment.
Entrainment is the way in which your internal processes, your biology and your psychology
are linked to some external thing.
And the most basic form of entrainment that we are all a slave to, all year round for our
entire life are so called
sircanual rhythms.
We have neurons, nerve cells in our eye, in our brain,
and in our body that are marking off the passage of time
throughout the year.
Literally, a calendar system in your brain and body.
And the way this works is beautifully simple. Light, seen by your eyes,
inhibits, meaning it reduces the amount of a hormone released in your brain called melatonin.
Melatonin has two major functions. One function is to make you sleepy at night, and the other
is to regulate some of the other hormones of the body, in particular testosterone and estrogen.
When we view light, we reduce the amount of melatonin released.
In fact, if you wake up in the middle of the night when melatonin typically is pretty
high in your brain and body, and you flip on a bright light in the bathroom, your melatonin
levels crash down to almost zero and stay there. Light is a very powerful modulator of melatonin and light
inhibits melatonin. Throughout the year, depending on where you live, day length varies. And as a
consequence, the amount of light from the sun that is available to you varies. So when days are long,
the amount of melatonin in your brain and body that's released tends to be less.
There's less of it and it's released for shorter amounts of time, okay, because light inhibits melatonin.
When days are very short, the amount of melatonin that's released and the duration that that melatonin exists in your brain and body tends to be much longer.
So melatonin correlates with
day length. And if we are viewing more light, we have less melatonin, we view less light,
we have more melatonin. You see different amounts of light each day, but we have a process
in our brain and body that averages the amount of light that you're seeing, both from
artificial sources and from sunlight, and measures that off. And it's so exquisitely precise that
for a given, say, eight hour day in the spring, because spring in the Northern hemisphere or elsewhere,
you know, days are getting longer, that means that the amount of melatonin is getting progressively less
and less, and that signal is conveyed to all the systems of your brain and body. And this
is why most people, not all, but most people feel like they have more energy in the spring.
Conversely, when you have an eight hour day in the winter, the amount of melatonin that
corresponds to that eight hour day
is getting progressively greater and greater
because why days are getting shorter.
So melatonin is increasing from day to day to day.
Every cell and system of your body pays attention to this
and as a consequence, most people not all,
but most people feel they have a little less
or sometimes a lot less energy
and a slightly lower mood in the winter months.
Now there are exceptions to this, of course, but the melatonin signal is the way in which
your internal state, your mood, your sense of energy, even your appetite is entrained, is
matched to some external event.
In this case, the event is the rotation of the Earth around the sun. There are other forms of entrainment, meaning the matching of your brain and body to things that
are happening in your external environment. One particularly interesting example of this was
published last year by Peric at all in cell report, Cell Press Journal, excellent journal,
showing that across the calendar year, the amount of testosterone and estrogen that human
beings make varies, such that in longer days, they tend to make more testosterone and estrogen
than in shorter days.
This was correlated with things like desire to seek out romantic partners or have romantic
interactions with their existing partners, even aggression, although not violent aggression, but sense of kind of willingness
to argue and to get into kind of combative states and overall energy and mood.
This is something that had been hypothesized for a long time, but it never really been
cleanly demonstrated.
And what they showed was that it's actually the skin that's taking information about
the amount of light and converting it into these increases in testosterone and estrogen.
Light exposure to the skin turns out about two hours a day.
This was sunlight in this case.
To the upper body, these people weren't naked.
They were wearing clothes, but their arms were exposed.
They're upper back and neck and face were exposed.
They were not wearing hats.
Resulted in large increases, significant increases in testosterone and estrogen.
Now, you could probably export a tool from that if you liked.
That's not really what this podcast is about, but it's very clear that because the skin is acting as an endocrine organ,
excuse me, as kind of a hormone influencing organ that getting light on the skin, not just to the eyes,
can influence our sense of well-being
by these hormone pathways.
And the threshold there, again, seem to be about two hours a day.
It doesn't have to be very bright outside.
There can be cloud cover and so on.
Many people will probably ask,
will sunscreen inhibit this effect?
And it doesn't appear that it does.
Obviously prioritize skin health
and avoiding skin cancer.
Sunscreen is kind of a controversial topic nowadays.
Maybe the topic for another podcast episode at some point.
But nonetheless, what the parakeet all study shows
and that's most relevant to today's podcast
is that we are entrained, we are matched
to the external light dark cycle.
And as the day length changes, our hormones change.
And we can override that with exposure to bright lights.
You know, people go sit on tanning beds.
That's not a practice I particularly myself engage in,
but you know, there are a number of different ways
that people can override these processes.
But the point is very simple.
The point is that our perception of time
is both conscious.
You know, it's waiting waiting watching the clock tick down
and they're the slower what we call oscillatory,
meaning up and down repeatedly,
slower oscillatory events related to day length
that are influencing our hormones,
like melatonin testosterone and estrogen
and therefore our mood, our outlook, and even our behavior.
The next level of time or bin of time as we say,
that we are all entrained or matched to is the so-called circadian time cycle, which is 24-hour rhythm. This is perhaps the most powerful rhythm
that we all contain and that none of us can escape from. We all have this circadian clock that
resides over the roof of our mouth. The cells in that circadian
clock fire, meaning they release chemicals into our brain and body on a very regular rhythm. So
across the 24 hour cycle, they will be very active at some periods and less active at others.
Not surprisingly, there are periods of every 24 hour cycle when we
are very active and we tend to be alert and others when we are asleep. I've talked a lot about
circadian rhythms and sleep on this podcast previously and so I don't want to repeat too much
of that information in detail, but I'm just going to give a summary of how circadian
entrainment works because I haven't really covered that in the context of time perception.
We have this circadian clock.
It oscillates, it goes up and down
once every 24 hours and then repeats.
Every cell of our body has a 24-hour oscillation
in the expression of various genes.
How that works is actually really simple, elegant,
and interesting.
DNA, genes, make RNA, RNA is converted into proteins.
Every cell in our body has this beautiful 24-hour timer where a gene is expressed, and the
important thing to understand about a given gene in this context is that that gene is
inhibited, meaning it's reduced by a particular protein,
by a little biological molecule in that cell.
So the gene gets expressed when there's very little of that other molecule around.
DNA then becomes RNA.
RNA is translated into a protein, and that protein goes way, way up, and the gene shuts down.
But as that protein gets used up, and its levels eventually drop low, low, low, low to
zero, the gene cycle kicks in again and the gene gets expressed, the RNA gets expressed
in the protein again.
This all happens on a 24 hour cycle.
So it's a little built in timer in each and every one of ourselves.
And I didn't list off the genes, but for the aficionados out there, they go by names like
per for period, b-mail, clock and all these different things. We call them the clock
genes. And those clock genes regulate a number of different functions. So every cell in
our body has a 24 hour cycle of gene and protein expression. And the earth rotates once every
24 hours. And the processes that are happening in every cell of our body are linked. They are entrained, as we say, to the outside light dark cycle because morning sunlight, evening
sunlight, and the lack of light in the middle of the night, make sure that the changes,
these oscillations that are occurring within the cells of our brain and body are matched to
the outside light dark cycle.
I want to go into all the details of how that and body are matched to the outside light dark cycle.
I want to go into all the details of how that happens, but there are some very simple
tools that one can use to ensure that your entrainment, your circadian entrainment, is
precise.
And I cannot emphasize enough how important it is that your circadian entrainment be precise.
Why?
Because disruptions in circadian entrainment cause huge health problems. They increase cancer risk. They increase obesity.
They increase mental health issues. They decrease wound healing. They decrease physical and mental
performance. They disrupt hormones. You want your cells to be linked to the circadian cycle
that's outside you. And the circadian cycle outside you mainly consists of when there's sunlight and when
there is not.
And that's why the simple protocols to fall out of this whole discussion about circadian
entrainment are the following view 10 to 30 minutes of bright light ideally sunlight within
an hour of waking, assuming that you're waking early in the day, especially you wake up early
in the day, get outside, see sunlight.
Do that again in the afternoon or around evening, 10 to 30 minutes, depending on how bright it is outside,
artificial lights throughout the day, or if you want to be awake and you wake up early,
and there's no sunlight outside, you can of course turn on artificial lights if you want to be awake,
but basically you want as much bright light, ideally from sunlight,
coming in through your eyes throughout the day.
And then in the evening, you want as little bright light, ideally from sunlight, coming in through your eyes throughout the day.
And then in the evening, you want as little bright light coming in through your eyes.
I've said this over and over and over again on this podcast.
There's always a lot of negotiations, but I want to make a few things clear.
Try not to wear sunglasses if you can do it safely.
Find to wear eyeglasses or contacts.
That's not going to be a problem.
The light viewing that you do and the avoidance of light at night, set the fundamental layer
of your time perception.
One of the best ways to disrupt your perception of time in the ways that we're going to talk
about in the subsequent portions of the podcast is to disrupt your circadian clock.
And that is not a good thing for a number of different reasons. There are other ways to so-called entrain your circadian clock. And that is not a good thing for a number of different reasons.
There are other ways to so-called
entrain your circadian clock.
One of the best ways to do that is to engage
in physical activity at fairly regular times of day.
You don't have to do it every day.
But if you're going to exercise,
try and exercise at a fairly consistent time of day,
probably better to exercise than to not exercise,
even if you have to move that time of day.
But light activity and we'll talk about the third in a minute.
Are the major ways that you entrain your internal perception of time to the external events
of the of the world, meaning that the turning of the earth and therefore the exposure to sunlight
or not.
So in addition, the sunlight viewing in the morning and throughout
the day and avoiding bright light at night of any kind, not just blue light, trying to get your
activity, your exercise at fairly fairly regular within plus or minus two hours from each day to the
next is going to have a very positive effect on so-called circadian entrainment. And also eating at
fairly regular times. However, this is exciting.
The data mainly point to the fact that you need to eat
within more or less the same time window each day,
not that you always need to eat your meals
at exactly the same time.
So you don't necessarily have to eat lunch at noon
and a snack at four and dinner at eight in order
to keep your circadian entrainment aligned or sharp. You could, for instance, have a small snack at four and dinner at eight in order to keep your circadian entrainment aligned or
sharp. You could, for instance, have a small snack at noon and then eat at two and then
have dinner at six and then a small snack at eight. It doesn't so much matter when the
exact meals fall. So much is that they fall more or less within a consistent period or
phase of each 24 hour cycle. What happens when this circadian clock starts getting disrupted?
I mean, this is after all an episode about time perception. It's not an episode about circadian
rhythms and entrainment. Well, there's a classic study by Ashoft on in 1985 that's now been
repeated many times where they had people go into environments where they didn't have clocks and
they didn't have windows and they didn't have watches and they were sometimes even in
constant, dark or constant light.
And they evaluated how well people perceive the passage of time on shorter time scales.
And what they found was really interesting.
What they found is that people underestimate how long they were in these isolated environments.
So after 42 days or so, they'd ask people, how long do you think you've been in here
and people would say 28 days or 36 days?
They generally underestimated how long they'd been in this very odd environment with no
clocks or watches or exposure to sunlight or regular rhythms of artificial light.
In addition, they found that their perception of shorter time intervals
was also really disrupted. So if they asked them to measure off two minutes, they normally
people are pretty good at measuring off two minutes. People come within, you know, five to
15 seconds at most, if you kind of have to sit there and just wait, you have a pretty good idea
of when two minutes up, you say two minutes is up. Well, when people circadian clocks
or circadian entrainment, I should say, was disrupted,
their perception of time measurement
on shorter time scales of minutes or even seconds
was greatly disrupted.
And as we'll see in a couple of minutes,
that actually causes great problems
for how you contend with work,
how you contend with work, how you contend with challenges
of different kinds.
You want your circadian entrainment to be pretty locked in or pretty entrained to the
outside light, dark cycles so that your perception of time on shorter time intervals can be precise
because the ability to perceive time accurately for the given task or given thing that you're involved in
turns out to be one of the most fundamental ways
that predicts how well or poorly you perform
that thing or task.
So we've talked about circannual entrainment,
the matching of the cells and tissues
in the organs of our body to the 365 day journey
that the earth takes around the sun each year.
And we talked about circadian entrainment,
the way that the 24 hour genetic and protein clocks
of each and every one of ourselves is matched
to the rotation of the earth on its axis
and the exposure or lack of exposure to the sun
because of that rotation on its axis.
Next I'd like to talk about so-called or lack of exposure to the sun because of that rotation on its axis.
Next I'd like to talk about so-called,
Altradian entrainment.
Altradian rhythms are rhythms of about 90 minutes or so.
And all of our existence is broken up into these 90 minute
Altradian cycles.
When you go to sleep at night, whether night you sleep six hours or four hours or eight hours
or ten hours, that entire period of sleep is broken up into
these 90 minute ultra-dian cycles. Early in the night, you tend to have more slow wave
sleep. Later in the night, you tend to have more REM sleep. But nonetheless, your sleep
is broken up into these 90 minute cycles. However, when you wake up in the morning, many
of the things that you do are governed by these ultra-dian rhythms. For instance, if you were to work, meaning do math, or try and learn a language, or do
physical work of any kind, or work out, the 90-minute time block seems to be the one in which
the brain can enter a state of focus and alertness and do hard work and focus, focus, focus,
and then at about 90 minutes,
there's a significant drop in your ability to engage in this mental or physical work.
Now, everybody from, you know, the self-help literature to the business literature to the
pop psychology literature has tried to leverage these all trading cycles by saying, if you're going
to do something hard and you want to focus on
it, limit it to 90 minutes or less. And I am one of those people who's also joined that conversation.
And indeed, I use 90-minute work cycles. And I think they are extremely powerful. One should never
expect that you're going to drop immediately into a state of high focus at the beginning. And then
it's remained there for 90 minutes. We all, you know, struggle to varying degrees to achieve focus and motivation and drive within those 90-minute cycles. But it is true, meaning there is ample literature
to support the idea that after about 90 minutes, we tend to go into a state of less ability to focus.
So while this isn't time perception, per se, it is again an example of entrainment.
What are we in training to?
Just because we can focus for 90 minutes and then not so well at 100 minutes or 120 minutes,
what are we in training to?
What you're in training to is the release of particular neurochemicals, in this case,
acetycoline and dopamine that allow your brain to focus for particular periods of time,
90 minutes or so.
After about 90 minutes or so, the amount of those chemicals that can be released tends to drop
very low, which is why your ability to focus becomes diminished. If one would like to explore more
about the backbone and basis of these ultraden rhythms, it goes by a different name. This was
originally called the basic rest activity cycle.
This was proposed many years ago by an ethânial clitman.
It was established to be true within sleep states, as I mentioned before, then it was debated
for a long time, whether or not these 90-minute cycles also control our ability to focus and
perform work in wakeful states and it turns out that they do.
Now there's a lot of literature to support that. I always get the question, how do you know when
the 90-minute cycle begins? In other words, let's say you wake up at 8 a.m. and you just finished
a 90-minute sleep cycle. Does that mean that your next 90-minute cycle where you could do work begins
right at 801? No. The interesting thing about these basic rest activity cycles, these
ultradion rhythms, is that you can initiate them whenever you want. This is not like a circadian
rhythm, which is a hard-wired unerring signal of 24 hours. The ultradion rhythms that
occur during sleep are hard-wired unerring. You don't get the option of making your sleep cycles at 120 minutes or five minutes.
You don't get that option.
But if you decide that you want to apply
all trading rhythms to work in performance,
you can set a clock and decide, okay,
now the focus begins, now the work begins
and this 90 minute cycle is the period
in which I'm going to do work.
And I actually do this, mid-morning and sometimes twice a day, I do a 90-minute cycle where I limit all
distraction as much as possible, put away my phone, often turn off the internet as well.
I talked about this in an episode on kind of an optimal work day, at least for me, just
to give an example of how this might work.
But I want to emphasize again that these ultradian rhythms are ones that you set.
So you decide I'm going to work for 90 minutes.
What you can't negotiate, however, is that at about 100 minutes or 120 minutes, no matter
who you are, you're going to see a diminishment in performance.
You're not going to focus as well.
And that's again because of the way that these 90-minute cycles are linked to the ability
of the neurons that release acetocholine and dopamine and to some extent, noraponephrine, the things that
give us narrow focus, motivation, and drive, the way that these 90-minute cycles are involved
in those circuits.
After about 90 minutes, those circuits are far less willing to engage, and therefore, it's
much harder to continue to focus to a high degree.
Some people like to do multiple 90-minute cycles per day
of focus. In that case, you need to separate them out. You can't do one 90-minute cycle then go
right into another 90-minute cycle then, another 90-minute cycle. You can't cheat these circuits
related to acetycoline and dopamine and norepinephrine. Unfortunately, I suggest that people do
no more than three, and ideally it would be two or just one of
these 90-minute cycles.
Why do I say ideally?
Well, they are very taxing.
You are in a very narrow tunnel of focus.
So for me, I can do one mid-morning and I probably do another one in the afternoon.
This is not the kind of work that's like checking email or text messaging or social media.
This is very focused, hard work.
It's working on hard problems of various kinds, and this will be different for everybody.
So I recommend that they be spaced by at least two to four hours, and most people probably
won't be able to handle more than two per day.
There are probably some mutants out there that could do three or four, but that's exceedingly rare.
I think even one a day is going to feel like a significant mental investment.
And afterwards, you're going to feel pretty taxed.
So now we've talked about circadian and ultra-adian rhythms.
But we haven't really talked about time perception per se.
We've mainly talked about the subconscious slow oscillatory ways in which we are entrained
or matched to the year or to the day.
And these all trading cycles that we can impose on our work and that we can leverage toward
more focus if we like.
What about the actual perception of time?
What actually controls how fast or how slowly we perceive time going by?
There are basically three forms of time perception that we should all be aware of.
One is our perception of the passage of time in the present. How quickly or slowly things seem
to be happening for us. This is kind of like an interval timer ticking off time. Tick tick tick tick tick tick
tick. It's either fine slicing like that or tick tick tick tick. We have interval timers. I'll discuss the basis of those interval timers. We
also engage in what's called prospective timing, which is like a stopwatch, measuring off things
as they go forward. That might sound a little bit like what I just described, but it's actually
a little bit different. For instance, if I told you to start measuring off a two-minute time interval
you to start measuring off a two-minute time interval into the future, you could do that pretty well. But if I told you you had to measure a five-minute time interval into the future
and you couldn't use any clocks or watches or your phone or anything like that,
you would have to set the tick marks. You would have to decide how many times you were going to
count off during that five minute time block.
There's also retrospective time,
which is how you measure off time in the past.
So if I say, you know, last week I know you went to the park,
you did some things with friends,
you know, you went out in the evening,
how long was it between lunch
and when you went to dinner with friends?
You probably think, okay, well,
I remember I went to dinner at seven and we had lunch right
around two.
You're using memory to reconstruct certain sets of events in the past and get a sense
of their relative positioning within time.
Okay.
So we have retrospective current time interval measurements and then prospective time measurement
into the future. The beauty of time perception in the human nervous system is that it boils down to a couple
of simple molecules that govern whether or not we are fine slicing time or whether or
not we are batching time in larger bins.
Those molecules go by names that maybe you've heard, things like dopamine and noripinephrine,
neuromodulators, called neuromodulators because they modulate, they change the way that other
neural circuits work.
Also things like serotonin.
Serotonin is released from a different site in the brain than dopamine and epinephrine
is and has a different effect on time perception.
So just to give you an example of how things like dopamine
and serotonin can modulate our perception of time, want to focus on a little bit of literature
that now has been done, unfortunately, in animals and humans, and which essentially shows that
the more dopamine that's released into our brain, the more we tend to overestimate the amount of time
that is just past.
Let me repeat that.
The more dopamine that is released into our brain,
the more we tend to overestimate how much time has passed.
These experiments are very straightforward, excuse me,
and they're very objective, which is really nice,
which is you can give people or an animal a drug
that increases the amount
of dopamine and then ask them to measure off without any measurement device like a watcher
o'clock when one minute has passed. As dopamine levels rise in the brain, people tend to think
that the minute is up before a minute. So they at the 38 second mark, they'll say, okay, I think a minute is up. So
they've overestimated how much time has passed. Okay. The higher the level of dopamine, the more
people tend to overestimate. Now, it's also true that Norepinephrine, also called Noregenalin,
plays a role and its role is very similar to that of dopamine. And that's because Norepinephrine
and dopamine are close cousins. And some of you may recall that they are actually manufactured from one another. Okay,
so dopamine can actually make epinephrine and norepinephrine. Biochemically, it's, there's a cascade
in which dopamine can be made into norepinephrine and epinephrine, which is remarkable.
How does having elevated levels of dopamine and norepinephrine cause one to overestimate how much time has passed?
Well, it does it because of the way that it causes
fine slicing of your time bins.
So fine slicing of time bins is like increasing the frame rate
on your camera.
Right? Slow motion is achieved in movies and elsewhere by increasing the frame rate.
So, if you take a movie at 30 frames per second and watch it, it will appear to have a certain
speed, right? Because those are just snapshots, 30 frames per second.
In contrast, if you took that same movie at 4,000 frames per second, you are fine slicing
and you're going to see every little detail
and as you play each one of those frames it's going to look like it moved slower. Okay, whatever.
So the kind of jump shot and basketball that's done slowly any any slow motion is the consequence
of higher frame rate. So dopamine and noraprine for an increased frame rate. And as a consequence, they tend to lead us
to overestimate the amount of time that's passed.
Conversely, the neuromodulator serotonin
causes people to underestimate the amount of time
that's passed.
So they've done these experiments.
They actually have done these experiments
using inhumans with drugs that increase serotonin.
They've also done them with cannabis, which increases serotonin among other things, including
the cannabinoid receptor activation.
And when people have elevated levels of 5HT or whether or not they've ingested cannabis,
they tend to underestimate how much time has passed.
You do the equivalent experiment.
You tell people that they have to guess or tell you when five minutes, for instance, has passed, just to use five minutes as
example this time. And generally, they will miss the five minute mark. They will think,
they'll let six minutes pass. And they'll say, it was five minutes when they've underestimated
how much time has passed. And that's because serotonin and some of the related molecules in the brain tend to
lead to slower frame rates, right?
They take the frame rate from, in the example I used before, from 4,000 frames per second
down to say 20 frames per second.
So this is very interesting.
It's interesting in terms of how pharmacology can be used to adjust time perception, but
it's also interesting in the
context of that circadian rhythm. There's some emerging evidence that throughout the 24 hour cycle,
there are robust changes in the amount of dopamine, norapinephrine, and serotonin that are present
in the brain and bloodstream and body, depending on time of day within the circadian cycle.
I'm not talking about during sleep.
During sleep there are definitely variations in things like dopamine, nor epinephrine,
and serotonin.
I talked about them the episodes on sleep.
Here I'm just talking about the role of these molecules in time perception during wakefulness.
So, much of the evidence points to the fact that in the first half of the day, approximate
first half of the day, dopamine and norepinephrine are elevated in the brain body and blood stream,
much more than is serotonin.
And that in the second half of the day and in particular towards evening and night time,
serotonin levels are going up.
I think that's fairly well established now.
What that means based on what we just discussed about the role of dopamine, or epinephrine, and serotonin in setting the frame rate of time perception,
is that our perception of the passage of time will be very different in the early part
of the day and in the latter half of the day. And there's starting to be some evidence
to support this, that early in the day, people tend to overestimate how much time has passed.
And later in the day, they tend to overestimate how much time has passed.
And later in the day, they tend to underestimate how much time has passed.
And this is independent of taking any kind of substance that would increase or decrease dopamine
or serotonin.
Now, this is important in terms of how one thinks about structuring their day, because I know
many people are thinking about the various tasks that they need to do throughout their day.
Many, or I should say, all of the literature, at least,
that I can find on productivity and things of that sort,
point to the idea that we should be doing the hardest task,
the thing that we want to do the least
or the most important task early in the day,
as a kind of a psychological tool for getting it done
and feeling as if we accomplished something.
And I think that's an excellent protocol, frankly.
But I'm not sure it's an excellent protocol because of the way that we sense accomplishment,
or at least it's not only an excellent protocol because of the way that we sense accomplishment.
Another reason to move something that's very hard into the early part of the day is that
if indeed the dopaminergic and noradrenergic circuits are more active at that time,
we are actually in a better position cognitively to parse that hard problem because of the way that
we are able to find slice our perception of time and find slice all the perceptual events outside
us. So what I'm really saying is that early in the day, you are a much more high-resolution camera,
so to speak, than you are later in the day.
Now, different types of tasks and different types of things
require different frame rates
or different ways of perceiving time.
And indeed, this also lends itself to a tool
whereby for activities that involve
more kind of creative thinking that aren't as constrained
by particular answers or outcomes, and in which we need to kind of blend different aspects
of our memory, different aspects of task utilization.
In other words, for creative works, for brainstorming, for things that are a bit more fluid, so to
speak, the more serotonergic second
half of the day, and because of the way the serotonergic second half of the day lends itself
to our time perception, may actually be more beneficial for those sorts of tasks.
And I'll put a reference to a couple of the studies that point to this idea that in these
higher dopaminergic states, we are better at doing certain sorts of tasks.
And in these more serotonergic states, we're better at doing other sorts of tasks.
And how the dopamine tends to be earlier than the day and the serotonin later in the day.
So to speak, these are broad, I'm painting with broad strokes here.
But I think these lend themselves to some really excellent tools because I think we all understand
the value of doing something that's harder challenging early in the day, but we should ask ourselves harder challenging how. What does that task actually really
require in terms of time perception? Some people might appreciate some examples of how this might work.
Basically, what I'm saying is if you are doing work that involves adhering to some rigid rules,
so math or a recipe or execution of musical scales or physical skills or
Accounting or something that requires a lot of precision where there's a right and wrong answer and it's hard
I would suggest that you do that in the early part of the day because of the way that dopamine the norepinephrine
Impact time perception you are literally better at slicing up time. You are a higher resolution
brain during those times. And so that's going to lend itself better to events and demands
that require high resolution. Whereas in the afternoon, in this more what I'm calling
serotonergic state, that's when you're going to be better at brainstorming and creative
works where there's some flexibility in terms of how you're
batching time and perceiving time, and there isn't so much rigid oversight of a right or
wrong answer.
And as an aside, to support what I said, but also to take us back to this critical role
of the circadian rhythm, there is a lot of evidence that when one's sleep is disrupted,
when sleep is either too short or is fragmented
or is not of high enough quality for enough days, one of the first things to happen is
that there is a dysregulation of these dopaminergic, noragenergic and serotonergic states throughout
the day.
They get kind of mishmashed up.
It's not that they're a total mess, but they aren't as cleanly defined. And I think
this is one of the reasons why when we haven't slept well or we haven't slept enough, we tend
to feel a little off, like we can't concentrate. Part of that lack of concentration is due
to other things. But part of that concentration could be due to the fact that our sense of
the passage of time is disrupted. So there seems to be some value in keeping the dopamine
and urgent, noradginergic
state kind of limited to the early part of the day. And this surrogic state is we're
calling it kind of push towards the second half of the day. Now there is a version of how
dopamine and norapinephrine can impact our perception of the passage of time in ways
that can be very disruptive or even maladaptive. And the best example that I'm aware of is trauma.
Many people who have been in car accidents
or who have experienced some other form of major trauma,
do what's called overclocking.
Overclocking is when levels of dopamine and norepinephrine
increase so much during a particular event,
our level of alertness has increased so much during a given event that we find slice. In other words, the frame
rate is increased so much so that we perceive things as happening in ultra slow motion.
Now that might not seem like a bad thing overall, but the problem with overclocking is the way
in which that information gets stamped down into the memory system.
So the memory system, which involves areas of the brain like the hippocampus, but also
the neocortex, is basically a space-time recorder.
What do I mean by space-time recorder?
Well, your nervous system, of course, is housed in the
darkness of your skull. It doesn't have a whole lot of information about the outside world,
except light coming in through the eyes and whatever happens to hit our ears and in terms of sound
waves and skin and so forth. So it has to take all those neural signals and it has to create a record
of what happened. Now, it doesn't create a record of everything that happened, but car accidents and trauma and things of that sort, oftentimes are stamped down into our record of what happened.
And what gets stamped down, what we actually mean by the phrase stamped down, is that the precise
firing of the sequence of neurons that reflected some event. So let's say I'm in a car accident,
certain neurons are firing because of the flipping of the car or
their screams or there's blood or you know things of that sort. All of that neural activity gets
repeated in the hippocampus and then the sequence of the firing of those neurons is also remembered. So it's not just that neuron
those neurons is also remembered. So it's not just that neuron one two three four fired in that sequence. It's also that neuron one two three four fired at a particular rate.
So it would be one two three four during the actual event. And then the memory is stored
as firing of those neurons as one two three four, right? If it if during the event, it was
one two three four at that rate, the storage of the memory is not going to be one, two, three, four.
In other words, there's both a space code, as we say, meaning the particular neurons that
fire is important, and there's a rate code.
How quickly those neurons fire or the relative firing, the timing of the firing of those neurons
is also part of the memory.
This affords our memory system tremendous flexibility.
What it means is that you can take the same set of neurons in the hippocampus and stamp down many, many more
memories because all you have to do is use a match of the different rates of the different
neurons that we're firing in order to set that code, right? You don't otherwise if you
needed a different set of neurons for every memory, you need any enormous hippocampus,
you need an enormous head. So I. I think you get the basic idea.
Overclocking is a case in which the frame rate is so high that a memory gets stamped down
and people have a very hard time shaking that memory and the emotions associated with
that memory.
It's not the topic of today's conversation, but we will cover trauma in a future episode
in detail, but many of the treatments for trauma.
EMDR, nowadays there's a lot of excitement also about ketamine therapies, exposure therapies,
like cognitive behavioral therapies, involved not just trying to reduce the amount of emotion
associated with a memory, but also a deliberate speeding up or slowing down of that memory.
In other words, trying to allow the person who experienced the trauma to take control
of the rate of the experience in their memory, not just whether or not the memory happened
at all.
In fact, one of the first things that trauma victims learn is that they aren't going
to forget what happened, what's eventually going to happen, ideally with good treatment, is that the emotional weight of the experience will eventually be divorced from the memory
of the experience.
And that's done again by trying to reduce the amount of emotional activation during the
recall of that experience.
And one of the best ways to do that is to alter the rate of the memory playback.
In other words, taking that firing of neurons
that might have been 1, 2, 3, 4,
again, it would be much more complicated,
but 1, 2, 3, 4 for the car crash
and getting the memory to playback at a rate of 1, 2, 3, 4,
or even 1, 2, 3, 4, 1, 2, 3, 4.
In other words, allowing the person
or instructing the person to take control of the rate of the playback.
And in that way, there seems to be still yet unknown mechanism by which people can
uncouple some of the emotional weight that's associated with that memory. So
overclocking is a kind of extreme example of where the dopaminergic and the norageinuric system is ramped up so high
that people have this, unfortunately, what seems like indelible mark in their brain of a particular
event, but again, trauma treatment is designed to uncouple the emotional load of that event.
Some of you are probably saying, why dopamine during trauma? I thought dopamine was the feel
good molecule. Well, in reality, dopamine is not necessarily a molecule
of reward, it's a molecule of motivation, pursuit, and drive.
And because of the close relationship between dopamine
and norepinephrine, oftentimes they are co-released.
So whether or not dopamine is released during car crashes
or other forms of trauma, we don't know.
But what we do know is that both the dopamine system
and the noreager-nergic system, when we say nage and urgent, we we know our penneferin. Those
systems are greatly increased anytime there's a heightened state of arousal and
arousal can have negative valence, like a meaning associated with an event that
we really hate that we would prefer not to be of molten or can have positive
valence. But dopamine and the noripinneferin are kind of the common hallmark of
all things of elevated
arousal.
And so that's why we see evidence for dopamine being associated with these changes in
time perception, both for positive events and for negative events.
There's a very interesting relationship between arousal, dopamine, time perception, and blinking.
And this is all supported by a really interesting paper.
First author, Terroon, is the last name,
T-E-R-H-U-N-E, is published in current biology,
self-presternal, excellent journal.
The title of the paper is Time Dilates
after Spontaneous Blinking.
So heightened states of arousal are associated
with heightened levels of dopamine.
You now know that dopamine leads to a kind of fine slicing
of time.
And one of the ways that we find sliced time is by blinking.
You know, we think of blinking as just a thing to like lubricate our eyes or to limit the
amount of light coming into our eyes, but it's a shutter on our experience.
So much of the information that coming into the brain through our eyes impacts our attention.
I've said it before on this podcast that cognitive attention follows visual attention, at least for sighted individuals.
Well, it turns out that dopamine and increases in dopamine
are associated with increases in spontaneous blink rate.
So the more aroused we are, the more awake we are,
their number of effects, pupils,
dilate, heart rate increases, et cetera,
but also blink rate increases.
And every time we blink, this study cleanly shows, we shift our perception of time, leading
to, as I mentioned before, over estimations of time.
So it seems as though in some way, blink rate is actually related to frame rate.
And so this is very, very interesting.
And the way that you could think about leveraging this would be,
if you wanted to actually slow down your perception of time, you would blink less. And if you wanted to speed up your perception of time, you would blink more. Now, you'd have to think of a
scenario in which that would be useful to you. Obviously, if you're going to blink, you're going to
miss things as well. But I think it's a very interesting parameter of our visual attention as it relates
to time perception because what it really speaks to is that these neuromodulators, like dopamine
or serotonin, that adjust frame rate, they're not doing it through some magical mechanism.
In fact, there's no single brain area that we can say controls time perception.
I haven't said today, oh, you know, it's the striatum.
Well, it involves the striatum, but I'm not going to say, for, you know, it's the striatum. Well, it involves the striatum, but I'm not gonna say, for instance,
oh, it's the cerebellum.
The cerebellum is definitely involved
in timing of movement, something for a future podcast,
but time perception is what we call a distributed phenomenon.
It's a network of areas in the brain working together,
but dopamine in the way that it relates to
the shuttering of your eyes
seems to be controlling the
frame rate on your experience.
Numbers times on this podcast, I've talked about cold exposure.
And nowadays there's a lot of interest in things like cold showers, ice baths, immersion,
in cold water tanks and lakes and oceans and things of that sort.
There are a lot of different positive effects of cold exposure.
Provide it's done properly.
It can lead to increases in metabolism,
brown fat stores, which are the good fat stores
that you want.
There's sort of like a furnace to allow you
to heat yourself up.
Stay warming cold environments,
to reduce inflammation, to increase resilience and so forth.
There's a study published in the European Journal
of Physiology showing that cold exposure
can increase our baseline levels of dopamine robustly, 2.5x, and it's a long lasting increase
in dopamine.
It appears to be a healthy one, meaning it doesn't seem to be addictive.
I'm sure there are some people out there addicted to ice baths, but when you think about
the range of dopamine-inducing behaviors that are addictive, it seems to be more on the
health-promoting side.
What's interesting is that because cold water exposure
increases dopamine, it will also change your perception of time.
And if you've ever done one of these cold water exposures,
you've experienced this, you've experienced getting in
and feeling like, wow, making it three minutes
is a really, really long time.
And you are fine slicing time, your framerate is going up.
Part of that, just at a course level
as you're thinking, this is painful, I don't like this,
I want to get out, right?
But part of it is also that your dopamine levels
are going up very quickly.
And therefore, your perception of that discomfort
is also being fine sliced.
And so you could leverage a tool, for instance,
where you try and entrain your thinking
to something other than your immediate experience.
Right? This is a kind of a controversy, if you will, in the cold exposure world.
The question is, do you try and lean into the experience and really feel it?
Or do you try and distract yourself? You know, sing a song or count off, you know, from one to a hundred.
Just know that whatever tactic you use to get through the cold exposure, that the dopamine
level that's now increased in your system is going to cause you to find slice or experience
that at slow motion.
So a minute is going to seem like a lot longer than a minute in reality. So you could, for instance, decide to pay attention to some external cue.
Maybe it's a metronome that takes once every 10 seconds.
You could decide to think about something else.
You could decide to sing a song in your head or sing a song out loud.
All of that will divorce you from the sensation that you're experienced somewhat, but more so, it will divorce you from the perception of your experience as governed by that dopamine
increase in frame rate.
If that isn't clear, just know this.
When you're in the ice bath, your dopamine levels are high.
When your dopamine levels are high, your experience of the discomfort that ice bath is at higher
resolution.
Now, up until now, I've been talking about how dopamine and to some
extent serotonin can differentially impact your perception of how fast or how slowly things
are happening in the moment. But remember, we have prospective time, we have our experience
of time in the moment and we have retrospective time. And there are beautiful studies that have showed that the dopaminergic state
changes the way not just that we experience things now, but that it changes the way in which we
remember things in the past and the rate at which those things occurred. And those are in opposite
direction. So to make this very simple, if something that you experience
is fun or varied, meaning it has a lot of different components
in it, and in other words, it's associated
with an increase in dopamine in your brain,
you will experience that as going by very fast.
Now, this is different than the ice bath, which I just said that you experience
is going by very slowly,
but here I'm talking about something that's fun
and very that you really like,
and you feel like it goes by very, very fast.
Imagine an amazing day for a kid in an amusement park.
They can do a ton of things that's all new,
they're very excited,
and they'll feel like it goes by very fast.
But later, they will remember that experience as being very long, that
it was a long day full of many, many events. And so there's this paradoxical relationship
between how we perceive fun, exciting, varied events in the present and how we remember them
in the past. For those of you who have gone on vacation, if you've had an amazing day
on vacation, it'll seem like, or an amazing vacation overall.
It will seem like it goes by very fast.
The last day of vacation, you sort of go,
whoa, it went by so fast, because there's so much happening.
But in memory, six, eight months later, you were remembered,
wow, that just went, you know,
that was a long, long thing.
We had this, then we had that, then we did this,
then we had that, it tends to spool out in a
longer memory than the actual experience. Conversely, if you are bored with something, or it's something
you really don't like, it's going to seem like it takes a long time to go through that experience in
the moment. But retroactively looking back, it will seem like that moment was very short. So if the
other day I was waiting in the waiting room
for the dentist, it was pretty boring.
I was just gonna sit there, there wasn't much going on.
And it did seem like it was going on
in awfully long time, but indeed looking back,
it just seems like, okay, I sat in that room,
not much happened, and so it seems like
a very short time been.
This seems to be an efficiency of how
the brain stores information.
Dopamine being associated, of course, with
fun and varied experiences and low dopamine being associated with kind of empty boring
or what at the time seem like long experiences. And this whole thing has been stamped down
into the scientific literature by those earlier experiments where they take human beings and
isolate them in certain environments. You know, take away all the clocks and watches and cues
and about what time of day it is and what time of night it is
and allow people to have a life
where they can either read and work and do things
or where they have very little to do.
When people are isolated in very boring environments
and they don't have access to time cues. Time dilates.
They tend to assume that time has gone on very, very long.
And so the reason I bring this up is we aren't just driven by these circadian clocks and
these circanian clocks and these ultra-adiene clocks.
We are driven by these timers that vary depending on our level of excitement and they vary depending on our level of excitement
because of these neuromodulators dopamine and serotonin.
So the way I like to think about it
is that you have two clocks, two stopwatches.
One is a dopamine-ergic stopwatch
that fine slices really closely.
It's like counts off milliseconds,
and it's grabbing a movie of your experience
at very high resolution.
And then the other hand, you have a stopwatch that's gathering big time bins, big ticks
along that the, you know, that the hand is moving at bigger intervals, you know, marking off time.
And depending on whether or not you're excited or whether or not you're bored,
you're using different stopwatches on time and therefore you're perceiving your experience
differently.
One very interesting aspect to the way that neuromodulators like dopamine and novelty interact
with time perception and memory is how we perceive our relationship to places and people.
So really interesting literature showing that the more novel experiences we have in a place,
the more we feel we know that place, obviously, but the longer we feel we've been there.
So, here's the kind of Gedunken or Thought Experiment that illustrates what's in the literature.
Let's say I were to move to New York City.
I happen to really like New York City.
I've never lived there, but let's say I lived there.
I lived in a given apartment for a year,
and I would have a number of different experiences
in this mental experiment.
Let's say I had a hundred different, exciting
and new experiences.
I would, at the end of that year,
feel as if I lived there a certain period of time,
one year, I would actually know, I lived there one year.
If however, I lived in three different places in New York City,
and I met three times as many people, I'm one year, I would actually know I lived there one year. If however, I lived in three different places in New York City,
and I met three times as many people,
and I had three times as many novel experiences,
I would actually feel as if I had been there much longer
than had I only lived in one location.
This is also true for social interactions.
When we move to multiple or several novel environments
with somebody else.
We tend to feel as if we know that person much better and that they know us much better.
Now, of course, we get the opportunity to interact with those people in different contexts.
So, indeed, we do get the opportunity to see them, for instance, at the coffee shop,
how they order coffee.
You maybe go to a sports event, how they act there, maybe how they interact with your family.
You're getting a sense of them in different contexts.
That's certainly playing a role.
But it seems that if the more novelty you experience with somebody, not only the more
familiar they are to you, but the more time you feel you've spent with them, even though
the total amount of time can be exactly the same.
And so that's a very interesting aspect of how our perception of time in these neuromodulators and novelty can shape the way not just that we perceive a given event in our world, but
how we relate to a place or relate to a person.
So we've talked a lot about the different neurochemicals and how those neurochemicals
can influence our perception of time.
We haven't talked a lot about the neural circuits and the various areas of the brain that underlie
this. I do want to touch on that by highlighting a really wonderful study.
This was a study published in neuron, also a cell press journal, excellent journal.
The title of the paper is behavioral, physiological, and neural signatures of surprise during naturalistic
sports viewing.
This experiment is really cool.
They did brain imaging on individuals who are watching
basketball games. These were basketball games that actually took place that were recorded and
the subjects watching these basketball games in some cases not all had some interest in who would
win or lose. And in some cases not all the subjects in these studies had some prior knowledge of
which team they thought was better,
which team was likely to win or not likely to win.
The basic findings of the study were that they could measure surprise by the release of
dopamine in two areas of the brain, part of what are called is called, excuse me, the
mesolimbic reward pathway.
So the two areas of the brain that are important here are the nucleus accumbens and the VTA,
the ventral tegmental area.
These are areas that release dopamine
as kind of a token of reward anytime something is surprising
or a positive expectation is met, okay?
So if I predict that my team dribbling down court
is going to score on this drive
and they get the ball in the basket.
A little bit of dopamine is released. These two brain areas light up in the functional imaging,
so-called FMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging that they used in the study.
What's really interesting about this study is not just that dopamine was released any time that
something the subject wanted to see happened, right?
Anytime they wanted to see their team score, they scored.
But also during surprise.
So if they thought, for instance, and they would hit a button to predict that their team
was going to score in this particular drive and they didn't, well, then dopamine could
also be released in response to that surprise.
So this speaks again to dopamine being something that's important, not just for positive events,
but for unexpected events.
Now that's all very interesting, and speaks to the fact that dopamine is a kind of flexible
currency in the brain.
It's doled out, if you will, or released when something that one hopes will happen happens,
and it's released when there's a surprise, even if it's a kind of a negative surprise
It's not something that the subject wanted to happen
But the more interesting thing is how that relates to time perception
What they found was regardless of what caused the dopamine release the frequency of dopamine release
predicted how the subjects parsed the time bins of the game
they were watching.
What do I mean by that?
Well, when you watch a basketball game or you watch anything, children playing or talking
to your spouse or whatever, you're batching time.
How are you batching time?
Well, you could batch a meal by the, I don't know, the appetizer, the main course and the
dessert,
but it turns out that's not what you're doing. You're batching time according to the frequency of dopamine pulses,
the frequency of dopamine release. And that's what they saw in this study. If they evaluated people's perceptions of the passage of time,
what they found is that that matched not whether or not the, you know not it was a particular time point in the game,
not whether or not their team was going down court or running back up court to play defense.
But the dopamine released served as markers which would predict the frame rate of their
perception of the experience. And if that sounds complicated, what I mean is how often and when
you release dopamine is actually setting the frame rate
on the entire perception of everything, not just of her positive events or negative events.
So what this means is as you're going through life, dopamine and the release of dopamine
is saying that's over and now you're in a new phase of your life. Even if it's very short, right?
So if I get up in the morning,
and I really need a cup of coffee,
as you probably all know, I wait 90 minutes to 120 minutes
before I drink my coffee,
but then I get my coffee and surely there's a dopamine hit
there, I promise you.
I actually am starting to carve up my day
according to dopamine hits.
I am with consciously or subconsciously,
I'm actually carving up my experience according to when I'm getting dopamine throughout my day. This governance over our
perception of time that dopamine has points to a very clear,
very actionable and very powerful tool. And that is a tool that many people have talked about before,
which are habits. People have discussed habits in a variety of contexts. But in the context of
dopamine reward and time perception, what this means is that placing specific habitual routines
is that placing specific habitual routines at particular intervals throughout your day is a very not just convenient but a very good way to incorporate the dopamine system
so that you divide your day into a series of what I would call functional units.
What would this look like? It would mean waking up and having one specific habit
that you always engage in that causes
a release of dopamine.
You could say, well, great, that'll make me feel good.
And I would agree, dopamine release generally makes us feel motivated, but it would have an
additional effect of marking that time of day as the beginning of a particular time bin.
Then inserting another habit, perhaps the beginning of your breakfast or something, but
recognizing that that's a habit and being fairly habitual, you don't have to be obsessively
precise about the timing, but that regular sequencing of things is going to lead not just to
dopamine release as it relates to reward and motivation and feeling good, but it actually becomes the way in which we carve up our entire experience of our day.
And this is almost a circular argument.
You can say, well, of course, you know, I do one thing, then I do the next, then I do the
next.
And that's how I perceive my day.
That's my day.
It's my list.
It's my to-do list, et cetera.
But what I'm saying is that on the basis of this study, I should mention the first author, his last name is Antony.
It was Antony at all.
It was published in 2020.
The study on basketball viewing, what it points to is that by engaging in specific habits
that we know we can perform well, we are actually setting the frame rate on our day.
And so I think there will soon come a time where human beings are not just thinking,
okay, my morning routine and my afternoon routine,
I think that can be useful.
And in fact, I used or mentioned a structure
of that sort earlier in the episode.
But rather thinking about what's actually going on
at the level of our biology,
which is that dopamine is marking time.
Habits are a very clear way in which we can
Envoke dopamine release and therefore provide time markers. And what this means is that for instance during your morning
You might insert habit one and habit two at say I don't know 8 a.m. and 10 a.m. and in doing that that marks an epoch a little
batch of time in your morning routine that's distinct from the second half of your morning.
In other words, habits serve as flankers or markers for the passage of your day.
Now, if that seems kind of hyper neurotic or why would I want to structure my life like that,
I would say that many people would do well to structure their life like that,
and to utilize habits not just for sake of what you do
during the habit, but because of the fact that the habit
serve as a marker because of the way they can evoke
dopamine release.
And in doing that, you are able to segment your day
into a bunch of smaller, if you want them to be smaller,
or larger functional units.
If anyone wants to experiment with this,
the Huberman Lab podcast puts out a newsletter. It's called the Neural Network newsletter.
You can sign up for it at HubertmanLab.com. We put it out each month. You can see the
previous newsletters. There's zero cost. We have our privacy statement there. We don't
share your email or anything. And there you'll find the 12 steps to improving sleep was
the first one. There's another, the second newsletter was all about neuroplasticity and using scientific
literature to improve learning and teaching.
In the next newsletter, I intend to include an example protocol of how one could use habits
and the relationship between habits and dopamine, dopamine and time perception to structure
your day according to performance of particular types of tasks. Today, we covered a lot about time perception to structure your day according to performance of particular types of tasks.
Today, we covered a lot about time perception. We certainly didn't cover everything about time
perception, but we covered things like entrainment, the role of dopamine, habits, and various routines
that can adjust your sense of time for sake of particular goals. If you're interested in learning
more about time perception, I'd like to point you to a really excellent book called Your Brain is a Time Machine,
the Neuroscience and Physics of Time.
The book was written by Professor,
Dr. Dean Bonamano, who's a professor at UCLA
and a world expert in the Neuroscience and Physics of Time.
I do hope to get Dean on the podcast
in the not too distant future.
If you're learning from and you're enjoying this podcast,
please subscribe to our podcast
channel on YouTube.
It's simply Huberman Lab on YouTube.
And there you can also leave us suggestions for future guests and topics and questions
about the podcast episodes in the comment section on YouTube.
In addition, please subscribe to our podcast on Apple and or Spotify.
And on Apple, you have the opportunity to leave us up to a five star review.
You can also follow us on Instagram.
On Instagram, I do short neuroscience tutorials and tools and protocols, I cover recent
papers, many of which are not included on the podcast.
And as mentioned at the beginning of today's episode, we are now partnered with momentous
supplements because they make single ingredient formulations that are of the absolute highest
quality and they ship international.
If you go to livemomentus.com slash huberman, you will find many of the supplements that
have been discussed on various episodes of the huberman lab podcast, and you will find
various protocols related to those supplements.
Thank you for your time and attention today and last, but certainly not least, thank you
for your interest in science.
Thank you for your interest in science.