Huberman Lab - Tools to Enhance Working Memory & Attention
Episode Date: January 29, 2024In this episode, I discuss working memory, which is critical for learning and productivity, strategy setting, goal seeking, and navigating new environments. I explain the key role of dopamine and the ...biological mechanisms underlying working memory and how working memory differs from both short- and long-term memory. I also describe science-supported tools to enhance working memory and attention—including zero- or low-cost behavioral, supplemental and pharmacologic approaches. I include how to assess your working memory and how to use memory tasks to determine your baseline dopamine levels in certain brain circuits. This episode provides listeners with highly actionable tools to assess and improve their attention to specific tasks and task-switching capacity and to enhance their overall productivity. For show notes, including referenced articles and additional resources, please visit hubermanlab.com. Thank you to our sponsors AG1: https://drinkag1.com/huberman MateÃna: https://drinkmateina.com/huberman BetterHelp: https://betterhelp.com/huberman Helix Sleep: https://helixsleep.com/huberman LMNT: https://drinklmnt.com/huberman Momentous: https://livemomentous.com/huberman Timestamps (00:00:00) Working Memory (00:01:12) Sponsors: Mateina, BetterHelp & Helix Sleep (00:05:00) Short- vs. Long-Term Memory (00:09:59) Neuroplasticity (00:15:42) Working Memory; Attention & Focus (00:20:04) Working Memory Test (00:25:35) Sponsor: AG1 (00:27:02) Brain & Working Memory; Dopamine (00:36:13) Working Memory Capacity Test (00:44:37) Increasing Dopamine & Working Memory (00:49:26) Task Switching, Distractions (00:54:42) Sponsor: LMNT (00:56:04) Tool: Yoga Nidra, Non-Sleep Deep Rest (NSDR) & Dopamine (01:03:08) Tool: Deliberate Cold Exposure & Dopamine (01:11:02) Tool: Working Memory & Binaural Beats (01:15:23) Supplements to Increase Dopamine: L-Tyrosine, Mucuna Pruriens (01:22:53) Dopamine Prescriptions, Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) (01:29:12) Zero-Cost Support, Spotify & Apple Reviews, YouTube Feedback, Sponsors,  Momentous, Social Media, Neural Network Newsletter Disclaimer
Transcript
Discussion (0)
Welcome to the Huberman Lab Podcast, where we discuss science and science-based tools
for everyday life.
I'm Andrew Huberman, and I'm a professor of neurobiology and ophthalmology at Stanford
School of Medicine.
Today we are discussing working memory.
Working memory is a special category of memory in which we are able to hold small amounts
of information in our mind for short
periods of time.
Working memory is also very closely related to attention.
So for any of you that are interested in how to develop better focus and attention, understanding
what working memory is and some of the things that you can do to improve your working memory
can be very beneficial.
Today I'm going to talk about what working memory is, including some of the underlying
biology,
although I promise,
irrespective of whether or not you know any biology
or you are an expert in biology,
I'll make the conversation accessible to you.
In addition, I will talk about tools
to improve working memory.
And I'll also compare working memory to other forms of memory,
like long-term memory and short-term memory.
And through that understanding,
I'm confident that you'll be able to develop better focus
as well as be able to commit certain forms of information
to your short and long-term memory stores.
Before we begin, I'd like to emphasize that this podcast
is separate from my teaching and research roles at Stanford.
It is, however, part of my desire and effort
to bring zero cost to consumer information about science
and science-related tools to the general public.
In keeping with that theme, I'd like to thank the sponsors of today's podcast.
Our first sponsor is Matina.
Matina makes loose leaf and ready to drink yerba mate.
I often discuss yerba mate's benefits, such as regulating blood sugar, its high antioxidant content,
the ways that it can improve digestion, and possible neuroprotective effects.
I also drink yerba mate because I love the taste.
While there are a lot of different choices
of yerba mate drinks out there,
I love Matina because, again,
they have the no sugar variety,
as well as the fact that both their loose leaf
and their canned varieties are of the absolute best quality.
So much so that I decided to become a partial owner
in the
company. Although I must say, even if they hadn't allowed me to do that, I would
be drinking Matina. It is the cleanest tasting and best yerba mate you can find.
I love the taste of brewed loose leaf Matina yerba mate and I particularly
love the taste of Matina's new canned cold brew zero sugar yerba mate which I
personally help them develop.
If you'd like to try Matina,
go to drinkmatina.com slash huberman.
Right now, Matina is offering a free one pound bag
of loose leaf yerba mate tea and free shipping
with the purchase of two cases of their cold brew yerba mate.
Again, that's drinkmatina.com slash huberman
to get the free bag of Yorba Mate loose leaf tea
and free shipping.
Today's episode is also brought to us by BetterHelp.
BetterHelp offers professional therapy
with a licensed therapist carried out online.
I've been going to therapy for well over 30 years.
Initially, I didn't have a choice.
It was a condition of being allowed to stay in school.
But pretty soon I realized that therapy is extremely valuable.
In fact, I consider doing regular therapy
just as important as getting regular exercise,
including cardiovascular exercise and resistance training,
which of course I also do every week.
The reason I know therapy is so valuable
is that if you can find a therapist
with whom you can develop a really good rapport,
you not only get terrific support
for some of the challenges in your life,
but you also can derive tremendous insights
from that therapy.
Insights that can allow you to better,
not just your emotional life and your relationship life,
but of course also the relationship to yourself
and to your professional life, to all sorts of career goals.
In fact, I see therapy as one of the key components
for meshing together all aspects of one's life
and being able to really direct one's focus and attention toward what really matters.
If you'd like to try BetterHelp, go to betterhelp.com slash huberman to get 10% off your first
month.
Again, that's betterhelp.com slash huberman.
Today's episode is also brought to us by Helix Sleep.
Helix Sleep makes mattresses and pillows that are of the absolute highest quality.
I've spoken many times before on this and other podcasts about the fact that sleep is
the foundation of mental health, physical health, and performance.
One of the key things to getting a great night's sleep is to make sure that your mattress matches
your sleep requirements.
The Helix website has a brief two-minute quiz that if you go to it, we'll ask you questions
such as, do you sleep on your back, your side, or your stomach?
Do you tend to run hot or cold during the middle of the night?
As well as some other questions
that allow you to determine the optimal mattress for you.
When I took the quiz,
I personally matched to their Dusk mattress,
D-U-S-K, which has allowed me
to significantly improve my sleep.
So if you're interested in significantly improving your sleep,
go to helixsleep.com slash huberman.
Take their brief two minute quiz
and they'll match you to a customized mattress.
And you'll get up to $350 off any mattress order
and two free pillows.
So again, if you're interested in trying Helix,
go to helixsleep.com slash Huberman
for up to $350 off and two free pillows.
Okay, let's talk about working memory.
And let's start off this discussion
by comparing working memory to other forms of memory that
most people are more familiar with, or at least when most people hear the word memory,
they typically are thinking about long-term memory, like one's ability to remember the
capitals of states or countries, the different continents, directions from one location to
another, even one's name.
All of those things are examples of
long-term memory. Now I want to emphasize that long-term memory really has two components.
There are what we call declarative long-term memories, so these are the things that we can
declare, things like facts about ourselves or the world or others. And then there are procedural
long-term memories. Procedural long-term memories, as the name suggests, are aspects of our memory that allow us
to perform certain procedures.
They are literally action steps that we take
to, for instance, ride a bicycle or drive a car,
which, by the way, we might not be conscious
of ourselves doing after we learn,
that is, after we pass information
into our procedural long-term memory.
But even once those things become reflexive, they are stored in procedural long-term memory. But even once those things become reflexive,
they are stored in our long-term memory.
Now a discussion of long-term memory is not the focus today, but
me being a neuroscientist and I like to think you all generally being interested in the underlying
biology, I'll just mention that there's a key structure within the brain that is part of a
larger neural network that is a collection of structures
which is absolutely essential for the formation
and storage of long-term memories.
And that's the hippocampus, which in Latin means seahorse.
And it does look a little bit like a seahorse,
but we actually have one on each side of your brain.
So we say hippocampi plural.
And so what we know is that if people have damage
to their hippocampus of any kind,
that people have trouble accessing
or forming long-term memory, sometimes both.
And there's a lot more that we could say
about long-term memory.
Indeed, I did an entire episode
of the Huberman Lab podcast
about the formation and storage of long-term memories,
including some tools to improve long-term memory.
We'll touch on a few of those tools later today,
but you can access that episode
if you go to hubermanlab.com
and just put memory into the search function and you'll find it there. In the meantime,
if we want to understand working memory, we not only have to understand how it's different from
long-term memory, but also how it's different from short-term memories. Short-term memory is a
capacity that we all have that, as the name suggests, represents a short-term memory bank for information
that may or may not get passed into long-term memory.
So for instance, if you've learned anything,
and of course you have,
if you can understand what I'm saying,
you've learned English language,
if you can write, you've learned how to write, et cetera.
Well, in order to learn those things
and to commit them to long-term memory,
the information required to do those things
and to have that knowledge needed to be held in short-term memory.
Short-term memories are the sorts of memories that we maintain for somewhere between a few
minutes and potentially a few hours, maybe a little bit longer, but only a certain percentage
of that is passed into our long-term memory.
For instance, if you listen to this podcast or you go to a course lecture,
whether or not that lecture is about cognitive material
or whether or not it's about learning a new physical skill,
regardless of what you learn,
you're only going to learn a certain amount
of that information.
But were we to examine how much of the information
you just heard or that you're hearing now,
you remember immediately after this podcast episode,
as compared to
say a week later, we know based on gosh, probably millions of scientific papers and studies
that you are going to have more information in your short-term memory stores shortly after
being exposed to new information than you will later.
In other words, only a small percentage of what we perceive, what we see, what we hear,
et cetera, gets passed into short-term memory and then only a fraction of that gets passed into long-term memory.
Now the neural circuits for short-term memory and the passage of short-term memories into
long-term memory involve a lot of different brain structures, but here again, we can implicate
the hippocampus because the actual passage of short-term memories into long-term memories
occurs in part
within the hippocampus.
And then a lot of people don't know this.
Some of the memories that we think of as long-term memories
are actually distributed into the neocortex,
which is the outer portion of the brain.
Now the point here is less to fill your mind
with different names of things and nomenclature,
but rather to get you thinking about what's involved
in creating short and long-term memories
and equally important that even though the hippocampus is critically involved in the formation of short and long-term memories,
that the formation of short and long-term memories is really a network phenomenon.
In fact, among the more important themes that comes up again and again on this podcast,
anytime that we're talking about neuroscience or actually biology in any case,
is that rarely, if ever,, or actually biology in any case,
is that rarely if ever is there one location in the brain
where something happens.
Typically it's a network phenomenon,
meaning it's the collaboration of a bunch
of different brain areas, passing information
from one location to the next,
and storing it in a kind of distributed way.
Now, another key thing to understand about working memory
and how it is different from short and long-term memory
is that the formation of short and long-term memories
almost always involves neuroplasticity.
Neuroplasticity is the nervous system's ability
to change in response to experience.
Now there are different types of neuroplasticity.
So often when we hear about neuroplasticity
in the popular sphere,
people don't emphasize that there are different types
of neuroplasticity and it's worth paying a little bit
of attention to what those different types are.
There is for instance, what we call long-term potentiation.
Long-term potentiation or LTP as the acronym goes
is the strengthening of connections between neurons
as a consequence of their repeated firing
very closely together in time.
Okay, there's a lot more to it,
but if you've ever heard the phrase fire together,
wire together, sometimes that is misattributed
to Donald Hebb who did talk about neuroplasticity.
By the way, Donald Hebb was a psychologist up in Canada
who talked about neuroplasticity in the context
of lots of different forms of learning,
but that fire together together wire together phrase
was not actually stated by Donald Heb.
It was stated by Carla Shatz, my colleague at Stanford.
And she was referring to LTP,
but other forms of neuroplasticity
that occur mainly in development
when neurons fire very closely in time
and thereby strengthen those connections,
which can include LTP, okay?
So for now, think of LTP as any time
that some small group of neurons,
could be two neurons, could be 2000 neurons,
are very active closely together in time,
and they have access to one another physically.
And the consequence is often, not always,
but is often LTP.
That is the strengthening of those connections
such that after that barrage of activity subsides,
those neurons can speak to each other.
They can communicate through electrical activity
and chemical activity much more easily.
Their communication is more robust.
It's like removing a wall between a conversation
such that the conversation can take place more fluidly.
Now there are other forms of neuroplasticity,
including LTD, long-term depression,
which unfortunately the name often calls to mind
ideas about depression as a psychiatric
or a psychological symptom,
but it has nothing to do with that.
Long-term depression is simply the inverse of LTP.
It's actually the weakening or the removal of connections
that we call synapses between neurons.
I wanna emphasize that both LTP and LTD
are both critically involved
in lots of different kinds of learning
and both of them tend to be involved
in the formation of both short-term memories
and the long-term memories.
And this is very important in the removal
of short-term memories and long-term memories,
literally forgetting of certain things
because as we all know, there are many things that we will never forget and there are also things
that we almost always forget.
Now there's a third form of neuroplasticity that's involved in the formation of short
and long-term memories that's important for us to discuss just briefly.
But I do want to emphasize that there are not just three forms of neuroplasticity.
There are many other forms, dozens, if not more,
things like spike timing dependent plasticity,
paired pulse facilitation, and on and on.
But the third type of neuroplasticity
that I'd like to mention now is neurogenesis.
Neurogenesis is the formation of new neurons.
Now neurogenesis is robust in the developing nervous system.
We know this.
It's robust in the developing nervous system
of animals and humans.
However, neurogenesis, the literal formation
of new neurons in the brain is a very exciting idea
and it does occur and it's very exciting in a way
that has motivated lots of popular press outlets
to talk about or to discuss papers
that have discovered neurogenesis in the adult brain.
Because let's be honest, what's more exciting than the idea that your brain can add new brain
cells later in life. And indeed, that has been shown even in people well into their 80s and 90s.
However, it's very important to know that the total amount of neurogenesis that occurs in the
adult human brain is infinitesimally small as a mechanism
for neuroplasticity and learning
as compared to the other forms of neuroplasticity
that we discussed such as long-term potentiation
and long-term depression.
So I don't wanna throw cold water
on the topic of neurogenesis.
It's an incredibly interesting and important topic,
but all too often they tend to eclipse
the much more common mechanism
for the formation of short and long-term memories,
which are those other forms we just talked about,
LTP, LTD, et cetera.
So the point here is that, yes indeed,
there are new neurons that can be added in the adult brain,
maybe even in the adult human brain.
And there is some evidence that some of those new neurons
are added to the hippocampus.
In fact, a particular region of the hippocampus. In fact, a particular region of the hippocampus
called the dentate gyrus of the hippocampus.
And there's been a lot of controversy
about how much neurogenesis occurs or doesn't occur
and whether or not it occurs after puberty or not.
There's a whole field of people battling over this
now for several decades, but one thing is very clear.
Neurogenesis, while it's very exciting and intriguing,
is not the main mechanism by which the formation
of short and long-term memories occurs.
When you learn new information, as you are right now,
the storage of that information
in your short-term memory networks,
which is then passed on to your long-term memory networks,
and that can be recalled,
that allows you to state certain facts about,
for instance, the existence of this thing
called a hippocampus.
Hopefully you will remember that going forward. or your ability to perform any kind of
motor movement that you learned now or way back in childhood. Most of that is the consequence of the
strengthening of particular connections and the weakening of other types of connections. Those
are the two major forms of neuroplasticity. Okay, so I don't want you to get the impression that
there's something wrong with my memory and that I forgot that this episode is not about short or Those are the two major forms of neuroplasticity. Okay, so I don't want you to get the impression
that there's something wrong with my memory
and that I forgot that this episode
is not about short or long-term memory,
but it's about working memory.
And indeed, I have not forgotten.
So now is where I tell you why I've been talking
about short and long-term memory
and the mechanisms of those
because I want them to provide a stark contrast
for what we call working memory.
Working memory, as far as we know,
does not involve neuroplasticity,
or at least if it does,
it's not a particularly robust aspect of working memory.
Rather, working memory is the reflection
of a particular neural circuit running an algorithm
over and over and over for different types of information,
but the information is in stored,
it is actually intentionally discarded.
Now, what sorts of daily activities
and life activities would require working memory?
The answer to that is basically everything
that you need to do, but that you don't want to remember.
Now, what types of things would those be?
Well, let's think about it.
Most all of us learned at some point in our life
to tie our own shoes.
Presumably, you know how to tie your own shoes.
If you don't, perhaps you should learn
or wear Velcro or slippers, I don't know.
But assuming you can tie your own shoes,
that's something that you know how to do
and you can do it as a procedural long-term memory.
You can do that action.
You don't have to think about it too much.
Working memory would come into play
when say you wake up in the morning
and you know that you need to head out for a jog
but you also need to make a cup of coffee first
and you need to remember where the coffee
is where your shoes are
and perhaps you're making a phone call
or you're having a conversation
while you need to tie your shoes
and so on and so forth.
Working memory is basically the taking in of information
that's critical for you to sequence your actions
over a short period of time and then forget that sequence.
For instance, I'm willing to bet
that you put your shoes on to go running
before you go running, that's sort of a duh.
And if you're like me, you drink your water,
your coffee, your yerba mate before you go running.
The point here is that if you wake up in the morning
and you like caffeine before you go for a run,
there are a certain series of action steps
that you need to carry out to hydrate,
make that cup of coffee or tea,
drink it, put on your shoes, head out the door.
You need to sequence things properly,
but you don't want to commit your long-term
or even your short-term memory stores
to carrying out that sequence.
You simply want to be able to carry out that sequence
and then discard that information about the sequence
and focus your attention on, for instance,
what trajectory you're going to run
through the park or around your neighborhood.
Then you want to discard that information
and you want to lean into the next portion of your day
and so on and so on.
In fact, working memory is involved
in essentially every activity,
both cognitive and motor,
from the point you wake up in the morning
until the time you go to sleep at night
for every single day of your life.
And we know this because there are indeed people
who have diminished working memory
or even lack working memory entirely,
although the latter is somewhat rare, it has happened.
And as you can imagine,
they have a complete failure of ability
to sequence activities and their lives
are extremely difficult.
They need a ton of assistance from other people.
Even more assistance than do people
who have minimal or no long-term memory.
So this is really highlighting
just how important working memory is.
Working memory is basically the way
that you navigate any immediate environment.
And as I mentioned earlier,
it's very closely tied to attention
because in order to know what to do now
and then what to do subsequently
and then subsequent to that,
you need to be able to hold your attention
to the things you need to do.
So working memory and attention collaborate,
literally at a neural circuit level
and at a neurochemical level
in order to allow you to move through your day
in an adaptive functional way.
And people who have challenges with attention or focus
or working memory,
and sometimes it can be hard to dissociate
which one they're having challenges with,
really have a hard time moving through life
as compared to people whose attention
and working memory is more robust.
Now, the good news is today we're gonna talk about
working memory, some of the neural circuits involved,
and some of the neurochemicals involved
that can augment or improve working memory,
and we're also going to talk about what one can do
to directly increase the amount of neurotransmission
of those particular chemicals within the circuits
that control working memory, in other words,
to improve your working memory.
Now I can talk about working memory and the mechanisms, etc., all day long.
But as is often the case, sometimes it's better to not just learn about concepts, but actually
to experience them in real time.
So what we're going to do now is I'm actually going to give you a working memory test.
This is the sort of working memory test that you would take if you were to go into a psychology laboratory
or a neuroscience laboratory
and they were studying working memory in humans.
Now there's another advantage just doing this in real time
right here as you're listening
or as you're listening and watching.
And that's because you're going to get data,
you're going to get information
about what your baseline working memory capacity is
and you're going to want to keep those data
in your short-term memory stores,
maybe even your long-term memory stores,
but certainly your short-term memory stores
because shortly later in this episode,
I'm going to talk about different ways
to improve your working memory
depending on where your baseline working memory starts,
which by the way turns out to be a pretty good proxy for the levels
of a neuromodulator called dopamine within the neural circuits that control working
memory.
So right now, let's take a working memory task.
We're going to do this purely through audio form because I realize some people are watching
and listening to this on YouTube and others are just listening to this episode.
So there are not going to be any visual cues or slides that I present.
And that's perhaps what distinguishes what we're about
to do most from what would happen in a laboratory.
Typically in a laboratory,
there would be some visual presentation
of what I'm about to say.
But here, because of the format
that most of you are consuming this information by,
we're going to do this purely by audio.
So the first test of your working memory is very simple.
I'm going to read off a series of letters.
And your task is to remember
as many of those letters as you can.
The first string of letters is J, K, Z, P, I.
Okay, just to make this really easy,
I'm gonna say it twice.
Although typically in a working memory task,
it would just be said once,
but I'm gonna make this extra easy.
J-K-Z-P-I.
Okay?
Now, you in your own head can try
and recite back those letters if you like.
Okay, second string of letters.
R-O-M-K-L-E.
I'm gonna make this extra simple and do it again.
Not typical for a working memory task,
but there are some working memory tasks where that happens.
R-O-M-K-L-E.
Okay, now a third string of letters,
this one's gonna be a little bit longer.
So Q-up that working memory and attention.
W-A-C-Q-V-D-N.
I'll repeat that again.
W-A-C-Q-V-D-N. How many of the letters I just read can you remember?
Okay, so depending on how many letters you can remember, perhaps you have a low, moderate,
or high degree of working memory. Keep in mind that some of you are perhaps doing other things,
you're attending to driving, or other tasks within your home or your office.
And so perhaps you weren't able to pay full attention.
So there'll be some variation there.
But nonetheless, after reading each of those strings
of letters, you were asked to recall those letters
in your mind.
And if you wrote them down and you're rereading them,
yes, that's cheating.
But how about this?
What if I were to ask you now
about the simplest first string of letters,
the one that consisted of only five letters?
How many of you can remember any of those five letters now?
Okay, I can't hear you if you're shouting them out.
I can't see you if you're raising your hand.
But chances are most of you have forgotten
the first series of letters,
even though it was quite short and you could remember it early on.
That ability to remember that string of letters
when you first heard them,
and indeed I read them twice.
So I'd be very surprised if any of you couldn't remember
that string of letters after hearing them twice,
but I also read you some other letters in the interim,
so that now, just a couple minutes later,
I'm asking you to remember that first string of five letters
and assuming that you didn't write it down
and you're not cheating, chances are you remember
anywhere from two to zero of those letters
in that first word, which is a perfect example
of your working memory.
Nothing got committed to short term,
much less long term memory,
rather your working memory was able to work
with that information, hold it in mind
for just as long as you thought you needed
to know that information,
but then, thank goodness, that information was discarded.
You didn't know that I was gonna ask you
for that first string of letters.
Again, after reading you the longer string of letters,
but I did that deliberately
to show you how your working memory works.
So in some sense, the working memory task is a bit unusual
in that it's a test of yes, memory in the very, very short-term,
but also a test of your ability to forget,
to discard information that's not critical.
And that gets us back to the original definition
of working memory, which is our ability to attend
to specific small batches of information,
remember it for just as long as we think we need to,
and then to discard that information.
And by the way, if you want to know
what those first five letters were,
they were JKZPI.
I'd like to take a brief moment
and thank one of our sponsors, and that's AG1.
AG1 is a vitamin mineral probiotic drink
that also contains adaptogens.
I started taking AG1 way back in 2012.
The reason I started taking it
and the reason I still take it every day
is that it ensures that I meet all of my quotas
for vitamins and minerals.
And it ensures that I get enough prebiotic
and probiotic to support gut health.
Now gut health is something that over the last 10 years
we realized is not just important
for the health of our gut,
but also for our immune system
and for the production of neurotransmitters
and neuromodulators, things like dopamine and serotonin.
In other words, gut health is critical
for proper brain functioning.
Now, of course, I strive to consume healthy whole foods
for the majority of my nutritional intake every single day,
but there are a number of things in AG1,
including specific micronutrients that are hard
to get from whole foods or at least in sufficient quantities.
So AG1 allows me to get the vitamins and minerals
that I need, probiotics, prebiotics, the adaptogens
and critical micronutrients.
So anytime somebody asks me if they were to take
just one supplement, what that supplement should be,
I tell them AG1 because AG1 supports so many different
systems within the body that are involved in mental health, physical health and performance. what that supplement should be, I tell them AG1, because AG1 supports so many different systems
within the body that are involved in mental health,
physical health, and performance.
To try AG1, go to drinkag1.com slash huberman,
and you'll get a year's supply of vitamin D3K2
and five free travel packs of AG1.
Again, that's drinkag1.com slash huberman.
So now's where we talk a little bit about
the neural circuitry and the neurochemistry
of working memory.
Now it's important that we do this because in a few minutes you're also going to learn
that people generally fall into two broad bins of having a high or low baseline of a
certain neurochemical in the brain that affords them either high or low working memory capacity.
Now in reality, it's a distribution.
In fact, it's what we call a normal distribution.
So it really isn't two bins,
but during today's discussion,
and in fact, in a lot of laboratory studies,
we can actually bin people into these two groups.
The neural circuitry underlying working memory
involves a lot of different brain locations,
that is a lot of different neural networks
collaborating to create this thing we call working memory.
However, there are a couple of key hubs,
that is locations within the brain
that are especially important for working memory.
The ones that I'd like to focus on today
involve the prefrontal cortex.
So this is neural real estate
that resides just behind the forehead.
And the neurons in the brainstem,
so further back in the brain,
that manufacture dopamine and that send their little wires
that we call axons up to the prefrontal cortex
to release dopamine.
Dopamine is a neuromodulator.
Many people are familiar with dopamine
and familiar with it in the context of motivation and drive.
Sometimes people mistakenly think
it's only involved in pleasure,
but dopamine is involved in motivation and drive.
When dopamine systems go awry,
that is if their levels get too high,
that can create manic states, it can create addictive states.
When dopamine levels are too low,
you can get movement challenges such as in Parkinson's,
which is a deficit,
where a literal destruction of the neurons
that manufacture dopamine.
There are a bunch of different areas of the brain
that those dopamine neurons in the brainstem project to,
but for right now we're going to focus
almost entirely on the dopamine projections
from the brainstem to the prefrontal cortex,
which is called the mesocortical circuitry.
I'm not gonna get into the origins or the meaning of the mesocortical circuitry. I'm not gonna get into the origins
or the meaning of the mesocortical
versus other dopamine projection systems.
I did that in a couple of episodes about ADHD
and attention and dopamine in particular.
And you can find those at hubermanlab.com.
Just put dopamine and circuits into the search function
and it will take you to those particular timestamps
where I described that.
But since we wanna keep things fairly top contour
at the level of neural circuitry here,
just know that there are a bunch of neurons
that manufacture dopamine back in the brainstem
that send their axons, those little wires,
up to the prefrontal cortex,
and that the amount of dopamine released per unit time,
so in a certain amount of time,
strongly dictates the extent to which
working memory capacity is going to be high, medium, or low.
Now I want to be very clear,
because I'm gonna come back to this a little bit later,
again and again.
It is the case that when dopamine levels are lower,
that is either there are fewer neurons
that have the potential to release dopamine
in the frontal cortex, or for whatever reason reason less is being released in the frontal cortex,
that working memory performance tends to be lower as compared to conditions where dopamine release
or the availability of dopamine is higher. However, it is not the case that more dopamine is always going
to equate to improved working memory. This is so important that I'm going to say it again.
It is not always the case that increasing
the amount of dopamine transmission
in the frontal cortex leads to improvements
in working memory.
There is a specific criteria that allows us to predict
whether or not it will improve or maintain
or actually degrade working memory performance.
So before you head to the end of the podcast
to try and figure out ways to increase dopamine
to improve working memory, please keep that fact in mind figure out ways to increase dopamine to improve working memory,
please keep that fact in mind.
Don't just commit it to your working memory,
commit it to your short and long-term memory,
because that's very important if your goal
is to improve your working memory.
With that said, I do want to describe just a little bit
of research showing the relationship between
having a low working memory span, as it's called,
the ability to only remember a few letters or numbers
or short batches of information
as compared to a high working memory span,
meaning longer strings of letters, longer strings of numbers,
which of course in the real world translates
to being able to carry out shorter
versus longer action sequences,
as described earlier in the scenario
where you're getting up in the morning
and you're making coffee and you're heading out for run,
et cetera, et cetera.
People do differ in terms of their working memory capacity.
And there's a classic study done by Kuhls
and Desposito and colleagues.
This was published in 2008,
where they had a way to label the amount of dopamine
that is available for release in the frontal cortex
in human subjects.
They did this by the injection of a specific dye
that dye gets taken up specifically by the neurons
in the brain that manufacture dopamine.
Then they were able to image the brains of those people
while those people were wide awake
using something called positron emission tomography.
Again, the specific tool isn't necessarily important
but since some of you like to know.
And what they found is that for people
that had a high working memory span, that is, could remember long strings of numbers or letters
or other information, they tended to be the people that had more dopamine available for
release in the frontal cortex, either because they had more of the dopamine neurons themselves
or similar number of neurons, but those neurons had more dopamine to release, okay?
And they also found the converse.
Individuals that had a low working memory span
and ability had less dopamine available for release.
So that establishes a correlation, but it's not causal.
A different study, which is also a classic,
was carried out by Brasovsky, Brown, Rosswald and Goldman.
And this is a really important study
because in this study,
they were able to introduce small amounts of dopamine
directly into the cortex
and evaluate working memory capacity.
Now, anytime a working memory test is done,
the same pattern always emerges.
This is regardless of any dopamine
being infused into the brain,
which is people and animals for that matter
are very good at remembering short spans
of numbers, letters, or other types of information.
So if you tell them one thing, like the letter A,
and then you ask them, do you remember the letter?
Almost everybody remembers that.
But if you give them a string of 10 letters,
they remember fewer of those 10 letters.
That's sort of obvious,
but it's important point to emphasize nonetheless.
And so there's a kind of a dropping off curve
of performance as one progresses from fewer
to greater number of items to be remembered.
In this study, when dopamine was introduced
to the frontal cortex, the number of things
that individuals could remember simply increased.
It was a very straightforward result. More dopamine
introduced allowed longer letter number and information strings to be remembered. And
of course forgotten because that's what working memory involves. Remembering and then discarding
of information shortly thereafter. Now, the findings I just described complement what
I said before, which is the naturally occurring experiment. Bring people into the lab, measure
their working memory span,
look at how much dopamine they make,
higher dopamine, better working memory,
lower dopamine, lower working memory.
The experiment I just described
was one in which dopamine is introduced,
showing that dopamine is very likely the rate limiting
or the capacity limiting,
that's probably the better way to put it,
the capacity limiting neuromodulator
for improving working memory. That's a fancy better way to put it, the capacity limiting neuromodulator for improving working memory.
That's a fancy nerd speak way of saying more dopamine
allows for better working memory.
But a critical feature of this experiment
is that they did a number of experiments
where they didn't introduce dopamine,
but instead they introduced other neuromodulators
to the prefrontal cortex such as norepinephrine or serotonin.
And the interesting finding is that the addition prefrontal cortex, such as norepinephrine or serotonin.
And the interesting finding is that the addition
of norepinephrine or serotonin, which of course,
are other neuromodulators that can change the firing patterns
of neurons in the prefrontal cortex, but elsewhere as well.
It's just that in this case,
they were added to the prefrontal cortex,
had no effect on working memory.
It neither improved nor degraded working memory
when those neuromodulators were introduced.
In other words, dopamine and perhaps only dopamine
seems to be the dominant neuromodulator
for regulating the degree that is whether or not
you have small, medium, or large amounts
of working memory capacity in the prefrontal cortex.
And of course, there have been a bunch of other experiments
that are worth mentioning briefly in this context,
such as taking people that have a high working memory
capacity and indeed have their brains imaged
and one sees that they have high levels of baseline dopamine,
especially the dopamine projecting
to the prefrontal cortex.
And then they're given a drug that depletes dopamine
within the prefrontal cortex
and their performance drops.
And so it's so nice about the literature
around working memory is that while I'm not covering
all of that literature exhaustively, it all tends to jive.
It all points in a direction whereby the levels of dopamine
being released in the prefrontal cortex
during working memory tasks correlates very strongly
with capacity to perform working memory tasks.
Lower dopamine, lower working memory span as it's called.
Higher dopamine, higher working memory span.
Okay, so next we're going to do another working memory task, different than the one we did
earlier.
And we're going to do that with a specific purpose in mind, which is for you to be able
to determine what your working memory capacity is.
And by extension, your baseline levels of dopamine,
or at least the levels of dopamine
that are likely being released into your prefrontal cortex
while you do these working memory tasks.
In other words, we're going to try and figure out
whether or not you are of the low,
medium, or high working memory capacity.
And of course, we're doing that in part
to try and establish whether or not
you likely have low, medium, or high amounts
of dopamine available for release
in the prefrontal cortex.
Now of course we're not putting you
into a positron emission tomography scanning device.
We aren't able to do that for obvious reasons.
But keep in mind that what we were about to do
is very similar and in some cases identical
to laboratory studies where the researchers were trying to determine what people's levels
of dopamine within these particular neural networks we've been discussing, the mesocortical
pathway are likely to be.
In other words, performance on the working memory tasks that we are about to do is a
decent indication of what the dopamine levels that are available for release in your prefrontal
cortex perhaps might be.
Now I say perhaps might be because I don't want to cause
any unnecessary alarm if for instance you fall
into the low working memory span group.
In fact, if you fall into the low working memory span group,
there are actually some terrific tools that you can use
to improve dopamine transmission in those pathways
and improve your working memory.
I also don't want people to get the impression
that somehow performance on this working memory task
is reflective of some larger dopamine issue in the brain.
And certainly it is not, I repeat,
it is not diagnostic of Parkinson's
or any kind of neurodegenerative condition.
Although I will say that deficits in working memory
are common in patients with Parkinson's for obvious reasons.
Those patients have deficits in dopamine neurons,
not only production, but the number of dopamine neurons.
It's one of the hallmark features of Parkinson's,
but also in things like traumatic brain injury, et cetera.
But the working memory tasks that you're about to take,
when given to a general population or a group of undergraduates
or so-called normals or typical control subjects,
which all of you are, okay?
So unless you're dealing with a traumatic brain injury
or you know you have Parkinson's,
we know that the data that you're gonna get back right now
is very similar to the data that people get back
when they do these sorts of studies in a laboratory.
That is, it's typical for some people
to have a short working memory span,
some people to have a medium working memory span,
and some people to have a high working memory span. And today we're actually just going to divide into two bins, short working memory span, some people have a medium working memory span, and some people have a high working memory span.
And today we're actually just going to divide into two bins,
short working memory span and high working memory span.
And we can have some degree of confidence
that correlates with the amount of dopamine available
for release in the frontal cortex.
But, and this is a very important point,
as we progress along this discussion of working memory,
the neural circuits, dopamine, et cetera,
I wanna make clear something that I said earlier,
which is that it is not the case
that increasing the amount of dopamine that's available
always increases working memory spent.
In fact, there's a common circumstance
whereby people with a relatively high degree
of working memory capacity
increase their dopamine levels even further
using pharmacology or other methods that we'll discuss
and their performance actually can degrade, okay?
So if any of that is confusing now,
we'll make it all very simple going forward
so that if you decide to implement any of the protocols
discussed in this episode,
that you are aware of what you can expect
and whether or not you are in the category of people
that should or perhaps should not incorporate
those protocols.
Okay, let's test your working memory again.
This time, the working memory task
is going to be a little bit different
than the one you did previously.
This working memory task involves me
reading six different sentences to you.
And your job is to pay attention to these six sentences
because you're going to be asked some information
about these sentences in a few moments.
The first sentence is real estate costs are going up.
The second sentence is the Atlantic Ocean
is warm in summer.
The third sentence is there's a lot of interest now
in electric cars.
The fourth sentence is some reptiles eat only once a year.
The fifth sentence is,
kids nowadays look at screens more than 60%
of their waking life.
And the sixth and final sentence is,
football can mean different sports
depending on the country.
Okay, so I read you six sentences.
They were moderately long, I confess.
Your job for the working memory task is now to recall
as many of the final words of each of those sentences
as you can.
I'll give you a few moments to do that.
Now, before I tell you what the final word
of each of those sentences actually is,
I wanna remind everybody that working memory capacity
follows a normal distribution.
So some of you will be able to remember
the final word of perhaps five or even six
of those sentences, although I must say
that is exceedingly rare.
Some of you are going to be able to remember
three to four of the final words of those sentences,
and that's more typical.
That actually represents the average,
or the mean as we call it.
And then fewer people, although still many of you,
will only be able to remember one or two
of the final words of those sentences.
Okay, so now I'm assuming that most of you
have tried to call to memory the final word
of as many of those six sentences as you can.
And maybe you've written them down
or you've typed them into your phone
or you have some record of what you recall
those six final words of those sentences are.
Now I'm going to tell you the actual final word
of each of those sentences.
The final word of the first sentence was up
because as you may recall,
the sentence was real estate costs are going up.
The final word of the second sentence was summer
because the sentence was the Atlantic ocean
is warm in summer.
The final word of the third sentence was cars
because the sentence was there is a lot of interest
in electric cars.
The final word of the fourth sentence was year
because the sentence was,
some reptiles eat only once a year.
The final word of the fifth sentence was life
because the sentence was,
kids nowadays look at screens more than 60%
of their waking life.
And the final word of the sixth sentence was country
because the sentence was,
football can mean different sports
depending on the country.
Okay, so be honest with yourself and tell yourself,
and you don't have to tell anyone else if you don't want to,
how many of the final words of those six sentences
you could remember correctly?
It's important that you remember them correctly.
Again, the number of words that you can recall,
that is your working memory span,
is going to vary from person to person.
But we can take the normal distribution of those scores
and sort of draw a line down the middle
and say that if you could remember three to six
of the final words of those sentences correctly,
you're going to fall into the high working memory span group.
Whereas if you could only remember one or two
or maybe zero of the final words of those six sentences,
then you're going to be in the low working memory span group.
Again, I don't wanna alarm anybody.
This doesn't mean that you have any global memory deficits
or dopamine deficits, but it is important,
especially if you plan to apply any of the protocols
to improve working memory that you faithfully,
that is you accurately report your working memory performance
at least to yourself.
Now, as you recall, whether or not you have low or high,
and here we are just bending into low and high,
there's no medium.
We've divided right at that line,
we're saying if you can remember three to six,
we're calling that high working memory span,
at least for this discussion.
And if you can remember fewer than three,
even down to zero of the final words of those sentences,
that's low working memory span.
We're dividing it in two.
We divided you into two groups.
And we do know when this has been done
in large numbers of human subjects,
and some, in some cases all of those subjects
have their brains imaged for the amount of dopamine
available for release in their prefrontal cortex,
that short working memory span correlates
with lower amounts of dopamine,
whereas higher working memory span
or longer working memory span,
whatever you want to call it,
correlates with more dopamine available
for release in the prefrontal cortex.
Now, this is where things get really interesting
and frankly, really exciting for everybody,
especially the folks in the low working memory
span group.
Work from Martis Pizito and colleagues at UC Berkeley, as well as other laboratories,
have explored the consequences of increasing dopamine levels in the brain of typical populations
of individuals.
So these are not people with Parkinson's or TBI, but undergraduate students,
which we do realize is not completely representative
of the quote unquote normal population
outside the university,
but also people from the community.
So people who are not university students and so on.
And the ways that they've increased dopamine
in those individuals
had tended to rely on pharmacology.
So these are prescription drugs
that most often have been developed
for the treatment of Parkinson's
in order to increase dopamine levels,
but for some other purposes as well.
Drugs like baromocryptine,
which we know are so-called dopamine agonists.
An agonist is a drug that has the consequence
of increasing the amount of a given neurochemical,
in this case, dopamine.
Whereas an antagonist is a drug
that either blocks
or prevents or somehow lowers the total available amount
of a certain chemical, such as dopamine or serotonin, et cetera.
So bromocryptine is a drug that increases dopamine.
So when human subjects came into a laboratory,
didn't take any drug, no bromocryptine yet,
and of course they were being evaluated
for whether or not they were taking any meds for ADHD,
their caffeine consumption, et cetera.
There were certain rule ins and rule outs for that study.
But certainly people that were taking
any kind of prescription medication for ADHD
were not included in the study
or were eliminated from the study
because those drugs can indeed increase dopamine
as well as some other neuromodulators
such as norepinephrine and epinephrine.
I covered all that in the two ADHD episodes that I did, which again, you can find at
hubermanlab.com.
Just go to the search function, put in ADHD.
In any event, in these studies, they took people that had not taken any drugs to increase
dopamine, had their working memory measured very similarly to the way that you measured
your working memory a few minutes ago with the six sentence business that we did.
And then they took bromocryptine
and they either took a low, a moderate,
or high dose of bromocryptine.
And 90 minutes later, they took a working memory task.
And what was observed was very interesting.
You can probably predict what it is
based on everything I've set up until now.
Individuals that initially had low baseline levels
of dopamine and therefore shorter working memory span,
so they only remembered zero to about three
of the final words of that sixth sentence series,
their performance significantly improved.
They were able to remember four and in some cases
up to six of the final words of those sentences.
Now, that is in complete agreement
with everything we set up until now.
Simply says that dopamine is important for working memory.
If you start off with lower dopamine stores
or dopamine availability for release
in the prefrontal cortex,
lower working memory performance,
increased dopamine through ingestion of bromocryptine,
which is this dopamine agonist.
All of the circuit changes that we want
and would expect to improve working memory occur,
and indeed working memory improves.
Very straightforward.
That's interesting, but the even more interesting part
of the study is that individuals
that already had high working memory span,
when they took bromocryptine at a low or a moderate dose,
their working memory did not increase further.
Now, if somebody was already getting six of the final words
of those six sentences, well then of course,
they couldn't improve their performance anymore.
But many of the people in the high working memory span group,
of course, only remembered four in some cases, three,
typically it'll be four, five or six
of the final words of those sentences. When they took bromecryptine at low or moderate doses, their working memory
did not improve significantly. There was either no change or a very modest change. And here's where
things get really interesting. When individuals who already had a high working memory span took
the highest dose of bromecryptine, and by the way, studies verified that the amount of dopamine available indeed increased.
So that was important to do and they did that.
Well their more working memory performance actually decreased such that now they had
a short or a low working memory span.
So what this tells us is that the relationship between dopamine and working memory follows
an inverted U-shaped function.
So imagine a U and then just flip it over,
meaning if you have low dopamine availability
in the prefrontal cortex, working memory span is short.
As you increase that amount, working memory becomes greater,
but if you increase the amount of dopamine
in the prefrontal cortex too much,
working memory span actually drops significantly below
the baseline that you started with.
Now this is important for a number of reasons,
not the least of which is the known relationship
between working memory and attention.
Now this is very important to understand
in the context of ADHD,
but also for people who don't have ADHD
and are struggling to maintain focus and attention
and carry out working memory tasks
throughout their normal everyday life,
not in the laboratory, but just moving through life.
Because these days we hear a lot, a lot, a lot
about people struggling with focus and attention.
Perhaps, we don't know, perhaps in part due to
overuse of smartphones, social media, et cetera,
although there's not yet a direct causal relationship
that's been established.
The data that are emerging suggests that indeed overuse
of those things can cause problems,
but regardless of the source,
there does seem to be more ADHD both in kids and in adults
and subclinical challenges in focus and attention.
And here's where things get really interesting
as it relates to the neural circuitry.
Work from Desposito and colleagues
and other laboratories as well
have shown using the similar paradigm
that I described before,
giving people drugs to increase their baseline levels
of dopamine above their initial starting point
of short or long-term memory span capacity.
And then had people perform different types
of working memory tasks that tap into two different aspects
of attention and working memory. Up until now two different aspects of attention and working memory.
Up until now, we've been talking about working memory,
it's kind of just one thing.
But working memory actually involves two things,
or at least two things.
The first is that in order to carry out
a working memory task and to attend to something,
to really focus, we need the ability
to rule out distractors.
We need to be able to not pay attention
to things that would otherwise distract us. In addition to that, we need to be able to not pay attention to things that would otherwise distract us.
In addition to that, we need to be able to switch
from one context to the next, making the cup of coffee,
to putting on one shoes and heading out the door,
and in some cases, layering different contexts together,
talking on the phone while tying one's shoes,
and so on and so forth.
What this work shows us is that the ability to task switch
and context switch, that is to
shift around what it is that we're paying attention to and interleave different things
that we're paying attention to, something that's so critical for moving through our
daily lives, is largely dependent on the dopamine projections to a structure in the brain called
the basal ganglia, which is a structure I've talked about before on this podcast, but if
you didn't hear about it, we can just broadly define this structure
as being involved in movement generation
and stopping movement generation.
In fact, it's often discussed as the neural circuitry
that generates go as in do commands and no go,
don't do commands.
So the basal ganglia are involved in task switching
and they are involved in task switching in part
by sending certain commands to go, do certain things,
and no go to not do other things, okay?
Task switching, stop doing this, start doing that.
Start doing that, stop doing this.
And sometimes to varying extents, right?
I mean, we could take any real world scenario
of tying one's shoes while talking on the phone,
and we could microanalyze it in the context of this,
but I think if you think about it just a little bit,
you understand that in order
to perform daily tasks, we need to be able to task switch
and that's not always a start one task end,
start a new task end.
Oftentimes we're interleaving different tasks
to varying degrees.
Now the other aspect of working memory and attention
is to eliminate distractions, to not pay attention
to the irrelevant stuff in one's environment
or even the irrelevant stuff on your own body.
Like you can't get distracted by a button
that might be only partially buttoned,
or maybe a little something on your sleeve,
if you're trying to do something else at that moment.
Okay, and people with ADHD,
and people who have subclinical challenges and focus
really have a hard time with this, right?
You know, the sort of stereotype is,
you know, the, oh look, a squirrel, that whole thing.
But really this typically exists as a more subtle and challenging phenomenon for people
where they either can't remember what they were doing or they're simply drawn down different
trajectories, different thought trajectories or action trajectories.
And then they have a hard time making it back to the original thing that they were trying
to focus on.
And we know based on these studies of dopamine and neuroimaging that eliminating distractors
is largely the consequence of dopamine neurons
projecting to the prefrontal cortex.
Okay, now why am I telling you all this neural circuitry stuff?
Well, yes, there are a bunch of studies
showing that if you selectively activate the neurons
that send dopamine into the basal ganglia,
you improve task switching ability
without an improved ability to rule out distractors.
Or if you selectively increase the amount of dopamine
from neurons projecting the prefrontal cortex
that you're able to selectively improve
the elimination of distractors
without improving task switching ability.
For practical purposes in this discussion,
we wanna pay careful attention to whether or not
the data tell us that those particular protocols,
those particular approaches are globally increasing dopamine,
that is increasing the activity of dopamine neurons
projecting to the basal ganglia and the prefrontal cortex
or selectively to the basal ganglia
or selectively to the prefrontal cortex.
And what I can tell you now is that fortunately,
there are several protocols, some of which are behavioral,
some of which involve specific over-the-counter supplements
and some of which involve prescription pharmacology
that can tap into each of these systems independently
as well as globally increased dopamine
to improve focus and working memory at large.
I'd like to take a quick break
to acknowledge our sponsor, Element. Element is an electrolyte drink that has everything you need and nothing memory at large. I'd like to take a quick break to acknowledge our sponsor, Element.
Element is an electrolyte drink
that has everything you need and nothing you don't.
That means zero sugar
and the appropriate ratios of the electrolyte,
sodium, magnesium and potassium.
And that correct ratio of electrolytes
is extremely important because every cell in your body,
but especially your nerve cells, your neurons,
relies on electrolytes in order to function properly.
So when you're well hydrated
and you have the appropriate amount of electrolytes
in your system, your mental functioning
and your physical functioning is improved.
I drink one packet of element dissolved
in about 16 to 32 ounces of water
when I wake up in the morning,
as well as while I exercise.
And if I've sweat a lot during that exercise,
I often will drink a third element packet dissolved
in about 32 ounces of water after I exercise.
Element comes in a variety of different flavors,
all of which I find really tasty.
I like the citrus, I like the watermelon,
I like the raspberry, frankly I can't pick just one.
It also comes in chocolate and chocolate mint,
which I find taste best if they are put into water dissolved
and then heated up.
I tend to do that in the winter months
because of course you don't just need hydration on
hot days and in the summer and spring months but also in the winter when the temperatures
are cold and the environment tends to be dry.
If you'd like to try Element you can go to drinkelementspelled.com.com.
To try a free sample pack again that's drinkelement.com.
Okay so let's talk about protocols to improve working memory specifically by way of to try a free sample pack. Again, that's drinkelement.com slash Huberman.
Okay, so let's talk about protocols
to improve working memory,
specifically by way of changing levels of dopamine
in the brain.
Now I've discussed dopamine many times before
in this podcast.
In fact, we have entire episodes
devoted to optimizing and regulating dopamine.
And of course, dopamine comes up
within the context of the ADHD episodes
and other episodes
as well.
And again, if you have specific questions about dopamine or any other topic for that
matter, if you go to hubermanlab.com, that website has been engineered so that you can
put one word such as dopamine, but also multiple keywords.
So perhaps dopamine exercise or dopamine cold plunge, et cetera, into the search function.
And it will take you to the specific time stamps
of multiple episodes where those topics were discussed,
as well as newsletters where some of that information
has been condensed into short PDF form, et cetera.
So we certainly are going to cover some material
about improving dopamine for sake of improving
working memory now.
But if you're generally interested in the science
and pharmacology of dopamine and protocols
to modulate dopamine levels,
all of that can be found at hubermanlab.com.
Okay, so let's say you have a short working memory span
or a moderate working memory span
and you want to experiment with increasing levels
of dopamine for sake of improving working memory.
Now, there are a lot of different ways
that one could imagine doing that.
Let's start with the behavioral tools known
to increase dopamine stores.
That is shown in peer reviewed studies to increase dopamine stores. That is shown in peer reviewed studies
to increase dopamine stores within certain circuits
of the brain that are relevant for working memory performance.
And the protocol that immediately leaps to mind
is the use of certain non-sleep deep rest protocols.
Now non-sleep deep rest or NSDR is actually a term
that I coined because there is a practice
that's been established for many hundreds of years
called yoga-needra, which actually means yoga sleep,
whereby individuals, potentially you,
if you decide to do them, lie down, listen to a script,
that is, listen to an audio script,
which generally instructs you to do long exhale breathing,
to deliberately relax your musculature of your face
and of your body.
And yoga-neidra typically also involves
doing certain intentions.
And the instruction always given at the beginning
of yoga nidra is that you should try to not fall asleep.
Now, some people sometimes fall asleep.
Some people don't fall asleep,
but the idea and there are data support
that yoga nidra puts people into
kind of a shallow pattern of sleep.
Certainly not deep sleep and not rapid eye movement sleep,
but it's a very interesting and unusual brain state
for which we're starting to understand more
and actually I have some plans
in the not too distant future to collaborate
with Matthew Walker, the author of the book,
Why We Sleep and some other colleagues
to try and figure out what exact patterns of neural activity
are taking place in the brain and rest of nervous system
during yoga, knee, and this similar protocol which I call non-sleep deep rest. what exact patterns of neural activity are taking place in the brain and rest of nervous system during Yoga Nidra
and this similar protocol which I call non-sleep deep rest.
The difference between Yoga Nidra and non-sleep deep rest
is that non-sleep deep rest doesn't include
any of the intentions and removes a lot of the kind of opaque
or sometimes called mystical language from the protocol.
Now, a great thing is that Yoga Nidra scripts or protocols
as well as NSDR scripts or protocols are available,
totally zero cost.
You can find them certainly on apps like Waking Up,
but also on YouTube.
For instance, if you put NSDR in my last name,
there's a 10 minute NSDR script there.
There are a lot of YogaNedra scripts.
If you prefer a female voice,
there are a lot of different excellent female voices
out there.
One in particular that I like very much is Kelly Boys.
First name Kelly, last name B-O-Y-S.
She has both Yoganidra and NSDR scripts
of various durations of anywhere from eight minutes
all the way out to I believe 45 minutes.
Why am I telling you all this?
Well, there've been several studies, but in particular one,
and I do realize we're talking about only one study,
but the results are really intriguing as it relates do realize we're talking about only one study, but the results are really intriguing
as it relates to what we're talking about today.
In this study, they had individuals do,
effectively, an NSDR protocol.
They call it Yoganidra, and the protocol they used
was essentially a Yoganidra script.
They had people lie down and listen to a Yoganidra script
and to perform Yoganidra, and they evaluated
the amount of dopamine available
within the brain both prior to
and after performing this Yoga Nidra script.
And what they discovered was that
after performing a Yoga Nidra protocol,
the baseline levels of dopamine,
that is the amount of dopamine available
in the basal ganglia and a few other structures
of the human brain, of course these are humans,
was increased by as much as 60%
as compared to individuals that did a different protocol,
not Yoganidra, not NSDR.
Now, did that study evaluate lots of different durations
of Yoganidra, aka NSDR?
No, they looked at fairly long hour plus Yoganidra sessions.
However, there's some other data that have explored
Yoganidra aka NSDR in the context of cognitive performance
and a few other circumstances,
all of which point to the fact that cognitive performance
and in particular, cognitive performance tasks
that have a working memory element to them.
So they weren't the exact working memory tasks that you did earlier, but they have a working memory element to them. So they weren't the exact working memory tasks
that you did earlier,
but they have a working memory element to them.
That is, subjects had to keep certain small batches
of information in mind and then discard that information
in order to be able to perform the task well.
All of those show significant improvements
in task performance.
So while something like NSDR or Yoga Nidra
might sound kind of mystical or kind of, you know, wishy-washy or I
guess as the kids say weak sauce to some of you, it is anything
but weak sauce. It is really powerful stuff. And it's powerful
stuff as it relates to the very neurochemicals and neural
circuits that are involved in working memory. So if I were to
take a step back and just say, okay, what are some zero cost, very low, if any risk,
protocols that one could perform
in order to improve dopamine levels
without having to ingest anything, take anything,
really do much of anything at all, except lie there,
do this progressive muscle relaxation.
There are a few other things involved in NSDR as well,
which you'll learn if you decide to try them,
and improve or
increase the levels of dopamine availability in the brain significantly.
Well then, NSDR and Yoga Nidra really are the first line tools if one wants to do that.
I think it's reasonable to say that.
And as I mentioned before, there's no reason to think that there's any risk of doing NSDR
Yoga Nidra, you know, provide that you're lying down in a safe place as opposed to like
in the middle of the road or something.
But assuming you do it in a safe location,
I would encourage you to try it for really
for 20 to 30 minutes when you first explore it.
Perhaps you do longer, although I personally have a hard time
doing long yoga nidra scripts regularly.
A full hour is a big commitment.
I don't generally have that much time.
I often will do a 10 minute NSDR. Have there been brain imaging experiments done for each and all of these Yoga Nidra
scripts to determine the amount or if there's any dopamine increase within the brain? No.
But I think that we can safely extrapolate from that wonderful study out of skin and
avial that showed that when human subjects do this Yoga Nidra protocol that there's
a significant increase in baseline dopamine levels within key neural structures
that relate to working memory.
Now, many of you have perhaps heard
that getting in a cold plunge or taking a cold shower
or provided you can do it safely,
getting into a cold ocean or a cold lake
can significantly, maybe even double or even triple,
your circulating dopamine levels.
And indeed, that is true.
It has been shown that when people get into cold water,
typically up to their neck,
and that cold water, by the way,
can range in temperature anywhere from low 40s to low 60s,
depending on how long you stay in,
that there is a significant increase
in the so-called circulating catecholamines.
What are the catecholamines?
The catecholamines are dopamine, norepinephrine, and epinephrine. Now, the evidence for the catecholamines. What are the catecholamines? The catecholamines are dopamine,
norepinephrine, and epinephrine.
Now the evidence for the catecholamine increase
in response to cold water
mainly stems from two studies and in particular one.
And in that particular study, they had people get into,
I wouldn't say super cold water,
it was in the low 60 degrees.
And by the way, I'm speaking in Fahrenheit here.
And they had those human subjects
submerged in water up to their neck.
I think they actually had them sitting in lawn chairs
on the bottom of a pool,
but again, their heads were above water
so they could breathe.
And they stayed in for quite a long while,
45 minutes or longer.
And it was observed that there was a big,
big statistically significant increase
in epinephrine nor epinephrine endopamine
that lasted several hours or more.
This is one of the reasons why if you've ever done deliberate cold exposure as it's called,
it often is uncomfortable when you get in, but then when you get out, you feel different.
You feel really good in most cases provided if you're me, you take a warm shower afterwards.
Yes, I like to do that.
I realize if you want to increase your metabolism, perhaps it's better to not warm up afterwards.
I like a nice warm shower or to get in the sauna afterwards. That's just me.
But nonetheless, deliberate cold exposure clearly induces a state shift of mind and body
that most people provided they do it correctly and they don't go into water that's far too cold for
them for too long. They report as pleasant. And I think it's reasonable to assume that some of that
is the consequence of these increases in catecholamines, which is why many people opt for a cold shower, which if you're me,
cold shower followed by a warm or hot shower, or a cold plunge in the morning, or maybe even just
once or twice a week. Many people like them, typically people like getting out of them and
the feeling that they have after they do them, although some of you sickos really like the
feeling of getting in and being in it, but not me.
The point here is that if we were to take a look
at the landscape of zero cost behavioral tools,
in fact, behavioral tools that could potentially
save you money, meaning reduce your heating bill,
that are known to increase the very neurochemicals,
AKA dopamine, that are involved in improving working memory,
I think it's reasonable to assume that a cold shower,
about 30 to 60 minutes prior to doing any kind
of working memory task or any kind of activity
that would require increased focus could be, okay?
We don't know, the specific studies have not been done,
but could be in theory, it makes sense mechanistically,
it's logically sound, could be done
after deliberate cold exposure.
And indeed, many people report not just feeling
a bit of mild euphoria or feeling good
after deliberate cold exposure,
but also an increased capacity to focus.
In fact, so much so that a lot of people
who do deliberate cold exposure say
that they don't require as much caffeine
in order to maintain their alertness and energy,
which shouldn't be surprising to us at all, right?
I mean, it's increasing catecholamines.
We know this.
So that's another protocol that you could explore as well.
Is there an important difference or not
between deliberate cold exposure done by cold shower
or deliberate cold exposure in a cold plunge or the ocean?
Frankly, there haven't been a lot of studies comparing those,
but I think it stands to reason that if you have access
to a cold plunge or a cold body of water
that you can safely get into up to your neck
for 30 seconds to a minute,
if it's 50 degrees or less, right?
If you get in colder water, we know.
For instance, if you get into, say, 45 degree water
and you only get in for 30 seconds,
you're going to get a big increase in the catecholamines,
perhaps as big as the catecholamine increase
that you would get from being in 60 degree water
for 45 minutes.
Most people don't have 45 minutes
to sit around in water up to their neck.
So most people opt for 30 seconds
to as much as three minutes,
deliberate cold exposure in a shower
or cold plunge or other body of water.
Again, only do this if you can do it safely.
Never ever, please, for the love of God,
please never ever do any kind of hyperventilation,
breathing or breath holding
while doing deliberate cold exposure because you can pass out, you can die. Don't combine
breath work and deliberate cold exposure. Just don't separate those two things completely.
Okay? But deliberate cold exposure, we know is a very reliable way to increase the catecholamines,
which includes dopamine. So if you want to explore deliberate cold exposure protocols,
we get into the nuance of temperature and duration, et cetera,
you can find that completely zero cost.
Go to hubermanlab.com, go to the menu tab,
scroll down a newsletter,
and go to the cold exposure newsletter
where it details all of that in short PDF form.
Now, some of you are probably asking,
hey, what if I was in the high
or long working memory span group?
I ought to have high baseline levels of dopamine.
Should I not do yoga,Nedra or NSDR?
Should I not do deliberate cold exposure?
Well, there you're just going to have to experiment.
Again, there's essentially zero risk
to doing YogaNedra, NSDR, as I mentioned before.
Deliberate cold exposure, there's always some risk
getting into water, cold water.
People always want to know how cold.
Well, the newsletter gets to this,
but I'll just tell you right now as well.
The ideal temperature is the temperature
that you can safely get into and stay in for a duration
of 30 seconds to three minutes before getting out.
Some people opt to go longer,
but I think 30 seconds to three minutes
is a good duration to work with for most people,
especially if you're gonna do it frequently.
So the temperature should be safe for you
to stay in for that duration,
but uncomfortable enough that there's some impulse
to want to get out, that you have to work to stay in there,
that you have to kind of overcome that adrenaline release
and the impulse to get out.
So for some people, that's going to be 45 degrees,
for some people, it'll be 40 degrees,
depends on how cold adapted you are,
depends on how rested you are,
there is no specific temperature.
You have to really gauge for yourself.
And so err on the side of caution
and you can experiment, provided you experiment
within the margins of safety.
So if you found during the working memory tasks
that you took today that you have a very good working memory,
I don't think there's any reason to avoid
Yoganidra NSDR and deliberate cold exposure.
In fact, there may be reasons to increase your dopamine
and other catecholamines by way of NSDR, Yoganidra, deliberate cold exposure. In fact, there may be reasons to increase your dopamine and other catecholamines by way of NSDR,
yoga, and NEDRA, deliberate cold exposure.
Perhaps for working memory performance,
maybe it could increase further.
Perhaps it would decrease performance.
In which case there, you got your answer.
You don't have to do those protocols again,
and you certainly wouldn't want to do them before anything
that involves a lot of working memory and attention.
But of course, those protocols have other benefits as well.
So there's no reason to avoid them entirely, just perhaps avoid them within the context of trying to of working memory and attention. But of course, those protocols have other benefits as well. So there's no reason to avoid them entirely,
just perhaps avoid them within the context
of trying to improve working memory.
However, if you're somebody that has challenges
with working memory, challenges with attention,
challenges with focus, well, and I think that the protocols
I've been talking about up until now
would be an excellent first foray
into the sorts of things that you could do
to increase dopamine and of course those other
catecholamines as a way to see whether or not
it augments your focus and attention
and work in memory capacity.
Now some of you are probably shouting, shouting, shouting.
What about exercise?
Does an exercise increase dopamine?
It does, yes, there are other things that increase dopamine.
It's not just exercise.
There are activities that increase dopamine.
Some people are probably saying,
wait, doesn't playing video games increase dopamine?
Sex increases dopamine?
Chocolate increases dopamine?
Yes.
Yes indeed, those things can increase dopamine.
What's interesting and important about the protocols I've been talking about however,
NSDR, yoga, knee-drift, deliberate cold exposure is not just that they increase dopamine but
the duration over which they increase dopamine. This is
very important. If you want to understand more about the relationship between dopamine spikes,
as they're called, and dopamine baseline, and why I'm emphasizing these tools that
cause large, long-lasting increases in baseline dopamine, check out the episodes I did on optimizing
dopamine. We've got a link to them in the show note captions. Now, before I talk about other ways to
increase dopamine
for sake of improving working memory,
it's like over the counter supplements
like L-tyrosine, Macuneapurine, things like that.
I do briefly want to mention, and I promise briefly,
I know sometimes I say briefly
and then I spend 20 minutes telling you about something,
but very briefly, I just want to spend two minutes
telling you about protocols that we do not yet know
whether or not they
increase dopamine levels, but we do know that they improve working memory because after all this
episode is about working memory, not just about dopamine and working memory. It has been shown
that the use of binaural beats, okay, binaural beats being the presentation or the listening to
sounds of different frequencies in the two ears, typically by headphones. I think that's been shown to work best.
And there's a subtraction between the two frequencies
such that the brain tends to entrain
or start to follow a particular frequency
within not the entire brain, but certain neural circuits.
So if you've heard of, say, 15 Hertz binaural beats
or 40 Hertz binaural beats,
that doesn't mean that you listen to a 15 Hertz sound
or a 40 Hertz sound.
You listen to two different frequencies of sound.
Hertz is just a measurement of sound frequency
in each of the two ears.
And then the difference between them
is 40 Hertz or 15 Hertz.
And there are several studies that show not enormous.
Okay, I wanna be clear,
small to moderate improvements
in working memory performance,
but in some cases, significant improvement.
And I'll provide a link to these two papers
in the show note captions,
but I'll just briefly describe them by way of their title
and their major conclusions.
The first is a study entitled,
the effects of binaural and monoreal beat stimulation
on cognitive functioning in subjects
with different levels of emotionality.
A really interesting study published in 2019,
it was a relatively small number of subjects,
so only 24 participants, 16 males,
eight males, between 19 and 31 years old.
Listen to these 40 Hertz binaural beats.
And by the way, it's very easy to find apps
and other sources of 40 Hertz binaural beats
at zero cost or nominal cost out there.
You simply look for 40 Hertz binaural beats
and looked at performance on working memory tasks,
as well as some other cognitive tasks,
and found in some cases a small to moderate,
but significant improvement
in cognitive performance on working memory tasks.
The aspect of the study looking at emotionality
did not find a significant effect,
so it doesn't seem that emotionality impacts things there,
but nonetheless, that study study plus the other one
entitled the effect of binaural beats
on visual spatial working memory and cortical connectivity.
This was a study published in 2016,
found generally something similar.
In this case, they were using 15 Hertz binaural beats
and here I'm paraphrasing,
produced network activity characteristic
of high information transfer
with consistent connection strengths.
What they're really talking about is changes
in neural activity patterns within the brain that led to,
or at least were correlated with,
improvements on visual spatial working memory.
Visual spatial working memory tasks are different
than the working memory tasks that you performed earlier.
Visual spatial working memory tasks involve
the cognitive generation that is within your head of the so-called
Visio-spatial sketch pad.
So it's this idea that you see something and then you kind
of sketch it out in your mind.
You have to know the relationships between things
in space, pay attention to what they are, keep those in mind
again, because it's working memory just as long as it's
necessary to perform a task.
That's what Visio-spatial working memory is.
As you can imagine, it translates to an enormous number
of everyday activities required for focus and attention
and learning and performance.
And indeed, 15 Hertz binaural beats was able to produce
a small but significant improvement
in that sort of working memory task.
So I wanna emphasize again, we don't know the relationship
between binaural beats and dopamine,
at least not from these studies,
but I felt I'd be remiss if I didn't mention
these two studies that show that 40 Hertz binaural beats,
15 Hertz binaural beats,
can indeed improve working memory performance.
And in these sorts of scenarios,
individuals are listening to the binaural beats
while they are doing the working memory task.
And in some cases,
before they are doing the working memory task. Either seems to some cases before they are doing the work in memory task.
Either seems to work, it depends on the study,
there are a bunch of other studies,
but I thought I'd mention binaural beats
because I know a number of people are interested in them.
Again, non-pharmacologic, zero cost
because you can find tools for binaural beat generation,
zero cost out there approaches to improving working memory.
Okay, what about over-the-counter compounds
that are known to increase circulating dopamine
that can potentially improve working memory
and that indeed have been shown in peer reviewed studies
to improve working memory by way of increasing circulating
presumably brain levels of dopamine?
Well, I can think of two specific categories of supplements
that is over-the-counter compounds that at least at this point in time are legal in the United States, that can increase dopamine
levels. Those two are L-tyrosine, which is an amino acid precursor to dopamine, and Mekuna
prurines, which is a, believe it or not, it's the velvety bean or the outer component of this velvety bean that contains or is equivalent to 99% L-dopa.
L-dopa is a key component in the biochemical cascade
leading to the production of dopamine.
In fact, L-dopa is often prescribed
for Parkinson's patients as a means
to increase their dopamine levels.
There are at least three studies that I am aware of
of the use of macuna prurines to increase dopamine
for the treatment of Parkinson's.
In other words, mucuna prurines increases dopamine levels.
And yes, it has been shown to improve some of those symptoms
of Parkinson's patients.
We're not talking about treatment of Parkinson's today.
I want to caution people against any sort of use
of supplements to treat Parkinson's or other conditions
without consulting your doctor.
All right, that's very, very critical to point out.
If we're talking about ways to increase dopamine
for sake of improving working memory
by way of supplementation,
I think we should start with L-tyrosine
because L-tyrosine, unlike Macuna Parins,
is a bit further up.
Actually, it's way further up
the biochemical cascade leading to dopamine production.
However, it has been shown in several studies
that L-tyrosine supplementation can indeed increase dopamine.
And moreover, and here I'm quoting the title of a study
published in 1999, which I realized is a few years back,
but of course there's some excellent studies
from a few years back or more.
Tyrosine improves working memory
in a multitasking environment.
Now, this particular study from Thomas et al.
has some interesting aspects
and some aspects that made me go a little bit wide-eyed, but not necessarily wide-eyed
because the results are so dramatic. In fact, when one looks at all of the data in this
paper, what you find is that supplementing with L-tyrosine as they did in this study
did indeed lead to improvements in working memory under multitask conditions, as the
title suggests. Those improvements
were significant, but they weren't enormous. Okay, they were statistically significant,
but they were not enormous increases. Now, what was enormous and the reason I got wide-eyed and
still get wide-eyed is that the dosages of tyrosine used in the study are really big. They had subjects
take 150 milligrams per kilogram of L-crystalline tyrosine. I had subjects take 150 milligrams per kilogram
of L-crystalline tyrosine.
I had them take it in applesauce for whatever reason
or placebo and they did a number of different
control conditions to make sure that whatever effects
of L-tyrosine they observed were in fact due
to L-tyrosine supplementation.
Why am I going wide eyed when I see this 150 milligrams
per kilogram of tyrosine, well I weigh 220 pounds,
so that's about 100 kilograms. So if I weigh 100 kilograms and it's 150 milligrams for every
kilogram, that means that if I were a subject in the study that they would give me 15,000 milligrams, that is 15 grams of L-tyrosine
prior to doing these cognitive tasks.
Now, 15 grams of tyrosine to me seems like a very, very high dose
and I frankly can't in good conscience recommend that.
Why? Well, maybe I'm just hypersensitive to L-tyrosine,
but I've taken 1,000 or 1,500 milligrams of L-tyrosine and but I've taken 1000 or 1500 milligrams
of L-tyrosine and I've definitely experienced
an increase in alertness from taking 1.5 grams,
not 15, 1.5 grams of L-tyrosine.
And in fact, at a subjective level,
I can feel a meaningful increase in alertness
and focus from 500 milligrams of L-tyrosine.
So I can't in good conscience suggest
that people replicate the exact dose protocols within the study study. Nonetheless, the study as the title suggests shows that
supplementing with L-tyrosine can indeed increase working memory capacity, especially in a multitasking
environment, which in many ways carries over to the sorts of requirements for working memory and
attention capacity to get through life in a very focused, for lack of a better word, way in a very regimented,
do this, do that, task switch, multiple things, interleaf.
That's what moving through one's day, or at least workday, or anything that requires cognition
and focus entails.
So first of all, I'll just say what I always say when discussing any kind of compound or
prescription drug, never add or remove any supplement from your supplement regimen, if
you have one,
without consulting with your health provider first
to make sure that you are safe
to take that particular supplement.
Now, many physicians, MDs,
are not familiar with most supplements,
so you'll probably need to bring some literature
to the phone call or to the visit.
But of course, there are many healthcare providers,
including some MDs that are open to supplementation,
especially these days, as supplements have become,
I would say generally more accepted.
I mean, there are certain ones like vitamin D3
that and fish oils and things like that
that are more common than L-tyrosine.
But there are many physicians who are open to discussions
about supplements such as L-tyrosine.
If you know that you can supplement with L-tyrosine safely
and you opt to do so,
what dosages would you potentially take?
Well, here we have to look at the dosages used
in these studies.
I think it's only fair, it's only safe
that we acknowledge that these dosages are really, really high.
And I think the logical, the safe thing to do
would be to start with the minimal effective dose.
So if you weigh 50 kilograms,
rather than start right off with, you know, the equivalent dosage
to this study, maybe you start with 250 milligrams of L-tyrosine. If you weigh a bit more like me,
you're 100 kilograms or 75 kilograms, maybe you take 500 milligrams of L-tyrosine and see whether
or not you experience a significant effect on working memory, attention, and performance.
So the idea here is to establish the minimal effective dose.
I should also point out that some people, not all,
but some people experience a bit of a crash
after L-tyrosine supplementation,
such that they feel more alert, more focused,
better ability to perform working memory tasks,
move about their day,
but then three or four hours later,
experience kind of a drop.
So you need to be mindful of that.
In fact, you need to be mindful of any kind of pharmacology
where you're increasing dopamine.
This is one of the reasons why I like the behavioral protocols
that we talked about earlier,
because they're known to create big, but long lasting,
and slowly tapering off increases
in dopamine and other catecholamines.
Now, for those who are curious about,
and perhaps even wanna try mucuna purines,
please absolutely talk to your doctor first.
Mucuna purines is essentially the equivalent of L-dopa.
L-dopa is a prescription drug, as I mentioned before,
and macune appearance potently increases dopamine.
What dosages of macune appearance can increase dopamine?
Well, typically in studies of Parkinson's patients,
but also studies exploring typical people
who don't have Parkinson's in cognitive tasks
or in sports performance,
have explored anywhere
from one to five grams of mucuna prurines.
Mucuna prurines, again, is a very potent way
to increase dopamine.
And here, if your healthcare provider approves it
and you decide to try it,
I would suggest starting with a very, very low dose.
Again, to find the minimal effective dose.
So maybe even just 500 milligrams,
not even going to the one gram dose,
maybe even 250 milligrams and really evaluating
how much Macune Appearance can produce a meaningful impact
on working memory and attention for you.
So Macune Appearance is kind of a bridge
between over-the-counter supplements and prescription drugs.
I say it's a bridge because it is oh so similar
to that prescription drug, Aldopa.
And of course there is a long list of prescription drugs
that are known to be dopamine agonists,
several of which, many of which in fact,
have been shown to improve working memory.
You already learned about one of those before,
which is bromocryptine.
Now you need a prescription from a physician
to get bromocryptine, but bromocryptine we know,
based on that work from Desposito and colleagues
that I talked about earlier,
increases dopamine.
It does so in about 90 minutes.
It achieves peak levels of dopamine about 90 minutes
and improves working memory in individuals
that start off with a low working memory span
and we know from neuroimaging,
those are the individuals with lower baseline
levels of dopamine.
So should you run out and ask your doctor for Bromocryptin?
Maybe, most doctors won't prescribe Bromocryptin
for that reason.
I should mention that work from Desposito Lab
and other laboratories has shown that
one of the hallmark features of traumatic brain injury,
especially frontal lobe injury,
as well as certain neurodegenerative conditions
like Parkinson's, but other forms of dementia,
as well as ADHD involve deficits in working memory
and attention,
which makes sense, given what we know
about the symptoms of those conditions.
And that bromocryptine has been prescribed off-label
for the treatment of those conditions
to some degree of success.
However, those are off-label circumstances.
Right now, as far as I know,
bromocryptine is not prescribed specifically
for those conditions at a kind of whole population level.
It's not one of the drugs on the lookup table for ADHD
or TBI, but certain well-informed neurologists
and physicians do prescribe it for that reason.
There are other dopamine agonists
that are relevant in this context.
The ones that I think most of you will be familiar with
are the drugs that increase dopamine and norepinephrine
for the treatment of ADHD.
And I did an entire episode of the Huberman Lab podcast
about those compounds, things like Adderall,
things like Ritalin, which by the way,
is quite different than Adderall
in terms of how much dopamine relative to norepinephrine
it causes the increase of.
I cover all that in those episodes.
And you can simply go to HubermanLab.com,
put ADHD, Adderall or ADHD, Ritalin. And I talk about other things as well. I also talk a little bit about Modafinil,
which is a entirely different category of drug known to improve cognitive performance,
in some cases an ADHD, but in everybody. So there are a lot of different drugs that can
improve working memory. Most of those do so by increasing transmission of dopamine or
availability of dopamine, somehow changing dopamine levels in do so by increasing transmission of dopamine or availability of dopamine,
somehow changing dopamine levels in the brain
by increasing them.
So if you're somebody that has challenges
with working memory, focus and attention,
please see those episodes and please talk to your doctor
about potentially using pharmacology
to increase dopamine.
However, and this is very important,
many people who have challenges
with focus, attention and working memory and fall under the category of subclinical levels of ADHD.
And even some individuals with ADHD, young and old, manage their symptoms and in some cases,
improve their focus through the use of behavioral tools, nutritional tools, supplement-based tools
in ways that either allow them to reduce their total prescription drug dosages
and in some cases come off them entirely.
Now, I am definitely not saying that people should come off those drugs entirely.
And in fact, I want to take a really firm stand here because I know this is a bit controversial,
but I'm just going to tell you having evaluated the whole literature several times over now,
I do think, I personally believe that there is a strong
case for certain children and adults to take these compounds that increase dopamine and epinephrine.
Yes, those compounds are different forms of amphetamine, but those compounds we know can
increase neuroplasticity, the rewiring, LTP, LTD, et cetera, within the neural circuits that control focus, attention, and working memory.
And so they do have their place for certain individuals.
We don't want to rule those out.
Are they over-prescribed?
My feeling is that, yes, they are probably over-prescribed.
However, there are a number of individuals
that strongly benefit from them as well.
So if you are going to explore the use of those compounds
for sake of improving working memory, certainly if you're going to explore the use of those compounds for sake of improving working memory,
certainly if you're going to explore them
for sake of improving working memory
and focusing young kids,
please, please, please talk to your physician.
Because they're prescription drugs,
you would need to talk to a physician anyway.
But regardless of whether or not you're trying to improve
focus and working memory in a child, in an adult,
someone with TBI, someone with Parkinson's,
I think it stands to reason that you would arrive to that conversation
with some knowledge of not just the prescription drugs
that are potentially available,
but also some of the supplement-based tools,
some of the behavioral tools,
because as we know, and as a good friend of mine
who's an excellent physician says,
better living through chemistry
still requires better living.
Meaning, yes, prescription drugs can have a positive impact on these aspects of brain function in a way that can really improve lives, but that behavioral
tools also work. In fact, they can collaborate in a very synergistic way to increase the amount of
neuroplasticity in the relevant circuits. So I'm of the mind and I think more and more people out
there, I like to think are of the mind that behaviors, nutrition, supplement-based tools,
and prescription drugs all can have their place to varying degrees depending on the
circumstances and the individual. Okay, so today we talked about working memory,
this incredible capacity of our brain. In fact, a specific set of brain circuits designed for us to
absorb information that is perceived in our environment, use the relevant parts, and then chuck it.
Just get rid of it, forget it.
So very different than short and long-term memory,
which we also discussed,
and we talked about a few of the mechanisms as well.
I think you'll agree that working memory
is one of the more incredible aspects to brain function.
I mean, if you think about it,
this is a set of neural circuits
that engage the same algorithm over and over
in different contexts in order for us
to be able to navigate new environments,
familiar environments, to interleave different activities,
different strategies, to task switch,
to rule out distractors.
It's oh so critical to every aspect of our waking life.
And fortunately, there are also zero cost
and low cost behavioral supplement based
and prescription drug approaches to improving this incredible thing we call working memory.
So it was a pleasure to share some of those with you today as well.
If you're learning from and or enjoying this podcast,
please subscribe to our YouTube channel. That's a terrific zero cost way to support us.
In addition, please subscribe to the podcast on both Spotify and Apple.
Please also check out the sponsors mentioned
at the beginning and throughout today's episode.
That's the best way to support this podcast.
If you have questions for me or comments about the podcast
or guests or topics that you'd like me to host
on the Huberman Lab podcast,
please put those in the comments section on YouTube.
I do read all the comments.
During today's episode and on many previous episodes
of the Huberman Lab podcast, we discuss supplements.
While supplements aren't necessary for everybody, many people derive tremendous benefit from
them for things like improving sleep, for hormone support, and for focus.
To learn more about the supplements discussed on the Huberman Lab podcast, you can go to
Live Momentus, spelled O-U-S, so that's livemomentus.com slash Huberman.
If you're not already following me on social media, I am Huberman Lab on all social media
platforms.
So that's Instagram, Twitter, now called X, Threads, Facebook, and LinkedIn.
And on all of those platforms, I discuss science and science-related tools, some of which overlaps
with the content on the Huberman Lab podcast, but much of which is distinct from the content
on the Huberman Lab podcast.
Again, that's Huberman Lab on all social media platforms.
If you haven't already subscribed to our monthly neural network newsletter, the neural network
newsletter is a completely zero-cost newsletter that provides podcast summaries and protocols
in the form of brief PDFs of one to three pages in which I detail things like how to
do deliberate cold exposure and some of the science behind it, how to regulate your dopamine
levels, how to improve your sleep, and on and on. Everything from focus, neuroplasticity, and all of which is available, again, completely zero cost by going to hubermanlab.com,
go to the menu tab, scroll down a newsletter, and simply sign up by providing your email. And I
want to emphasize that we do not share your email with anybody. Thank you once again for joining
me for today's discussion about working memory and ways to improve your working memory. And last,
but certainly not least,
thank you for your interest in science.