Huberman Lab - Understanding and Using Dreams to Learn and to Forget
Episode Date: February 1, 2021This episode is all about the two major kinds of dreams and the sorts of learning and unlearning they are used for. I discuss REM-associated dreams that control emotional learning and their similarity... to various trauma treatments such as ketamine and EMDR. I also discuss Non-REM dreams and their role in motor learning and learning of detailed, non-emotionally-laden information. I relate this to science-backed tools for accessing more of the types of sleep and learning people may want. Other topics are listed in the time stamps below. For the full show notes, visit hubermanlab.com. Thank you to our sponsors AG1 (Athletic Greens): https://athleticgreens.com/huberman LMNT: https://drinklmnt.com/huberman Supplements from Momentous https://www.livemomentous.com/huberman Timestamps (00:00:00) Introduction (00:00:30) Sponsors: AG1, LMNT (00:03:00) The Dream Mask (00:06:00) Cycling Sleep (00:08:10) Chemical Cocktails of Sleep (00:13:00) Motor Learning (00:16:30) High Performance with Less Sleep (00:17:45) Rapid Eye Movement Sleep (00:20:30) Paralysis & Hallucinations (00:23:35) Nightmares (00:24:45) When REM & Waking Collide (00:25:00) Sleeping While Awake (00:26:45) Alien Abductions (00:29:00) Irritability (00:30:00) Sleep to Delete (00:32:25) Creating Meaning (00:34:10) Adults Acting Like Children (00:36:20) Trauma & REM (00:37:15) EMDR (00:39:10) Demo (00:44:25) Ketamine / PCP (00:45:45) Soup, Explosions, & NMDA (00:48:55) Self Therapy (00:50:30) Note About Hormones (00:51:40) Measuring REM / SWS (00:53:15) Sleep Consistency (00:56:00) Bed Wetting (00:58:00) Serotonin (00:59:00) Increasing SWS (00:59:50) Lucidity (01:02:15) Booze / Weed (01:03:50) Scripting Dreams (01:04:35) Theory of Mind (01:07:55) Synthesis (01:10:00) Intermittent Sleep Deprivation (01:11:10) Snoring Disclaimer (01:11:40) New Topic (01:15:50) Corrections (01:17:25) Closing Remarks As always, thank you for your interest in science! Title Card Photo Credit: Mike Blabac Disclaimer
Transcript
Discussion (0)
Welcome to the Huberman Lab podcast where we discuss science and science-based tools for everyday life.
I'm Andrew Huberman and I'm a professor of neurobiology and ophthalmology at Stanford School of Medicine.
This podcast is separate from my teaching and research roles at Stanford.
It is, however, part of my desire to bring you zero cost to consumer information about science and science-related tools.
In keeping with that theme, I'd like to thank the sponsors of today's podcast.
Our first sponsor is Athletic Greens.
Athletic Greens is an all-in-one vitamin mineral probiotic drink.
I've been taking Athletic Greens since 2012, so I'm delighted that they're sponsoring
the podcast.
The reason I started taking Athletic Greens and the reason I still take athletic greens
once or twice a day is that it helps me cover all of my basic nutritional needs.
It makes up for any deficiencies that I might have.
In addition, it has probiotics, which are vital for microbiome health.
I've done a couple of episodes now on the so-called gut microbiome and the ways in which
the microbiome interacts with your immune system, with your brain to regulate mood,
and essentially with every biological system relevant to health throughout your brain and body.
With athletic greens, I get the vitamins I need, the minerals I need,
and the probiotics to support my microbiome.
If you'd like to try athletic greens, you can go to athleticgreens.com slash Huberman and claim a special offer.
They'll give you five free travel packs plus a year supply of vitamin D3K2.
There are a ton of data now showing that vitamin D3
is essential for various aspects of our brain and body health,
even if we're getting a lot of sunshine.
Many of us are still deficient in vitamin D3.
And K2 is also important because it regulates things
like cardiovascular function, calcium in the body, and so on.
Again, go to athleticgreens.com slashubramin
to claim the special offer of the 5 free travel packs
and the year supply of vitamin D3 K2.
Today's episode is also brought to us by Element.
Element is an electrolyte drink that has everything you need
and nothing you don't.
That means the exact ratios of electrolytes are an element
and those are sodium, magnesium, and potassium,
but it has no sugar. I've talked
many times before on this podcast about the key role of hydration and electrolytes for nerve
cell function, neuron function, as well as the function of all the cells and all the tissues
in organ systems of the body. If we have sodium, magnesium, and potassium present in the proper
ratios, all of those cells function properly and all our bodily systems can be optimized.
If the electrolytes are not present
and if hydration is low, we simply can't think
as well as we would otherwise.
Our mood is off, hormone systems go off,
our ability to get into physical action,
to engage in endurance and strength,
and all sorts of other things is diminished.
So with element, you can make sure that you're staying
on top of your hydration, and that you're getting the proper ratios of electrolytes. If you'd. So with element, you can make sure that you're staying on top of your hydration and that
you're getting the proper ratios of electrolytes.
If you'd like to try element, you can go to drinkelement.
That's L-M-N-T dot com slash huberman, and you'll get a free element sample pack with your
purchase.
They're all delicious.
So again, if you want to try element, you can go to element element dot com slash huberman.
Today we're going to talk about dreaming, learning during
dreaming, and unlearning during dreaming, in particular, unlearning of troubling
emotional events. Now my interest in dreaming goes way back. When I was a child, I
had a friend and he came over one day and he brought with him a mask that had a
little red light in the corner. He had purchased this thing through some magazine ad that he had seen. And this mask was supposed
to trigger lucid dreaming. Lucid dreaming is the experience of dreaming during sleep,
but being aware that one is dreaming. And in some cases being able to direct one's dream
activities.
So if you're in a lucid dream and you want to fly, for instance, some people report being
able to initiate that experience of flying or to contort themselves into an animal or
to transport themselves to wherever they want within the dream.
I tried this device.
The way it worked is you put on the mask during a waking state, wide
awake, and you look at the little light flashing in the corner, and then you'd also wear it
when you went to sleep at night. And indeed, while I was asleep, I could see the red light
presumably through my eyelids, although I know I had opened my eyes, I don't know, I was
asleep. And then, because I was dreaming and I was experiencing something very vivid, I was
able to recognize that I was dreaming and then start to direct some of the events within
that dream. Now, lucid dreaming occurs in about 20% of people. And in a small percentage
of those people, they lucid dream almost every night, so much so that many of them report
their sleep not being as restorative as it would be otherwise. Now all of this is to say that lucid dreaming and dreaming are profound experiences.
We tend to feel extremely attached to our dream experience.
This may explain the phenomenon of people who have a very intense dream.
They need to somehow tell everybody about that dream or tell someone about that dream.
I don't really know what that behavior is about, but sometimes we wake up and we feel so attached to what happened in this state that we call dreaming that there seems to be an intense need to share it with other people, presumably to process it and make sense of it.
Now numerous people throughout history have tried to make sense of dreams in some sort of organized way, the most famous of which of course is a Sigmund Freud who talked about symbolic representations and dreams.
A lot of that has been kind of debunked, although I think that there's some interest in what the symbols of dreaming are.
And this is something that we'll talk about in more depth today, although not Freudian theory in particular. So I think in order to really think about dreams and what to do with them and how to
maximize the dream experience for sake of learning and unlearning, the best way to address this is to
look at the physiology of sleep. To really just what do we know concretely about sleep? So first of all,
as we get sleepy, we tend to shut our eyes, and that's because there are some
autonomic centers in the brain, some neurons that control closing of the eyelids when we get sleepy.
And then we transition into sleep. And sleep, regardless of how long we sleep, is generally broken
up into a series of 90-minute cycles, these ultradian cycles. So early in the night,
trading cycles. So early in the night, these 90-minute cycles tend to be comprised more of shallow sleep and slow wave sleep. So stage one, stage two, etc. and what we call slow wave sleep. I'll go into
detail about what all this means in a moment. And we tend to have less so-called REM sleep, REM
sleep, which stands for rapid eye movement sleep. And I'll talk about rapid eye movement sleep in detail. So early in the night, a lot more slow wave sleep and less REM.
For every 90 minute cycle that we have during a night of sleep,
we tend to start having more and more REM sleep.
So more of that 90 minute cycle is comprised of REM sleep
and less of slow wave sleep.
Now this is true, regardless of whether or not you wake up the mill the night to use the
restroom or your sleep is broken.
The more sleep you're getting across the night, the more REM sleep you're going to have.
And REM sleep and non-REM, as I'll refer to it, have distinctly different roles in learning
and unlearning and they are responsible for learning and unlearning of distinctly different types of information.
And this has enormous implications
for learning of motor skills,
for unlearning of traumatic events
or for processing emotionally challenging,
as well as emotionally pleasing events.
And as we'll see, one can actually leverage
their daytime activities in order to access more slow wave sleep or non-REM sleep, as we'll see, one can actually leverage their daytime activities in order to access
more slow wave sleep or non-REM sleep, as we'll call it, or more REM sleep, depending on
your particular emotional and physical needs.
So, it's really a remarkable stage of life that we have a lot more control and power over
than you might believe.
We'll also talk about lucid dreaming.
We're also going to talk about hallucinations and how drug-induced hallucinations have
a surprising similarity to a lot of dream states and yet some really important differences.
Okay, so let's start by talking about slow wave sleep or non-REM sleep.
Now, I realize that slow wave sleep and non-REM sleep aren't exactly the same thing.
So for you sleep of fissionados out there, I am lumping right now.
As we say in science, there are lumpers
and there are splitters.
And I am both.
Sometimes I lump, sometimes I split.
For sake of clarity and ease of conversation right now,
I'm gonna be a lumper.
So when I say slow wave sleep,
I mean non-REM sleep generally.
Although I acknowledge there is a distinction.
Slow wave sleep.
So slow wave sleep is characterized
by a particular pattern of brain activity
in which the brain is metabolically active
but that there's these big sweeping waves of activity
that include a lot of the brain.
If you want to look this up there,
you can find evidence for sweeping of waves
of neural activity across association
cortex, across big swaths of the brainstem, the so-called pons' chaniculate occipital pathway.
This is brainstem, phalamus, and then cortex for those of you that are interested, although
more of that is going to occur in REM sleep.
Now the interesting thing about slow wave sleep
are the neuromodulators that tend to be associated
with it that are most active and least active
during slow wave sleep, and here's why.
To remind you, neuromodulators are these chemicals
that act rather slowly, but their main role
is to bias particular brain circuits to be active and other brain circuits to not be active.
These are like the music playlist. So think of neuromodulators and these come
in the names of acetocholine, norepinephrine, serotonin, and dopamine.
Think of them as suggesting playlists on your audio device.
So classical music is distinctly different in feel and tone
and a number of other features from like third wave punk
or from hip hop.
So think of them as biasing toward particular genres
of neural circuit activity.
Melo music versus really aggressive fast music
or rhythmic music that includes lyrics versus rhythmic music that doesn't include lyrics.
That's more or less the way to think about these neuromodulators. And they are associated as a consequence with certain brain functions.
So we know for instance, and just to review aceto-coline in waking states, is a neuromodulator that tends to amplify the activity of brain circuits associated with focus and attention.
Norapinephrine is a neuromodulator that tends to amplify the brain circuits associated with alertness and the desire to move.
Serotonin is the neuromodulator that's released and tends to amplify the circuits in the brain and body that are associated with bliss and the desire to remain still. Dopamine is the neuromodulator that's released and is associated with amplification of the
neural circuits in the brain and body associated with pursuing goals and pleasure and reward.
Okay?
So, in slow wave sleep, something really interesting happens.
There's essentially no aceto-calling.
Aceto-calling production and release and action from the two major sites, which are in the
brain stem, which from a nucleus, the parabuygemonal nucleus, if you really want to know, or from the
four brain, which is nucleus basalus, and you don't need to know these names, but if you
like, that's why I put them out there.
Aceto-coling production plummet.
It's just almost to zero.
And acetylcholine, as I just mentioned, is associated with focus.
So you can think of slow wave sleep as these big sweeping waves of activity through the
brain and a kind of distortion of space and time so that we're not really focusing on
any one thing.
Now the other molecules that are very active at that time are norapinephrine, which is
a little bit surprising because normally in waking states, norapinephrine is going to be
associated with a lot of alertness and the desire to move.
But there's not a ton of norapinephrine around in slow-wave sleep, but it is around.
So there's something associated with the movement circuitry going on in slow-wave sleep.
And remember, this is happening mostly at the beginning of the night. Your sleep is on in slow-wave sleep. And remember, this is happening mostly at the beginning of the night.
Your sleep is dominated by slow-wave sleep.
So no aceto-calling, very little noripinepinepinel, though there is some, and a lot of serotonin.
And serotonin, again, is associated with this desire, the sensation of kind of bliss or
well-being, but not a lot of movement.
And during sleep, you tend not to move.
Now, in slow-wave sleep, you can move. You're not paralyzed, so you can roll over. If people are going
to sleepwalk, typically it's going to be during slow wave sleep. And what studies have shown
through some kind of sadistic experiments where people are deprived specifically of slow wave
sleep, and that can be done by waking them up as soon as
the electrode recording show that they're in slow wave sleep, or by chemically altering their sleep
so that it biases them away from slow wave sleep. What studies have shown is that motor learning
is generally occurring in slow wave sleep. So let's say the day before you go to sleep,
you were learning some new dance
move, or you were learning some specific motor skill, either a fine motor skill, or a course motor
skill. So let's say it's a new form of exercise or some new coordinated movements. This could be
coordinate movement at the level of the fingers, or it could be coordinate movement at the level
of the whole body and large limb movements. It could involve other people, or it could be coordinate movement that level the whole body and large limb movements. It could involve other people or it could be a solo activity.
Learning of those skills is happening primarily during slow wave sleep in the early part
of the night.
In addition, slow wave sleep has been shown to be important for the learning of detailed
information.
Now, this isn't always cognitive information.
We're going to talk about
cognitive information, but the studies that have been done along these lines involve having people
learn very detailed information about very specific rules and the way that certain
words are spelled. They tend to be challenging words. So if people are tested in terms of their
performance on these types of exams and they're deprived
of slow wave sleep.
They tend to perform very poorly.
So we can think of slow wave sleep as important for motor learning, motor skill learning, and
for the learning of specific details about specific events.
This turns out to be fundamentally important because now we know that slow wave sleep
is primarily in the early part of the night
and motor learning is occurring primarily early in the night.
And detail learning is occurring early in the night.
Now for those of you that are waking up
after only three, four hours of sleep,
this might be informative.
This might tell you a little something
about what you are able to learn and not able to learn
if that were to be the only sleep that you get.
Although hopefully that's not the only sleep that you get.
But we're going to dive deep into how it is
that one can maximize motor learning
in order to extract, say, more detail information
about coordinated movements
and how to make them faster or slower.
So that might be important for certain
sports. That might be almost certainly important for certain sports. It's going to be important for
any kind of coordinated movement like say learning to play the piano or for instance how to
learn synchronized movements with somebody else. So maybe I mentioned the example of dance earlier. If you like me a few years ago, I set out to learn tango because I have some Argentine
relatives and I was abysmal. I need to return to that at some point. I was just
abysmal. And one of the worst things about being abysmal learning dance is that
somebody else has to suffer the consequences also. So I don't know, maybe in the month on Neuroplasticity,
I'll explore that again as a self-experimentation.
But the key things to know are slow wave sleeps
involved in motor learning and detailed learning.
There's no acetacoling around at that time.
Has these big amplitude activity sweeping throughout the brain
and that there's the release of these neuromodular,
there's no epinephrine and serotonin. And again, that's all happening early in the night.
So athletes, people that are concerned about performance, if you happen to wake up
after just a couple hours of, you know, three, four hours of sleep because you're
excited about a competition the next day, presumably if you've already trained
the skills that you need for the event,
you should be fine to engage in that particular activity.
Now it's always going to be better to get a full night sleep and a full night sleep for
you at six hours, then it's always going to be better to get more sleep than it is to
get less.
However, I think some people get a little bit overly concerned that if they didn't get
their full night sleep before some sort of physical event, that their performance is going
to plummet.
Presumably, if you've already learned what you need to do and it's stored in your neural
circuits, and you know how to make those coordinated movements, what the literature on slow wave
sleep suggests is that you would be replenished, that the motor learning and the recovery from
exercises is going to happen early in the night.
Okay?
So we'll just pause there and kind of shelve that for a moment and then we're going to
come back to it.
But I want to talk about REM sleep, a rapid eye movement sleep.
REM sleep and rapid eye movement sleep, as I mentioned before, occurs throughout the
night, but you're going to have more of it, a larger percentage of these 90 minute sleep
cycles is going to be comprised of REM sleep
as you get toward morning.
REM sleep is fascinating.
It was discovered in the 50s when sleep laboratory in Chicago, the researchers observed that people's
eyes were moving under their eyelids.
Now, something very important that we're going to address when we talk about trauma later
is that the eye movements are not just side to side, they're very erratic in all different directions.
One thing that I don't think anyone, I've never heard anyone really talk about publicly,
is why eye movements during sleep, right? Eyes are closed and sometimes people's eyelids
will be a little bit open and their eyes are darting around, especially in little kids.
I don't suggest you do this, I'm not even sure it's ethical, but it has been done where you pull back the eyelids
of a kid while they're sleeping and their eyes are darting all over the place.
I think people do this to their past out friends at parties and things like that.
So again, I don't suggest you do it, but I'm tell you it because it's been done before
and therefore you don't have to do it again.
But rapid eye movement sleep is fascinating and occurs because there are connections between
the brainstem, an area called the ponds, and areas of the thalamus and the top of the brain
stem that are involved in generating movements in different directions.
Sometimes called saccades, although sometimes during rapid eye movement sleep it's not just
rapid, it's kind of a jeerty side to side thing and then the eyeballs kind of roll.
It's really pretty creepy to look at if you see.
So what's happening there is the circuitry that is involved in conscious eye movements is
kind of going haywire, but it's not haywire.
It's these waves of activity from the brainstem up to the so-called phalmus, which is an
area that filter sensory information that up to the so-called phalmus, which is an area that filter sensory information that up to the cortex. And the cortex, of course, is involved in conscious perceptions.
So in rapid eye movement sleep, there are a couple things are happening besides rapid eye movements.
The main ones are that they're in contrast to slow wave sleep. In REM sleep, serotonin is essentially absent.
So this molecule, this neuromodular that tends to create the feeling of bliss and well-being
and just calm, placidity is absent. So that's interesting.
In addition to that, norapinephrine, This molecule that's involved in movement and alertness is absolutely absent.
It's probably one of the few times in our life that epinephrine is essentially at zero activity
within our system. And that has a number of very important implications for the sorts of dreaming
that occur during REM sleep and the sorts of learning that can occur
in REM sleep and unlearning.
First of all, in REM sleep, we are paralyzed.
We are experiencing what's called atonia,
which just means that we're completely laid out and paralyzed.
We also tend to experience whatever it is
that we're dreaming about as a kind of hallucination
or a hallucinatory activity.
Long ago I looked into hallucinations and dreaming.
I was just fascinated by this in high school.
And there's some great books on this if you're interested in exploring the relationship between
hallucinations and dreaming.
The most famous of which are from a guy, researcher at Harvard, Alan Hobbson.
I wrote a book called Dream Drug Store and talked all about the similarities between drugs
that induce hallucinations and dreaming in REM.
So you can explore that, if you like.
So in REM, our eyes are moving, but the rest of our body is paralyzed and we are hallucinating.
There's no epinephrine around.
Epinephrine doesn't just create a desire
to move an alertness. It is also the chemical signature of fear and anxiety. It's what's
released from our adrenal glands when we experience something that's fearful or alerting.
So if a car suddenly screeches in front of us or we get a troubling text message,
adrenaline is deployed into our system. Adrenaline is epinephrine, those are equivalent molecules.
And epinephrine isn't just released from our adrenals, it's also released within our brain.
So there's this weird stage of our life that happens more toward morning that we call REM sleep,
where we're hallucinating and having these outrageous
experiences in our mind, but the chemical that's associated
with fear and panic and anxiety is not available to us.
And that turns out to be very important.
And you can imagine why that's important.
It's important because it allows us to experience things,
both replay of things that did occur,
as well as elaborate contortions of things
that didn't occur, and it allows us to experience those
in the absence of fear and anxiety.
And that, it turns out, is very important
for adjusting our emotional relationship to
challenging things that happened to us while we were awake.
Those challenging things can sometimes be in the form of social anxiety or just having been
working very hard or concern about an upcoming event or sometimes people report, for instance, dreams where they find themselves late to an exam
or naked in public or in some sort of situation that would be very troubling to them.
And that almost certainly occurs during REM sleep.
So we have this incredible period of sleep in which our experience of emotionally laid-in events is dissociated.
It's chemically blocked from us having the actual emotion.
Probably immediately, some of you are thinking,
what about nightmares?
I have nightmares, and those carry a lot of emotion,
or sometimes I'll wake up in a panic.
Let's consider each of those two things separately,
because they are important in understanding REM sleep.
There's a good chance that nightmares are occurring during slow-wave sleep.
There are actually some drugs that I don't suggest people take.
In fact, so much so I'm not going to mention them, that give people very kind of scary
or eerie dreams and this kind of feeling that things are pursuing them or that they can't
move when they are being chased.
That's actually a common dream that I've had.
I guess it's more or less a nightmare.
The feeling that one is paralyzed and can't move and is being chased.
A lot of people have said, oh, that must be in REM sleep because you're paralyzed and so
you're dreaming about being paralyzed and you can't move.
I think that's probably false.
The research says that because Norepinephrine is absent during REM sleep, it's very unlikely
that you can have these intense, fearful memories.
So those are probably occurring in slow-wave sleep, although there might be instances where
people have nightmares in REM sleep.
The other thing is some people experience, certainly I've had this experience, waking up and feeling very stressed about whatever it was
that I happen to be thinking about or dreaming about
in the moments before.
And that's an interesting case of an invasion
of the dream state into the waking state.
And the moment you wake up, epinephrine is available.
So the research on this isn't fully crystallized, but most
of it points in the direction of the experience of waking up and feeling very panicked. Maybe,
I want to highlight may, but maybe that you were experiencing something that was troubling
in the daytime, you're repeating that experience in your sleep. Epinephrine is not available, and therefore the brain circuits associated with fear and
anxiety are shut off, and so you're able to process those events.
Then suddenly you wake up, and there's a surge of adrenaline of epinephrine that's now
coupled to that experience.
Nightmare is very likely in slow-wave sleep and that kind of panic on waking from something very likely to be an invasion of the thoughts and ideas, however distorted in REM sleep
invading the waking state.
In fact, that brings to mind something that I've mentioned once before, but I want to mention
again, this atonia, this paralysis that we experience during sleep, can invade the waking
state.
Many people report the experience of waking up and being paralyzed.
They're legitimately waking up.
It's not a dream.
Waking up and being paralyzed and it is terrifying.
I've had this happen before.
It is, I can tell you, terrifying to be wide awake and as far as I could tell, fully conscious,
but unable to move.
And then generally you can jolt yourself out of it in a few seconds, but it is quite
frightening.
Now, some people actually experience waking up, being fully paralyzed and hallucinating.
And there is a theory in the academic and scientific community, at least, that what people report as alien abductions
have a certain number of core characteristics that map quite closely, eerily, similarly,
to these experiences.
A lot of reports of alien abduction involve people being unable to move, seeing particular
faces, hallucinating, extensively feeling that their body is floating or they were transported.
This is very similar to the experience of invasion of atonia into the waking state, waking
up and still being paralyzed, as well as the hallucinations that are characteristic of
dreaming and REM sleep.
Now, I'm not saying that people's alien abductions were not legitimate alien abductions.
How could I?
I wasn't there?
And if I was there I wouldn't tell you because that would make me an alien and I wouldn't want you to know but
It is quite possible that people are experiencing these things and they are an invasion of the sleep state into the waking state
And they can last several minutes or longer and because in in dreams, space and time are distorted, our perception of these events could
be that they lasted many hours, and we can really feel as if they lasted many hours, when
in fact they took only moments.
And we're going to return to distortion of space and time in a little bit.
So to just recap where we've gone so far, slow wave sleep early in the night. It's been shown to be important for motor learning and for detail learning.
REM sleep has a certain dream component when which there's no epinephrine.
Therefore we can't experience anxiety.
We are paralyzed.
Those dreams tend to be really vivid and have a lot of detail to them.
And yet in REM sleep, what's very clear is that the sorts of learning that
happen in REM sleep are not motor events, it's more about unlearning of emotional events.
And now we know why, because the chemicals available for really feeling those emotions are not present.
Now that has very important implications. So let's address those implications from two sides.
First of all, we should ask what happens
if we don't get enough REM sleep?
And a scenario that happens a lot
where people don't get enough REM sleep is the following.
I'll just explain the one that I'm familiar with
because it happens to me a lot,
although I've figured out ways to adjust.
I go to sleep around 10, 30, 11 o'clock,
I fall asleep very easily,
and then I wake up around three or four a.m.
I now know to use a NSDR
a non-sleep deep rest protocol,
and that allows me to fall back asleep,
even though it's called non-sleep deep rest.
It's really allows me to relax my body and brain,
and I tend to fall back asleep and sleep till about 7am.
During which time I get a lot of REM sleep.
And I know this because I've measured it
and I know this because my dreams tend to be very intense
of the sort that we know is typical of REM sleep.
In this scenario, I've gotten my slow wave sleep early
in the night and I've got my REM sleep toward morning. However, there are times when I don't go back to sleep,
maybe I have a flight to catch, that's happened, sometimes I've got a lot of my
mind and I don't go back to sleep. I can tell you, and you've probably
experienced, that the lack of REM sleep tends to make people emotionally
irritable. It tends to make us feel as if the little things are the big things.
So, it's very clear from laboratory studies where people have been deprived selectively of
REM sleep that our emotionality tends to get a little bit unhinged and we tend to catastrophize
small things. We tend to feel like the world is really daunting, we're never going to move
forward in the ways that we want.
We can't unlearn the emotional components of whatever it is
that's been happening, even if it's not traumatic.
The other thing that happens in REM sleep
is a replay of certain types of spatial information
about where we were and why we were in those places.
And this maps to some beautiful data
and studies that were initiated by a guy named Matt Wilson
at MIT years ago showing that in rodents, and it turns out in other non-human primates and
in humans, there's a replay of spatial information during REM sleep that almost precisely maps
to the activity that we experience during the day as we move from one place to another.
So here's a common world scenario.
You go to a new place, you navigate through that city
or that environment.
This place doesn't have to be at the scale of a city.
It can be a new building, it could be a finding,
particular rooms, new social interaction.
You experience that, and if it's important enough,
that becomes solidified a few days later
and you won't forget it.
If it's unimportant, you'll probably forget it.
During REM sleep, there's a literal replay of the exact firing of the neurons that occurred
while you were navigating that same city you're building earlier.
So REM sleep seems to be involved in the generation of this detailed spatial information.
But what is it that's actually happening in REM sleep? So there's this
uncoupling of emotion, but most of all what's happening in REM sleep is that we're forming
a relationship with particular rules or algorithms. We're starting to figure out, based on all
the experience that we had during the day, whether or not it's important that we avoid certain
people or that we approach certain people. Whether or not it's important that we avoid certain people or that we approach certain people.
Whether or not it's important that when we enter a building that we go into the elevator
and turn left where the bathroom is, for instance, these general themes of things and locations
and how they fit together.
And that has a word.
It's called meaning.
During our day, we're experiencing all sorts of things. Meaning is how we each individually
piece together the relevance of one thing to the next, right? So if I suddenly told you that,
you know, this pen was downloading all the information to my brain that was important to deliver
this information, you'd probably think I was a pretty strange character because typically we don't
think of pens as downloading information into brains. But if I told you think I was a pretty strange character because typically we don't think of pens as downloading information into brains.
But if I told you that I was getting information from my computer that was allowing me to say things to you, you'd say, well that's perfectly reasonable.
And that's because we have a clear and agreed upon association with computers and information and memory.
And we don't have that same association with pens. You might say, well, duh, but something in our brain needs to solidify those
relationships and make sure that the certain relationships
don't exist.
And it appears that REM sleep is important for that
because when you deprive yourself or people of REM,
they start seeing odd associations.
They tend to lump or batch things.
I know this from my own experience
if I've ever been sleep deprived, which unfortunately happens too often because of terrible with
deadlines, pulling all night out, the word the starts to look like it's spelled incorrectly.
And the the is a very simple word to spell, but things start to look a little distorted.
And we know that if people are deprived of REM sleep
for very long periods of time, they start hallucinating.
They literally start seeing relationships
and movement of objects that isn't happening.
And so REM sleep is really where we establish
the emotional load, but where we also start discarding
of all the meanings that are irrelevant.
And if you think about emotionality,
a lot of over-emotionality or catastrophizing
is about seeing problems everywhere.
And you could imagine why that might occur
if you start linking the web of your experience
too extensively.
It's very important in order to have healthy,
emotional, and cognitive functioning
that we have fairly narrow channels
between individual things.
If we see something on the news that's very troubling,
well, then it makes sense to be very troubled.
But if we're troubled by everything,
and we start just saying,
everything is bothering me,
and I'm feeling highly irritable,
and everything is just distorting and troubling me.
Chances are, we are not actively removing the meaning,
the connectivity between life experiences, as well as we could,
and that almost always maps back to a deficit in REM sleep.
So REM sleep is powerful and has this amazing capacity to eliminate the meanings that don't
matter.
It's not that it exacerbates the meanings that do matter, but it eliminates the meanings
that don't matter.
That bears a striking resemblance to what happens early in development.
This isn't a discussion about early in development, but early in development, the reason a baby
can't generate coordinated movements, and the reason why children can get very emotional
about what seems like trivial events or what adults know to be trivial events like, ah, the ice cream shop is closed and they just kind of, and then the kid just dissolves into,
you know, a puddle of tears. And the parents say, okay, well, it'll be open again another time.
The children, one of the reasons that they can't generate coordinated movement or place that event
of the ice cream shop being closed into a larger context is because they have too much connectivity.
And much of the maturation of the brain and nervous system that brings us to the point
of being emotionally stable, reasonable, rational human beings is about elimination of connections
between things.
So REMSleep seems to be where we uncouple the potential for emotionality between various
experiences.
And that brings us to the absolutely fundamental relationship
and similarity of REM sleep to some of the clinical practices
that have been designed to eliminate emotionality
and help people move through trauma
and other troubling experiences.
Whether or not those troubling experiences
are a death in
the family or a close loved one, something terrible that happened to you or somebody else, or
you know, an entire childhood or some event that in your mind and body is felt as an experienced
as bad, terrible, or concerning. Many of you, perhaps, have heard of trauma treatments such as EMDR,
eye movement desensitization, reprocessing, or ketamine treatment for trauma, something that
recently became legal and is in fairly widespread clinical use. Interestingly enough, EMDR and ketamine
enough EMDR and ketamine at kind of a core level bear very similar features to REM sleep. So let's talk about EMDR first.
EMDR, I've movement desensitization reprocessing is something that was developed by a psychologist
Francine Shapiro.
She actually was in Palo Alto, and the story goes that she was walking, not so incidentally,
in the trees and forests behind Stanford, and she was recalling a troubling event in
her own mind.
So this would be from her own life.
And she realized that as she was walking, the emotional load of that experience was not
as intense or severe.
She extrapolated from that experience of walking and not feeling as stressed about the stressful event,
to a practice that she put into work
with her clients, with her patients,
and that now has become fairly widespread.
It's actually one of the few behavior treatments that are approved
by the American Psychological Association for the Treatment of Trauma. What she had her clients
and patients do was move their eyes from side to side while recounting some traumatic or troubling
event. Now this was of course in the clinic And I'm guessing that she removed the walking component and just took the eye movement component to the clinic
because while it would be nice to go on therapy sessions
with your therapist and take walks,
it has there are certain boundaries to that
such as confidentiality, you know,
if there are a lot of people around,
the person might not feel as open to discussing things
or weather barriers and things like, you know,
if it's raining or hailing outside, it gets tough to do. Why eye movements? Well, she never really
said why eye movements, but soon I'll tell you why the decision to select these
lateralized eye movements for the work in the clinic was the right one. So these
eye movements, they look silly. I'll do them because that's why I'm here.
They look silly, but they basically involve sitting
in a chair and moving one's eyes from side to side,
not while talking, but for me.
And then recounting the event.
So it's sometimes talking while moving the eyes,
but usually it was moving the eyes from side to side
for 30, 60 seconds, then describing this challenging procedure.
Now as a vision scientist who also works on stress, when I first heard this, I thought it
was crazy, frankly. People would ask me about EMDR and I just thought that's crazy. I
went and looked up some of the theories about why EMDR might work.
And there were a bunch of theories, oh, it mimics the eye movements during REM sleep, that
was one.
Turns out that's not true.
And I'll explain why.
The other one was, oh, it synchronizes the activity on the two sides of the brain.
Well, sort of.
I mean, when you look into both sides of the binocular visual field, you actuate the visual cortex. But this whole idea of synchrony between the two sides of
the brain is something that I think modern neuroscience is starting to, let's just
say gently or not so gently move away from this whole right brain, left brain business.
Turns out, however, that I move of the sort that I just did and
that Francine Shapiro took from this walk experience and brought to the her clients in the clinic
are the sorts of eye movements that you generate whenever you're moving through space when you
are self generating that movement. So not so much when you're driving a car, but certainly
if you are riding a bicycle or you are walking or you were running, you don't realize that you have these reflexive subconscious eye movements that
go from side to side, and they are associated with the motor system.
So when you move forward, your eyes go like this.
There have been a number of studies showing that these lateralized eye movements helped people
move through or dissociate the emotional experience of particular traumas with those experiences
such that they could recall those experiences after the treatment and not feel stressed about them
or they didn't report them as traumatic any longer. Now the success rate wasn't 100% but they were
statistically significant in a number of studies and yet there are still some critics of EMDR and
frankly for a long time I still thought,
well, I don't know, this just seems like kind of a hack.
It just seems like kind of a, this is something for which we don't know the mechanism and
we can't explain.
But in the last five years, there have been no fewer than five and there's a sixth on
the way.
High quality peer reviewed manuscripts published in journal of neuroscience,
neuron, cell press journal, excellent journal, nature, excellent journal.
These are very stringent journals and papers showing that lateralized eye movements of the
sort that I just did.
And if you're just listening to this, it's just moving the eyes from side to side with
eyes open, that those eye movements, but not vertical eye movements, suppress the activity of the amygdala, which is this brain region
that is involved in threat detection, stress, anxiety, and fear.
There are some forms of fear that are not amygdala dependent, but the amygdala, it's not a
fear center, but it is critical for the fear response and for
the experience of anxiety.
So that's interesting.
We've got a clinical tool now that indeed shows a lot of success in a good number of people
where eye movements from side to side are suppressing the amygdala and the general theme is to
use those eye movements to suppress the fear response,
and then to recount or repeat the experience and over time, on couple, the heavy emotional
load, the sadness, the depression, the anxiety, the fear from whatever it was that happened
that was traumatic.
This is important to understand, because you know, I'd love to be able to tell somebody
who had a traumatic
experience that they would forget that experience, but the truth is you never forget the traumatic
experience. What you do is you remove the emotional load. Eventually it really does lose its potency.
The emotional potency is alleviated. Now EMDR I should just mention tends to be most
successful for single event or very
specific kinds of trauma that happen over and over, as opposed to, say, an entire childhood
or an entire divorce.
They tend to be most effective for single event kinds of things, car crashes, et cetera,
where people can really recall the events in quite a lot of detail.
So it's not for everybody and it should be done,
if it's going to be done for trauma,
it should be done in a clinical setting
with somebody who's certified to do this.
But that bears a lot of resemblance to REM sleep, right?
This experience in our sleep where our eyes are moving,
excuse me, although in a different way,
but we don't have the chemical epinephrine in order
to generate the fear response, and yet we're remembering the event from the previous day or days.
Sometimes in REM sleep, we think about things that happen a long, long time ago.
So that's interesting.
And then now there's this new treatment, this chemical treatment with the drug ketamine,
which also bears a lot of resemblance to the sorts of things that happen in REM sleep.
ketamine is getting a lot of attention now and I think a lot of people just don't realize what ketamine is.
ketamine is a dissociative anesthetic. It is remarkably similar to the drug called PCP, which is certainly a hazardous drug for people to use.
Ketamine and PCP both function to disrupt the activity of a particular receptor in the
brain called the NMDA receptor, N-methodiaspartate receptor.
This is a receptor that's in the surface of neurons, or on the surface of neurons, for which most of the time it's not active. But when something very extreme happens,
and there's a lot of activity in the neural pathway that impinges on that receptor,
it opens, and it allows the entry of molecules, ions, that trigger a cellular process that we call long-term
potentialation. And long-term potentialation translates to a change in
connectivity so that later you don't need that intense event for the
neuron to become active again. Let me clarify a little bit of this. The NMDA
receptor is gated by intense experience. One way you could think about this is typically I walk in my home, I might make some food and
sit down at my kitchen table, and I don't think anything about explosions.
But were I to come home one night, sit down to a bowl of chicken soup, and there was a massive
explosion, the neurons that are associated with chicken soup in my kitchen table would
be active in a way that was different than they were previously.
And would be coupled to this experience of explosions such that the next time and perhaps every other time that I go to sit down at the kitchen table, no matter how rational I am about the origins of that explosion, maybe it was a gas truck that was down the road, and there's no reason to think it's there today, but I would have the same experience.
Those neurons would become active,
and I'd get an increase in heart rate,
I'd get an increase in sweating, et cetera.
Ketamine blocks this NMDA receptor
and prevents that crossover and the addition of meaning
to the kitchen table, kitchen soup,
excuse me, chicken soup, explosion experience.
So how is ketamine being used?
Ketamine is being used to prevent learning of emotions very soon after trauma.
So ketamine is being stocked in a number of different emergency rooms where if people
are brought in quickly and these are hard to describe even, but a horrible experience
of somebody seeing a loved one next to them
killed in a car accident and they were driving that car.
This isn't for everybody, certainly, and you need to talk to your physician, but ketamine
is being used.
So they might infuse somebody with ketamine so that their emotion is, it can still occur,
but that the plasticity, the change in the wiring of their brain, won't allow that intense emotion to be attached to the experience.
Now, immediately you can imagine the sort
of ethical implications of this, right?
Because certain emotions need to be coupled to experiences.
I'm not saying that people should be using ketamine
or shouldn't be using ketamine.
Certainly not recreationally, it's quite dangerous.
It can be lethal.
And like PCP, it can cause pretty dramatic changes in perception and behavior.
But in the clinical setting, the basis of ketamine assisted therapies are really to remove
emotion.
And I think the way I've been hearing about it talked in the general public is a lot
of people think it's a little bit more like the kind of psilocybin trials or the MDMA
trials where it's about becoming more emotional or getting in touch with a certain
experience.
Ketamine is about becoming dissociative or removed from the emotional component of experience.
So now we have ketamine which chemically blocks plasticity and prevents the connection between
an emotion and an experience.
That's a pharmacologic intervention.
We have EMDR, which is this eye movement thing
that is designed to suppress the amygdala
and is designed to remove emotionality
while somebody recounts an experience.
And we have REM sleep, where the chemical epinephrine
that allows for signaling of intense emotion
to end the experience of intense emotion in the brain and body is not allowed.
And so we're starting to see a organizational logic, which is that a certain component of our sleeping life is acting like therapy.
And that's really what REM sleep is about.
So we should really think about REM sleep and slow wave sleep as both critical slow wave sleep for motor learning and detailed learning REM sleep for attaching of emotions to particular experiences and then for
making sure that the emotions are not attached to the wrong experiences and for unlearning emotional responses if they're too intense or severe. And this all speaks to the great importance of mastering one sleep,
something that we talked about in episode two of the podcast and making sure that if life
has disruptive events, either due to travel or stress or changes in school or or food
schedule, something that we talked about in episodes three and four, that one can still grab a hold and manage one's sleep life.
Because fundamentally, the unlearning of emotions that are troubling to us is what allows
us to move forward in life.
And indeed, the REM deprivation studies show that people become hyper-emotional.
They start to catastrophize, and it's no surprise no surprise therefore that sleep disturbances correlate
with so many emotional and psychological disturbances.
It's just by now it should just be obvious why that will be the case.
In fact the other day I was in a discussion with a colleague of mine who's down in Australia,
Dr. Sarah McKay, I've known her for two decades now from the time she was
at Oxford, and Sarah studies among other things menopause in the brain. And she was saying
that a lot of the emotional effects of menopause actually are not directly related to the hormones.
There have been some really nice studies showing that the disruptions in temperature regulation and menopause map to changes in
sleep regulation that then impact emotionality and an inability to correctly adjust the circuits
related to emotionality.
And I encourage you to look at her work.
We'll probably have her as a guest on the podcast at some point in the future because she's
so knowledgeable about those sorts of issues as well as issues related to testosterone and people with all sorts
of different chromosomal backgrounds.
So, sleep deprivation isn't just deprivation of energy,
it's not just deprivation of immune function,
it is deprivation of self-induced therapy
every time we go to sleep.
Okay, so these things like EMDR and ketamine
therapies are inclinic therapies, but REM sleep is the one that you're giving
yourself every night when you go to sleep, which raises I think the other
important question, which is how to get and how to know if you're getting the
appropriate amount of REM sleep and slow wave sleep. So that's what we'll talk
about next. So how should one go about getting the appropriate amount of slow wave sleep and REM sleep and
knowing that you're getting the right amount?
Well, short of hooking yourself up to an EEG, it's going to be tough to get exact measurements
of brain states from night to night.
Some people nowadays are using things like the oral ring or a wound band or some other device to measure the quality and depth and duration of
their sleep. And for many people those those devices can be quite useful. Some
people are only gauging their sleep by way of whether or not they feel
rested, whether or not they feel like they're learning and they're getting
better or not. There are some things that one can really do.
And the first one is might surprise you in light of everything I've said.
And probably everything you've heard about sleep.
There was a study done by a Harvard undergraduate,
Emily Hogueland, who was in Robert Strick Gold's lab at the time.
And that study explored how variations in total sleep time related to learning as compared
to total sleep time itself.
And to summarize the study, what they found was that it was more important to have a regular amount of sleep each night as opposed to the total duration.
In other words, and what they showed was that improvements in learning or deficits in learning were more related to whether or not you got six hours, six hours, five hours, six hours, that was better than
if somebody got, for instance, six hours, 10 hours, seven hours, four or five hours.
You might say, well, that's crazy because I thought we were just also supposed to get
more sleep and there's more room towards morning.
It turns out that for sake of learning new information and performance on exams in particular, that's what was measured,
limiting the variation in the amount of your sleep is at least as important and perhaps more important
than just getting more sleep overall. And I think this will bring people great relief,
many people great relief, who are struggling to quote unquote get enough sleep.
Remember a few episodes ago I talked about the difference between fatigue and insomnia.
You know fatigue tends to be when we are tired. Insomnia tends to lead to a sleepiness during
the day when we're falling asleep and you don't want that. You don't want either of those things really. But I found it striking that the data from this study
really point to the fact that consistently getting
about the same amount of sleep is better
than just getting more sleep.
And I think nowadays so many people are just aiming
for more sleep and they're rather troubled about the fact
that they're only getting five hours
or they're only getting six hours in some cases.
It may be the case that they are sleep deprived and they need more sleep, but some people just
have a lower sleep need and I find great relief personally in the fact that consistently
getting for me about six hours or six and a half hours is going to be more beneficial than
constantly striving for eight or nine and finding that some nights
I'm getting five and sometimes I'm getting nine and
Varing around the mean as I recall and I think I'm gonna get this precisely right
But if not, I know that I'm at least close for every hour
variation in sleep regardless of whether I was more sleep than one typically got there was a
17% reduction in performance on this particular
exam type.
And so this is powerful.
This means that we should strive for a regular amount of sleep.
And for some of us, that means falling asleep and waking up and going back to sleep.
For some people, it means falling asleep and waking up and not getting back to sleep.
Now ideally, you're getting the full compliment of
slow-wave sleep early in night and sleep toward morning, which is REM sleep, which brings us to
how to get more REM sleep. Well, there are a couple different ways, but here's how to not get more
REM sleep. All right, first of all, drink a lot of fluid right before going to sleep. One of the
reasons why we wake up in the middle of the night
to use the bathroom is because when our bladder is full,
there is a neural connection, literally,
and a set of neurons in a nerve circuit
that goes to the brain stem that wakes us up.
And actually some people that I know
and I won't be mentioned,
actually use this to try and adjust for their jet lag
when they're trying to stay awake.
Having to use the bathroom, have any urinate
is one of the most anxiety of oaking experiences
anyone can have.
If you really have to go to the bathroom,
it's very hard to fall asleep or stay asleep.
And bed wetting, which happens in kids very early on,
is a failure of those circuits to mature until, you know, I think we all assume that babies
are going to pee in their sleep, but adults aren't supposed to do that.
And the circuits take some time to develop, and in some kids they develop a little bit
later than others.
So having a full bladder is one way to disrupt your sleep.
You don't want to go to bed dehydrated, but that's one way.
On the other hand, there is evidence that if you want to go to bed dehydrated, but that's one way. On the other
hand, there is evidence that if you want to remember your dreams more, or remember more
of your dreams, there is a tool that you can use. I don't necessarily recommend it, which
is to drink a bunch of water before you go to sleep. And then what happens is you tend
to break in and out of REM sleep. It tends to be fractured. And with a sleep journal,
then they've done
these laboratory studies, believe it or not, people will recall more of their dreams because
they're in this kind of semi-conscious state because they're constantly waking up throughout
the night. I suggest not having a full bladder before you go to sleep. That one's kind of
an obvious one, but nonetheless. The other one is if you recall that during REM sleep, we have a shift in neurotransmitters
such that we have less serotonin, right?
Just wanna make sure I got that right.
Excuse me, less serotonin.
There are a lot of supplements out there
geared toward improving sleep.
I've taken some of them and I've taken many of them
if not all of them at this point
so I could report back to you. And I think I mentioned on a previous episode that when I take
trip to fan or anything that contains 5HTP which is serotonin or precursor to serotonin
serotonin is made from trip to fan, I tend to fall very deeply asleep and then wake up a few hours
later. And that makes sense now, based on the fact that,
you just don't want a lot of REM sleep early on.
What was probably happening is that I was getting
a lot of REM sleep early on
because low levels of serotonin are typically associated
with slow wave sleep and that comes early in the night.
So for some people, those supplements might work,
but beware serotonin supplements
could disrupt the timing of REM sleep and slow wave sleep, and in my
case, it led to waking up very shortly after going to sleep and not being able to get back
to sleep.
Now, if you want to increase your slow wave sleep, that's interesting.
There are ways to do that.
One of the most powerful ways to increase slow wave sleep, the percentage of slow wave
sleep, apparently without any disruption to
the other components of sleep and learning, is to engage in resistance exercise.
It's pretty clear that resistance exercise triggers a number of metabolic and endocrine
pathways that lend themselves to release of growth hormone, which happens early in the
night.
And resistance exercise, therefore, can induce a greater percentage of slow wave sleep.
It doesn't have to be done very close to going to bedtime.
In fact, for some people that the exercise
could be disruptive for reasons I've
talked about in previous episodes.
But resistance exercise, unlike aerobic exercise,
does seem to increase the amount of slow wave sleep, which,
as we know, is involved in motor learning
and the acquisition of slow wave sleep, which, as we know, is involved in motor learning and the acquisition of fine detailed information, not general rules or the emotional components
of experiences. For those of you that are interested in lucid dreaming and would like to
increase the amount of lucid dreaming that you're experiencing, I haven't been able to track down
that device with the red light that I described at the beginning, but there are a number of just simple zero technology tools that one could use in principle.
One is to set a queue.
The way this works is you come up with a simple statement about something that you'd like
to see or experience later in dreams. You can, for instance, write down something like,
I want to remember the red apple.
I know it sounds silly and trivial.
And you look at that, you would probably want to write it down on a piece of paper.
You might even want to draw a red apple.
And then before you go to sleep, you would look at it.
And then you would just go to sleep.
There are some reports that doing that
for several days in a row can lead to a situation
in which you are suddenly in your dream
and you remember the red apple.
And that gives you a sort of tether to reality
between the dream state and reality
that allows you to navigate and shape
and kind of adjust your dreams.
Lucid dreaming does not have to be,
or include the ability to alter features of the dream,
you know, to be able to control things in the dream.
Sometimes it's just the awareness that you are dreaming,
but nonetheless, some people enjoy lucid dreaming
and then for people that have a lot of lucid dreams
that feel kind of overwhelmed by those,
that's going to involve trying to embrace protocols that can set the right duration
of sleep. There's a little bit of literature, not a lot, that shows that keeping the total amount
of sleep per night to say six hours such that you begin sleep and end at the beginning and end
of one of these ultra-adiene cycles can be better than waking the beginning and end of one of these ultra-adion cycles,
can be better than waking up in the middle
of one of these ultra-adion cycles.
So, try and find the right amount of sleep
that you need that's right for you,
and then try and get that consistently night tonight.
If you're a lucid dreamer and you don't like it,
then you may want to start to make sure
that you're waking up at the end of one of these ultra-adion
cycles. So, in this case, it would be better to wake up after six hours
than after seven.
And if you did sleep longer than six hours,
maybe you'd want to get to seven and a half hours
because that's going to reflect the end of one of these 90
minute cycles as opposed to waking up in the middle.
Alcohol and marijuana are well known to induce states
that are pseudo sleep like, especially
when people fall asleep after having consumed alcohol or THC, the active component, one
of the active components in marijuana.
Alcohol, THC, and most things like them, meaning things that increase serotonin or GABA are going to disrupt the pattern of sleep.
They're going to disrupt the depth, they're going to disrupt the overall sequencing of
more slow wave sleep early in the night and more REM sleep later in the night.
That's just the reality.
There are some things that, at least in a few studies that I could find, seem to suggest
that you could increase the amount of slow wave sleep using things
like arginine, the amino acid alharginine, although you really want to check arginine can
have effects on heart, et cetera, has other effects.
But alcohol, THC, not going to be great for sleep in depth of sleep.
You might feel like you can fall asleep faster, but the sleep that you're accessing really
isn't the kind of deep restorative sleep that you should be getting. Now of of course, if that's what you need in order to sleep and that's within your
protocols, I've said here before, I'm not suggesting people take anything, I'm not a
medical doctor, I'm not a cop, so I'm not trying to regulate anyone's behavior.
I'm just telling you what the literature says. Some of you may want to explore your
dreams and meaning of dreams, et cetera. There's not a lot of hard data about how to do this, but a lot of people report keeping
a sleep journal where a dream journal can be very useful.
So they mark when they think they fell asleep the night before, when they woke up.
And if they wake up in the middle of the night or early in the morning, they'll just write
down what they can recall of their dreams.
And even if they recall nothing,
many people have the experience of mid-morning
or later afternoon that suddenly comes to them
that they had a dream about something
and writing that down.
I kept a dream journal for a while.
It didn't really afford me much.
I didn't really learn anything except
of my dreams where very bizarre.
But there are some things that happen in dreams
that are associated with REM sleep as a compared to slow wave sleep, which can tell you whether or not your dream likely happened in REM sleep or slow wave sleep.
And the distinguishing feature it turns out is something called theory of mind.
Theory of mind is actually an idea that was developed for the study and assessment of autism. And it was
initially that phrase theory of mine was brought about by
Simon Baron Cohen, who is Sasha Baron Cohen, the comedian's brother. Simon Baron Cohen is a
psychologist and to some extent a neuroscientist at Oxford. And theory of mind tests are done on children. And the theory of mind test is
some, like the following. A child is brought into a laboratory and watches a video of a
child playing with some sort of toy. And then at the end of playing with that toy, they
put the toy in a drawer and they go away. And then another child comes in and is looking around and then the
experimenter asks the child who's in the experiment, the real child, and says,
you know, what does the child think? You know, what are they feeling? And most
children of a particular age, five or six or older will say, oh, you know, he or
she is confused. They don't know where the toy is. Or they'll say
something that implies what we call theory of mind, that they can put their ideas into and their
mind into what the other child is likely to be feeling or experiencing. That's theory of mind.
And it turns out that this is used as one of the assessments for autism because some children, not all, but some children
that have autism or that go on to develop autism don't have this theory of mind.
They tend to fixate on the fact that the first child put the toy in the drawer.
They'll say, it's in the drawer as opposed to answering the question, which is how does
the second child feel about it or what are they experiencing?
So theory of mind is something
that is emerges early in life as a part of the maturation of the circuits in the brain associated
with emotional learning and social interactions. And we experience this in certain dreams. So
if you had a dream that you're puzzled about or that you're fixated on and you're thinking about, you might ask, in that dream was I assessing somebody else's emotion and feeling or was I very much in my own first person experience.
And the tendency is that theory of mind tends to show up most in these REM associated dreams.
Now, this isn't a hard and fast rule, but chances are if you're in a dream and you
were thinking about other people who wanted to do something to you, you were thinking about their
desire to chase you or help you or something that was related to someone else's emotional experience,
it was probably a REM dream. That dream occurred in rapid eye movement sleep as opposed to slow wave sleep. And that makes sense when you think about the role of REM in emotional
unlearning of associations with particular life events that REM is rich with
all sorts of exploration of the emotional load of being chased or the emotional
load of having to take an exam the next day or being late for something.
But again, if you're fixated or you can recall thinking a lot about or feeling a lot about what somebody else's motivations were,
then chances are it was in REM and if not, chances are it was in slow wave sleep.
Today we've been in a deep dive of sleep and dreaming, learning and unlearning, and I
just want to recap a few of the highlights and important points.
A lot more slow wave sleep in less REM early in the night, more REM and less slow wave sleep
later in the night.
REM sleep is associated with intense experiences without this chemical epinephrine that allows us the anxiety or fear,
and almost certainly has an important role in uncoppling of emotion from experiences,
kind of self-induced therapy that we go into each night. That bears striking resemblance to things
like EMDR and ketamine therapies and so forth. Slow wave sleep is critical, however. It's critical mostly for motor
learning and the learning of specific details. So REM is kind of emotions and general themes
and meaning and slow wave sleep, motor learning and details. I personally find it fascinating
that consistency of sleep, meaning getting six hours every night is better than getting 10, 1 night, 8, the next,
5, the next, 4, the next. I find that fascinating and I think I also like it because it's something
I can control better than just trying to sleep more, which I think I'm not alone in agreeing that that's just
hard for a lot of people to do. This episode also brings us to the conclusion of a five episode streak where we've been focusing
on sleep and transitions in and out of sleep, non-sleep, depressed.
We've talked about a lot of tools, morning light, evening light, avoiding lights, blue
blockers, supplements, tools for measuring sleep duration and quality.
We've been covering a lot of themes.
I like to think that by now you're armed with
a number of tools and information, things like knowing when your temperature minimum is, knowing
when you might want to light or not, when you might want to eat or take hot showers or
God forbid a cold shower, something that most people, including me, more or less loath, but can have
certain benefits. And that will allow you to shape your sleep life and get this consistent or more or less consistent amount
of sleep on a regular basis.
Nobody's perfect.
In fact, I have this little joke that I sometimes tell
it's not funny.
Most of the jokes I tell them told they're not funny.
But there's so much excitement now about intermittent fasting.
Sometimes I think that someone should start
something on intermittent sleep deprivation,
although we're already doing that.
We are all experiencing lack of sleep from time to time.
And I don't think we should catastrophize that too much.
I think that what we want to do, rather than accumulate a sleep anxiety, is to, you know,
if we get a bad night's sleep, we want to adjust.
We want to get back on track and just get the consistent amount of sleep.
Use those non-sleep deep-rest protocols to help us relax when we're feeling anxious.
We're having trouble waking up in the middle of the night.
There are a lot of tools out there and most of them are zero cost.
I hope you'll find those beneficial.
If you've been hearing Costello snoring throughout this episode, I apologize in his behalf.
As I said in the welcome video to this podcast, he's an integral part of the podcast.
A few people have said, hey,
that noise in the background is really disruptive.
Hey, what can I say?
Costello is a 10-year-old bulldog mastiff.
The lifespan on those animals is about 10 years.
So I'm not trying to make you feel guilty,
but after he's gone, there won't be any snoring,
although I'll probably get a different dog.
So sort of what would the kids say?
Sorry, not sorry.
Sorry, not sorry about the snoring.
And I'm sorry if it's disruptive genuinely, but he's here for the hall.
So that's what that's about.
As we close out this segment on sleep, we are moving into a new theme and topic for the
next four to five episodes.
We are going to discuss the science and the tools related to neuroplasticity.
Neuroplasticity is a remarkable feature of the nervous system.
In fact, it's the defining feature of the nervous system, which is its ability to change
itself in response to experience.
That is, unlike every other tissue and collection of cells and organ in our body,
it's really what makes us, us as a species, and it's what makes us, us as individuals,
and it's really where our potential lies.
Everything that we know, everything we can do, and our true potential,
in terms of what we will ever be able to know, do, say, in life
is set by the limits of neuroplasticity.
So, we're going to explore learning and childhood, learning and adulthood.
We're going to discuss detailed protocols as they relate to sensory plasticity, learning
new sensory information versus motor plasticity or sensory motor integration.
We're going to talk about language acquisition. We're going to be talking about emotional acquisition
and breadth as well as I think a topic a lot of people are going to find fascinating is
the relationship between plasticity set during childhood attachment to parent or
other caregiver and how that maps onto adult relationships. You know, many of you have probably heard about
secure attach or insecure attach the AB and CD babies
as they're called from the classic studies
of bullby and others.
But now there's actual neuroscience that can say
which circuits were active during those early life attachment
and how those map to adult attachment styles challenges
and what makes us more likely to select certain partners
and styles of attachment, as well as how to change those.
It's really fascinating, and I think neuroscience
is time has come for neuroplasticity.
We're also gonna talk, of course, about supplements
and chemicals and machines and devices
that can assist in speeding up the plasticity process.
Or believe it or not, there are some cases where you might want to delay plasticity
in order to get more depth of learning and have that learning last longer,
something that is just absolutely spectacular literature.
So I'm very excited to move on to that topic soon.
I hope that the tools that you've acquired so far and the knowledge that you've acquired so far
is helping you with your self- self evaluation and experimentation as you see fit
and is allowing you to not just sleep better but feel better while you're awake.
And hopefully, as set the stage for you to learn better as we start to march into the month
on neural plasticity. Many of you have asked how you can help support the Huberman Lab podcast.
And we greatly appreciate the question. You can help support the Hubertman Lab podcast. And we greatly appreciate the question.
You can help support the podcast by subscribing to the YouTube channel, if you haven't already,
and leaving comments and questions in the comment section. If you could subscribe on Apple and
or Spotify, that's helpful. And there's a place on Apple podcasts to leave a rating as well as
comments about how you feel about the podcast. If you could suggest the podcast to leave a rating as well as comments about how you feel about the podcast.
If you could suggest the podcast to friends and coworkers and anyone else that you think
would benefit from the information, that also really helps us get the word out.
And of course, check out our sponsors because that's a very direct way to help us continue
to get this information out to the general public.
And as I mentioned at the beginning of today's episode, we are now partnered with momentous
supplements because they make single ingredient formulations that are of the absolute highest
quality and they ship international.
If you go to livemomentus.com slash huberman, you will find many of the supplements that
have been discussed on various episodes of the huberman lab podcast and you will find
various protocols related to those supplements.
Last but not least, a few people
wrote to me with some questions slash corrections about things that I said in previous podcasts.
So in keeping with my goal of making the information accurate and clear, I just want to correct
myself about a few things that I said. One of those, and I'm guessing it probably came from an endocrinologist
or somebody else that knows a lot about testicles, said, Huberman, you mentioned that testosterone
is made by the serotonly cells of the testes, and it's not, it's made by the lidig cells
of the testes, and indeed you are correct. And so I want to make sure that I clarify
that. Testosterone is made by the lidig cells of the testes,
not by the Certoli cells, the Certoli cells, make five alpha reductase and aromatease and some
other enzymes involved in conversion of testosterone into things like DHT and estrogen.
So thank you for that correction. I genuinely appreciate it. I misspoke.
The other thing I said was at one point I said typical temperature is 96.8 when I actually
meant to say 98.6.
So it was a dyslexic slip on my part and I apologize.
I don't know that I'm dyslexic.
I know I'm clinically diagnosed with dyslexia, but I swapped them, which sometimes happens
when I'm going fast.
So I apologize.
I'll use this as a moment to just say, temperature varies a lot across the day and night.
That was a theme of previous podcasts.
So we can't really talk about average temperature anyway,
but I do want to be clear that most people think
about average temperature as 98.6.
I misspoke my error and I apologize.
Thank you for joining me in this journey
of the nervous system and biology
and trying
to understand the mechanisms that make us who we are and how we function in sleep and
in wakefulness.
It's really an incredible landscape to consider and I hope that you're getting a lot out
of the information.
As always, thank you for your interest in science.
you