If Books Could Kill - Malcolm Gladwell's "Outliers"

Episode Date: November 10, 2022

In "Outliers," Malcolm Gladwell posited that it takes 10,000 hours of practice to become an expert in something. Mike and Peter prove him wrong by mastering his dumb book over the span of ab...out 50 minutes.Support us on PatreonLinks:Malcolm Gladwell talks about responding to critics, and the perils of ‘Talking to Strangers’Gladwell for Dummies Malcolm Gladwell interview The Role of Deliberate Practice in the Acquisition of Expert Performance The Dangers of Delegating Education To Journalists Good intuition takes years of practice Blinkered How Malcolm Gladwell Tricks You Into Believing Complexity and the Ten-Thousand-Hour Rule Trends in International Mathematics and Science Study

Transcript
Discussion (0)
Starting point is 00:00:00 Michael! Peter! Have you ever read a book by Malcolm Gladwell? Uh, I have read one of his essay collections, if that counts. It does count, because that is what his books are. I'm going into this with complicated feelings because I actually think that some of his early work is pretty good. I'm also very aware of the difference between the fact-checking standards at the New Yorker, which are legendarily high, and the fact-checking standards at major book publishers, which are non-existent. That's great analysis for someone who has never read a Malcolm Gladwell book. I'm trying to predict what you're getting me into with this.
Starting point is 00:00:54 Well, I went into this thinking Gladwell was just sort of a bog-standard pop science hack. I left it thinking that he is possibly an actual psychopath who doesn't believe in anything other than his own need for fame and approval. Now that you've spent 10,000 hours reading his work, you've got to be honest. So he's a journalist. He gets, he goes to the New Yorker in his early 30s. I think he's, it's 1996. And he's just chugging along at the New Yorker until the year 2000 when he publishes the tipping point. Right. So in the tipping point, he posits a theory of how ideas spread across society. It says that they spread similarly to epidemics.
Starting point is 00:01:35 His primary idea is something he calls the law of the few, which claims that a small number of people are responsible for the promulgation of ideas across society. Oh, this is his thing where he makes up a capital letter name. Yes. Like, the law of people you don't know in college or something and then he's like, this is what explains it. Yeah, it reminds you a lot of academic papers that I've read where they make up these acronyms to make very ordinary things seem kind of exotic.
Starting point is 00:02:02 Yeah. And what's interesting about Gladwell doing this is he's often repackaging another idea that already has a name. Like there's a very well-known principle in economics called the 80-20 rule. The idea that 80% of the work in a given situation is generally done by 20% of the people.
Starting point is 00:02:20 Gladwell mentions this in the tipping point and he sort of acts as if it supports the law of the few. When in reality, it is the law of the few just described a little more accurately. This is also the first time, the first indication we get that Gladwell is maybe a little bit obsessive about like the exceptional people that live among us. Oh, maybe the 20% are really good for the other 80% of us. So the tipping point blows up. People love it. It ultimately ends up spending eight years
Starting point is 00:02:55 on the New York Times bestseller's list and catapults glad well to, you know, he's chattering class royalty at this point. Yeah. In 2005, he drops Blink. The thesis of Blink is essentially that human intuition is such that our split second impressions and decisions are often more accurate than more informed,
Starting point is 00:03:16 thought out decisions. So the book starts off with an example of an ancient Greek statue that was brought to him using him in California and various experts had an initial impression that it was in a forgery. However, after a closer study for several months, the museum determined that it was genuine. Now it took a long time to prove it, but it turns out that the initial impression was probably right. It was likely a forgery.
Starting point is 00:03:40 Gladwell uses this and some other data and anecdotes to weave together this idea that first impressions contain these valuable bits of wisdom. And not just that, but that first impressions are in many cases more useful than conclusions drawn after careful thinking. I- it's so perfect. It's such the perfect idea for that era too, because I'm sure that if you go around the world and history looking for anecdotes that illustrate this principle, you'll find a lot of really good stories. Yeah. But it's like you can find a million good stories that illustrate that principle, but I'm sure if you looked for the exactly the opposite conclusion that we should never trust our first instincts, you would also find a bunch of really good stories. Yeah. So it's like, it's not clear that there's actually a universal rule for on either side. It's just a bunch of stories.
Starting point is 00:04:29 Exactly right. I mean, it's not that there's nothing to this. It's that Gladwell overstates this thesis like brutally. Yeah. So Blink hits the top of the best seller charts, but this time like the criticism from the scientific community is a little bit more aggressive. Yeah. The daily telegraph publishes review, calling the criticism from the scientific community is a little bit more aggressive. The daily telegraph publishes review, calling the evidence for the thesis flimsy.
Starting point is 00:04:54 The new republic publishes a scathing review by federal judge Richard Posner. Oh, behavioral economist. Daniel Coniman, I believe I'm pronouncing that incorrectly, but whatever, criticizes the book publicly for fostering the implication that human intuition is magic. And that last bit is important because I read this book when it first came out and I don't remember much about it, but what I remember is that although he's often drawing on like what feels like reputable science,
Starting point is 00:05:19 at least a bit, he's also sort of hinting at something that's almost paranormal. By the way, that statute story, like when you, when he asked the experts why their first impression was that it was a forgery, they were like, well, it's not dirty enough. Statues come out of the ground. So it wasn't dirty enough. And I was like, is that, that's the, that's the brilliant intuition you're making your book around? It's also, I mean, that's also such an interesting example too, because these are people who are experts. It also seems really obvious that like my first impression of a statue would be completely worthless.
Starting point is 00:05:53 Whereas the first impression of somebody who's been looking at Greek statues for 30 years, theirs might actually be better than my considered response, but that's as a result of all of the training. So it's like, well, yeah, it just, it depends. Like that's the only real rule you can pull out of it. Right, and he does, I mean, he does talk about that, right? Like it matters that they were experts,
Starting point is 00:06:11 but by the time, this is a glad well thing that I'll talk about a bit more, by the time he adds all of his qualifiers, you're sort of like, well, yeah, well, then it doesn't mean anything, right? Then this thesis doesn't matter. It's not interesting. And I, Blink is great because I just, I'm just imagining someone living by this thesis doesn't matter. It's not interesting. And I blink is great because I just,
Starting point is 00:06:25 I'm just imagining someone living by this thesis, you know, rolling with their gut instinct for everything. How quickly would you die? You're driving somewhere in your buddies like, hey, I'm going to pull up Google Maps and you're like, no, man, I'm, I'm just going to blink it. Right. I almost wish that I had had the energy to go back and read blink for this because it's by far of his major books, it's got to be the dumbest thesis. Like you read it and you're like, what the fuck are you talking about? You know, how is that? I'm a little bit disappointed in myself for not doing it, but at the same time, I, it was the right move. I know it was.
Starting point is 00:06:55 It's funny that your first impression of the book Blink was actually incorrect, and it's only interesting that you learned that it's full of shit. The only answer you've learned is full of shit. All right, outliers. In 2008, Gladwell releases Outliers, the story of success. This is the book that this episode is about, and I'm going to present you with my best effort to sum up the thesis of Outliers. Here it is. Success is driven by a combination of luck and hard work.
Starting point is 00:07:26 Ha ha ha ha ha ha. Merrill Street voice groundbreaking. Hold on, I am gonna send you the back of the jacket and I just want you to read the first two sentences. I feel like it's gonna be a poll quote from like Oprah or something. It says, in understanding successful people, we have come to focus far too much
Starting point is 00:07:47 on their intelligence and ambition and personality traits. Instead, Malcolm Gladwell argues in Outliers, we should look at the world that surrounds the successful, their culture, their family, their generation, and the idiosyncratic experiences of their upbringing. Oh, so it's like some fucked up genetic shit. We will be taking a very dark turn towards the end of this episode.
Starting point is 00:08:10 When we get to the, let's say genetic legacy portion. Oh, we're two for two on dark turn for this podcast. So, you know, the initial pitch is just like you thought that rich and successful people, 100% earned their social and economic position. But Malcolm Gladwell is here to tell you that that's not entirely true. It's like, well, yeah, no one fucking thinks that. I'm going to read to you a quote from early on in his book where he sort of defines the thesis. In Outliers, I want to convince you that these kinds of
Starting point is 00:08:40 personal explanations of success don't work. People don't rise from nothing. We do owe something to parentage and patronage. The people who stand before kings may look like they did it all by themselves, but in fact, they are invariably the benefit series of hidden advantages and extraordinary opportunities and cultural legacies that allow them to learn and work hard and make sense of the world in ways others cannot.
Starting point is 00:09:01 I mean, on some level, this sounds like something I agree with. Yes. Jeff Niggas is not rich this sounds like something I agree with. Yes. Jeff Nizzo's not rich because he's like an extraordinary man. He's rich because he had all kinds of invisible advantages that he himself refuses to acknowledge. So on some level, it's like, yeah, there's like structural wind at the back of rich people. Yeah, I wanted to like this thesis.
Starting point is 00:09:19 I'm very sympathetic to the basics, because America's a very individualist to culture that lionizes people for their achievements. And it's nice to see pushback of any type against that. pathetic to the basics, because America is a very individualist to culture that lianizes people for their achievements, and it's nice to see pushback of any type against that. But on the other hand, the ideas he's debunking are ideas that like no one to the left of Iron Rand actually believes, right? Like I mentioned, his books are essentially a series of discrete essays, so I want to like walk through the chapters.
Starting point is 00:09:43 The first chapter is about how star Canadian hockey players almost all had birthdays in the first few months of the year. This is explained by the fact that the cutoff date for Canadian junior leaks was in January. Right. What was happening was that at a young age being a few months older is a big advantage. So the older kids in leagues were outperforming. Then in turn, they'd be selected for better teams, receive better coaching, have more resources developed, dedicated to
Starting point is 00:10:09 their development. So if you've heard this before, it might be because outliers popularized it, or it might be because this research has been around since the 1980s. And by the time outliers came out, it's something that was like so widely known that many schools factor it into how they evaluate students. Oh, interesting. So right off the bat, I felt like I hadn't, I hadn't learned anything in chapter one, and that feels weird. And then you get to chapter two.
Starting point is 00:10:34 Chapter two is called the 10,000 hour rule. Oh, this fucking thing. This is the big one, right? The central claim in this chapter is that for anyone to be an expert in something, they need to have practiced for about 10,000 hours. Gladwell expressly states, quote, researchers have settled on what they believe is the magic number for true expertise, 10,000 hours. But he doesn't present any sort of like meta-analysis of research or anything.
Starting point is 00:11:01 Right. There's no evidence of a purported consensus among researchers, because there is absolutely no such consensus. Yeah, that's a weirdly round number. Yeah. From all the researchers that I've interviewed, a more researchy thing would be like,
Starting point is 00:11:15 somewhere between 8,000 and 12,000 hours, and this school of thought disagrees with that, and this school of thought says it's 4,000, this just doesn't sound like the way that researchers, quote unquote, conclude things. I would also say that the concept of expertise is unbelievably imprecise, right? Like I believe that I'm about as good at wordle,
Starting point is 00:11:36 as you can be. The best of persons the best in the world at wordle is only a little bit better than me, because the ceiling is shockingly low. Right, where it's something like basketball or something or like cello, you really do need like tens of thousands of hours like an incredible input. Where some skills like I guess checkers or something
Starting point is 00:11:53 is probably not as complex as chess. So to master chess, you just need a lot more hours than mastering checkers. Good instincts, because he does talk about both chess and basketball in an essay. I think that he is to his credit, talking about things like that, where practice will improve you over the course
Starting point is 00:12:11 of thousands of hours. Now, he's extrapolating mostly from a single study about elite violinists that found that they averaged about 10,000 hours of lifetime practice by age 20. He quotes one neurologist who cites the 10,000 hour claim as well, that neurologist is Daniel Levittin, quoted as saying, no one has yet found a case in which true world class expertise was accomplished
Starting point is 00:12:36 in less time. Okay. Which is a statement that just rings a bullshit. Right. You know, like that's, it's just way too sweeping of a claim to be reliable. No one has ever found a case. What does that mean? It's a weird thing to say. It's weird that a scientist said it. Now adding to my suspicions that perhaps the case is being overstated
Starting point is 00:12:58 here. On the very next page, Gladwell says, to become a chess grand master seems to take about 10 years. Only the legendary Bobby Fisher got to that elite level in less than that amount of time. As a chess fan myself, that immediately jumped out to me as incorrect. Magnus Carlson, the current world champion, was a grand master at age 13, and started playing at age five years.
Starting point is 00:13:26 Wu Zhang Ji was a grandmaster just shy of his 14th birthday and was introduced to the game at the age of six. Sergei Karayakken was at the time of Outliers publication the youngest grandmaster ever, 12 years and seven months old started playing when he was five. So he's basically, he's saying that the only person to ever become a chess grandmaster at that young age without these years and years of practice is Bobby Fisher. And then it's like, yeah, five minutes on Google
Starting point is 00:13:54 and there's like three other people that have done this. So it's really not, he's not a huge outlier. No. It's one way to say it. So this is the first specific example of the rule and it is demonstrably false. I checked the end notes to find the source he used for this and I was sort of assuming that he was using a very outdated source because most of those guys are
Starting point is 00:14:13 fairly recent at the time of Outliers publication. Sure enough, he's using a study done in 1996. Oh. Now, I couldn't find a copy of that study, but I'm pretty sure that Gladwell misrepresented it regardless because it was still untrue in 1996. Right. And from what I can tell has been untrue since 1994. So you know, when you mentioned the fact-checking standards of the New Yorker versus the big publishers,
Starting point is 00:14:38 yeah, this is when it first hit me, you know, Gladwell and his editors were at all times one Google search away from figuring any of this out. And to be clear, I'm just a casual chess fan. And as soon as he mentioned something that I'm a little familiar with, it was incorrect. Yeah, that's a bad sign. That's a red flag, right? Yeah. There's also, okay, I, because this factoid has bounced around the culture, of course, for like more than a decade now, and various, you come across various versions of it. And the version of it that I've always kind of appreciated,
Starting point is 00:15:10 it illustrates that there isn't a huge distinction between like inborn talent and practice. That I think what he's kind of trying to debunk with this, or at least the way that I remember it, was he's saying that like, okay, you have an idea, like yeah, Michael Jordan's this like amazing backable player, he was born with these incredible skills. But actually, like that dude is like working his ass off, you know, four or five, six hours a day for years before he gets to that skill level.
Starting point is 00:15:31 Does he have the dumb thing about the Beatles playing shows in Hamburg? Let's talk about the Beatles thing. So the Beatles got their start as the house band in a strip club in Germany, where they played eight hours a day every day. And Gladwell uses this as like an example of the effect of practice and hard work, essentially arguing that the driving force behind the Beatles greatness was this period of time where they were just like practicing nonstop. A couple weird things here. Clearly tons of successful music X did not have this sort of experience. They were playing covers too and like a lot of cover bands did not have this sort of experience, right? Yeah. They were playing covers too, and like a lot of cover bands did not become the Beatles.
Starting point is 00:16:07 And, you know, even if you're using aggressive calculations, it looks like these gigs totaled somewhere between one and 2,000 hours. Okay. And maybe a good time to note that Gladwell's claim here frequently switches between being a very specific statement about the importance of like 10,000 or 10,000-ish hours of practice, and a general statement that like, a lot of practice will probably make you very good at stuff.
Starting point is 00:16:27 Right. And his ability to like seamlessly move from one another's claims to the other is what makes it feel like you're actually sort of learning something because you're not paying attention to the fact that the basic claim here is like, if you practice 10,000 hours or so, you're going gonna be pretty fucking good. And it's like, yeah, man, I know. I did not need a book about this. It's almost just a real chapter to read,
Starting point is 00:16:52 because in hindsight, we know that this is the most influential chapter of the book, but there is not an ounce of insight in this, right? For anyone who's, who even thinks about it for a second, it's bizarre. The last thing I'll say about this on the merits is that the guy who actually did the violin study that Gladwell essentially relies on to develop this,
Starting point is 00:17:12 he calls Gladwell's chapter a provocative generalization, which I thought was. He still wants to get invited to Davos, whoever that's signed into. He's like, I don't want to burn any bridges, but like, yeah, this is bullshit. Right. Right.
Starting point is 00:17:28 I have this big picture complaint about the 10,000 hours idea, because like, if you read the back of the jacket and the thesis that Gladwell is pushing in the early chapters, it sounds sort of progressive. He's pushing the idea that successful people are often the product of circumstance. And you can see a progressive angle to this chapter too,
Starting point is 00:17:45 like you mentioned, right? That it's not just about innate talent, right? It's about hard work. But a lot of the framing in this chapter is that successful people are extraordinarily hard workers. And you realize that big chunks of this book are actually statement that we do live in somewhat of a meritocracy, right?
Starting point is 00:18:01 Or at least that highly successful people tend to have earned their success in some way. And that's more than just like a statement about how much practice you need to be good at a fucking violin, right? It's a statement about the fairness of the system about whether rich people deserve their wealth, you know? Elon Musk put in his 10,000 hours, did you?
Starting point is 00:18:19 It also feels like another one of these concepts that sounds smart and insightful upon, yeah, like blink, upon first encountering it. You're like, oh, 10,000 hours. Like, yeah, okay, there is this sort of threshold. But then as soon as you start to think about it, it does actually boil down to just, it depends. It depends on the skill.
Starting point is 00:18:38 It depends on the person. I'm sure some people do 10,000 hours of a thing and they're still bad at it. I'm sure some people do a thousand hours of something and they're super good at it. So even if you believe in the 10,000 hours of a thing and they're still bad at it. I'm sure some people do a thousand hours of something and they're super good at it. So even if you believe in the 10,000 hours thing, you're still gonna have to go on a case-by-case basis to be like, oh, well, is this an interesting standard
Starting point is 00:18:52 to apply? And by the time you've done that, you're just doing it on a case-by-case basis anyway. I think that's, that's well put. I mean, this chapter has like a hallmark of Gladwell's books. They're marketed on the back of what sounds like a flashy and compelling thesis.
Starting point is 00:19:06 But then when you get into it, there's a lot of qualifying and watering down of the thesis, right? Which from a scientific perspective is a reasonable thing to do. But at the same time, maybe means that the thesis was a little bit overstated from begin with. It's clickbait. Yeah, it creates a situation where there are two versions of the book in the public consciousness. One is presented to the general public. It's meant to draw in readers
Starting point is 00:19:31 with like a punchy headline thesis. And the other is like this more holistic and scientifically defensible version of the book, which isn't really marketed, but Gladwell can fall back on it when his headline thesis is criticized. And so there's often a big gap between what the conclusions of the book are and what the average reader thinks the conclusions of the book are. There was an interview in 2019 with him where he talked about responding to critics and he said, I'm always struck by how the task of responding to critics is more often than not not refuting the critic but correcting the critic.
Starting point is 00:20:03 Right. He will often respond to criticism by saying that the criticism oversimplifies his conclusions, and he's not always wrong, but the irony is that the marketing and presentation of his book is itself predicated on oversimplified conclusions, and that is why it sells, right?
Starting point is 00:20:20 Right. So he claims he's correcting his critics, but those critics are largely reacting to the thesis that Gladwell himself is sort of pitching to the public. I mean, on some level, I actually, I actually feel a little bit of sympathy for Gladwell. I do think that he probably has like nuanced beliefs on all of these things. And on some level, you can say that like authors are not responsible for how the fucking airport markets their books. But then I also think that the book itself doesn't have the kind of rigor that you can really fall back on.
Starting point is 00:20:51 Yeah. So there was a 2013 essay published in the New Yorker responding to critics called complexity and the 10,000 hour rule. There were various critics that pointed out that this is a rough average, very imprecise, and Gladwell sort of hastily admits that's true. He just responds by saying, like, well, sure, but my real point was that the biggest difference between people who are good at something and people who are great at something is the amount they practice, which like, okay, but if that was your real point, why do you name the chapter the 10,000 hour rule?
Starting point is 00:21:21 Right. Some of his critics pointed to studies showing that people became chess masters and pro basketball players in other countries with far less than 10,000 hours of practice. And Gladwell says, okay, well, but chess master is a lower rank than chess grandmaster. And international basketball is easier to get into than the NBA. So they're not really experts. You're starting to see the development of like a circular logic, right? Takes 10,000 hours of practice to be an expert and what defines an expert? 10,000 hours of practice.
Starting point is 00:21:47 Right, right, right. So the next couple of chapters are actually, I think, the chapters that I liked the most. They are both called the trouble with geniuses, part one and part two. The first chapter is essentially about the limits of the usefulness of IQ based on various findings that once you hit a certain threshold, the ability of IQ to predict your success in life starts to rapidly plateau. Oh, that's actually kind of interesting. Yeah, I thought it was fascinating. The second part is the same basic thesis, but it focuses on the plight of super high IQ guy Chris Langen. Gladwell sort of highlights this guy and points out that he lacks a sort of practical intelligence,
Starting point is 00:22:31 which from what I can tell is sort of the same thing as emotional intelligence, the way that Gladwell presents it. The basic premise of this chapter is that this guy hasn't really done anything with his life and Gladwell's trying to figure out why. Why is this, why is the smartest guy in the world just sort of fluttering about? He sort of sites a single study about parenting styles across different income groups and concludes that wealthier families tend to have more successful
Starting point is 00:22:56 kids, which he attributes at least in part to learned behaviors that get passed down from parents who are more capable of navigating various important social situations that he says, someone like Chris Langen can't navigate because he didn't have that instruction or didn't have that model. So, basically two chapters to say IQ plateaus, which is cool, and then rich kids do better than poor kids,
Starting point is 00:23:19 which, okay. Ground breakouts. It's very weird to say that rich kids are doing better than poor kids, and then say it's about emotional intelligence. Right. The links between intelligence and financial status
Starting point is 00:23:32 and success, like the literature on those things are just unbelievably vast and Gladwell barely touches it. Right. We're talking, these chapters are 40 pages that reference two or three studies and a bunch of cute little anecdotes. It's just like, you know, there might be an interesting grain or two in here, but you're way out of your depth. There's also some like IQ stuff that is, that's just sitting there ready to be debunked. Like this bright flashing red light. It's not clear to me that it's actually all that much of a paradox that somebody with a very high IQ
Starting point is 00:24:04 would not necessarily be, quote unquote, successful in this societally defined way, if you think of IQ as being really good at taking tests, it's supposed to be this generalized intelligence, but if you're then cutting out emotional intelligence from it and like social intelligence from it, then in what way is it generalized? We don't live in a world where we're all doing fucking calculus all day.
Starting point is 00:24:26 What you're really talking about is there certain kinds of tests that he does very well on, but that's not the same as saying that he's smart in the way that that term is used colloquially. To me, it says more about the concept of IQD, and it says about this dude. Yeah, I mean, so what Gladwell is debunking is like the idea that if someone has a very high IQ, they would be automatically successful, both financially and socially, right?
Starting point is 00:24:54 And that's not something that I think that intelligent people think about IQ. Yeah, I don't think anyone believes that. Yeah, that's something that might annoy IQ purists, but who cares? There's a part two of the book. You're between chapters and it's like part two, legacy. I was very confused at first. What he means is cultural legacy.
Starting point is 00:25:16 This is the race science section of the book. Oh, good. Get the calipers out. The next chapter is called Harlan Kentucky, and it's about the tendencies toward violence in so-called cultures of honor. Okay. Gladwell basically says that,
Starting point is 00:25:30 you know, this accounts for many regional differences in the violence, and that southerners are more violent than other people in certain circumstances. And that's the entire point of the chapter. Essentially, if you sort of trigger their sense of honor, southerners are much more violent. In other situations, they're less violent.
Starting point is 00:25:48 I'm sort of like, where's he going? Right? Where's all the momentum here going? And then you flip the page and you hit chapter seven, the ethnic theory of plane crashes. Oh, no. Oh, no. Oh, is this the fucking South Korean thing?
Starting point is 00:26:04 Oh, yeah. You would literally crash into a mountain because you didn't want to talk back to your superior or something. That is correct. Okay. Now, I want to say, before I got to this chapter, I wasn't sure about doing outliers for the show. I was like 10,000 hour rule. Yeah, it's pop science bullshit, but is there enough here? And then I got to this and I was like, oh my god, this episode is on, you know, we're fucking doing it. The chapter tells the story of Korean air, an airline that over a several decades span had significantly more plane crashes than other airlines.
Starting point is 00:26:40 And as the chapter title suggests, glad well thinks that this is because they are Korean. Specifically, he says that there are certain Korean cultural norms of communication that make crashes more likely, including more indirect manners of speaking and deference towards people in authority. Essentially, a well-functioning plane requires consistent and clear communication among the crew and with air traffic control. And according to Gladwell, these cultural norms get in the way. Oh my god. consistent and clear communication among the crew and with air traffic control, and according to Gladwell, these cultural norms get in the way.
Starting point is 00:27:07 Oh my God. In an interview, Gladwell once said, the single most important variable in determining whether a plane crashes is not the plane, it's not the maintenance, it's not the weather, it's the culture the pilot comes from. Oh, yeah, he doesn't back that up with data. I think it's also the weather and shit. I think it's a lot of other stuff. But this is so widespread that in 2013, when a jiana flight 214 crash at SFO, several prominent news organizations covered the question
Starting point is 00:27:37 of like whether culture was a factor. The Washington Post runs a piece with the headline, lack of cockpit communication recalls 1999 Korean Airlines crash near London. NBC ran a story titled Korean Culture May offer clues in Aegean a crash. A reporter asked the head of the National Transportation Safety Board in a press conference whether culture might have been the cause of the crash. So you might notice that this is essentially an unfalcifiable thesis. Yeah. As Gladwell himself admits, So you might notice that this is essentially an unfalsifiable thesis, right? Yeah.
Starting point is 00:28:05 As Gladwell himself admits, airplane crashes are super rare in large part because they require the confluence of events. It's not like it happens in movies where like one engine blows up. You need bad equipment, bad weather, tired or inexperienced pilots, poor communication, all of that. So the idea that you can sort of yank a specific conclusion about the role of culture out of all this on its face suspicious. But it gets worse as you dig in. One of the first Korean air crashes Gladwell sites is Flight 801 in 1978. That flight was shot down by a Russian fighter jet.
Starting point is 00:28:42 Now Gladwell even states this. He like acknowledges that very quickly, but then he still folds it into this broader thesis about Korean flight crews and continues to use to count it as one of the plane crashes that he's talking about when he when he sites the number of plane crashes. He then goes on to list several other Korean air crashes throughout the 1980s, trying to make the case that something weird is happening with Korean air. He does not give details about these flights,
Starting point is 00:29:14 so I looked them all up. The very first one is a 1983 flight flight 7 that was also shot down by the Soviets. This time, Gladwell doesn't even mention that, flight 7 that was also shot down by the Soviets. This time, Gladwell doesn't even mention that, which is notable, especially because this was a very big event in the Cold War. You can look up flight 007. The plane had taken off in New York and stopped in Anchorage, so many Americans died. Reagan called it a crime against humanity. Huge deal. huge deal. But Gladwell just listed off as if it were another example of Korean flight incompetence.
Starting point is 00:29:51 Next on his list is what he describes only as a Boeing 707 that went down over the end of man's sea in 1987. Do you want to guess how that plane went down? Was it shot down by India? No, you got it way wrong. North Korean terrorist planted a bomb on it. Oh. Oh.
Starting point is 00:30:09 So all in all, Gladwell identifies seven significant Korean air crashes between 1978 and 1997. And that is sort of the basis of his thesis here. But three of them, where the results of terrorists or military attacks. Yeah. I mean, frankly, this should be them were the results of terrorists or military attacks. I mean, frankly, this should be enough to just write off this fucking chapter entirely, but I want to mention one more thing.
Starting point is 00:30:31 Gladwell uses the National Transportation Safety Board's crash report for Korean air flight 801 throughout the chapter and misquotes it several times. And as Korean speakers have pointed out, grossly mistranslates the Korean. The upshot is that Gladwell wants you to believe that the first officer sensed some danger due to the weather, but was so deferential to the captain that he didn't properly communicate it. In reality, Korean speakers who analyzed it said he very plainly told the captain that the weather was bad. I don't want to get into every little detail here, but a Korean American blogger put it like this. This so-called interpretation of the pilot's true intentions is pure garbage.
Starting point is 00:31:08 It is so ludicrously wrong that I cannot think of enough superlatives to describe how wrong this is. Gladwell's exposition on Korean language is completely, definitely, utterly, entirely 120% laughable to anyone who has spoken Korean in a professional setting. Wow. So what's the actual explanation for how the plane went down? It's just like straightforward pilot error. He had the information and he crashed anyway. I think that the bottom line with that one,
Starting point is 00:31:33 and I look into the other ones a bit, and it seems like if you look at the reports on the crashes, it was a combination of events, but circling around bad weather and a couple of poor decisions. Right. And that's basically it. It's, you know, it's not much more complicated than that.
Starting point is 00:31:48 It's also a bigger problem, I think, existentially, too, because if it actually was the case that the copilot knew this thing and didn't say anything because of deferentialness, even that is not necessarily a cultural explanation that could also just be that individual happens to be very deferential. Absolutely. Like, let's put behind us the fact that this is unbelievably dishonest and that he full on lies about half of these crashes or at least gets them grossly wrong. He said
Starting point is 00:32:14 the single most important variable in determining whether a plane crashes is not the plane, it's not the maintenance, it's not the weather, it's the culture, the pilot comes from. And he uses Korean culture as his case study, but all he has as evidence is a single flight transcript and a handful of plane crashes, again, half of which were either shot down or blown up. There's no analysis of data from other Korean airlines, no data about other airlines with higher crash rates
Starting point is 00:32:41 than average, nor from what I can tell that he consult with a single Korean person. And finally, as Gladwell himself points out, reforms to internal training at Korean Air were able to completely remediate the Korean Air crash rate, which really undermines the idea that these ingrained cultural norms
Starting point is 00:33:02 are playing a big role. The interesting thing about this too, is that it's in this one, it's not like he's necessarily taking a body of scientific work, right? That there's a meta-review of every plane crash or something. He's actually doing some sort of quasi-academics study himself.
Starting point is 00:33:20 He himself is going through the flights, but what's weird is if it's a pop science book, there's dozens of scholars who research this. People have logs of like every single plane that's crashed, all the reasons. There's probably actually like some interesting stuff there that we don't know about. And you could actually do some sort of comparative study of like, yeah, there's some cultures that are more collectivistic. There's others that are more individualistic. You could list those as a kind of like on, you know, where are they on that spectrum?
Starting point is 00:33:46 And then say, you know, because like Germany is actually also very hierarchical. Like maybe Germany and South Korea do have like 0.2% more plane crashes than other countries. And that might actually be partly a little bit due to their more hierarchical cultures or something. But like you could actually do this in a scientific way and someone probably has, honestly,
Starting point is 00:34:06 because it's actually kind of an interesting question, like does culture play into plane crashes? But instead of doing that, or engaging with the literature at all, he's just being like, well, I found six crashes. Right, you said, maybe there's 0.2% more plane crashes. The problem with plane crash data is that planes almost never crash.
Starting point is 00:34:23 Yeah, exactly, yeah. We're talking about crash rates that are something like four out of every million flights for Korean air. And that's extraordinarily high. The sample sizes we're working with here are such that science almost can't be done. Yeah. I also just realized that this book is called Outliers, The Story of Success. And this chapter is just about freeing people being bad at flying planes.
Starting point is 00:34:46 That's a better chapter title though. So look, that was chapter seven, the ethnic theory of plane crashes. I figured we had hit rock bottom. And then I turned the page chapter eight, rice patties and math tests. Oh, we're back in Asia, aren't we? We are spending an extraordinary amount of time in Asia.
Starting point is 00:35:10 It's only half the population of the planet, Peter. I don't see why so much ineralizations wouldn't be justified. Now, the thesis of Chapter 8 is that you can track the purported success of Asians in mathematics to rice farming. Okay. He opens up talking about how important rice is in China, and then he moves on to a very different topic, Asian numbering systems.
Starting point is 00:35:34 Now, before we go on, I want to note that the fluidity with which Gladwell switches between talking about China specifically and Southern China specifically, and Asia generally in this chapter is unreal. Now, he deposits a very simple and somewhat compelling thesis here, predicated, of course, on actual research's work, which is that Asian numbering systems use simpler rules and simpler words than their Western counterparts, which seems to lead to Asian children developing their counting skills much faster than Western children, giving them a huge
Starting point is 00:36:08 developmental head start in mathematics. Of course, I'm saying Asian numbering systems, we're really talking about Chinese numbering systems. And then for reasons I genuinely cannot comprehend, he basically says, you know, and also I think it might have something to do with rice farming. You're sort of all of a sudden he's talking about how rice farming is very difficult and requires a lot of diligence and persistence. And those are all important in math. And rice farming was an integral part of society in southern China. And that helps explain why Asians are better at math.
Starting point is 00:36:40 I want to accuse you of oversimplifying this because that sounds so unconvincing. I mean, the weird thing is that he mentions the numbering system. I thought he was just going to go in a completely different direction. And that the rice paddy's thing was going to be some cute fucking anecdote that helps highlight why the numbering system works or something. But no, the only reason he mentions the numbering system from what I can tell is because his actual thesis is so off base that he has to put in what seems like a much more likely explanation so that if anyone ever says what the fuck are you talking about with his rice patty bullshit, he can be like, well, it could also be the numbering
Starting point is 00:37:20 system. I want to point out that there is absolutely no research or data presented to try to specifically tie rice farming to success in math. Like, one simple thing to do would be to look at people within Asia who farm rice and people who farm something else and prepare their math scores. He does not do that. In fact, and here's the weird thing,
Starting point is 00:37:41 he barely mentions math scores at all. The fact that like Asians are good at math is supposed to be something that you already believe. Right. Right. He mentions a single piece of research, the trends in international mathematics and science study. He notes that students from Japan, South Korea, Hong Kong, Singapore, and Taiwan all score high in math in that study. What you may notice is that China is not on that list. Right. Despite the fact that his entire spiel about rice farming is centered entirely around southern China.
Starting point is 00:38:14 Right. Let me pull out a footnote here where he explains this. mainland China isn't on the list because China doesn't yet take part in the Tim study. But the fact that Taiwan and Hong Kong ranks so highly suggests that the mainland probably would, that the mainland would probably also do really well. What? So, okay. One quick thing to note is that both Hong Kong and Taiwan are tiny little rich islands.
Starting point is 00:38:46 Right. Another thing you can call from this is that Singapore, which is one of those top performing countries in the study, is not a rice farming country at all. On the other end, Thailand is a country with large rice production, and they were actually one of the poor scoring countries in the study. Indonesia, the third largest producer of rice in the world performed even worse. Gladwell addresses none of this.
Starting point is 00:39:10 Nor does he address how common is rice farming in Japan and South Korea. So this chapter is basically a terrible educated guess about why Asians are so good at math. And it doesn't actually show in any meaningful way that Asians are so good at math. It it doesn't actually show in any meaningful way that Asians are so good at math. It's mind-boggling. I mean, this is the worst chapter in the book in terms of like presenting evidence. And we're after, you know, we're following the the plane crash chapter.
Starting point is 00:39:37 Asians can't fly planes chapter. Yeah. He doesn't explain why why Korean math skills did not translate into the cockpit. That's what I would have loved to hear him weave these together. You might as well at this point, right? Just make it all one fucking thesis, who cares? Wait, what is his actual argument that it's the rice patties though? It's like, there's something specific about growing rice that you have to like count the plants or something? Yeah, it's not counting per se.
Starting point is 00:40:00 It's basically that it's very hard and requires a lot of persistence. And he sets some studies that show that some Asian students basically showed higher levels of persistence than Western students in math. And that a lot of being good at math is actually is not that you're calculating more. It's that you're willing to spend longer on it. Yeah. A.K. A.K. You're more persistent. That part seemed to be true, like that sort of little bit of research
Starting point is 00:40:28 or whatever, for what I could tell. But that's it. There really is nothing specific about rice farming. It's an argument you can make about just about any sort of peasant culture, right? Right. Now rice farming is still happening, of course, but like 100 years ago, all of our ancestors were struggling a lot more. Right. And if you know, rice farming is still happening, of course, but like 100 years ago, all of our ancestors were struggling a lot more, right?
Starting point is 00:40:47 And if you felt like building a narrative around the struggles of like your immigrant ancestors in New York City in the 1880s or whatever and saying that that helped sort of instill a work ethic into you or whatever, you could do it, right? You could do it with any fucking culture. Everyone has a history of working hard and persevering of whatever. That's a large chunk of why we're all alive. So I truly don't understand why he lashed onto rice patties.
Starting point is 00:41:18 I really feel like he was just like, and here's the Asian section of my book. I'm gonna explain what they're up to. What's the first thing I know about Asia, math and rice? I'm just gonna Google those and figure it out. Yeah, thank God. Because, again, I can imagine this being like kind of an interesting theory, if you actually did it academically.
Starting point is 00:41:38 Like, there probably are, I guess, some crops that are much easier, some crops that take more perseverance. But surely, rice is not the most difficult crop in the whole world. So then you would look to other places that maybe the far north or something where you basically can't grow anything. Life is really hard there, so they should be super good at math, right?
Starting point is 00:41:55 But it's like he's not really doing any of the basic comparative work that you would need to do to prove a cultural explanation. Yeah, I mean, he himself says, well, like wheat farming, rice farming is a southern China thing. Wheat farming is much more common in the north, but he's just like, yeah, but we don't have that data. And it's like, well, that's, you're going to need that data, bro. Come back to me when you have the data.
Starting point is 00:42:20 Right. And then I would like to see like maybe some controls for income, etc. You know, like you're right. You have really not done even the slightest amount of the legwork necessary to establish this. Although he probably tells it in a really compelling way. Oh, yeah. This is also the gladwell thing that he's a, he is like a really gifted storyteller. No, he is. I mean, I was kind of struck here because sometimes he weaves the research and the story together in a way where your initial instinct is that makes sense and then you think for a second and you're sometimes like maybe not right right. But with this it was really just two separate things one Asians good at math two rice farming rice farming difficult. Right. That's it. I like, I couldn't fucking believe this chapter.
Starting point is 00:43:07 And if you're gonna make a chapter, built entirely around one of the most common stereotypes in one way that you, that, you know, that is promulgated in a Western society, maybe, maybe be careful, right? Right. I couldn't believe it. Even after the ethnic theory of plane crashes thing,
Starting point is 00:43:24 I was, I was like, jolted by this chapter. This is when I write a pop science book. One of the chapters is going to be called Winning Be Shopping. Sorry, it's just science. I also think thematically, because I write a million of these fucking pop science books for various projects. And one thing that I've found, especially with this tranche of journalists that are really into counterintuitive explanations, right?
Starting point is 00:43:46 It's always about like, you think it's this, but it's actually this, right? The problem with those explanations is that they elevate cultural explanations for things that are really straightforwardly just policy outcomes. Like I think if you want to understand why Taiwan and Hong Kong and Singapore do really good on these international math scores, it's probably related to their educational policies. It's probably related to something that is like really fucking obvious,
Starting point is 00:44:13 like their per capita GDP, and they're spending on education and like these specific structures of their specific education systems. Interesting, you bring this up, because the last chapter is about a Bronx school that succeeds with poorer inner city students basically by having very long school days because they figured out that
Starting point is 00:44:31 a lot of why poor students tend to fall behind so they aren't learning as much as richer kids when they're at home or over the summer. So they made the school day longer and they help mitigate that. One thing that Gladwell notes in this chapter is that school kids in Asia in those successful countries are in school a lot more. And he just kind of throws that out there and he's like, you know, so obviously that's why they're doing great. And you're like, I thought it was the fucking rice paddies, dude. What happened? Right. Which is it? He ends that Bronx chapter by saying that the school is, quote, bringing the lessons of the rice patty to the American inner city.
Starting point is 00:45:14 That's a little kicker. He had to kick her ready first. He had to write the whole chapter around. So that's the end of the book. There's an epilogue, and it's called a Jamaica story, which made me nervous, but it turns out it's just about his ancestors or whatever, and like his family history, and he's essentially talking about his own family and the advantages that they had, and that paved the way for his success.
Starting point is 00:45:40 It seemed like a relatively humble sort of, I've been very successful over the past decade, and I realized that I got a bit lucky. Yeah, that sounds fine. Yeah, it was totally fine. I would have preferred if right before it, he wasn't talking in really overstated generalizations about Asian people for 50 pages. It's also interesting,
Starting point is 00:46:01 because I always thought he had gotten worse over time than the earlier books were a lot, you know, on a lot firmer footing because you, to keep putting out these pop science books, you can start out with some good ideas and some good insights, but you only have a couple of those in your life, right? Like big, true ideas. But then it sounds like even from the earliest days, he was on pretty shaky foundations with the stuff. It's true that his later books have been increasingly criticized.
Starting point is 00:46:25 Yeah. There are some real takedowns of his work over the last few years. Yeah. His responses have become increasingly defensive from what I can gather. So it's true. The tolerance for Gladwell has fallen off a cliff. And I'm not, yeah, I'm not sure if that's because it has gotten worse or people have just sort of gotten wise to it.
Starting point is 00:46:45 But I will tell you this, it was 2008 and this shit sucks. This shit sucks. Well, I'm stealing this from a tweet of yours that a lot of the general interest blogging that we had during the blog glory days. A lot of those guys are getting cringier and cringier, partly because social media has exposed them to genuine expertise. Right. And I wonder if the end of the TED Talk era, which I think we're at like a post-Ted world now, even though of course those things exist and pop science books still exist,
Starting point is 00:47:17 the cultural hegemony that it used to have, I think is over partly because if, you know, he writes a book about like rice farming in Southern China, someone who like studies rice farming in Southern China can go on Twitter and be like, hello, this is what I do. This is how this book is bullshit in a way that the academics that were complaining about his books earlier were probably just like doing it at a cafeteria table
Starting point is 00:47:40 with themselves and it wasn't really getting noticed. Yeah, I think that's definitely part of it. I also wonder, you know, I was having a conversation with some zoomers about some of like the mid-Auts racial comedy. Oh, yeah. I think we were talking about an episode of the office, like Diversity Day, which I think like is no longer on peacock. Right?
Starting point is 00:48:01 Oh, yeah. And I was like, I think that there was a time after the 90s in this sort of end of history era, when most of us sort of had this agreement that racism had largely been solved. And that it was because of that, something that we could now joke about, because we're all on the site page. And as we sort of witnessed the Obama era, the rise of the Tea Party, the rise of the tea party, the rise of the trumpwings of the Republican party, it became very obvious that we actually have very different ideas about what racism is, why it's significant, whether it's behind us,
Starting point is 00:48:36 etc. And that stuff became less acceptable. And I see bits of that in Gladwell's discussions of like Asians being good at math. You probably wouldn't get away with this now because some editor would be like, Jesus, dude. Is this sort of stereotyping necessary? Is it accurate, et cetera? Back then, you're operating off the assumption that racism is really behind us.
Starting point is 00:49:03 So let's talk about this shit, right? We can talk about it now. We can now admit that a lot of these stereotypes are true. Right. Rather than dismantling the stereotypes you're thinking about why we believe they're true. Or actually even like doing basic work to investigate them because it doesn't sound like he even did that.
Starting point is 00:49:17 Well, I mean, and jacking a little bit of nuance into it. Right. It's not Asians are good at math. It's like a handful of largely small island countries that are very wealthy, are performing very well on math tests. Is that really a mystery? I don't know that it is.
Starting point is 00:49:32 Right. I do want to leave with one Gladwell quote, unrelated to this book. I don't remember much except being baffled as to who this Epstein guy was and why you were all on his plane. This did get dark. This did get dark. you

There aren't comments yet for this episode. Click on any sentence in the transcript to leave a comment.