Lex Fridman Podcast - #430 – Charan Ranganath: Human Memory, Imagination, Deja Vu, and False Memories

Episode Date: May 25, 2024

Charan Ranganath is a psychologist and neuroscientist at UC Davis, specializing in human memory. He is the author of a new book titled Why We Remember. Please support this podcast by checking out our ...sponsors: - Riverside: https://creators.riverside.fm/LEX and use code LEX to get 30% off - ZipRecruiter: https://ziprecruiter.com/lex - Notion: https://notion.com/lex - MasterClass: https://masterclass.com/lexpod to get 15% off - Shopify: https://shopify.com/lex to get $1 per month trial - LMNT: https://drinkLMNT.com/lex to get free sample pack EPISODE LINKS: Charan's X: https://x.com/CharanRanganath Charan's Instagram: https://instagram.com/thememorydoc Charan's Website: https://charanranganath.com Why We Remember (book): https://amzn.to/3WzUF6x Charan's Google Scholar: https://scholar.google.com/citations?user=ptWkt1wAAAAJ Dynamic Memory Lab: https://dml.ucdavis.edu/ PODCAST INFO: Podcast website: https://lexfridman.com/podcast Apple Podcasts: https://apple.co/2lwqZIr Spotify: https://spoti.fi/2nEwCF8 RSS: https://lexfridman.com/feed/podcast/ YouTube Full Episodes: https://youtube.com/lexfridman YouTube Clips: https://youtube.com/lexclips SUPPORT & CONNECT: - Check out the sponsors above, it's the best way to support this podcast - Support on Patreon: https://www.patreon.com/lexfridman - Twitter: https://twitter.com/lexfridman - Instagram: https://www.instagram.com/lexfridman - LinkedIn: https://www.linkedin.com/in/lexfridman - Facebook: https://www.facebook.com/lexfridman - Medium: https://medium.com/@lexfridman OUTLINE: Here's the timestamps for the episode. On some podcast players you should be able to click the timestamp to jump to that time. (00:00) - Introduction (10:18) - Experiencing self vs remembering self (23:59) - Creating memories (33:31) - Why we forget (41:08) - Training memory (51:37) - Memory hacks (1:03:26) - Imagination vs memory (1:12:44) - Memory competitions (1:22:33) - Science of memory (1:37:48) - Discoveries (1:48:52) - Deja vu (1:54:09) - False memories (2:14:14) - False confessions (2:18:00) - Heartbreak (2:25:34) - Nature of time (2:33:15) - Brain–computer interface (BCI) (2:47:19) - AI and memory (2:57:33) - ADHD (3:04:30) - Music (3:14:15) - Human mind

Transcript
Discussion (0)
Starting point is 00:00:00 The following is a conversation with Charan Ranganath, a psychologist and neuroscientist at UC Davis, specializing in human memory. He's the author of Why We Remember, Unlocking Memory's Power to Hold On to What Matters. And now a quick few second mention of each sponsor. Check them out in the description. It's the best way to support this podcast. We got Riverside for recording remote podcasts, ZipRecruiter for hiring, Notion for note taking and team collaboration, Masterclass for learning, Shopify for e-commerce and Element for delicious, delicious hydration.
Starting point is 00:00:38 Choose wisely my friends. Also, if you want to work with our amazing team or you just want to get in touch with me, go to Lexfreedman.com slash contact. And now onto the full ad reads. As always, no ads in the middle. I try to make these interesting, but if you must skip them friends, please do check out our sponsors. I enjoy their stuff. Maybe you will too.
Starting point is 00:01:00 This episode is also brought to you by Riverside the platform for recording remote podcasts and studio quality I've used them a bunch of times in the past. They're amazing It is the thing I recommend for anybody especially for people starting a podcast Studio quality exceptionally easy to use a million features that are all extremely useful for the whole Pipeline of creating a podcast. I mean, where do I start? First of all, they do the editing. And you could do text-based editing of the audio and the video.
Starting point is 00:01:27 So whatever you say, there's an AI-generated transcript in like 100 plus languages, whatever the language is, you can use then the text to do the editing. It does speaker detection, so it figures out who's speaking. All the synchronization obviously is done, not obviously because Some things seem obvious, but a really effortless beautiful execution of it Just is a breath of fresh air so in case you don't know it's a through the browser you
Starting point is 00:01:57 Record the video and the audio both sides the conversation everything is synchronized everything is stored Just everything is done really well. They have a lot of recommendations of what kind of hardware to use. I think in the video they provide, they say the most important thing is the microphone and lighting, and I agree with that. Good audio is number one. Second to that is indeed lighting
Starting point is 00:02:18 because basically every kind of camera that's available now will do all right. Anyway, Riverside makes that whole process super easy. I record my remote interviews with Riverside. Give it a try at riverside.fm and use code Lex for 30% off. That's riverside.fm and use code Lex. This episode is also brought to you by ZipRecruiter, a site that connects employers and job seekers.
Starting point is 00:02:45 To me, one of the most fulfilling things in life is working together with a great team. I love working. I love what I do. Everywhere I've ever worked, I loved doing it. And I love to be surrounded by people who also love doing it, and especially who are very good at it, and are pushing themselves
Starting point is 00:03:04 to the limit and together we're creating something special. Whatever that is, it could be a small thing or it could be a world changing thing or the mission is small or the mission is big. As long as there's a mission and we're in it together or we're constantly improving, I mean like a team that works great together
Starting point is 00:03:21 full of great people is one of the real joys of life. I think that's true for me, I think that's true for anybody, because so much of our lives is spent working. And that's where we really, especially in the realm of intellectual pursuits, really challenge ourselves. And so in the process of that challenge is where we find meaning. So build great teams and use the best tools to do it.
Starting point is 00:03:47 See why four out of five employers who post on ZipRecruiter get a quality candidate within the first day. Go to ziprecruiter.com slash Lex, they try it for free. That's ziprecruiter.com slash Lex, the smartest way to hire. This episode is brought to you by Notion, a note taking and team collaboration tool. I've used it for a long time now for note taking, for organizing my thoughts,
Starting point is 00:04:10 for connecting my thoughts, for searching through my thoughts, and now using AI to summarize, organize, generate drafts of things that I'm either planning or ideas that I'm working through or the research that I'm doing. Now that's for the individual. Where Notion really starts to shine is when there's multiple people working together. It is an incredible team collaboration tool. And again, the AI component gets integrated really nicely
Starting point is 00:04:40 because you can do the search, you can do the summarization, you can create a report of what everybody's been working on. It looks to the docs, the wikis do the search, you can do the summarization, you can create a report of what everybody's been working on. It looks to the docs, the wikis, the projects, and can basically do a Q and A for you to figure out like where do things stand from a manager position or from an individual contributor,
Starting point is 00:04:57 what am I supposed to be doing, what are the people doing, where can I help, that kind of stuff. Try Notion AI for free when you go to notion.com slash Lex. That's all lowercase, notion.com slash Lex to try the power of Notion AI today. This episode is brought to you by MasterClass, where you can watch over 180 classes
Starting point is 00:05:20 from the best people in the world in their respective disciplines. Phil Ivey on poker, Aaron Franklin on barbecue and brisket, Carlos Santana on guitar, Tom Morello on guitar, Terrence Tao on mathematical thinking, Martin Scorsese on filmmaking. In fact, I would really love, and I'm planning on talking to actors and directors more.
Starting point is 00:05:40 I love film. I love great TV. I love that medium of storytelling. And great actors and great directors are the way we consume stories. They are the medium, the channels, the wizards through which we, all of us, take in the stories, new exciting stories, or stories of old retold
Starting point is 00:06:07 better and better and better. So I would like to talk to those people. WTF podcast by Mark Maron. In the past, I really loved it when he interviewed actors and directors and he's done it really well. Inside Actor Studio was a program I really loved when long form interviews with actors, long form interviews with directors.
Starting point is 00:06:30 Even Charlie Rose did a really good job with that. Not the click bait sort of Hollywood style journalism, but more long form conversations. I would love to do more of those. Get unlimited access to every master class and get an additional 15% off an annual membership at Masterclass.com slash LexPod. That's Masterclass.com slash LexPod. This episode is also brought to you by Shopify, a platform designed for anyone to sell anywhere
Starting point is 00:07:01 with a great looking online store. I got a store, LexStreetMe.com slash store. It has a few shirts on there. If you want to get a shirt you can get it. It was so easy to set up. I like the machinery of humans selling stuff and buying stuff and through that capitalist machine figuring out together the things that bring happiness to our lives. In fact, the things isn't the source of happiness, of course. The things are the catalyst for human connection,
Starting point is 00:07:31 for humans to connect with each other. Like a T-shirt with Metallica or whatever band or whatever podcast or whatever show you like, its power is not in the fact that it looks good or something like this. It's power in the fact that it looks good or something like this. Its power in the connection you make when another person notices it and are also a fan of Metallica
Starting point is 00:07:50 or whatever is on the shirt. Or they don't know anything about Metallica but they like the logo and it starts a conversation where they'd be like, what is that? Metallica, is that some kind of machine shop thing? And you say, no. It is the greatest metal band of all time.
Starting point is 00:08:06 And there you grab a beer and the conversation begins. It's the human connection. The capitalist machine is not enough. It is merely a catalyst for the beauty of human connection. So join, if you want, the capitalist machine by signing up for a $1 per month trial period at Shopify.com slash Lex. That's all lowercase.
Starting point is 00:08:29 Go to Shopify.com slash Lex to take your business to the next level today. This episode is brought to you by Element. It's the delicious electrolyte drink, sodium potassium magnesium that I drink every day, a lot of it every day. I drink it in the jungle. When I was dying of thirst, when I was dehydrated and questioning whether I would be able to make it through the day,
Starting point is 00:08:54 not to mention the night, I had one pack of element with me waiting, knowing that I would disperse that pack of element into whatever water I would be able to find because you know Electrolytes is also really important But also I knew that I've had to drink sort of water Still water full of mud and all of that Elmwood would be the thing that makes it taste good
Starting point is 00:09:18 My favorite flavor the one I brought to the jungle the one I always drink is watermelon salt. But they also have cans now, which is like a carbonated fizzy thing to it and lots of great flavors. And I really love it. And I've been drinking that nonstop. Whenever I get some, I drink all of it very quickly. So I highly recommend that as well. Either the packs or the cans of Element Drink. Love it. Get a sample pack for free with any
Starting point is 00:09:46 purchase. Try it at www.drinkelement.com. This is the Lex Friedman Podcast. To support it, please check our sponsors in the description. And now, dear friends, here's Charron Ranganath. Danny Conway describes the experiencing self and the remembering self and that happiness and satisfaction you gain from the outcomes of your decisions do not come from what you've experienced but rather from what you remember of the experience. So can you speak to this interesting difference that you write about in your book of the experiencing self and the remembering self? Danny really impacted me because I was an undergrad at Berkeley and I got to take a
Starting point is 00:10:46 class from him long before he won the Nobel Prize or anything and it was just a mind-blowing class. But this idea of the remembering self and the experiencing self, I got into it because it's so much about memory even though he doesn't study memory. So we're right now having this experience, right? And people can watch it, presumably on YouTube or listen to it on audio. But if you're talking to somebody else, you could probably describe this whole thing in 10 minutes. But that's going to miss a lot of what actually
Starting point is 00:11:16 happened. And so the idea there is that the way we remember things is not the replay of the experience, it's something totally different. And it tends to be biased by the replay of the experience, it's something totally different. And it tends to be biased by the beginning and the end, and he talks about the peaks, there's also the best parts, the worst parts, etc. And those are the things that we remember. And so when we make decisions, we usually consult memory, and we feel like our memory is a record of what we've experienced, but it's not. It's this kind of very biased sample, but it's biased in an interesting and I think biologically relevant way.
Starting point is 00:11:54 So in the way we construct a narrative about our past, you say that it gives us an illusion of stability. Can you explain that? CB basically, I think that a lot of learning in the brain is driven towards being able to make sense. I mean, really, memory is all about the present and the future. The past is done. So, biologically speaking, it's not important unless there's something from the past that's useful. And so what our brains are really optimized for is to learn about the stuff from the past that's useful. And so what our brains are really optimized for is to learn about the stuff from the past that's going to be most useful in
Starting point is 00:12:31 understanding the present and predicting the future, right? And so cause-effect relationships, for instance, that's a big one. Now, my future is completely unpredictable in the sense that like you could, you know, in the next 10 minutes minutes pull a knife on me and slit my throat, right? I was planning on it. But having seeds of your work, it just, you know, generally my expectations about life, I'm not expecting that. I have a certainty that everything's gonna be fine, we're gonna have a great time talking today, right? But we're often right. It's like, okay, so I go to see a band on stage, you know, I know they're going to make me wait, the show's going to start late, and then, you know, they come on,
Starting point is 00:13:11 there's a very good chance there's going to be an encore. I have a memory, so to speak, for that event before I've even walked into the show, right? There's going to be people holding up their camera phones, try to take videos of it now because this is kind of the world we live in. So that's like everyday fortune telling that we do though. It's not real, it's imagined. And it's amazing that we have this capability and that's what memory is about. But it can also give us this illusion that we know everything that's about to happen. And I think what's valuable about that illusion is when it's broken, it gives us the information, right? So I mean, I'm sure being an AI, you know,
Starting point is 00:13:52 about information theory. And the idea is the information is what you didn't already have. And so those prediction errors that we make based on, you know, we make a prediction based on memory and the errors are where the action is. The error is where the learning happens. Exactly, exactly.
Starting point is 00:14:10 Well, just to linger on Danny Kahneman and just this whole idea of experiencing self versus remembering self, I was hoping you can give a simple answer of how we should live life. Based on the fact that our memories could be a source of happiness or could be the primary source of happiness.
Starting point is 00:14:35 That an event, when experienced, bears its fruits the most when it's remembered over and over and over and over. Maybe there is some wisdom in the fact that we can control to some degree how we remember it, how we evolve our memory of it, such that it can maximize the long-term happiness of that repeated experience.
Starting point is 00:15:00 Oh yeah, well first I'll say, I wish I could take you on the road with me, because that was such a great description. Can I be your opening act? Oh my God, no, I'm gonna open for you, dude. Otherwise it's like, you know, everybody leaves after you're done. Believe me, I did that in Columbus, Ohio once.
Starting point is 00:15:19 It wasn't fun. Like the opening acts, like drank our bar tab. We spent all this money going all the way there. There was only the, everybody left after the opening acts like drank our bar tab. We spent all this money going all the way there. There was only the, everybody left after the opening acts were done and there was just that stoner dude with the dreadlocks hanging out. And then next thing you know, we blew like our savings
Starting point is 00:15:35 on getting a hotel room. So we should, as a small tangent, you're a legit touring act. When I was in grad school, I played in a band and yeah, we traveled, we would play shows. It wasn't like we were in a hardcore touring band, but we did some touring and had some fun times. And yeah, we did a movie soundtrack.
Starting point is 00:15:54 Nice. Henry Portrait of Serial Killer. So that's a good movie. We were on the soundtrack for the sequel, Henry Two, Mask of Sanity, which is a terrible movie. How's the soundtrack? It's pretty good? It's bad ass.
Starting point is 00:16:06 At least that one part where the guy throws up the milkshake. That's my song. We're gonna have to see. We're gonna have to see it. All right, we're getting back to life advice. And happiness, yeah. One thing that I try to live by, especially nowadays, and since I wrote the book,
Starting point is 00:16:20 I've been thinking more and more about this, is how do I want to live a memorable life? I think if we go back to the pandemic, how many people have memories from that period? Aside from the trauma of being locked up and seeing people die and all this stuff, I think it's one of these things where we were stuck inside looking at screens all day doing the same thing with the same people. And so I don't remember much from that in terms of those good memories that you're talking about, right? You know, when I was growing up, my parents worked really hard for us and you know, we went on some vacations, but not very often. And I really try to do now vacations to interesting
Starting point is 00:17:06 places as much as possible with my family, because like, those are the things that you remember, right? So I really do think about what's going to be like something that's memorable, and then just do it even if it's a pain in the ass, because the experiencing self will suffer for that, but the remembering self will suffer for that, but the remembering self will be like, yes, I'm so glad I did that. Do things that are very unpleasant in the moment because those can be reframed
Starting point is 00:17:33 and enjoyed for many years to come. That's probably good advice, or at least when you're going through shit, it's a good way to see the silver lining of it. Yeah, I mean, I think it's one of these things where if you have people who you've gone through, since you said it, I'll just say, since you've gone through shit with someone,
Starting point is 00:17:53 and it's like, that's a bonding experience often. You know, I mean, that can really bring you together. I like to say it's like, there's no point in suffering unless you get a story out of it. So in the book, I talk about the power I like to say it's like there's no point in suffering unless you get a story out of it. So in the book I talk about the power of the way we communicate with others and how that shapes our memories. And so I had this near-death experience, at least that's how I remember it, on this paddle board where just everything they could have gone wrong did go wrong almost. So many mistakes were made and ended up at some point just basically away from my board,
Starting point is 00:18:30 pinned in a current in this corner, not a super good swimmer. And my friend who came with me, Randy, who's a computational neuroscientist, and he had just been pushed down past me so he couldn't even see me. And I'm just like, if I die here, you know, I mean, no one's around. It's like, you just die alone. And so I just said, well, failure's not an option. And eventually I got out of it and froze and got cut up. And I mean, the things that we were going through were just insane.
Starting point is 00:19:03 But short version of this is my wife and my daughter and Randy's wife, they gave us all sorts of hell about this because they were ready to send out a search party. So they were giving me hell about it. And then I started to tell people in my lab about this and then friends, and it just became a better and better story every time. And we actually had some photos of just the crazy things like this generator that was hanging over the water and were like ducking under this thing, or these metal gratings, and I'm like going flat on. And it was just nuts, you know?
Starting point is 00:19:39 But it became a great story. And it was definitely, I mean, Randy and I were already tight, but that was a real bonding experience for us. And yeah, I mean, and I learned from that that it's like, I don't look back on that enough actually, because I think we often, at least for me, I don't necessarily have the confidence to think that things will work out, that I'll be able to get through a certain thing. But my ability to actually get something done in that moment is better than I give myself credit for, I think, and that was the lesson of that story
Starting point is 00:20:13 that I really took away. Well, actually, just for me, you're making me realize now that it's not just those kinds of stories, but even things like periods of depression or really low points, to me at least it feels like a motivating thing that the darker it gets, the better the story will be if you emerge on the other side. That to me feels like a motivating thing.
Starting point is 00:20:39 So maybe if people listening to this and they're going through some shit, as we said, one thing that could be a source of light is that it'll be a hell of a good story when it's all over, when you emerge on the other side. Let me ask you about decisions. You've already talked about it a little bit, but when we face the world and we're making different decisions,
Starting point is 00:21:03 how much does our memory come into play? Is it the kind of narratives that we've constructed about the world that are used to make predictions that's fundamentally part of the decision making? Absolutely, yeah. So let's say after this, you and I decided we're going to go for a beer, right? How do you choose where to go? You're probably going to be like, oh yeah, this new bar opened up near me. I had a great time there. They had a great beer selection. Or you might say, oh, we went to this place and it was totally crowded and they're playing this horrible EDM or whatever. And so right there, valuable source of information, right? And then you have these things like where you do this counterfactual stuff like, well, I did this previously, but what if I had gone somewhere else and said, maybe I'll go to
Starting point is 00:21:48 this other place because I didn't try it the previous time. So there's all that kind of reasoning that goes into it too. I think even if you think about the big decisions in life, right? It's like you and I were talking before we started recording about how I got into memory research and you got into AI. And it's like we all have these personal reasons that guide us in these particular directions. And some of it's the environment and random factors in life, and some of it is memories of things that we want to overcome or things that we build on in a positive way, but either way they define us.
Starting point is 00:22:26 And probably the earlier in life the memories happen, the more defining power they have in terms of determining who we become. I mean, I do feel like adolescence is much more important than I think people give credit for. I think that there is this kind of a sense like the first three years of life is the most important part. But the teenage years are just so important for the brain, you know? And so that's where a lot of mental illness starts to emerge. You know, now we're thinking of things like schizophrenia as a neurodevelopmental disorder because it just emerges during that period of adolescence and early adulthood. And I think the other part of it is that, I guess I was a little bit too firm in
Starting point is 00:23:13 saying that memory determines who we are. The self is an evolving construct. I think we kind of underestimate that. And when you're a parent, you feel like every decision you make is consequential in forming this child. And it plays a role, but so do the child's peers. That's why I think the big part of education I think that's so important is not the content you learn. I mean, think of how much dumb stuff we learned in school, right? But a lot of it is learning how to get along with people and learning who you are and how you function. And that can be terribly traumatizing even if you have perfect parents working on you. CB Is there some insight into the human brain that explains why we don't seem to remember
Starting point is 00:24:06 anything from the first few years of life? Yeah. Yeah. In fact, actually, I was just talking to my really good friend and colleague, Simona Getty, who studies the neuroscience of child development. And so we were talking about this. And so there are a bunch of reasons, I would say. So one reason is there's an area of the brain called the
Starting point is 00:24:26 hippocampus, which is very, very important for remembering events or episodic memory. And so the first two years of life, there's a period called infantile amnesia. And then the next couple years of life after that, there's a period called childhood amnesia. And the difference is that basically in the lab and even during childhood and afterwards, children basically don't have any episodic memories for those first two years. The next two years, it's very fragmentary and that's why they call it childhood amnesia. So there's some, but it's not mine. So one reason is that the hippocampus is taking some time to develop, but another is the neocortex,
Starting point is 00:25:06 so the whole folded stuff of gray matter all around the hippocampus is developing so rapidly and changing, and a child's knowledge of the world is just massively being built up, right? So I'm gonna probably embarrass myself, but it's like if you showed like you trained a neural network and you give it like the first couple of patterns or something like that and then you bombard it with another like, you know, years worth of data try to get back those first couple of patterns, right? It's like everything changes and so the brain is so plastic. The cortex is so plastic during that time and We think that memories for events are very distributed across the brain. So imagine you're trying to get back that pattern of activity that happened during this one moment, but the roads that you would take to get there have been
Starting point is 00:25:55 completely rerouted, right? So I think that's my best explanation. The third explanation is a child's sense of self takes a while to develop. And so their experience of learning might be more learning what happened as opposed to having this first person experience of, ah, I remember, I was there. Well, I think somebody once said to me that kind of loosely, philosophically,
Starting point is 00:26:24 that the reason we don't remember the first few years of life, infantile amnesia, is because how traumatic it is. Basically, the error rate that you mentioned when your brain's prediction doesn't match reality, the error rate in the first few years of life, your first few months certainly, is probably crazy high. It's just nonstop freaking out.
Starting point is 00:26:49 The collision between your model of the world and how the world works is just so high that you want whatever the trauma of that is, not to linger around. I always thought that's an interesting idea because just imagine the insanity of what's happening in a human brain in the first couple of years. Just, you don't know anything.
Starting point is 00:27:11 And there's just this stream of knowledge and where somehow, given how plastic everything is, it just kind of molds and figures it out. But it's like an insane waterfall of information. I wouldn't necessarily describe it as a trauma. We can get into this whole stages of life thing, which I just love. Basically, those first few years, think about it. A kid's internal model of their body is changing, right? It's like just learning to move. If you ever have a baby, you'll know that the first three months, they're discovering their
Starting point is 00:27:45 toes, right? And it's just nuts. So everything is changing. But what's really fascinating is, and I think this is not at all me being a scientist, but it's one of those things that people talk about when they talk about the positive aspects of children is that they're exceptionally curious and they have this kind of openness towards the world. And so that prediction error is not a negative traumatic thing. I think it's like a very positive thing because it's what they use, they're seeking information. One of the areas that I'm very interested in is the prefrontal cortex. It's an area of the brain that, I mean, I could talk all day about it, but it helps us use our knowledge to say, hey, this is what I want to do
Starting point is 00:28:31 now. This is my goal. So this is how I'm going to achieve it and focus everything towards that goal. The prefrontal cortex takes forever to develop in humans. The connections are still being tweaked and reformed into late adolescence, early adulthood, which is when you tend to see mental illness pop up. So it's being massively reformed. Then you have about 10 years maybe of prime functioning of the prefrontal cortex, and then it starts going down again, and you end up being older and you start losing all that frontal function. So look at this and you'd say, okay, you sit around to episodic memory talks and always say children are worse than adults at episodic memory, older adults are worse than young adults at episodic memory. And I always would say, God, that's so weird. Why would we have this period of
Starting point is 00:29:21 time that's so short when we're perfect, right? Or optimal. And I like to use that word optimal now because there's such a culture of optimization right now. And it's like, I realized I have to redefine what optimal is because for most of the human condition, I think we had a series of stages of life where you have basically adults saying, okay, young adults saying, I've got a child and I'm part of this village and I have to hunt and forage and get things done. I
Starting point is 00:29:53 need a prefrontal cortex so I can stay focused on the big picture and long haul goals. Now, I'm a child, I'm in this village, I'm kind of wandering around and I've got some safety and I need to learn about this culture because I know so little. What's the best way to do that? Let's explore. I don't want to be constrained by goals as much. I want to really be free. Play and explore and learn. So you don't want a super tight prefrontal cortex. You don't even know what the goals should be yet, right? It's like if you're trying to design a model that's based on a bad goal, it's not going to work well, right? So then you go late in life and you say, oh, why don't you have a great prefrontal cortex then? But I think, I mean, if you go back and you think how many species actually stick around naturally long after their childbearing years are over, after their reproductive years are over. Like menopause, from what I understand, menopause is not all
Starting point is 00:30:49 that common in the animal world, right? So why would that happen? And so I saw Alison Gopnik said something about this, so I started to look into this, about this idea that really, when you're older in most societies, your job is no longer to form new episodic memories. It's to pass on the memories that you already have, this knowledge about the world, what we call semantic memory, to pass on that semantic memory to the younger generations, pass on the culture. Even now in indigenous cultures, that's the role of the elders. They're respected. They're not seen as people who are past it and losing it. And I thought that was a very poignant thing that memory is doing what
Starting point is 00:31:33 it's supposed to throughout these stages of life. So it is always optimal in a sense. It's just optimal for that stage of life. Yeah, and for the ecology of the system. So I looked into this and it's like another species that has menopause is orcas. Orca pods are led by the grandmothers, right? So not the young adults, not the parents or whatever, the grandmothers. And so they're the ones that pass on the traditions to the, I guess, the younger generation of orcas. And if you look from what little I understand, different orca pods have different traditions. They hunt for different things, they have different play traditions, and that's a culture, right? And so in social animals, evolution, I think,
Starting point is 00:32:19 is designing brains that are really around, you know, it's obviously optimized for the individual, but also for kin. And I think that the kin are part of this, when they're a part of this intense social group, the brain development should parallel the nature of the ecology. Well, it's just fascinating to think of the individual orca or human throughout his life
Starting point is 00:32:45 in stages doing a kind of optimal wisdom development. So in the early days you don't even know what the goal is and you figure out the goal and you kind of optimize for that goal and you pursue that goal and then all the wisdom you collect through that then you share with the others in the system, with the other individuals. And as a collective then you kind of converge
Starting point is 00:33:06 towards greater wisdom throughout the generation. So in that sense, it's optimal. Us humans and orcas got something going on. It works. Oh yeah, apex predators. I just got a megalon tooth tooth, speaking of Apex partners. It's, just imagine the size of that thing. Anyway, how does the brain forget
Starting point is 00:33:34 and how and why does it remember? So maybe some of the mechanisms, you mentioned the hippocampus, what are the different components involved here? So we can think about this on a number of levels. Maybe I'll give you the simplest version first, which is we tend to think of memories as these individual things and we can just access them,
Starting point is 00:33:52 maybe a little bit like photos on your phone or something like that. But in the brain, the way it works is you have this distributed pool of neurons and the memories are kind of shared across different pools of neurons. So what you have is competition where sometimes memories that overlap can be fighting against each other. So sometimes we forget because that competition just wipes things out.
Starting point is 00:34:19 Sometimes we forget because there aren't the biological signals which we can get into that would promote long-term retention. And lots of times we forget because we can't find the cue that sends us back to the right memory, and we need the right cue to be able to activate it, right? So for instance, in a neural network, there is no, you wouldn't go and you'd say this is the memory, right? It's like the whole ecosystem, I mean the whole ecosystem of memories is in the weights of the neural network and in fact you could extract entirely new memories depending on how you feed. You have to have the right query, the right prompt to access that whatever the part you're looking for. That's exactly right. That's exactly right. And in humans you have this more complex set of ways
Starting point is 00:35:02 memory works. There's, as I said, the knowledge or what you call semantic memory, and then there's these memories for specific events, which we call episodic memory. And so there's different pieces of the puzzle that require different kinds of cues. So that's a big part of it too, is just this kind of what we call retrieval failure. You mentioned episodic memory, you mentioned semantic memory.
Starting point is 00:35:23 What are the different separations here? What's working memory, short-term memory, you mentioned semantic memory. What are the different separations here? What's working memory, short-term memory, long-term memory? What are the interesting categories of memory? Yeah, and so memory researchers, we love to cut things up and say, you know, is memory one thing or is it two things? Is it two things or is it three things? And so one of the things that there's value in that, and especially experimental value in that, and especially
Starting point is 00:35:45 experimental value in terms of being able to dissect things, in the real world it's all connected. Speak to your question, working memory, is a term that was coined by Alan Batley. It's basically thought to be this ability to keep information online in your mind right in front of you at a given time, and to be able to control the flow of that information, to choose what information is relevant, to be able to manipulate it, and so forth. And one of the things that Alan did that was quite brilliant was he said, there's this ability to kind of passively store information, you know, see things in your mind's eye or hear your internal monologue. But you know, we have that ability to keep information in mind. But then we
Starting point is 00:36:27 also have this separate what he called a central executive, which is identified a lot with the prefrontal cortex. It's this ability to control the flow of information that's being kept active based on what it is you're doing. Now, a lot of my early work was basically saying that this working memory, which some memory researchers would call short-term memory, is not at all independent from long-term memory. That is that a lot of executive function requires learning and you have to have like synaptic change for that to happen. But there's also transient forms of memory. So one of the things I've been getting into lately is the idea that we form internal models of events. The obvious one that I always use is birthday parties, right? So you go to a child's birthday party, once the cake comes out and you just see
Starting point is 00:37:19 a candle, you can predict the whole frame set of events that happens later. And up till that point where the child blows out the candle, you have an internal model in your head of what's going on. And so if you follow people's eyes, it's not actually on what's happening, it's going where the action's about to happen, which is just fascinating, right? So you have this internal model, and that's a kind of a working memory product. It's something that you're keeping online that's allowing you to interpret this world around you. Now to build that model though, you need to pull out stuff from your general knowledge of the world, which is what we call semantic memory. And then you'd want to be able to pull out memories for specific events that
Starting point is 00:38:00 happened in the past, which we call episodic memory. So in a way they're all connected, even though it's different. Um, the things that we're focusing on and the way we organize information in the present, which is working memory, we'll play a big role in determining how we remember that information later, which people typically call long-term memory. So if you have something like a birthday party and you've been to many before. You're going to load that from disk into working memory, this model, and then you're mostly operating on the model. And if it's a new task, you don't have a model, so you're more in the data collection? Yes, one of the fascinating things that we've been studying, and we're not at all the first to do this. Jeff Sachs was a big pioneer in this and I've been working with many other people, Ken Norman,
Starting point is 00:38:51 Leila Devachiyan at Columbia has done some interesting stuff with this, is this idea that we form these internal models at particular points of high prediction error or points of, I believe also points of uncertainty, points of surprise or motivationally significant periods. And those points are when it's maximally optimal to encode an episodic memory. So I used to think, oh, well, we're just encoding episodic memories constantly, boom, boom, boom, boom, boom. But think about how much redundancy there is in all that, right? It's just a lot of information that you don't need. But if you capture an episodic memory at the point of maximum uncertainty for the singular experience, right? It's only going to happen once. But if you capture it at the point of maximum uncertainty or maximum surprise,
Starting point is 00:39:41 you have the most useful point in your experience that you've grabbed. And what we see is that the hippocampus and these other networks that are involved in generating these internal models of events, they show a heightened period of connectivity or correlated activity during those breaks between different events, which we call event boundaries. These are the points where you're like surprised or you cross from one room to another and so forth. And that communication is associated with a bump of activity in the hippocampus and better memory.
Starting point is 00:40:14 And so if people have a very good internal model, throughout that event, you don't need to do much memory processing, you're in a predictive mode, right? And so then at these event boundaries you encode and then you retrieve and you're like, okay, wait a minute, what's going on here? Brangunath's now talking about orcas, what's going on? And maybe you have to go back and remember reading my book to pull out the episodic memory to make sense of whatever it is I'm babbling about, right? And so there's this beautiful dynamics that you can see in the brain
Starting point is 00:40:45 of these different networks that are coming together and then deaffiliating at different points in time that are allowing you to go into these modes. And so to speak to your original question, to some extent when we're talking about semantic memory and episodic memory and working memory, you can think about it as these processes that are unfolding as these networks
Starting point is 00:41:06 kind of come together and pull apart. Can memory be trained and improved? This beautiful connected system that you've described, what aspect of it is a mechanism that can be improved through training? I think improvement, it depends on what your definition of optimal is. So what I say in the book is that you don't want to remember more, you want to remember better,
Starting point is 00:41:32 which means focusing on the things that are important. And that's what our brains are designed to do. So if you go back to the earliest quantitative studies of memory by Ebbinghaus, what you see is that he was trying so hard to memorize this arbitrary nonsense, and within a day, he lost about 60% of that information. And he was basically using a very, very generous way of measuring it, right? So as far as we know, nobody has managed to violate those basics of having people forget most of their experiences. So if your expectation is that you should remember everything and that's what your optimal is, you're already off because this is not what human brains are designed to do. On the other hand, what we see over and over again
Starting point is 00:42:16 is that the brain does basically one of the cool things about the design of the brain is it's always less is more, right? I've seen estimates that the human brain uses something like 12 to 20 watts in a day. That's just nuts, the low power consumption, right? So it's all about reusing information and making the most of what we already have. And so that's why basically again, what you see biologically is neuromodulators, for instance, these chemicals in the brain like norepinephrine, dopamine, serotonin, these are chemicals that are released during moments that tend to be biologically significant, surprise, fear, stress, etc. And so these chemicals promote lasting plasticity, right? Essentially, some mechanisms
Starting point is 00:43:08 by which the brain can prioritize the information that you carry with you into the future. Attention is a big factor as well, our ability to focus our attention on what's important. And so there's different schools of thought on training attention, for instance. So one of my colleagues, Amishi Jha, she wrote a book called Peak Mind and talks about mindfulness as a method for improving attention and focus. So she works a lot with military like Navy SEALs and stuff to do this kind of work with mindfulness meditation. Adam Ghazali, another one of my friends and colleagues, has worked on training through video games actually as a way of training attention. And so it's not clear to me. One of the challenges though in training is you tend to overfit to the
Starting point is 00:43:59 thing that you're trying to optimize, right? So if tend to, if I'm looking at a video game, I can definitely get better at paying attention in the context of the video game, but you transfer it to the outside world. That's very controversial. The implication there is that attention is a fundamental component of remembering something, allocating attention to it,
Starting point is 00:44:20 and then attention might be something that you could train. How you allocate attention, how you allocate attention and how you hold attention on a thing. I can say that in fact, we do in certain ways, right? So if you are expert in something, you are training attention. So we did this one study of expertise in the brain and so people used to think,
Starting point is 00:44:42 let's say if you're a bird expert or something, right? People will go like, if you get really into this world of birds, you start to see the differences in your visual cortex is tuned up and it's all about plasticity of the visual cortex. And vision researchers love to say everything's visual. But it's like we did this study of attention and working memory and expertise. And one of the things that surprised us were the biggest effects as people became experts in identifying these different kinds of just crazy objects that we made up.
Starting point is 00:45:12 As they developed this expertise of being able to identify what made them different from each other and what made them unique, we were actually seeing massive increases in activity in the prefrontal cortex. And this fits with some of the studies of chess experts and so forth that it's not so much that you learn the patterns passively,
Starting point is 00:45:30 you learn what to look for, you learn what's important, what's not, right? And you can see this in any kind of expert professional athlete, they're looking three steps ahead of where they're supposed to be. So that's a kind of a training of attention. And those are also what you'd call expert memory skills. So if you take the memory athletes,
Starting point is 00:45:50 I know that's something we're both interested in. So these are people who train in these competitions and they'll memorize like a deck of cards in like a really short amount of time. There's a great memory athlete, her name I think is pronounced Yenya Wintersole. So I think she's got like a giant Instagram following. And so she had this YouTube video that went viral where she had memorized an entire IKEA catalog, right? And so how do people do this? By all accounts from people who become memory athletes, they weren't born with some extraordinary memory, but they practice strategies over and over and over again. The strategy that they use for memorizing a particular thing, it can become automatic and you can just deploy it in an instant.
Starting point is 00:46:36 Right? So again, it's not necessarily going to one strategy for learning the order of a deck of cards might not help you for something else that you need, like remembering your way around Austin, Texas. But it's gonna be these, whatever you're interested in, you can optimize for that. And that's just a natural byproduct of expertise. There's certain hacks. There's something called the Memory Palace
Starting point is 00:47:01 that I played with. I don't know if you're familiar with that whole technique. And it works. It's interesting. So another thing I recommend for people a lot is I use Anki a lot every day. It's a app that does spaced repetition. So I think medical students and students use this a lot
Starting point is 00:47:19 to remember a lot of different things. Oh yeah, okay, we can come back to this. But yeah, go on. Sure. It's the whole concept of spaced repetition. You just, when the thing is fresh, different things. Oh yeah, okay, we can come back to this, but yeah. Sure, it's the whole concept of space repetition. You just, when the thing is fresh, you kind of have to remind yourself of it a lot, and then over time, you can wait a week, a month, a year,
Starting point is 00:47:36 before you have to recall the thing again. And that way, you essentially have something like note cards that you can have tens of thousands of, and can only spend 30 minutes a day and actually be refreshing all of that information, all of that knowledge. It's really great. And then for Memory Palace is a technique
Starting point is 00:47:55 that allows you to remember things like the IKEA catalog or by placing them visually in a place that you're really familiar with. Like I'm really familiar with this place so I can put numbers or facts or whatever you wanna remember. You can walk along that little palace and it reminds you. It's cool, like there's stuff like that
Starting point is 00:48:16 that I think athletes, memory athletes could use but I think also regular people can use. One of the things I have to solve for myself is how to remember names. I'm horrible at it. I think it's because when people introduce themselves, I have the social anxiety of the interaction where I'm like, I know I should be remembering that,
Starting point is 00:48:39 but I'm freaking out internally about social interaction in general. And so therefore I forget immediately. So I'm looking for internally about social interaction in general. And so therefore I forget immediately. So I'm looking for good tricks for that. So, uh, I'm, I feel like we've got a lot in common because when people introduce themselves to me, it's almost like I have this like just blank blackout for a moment and then I'm just looking at them like, what happened? I look away or something. what's wrong with me?
Starting point is 00:49:06 So I mean, I'm totally with you on this. The reason why it's hard is that there's no reason we should be able to remember names because when you say remembering a name, you're not really remembering a name. Maybe in my case you are, but most of the time, you're associating a name with a face and an identity. And that's a completely arbitrary thing,
Starting point is 00:49:26 right? I mean, maybe in the olden days, somebody named Miller, it's like they're actually making flour or something like that. But for the most part, it's like these names are just utterly arbitrary. So you have no thing to latch onto. And so it's not really a thing that our brain does very well to learn meaningless arbitrary stuff. So what you need to do is build connections somehow, visualize a connection, and sometimes it's obvious or sometimes it's not. I'm trying to think of a good one for you now, but the first thing I think of is Lex Luthor. But that's great. Yeah. So you think of Lex Luthor. Because this isn't Lex Luthor where a suit, I think.
Starting point is 00:50:04 I know he has a shaved head though, or he's bald, which you're not. You've got a great head but I can think of, yeah. So you think of Lex Luthor. Because this isn't Lex Luthor wear a suit, I think. I know he has a shaved head though, or he's bald, which you're not. You've got a great head if I trade hair with you any day. But something like that. But if I can come up with something, like I could say, okay, so Lex Luthor is this criminal mastermind,
Starting point is 00:50:19 and then I just imagine you. We talked about stabbing or whatever earlier. Yeah, yeah, exactly. So I'm just kind of connected and that's it. Yeah, yeah. But I'm serious though, that these kinds of weird associations now I'm building a richer network. I mean, one of the things that I find is if I've like, you can have somebody's name that's just totally generic, like John Smith or something, not that no offense to people that name, but you know, if I, if I see a generic name like that, but I've read John Smith's papers
Starting point is 00:50:46 academically and then I meet John Smith at a conference, I can immediately associate that name with that face because I have this pre-existing network to lock everything into. And so you can build that network. And that's what the method of loci or the memory palace technique is all about, is you have a pre-existing structure in your head of your childhood home or this memory palace technique is all about, is you have a pre-existing structure in your head of like your childhood home or this mental palace that you've created for yourself. And so now you can put arbitrary pieces of information in different locations in that mental structure of yours, and then you could walk through the different path and find all the pieces of information you're looking for.
Starting point is 00:51:25 So the method of loci is a great method for just learning arbitrary things because it allows you to link them together and get that cue that you need to pop in and find everything, right? We should maybe linger on this memory palace thing just to make obvious, because when people were describing to me
Starting point is 00:51:46 a while ago what this is, it seems insane. I just, you literally think of a place like a childhood home or a home that you're really visually familiar with and you literally place in that three dimensional space facts or people or whatever you wanna remember. And you just walk in your mind along that place visually. And you can remember, remind yourself
Starting point is 00:52:18 of the different things. One of the limitations is there is a sequence to it. So it's, I think your brain somehow, you need, you can't just like go upstairs right away or something. You have to like walk along the room. So it's really great for remembering sequences, but it's also not great for remembering like individual facts out of context. So the full context of the tour I think is important. But it's a, it's fascinating how the mind is able to do that. When you ground these pieces of knowledge into something that you remember well
Starting point is 00:52:49 already, especially visually fascinating. And you can just do that for any kind of sequence. And I'm sure she used something like this for the, for Ikea catalog. So, yeah, absolutely. Absolutely. Um, and I think the, the principle here is again, I was telling you this idea that memories can compete with each other, right? Well, I like to use this example, and maybe someday I'll regret this, but I've used it a lot recently, is like imagine if this were my desk, it could be cluttered with a
Starting point is 00:53:17 zillion different things, right? So imagine it's just cluttered with a whole bunch of yellow Post-it notes, and one of them I put my bank password on it, right? Well, it's gonna take me forever to find it. I might, you know, it's just gonna be buried under all these other Post-it notes. But if it's like hot pink, it's gonna stand out and I find it really easily, right? So that's one way in which if things are distinctive,
Starting point is 00:53:38 if you've processed information in a very distinctive way, then you can have a memory that's gonna last. And that's very good for instance for name face associations. If I get something distinctive about you, that it's like that you've got very short hair and maybe I can make the association with Lex Luthor that way or something like that, right? But I get something very specific, that's a great cue. But the other part of it is what if I just organized my notes so that I have my finances in one pile and I have my reminders, my to-do list in one pile and so forth, so I organized them? Well, then I know exactly if I'm going for my banking, my bank password,
Starting point is 00:54:18 I could go to the finance pile, right? So the method of loci works or memory palaces work because they give you a way of organizing. There's a school of thought that says that episodic memory evolved from this kind of knowledge of space and basically this primitive abilities to figure out where you are. And so people explain the method of loci that way. And whether or not the evolutionary argument is true, the method of loci is not at all special. So if you're not a good visualizer, stories are a good one. So a lot of memory athletes will use stories and they'll go like if you're memorizing a deck of cards, they have a little code for the different like the king and the Jack and the Ten and so forth. And they'll make up a story about things that they're doing and that'll work.
Starting point is 00:55:09 Songs are a great one, right? I mean, it's like, I can still remember there's this obscure episode of the TV show, Cheers. They sing a song about Albania that he uses to memorize all these facts about Albania. And I could still sing that song to you. It's just as I saw it on a TV show. So you mentioned space repetition. Do you like this process? Maybe can you explain it? Oh yeah. If I am trying to memorize something, let's say if I have an hour to memorize as many Spanish words as I can, if I just try to do like half an hour and then later in the day,
Starting point is 00:55:43 I do half an hour, I won't retain that information as long as if I do half an hour and then later in the day I do half an hour, I won't retain that information as long as if I do half an hour today and half an hour one week from now. And so doing that extra spacing should help me retain the information better. Now there's an interesting boundary condition, which is it depends on when you need that information. So many of us, for me, I can't remember so much from college and high school because I crammed because I just did everything at the last minute. And sometimes I would literally study in the hallway right before the test. And that was great because what would happen is I just had that information right there. And so actually not spacing can really help you if you need it very quickly, right? But the problem is that you tend
Starting point is 00:56:31 to forget it later on. But on the other hand, if you space things out, you get a benefit for later on retention. And so there's many different explanations. We have a computational model of this. It's currently under revision. But in our computer model, what we say is that maybe a good way of thinking about this is this conversation that you and I are having, it's associated with a particular context, a particular place in time. And so all of these little cues that are in the background, these little guitar sculptures that you have and that big light umbrella thing, right? All these things are part of my memory for what we're talking about, the content. So now later on, you're sitting around and you're at home drinking a beer and you think,
Starting point is 00:57:17 God, what a strange interview that was, right? So now you're trying to remember it, but the context is different. So your current situation doesn't match up with the memory that you pulled up. There's error. There's a mismatch between what you've pulled up and your current context. And so in our model, what you start to do is you start to erase or alter the parts of the memory that are associated with a specific place and time, and you heighten the information about the content. And so if you remember this information in different times in different places, it's more accessible at
Starting point is 00:57:53 different times in different places because it's not overfitted in a AI kind of way of thinking about things. It's not overfitted to one particular context. But that's also why the memories that we call upon the most also feel kind of like they're just things that we read about, almost you don't vividly reimagine them, right? It's like they're just these things that just come to us like facts, right? And it's a little bit different than semantic memory, but it's like basically these events that we have recalled over and over and over again, we keep updating that memory so it's less and less tied to the original experience. But then we have those other ones, which it's like you just get a reminder of that very specific context. You smell something, you hear a song,
Starting point is 00:58:37 you see a place that you haven't been to in a while, and boom, it just comes back to you. And that's the exact opposite of what you get with spacing, right? That's so fascinating. So with space repetition, one of its powers is that you lose attachment to a particular context, but then it loses the intensity of the flavor of the memory. That's interesting.
Starting point is 00:59:01 That's so interesting. Yeah, but at the same time, it becomes stronger in the sense that the content becomes stronger. Yeah. So it's used for learning languages, for learning facts, for learning, for that generic semantic information type of memory. Yeah. And I think this falls into a category we've done other modeling. One of these is published study in PLOS computational biology, where we showed that another way which is I think related to the spacing effect is what's called the testing effect. So the idea is that if you're trying to learn words, let's say in Spanish or something like that, and this doesn't have to
Starting point is 00:59:38 be words, it could be anything, you test yourself on the words and that act of testing yourself helps you retain it better over time than if you just studied it, right? And so from some learning theories anyway, this seems weird. Why would you do better giving yourself this extra error from testing yourself rather than just giving yourself perfect input that's a replica of what it is that you're trying to learn. And I think the reason is that you get better retention from that error, that mismatch that we talked about, right? So what's happening in our model, it's actually conceptually kind of similar to what happens with backprop in AI or neural networks. And so the idea is that you expose, here's the bad connections and here's the good connections. And so we can keep the parts of this cell assembly that
Starting point is 01:00:32 are good for the memory and lose the ones that are not so good. But if you don't stress test the memory, you haven't exposed it to the error fully. And so that's why I think this is a thing that I come back to over and over again is that you will retain information better if you're constantly pushing yourself to your limit, right? If you are feeling like you're coasting, then you're actually not learning. So it's like this always you should always be stress testing the memory system. Yeah, and feel good about it. You know, even though everyone tells me, Oh, my memory is terrible, So you should always be stress testing the memory system. Yeah. And feel good about it. You know,
Starting point is 01:01:07 even though everyone tells me, Oh, my memory is terrible in the moment, they're overconfident about what they'll retain later on. So it's fascinating. And so what happens is when you test yourself, you're like, Oh my God, I thought I knew that, but I don't. And so it can be demoralizing until you get around that and you realize, Hey, this is the way that I learned. knew that, but I don't. And so it can be demoralizing until you get around that and you realize, hey, this is the way that I learned. This is how I learned best. It's like if you're trying to star in a movie
Starting point is 01:01:35 or something like that, you don't just sit around reading the script, you actually act it out and you're gonna botch those lines from time to time, right? You know that there's an interesting moment you probably have experienced this. I remember a good friend of mine, Joe Rogan, I was on his podcast and we were randomly talking about soccer, football.
Starting point is 01:01:57 Somebody I grew up watching, Diego Armando Mardona, one of the greatest soccer players of all time. And we were talking about him and his career and so on, and Joe asked me if he's still around. Now, and I said, yeah. I don't know why I thought yeah, because that was a perfect example of memories.
Starting point is 01:02:23 He passed away. I tweeted about it, how heartbroken I was, all this kind of stuff, like a year before. I know this, but in my mind, I went back to the thing I've done many times in my head, visualizing some of the epic runs he had on goal and so on. So for me, he's alive. So I'm part of the, also the conversation
Starting point is 01:02:45 when you're talking to Joey, there's stress and like the focus is allocated, the attention is allocated in a particular way. But when I walked away, I was like, in which world was Diego Mardona still alive? Like in which, cause I was sure in my head that he was still alive. It was a, it's a moment that sticks with me.
Starting point is 01:03:06 I've had a few like that in my life, where it just kinda, like obvious things just disappear from mind. And it's cool, like it shows actually the power of the mind in a positive sense to erase memories you want erased, maybe. But I don't know, I don't know if there's a good explanation for that. One of the cool things that that I found is is that some people really just revolutionize a field by creating a problem that didn't
Starting point is 01:03:36 exist before. It's kind of like why I love science is like I engineering is like solving other people's problems and science is about creating problems I'm just much more like I wanted to break things and science is about creating problems. I'm just much more like I would break things and create problems. Not necessarily move fast though. But one of my former mentors, Marsha Johnson, who in my opinion is one of the greatest memory researchers of all time, she comes up, young woman in the field and it's mostly guy field, and she gets into this idea of how do we tell the difference between things that we've imagined and things that we actually remember? How do we tell,
Starting point is 01:04:10 I get some mental experience, where did that mental experience come from, right? And it turns out this is a huge problem because essentially our mental experience of remembering something that happened, our mental experience of thinking about something, how do you tell the difference? They're both largely constructions in our head. And so it is very important. And the way that you do it is, I mean, it's not perfect, but the way that we often do it and succeed is by, again, using our prefrontal cortex and really focusing on the sensory information or the place and time and the things that put us back into when this information happened. And if it's something you thought about, you're not going to have all of that vivid detail as you do for something that
Starting point is 01:04:57 actually happened. But it doesn't work all the time, but that's a big thing that you have to do. But it takes time, it's slow, and it's again, effortful, but that's a big thing that you have to do. But it takes time, it's slow, and it's again effortful, but that's what you need to remember accurately. But what's cool, and I think this is what you alluded to about how that was an interesting experience, is imagination is exactly the opposite. Imagination is basically saying, I'm just going to take all this information from memory, recombine it in different ways, and throw it out there. And so for instance, Dan Schachter and Donna Addis did cool work on this. Demis Hassabis did work on this with Eleanor McGuire in UCL. And this goes back actually to this guy, Frederick Bartlett,
Starting point is 01:05:38 who is this revolutionary memory researcher. Bartlett, he actually rejected the whole idea of quantifying memory. He said, there's no statistics in my book. He came from this anthropology perspective. And a short version of the stories, he just asked people to recall things. He would give people stories and poems, ask people to recall them. And what he found was people's memories didn't reflect all of the details of what they were exposed to, and they did reflect a lot more. They were filtered through this lens of prior knowledge, the cultures that they came from, the beliefs that they had, the things they knew. And so what he concluded was that he called
Starting point is 01:06:17 remembering an imaginative construction, meaning that we don't replay the past. We imagine how the past could have been by taking bits and pieces that come up in our heads. And likewise, he wrote this beautiful paper on imagination saying when we imagine something and create something, we're creating it from these specific experiences that we've had and combining it with our general knowledge. But instead of trying to focus it on being accurate and getting out one thing,
Starting point is 01:06:44 you're just ruthlessly recombining things without any necessary goal in mind. Or at least that's one kind of creation. So imagination is fundamentally coupled with memory in both directions. I think so. It's not clear that it is in everyone, but one of the things that's been studied is some patients who have amnesia, for instance, they have brain damage, say to the hippocampus. And if you ask them to imagine things that are not in front of them, like imagine what could happen after I leave this room, right? They find it very difficult to give you a scenario of what could happen, or if this room, right? They find it very difficult to give you a
Starting point is 01:07:26 scenario of what could happen, or if they do, it would be more stereotyped like, yes, this would happen. But it's not like they can come up with anything that's very vivid and creative in that sense. And it's partly because when you have amnesia, you're stuck in the present because to get a very good model of the future, it really helps to have episodic memories to draw upon. So that's the basic idea. In fact, one of the most impressive things, when people started to scan people's brains and ask people to remember past events,
Starting point is 01:07:58 what they found was there was this big network of the brain called the default mode network. It gets a lot of press because it's thought to be important. It's engaged during mind wandering. And if I ask you to pay attention to something, it only comes on when you stop paying attention. So people say, oh, it's just this kind of daydreaming network. And I thought, this is just ridiculous research, who cares? But then what people found was when people recall episodic memories, this network gets active. And so we started to look into it and this network of areas is really closely functionally interacting with the hippocampus. And so in fact, some would say the hippocampus is part of this
Starting point is 01:08:39 default network. And if you look at brain images of people or brain maps of activation, so to speak, of people imagining possible scenarios of things that could happen in the future, even things that couldn't really be very plausible, they look very similar. I mean, you know, to the naked eye, they look almost the same as maps of brain activation when people remember the past. According to our theory, and we've got some data to support this, we've broken up this network in various sub-pieces, is that basically it's kind of taking apart all of our experiences and creating these little Lego blocks out of them. And then you can put them back together if you have the right instructions to recreate these experiences that you've had,
Starting point is 01:09:21 but you could also reassemble them into new pieces to create a model of an event that hasn't happened yet. And that's what we think happens. And when our common ground that we're establishing in language requires using those building blocks to put together a model of what's going on. Well, there's a good percentage of time I personally live in the imagined world. I think of, I do thought experiments a lot. I take the absurdity of human life as it stands and play it forward in all kinds of different directions. Sometimes it's rigorous thoughts, thought experiments, sometimes it's fun ones. So I imagine that that has an effect on how I remember things.
Starting point is 01:10:09 And I suppose I have to be a little bit careful to make sure stuff happened versus stuff that I just imagined happened. And this also, I mean, some of my best friends are characters inside books that never even existed. And there's some degree to which they actually exist in my mind. Like these characters exist, authors exist,
Starting point is 01:10:33 Dostoevsky exists, but also Brothers Karamazov. I love that book. Yeah. It's one of the few books I've read. One of the few literature books that I've read, I should say. I read a lot in school that I don't remember, but Brothers Karamazov.
Starting point is 01:10:47 Al Biosha. They exist and I have almost kind of like conversations with them. It's interesting. It's interesting to allow your brain to kind of play with ideas of the past, of the imagined, and see it all as one. Yeah, there was actually this famous mnemonist.
Starting point is 01:11:04 He's kind of like back then the equivalent of a memory athlete, except he would go to shows and do this, that was described by this really famous neuropsychologist from Russia named Luria. And so this guy was named Solomon Cherashevsky, and he had this condition called synesthesia that basically created these weird associations between different senses that normally wouldn't go together. So that gave him this incredibly vivid imagination that he would use to basically imagine all sorts of things that he would need to memorize, and he would just imagine, like just create these incredibly detailed things in his head that allowed him to memorize all
Starting point is 01:11:45 sorts of stuff. But it also really haunted him by some reports that basically it was like he was at some point, you know, and again, who knows the drinking was part of this, but at some point had trouble differentiating his imagination from reality, right? And this is interesting because it's like, I mean, that's what psychosis is in some ways is you, first of all, you're just learning connections from prediction errors that you probably shouldn't learn. And the other part of it is that your internal signals are being confused with actual things in the outside world, right? Well, that's why a lot of this stuff is both feature and bug. It's a double-edged sword.
Starting point is 01:12:28 Yeah, I mean, it might be why there's such an interesting relationship between genius and psychosis. Yeah, maybe they're just two sides of the same coin. Humans are fascinating, aren't they? I think so. Sometimes scary, but mostly fascinating. Can we just talk about memory sport a little longer? There's something called the USA memory championship
Starting point is 01:12:51 Like what are these athletes like? What does it mean to be like elite level at this? Have you interact with any of them or reading about them? What have you learned about these folks? There's a guy named Henry Rodger who's studying these guys. And there's actually a book by Joshua Foer called Moonwalking with Einstein where he talks about he actually as part of this book just decided to become a memory athlete. They often have these life events that make them go, hey, why don't I do this? So there was a guy named Scott Hagwood who I write about who thought that he was getting chemo for cancer. And so he decided because there's a well-known thing called chemo brain where people become like they just lose a lot of their sharpness. And so he wanted to fight that
Starting point is 01:13:41 by learning these memory skills. So he bought a book and this is the story you hear in a lot of memory athletes is they buy a book by other memory athletes or other memory experts, so to speak. And they just learn those skills and practice them over and over again. And they start by winning bets and so forth. And then they go into these competitions and the competitions are typically things like memorizing long strings of numbers or memorizing orders of cards and so forth. So there tend to be pretty arbitrary things, not like things that
Starting point is 01:14:12 would be able you'd be able to bring a lot of prior knowledge. But they build the skills that you need to memorize arbitrary things. Yeah, that's fascinating. I've gotten a chance to work with something called end-back tasks. So there's all these kinds of tasks. Memory recall tasks that are used to kind of load up the quote unquote working memory. Yeah, yeah. And to see, psychologists use it to test all kinds of stuff,
Starting point is 01:14:37 like to see how well you're good at multitasking. We use it in particular for the task of driving. Like if you fill up your brain with intensive working memory tasks, how good are you at also not crashing? That kind of stuff. So it's fascinating, but again, those tasks are arbitrary and they're usually
Starting point is 01:15:00 about recalling a sequence of numbers in some kind of semi-complex way. Are you, do you have any favorite tasks of this nature in your own studies? I've really been most excited about going in the opposite direction and using things that are more and more naturalistic. And the reason is, is that we've really moved,
Starting point is 01:15:21 we've moved in that direction because what we found is, is that memory works very, very differently when you study memory in the way that people typically remember. And so it goes into a much more predictive mode and you have these event boundaries for instance. But a lot of what happens is this kind of fascinating mix that we've been talking about, a mix of interpretations and imagination with perception. And the new direction we're going in is understanding navigation in our memory first places. And the reason is that there's a lot of work that's done in rats, which is very good work.
Starting point is 01:16:05 They have a rat and they put it in a box and the rat goes, chases cheese in a box. You'll find cells in the hippocampus that fire when a rat is in different places in the box. And so the conventional wisdom is that the hippocampus forms this map of the box. And I think that probably may happen when you have absolutely no knowledge of the world, right? But I think one of the cool things about human memory is we can bring to bear our past experiences to economically learn new ones. And so for instance, if you learn a map of an Ikea, let's say if I go to the Ikea in Austin, I'm sure there's one here, I probably could go to this Ikea and find my way to where the
Starting point is 01:16:53 wine glasses are without having to even think about it because it's got a very similar layout. Even though Ikea is a nightmare to get around, once I learn my local Ikea, I can use that map everywhere. Why form a brand new one for new plays? And so that kind of ability to reuse information really comes into play when we look at things that are more naturalistic tasks. And another thing that we're really interested in is this idea of what if instead of basically mapping out every coordinate in a space, you form a pretty economical graph that connects basically the major landmarks together and being able to use that as, you know, emphasizing the things that are most important, the places that you go for food
Starting point is 01:17:41 and the places that are landmarks that help you get around. And then filling in the blanks for the rest because I really believe that cognitive maps or mental maps of the world, just like our memories for events are not photographic, I think there's this combination of actual verifiable details and then a lot of inference that you make.
Starting point is 01:18:04 So what have you learned about this kind of spatial mapping of places? How do people represent locations? There's a lot of variability, I think that, and there's a lot of disagreement about how people represent locations. In a world of GPS and physical maps, people can learn it from like basically what they call a survey
Starting point is 01:18:26 perspective, being able to see everything. And so that's one way in which humans can do it that's a little bit different. There's one way which we can memorize routes. I know how to get from here to, let's say if I walk here from my hotel, I can just rigidly follow that route back. And there's another more integrative way, which would be what's called a cognitive map, which would be kind of a sense of how everything relates to each other. And so there's lots of people who believe that these maps that we have in our head are isomorphic with the world. They're like these literal coordinates that follow
Starting point is 01:19:05 Euclidean space. And as you know, Euclidean mathematics is very constrained, right? And I think that we are actually much more generative in our maps of space so that we do have these bits and pieces. And we've got a small task. It's right now, not yet. we need to do some work on it for further analyses, but one of the things we're looking at is these signals called ripples in the hippocampus, which are these bursts of activity that you see that are synchronized with areas in the neocortex, in the default network actually. And so what we find is that those ripples seem to increase at navigationally important points when you're making a decision or when you reach a goal.
Starting point is 01:19:49 This speaks to the emotion thing, right? Because if you have limited choices, if I'm walking down a street, I could really just get a mental map of the neighborhood with a more minimal kind of thing by just saying, here's the intersections and here's the directions I take to get in between them and what we found in general in our MRI studies is basically the more people can reduce the problem whether it's space or any kind of decision-making problem the less the hippocampus encodes it really is very economical towards the points of highest information content and value. So can you describe the encoding in the hippocampus and the ripples you were talking about?
Starting point is 01:20:35 What's the signal in which we see the ripples? Yeah, so this is really interesting. There are these oscillations, right? So there's these waves that you basically see. And these waves are points of very high excitability and low excitability. And they happen actually during slow wave sleep too, so the deepest stages of sleep when you're just zonked out, right? You see these very slow waves where it's like very excitable and then very unexciting. It goes up and down. And on top of them you'll see these little sharp wave ripples. And when there's a ripple in the hippocampus,
Starting point is 01:21:10 you tend to see a sequence of cells that resemble a sequence of cells that fire when an animal is actually doing something in the world. So it almost is like a little people call it replay. I don't like that term, but it's basically a little bit of a compressed play of the sequence of activity in the brain that was taking place earlier. And during those moments, there's a little window of communication between the hippocampus and these areas in the neocortex. And so that I think helps you form new memories, but it also helps you I think stabilize them, but also really connect different things together in memory and allows you to build bridges
Starting point is 01:21:55 between different events that you've had. And so this is one of our least our theories of sleep and its real role in helping you see the connections between different events that you've experienced. So during sleep is when the connections are formed. The connections between different events. Yeah. Right. So it's like you see me now, you see me next week, you see me a month later. You start to build a little internal model of how I behave and you know what to expect of me. And we think sleep, one of the things that allows you to do
Starting point is 01:22:27 is figure out those connections and connect the dots and find the signal and the noise. So you mentioned fMRI. What is it and how is it used in studying memory? This is actually the reason why I got into this whole field of science. When I was in grad school, fMRI was just really taking off as a technique for studying brain activity. And what's beautiful about it is you can study the whole human brain, and there's lots of limits to it,
Starting point is 01:22:57 but you can basically do it in a person without sticking anything into their brains. And very non-invasive. I mean for me being an MRI scanner is like being in the womb. I just fall asleep. If I'm not being asked to do anything, I get very sleepy, you know. But you can have people watch movies while they're being scanned or you can have them do tests of memory like giving them words and so forth to memorize. But what MRI is itself is just this technique where you put people in a very high magnetic field. Typical ones we would use would be three Tesla to give you an idea. So with three Tesla magnet you put somebody in and what happens is you get this very weak but measurable magnetization
Starting point is 01:23:42 in the brain and then you apply a radio frequency pulse, which is basically a different electromagnetic field. And so you're basically using water, the water molecules in the brain as a tracer, so to speak. And part of it in fMRI is the fact that these magnetic fields that you mess with by manipulating these radio frequency pulses and the static field, and you have things called gradients which change the strength of the magnetic field in different
Starting point is 01:24:12 parts of the head. So we tweak them in different ways, but the basic idea that we use in fMRI is that blood is flowing to the brain, and when you have blood that doesn't have oxygen on it, it's a little bit more magnetizable than blood that does because you have hemoglobin that carries the oxygen, the iron basically in the blood that makes it red. And so that hemoglobin when it's deoxygenated actually has different magnetic field properties than when it has oxygen. And it turns out when you have an increase in local activity in some part of the brain, the blood flows there and as a result, you get a lower concentration of hemoglobin that is not oxygenated and then that gives you more signal. So I think I sent you a GIF,
Starting point is 01:25:06 as you like to say. Yeah, we had off-record intense argument about if it's pronounced GIF or GIF, but we shall set that aside as friends. We could have called it a stern rebuke, perhaps. Rebuke, yeah. I drew a hard line. It is true the creator of GIFs that is pronounced GIF,
Starting point is 01:25:27 but that's the only person that pronounces GIF. Anyway, yes, you sent a GIF of... This would be basically a whole, a movie of fMRI data. And so when you look at it, it's not very impressive. It looks like these like very pixelated maps of the brain, but it's mostly kind impressive. It looks like these like very pixelated maps of the brain, but it's mostly kind of like white. But these tiny changes in the intensity of those signals that you probably wouldn't be able to visually perceive, like about 1% can be statistically very, very large effects
Starting point is 01:25:57 for us. And that allows us to see, hey, there's an increase in activity in some part of the brain when I'm doing some task like trying to remember something. And I can use those changes to even predict is a person going to remember this later or not. And the coolest thing that people have done is to decode what people are remembering from the patterns of activity from because maybe when I'm remembering this thing like I'm remembering the house where I grew up I might have one pixel that's bright in the the patterns of activity from because maybe when I'm remembering this thing, like I'm remembering the house where I grew up, I might have one pixel that's bright in the hippocampus and one that's dark. And if I'm remembering, you know, something like more like the car that I used to drive when
Starting point is 01:26:37 I was 16, I might see the opposite pattern where a different pixel is bright. And so all that little stuff that we used to think of noise, we can now think of almost like a QR code for memory, so to speak, where different memories have a different little pattern of bright pixels and dark pixels. And so this really revolutionized my research. So there's fancy research out there where people really, I mean, not even that, I mean, by your standards, it would be Stone Age, but you know, applying machine learning techniques to do decoding and so forth. And now there's a lot of forward encoding models and you can go to town with this stuff, right? And I'm much more old-school designing experiments where you basically say, okay, here's a whole web of memories that overlap
Starting point is 01:27:21 in some way, shape, or form. Do memories that occurred in the same place have a similar QR code and do memories that occurred in different places have different QR code? And you can just use things like correlation coefficients or cosine distance to measure that stuff, right? Super simple, right? And so what happens is you can start to get a whole state space of how our brain area is indexing all these different memories. And it's super fascinating because what we could see is this little like separation between how certain brain areas are processing memory for who was there and other brain areas are
Starting point is 01:27:58 processing information about where it occurred or the situation that's kind of unfolding. And some are giving you information about what are my goals that are involved and so forth. And so, and the hippocampus is just putting it all together into these unique things that just are about when and where it happened. So there is a separation between spatial information, concepts, like literally there's distinct, as you said, QR codes for these?
Starting point is 01:28:28 So to speak. Let me try a different analogy too that might be more accessible for people, which should be like, you've got a folder on your computer, right? And you open it up, there's a bunch of files there. I can sort those files by alphabetical order. And now things that both start with letter A are lumped together, and things that start with Z versus A are far apart, right? And so that is one way of organizing the folder, but I could do it by date. And if I do it by date, things that were created close
Starting point is 01:28:58 together in time are close, and things that are far apart in time are far. So you can think of how a brain area or a network of areas contributes to memory by looking at what the sorting scheme is. And these QR codes that we're talking about that you get from fMRI allow you to do that. And you can do the same thing if you're recording from massive populations of neurons in an animal, and you can do it for recording local potentials in the brain, you know, so little waves of activity in, let's say, a human who has epilepsy and they stick electrodes in their brain to try to find the seizures. So that's some of the work that we're doing now. But all these techniques basically allow you to say, hey, what's the sorting scheme?
Starting point is 01:29:45 And so we've found that some networks of the brain sort information in memory according to who was there. So I might have like, we've actually shown in one of my favorite studies of all time that was done by a former postdoc, Zach Ray, and Zach did the study where we had a bunch of movies with different people in my labs. There are two different people and you filmed them at two different cafes and two different supermarkets. And what you could show is in one particular network, you could find the same kind of pattern of activity more or less, a very similar pattern of activity every time I saw Alex in one of these movies, no matter where he was, right?
Starting point is 01:30:25 And I could see another one that was like a common pattern that happened every time I saw this particular supermarket nugget, you know? And so, and it didn't matter whether you're watching a movie or whether you're recalling the movie. It was the same kind of pattern that comes up, right? It's so fascinating. It's fascinating.
Starting point is 01:30:44 And so now you have those building blocks for assembling a model of what's happening in the present, imagining what could happen, and remembering things very economically from putting together all these pieces, so that all the hippocampus has to do is get the right kind of blueprint for how to put together all these building blocks.
Starting point is 01:31:03 These are all like beautiful hints at a super interesting system that makes me wonder on the other side of it how to build it. But it's like, it's fascinating. Like the way it does the encoding is really, really fascinating. Or I guess the symptoms, the results of that encoding
Starting point is 01:31:21 are fascinating to study from this. Just as a small tangent, you mentioned sort of the measuring local potentials with electrodes versus FMRI. What are some interesting limitations, possibilities of FMRI? Maybe, the way you explained it is brilliant with blood and detecting the activations or the excitation because blood flows to that
Starting point is 01:31:48 area. What's the latency of that? What's the blood dynamics in the brain? How quickly can the task change and all that kind of stuff? Yeah, I mean, it's very slow to the brain. 50 milliseconds is like, you know, like it's an eternity. Maybe you have 50, you know, maybe like, you know, let's say half a second, 500 milliseconds, just so much back and forth stuff happens in the brain in that time, right? So in fMRI, you can measure these magnetic field responses about six seconds after that burst of activity would take place. All these things, it's like, is it a feature or is it a bug, right? So one of the interesting things that's been discovered about fMRI is it's not so tightly related to the spiking of the neurons. So we tend to think of the computation, so to speak, as being driven by spikes, meaning like there's just a burst of it's either on or it's off and the neurons
Starting point is 01:32:50 like going up or down. But sometimes what you can have is these states where the neuron becomes a little bit more excitable or less excitable. And so fMRI is very sensitive to those changes in excitability. Actually, one of the fascinating things about fMRI is how is it we go from neural activity to essentially blood flow to oxygen, all this stuff. It's such a long chain of going from neural activity to magnetic fields. And one of the theories that's out there is most of the cells in the brain are not neurons, they're actually these support cells called glial cells. And one big one is astrocytes, and they play this big role in regulating being a middle man, so to speak, with the neurons. So if you, for instance, like one neuron's talking to another, you release a neurotransmitter, like let's say glutamate, and that gets another neuron, starts getting active after you release
Starting point is 01:33:51 in the gap between the two neurons called synapse. So what's interesting is if you leave that, imagine you just flooded with this like liquid in there, right? If you leave it in there too long, you just excite the other neuron too much and you can start to basically get seizure activity. You don't want this. So you gotta suck it up. And so actually what happens is these astrocytes, one of their functions is to suck up the glutamate
Starting point is 01:34:16 from the synapse. And that is a massively, and then break it down and then feed it back into the neurons so that you can reuse it. But that cycling is actually very energy intensive. And what's interesting is, at least according to one theory, they need to work so quickly that they're working on metabolizing the glucose that comes in without using oxygen, kind of like anaerobic metabolism. so they're not using oxygen as fast as they are using glucose. So what we're really seeing in some ways may be in fMRI,
Starting point is 01:34:52 not the neurons themselves being active, but rather the astrocytes, which are meeting the metabolic demands of the process of keeping the whole system going. It does seem to be that fMRI is a good way to study activation, so with these estrus sites, even though there's a latency, it's pretty reliably coupled to the activations.
Starting point is 01:35:16 Oh, well this gets me to the other part of it. So now let's say for instance, if I'm just kinda like, I'm talking to you but I'm kinda paying attention to your cowboy hat, or I'm thinking about the right, even if I'm not kind of like, I'm talking to you, but I'm kind of paying attention to your cowboy hat, right? So I'm looking off to the, or I'm thinking about the right, even if I'm not looking at it. What you'd see is that there'd be this little elevation in activity in areas in the visual cortex, you know, which process vision around that point in space. Okay. So if then something happened, like, you know, suddenly the light
Starting point is 01:35:45 flashed in that part of, you know, right in front of your cowboy hat, I would have a bigger response to it. But what you see in fMRI is even if I don't see that flash of light, there's a lot of activity that I can measure because you're kind of keeping it excitable and that in and of itself, even though I'm not seeing anything there that's particularly interesting, there's still this increase in activity. And so it's more sensitive with fMRI. So is that a feature or is it a bug? You know, some people who study spikes in neurons and say, well that's terrible, we don't want that, you know. Likewise, it's slow and that's terrible for measuring
Starting point is 01:36:22 things that are very fast. But one of the things that we found in our work was when we give people movies and when we give people stories to listen to, a lot of the action is in the very, very slow stuff. Because if you're thinking about a story, let's say, you're listening to a podcast or you're listening to the Lex Friedman podcast, right? You're putting this stuff together and building this internal model over several seconds, which is basically we filter that out when we look at electrical activity in the brain because we're interested in this millisecond scale. It's almost massive amounts of information, right? So the way I see it is every technique gives you a little limited window into what's going on. FMRi is huge
Starting point is 01:37:06 problems. People lie down in the scanner. There's parts of the brain where you, I'll show you in some of these images, where you'll see kind of gaping holes because you can't keep the magnetic field stable in those spots. You'll see parts where it's like there's a vein and so it just produces big increases and decreases in signal or respiration that causes these changes. There's lots of artifacts and stuff like that. Every technique has its limits. If I'm lying down in an MRI scanner, I'm lying down. I'm not interacting with you in the same way
Starting point is 01:37:38 that I would in the real world. But at the same time, I'm getting data that I might not be able to get otherwise. And so different techniques give you different kinds of advantages. What kind of big scientific discoveries, maybe the flavor of discoveries have been done throughout the history of the science of memory,
Starting point is 01:37:57 the studying of memory, what kind of things have been understood? Oh, there's so many. It's really so hard to summarize it. I mean, I think it's funny because it's like, when you're in the field, you can get kind of blase about this stuff. But then once I started to write the book,
Starting point is 01:38:15 I was like, oh my God, this is really interesting. How did we do all this stuff? I would say that some of the, I mean, from the first studies just showing how much we forget is very important. Showing how much schemas, which is our organized knowledge about the world, increase our ability to remember information, just massively increase it. A studies of expertise showing how experts like chess experts can memorize so much in such a short amount of time because of the schemas they have for chess. But then also showing that those
Starting point is 01:38:52 lead to all sorts of distortions in memory. The discovery that the act of remembering can change the memory, can strengthen it, but it can also distort it if you get misinformation at the time. strengthen it, but it can also distort it if you get misinformation at the time. And it can also strengthen or weaken other memories that you didn't even recall. So just this whole idea of memory as an ecosystem, I think, was a big discovery. This idea of breaking up our continuous experience into these discrete events, I think, was a major discovery. So the discreteness of our encoding of events? Maybe, yeah. I mean, you know, and again, there's controversial ideas about this, right? But it's
Starting point is 01:39:31 like, yeah, this idea that, and this gets back to just this common experience of you walk into the kitchen and you're like, why am I here? And you just end up grabbing some food from the fridge, and then you go back and you're like, oh, wait a minute. I left my watch in the kitchen. That's what I was looking for. And so what happens is, is that you have a little internal model of where you are, what you're thinking about. And when you cross from one room to another, those models get updated. And so now when you're in the kitchen, you have to go back and mentally time travel back to this earlier point to
Starting point is 01:40:02 remember what, what it was that you went there for. And so these event boundaries, turns out like in our research, and again I don't want to make it sound like we've figured out everything, but in our research one of the things that we found is that basically as people get older, the activity in the hippocampus at these event boundaries tends to go down. But independent of age, if I give you outside of the scanner, you're done with the scanner, I just scan you while
Starting point is 01:40:31 you're watching a movie, you just watch it, you come out, I give you a test of memory for stories. What happens is you find this incredible correlation between the activity in the hippocampus at these singular points in time, these event boundaries, and your ability to just remember a story outside of the scanner later on. So it's marking this ability to encode memories, just these little snippets of neural activity. So I think that's a big one. There's all sorts of work in animal models that I can get into. Sleep, I think there's so much interesting stuff that's being discovered in sleep right now. Being able to just record from large populations
Starting point is 01:41:14 of cells and then be able to relate that. You know what I think the coolest thing gets back to this QR code thing, because what we can do now is I can take fMRI data while you're watching a movie or let's do better than that. Let me get fMRI data while you use a joystick to move around in virtual reality. You're in the metaverse, whatever, right? But it's kind of a crappy metaverse because there's always so much metaverse that you can do in an MRI scanner. So they do this crappy metaversing. So now I can can take a rat, record from its hippocampus and prefrontal cortex and all these areas with these really new electrodes,
Starting point is 01:41:50 I get massive amounts of data, and have it move around on a track ball in virtual reality in the same metaverse that I did, and record that rat's activity. I can get a person with epilepsy who we have electrodes in their brain anyway to try to figure out where the seizures are coming from. And it was a healthy part of the brain record from that person, right? And I can get a computational model. And one of the brand new members in my lab,
Starting point is 01:42:18 Tyler Bond is just doing some great stuff. He relates computer vision models and looks at the weaknesses of computer vision models and relates at the weaknesses of computer vision models and relates it to what the brain does well. And so you can actually take a ground truth code for the metaverse basically, and you can feed in the visual information, let's say the sensory information or whatever that's coming in to a computational model that's designed to take real-world inputs, right? And you could basically tie them all together by virtue of the state spaces that you're measuring in neural activity in these different formats, these different species, and in the computational model, which is just, I just find that mind-blowing.
Starting point is 01:43:03 You could do different kinds of analyses on language and basically come up with just like the guts of LLMs, right? You could do analyses on language, and you could do analyses on sentiment, analyses of emotions, and so forth. Put all this stuff together. I mean, it's almost too much, but if you do it right and you do it in a theory-driven way, as opposed to just throwing all the data at the wall and see what sticks, I mean, that to me is just exceptionally powerful.
Starting point is 01:43:35 So you can take fMRI data across species and across different types of humans and conditions of humans and what, find, construct models that help you find the commonalities or like the core thing that makes somebody navigate through the metaverse, for example? Yeah, yeah, I mean, more or less. I mean, there's a lot of details, but yes, I think,
Starting point is 01:44:00 and not just fMRI, but you can relate it to, like I said, recordings from large populations of neurons that could be taken in a human or even in a non-human animal that is, you know, where you think it's an anatomical homolog. So that's just mind-blowing to me. CB What's the similarities in humans and mice? RL That's what, smashing pumpkins were all just wrath in a cage what it's smashing pumpkins. We're all just wrath in a cage. Is that smashing pumpkins?
Starting point is 01:44:28 Despite all of your rage. Is that smashing pumpkins? I think. Despite all of your rage at GIFs, you're still just rat in a cage. Oh yeah. All right, good callback. Anyway.
Starting point is 01:44:39 Good callback. See these memory retrieval exercises I'm doing are actually helping you build a lasting memory of this conversation. And it's strengthening the visual thing I have of you with James Brown on stage. It's just becoming stronger and stronger by the second. But anyway, but animal studies work here as well.
Starting point is 01:45:00 Yeah. Yeah. So, okay. So let's go to the, um, so I think Reese, I've got great colleagues who I talk to who study memory in mice. And one of the valuable things in those models is you can study neural circuits in an enormously targeted way because you could do these genetic studies, for instance, where you can manipulate particular groups of neurons. And it's just getting more and more targeted to the point where you can actually turn on a particular kind of memory just by activating a particular set of neurons that was active during an experience, right?
Starting point is 01:45:38 So there's a lot of conservation of some of these neural circuits across evolution in mammals, for instance. And then some people would even say that there's genetic mechanisms for learning that are conserved even going back far, far before. But let's go back to the mice and humans question. There's a lot of differences. So for one thing, the sensory information is very different. a lot of differences. So for one thing, the sensory information is very different. Mice and rats explore the world largely through smelling, olfaction, but they also have vision that's kind of designed to kind of catch death from above. So it's like a very big view of the world. And we move our eyes around in a way that focuses on particular spots in space where you get very high resolution from a very limited set of spots in space. So that focuses on particular spots in space where you get very high resolution
Starting point is 01:46:25 from a very limited set of spots in space. So that makes us very different in that way. We also have all these other structures as social animals that allow us to respond differently. There's language, there's like, you know, so you name it, there's obviously gobs of differences. Humans aren't just giant rats. There's much more complexity to us. Timescales are very important. So primate brains and human brains are especially good at integrating and holding on to information across longer and longer periods of time, right? And also, finally, it's like our history of training data, so to speak, is very, very
Starting point is 01:47:04 different than, I mean, a human's world is very different than a wild mouse's world, and a lab mouse's world is extraordinarily impoverished relative to an adult human. But still, what can you understand by studying mice? I mean, just basic, almost behavioral stuff about memory? Well, yes, but that's very important, right? So you can understand, for instance, how do neurons talk to each other? That's a really big, big question.
Starting point is 01:47:32 Neural computation, you think it's the most simple question, right? Not at all. I mean, it's a big, big question. And understanding how two parts of the brain interact, meaning that it's not just one area speaking, it's not like Twitter, where one area of the brain is shouting and then another area of the brain is just stuck listening to this crap. It's like they're actually interacting on the millisecond scale, right?
Starting point is 01:47:59 How does that happen? And how do you regulate those interactions, these dynamic interactions? We're still figuring that out, but that's going to be coming largely from model systems that are easier to understand. You can do manipulations like drug manipulations to manipulate circuits and use viruses and so forth and lasers to turn on circuits that you just can't do in humans. So I think there's a lot that can be learned from mice, there's a lot that can be learned from non-human primates, and there's a lot that you need to learn from humans. And I think unfortunately some of the people in the
Starting point is 01:48:36 National Institutes of Health think you can learn everything from the mouse. It's like why study memory in humans when I could study learning in a mouse? just like oh my god I'm gonna get my funding from somewhere else Well, let me ask you some random fascinating questions How does deja vu work so deja vu is it's actually one of these things I think that some of the Surveys suggest that like 75% of people report having a deja vu experience one time or another. I don't know where that came from, but I've pulled people in my class and most of them
Starting point is 01:49:12 say they've experienced deja vu. It's this kind of sense that I've experienced this moment sometime before. I've been here before. And actually there's all sorts of variants of this. The French have all sorts of names for various versions of the chamois-vous, parley-vous, I don't know, whatever it's like. All these different vous. But deja vu is this sense that it can be like almost disturbing, intense sense of familiarity. So there is a researcher named Wilder Penfield. Actually, this goes back even earlier to some of
Starting point is 01:49:46 the earliest. Hewlings-Jackson was this neurologist who did a lot of the early characterizations of epilepsy. And one of the things he notices in epilepsy patients, some group of them right before they would get a seizure, they would have this intense sense of deja vu. So it's this artificial sense of familiarity. It's a sense of having a memory that's not there, right? And so what was happening was there was electrical activity in certain parts of these brains. And so this guy Penfield later on, when he was trying to look for how do we map out the brain to figure out which parts we want to remove and which parts don't we. He would stimulate parts of the temporal lobes of the brain and find you could elicit the sense of deja vu. Sometimes you'd actually get a memory that a person would
Starting point is 01:50:35 re-experience just from electrically stimulating some parts. Sometimes they just have this intense feeling of being somewhere before. And so one theory which I really like is that in higher order areas of the brain they're integrating from many, many different sources of input. What happens is that they're tuning themselves up every time you process a similar input, right? And so that allows you to just get this kind of a fluent sense that I'm very familiar, you're very familiar with this place, right? And so just being here, you're not going to be moving your eyes all over the place because you kind of have an idea of where everything is. And that fluency gives you a sense of like I'm here. Now I wake up in my hotel room and I have
Starting point is 01:51:22 this very unfamiliar sense of where I am, right? But there's a great set of studies done by Anne Cleary at Colorado State where she created these virtual reality environments. And we'll go back to the metaverse. Imagine you go through a virtual museum, right? And then she would put people in virtual reality and have them go through a virtual arcade. But the map of the two places was exactly the same. She just put different skins on them, so one looks different than the other. But they've got same landmarks and the same places, same objects and everything, but carpeting, colors, theme, everything's different. People will often not have any conscious idea that the two are the same, but they
Starting point is 01:52:03 could report this very intense sense of deja vu. So it's like a partial match that's eliciting this kind of a sense of familiarity. And that's why in patients who have epilepsy that affects memory, you get this artificial sense of familiarity that happens. And so we think that, and again, this is just one theory amongst many, but we think that we get a little bit of that feeling it's not enough to necessarily give you deja vu, even for very mundane things, right? So it's like if I tell you the word rutabaga, your brain's going to work a little bit harder to catch it than if I give you a word like apple, right? And that's because you hear apple a lot, so your brain's very bit harder to catch it than if I give you a word like apple, right?
Starting point is 01:52:49 That's because you hear apple a lot. So your brain's very tuned up to process it efficiently, but rutabaga takes a little bit longer and more intense. And you can actually see a difference in brain activity in areas in the temporal lobe when you hear a word just based on how frequent it is in the English language. So we think it's tied to this basic, it's basically a byproduct of our mechanism of just learning, doing this error-driven learning as we go through life to become better and better and better to process things more and more efficiently. So I guess deja vu is just thinking extra elevated, the stuff coming together, firing for this artificial artificial memories if it's the real memory This I mean, why does it feel so intense?
Starting point is 01:53:29 Well, it doesn't happen all the time but I think what may be happening is it's such a it's a partial match to something that we have and it's not enough to Trigger that sense of you know that ability to pull together all the pieces But it's a close enough match to give you that intense sense of familiarity without the recollection of exactly what happened when. But it's also like a spatio-temporal familiarity. So like it's also in time.
Starting point is 01:53:58 Like there's a weird blending of time that happens. And we'll probably talk about time because I think that's a really interesting idea, how time relates to memory. But you also kind of, artificial memory brings to mind this idea of false memories that comes in all kinds of contexts, but how do false memories form? Well, I like to say there's no such thing as true
Starting point is 01:54:23 or false memories, right? It's like Johnny Rotten from the Sex Pistols, he had a saying that's like, I don't believe in false memories any more than I believe in false songs, right? And so the basic idea is that we have these memories that reflect bits and pieces of what happened as well as our inferences and theories, right? So I'm a scientist and I collect data, but I use theories to make sense of that data. And so a memory is kind of a mix of all these things. So where memories can go off the deep end and become what we would call conventionally as false memories are sometimes little distortions where we filled in the blanks, the gaps in our memory based on things
Starting point is 01:55:06 that we know, but don't actually correspond to what happened. So if I were to tell you that I'm like a story about this person who's like worried that they have cancer or something like that, and then they see a doctor and the doctor says, well, things are very much like you would have expected or like, you know, what you were afraid of or something. When people remember that, they'll often remember, well, the doctor told the patient that he had cancer, even if that wasn't in the story
Starting point is 01:55:39 because they're infusing meaning into that story, right? So that's a minor distortion. But what happens is that sometimes things can really get out of hand where people have trouble telling the difference in things that they've imagined versus things that happen. But also, as I told you, the act of remembering can change the memory. And so what happens then is you can actually be exposed to some misinformation. And so Elizabeth Loftus was can actually be exposed to some misinformation. And so Elizabeth Loftus was a real pioneer in this work. And there's lots of other work that's been done since.
Starting point is 01:56:11 But basically it's like, if you remember some event and then I tell you something about the event, later on when you remember the event, you might remember some original information from the event, as well as some information about what I told you. And sometimes if you're not able to tell the difference, that information that I told you gets mixed into the story that you had originally. So now I give you some more misinformation or you're exposed to some more information somewhere else. And
Starting point is 01:56:40 eventually your memory becomes totally detached from what happened. And so sometimes you can have cases where people, this is very rare, but you can do it in lab too, where like a significant, not everybody, but you know a chunk of people will fall for this, where you can give people misinformation about an event that never took place, and as they keep trying to remember that event more and more, what happens is they start to imagine, they start to pull up things from other experiences they've had, and eventually they can stitch together a vivid memory of something that never happened because they're not remembering an event that happened, they're remembering the act of trying to remember what happened and basically putting it together
Starting point is 01:57:28 into the wrong story. So it's fascinating because this could probably happen at a collective level. Like this is probably what successful propaganda machines aim to do, is creating false memory across thousands if not millions of minds. Yeah, absolutely. I mean, this is exactly what they do. And so, all these foibles of human memory get magnified when you start to have social interactions. There's a whole literature on something called social contagion, which is basically when misinformation spreads like a virus.
Starting point is 01:58:02 Like you remember the same thing that I did, but I give you a little bit of wrong information, then that becomes part of your story of what happened. Because once you and I share a memory, like I tell you about something I've experienced and you tell me about your experience of the same event, it's no longer your memory or my memory, it's our memory. And so now the misinformation spreads. And the more you trust someone or the more powerful that person is, the more voice they have in shaping that narrative, right? And there's all sorts of interesting ways in which misinformation can happen. There's a great example of when John McCain and George Bush Jr. were in a primary, and there are these polls where they
Starting point is 01:58:49 would do these, I guess they were like not robocalls, but real calls where they would poll voters. But they actually inserted some misinformation about McCain's beliefs on taxation, I think, and maybe it was something about illegitimate children, I don't really remember. But they included misinformation in the question that they asked, like, how do you feel about the fact that he wants to do this or something? And so people would end up becoming convinced he had these policy things or these personal things that were not true, just based on the polls that were being used. So it was a case where interestingly enough the people who were using misinformation were actually ahead of the curve relative to the scientists who were trying to study these effects in memory.
Starting point is 01:59:37 Yeah it's really interesting. So it's not just about truth and falsehoods like us as intelligent reasoning machines, but it's the formation of memories where they become like visceral. You can rewrite history. If you just look throughout the 20th century, some of the dictatorships with Nazi Germany, with the Soviet Union, effective propaganda machines can rewrite our conceptions of history,
Starting point is 02:00:10 how we remember our own culture, our upbringing, all this kind of stuff. You could do quite a lot of damage in this way. And then there's probably some kind of social contagion happening there. Like certain ideas that may be initiated by the propaganda machine can spread faster than others. You can see that in modern day certain conspiracy theories,
Starting point is 02:00:32 there's just something about them that they are really effective at spreading. There's something sexy about them to people, to where something about the human mind eats it up and then uses that to construct memories as if they almost were there to witness whatever the content of the conspiracy theory is. It's fascinating,
Starting point is 02:00:54 because once you feel like you remember a thing, I feel like there's a certainty, there's a, it emboldens you to say stuff. It's not just you believe an idea is true or not, you're like, it's at the core of your being that you feel like you were there to watch the thing happen. Yeah, I mean, there's so much in what you're saying. I mean, one of the things is that people's sense
Starting point is 02:01:22 of collective identity is very much tied to shared memories. If we have a shared narrative of the past, or even better, if we have a shared past, we will feel more socially connected with each other and I will feel part of this group. They're part of my tribe if I remember the same things in the same way. And you brought up this weaponization of history. And you know, it really speaks to, I think, one of the parts of memory, which is that if you have a belief, you will find, and you have a goal in mind, you will find stuff in memory that aligns with it,
Starting point is 02:01:55 and you won't see the parts in memory that don't. So a lot of the stories we put together are based on our perspectives, right? And so let's just zoom out for the moment from like misinformation, take something even more fascinating but not as like scary. I was reading Thanh Viet Nguyen, but he wrote a book about the collective memory of the Vietnam War. He's a Vietnamese immigrant who was flown out after the war was over. And so we went back to his family to get their stories about the war and they
Starting point is 02:02:30 called it the American war, not the Vietnam war. Right. And that just kind of blew my mind to having grown up in the U S and I've always heard about it as a Vietnam war. But of course they call it the American war, cause that's what happened. America came in, right. And that's based on their perspective, which is a very valid perspective. And so that just gives you this idea of the way we put together these narratives based on our perspectives. And I think the opportunities that we can have in memory is if we bring groups together from different perspectives and we allow them to talk to each other and we allow ourselves to listen.
Starting point is 02:03:13 I mean, right now you'll hear a lot of just jammering, people going blah, blah, blah about free speech, but they just wanna listen to themselves, right? It's like, let's face it, the old days before people were supposedly woke, they were trying to ban two live crew or, you know, just think about, Letty Bruce got canceled for cursing, Jesus Christ.
Starting point is 02:03:34 It's like, this is nothing new. People don't like to hear things that disagree with them. But if you're in it, I mean, you can see two situations in groups with memory. One situation is you have people who are very dominant who just take over the conversation. Basically what happens is the group remembers less from the experience and they remember more of what the dominant narrator says. Now if you have a diverse group of people, and I don't mean diverse in necessarily the human resources sense of the word, I mean diverse in any way you want to take it, right? But diverse in every way, hopefully.
Starting point is 02:04:12 And you give everyone a chance to speak and everyone's being appreciated for their unique contribution. You get more accurate memories and you get more information from it, right? Even two people who come from very similar backgrounds, if you can appreciate the unique contributions that each one has, you can do a better job of generating information from memory. And that's a way to inoculate ourselves, I believe, from misinformation in the modern world. But like everything else, it requires a certain tolerance for discomfort. And I think when we don't have much time and I think when we're stressed out and when we are just tired, it's very hard to tolerate discomfort.
Starting point is 02:04:54 And I mean, social media has a lot of opportunity for this because it enables this distributed one on one interaction that you're talking about where everybody has a voice, but still our natural inclination, you see this on social media, is there's a natural clustering of people and opinions and you just kind of form these kind of bubbles. I think that's, to me personally, I think that's a technology problem that could be solved.
Starting point is 02:05:19 If there's a little bit of interaction, kind, respectful, compassionate interaction with people that have a very different memory, that respectful interaction will start to intermix the memories and ways of thinking to where you're slowly moving towards truth. But that's a technology problem because naturally, left to our own devices,
Starting point is 02:05:43 we wanna cluster up in a tribe. Yeah and that's the human problem. I think a lot of the problems that come up with technology aren't the technology itself as much as the fact that people adapt to the technology in maladaptive ways. I mean one of my fears about AI is not what AI will do, but what people will do. I mean, take text messaging, right? It's like it's pain in the ass to text people, at least for me. And so what happens is the communication becomes very Spartan and devoid of meaning. Right. And it's just very telegraphic and that's people adapting to the medium.
Starting point is 02:06:19 Right. I mean, look at you. You've got this keyboard, right? That's like got these like dome shaped things and you've adapted to that to communicate. Right. And that's not the technology look at you. You've got this keyboard, right? That's like got these like dome-shaped things and you've adapted to that to communicate, right? That's not the technology adapting to you as you adapting to the technology And I think with you know, one of the things I learned when Google started to introduce autocomplete in emails I started to use it and about a third of the time I was like this isn't what I want to say Third of the time I'd be like this is exactly what I wanted to say and a third of the time I'd be like, this is exactly what I wanted to say. And a third of the time I was saying, well, this is good enough. I'll just go with it, right? And so what happens is it's not that the technology necessarily is doing anything so bad as much as
Starting point is 02:06:56 it's just going to constrain my language because I'm just doing what's being suggested to me. And so this is why I say, you know, kind of like my mantra for some of what I've learned about everything in memory is to diversify your training data basically, because otherwise you're going to be so like humans have this capability to be so much more creative than anything generative AI will put together, at least right now, who knows where this goes. But it can also go the opposite direction where people could become much, much less creative if they just become more and more resistant to discomfort and resistant to exposing themselves to novelty, to cognitive dissonance and so forth. I think there is a dance between natural human adaptation
Starting point is 02:07:47 of technology and the people that design the engineering of that technology. So I think there's a lot of opportunity to create like this keyboard, things that on that are a positive for human behavior. So we adapt and all this kind of stuff, but when you look at the long arc of history across years and decades has humanity
Starting point is 02:08:09 Been flourishing our humans creating more awesome stuff our humans happier all that kind of stuff. So there I think technology on that is Has been and I think maybe hope will always be on that positive thing. Do you think people are happier now than they were 50 years ago or 100 years ago? Yes. Yes. I don't know about that. I think humans in general like to reminisce about the past,
Starting point is 02:08:38 like the times are better. That's true. And complain about the weather today or complain about whatever today because there's this kind of complainy engine that just, there's so much pleasure in saying, you know, life sucks for some reason. And-
Starting point is 02:08:55 That's why I love punk rock. Exactly, I mean, there's something in humans that loves complaining, even about trivial things, but complaining about change, complaining about everything. But ultimately I think on net, on every measure, things are getting better. Life is getting better.
Starting point is 02:09:16 Oh, life is getting better, but I don't know necessarily that attracts people's happiness, right? I mean, I would argue that maybe, who knows? I don't know this, but I wouldn't be surprised if people in hunter-gatherer societies are happier. I mean, I wouldn't be surprised if they're happier than people who have access to modern medicine and email and cell phones.
Starting point is 02:09:38 Well, I don't think there's a question whether you take hunter-gatherer folks and put them into modern day and give them enough time to adapt, they would be much happier. The question is, in terms of every single problem take hunter-gatherer folks and put them into modern day and give them enough time to adapt, they would be much happier. The question is, in terms of every single problem they've had is now solved.
Starting point is 02:09:51 There's now food, there's guaranteed survival shelter and all this kind of stuff. So what you're asking is a deeper sort of biological question, do we want to be, oh, Warner Herzog and the movie Happy People, Life and the Taiga. Do we want to be busy 100% of our time hunting, gathering, surviving, worried about the next day? Maybe that constant struggle ultimately creates
Starting point is 02:10:16 a more fulfilling life? I don't know, but I do know this modern society allows us to, when we're sick, to find medicine, to find cures, when we're hungry to get food, much more than we did even 100 years ago, and there's many more activities that you could perform while creative,
Starting point is 02:10:39 all this kind of stuff that enables the flourishing of humans at the individual level. Whether that leads to happiness, I mean, that's a very deep philosophical question. Maybe struggle, deep struggles necessary for happiness. CB Or maybe cultural connection. Maybe it's about functioning in social groups that are meaningful and having time. But I do think there's an interesting memory-related thing, which is that if you look at things like reinforcement learning, for instance, you're not learning necessarily every time you get a reward. If it's the same
Starting point is 02:11:18 reward, you're not learning that much. You mainly learn if it deviates from your expectation of what you're supposed to get, right? So it's like you get a paycheck every month from MIT or whatever, right? And it's like, you probably don't even get excited about it when you get the paycheck. But if they cut your salary, you're going to be pissed. And if they increase your salary that basically you learn to expect these things, I think, is a major source of, I guess, it's a major way in which we're kind of more, in my opinion, wired to strive and not be happy to be in a state of wanting. And if, you know, some people talk about dopamine, for instance, being this pleasure chemical. And it's like, there's a lot of compelling research to suggest it's not about pleasure at all. It's about the discomfort that energizes you to get things,
Starting point is 02:12:17 to seek a reward, right? And so you could give an animal that's been deprived of dopamine a reward and enjoy it. It's pretty good, but they're not going to do anything to get it. And just one of the weird things in our research is I got into curiosity from a postdoc in my lab, Matthias Gruber. And one of the things that we found is when we gave people a question, like a trivia question that they wanted the answer to, the more curious people were about the answer, the more activity in these dopamine-related circuits in the brain we would see. And again, that was not driven by the answer per se, but by the question.
Starting point is 02:13:00 So it was not about getting the information, it was about the drive to seek the information. But it depends on how you take that. If you get this uncomfortable gap between what you know and what you want to know, you could either use that to motivate you and energize you, or you could use it to say, I don't want to hear about this, this disagrees with my beliefs, I'm going to go back to my echo chamber, you know? I like what you said that maybe we're designed to be in a kind of constant state of wanting, which by the way is a pretty good either band name
Starting point is 02:13:36 or rock song name, state of wanting. That's like a hardcore band name. Yeah, yeah, yeah. Yeah, it's pretty good. I also like the hedonic treadmill. Hedonic treadmill is pretty good. Yeah. Yeah. We could use that for like our techno project, I think. You mean the one we're starting? Yeah, exactly. Okay, great. We're going on tour soon. This is our announcement.
Starting point is 02:14:02 We could build a false memory of a show in fact, if you want, let's just put it all together. So we don't even have to do all the work to play the show. We can just create a memory of it and might as well happen because the remembering self is in charge anyway. So let me ask you about, we talked about false memories, but in a legal system, false confessions, I remember reading 1984 where,
Starting point is 02:14:24 sorry for the dark turn of our conversation, but through torture you can make people say anything and essentially remember anything. I wonder to which degree there's like truth to that if you look at the torture that happened in the Soviet Union for confessions, all that kind of stuff. How much can you really get people to really, yeah, to force false memories, I guess? Yeah, I mean, I think there's a lot of history of this actually in the criminal justice system. You might've heard the term the third degree.
Starting point is 02:15:01 If you actually look it up, historically it was a very intense set of beatings and you know starvation and physical demands that they would place at people to get them to talk. And you know there's certainly a lot of work in the that's been done by the CIA in terms of enhanced interrogation techniques. And from what I understand, the research actually shows that they just produce what people want to hear, not necessarily the information that is being looked for. And the reason is that, I mean, there's different reasons. I mean, one is people just get tired of being tortured and just say whatever. But another part of it is that you create a very interesting set of conditions
Starting point is 02:15:47 where there's an authority figure telling you something that you did this, we know you did this, we have witnesses saying you did this. So now you start to question yourself. Then they put you under stress. Maybe they're not feeding you, maybe they're kind of like making you be cold or not feeding you. Maybe they're kind of like making you be cold or exposing you to music that you can't stand or something, whatever it is, right? It's like they're creating this physical stress. And so stress starts to down-regulate the prefrontal cortex. You're not necessarily as good at monitoring the accuracy of stuff. Then they start to get nice to you and they say, imagine, okay, I know you don't remember this, but maybe we can walk you through how it could have happened and they feed you the information. And so you're in this weakened mental state and
Starting point is 02:16:35 you're being encouraged to imagine things by people who give you a plausible scenario. And at some point, certain people can be very coaxed into creating a memory for something that ever happened. And there's actually some pretty convincing cases out there where you don't know exactly the truth. There's a sheriff, for instance, who came to believe that he had a false memory, I mean, that he had a memory of doing sexual abuse based on, you know, essentially, I think it was, you know, I'm not going to tell the story because I don't remember it well enough to necessarily accurately give it to you, but people could look this stuff up. There are definitely stories out there like this where people confess to crimes that they just didn't do and objective evidence came out later
Starting point is 02:17:20 on. But there's a basic recipe for it, which is you feed people the information that you want them to remember. You stress them out. You have an authority figure kind of like pushing this information on them, or you motivate them to produce the information you're looking for, and that pretty much over time gives you what you want. It's really tragic that centralized power can use these kinds of tools to destroy lives. Sad. Since there's a theme about music throughout this conversation One of the best topics for songs is heartbreak love in general but heartbreak Why and how do we remember and forget heartbreak asking for a friend? Oh god, that's so hard to asking for a friend I love that
Starting point is 02:18:22 It's such a hard one well, so mean, part of this is we tend to go back to particular times that are the more emotionally intense periods. And so that's a part of it. And again, memory is designed to kind of capture these things that are biologically significant. And attachment is a big part of biological significance for humans, right? Human relationships are super important. And sometimes that heartbreak comes with massive changes in your beliefs about somebody,
Starting point is 02:18:57 say if they cheated on you or something like that, or regrets and you kind of ruminate about things that you've done wrong. There's really so many reasons though. I've had this, my first pet I had was we got it for a wedding present as a cat, and got it after like, but it died of FIP when it was four years old and you know I just would see her everywhere around the house you know. We got another cat then we got a dog. Dog eventually died of cancer and the cat just died recently and you know so we got a new dog because I kept seeing the dog around and I was just so heartbroken about this. But I still remember the pets that died,
Starting point is 02:19:45 it just comes back to you. I mean, it's part of this, I think there's also something about attachment that's just so crucial that drives again, these things that we want to remember, and that gives us that longing sometimes. Sometimes it's also not just about the heartbreak, but about the positive aspects of it, right? Because the loss comes from not only the fact that the relationship is over, but you had all of these good things before that you can now see in a new light, right? And so one of the things that I found from my clinical background that really I think gave me a different perspective on memory is so much of the therapy process was guided towards reframing and getting people to look at the past
Starting point is 02:20:32 in a different way, not by imposing changing people's memories or not by imposing an interpretation, but just offering a different perspective and maybe one that's kind of more optimized towards learning and, you know, an appreciation maybe or gratitude, whatever it is, right, that gives you a way of taking, I think you said it in the beginning, right, where you can have this kind of like dark experiences and you can use it as training data to, you know, grow in new ways, but it's hard. to grow in new ways, but it's hard. This, I often go back to this moment, this show, Louis with Louis CK, where he's all heartbroken about a breakup
Starting point is 02:21:14 with a woman he loves. And an older gentleman tells him that that's actually the best part, that heartbreak, because you get to intensely experience how valuable this love was. He says the worst part is forgetting it, is actually when you get over the heartbreak. That's the worst part.
Starting point is 02:21:36 So I sometimes think about that because, you know, having the love and losing it, like the losing it is when you sometimes feel it the deepest, which is an interesting way to celebrate the past and relive it. It's, it sucks that you don't have a thing, but when you don't have a thing, it's a, it's a good moment to viscerally experience the memories of something that you now appreciate even more. So you don't believe that an owner of a lonely heart is much better than an owner of a broken heart. You think an owner of a broken heart is better than the owner of a lonely heart?
Starting point is 02:22:17 Yes, for sure. I think so. I think so. But I'm gonna have to day by day. I don't know. I'm gonna have to listen to some more Bruce Springsteen to figure that one out. Well, you know, it's funny because it's like, after I turned 50, I think of death all the time. Like, I just think that, you know, I'm in like, I probably, I have fewer, probably a fewer years ahead of me than I have behind me, right?
Starting point is 02:22:39 So you think about, think about one thing, which is what are the memories that I wanna carry with me for the next period of time? And also about just the fact that everything around me could be, I know more people who are dying for various reasons. And so I'm not lots, I'm not that old, right? But it's something I think about a lot. And I'm reminded of how I talked to somebody who's a Buddhist and I was like, the whole idea of Buddhism is renouncing attachments. Someway the idea of Buddhism is staying out of the world of memory and staying in the
Starting point is 02:23:19 moment, right? And they talked about, it's like, how do you renounce attachments to the people that you love, right? And they're just saying, well, I appreciate that I have this moment with them, and knowing that they will die makes me appreciate this moment that much more. I mean, you said something similar, right,
Starting point is 02:23:37 in your daily routine that you think about things this way, right? Yeah, I meditate on mortality every day. But I don't know, at the same time, that really makes you appreciate the moment and live in the moment. I also appreciate the full deep rollercoaster of suffering involved in life,
Starting point is 02:23:58 the little and the big too. So I don't know, the Buddhist kind of removing yourself from the world, or the stoic removing yourself from the world or the Stoic removing yourself from the world, the world of emotion, I'm torn about that one. I'm not sure. RL Well, you know, this is where Hinduism and Buddhism, or at least some strains of Hinduism and Buddhism differ. In Hinduism, if you read the Bhagavad Gita, the philosophy is not one of Bhagavad Gita, the philosophy is not one of renouncing the world because the idea is that not doing something is no different than doing something, right? So what they argue, and again, you could interpret in different ways, positive and negative, but the argument is that you don't
Starting point is 02:24:39 want to renounce action, but you want to renounce the fruits of the action. You don't do it because of the outcome, you do it because of the process, because the process is part of the balance of the world that you're trying to preserve, right? And of course you could take that different ways, but I really think about that from time to time in terms of like, you know, letting go of this idea of does this book sell or trying to impress you and get you to laugh at my jokes or whatever and just be more like I'm sharing this information with you and getting to know you
Starting point is 02:25:16 or whatever it is, but it's hard, right? It's like, because we're so driven by the reinforcer, the outcome. It's, you're just part of the process of telling the joke, and if I laugh or not, that's up to the universe to decide. Yep, it's my dharma. How does studying memory affect your understanding of the nature of time?
Starting point is 02:25:39 So like, we've been talking about us living in the present and making decisions about the future, We've been talking about us living in the present and making decisions about the future, standing on the foundation of these memories and narratives about the memories that we've constructed. So it feels like it does weird things to time. Yeah, and the reason is, is that in some sense, I think, especially the farther we go back, I mean, there's all sorts
Starting point is 02:26:05 of interesting things that happen. So your sense of like, if I ask you how different does one hour ago feel from two hours ago, you'd probably say pretty different. But if I ask you, okay, go back one year ago versus one year and one hour ago, it's the same difference in time. It won't feel very different, right? So there's this kind of compression that happens as you look back farther in time. So that is kind of like why when you're older, the difference between somebody who's like 50 and 45 doesn't seem as big as the difference between like 10 and five or something, right? When you're 10 years old, everything seems like it's a long period of time. Here's the point is that you know so one of the interesting things that I found when I was working on the book actually was during the pandemic I just decided to ask people in my class when we're doing the remote
Starting point is 02:26:54 instruction. So one of the things I did was I would poll people and so I just asked people do you feel like the days are moving by slower or faster or about the same? Almost everyone in the class said that the days were moving by slower. So then I would say, okay, so do you feel like the weeks are passing by slower, faster, or the same? And the majority of them said that the weeks were passing by faster. So according to the laws of physics, I don't think that makes any sense, right? But according to memory, it did because what happened was people were doing the same thing over and over in the same context. And without that change in context, their feeling was that they were in one long monotonous event. But then at the end of the week, you look back at that week
Starting point is 02:27:46 and you say, well, what happened? No memories of what happened. So it must, that week just went by without even my noticing it. But that week went by during the same amount of time as an eventful week where you might've been going out, hanging out with friends on vacation or whatever, right? It's just that nothing happened because you're doing the same thing over and over. So I feel like memory really shapes our sense of time, but it does so in part because context is so important for memory.
Starting point is 02:28:16 Well, that compression you mentioned, it's an interesting process. Because what I think about when I was like 12 or 15, I just fundamentally feel like the same person. It's interesting what that compression does. It makes me feel like it's all, we're all connected, not just amongst humans and spatially, but in terms, back in time,
Starting point is 02:28:40 there's a kind of eternal nature, like the timelessness, I guess, to life. That could be also a genetic thing just for me. I don't know if everyone agrees to this view of time, but to me it all feels the same. Like you don't feel the passage of time or? No, I feel the passage of time in the same way that your students did from day to day.
Starting point is 02:29:03 There's certain markers that let you know that time has passed, you celebrate birthdays and so on. But the core of who I am and who others I know are are events, it like, that compression of my understanding of the world, removes time. Because time is not useful for the compression. So like the details of that time, at least for me,
Starting point is 02:29:26 is not useful to understanding the core of the thing. Maybe what it is is that you really like to see connections between things. This is like really what motivates me in science actually too. But it's like when you start recalling the past to, you know, and seeing the connections between the past and present,
Starting point is 02:29:45 now you have this kind of web of interconnected memories, right? And so I can imagine in that sense there is this kind of the present is with you, right? But what's interesting about what you said too that struck me is that your 16 year old self was probably very complex, you know. And I'm, by the way, I'm the same way, but it's like, it really is the source of a lot of darkness for me. But when like, you can look back at like, let's say you hear a song that you used to play like before you would go do a sports thing or something like that. And you might not think of yourself as an athlete, but once you get back to that,
Starting point is 02:30:27 you mentally time traveled to that particular thing, you open up this little compartment of yourself that wasn't there before, right? That didn't seem accessible for them. Dan Schachter's lab did this really cool study where they would ask people to either remember doing something altruistic or imagine doing something altruistic or imagine doing something
Starting point is 02:30:46 altruistic and that act made them more likely to want to do things for other people. So that act of mental time travel can change who you are in the present. We tend to think of this goes back to that illusion of stability and we tend to think of memory in this very deterministic way that I am who I am because I have this past, but we have a very multifaceted past and can access different parts of it and change in the moment based on whatever part we want to reach for. Right.
Starting point is 02:31:21 How does nostalgia connect into this? right? How does nostalgia connect into this, like this desire and pleasure associated with going back? Yeah, so my friend Felipe de Brigarde wrote this and it just like blew my mind where the word nostalgia was coined by a Swiss physician who was actually studying traumatized soldiers. And so he described nostalgia as a disease. And the idea was it was bringing these people extraordinary unhappiness because they're remembering how things used to be. And I think it's very complex. So as people get older, for instance, nostalgia can be an enormous source of happiness, right? And being nostalgic can improve people's moods in the moment. But it just depends on what they do with it, because what you
Starting point is 02:32:12 can sometimes see is nostalgia has the opposite effect of thinking those were the good old days and those days are over, right? It's like America used to be so great and now it sucks. Or, you know, my life used to be so great when I was a kid and now it's not, right? And you're selectively remembering the things that, we don't realize how selective our remembering self is. And so, you know, I lived through the 70s, it sucked. I was like, partly it sucked more for me,
Starting point is 02:32:41 but I would say that even otherwise, it's like there's all sorts of problems going on gas lines People were like, you know worried about like Russia nuclear war blah blah blah So I mean it's just this idea that people have about the past can be very useful if it brings you Happiness in the present, but if it narrows your worldview in the present, you're not aware of those biases that you have, you will end up, you can end up, it can be toxic, right? Either at a personal level or at a collective level.
Starting point is 02:33:16 Let me ask you both a practical question and an out there question. So let's start with the more practical one. What are your thoughts about BCIs, brain computer interfaces, and the work that's going on with Neuralink? We talked about electrodes and different ways of measuring the brain,
Starting point is 02:33:33 and here Neuralink is working on basically two-way communication with the brain. And the more out there question would be like, where does this go? But more practically in the near term, what do you think about Neuralink? Yeah, I mean, I can't say specifics about the company because I haven't studied it that much.
Starting point is 02:33:49 But I mean, I think there's two parts of it. So one is they're developing some really interesting technology, I think with these like surgical robots and things like that. BCI though has like a whole lot of innovation going on. I'm not necessarily seeing any scientific evidence from Neuralink and maybe that's just because I'm not looking for it, but I'm not seeing the
Starting point is 02:34:11 evidence that they're anywhere near where the scientific community is. And there's lots of startups that are doing incredibly innovative stuff. One of my colleagues, Sergey Savitsky, is just like a genius in this area and they're working on it. I think speech prosthetics like that are incorporating, decoding techniques with AI and movement prosthetics, the rate of progress is just enormous. So part of the technology is having good enough data and understanding which data to use and what to do with it. And then the other part of it then is the algorithms for decoding it and so forth. And I think part of that has really resulted in some real breakthroughs in neurosciences results. So there's lots of new technologies like
Starting point is 02:34:57 neuropixels, for instance, that allow you to harvest activity from many, many neurons from a single electrode. I know Neuralink has some technologies that are also along these lines, but again, because they do their own stuff, the scientific community doesn't see it. But I think BCI is much, much bigger than Neuralink and there's just so much innovation happening. I think the interesting question which we may be getting into is I was talking to Sergey a while ago about, you know, so a lot of language is not just what we hear and what we speak, but also our intentions and our internal models. And you know, so are you really going to be able to restore language without dealing with that part of it?
Starting point is 02:35:42 And he brought up a really interesting question, which is the ethics of reading out people's intentions and understanding of the world, as opposed to the more, you know, the more concrete parts of hearing and producing movements, right? Just so we're clear, because you said a few interesting things, when we talk about language and BCIs, what we mean is getting signal from the brain
Starting point is 02:36:06 and generating the language, say you're not able to actually speak, it's as a kind of linguistic prosthetic. It's able to speak for you exactly what you want it to say. And then the deeper question is, well, saying something isn't just the letters, the words you're saying, it's also the intention behind it, the feeling behind all that kind of stuff,
Starting point is 02:36:33 and is it ethical to reveal that full shebang, the full context of what's going on in our brain? That's really, that's really interesting. That's really interesting. I mean, our thoughts. Is it ethical for anyone to have access to our thoughts? Because right now the resolution is so low that we're okay with it, even doing studies
Starting point is 02:36:57 and all this kind of stuff. But if neuroscience has a few breakthroughs to where you can start to map out the QR codes for different thoughts, for different kinds of thoughts, maybe a political thoughts, you know, the McCarthyism, what if I'm getting a lot of them communist thoughts or however we want to categorize or label it? That's interesting. That's really interesting. I think ultimately this always,
Starting point is 02:37:22 That's interesting, that's really interesting. I think ultimately this always, the more transparency there is about the human mind, the better it is, but there could be always intermediate battles with how much control does a centralized entity have, like a government and so on, what is the regulation, what are the rules, what's legal and illegal? You know, if you talk about the police,
Starting point is 02:37:47 whose job is to track down criminals and so on, and you look at all the history, how the police could be abused its power to control the citizenry, all that kind of stuff. So people are always paranoid and rightfully so. It's fascinating. It's really fascinating. You know, we talk about freedom of speech,
Starting point is 02:38:08 you know, freedom of thought, which is also a very important liberty at the core of this country and probably humanity, starts to get awfully tricky when you start to be able to collect those thoughts. But what I wanted to actually ask you is, do you think for fun and for practical purposes, you'll be able to, we would be able to modify memories?
Starting point is 02:38:38 So how difficult is it to, how far away we are from understanding the different parts of the brains everything we've been talking about in order to figure out how can we adjust this memory at the crude level from unpleasant to pleasant you talked about we can remember the mall and the people like location the people can we keep the people and change the place like this kind of stuff how difficult is that well I mean in some sense we know we can do it just behaviorally right can we keep the people and change the place? Like this kind of stuff. How difficult is that? Well, I mean, in some sense,
Starting point is 02:39:06 we know we can do it just behaviorally, right? Behaviorally, yes. I can just like tell you, under certain conditions anyway, I can give you the misinformation and then you can change the people, places and so forth, right? On the crude level,
Starting point is 02:39:21 there's a lot of work that's being done on a phenomenon called reconsolidation, which is the idea that essentially when I recall a memory, what happens is that the connections between the neurons in that cell assembly that give you the memory are going to be more modifiable. And so some people have used techniques to try to like, for instance, with fear memories, to reduce that physical visceral component of the memory when it's being activated. Right now, I think as an outsider looking at the data, I think it's like mixed results. And part of it is, and this speaks to the more complex issue, is that you need somebody to actually fully recall that traumatic memory in the first place in order to actually modify it. Then what is the memory? That is the key part of
Starting point is 02:40:15 the problem. So if we go back to reading people's thoughts, what is the thought? I mean, people can sometimes look at this like behaviorists and go, well, the memory is like, I've given you A and you produce B. But I think that's a very bankrupt concept about memory. I think it's much more complicated than that. And, you know, one of the things that when we started studying naturalistic memory, like memory from movies, that was so hard was we had to change the way we did the studies. Because if I show you a movie and I show and I watch the same movie and you recall everything that happened and I recall everything that happened, we might take a different amount of time to do it. We might use different words and yet
Starting point is 02:40:56 to an outside observer, we might have recalled the same thing, right? So it's not about the words necessarily and it's not about how long we spent or whatever. There's something deeper that is there that's this idea, but it's like, how do you understand that thought? I encounter a lot of concrete thinking that it's like, if I show a model, like, you know, the visual information that a person sees when they drive, I can basically reverse engineer driving. Well, that's not really how they drive. I can basically reverse engineer driving. Well that's not really how it works. I once saw a talk by somebody or I saw somebody talking in this discussion of between neuroscientists and AI people
Starting point is 02:41:35 and he was saying that the problem with self-driving cars that they had in cities as opposed to highways was that the car was okay at doing the things it's supposed to, but when there were pedestrians around, it couldn't predict the intentions of people. And so that unpredictability of people was the problem that they were having in the self-driving car design, because it didn't have a good enough internal model of what the people were,
Starting point is 02:42:05 what they were doing, what they wanted. And what do you think about that? Well, I spent a huge amount of time watching pedestrians, thinking about pedestrians, thinking about what it takes to solve the problem of measuring, detecting the intention of a pedestrian, really of a human being in this particular context of having to cross a street. And it's fascinating.
Starting point is 02:42:31 I think it's a window into how complex social systems are that involve humans. Because I would just stand there and watch intersections for hours. And what you start to figure out is every single intersection has its own personality. So like, there's a history to that intersection. Like jaywalking, certain intersections
Starting point is 02:43:05 allow jaywalking a lot more. Because what happens is we're leaders and followers. So there's a regular, let's say, and they get off the subway and they start crossing on red light, and they do this every single day. And then there's people that don't show up to that intersection often, and they're looking for cues of how we're supposed to behave here. And if a few people start to jaywalk
Starting point is 02:43:26 and cross on red light, they will also, they will follow. And there's just a dynamic to that intersection. There's a spirit to it. If you look at Boston versus New York, versus a rural town, even Boston, San Francisco, or here in Austin, there's different personalities city-wide, but there's different personalities area-wide, region-wide, and there's different personalities,
Starting point is 02:43:49 different intersections. And it's just fascinating. For a car to be able to determine that is tricky. Now, what machine learning systems are able to do well is collect a huge amount of data. So for us, it's tricky because we get to like, understand the world with very limited information and make decisions grounded in this big foundation model
Starting point is 02:44:12 that we've built of understanding how humans work. AI could literally, in the context of driving, this is where I've often been really torn in both directions. If you just collect a huge amount of data, all of that information and then compress it into a representation of how humans cross streets, it's probably all there.
Starting point is 02:44:35 In the same way that you have a Noam Chomsky who says, no, no, no, AI can't talk, can't write convincing language without understanding language. And more and more you see large language models without quote unquote understanding can generate very convincing language. But I think what the process of compression from a huge amount of data compressing
Starting point is 02:44:57 into a representation is doing is in fact understanding. Deeply, in order to be able to generate one letter at a time, one letter at a time, one word at a time, you have to understand the cruelty of Nazi Germany and the beauty of sending humans to space, and like, you have to understand all of that in order to generate, like, I'm going to the kitchen to get an apple
Starting point is 02:45:24 and do that grammatically correctly. You have to have a world model that includes all of human behavior. You think an LLM is building that world model. It has to in order to be good at generating one word at a time, a convincing sentence. And in the same way, I think AI that drives a car, if it has enough data, will be able to form a world model
Starting point is 02:45:46 that will be able to predict correctly what the pedestrian does. But when we, as humans, are watching pedestrians, we slowly realize, damn, this is really complicated. In fact, when you start to self-reflect on driving, you realize driving is really complicated. There's like subtle cues we take about like just, there's a million things I could say,
Starting point is 02:46:10 but like one of them determining who around you is an asshole, aggressive driver, potentially dangerous. I was just thinking about this, yeah. You can read it, once you become a great driver, you can see it a mile away, this guy's gonna pull an asshole move in front of you. It's like way back there, but you know it's gonna happen. And I don't know what,
Starting point is 02:46:31 because we're ignoring all the other cars, but for some reason the asshole, like a red, like a glowing obvious symbol is just like right there, even in the periphery vision. Because we're, again, we're usually when we're driving, just looking forward, but we're like, using the periphery vision, because we're, again, we're usually when we're driving just looking forward, but we're like using the periphery vision to figure stuff out, and it's like a little puzzle
Starting point is 02:46:50 that we're usually only allocating a small amount of our attention to, at least our cognitive attention to. And it's fascinating, but I think AI just has a fundamentally different suite of sensors, in terms of the bandwidth of data that's coming in that allows you to form the representation that perform inference on the representation you using the representation you form that for the case of driving I think it could be quite effective but one of the things that's currently missing even though OpenAI just recently announced
Starting point is 02:47:25 adding memory. And I did want to ask you like how important it is, how difficult is it to add some of the memory mechanisms that you've seen in humans to AI systems? I would say superficially not that hard, but then in a deeper level very hard because we don't understand episodic memory. So one of the ideas I talk about in the book is one of the oldest kind of dilemmas in computational neuroscience is what Steve Krosberg called the stability plasticity dilemma. When do you say something is new and overwrite your pre-existing knowledge versus going with what you had before and making incremental changes. And so, you know, part of the problem with going through
Starting point is 02:48:12 like massive, you know, I mean, part of the problem of things like if you're trying to design an LLM or something like that is especially for English, there's so many exceptions to the rules, right? And so if you want to rapidly learn the exceptions, you're gonna lose the rules. And if you want to keep the rules, you have a harder time learning the exception. And so David Marr is one of the early pioneers in computational neuroscience. And then Jay McClelland and my colleague, Randy O'Reilly, some other people like Neil Cohen, all these people started to come up with the idea that maybe that's part of what we need in what the human brain is doing is we have this kind of a actually a fairly dumb system which just says this happened once at this point in
Starting point is 02:48:58 time, which we call episodic memory so to speak, and then we have this knowledge that we've accumulated from our experiences as semantic memory. So now when we want to, we encounter a situation that's surprising and violates all our previous expectations, what happens is that now we can form an episodic memory here. And the next time we're in a similar situation, boom, we could supplement our knowledge with this
Starting point is 02:49:25 information from episodic memory and reason about what the right thing to do is, right? So it gives us this enormous amount of flexibility to stop on a dime and change without having to erase everything we've already learned. And that solution is incredibly powerful because it gives you And that solution is incredibly powerful because it gives you the ability to learn from so much less information, really, right? And it gives you that flexibility. So one of the things I think that makes humans great is having both episodic and semantic memory. Now, can you build something like that? I mean, you know, computational neuroscience people would say, well, yeah, you just record a moment and you just get it and you're done, right?
Starting point is 02:50:09 But when do you record that moment? How much do you record? What's the information you prioritize and what's the information you don't? These are the hard questions. When do you use episodic memory? When do you just throw it away? And these are the hard questions
Starting point is 02:50:22 we're still trying to figure out in people. And then you start to think about all these mechanisms that we have in the brain for figuring out some of these things. And it's not just one, but it's many of them that are interacting with each other. And then you just take not only the episodic and the semantic, but then you start to take the motivational survival things, right? It's just like the fight or flight responses that we associate with particular things or the kind of like reward motivation that we associate with certain things so forth. And those things are absent from AI. I frankly don't know if we want it. I don't necessarily want a self-motivated LLM, right? And then there's the problem of how do you even build the motivations that should guide a
Starting point is 02:51:08 proper reinforcement learning kind of thing, for instance. So a friend of mine, Sam Gershman, I might be missing the quote exactly, but he basically said, if I wanted to train a typical AI model to make me as much money as possible, first thing you might do is sell my house. So it's not even just about having one goal or one objective, but just having all these competing goals and objectives, right? Then things start to get really complicated. Well, it's all interconnected.
Starting point is 02:51:38 I mean, just even the thing you've mentioned is the moment. You know, if we record a moment, like it's difficult to express concretely what a moment is. Like how deeply connected it is to the entirety of it. Maybe to record a moment, you have to make a universe from scratch. You have to include everything. You have to include all the emotions involved, all the emotions involved all the context all the things that built
Starting point is 02:52:07 around there all the social connections all the Visual experiences all the sensory experience all of that all the history that came before That moment is built on and we somehow take all that and we compress it and keep the useful parts and then Integrate into the whole thing, into our whole narrative. And then each individual has their own little version of that narrative and then we collide in a social way and we adjust it and we evolve.
Starting point is 02:52:36 Yeah, yeah, I mean, well, even if we wanna go super simple, right? Like Tyler Bonnen, who's a postdoc who's collaborating with me, he actually studied a lot of computer vision at Stanford. And so one of the things he was interested in is some people who have brain damage in areas of the brain that were thought to be important for memory.
Starting point is 02:52:57 But they also seem to have some perception problems with particular kinds of object perception. And this is super controversial. Some people found this effect, some didn't. And he went back to computer vision, and he said, let's take the best state of the art computer vision models and let's give them the same kinds of perception tests that we were giving to these people. And then he would find the images
Starting point is 02:53:19 where the computer vision models would just struggle, and you would find that they just didn't do well. Even if you add more parameters, you add more layers on and on and on, it doesn't help, right? The architecture didn't matter. It was just there, the problem. And then he found those were the exact ones where these humans with particular damage to this area called the periorrhinal cortex, that was where they were struggling. So somehow this brain area was being, was important for being able to do these things that were adversarial to these computer vision models. So then he found that it only happened
Starting point is 02:53:55 if people had enough time, they could make those discriminations. But without enough time, if they just get a glance, they're just like the computer vision models. So then what he started to say was, well, maybe let's look at people's eyes, right? So computer vision model sees every pixel all at once, right? It's not, you know, and we don't. We never see every pixel all at once. Even if I'm looking at a screen with pixels, I'm not seeing every pixel at once. I'm grabbing little points on the screen by moving my eyes around and getting a very high resolution picture of what I'm focusing on and Kind of a lower resolution information about everything else but I'm I'm not necessarily choosing but I'm directing that exploration and
Starting point is 02:54:39 Allowing people to move their eyes and integrate that information gave them something That the computer vision models weren't able to do. So somehow integrating information across time and getting less information at each step gave you more out of the process. I mean the process of allocating attention across time seems to be a really important process. Even the breakthroughs that you get with machine learning, mostly has to do attention is all you need as about attention, transform is about attention.
Starting point is 02:55:19 So attention is a really interesting one. But then like, yeah, how you allocate that attention, again, is at the core of what it means to be intelligent, what it means to process the world, integrate all the important things, discard all the unimportant things. Attention is at the core of it, it's probably at the core of memory too.
Starting point is 02:55:48 There's so much sensory information, there's so much going on, there's so much going on. To filter it down to almost nothing and just keep those parts. And to keep those parts and then whenever there's an error to adjust the model such that you can allocate attention even better to new things that would result, maybe maximize the chance of confirming the model
Starting point is 02:56:10 or disconfirming the model that you have and adjusting it since then. Yeah, attention is a weird one. I was always fascinated. I mean, I got a chance to study peripheral vision for a bit and indirectly study attention through that. And it's just fascinating how humans, how good humans are looking around
Starting point is 02:56:31 and gathering information. Yeah, at the same time, people are terrible at detecting changes that can happen in the environment if they're not attending in the right way, if their predictive model is too strong, you know? So you have these weird things where the machines can do better than the people. So this is the thing, as people go, oh, the machines can do this stuff that's just like humans. It's like, well, the machines make different kinds
Starting point is 02:56:58 of mistakes than the people do. And I will never be convinced unless we've replicated human... I don't even like the term intelligence because I think it's a stupid concept, but I don't think we've replicated human intelligence unless I know that the simulator is making exactly the same kinds of mistakes that people do. Because people make characteristic mistakes, They have characteristic biases. They have characteristic heuristics that we use. And those, I have yet to see evidence that chat GPT will do that. Since we're talking about attention, is there an interesting connection to you
Starting point is 02:57:38 between ADHD and memory? Well, it's interesting for me because when I was a child, I was actually told my school, I don't know if it came from a school psychologist, they did do some testing on me, I know for like IQ and stuff like that. Or if it just came from teachers who hated me, but they told my parents that I had ADHD. And so this was of course in the 70s. So basically they said like, you know, he has poor motor control and he's got ADHD. And so, and you know, there was social issues. So like I could have been put a year ahead in school, but then they said, oh, but he doesn't have the social, he doesn't have the social capabilities. So I still ended up being like, you know, an outcast even in my own grade.
Starting point is 02:58:26 But then like, so then my parents said, okay, well, they got me on a diet free of artificial colors and flavors because that was the thing that people talked about back then. So I am interested in this topic because I've come to appreciate now that I have many of the characteristics, if not, you know, full blown. It's like I'm definitely timeline-ness, rejection-sensitive, you name
Starting point is 02:58:51 it. They talk about it. It's like impulsive behavior. I can tell you about all sorts of fights I've gotten into in the past. Just you name it. But yeah, so ADHD is fascinating though, because right now we're seeing like more and more diagnosis of it and I don't know what to say about that. I don't know how much of that is based on kind of inappropriate expectations, especially for children, and how much of that is based on true kind of like maladaptive kinds of tendencies. But what we do know is this, is that ADHD is associated with differences in prefrontal function, so that attention can be both more, you're more distractible, you have harder time focusing
Starting point is 02:59:38 your attention on what's relevant, and so you shift too easily. But then once you get on something that you're interested in, you can get stuck. And so, you know, the attention is this beautiful balance of being able to focus when you need to focus and shift when you need to shift. And so it's that flexibility plus stability again, and that balance seems to be disrupted in ADHD. And so as a result, memory tends to be poor in ADHD. But it's not necessarily because there's a traditional memory problem, but it's more because of this attentional issue, right? And so, and people with ADHD often will have great memory for the things that they're
Starting point is 03:00:22 interested in, and just no memory for the things that they're interested in, and just no memory for the things that they're not interested in. Is there advice from your own life on how to learn and succeed from that? From just how the characteristics of your own brain with ADHD and so on, how do you learn, how do you remember information, how do you flourish in this sort of education context?
Starting point is 03:00:49 I'm still trying to figure out the flourishing per se, but education, I mean, being in science is enormously enabling of ADHD. It's like you're constantly looking for new things, you're constantly seeking that dopamine hit, and that's great, you know great. They tolerate your being late for things. Nobody's going to die if you screw up. It's not like being a doctor or something where you have to be much more responsible and focused. You can just freely follow your curiosity, which is just great. But what I'd say is that I'm learning now about so many things,
Starting point is 03:01:29 like about how to structure my activities more and basically say, okay, if I'm going to be, email is like the big one that kills me right now. I'm just constantly shifting between email and my activities. And what happens is that I don't actually get the email. I'm just constantly like shifting between email and my activities. And what happens is that I don't actually get the email. I just look at my email and I get stressed because I'm like, oh, I have to think about this. Let me get back to it. And I go back to something else. And so I've just got fragmentary memories of everything, right? So what I'm trying to do is set aside a timer. Like this is my email time. This is my writing time. This is my goofing off time. And so blocking these things off,
Starting point is 03:02:09 you give yourself the goofing off time. Sometimes I do that and sometimes I have to be flexible to go like, okay, I'm definitely not focusing. I'm going to give myself the downtime and it's an investment. It's not like wasting time. It's an investment in my attention later on. And I'm very much with Cal Newport on this.
Starting point is 03:02:28 He wrote deep work and a lot of other amazing books. He talks about task switching as a sort of, the thing that really destroys productivity. So like, you know, switching, it doesn't even matter from what to what, but checking social media, checking email, maybe switching to a phone call, and then doing work, and then switching.
Starting point is 03:02:47 Even switching between if you're reading a paper, switching from paper to paper to paper, because curiosity and whatever the dopamine hit from the attention switch, limiting that, because otherwise your brain is just not capable to really load it in really Do that deep deliberation, I think that's required to Remember things and to really think through things. Yeah, I mean you probably see this I imagine in AI conferences, but definitely in
Starting point is 03:03:21 Neuroscience conferences. It's now the norm that people have their laptops out during talks. And conceivably they're writing notes, but in fact, what often happens if you look at people, and we can speak from a little bit of personal experience, is you're checking email and you're like, or I'm working on my own talk, but often it's like you're doing things
Starting point is 03:03:44 that are not paying attention. I have this illusion, well, I'm paying attention and then I'm working on my own talk, but often it's like you're doing things that are not paying attention. I have this illusion, well I'm paying attention and then I'm going back. And then what happens is I don't remember anything from that day, it just kind of vanished. Because what happens is I'm creating all these artificial event boundaries. I'm losing all this executive function every time I switch. I'm getting like a few seconds slower and I'm catching up mentally to what's happening. And so instead of being in a model where you're meaningfully integrating everything and predicting and generating this kind of like rich model, I'm just catching up, you know. And so yeah, there's great
Starting point is 03:04:19 research by Melina Unkaffer and Anthony Wagner on multitasking and people can look up that talks about just how bad it is for memory and it's becoming worse and worse of a problem. So you're a musician. Take me through how did you get into music? What made you first fall in love with music, with creating music? Yeah, so I started playing music just when I was like doing trumpet in school For school band and I would just read music and play and you know, it was pretty decent at it Not great, but I was decent. How'd you go from trumpet to? Guitar to guitar especially the kind of music you're into. Yeah, so basically in high school
Starting point is 03:05:00 Yeah, so I kind of was a late bloomer to music, but just kind of MTV grew up with me. I grew up with MTV. And so then you started seeing all this stuff. And then I got into metal was kind of like my early genre. And I always reacted to just things that were loud and had a beat like, I mean, ADHD, right? Like, you know, everything from Sergeant Pepper's by the Beatles to like Led Zeppelin II, my dad had both, my parents had both those albums, so I'd listen to them a lot. And then like The Police, Ghost in the Machine.
Starting point is 03:05:38 But then I got into metal, Def Leppard and, you know, ACDC, Metallica, went way down the rabbit hole of speed metal. And that time was kind of like, oh like why don't I play guitar? I can do this. And I had friends who were doing that and I just never got it. Like I took lessons and stuff like that. But it was different because when I was doing trumpet I was was reading sheet music. And this was like, I was learning by looking, there's a thing called tablature, you know, this where it's like, you see like a drawing of the fretboard with numbers and that's where you're supposed to put your, it's kind of like paint by numbers, right? And so I learned it in a completely different way, but I was still terrible at it. And I didn't get it. It's actually taken me a long time to
Starting point is 03:06:26 understand exactly what the issue was. But it wasn't until I really got into punk and I saw bands like I saw Sonic Youth, I remember especially, and it just blew my mind because they violated the rules of what I thought music was supposed to be. I was like, this doesn't sound right. These are not power chords. And this isn't just have like a shouty verse and then a chorus part. It's not going, but this is just like weird. And then it occurred to me, you don't have to write music the way it's people tell you it's supposed to sound. I just opened up everything for me and I was playing in a band and I was struggling with writing music because I would try to write like you know whatever was popular at the time and or whatever sounded like other bands that I was listening to and somehow I kind of morphed into
Starting point is 03:07:16 just like just grabbing a guitar and just doing stuff and I realized a part of my problem with doing music before was I didn't enjoy trying to play stuff that other people play it. I just enjoyed music just dripping out of me and just, you know, spilling out and just doing stuff. And so I started to say, what if I don't play a chord? What if I just play like notes that shouldn't go together and just mess around with stuff? And I said, well, what if I don't do four beats going, na na na na, one two three four, one two three four, one two three four, whatever I go, one two three four five, one two three four five, and started messing around time signatures. Then I was playing in
Starting point is 03:07:56 this band with a great musician who was really Brent Ritzel, who was in this band with me, and he taught me about arranging songs. And it was like, what if we take this part and instead of make it go like back and forth we make it like a circle or what if we make it like a straight line you know or zigzag you know just make it like non-linear in these interesting ways and then you know it's like the whole world sort of opens up as like the and then what I started to realize especially so you could appreciate this as a musician I think so time signatures right so we are so brainwashed to think in four four right every rock song you could think of almost is in four four I know you're a Floyd fan so think of money by Pink Floyd right yeah bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum Bum B interesting. But you're thinking in four because that's how we use it. We're used to thinking. So the music flows a little bit faster than it's supposed to and you're getting a little bit of
Starting point is 03:09:11 prediction error every time this is happening. And once I got used to that, I was like, I hate writing in four four because I was like, everything just feels better. If I do it in seven four, if I alternate between four and three, and doing all this stuff, and then it's like, you just, jazz music is like that, they just do so much interesting stuff with us. So playing with those time signatures allows you to really break it all open,
Starting point is 03:09:35 and just, I guess there's something about that where it allows you to actually have fun. Yeah, yeah, and it's like, so I'm actually a very, one of the genres we used to play in was math rock. That's what they called it. It was just like, this is so many weird times. Oh, interesting. Yeah. So that's, that's the math part of rock is what the mathematical disturbances of it or what?
Starting point is 03:10:00 Yeah, I guess it would be like, so instead of, you might go like, instead of playing four beats in every measure No, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, you go. No, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no You know and just do these things and then you might arrange it in weird ways so that there might be three measures of verse and then One, you know and then five measures of chorus and then two measures so So you could just mess around with everything, right? What does that feel like to listen to? There's something about symmetry or like patterns
Starting point is 03:10:31 that feel good and like relaxing for us or whatever. It feels like home and disturbing that can be quite disturbing. Yeah. So is that the feeling you would have if you were keep messing? Math rock, I mean, that's stressing me out just listening, learning about it.
Starting point is 03:10:49 So, I mean, it depends. So a lot of my style of songwriting is very much like in terms of like repetitive themes, but messing around with structure, because I'm not a great guitarist technically. And so I don't play complicated stuff. And there's things you can hear stuff where it's just like so complicated, you know. But often what I find is having a melody and then adding some dissonance to it, just enough, and then adding some complexity that gets you going just enough. But I have a high tolerance for that kind of dissonance and prediction. I think I have a theory, a pet theory,
Starting point is 03:11:29 that it's like basically you can explain most of human behavior as some people are lumpers and some people are splitters, you know? And so it's like some people are very kind of excited when they get this dissonance and they wanna like go with it. Some people are just like, no, I wanna lump every, you know, I don't know, maybe that's even a different thing, but it's like, basically it's like,
Starting point is 03:11:48 I think some people get scared of that discomfort. And I really grab it. Thrive on it. Yeah. You know? I love it. What's the name of your band now? The cover band I play in is a band called Pavlov's Dogs.
Starting point is 03:12:03 And so, it's a band unsurprisingly, of mostly memory researchers, neuroscientists. I love this. I love this so much. Actually, one of your MIT colleagues, Earl Miller, plays bass. Plays bass. Do you play rhythm or a leader?
Starting point is 03:12:19 You could compete if you want. Maybe we could audition you. For audition? Oh yeah. Coming for you, Earl. Put me in spot. For audition? Oh yeah. I'm coming for you Earl. Earl's gonna kill me. He's like very precise though. I'll play triangle or something. Or is it where's the cowbell? Yeah I'll be the cowbell guy. And you guys what kind of songs do you guys do? So it's mostly uh uh seven late 70s punk and 80s new wave and post-punk, blondie, Ramones, Clash.
Starting point is 03:12:50 I sing Age of Consent by New Order and Love Will Terrorist. You said you have a female singer now? Yeah, yeah, yeah. Carrie Hoffman and also Paula Crocks. And so they do, yeah, so Carrie does Blondie amazingly well. And we do like Gigantic by the Pixies, Paula does that one. Which song do you love to play the most? What kind of song is super fun for you? A song of someone else's?
Starting point is 03:13:17 Yeah, cover, yeah. Cover, okay. And it's one we do with Pavlov's Dogs. Mm-hmm. I really enjoy playing I Wanna Be Your Dog by Iggy and the Stooges. Which is perfect because we're Pavlov's dogs. And Pavlov of course was like basically created learning theory. So, you know, there's that. But also it's like, but I mean, Iggy and the Stooges, that song, so I play and sing on it, but it's just like it devolves into total noise and I just like fall on the floor and generate feedback. I was like, I think in the last version it might have been that or a Velvet Underground cover in our
Starting point is 03:13:53 last show I actually, I have a guitar made of aluminum that I got made and I thought this thing's indestructible and so I kind of like was just you know moving it around, had it upside down and all this stuff to generate feedback. And I think I broke one of the tuning pegs. Oh wow. Yeah, so I've managed to break an all metal guitar. Go figure. A bit of a big ridiculous question,
Starting point is 03:14:17 but let me ask you, we've been talking about neuroscience in general. You've been studying the human mind for a long time. What do you love most about the human mind? Like when you look at it, we look at the fMRI, just the scans, and the behavioral stuff, the electrodes, you know, the psychology aspect,
Starting point is 03:14:39 the reading, the literature on the biology side, neurobiology, all of it. When you look at it, is most like beautiful to you? I think the most beautiful but incredibly hard to put your finger on is this idea of the internal model. That it's like there's everything you see and there's everything you hear and touch and taste you know every breath you, whatever. But it's all connected by this dark energy that's holding that whole universe of your mind together. And without that, it's just a bunch of stuff. And somehow we put that together and it forms so much of our experience. And being able to figure out where
Starting point is 03:15:28 that comes from and how things are connected to me is just amazing. But just this idea of that the world in front of us, we're only sampling this little bit and trying to take so much meaning from it. And we do a really good job, not perfect, I mean. But that ability to me is just amazing. Yeah, it's an incredible mystery, all of it. It's funny you said dark energy, because the same in astrophysics. You look out there, look at dark matter and dark energy,
Starting point is 03:15:58 which is this loose term assigned to a thing we don't understand, which helps make the equations work in terms of gravity and the expansion of the universe in the same way. It seems like there's that kind of thing in the human mind that we're striving to understand. Yeah, yeah. It's funny that you mentioned that. One of the reasons I wrote the book amongst many is that I really felt like people needed to hear from scientists and COVID was just a great example of this because like people weren't hearing from scientists. One of the things I think that people didn't get was the uncertainty of science and how much we don't know. And I think every scientist lives in this world of uncertainty. And when I was writing the book, I just became aware of all of these things we
Starting point is 03:16:46 don't know. And so I think of physics a lot. I think of this idea of like overwhelming majority of the stuff that's in our universe cannot be directly measured. I used to think, haha, I hate physics. Physicists get the Nobel Prize for doing whatever stupid thing. It's like there's 10 physicists out there. I'm just kidding. Just dishing out words. Yeah, no, no, no, I'm kidding. It's the physicists who do neuroscience could be rather opinionated. So sometimes I like to dish on it. It's all love. It's all love. That's right. I, this is the ADHD talking. So, um, uh,
Starting point is 03:17:21 but at some point I had this aha moment where I was like, to be aware of that much that we don't know and have a bead on it and be able to go towards it, that's one of the biggest scientific successes that I could think of. You are aware that you don't know about this gigantic section, overwhelming majority of the universe, right? And I think the more what keeps me going to some extent is realizing the changing the scope of the problem and figuring out, oh my God, there's all these things we don't know. And I thought I knew this
Starting point is 03:18:01 because science is all about assumptions, right? So have you ever read the structure of scientific revolutions by Thomas Kuhn? Yes. That's my only philosophy really that I've read. But it's so brilliant in the way that he frames this idea of assumptions being core to the scientific process. And the paradigm shift comes from changing those assumptions. And this idea of finding out this whole zone of what you don't know to me is the exciting part. Well, you are a great scientist
Starting point is 03:18:36 and you wrote an incredible book, so thank you for doing that. And thank you for talking today. You've decreased the amount of uncertainty I have just a tiny little bit today and revealed the beauty of memory. This is fascinating conversation. Thank you for talking today.
Starting point is 03:18:54 Oh, thank you. It's been a blast. Thanks for listening to this conversation with Charon Ranganath. To support this podcast, please check out our sponsors in the description. And now, let me leave you with some words from Haruki Murakami. Most things are forgotten over time. Even the war itself, the life and death struggle people
Starting point is 03:19:17 went through, is now like something from the distant past. We're so caught up in our everyday lives that events of the past are no longer in orbit around our minds. There are just too many things we have to think about every day, too many new things we have to learn. But still, no matter how much time passes, no matter what takes place in the interim, there are some things we can never assign to oblivion, memories we can never rub away. They remain with us forever, like a touchstone. Thank you for listening. I hope to see you next time. You

There aren't comments yet for this episode. Click on any sentence in the transcript to leave a comment.