Lex Fridman Podcast - Paola Arlotta: Brain Development from Stem Cell to Organoid

Episode Date: August 12, 2019

Paola Arlotta is a professor of stem cell and regenerative biology at Harvard University. She is interested in understanding the molecular laws that govern the birth, differentiation and assembly of t...he human brain’s cerebral cortex. She explores the complexity of the brain by studying and engineering elements of how the brain develops. This conversation is part of the Artificial Intelligence podcast. If you would like to get more information about this podcast go to https://lexfridman.com/ai or connect with @lexfridman on Twitter, LinkedIn, Facebook, Medium, or YouTube where you can watch the video versions of these conversations. If you enjoy the podcast, please rate it 5 stars on iTunes or support it on Patreon.

Transcript
Discussion (0)
Starting point is 00:00:00 The following is a conversation with Paola Aralada. She is a professor of stem cell and regenerative biology at Harvard University and is interested in understanding the molecular laws that govern the birth, differentiation, and assembly of the human brain's cerebral cortex. She explores the complexity of the brain by studying and engineering elements of how the brain develops. This was a fascinating conversation to me. It's part of the Artificial Intelligence Podcast.
Starting point is 00:00:28 If you enjoy it, subscribe to my YouTube, give it 5 stars and iTunes, support it on Patreon, or simply connect with me on Twitter and Lex Friedman spelled F-R-I-D-M-A-N. And I'd like to give a special thank you to Amy Jeffers for her support of the podcast on Patreon. She's an artist and she definitely check out her Instagram at Love Truth Good. Three beautiful words. Your support means a lot and inspires me to keep the series going. And now here's my conversation with Paola Arlada.
Starting point is 00:01:19 You studied the development of the human brain for many years, So let me ask you an out of the box question first. How likely is it that there's intelligent life out there in the universe outside of earth with something like the human brain? So I can put it another way. How unlikely is the human brain? How difficult is it to build a thing through the evolutionary process? Well, it has happened here, right, on this planet. Once, yes. Once. So that simply tells you that it could, of course, happen again.
Starting point is 00:01:57 Other places is only a matter of probability, what the probability that you would get a brain, like the ones that we have, like the human brain. So how difficult is it to make the human brain? It's pretty difficult. But most importantly, I guess we know very little about how this process really happens. And there is a reason for that,
Starting point is 00:02:22 actually multiple reasons for that. Most of what we know about how the mammalian brains, or the brain of mammals, develop, comes from studying in labs, other brains, not our own brain, the brain of mice, for example. But if I showed you a picture of a mouse brain, and then you put it next to a picture of a human brain, they don't look at all like each other. So they're very different. And therefore there is a limit to what you can learn about how the human brain is made by studying the mouse brain.
Starting point is 00:02:56 There is a huge value in studying the mouse brain. There are many things that we have learned, but it's not the same thing. So, in having studied the human brain, or through the mouse, and through other methodologies that we'll talk about, do you have a sense, I mean, you're one of the experts in the world, how much do you feel, you know, about the brain? And how much, how often do you find yourself in awe of this mysterious thing? Yeah, you pretty much find yourself in awe all the time. It's an amazing process, it's a process by which, by means that we don't fully understand,
Starting point is 00:03:37 at the very beginning of embryogenesis, the structure called the neural tube, literally self-assembles. And it happens in an embryo and it can happen also from stem cells in a dish. Okay. And then from there, this stem cells that are present within the neural tube give rise to all of the thousands and thousands of different cell types are present in the brain through time, right? as an end of the brain through time, right? With the interesting, very intriguing, interesting observation is that the time that it takes for the human brain to be made, it's human time, meaning that for me and you,
Starting point is 00:04:18 it took almost nine months of gestation to build the brain and then another 20 years of learning postnatally to get the brain. We have today that allows us to this conversation. A mouse takes 20 days or so to for an embryo to be born. And so the brain is built in a much shorter period of time. And the beauty of it is that if you take mouse stem cells and you put them in a cultured dish, the brain, the brain organoid that you get from a mouse is formed faster than if you took human stem cells and put them in the dish and let them make a human brain organoid.
Starting point is 00:04:58 So the very developmental process is... controlled by the speed of the species. is controlled by the speed of the species. Which means it's by its own purpose, it's not accidental, or there is something in that temporal, it's very good, exactly, that is very important for us to get the brain. We have, and we can speculate for why that is.
Starting point is 00:05:24 It takes us a long time as human beings after we're born to learn all the things that we have to learn to have the adult brain. It's actually 20 years. Think about it. From when a baby is born to when a teenager goes through puberty to adults, it's a long time. Do you think you can maybe talk through the first few months
Starting point is 00:05:48 and then on to the first 20 years and then for the rest of their lives? What is the development of the human brain look like? What are the different stages? At the beginning you have to build a brain, right? And the brain is made of cells. What's the very beginning, which beginning are we talking about? In the embryo, as the embryo is developing in the womb, in addition to making all of the other tissues of the embryo, the muscle, the heart, the blood, the embryo is also building the brain. And it builds from a very simple structure called the neural tube, which is
Starting point is 00:06:26 basically nothing but a tube of cells that spans sort of the length of the embryo from the head all the way to the tail, let's say, of the embryo. And then over in human beings, over many months of gestation, from that neural tube, which contains stem cell-like cells of the brain, you will make many, many other building blocks of the brain. So all of the other cell types, because there are many, many different types of cells in the brain, that will form specific structures of the brain. So you can think about embryonic development of the brain as just
Starting point is 00:07:06 the time in which you are making the building blocks, the cells. Are the stem cells relatively homogenous, like uniform, or are they all different? It's a very good question. It's exactly how it works. You start with a more homogenous, perhaps more multi potent type of stem cell. That multipotent. That multipotent means that it has the potential to make many, many different types of other cells. And then with time, these progenitors become more heterogeneous, which means more diverse,
Starting point is 00:07:39 there are gonna be many different types of these stem cells. And also they will give rise to progeny to other cells that are not stem cells, that are specific cells of the brain that are very different from the mother stem cell. And now you think about this process of making cells from the stem cells over many, many months of development for humans.
Starting point is 00:08:00 And what you're doing, you're building the cells that physically make the brain, and then you arrange them in specific structures that are present in the final brain. So you can think about the embryonic development of the brain as the time where you're building the bricks, you're putting the bricks together to form buildings, structures, regions of the brain, and where you make the connections between these many different types of cells, especially nerve cells, neurons, right, that transmit action potentials and electricity. I've heard you also say somewhere, I think, correct me if I'm wrong, that the order of
Starting point is 00:08:39 the way this bill matters. Oh, yes. If you are an engineer and you think about development, you can think of it as well. I could also take all the cells and bring them all together into a brain in the end. But development is much more than that. So the cells are made in a very specific order that subserve the final product that you need to get. And so for example, all of the nerve cells, the neurons are made first, and all of the supportive cells of the neurons
Starting point is 00:09:10 like the glia is made later. And there is a reason for that because they have to assemble together in specific ways. But you also may say, well, why don't we just put them all together in the end? It's because as they develop next to each other, they influence their own development. So it's a different thing for Eglia to be made alone
Starting point is 00:09:30 in a dish than Eglia in Eglia cell be made in a developing embryo with all these other cells around it that produce all these other signals. First of all, that's mind blowing that this development process, from my perspective in artificial intelligence, you often think of how incredible the final product is. The final product, the brain.
Starting point is 00:09:51 But you're making me realize that the final product is just the beautiful thing is the actual development and development process. Do we know the code that drives that development? Do we have any sense? First of all, thank you for saying that it's really the formation of the brain. It's really its development, this incredibly choreographed dance that happens the same way every time each one of us builds the brain, right? And that builds an organ that allows us to do what we're doing today Right, that is mind-blowing and this is why developmental neurobiologist never get tired
Starting point is 00:10:36 Of studying that now you're asking about the code what drives this? How is this done? Well, it's you know millions of years of evolution, of really fine tuning Gina's Prussian programs that allow certain cells to be made at a certain time and to become a certain, you know, cell type, but also mechanical forces of pressure, bending. This embryo is not just, it will not stay a tube, this brain for very long. At some point, this tube in the front of the embryo will expand to make the primordium of the brain, right? Now, the forces that control the cells feel,
Starting point is 00:11:19 and this is another beautiful thing, the very force that they feel, which is different from a week before, a week ago, will tell the cell, oh, you're being squished in a certain way, begin to produce these new genes, because now you are at the corner or you are, you know, in a stretch of cells or whatever it is. And that, so that mechanical physical force shapes the fate of the cell as well. So it's not only chemical It's also mechanical mechanical. So from my perspective biology is this incredibly complex mess
Starting point is 00:11:54 gooey mess So you're saying mechanical forces. Yes, how different is like a computer or any kind of mechanical machine that we humans build and the biological systems? Have you been, because you've worked a lot with biological systems? Yes. Are they as much of a mess as it seems from a perspective of an engineer, a mechanical engineer? Yeah. They are much more prone to taking alternative routes, right?
Starting point is 00:12:28 So if you, we go back to printing a brain versus developing a brain. Of course, if you've printed a brain, given that you start with the same building blocks, the same cells, you could potentially printed the same way every time, but that final brain may now work the same way as a brain built during development does, because the build, very, very same building blocks that you're using, developed in a completely different environment, right, that was not the environment
Starting point is 00:12:58 of the brain, therefore they're gonna be different, just by definition. So if you instead use development to build let's say a brain organoid which maybe we'll be talking about in a few minutes. For sure. Those things are fascinating. Yes. So if you use processes of development, then when you watch it, you can see that sometimes things can go wrong in some organoids and by wrong I mean different one organoid from the next.
Starting point is 00:13:27 While if you think about that embryo, it always goes right. So it's this development it's for as complex as it is. Every time a baby is born has, you know, with very few exceptions, the brain is like the next baby. But it's not the same if you develop it in a dish. And first of all, we don't even develop a brain. You develop something much simpler in the dish. But there are more options for building things differently,
Starting point is 00:13:55 which really tells you that evolution as played a really tight game here. For how in the end, the brain is built in vivo. So just a quick maybe dumb question, but it seems like this is not, the building process is not a dictatorship. It seems like there's not a centralized, like high level mechanism that says,
Starting point is 00:14:23 okay, this cell built itself the wrong way. I'm going to kill it. It seems like there's a really strong distributed mechanism. Is that in your sense for me? There are a lot of possibilities, right? And if you think about, for example, different species building their brain, each brain is a little bit different.
Starting point is 00:14:45 So the brain of a lizard is very different from that of a chicken, from that of one of us, and so on and so forth, and still is a brain, but it was built differently, starting from stem cells that pretty much had the same potential, but in the end, evolution builds different brains, in different species, because that serves in a way the purpose of the species and the well-being of that organism.
Starting point is 00:15:13 And so there are many possibilities, but then there is a way and you were talking about a code. Nobody knows what the entire code of development is. Of course, we don't. We know bits and bits and pieces of very specific aspects of development of the brain, what genes are involved to make a certain cell type, so to self-interact, to make the next level structure that we might know, but the entirety of it, how it's so well controlled, it's really mind-blowing. So in the first two months in the embryo or whatever
Starting point is 00:15:46 the first few weeks months, so yeah, the building blocks are constructed. They actually, the different regions of the brain, I guess, and the nervous system. Well, this continues way longer than just the first few months. So over the very first few months, you build a lot of this cells, but then there is continuous building of new cell types all the way through birth. And then even postnatally, I don't know if you've ever heard of myelin, myelin is this sort of insulation that is built around the cables of the neurons so that the electricity can go really fast from the axons, I guess, the problems. The axons, they're called axons, exactly.
Starting point is 00:16:32 And so as human beings, we myelinate ourselves, posnatally, a kid, a six-year-old kid has barely started the process of making the mature oligodendrocytes, which are the cells that then eventually will wrap the axons into myelin. And this will continue, believe it or not, until we are about, you know, 25, 30 years old. So there is a continuous process of maturation and tweaking and additions and also in response to what we do. I remember taking AP biology in high school and in the textbook it said that I'm going by memory here that scientists disagree on the purpose of myelin in the brain. Is that is that totally wrong?
Starting point is 00:17:28 brain. Is that is that totally wrong? So like it's I guess it speeds up the okay, I'll be wrong here, but I guess it speeds up the electricity traveling down the accent or something. Yeah. So that's the most sort of canonical and definitely that's the case. So you have to imagine an accent and you can think about it as a cable of some type with electricity going through. And what Miley does is by insulating the outside, I should say there are tracks of Miley and pieces of axons that are naked without Miley. And so by having the insulation, the electricity instead of going straight through the cable, it will jump over a piece of Miley, right, to the next naked little piece and jump again and therefore you, you know, that's the idea that you go faster. And it was
Starting point is 00:18:10 always thought that in order to build a big brain, a big nervous system, in order to have a nervous system, it can do very complex type of things, then you need a lot of myelin because you want to go fast with this information from point A to point B. Well, a few years ago, maybe five years ago or so, we discovered that some of the most evolved, which means the newest type of neurons that we have as non-human primates, as human beings, in the top of our cerebral cortex,
Starting point is 00:18:45 which should be the neurons to do some of the most complex things that we do. Well, those have axons that have very little myelin. Wow. And they have very interesting ways in which they put this myelin on their axons, you know, little piece here, then a long track with no myelin, another chunk there, and some don't have
Starting point is 00:19:05 amiling at all. So now, you have to explain where we're going with evolution. And if you think about it, perhaps as an electrical engineer, when I looked at it, I initially thought, in our developmental neurobiology, I thought maybe this is what we see now, but if we give evolution another few million years, we'll see a lot of myelin on these neurons too. But I actually think now that that's instead the future of the brain, less myelin, myelow for more flexibility on what you do with your axons and therefore more complicated and unpredictable
Starting point is 00:19:46 type of functions, which is also a bit mind blowing. So it seems like it's controlling the timing of the signal. So in the timing, you can encode a lot of information. And so the brain, the timing, the chemistry of that little piece of axon, perhaps is a dynamic process where the miling can move. Now, you see how many layers of variability you can add, and that's actually really good. If you're trying to come up with a new function or a new capability or something unpredictable in a way. So, we're going to jump out a little bit, but the old question of how much is nature and
Starting point is 00:20:29 how much is nurture. In terms of this incredible thing after the development is over, we seem to be kind of somewhat smart, intelligent, cognition, consciousness. All these things are just incredible, ability of reason, so on, emerge. In your sense, how much is in the hardware, in the nature, and how much is in the nurture is learned through, with our parents, through interacting with the environment, so on. It's really both, right?
Starting point is 00:21:00 If you think about it. So we are born with a brain as babies that has most of his cells and most of his structures. And that will take a few years to grow, to add more, to be better. But really, then we have this 20 years of interacting with the environment around us. And so what that brain was so, you know, perfectly built or imperfectly built due to our genetic cues will then be used to incorporate the environment in its further maturation and development.
Starting point is 00:21:39 And so your experiences do shape your brain. I mean, we know that, like if you and I may have had a different childhood or a different, we have been going to different schools, we have been learning different things, and our brain is a little bit different because of that, we behave differently because of that. And so especially postnatally,
Starting point is 00:22:00 experience is extremely important. We are born with a plastic brain. What that means is a brain that is able to change in response to stimuli. They can be sensory. So perhaps some of the most illuminating studies that were done were studies in which the sensory organs were not working.
Starting point is 00:22:23 If you are born with eyes that don't work, then you're very brain, then piece of the brain, and then normally would process vision, the visual cortex, develops postnatally differently, and it might be used to do something different, right? So that's the most extreme. The plasticity of the brain, I guess,
Starting point is 00:22:43 is the magic hardware. And then it's flexibility in all forms is what enables the learning most natively. Can you talk about organoids? What are they? And how can you use them to help us understand the brain and the development of the brain? This is very, very important.
Starting point is 00:23:03 So the first thing I like to say, please keep this in the video. The first thing I like to say is that an organoid, a brain organoid, is not the same as a brain. Okay, it's a fundamental distinction. It's a system, a cellular system It's a system, a cellular system that one can develop in the culture dish, starting from stem cells, that will mimic some aspects of the development of the brain, but not all of it. They are very small, maximum they become about, you know, four to five millimeters in diameters. They are much simpler than our brain, of course.
Starting point is 00:23:49 But yet, they are the only system where we can literally watch a process of human brain development unfold. And by watch, I mean study it. Remember when I told you that we can't understand everything about development our own brain by studying a mouse? Well, we can't study the actual process of development of the human brain because it all happens in uterus. So we will never have access to that process ever. And therefore, this is our next best thing, like a bunch of stem cells that can be coaxed into starting a process of neural tube formation,
Starting point is 00:24:27 remember that tube that is made by the embryo ion, and from there a lot of the cell types that are present within the brain, and you can simply watch it and study, but you can also think about diseases where development of the brain does not proceed normally, right? Properly. Think about neurodevelopmental diseases that are many, many different types. Think about autism spectrum disorders. There are also many different types of autism. So there you could take a stem cell, which really means either a sample of blood or a sample of skin from the patient, make a stem cell. And then with that stem cell, watch a process of formation of a brain organoid of that person
Starting point is 00:25:15 with that genetics, with that genetic code in it. And you can ask, what is this genetic code doing to some aspects of development of the brain? this genetic code doing to some aspects of development of the brain. And for the first time, you may come to solutions like what cells are involved in autism. So, so many questions around this. So, if you take this human stem cell for that particular person, with that genetic code, how and you try to build an organoid. How often will it look similar? What's the reproducibility?
Starting point is 00:25:49 Yes, or how much variability is the flip side of that? There is much more variability in building organoids than there is in building brain. It's really true that the majority of us, when we are born as babies, our brains look a lot like each other. This is the magic that the embryo does, where it builds a brain in the context of a body, and there is very little variability there. There is disease, of course, but in general, a little variability. When you build an organoid, you know, we don't
Starting point is 00:26:23 have the full code for how this is done. And so, in part, the organoid somewhat builds itself because there are some structures of the brain that the cells know how to make. And another part comes from the investigator, the scientist, adding to the media factors that we know in the mouse, for example, would foster a certain step of development. But it's very limited. And so as a result, the kind of product you get in the end is much more reduction.
Starting point is 00:26:55 It's much more simple than what you get in vivo in mimics early events of development as of today. And it doesn't build very complex type of anatomy and structure does not as of today Which happens instead in in vivo and also the variability that you see One organ to the next Tends to be higher than will you compare and embryo to the next? So okay, then the next question is how hard and maybe another flip side of that
Starting point is 00:27:26 expensive is it to go from one stem cell to an organoid? How many can you build in like, this sounds very complicated. It's work definitely and it's money definitely, but you can really grow a very high number of these organoids. You know, can go perhaps, I told you the maximum they become about five millimeters in diameter. Which is the result of that. So this is about the size of a tiny, tiny, you know, raising. Yeah. Or perhaps the seat of an apple.
Starting point is 00:27:58 Yeah. And so you can grow 50 to 100 of those inside one big bioreactors, which are these flasks where the media provides nutrients for the organoids. So the problem is not to grow more or less of them. It's really to figure out how to grow them in a way that they are more and more reproducible. For example, organoids who can be used to study a biological process, because if you have too much of variability, then you never know if what you see is just an exception or really the rule. So what is an organ that look like? Are there different neurons already emerging? Well, first, can you tell me what kind of neurons are there?
Starting point is 00:28:46 Yes. Are they sort of all the same? Are they not all the same? How much do we understand? And how much of that variance, if any, can exist in organoids? Yes. So you could grow. I told you that the brain has different parts. So the cerebral cortex is on top, the top part of the brain, but there is another region called the striatum, there is below the cortex and so on and so forth. All of these regions have different types of cells in the actual brain. Okay, and so scientists have been able to grow organoids that may mimic some aspects of development of these different regions of actual brain. And so scientists have been able to grow organoids that may mimic some aspects of development
Starting point is 00:29:27 of these different regions of the brain. And so we are very interested in the cerebral cortex. That's the coolest part, right? Very cool. I agree with you. We were here talking if we didn't have a cerebral cortex. It's also like to think the part of the brain that really truly makes us human, the most evolved in recent evolution.
Starting point is 00:29:46 And so in the attempt to make the cerebral cortex and by figuring out a way to have these organoids continue to grow and develop for extended periods of times, much like it happens in the real embryo, months and months in culture, then you can see that the many different types of neurons of the cortex appear. And at some point also the astrocytes of the glia cells of the cerebral cortex also appear. What are these astrocytes? The astrocytes are not neurons, so they're not nerve cells, but they play very important roles. One important role is to support the neuron, but of course they have much more active type of roles. They are very important, for example, to make the synapses, which are the point of contacts
Starting point is 00:30:32 and communication between two neurons. So all that chemistry fun happens in the synapses happens because of these cells, are they the medium and which? It happens because of these cells are they the medium and which be happens because of the interactions happens because you are making the cells And they have certain properties including the ability to make You know neurotransmitters which are the chemicals that are secretive to the synapses Including the ability of making these axons grow with their growth cones and so on and so forth
Starting point is 00:31:05 And then you have other cells around that release chemicals or touch the neurons or interact with them in different ways to really foster this perfect process in this case of synaptogenesis. And this does happen within organ. Or with organising. So the mechanical and the chemical stuff happens. Yeah, the connectivity between neurons. This in a way is not surprising because scientists have been culturing neurons forever and when you take a neuron even a very young one and you culture it eventually finds another cell or another neuron to
Starting point is 00:31:40 talk to, it will form a synapse. We're talking about mice neurons, we're talking about human neurons. It doesn't matter both. So you can culture a neuron like a single neuron and give it a little friend and it starts interacting? Yes. So neurons are able to, it sounds, it's more simple than what it may sound to you. Neurons have molecular properties and structural properties that allow them to really communicate with other cells.
Starting point is 00:32:07 And so if you put not one neuron, but if you put several neurons together, chances are that they will form synapses with each other. Okay. So, an organoid is not a brain. No. But there's some, it's able to, especially what you're talking about, mimic some properties of the cerebral cortex, for example. So what can you understand about the brain by studying an organoid of a cerebral cortex?
Starting point is 00:32:37 I can literally study how all this incredible diversity of cell type, all these many, many different classes of cells. How are they made? How do they look like? What do they need to be made properly? And what goes wrong if now the genetics of that stem cell that I used to make the organ came from a patient with a neurodevelopmental disease? Can I actually watch for the very first time what may have gone wrong
Starting point is 00:33:05 years before in this kid when its own brain was being made? Think about that look. In a way, it's a little tiny rudimentary window into the past, into the time when that brain, in a kid that had this neurodevelopmental disease was being made. And I think that's unbelievably powerful because today we have no idea of what cell types we barely know what brain regions are affected in these diseases. Now we have an experimental system that we can study in the lab and we can ask what are the cells affected?
Starting point is 00:33:44 When during development, things went wrong. What are the molecules, among the many, many different molecules that control brain development? Which ones are the ones that really messed up here, and we want perhaps to fix? And what is really the final product? Is it a less strong kind of circuit and brain? Is it a brain that lacks a cell type?
Starting point is 00:34:06 Is it what is it? Because then we can think about treatment and care for these patients that is informed rather than just based on current diagnostics. So how hard is it to detect through the development of process? It's a super exciting tool to see how different conditions develop. How hard is it to detect that, wait a minute, this is abnormal development?
Starting point is 00:34:36 Yeah. How much signals there, how much of it is it a mess? Because things can go wrong at multiple levels, right? You could have a cell that is born and built, but then doesn't work properly, or a cell that is not even born, or a cell that doesn't interact with other cells differently and so on and so forth.
Starting point is 00:34:58 So today we have technology that we did not have even five years ago that allows us to look, for example, at the molecular picture of a cell, of a single cell in a CO cells with high precision. And so that molecular information, where you compare many, many single cells for the genes that they produce between a control, individual and an individual with a neurodevelopmental disease,
Starting point is 00:35:26 that may tell you what is different molecularly. Or you could see that some cells are not even made, for example, or that the process of maturation of the cells may be wrong. There are many different levels here. And we can study the cells at the molecular level, but also we can use the organoist to ask questions about the properties of the neurons, the functional properties, how they communicate with each other, how they respond to stimulus and so on and so forth,
Starting point is 00:35:57 and we may get abnormalities there, right? Detect those. So how early is this work in the, maybe in the history of science? So, so. So, I mean, like, so if you were to, if you and I time travel a thousand years into the future, or going to seem to be, maybe I'm romanticizing the notion, but you're building not a brain, but something that has properties of a brain. So it feels like you might be getting close to
Starting point is 00:36:35 in the building process to build this to understand. So how far are we in this understanding process of development? A thousand years from now, it's a long time from now. So if this planet is still going to be here, a thousand years from now. So I mean, if they write a book, obviously, they'll be a chapter about you. That's right, that science fiction book today. Yeah, today. I mean, I guess we really understood very little about the brain a century ago. I was a big fan in high school reading Freud and so on.
Starting point is 00:37:13 Still am of psychiatry. I would say we still understand very little about the functional aspect of just, yeah. But how in the history of understanding the biology of the brain, the development, how far are we along? It's a very good question. And so this is just, of course, my opinion. I think that we did not have technology, even 10 years ago, or certainly not 20 years ago, to even think about experimentally investigating
Starting point is 00:37:44 the development of the human brain. So we've done a lot of work in science to study the brain on many other organisms. Now we have some technologies which I'll spell out that allow us to actually look at the real thing and look at the brain, at the human brain. So what are these technologies? There has been huge progress in stem cell biology. The moment someone figured out how to turn a skin cell into an embryonic stem cell basically, and that how that embryonic stem cell could begin a process of development again
Starting point is 00:38:18 to, for example, make a brain. There was a huge advance, and in fact, there was a Nobel Prize for that. That started the field really of using stem cells to build organs. Now, we can build on all the knowledge of development that we build over the many, many years to say, how do we make these stem cells? Now, make more and more complex aspects of development of the human brain.
Starting point is 00:38:41 So, this field is young, the field of brain organoids, but it's moving fast. And it's moving fast in a very serious way that is rooted in labs with the right ethical framework and really building on solid science for what reality is and what is not. But it will go fast and it will be more and more powerful. We also have technology that allows us to basically study the properties of single cells across many, many millions of single cells, which we didn't have perhaps five years ago. So now with that, even an organoid that has millions of cells can be profiled in a way looked at with a very, very
Starting point is 00:39:26 high resolution, the single cell level, to really understand what is going on. And you could do it in multiple stages of development, and you can build your hypothesis and so on and so forth. So it's not going to be a thousand years. It's going to be a shorter amount of time. And I see this as sort of an exponential growth of this field enabled by these technologies that we didn't have before. And so we're gonna see something transformative that we didn't see at all in the prior thousand years.
Starting point is 00:39:57 So I apologize for the crazy sci-fi questions, but the development process is fascinating to watch and study. But how far are we away from and maybe how difficult is it to build not just an organoid, but a human brain from us, themselves? Yeah. First of all, that's not the goal for the majority of the serious scientists that work on this because you don't have to build the whole human brain to make this model useful for understanding how the brain develops or understanding disease. You don't have to build the whole thing.
Starting point is 00:40:38 So let me just comment on this fascinating. It shows to me the difference between you and I is you're actually trying to understand the beauty of the human brain and to use it to really help thousands or millions of people with disease and so on right from an artificial intelligence perspective. We're trying to build systems that we can put in robots and try to create systems that have echoes of the intelligence about reasoning about the world navigating the world. It's different objectives, I think. That's very much science fiction. Science fiction.
Starting point is 00:41:15 But we operate in science fiction a little bit. So on that point of building a brain, even though that is not the focus or interest perhaps of the community, how difficult is it? Is it truly science fiction at this point? I think the field will progress, like I said, and that the system will be more and more complex in a way, right? But there are properties that emerge from the human brain that have to do with the mind, that may have to do with conscious, and may have to do with intelligence or whatever, the we don't really don't understand even how they can emerge from
Starting point is 00:41:50 an actual real brain and therefore we can now measure or study in an organoid. So I think that this field many, many years from now may lead to the building of better neural circuits of that really are built out of understanding of how this process really works. And it's hard to predict how complex this really will be. I really don't think, we're so far from, it makes me laugh really, it's really that far, from building the human brain,
Starting point is 00:42:21 but you're gonna be building something that is always a bad version of it, but that may have really powerful properties. And might be able to respond to stimuli or be used in certain contexts. And this is why I really think that there is no other way to do this science, but within the right ethical framework. Because where you're going with this is also, we can talk about science fiction and write that book
Starting point is 00:42:50 and we could today. But this work happens in a specific ethical framework that we don't decide just the scientists, but also as a society. So the ethical framework here is a fascinating one, is a complicated one. Do you have a sense, a grasp of how we think about ethically of building organoids from human stem cells to understand the brain? It seems like a tool for helping potentially millions of people cure diseases or at least start the cure by understanding it.
Starting point is 00:43:31 But is there more, is there a gray area that we have to think about ethically? Absolutely. We must think about that. Every discussion about the ethics of this needs to be based on actual data from the models that we have today and from the ones that we will have tomorrow. So it's a continuous conversation, it's not something that you decide now. Today, there is no issue really. Very simple models. They clearly can help you in many ways without much, much think about. But tomorrow we need to have another conversation and so forth. And so the way we do this is to actually really bring together
Starting point is 00:44:13 a constantly group of people that are not only scientists, but also bioethics, the lawyers, philosophers, psychiatrists, psychologists, and so forth, to decide as a society, really, what we should and what we should not do. So that's the way to think about the ethics. Now, I also think though, that as a scientist, I have a moral responsibility. So if you think about how transformative it could be
Starting point is 00:44:45 for understanding, and curing a neuropsychiatric disease, to be able to actually watch and study and treat with drugs, the very brain of the patient that you are trying to study. How transformative at this moment in time, this could be. We couldn't do it five years ago, we could do it now, right?
Starting point is 00:45:06 If we didn't do it. Taking a stem cell of a particular patient and make an organoid for a simple and, you know, different from the human brain, it still is his process of brain development with his, with his or her genetics. And we could understand perhaps what is going wrong. Perhaps we could use as a platform, as a cellular platform to screen for drugs, to fix a process, and so on and so forth, right? So we could do it now, we couldn't do it five years ago. Should we not do it?
Starting point is 00:45:35 What is the downside of doing it? I don't see a downside. But if you were to put, if we invited a lot of people, yes, I'm sure there would be somebody who would argue against it, what would be the devil's advocate argument? Yeah. So it's exactly perhaps what you alluded at with your question, that you are making a enabling, you know, some process of formation of the brain that could be misused at some point, or that could be showing properties that, ethically, we don't want to see in a tissue. So today, this is not an issue. And so you just gain dramatically from the science without,
Starting point is 00:46:27 because the system is so simple and so different in a way from the actual brain. But because it is the brain, we have an obligation to really consider all of this, right? And again, it's a balanced conversation where we should put disease and betterment of humanity also on that plate. What do you think, at least historically, there was some politicization of embryonic stem cells, a stem cell research. Do you still see that out there? Is that still a force that we have to think about, especially in this larger discourse that we're having about the role of science
Starting point is 00:47:13 in at least American society? Yeah, this is a very good question. It's very, very important. I see a very central role for scientists to inform decisions about what we should or should not do in society. And this is because the scientists have the first hand look and understanding of really the work that they are doing. And again, this varies depending on what we are talking about here. So now we're talking about brain organoids. I think the scientists need to be part of that conversation about what is, will be allowed in the future or not allowed in the future to do with the system. And I think that is very, very important because they bring reality of data to the conversation.
Starting point is 00:48:05 And so they should have a voice. So data should have a voice. Data needs to have a voice. Because not only data, we should also be good at communicating with non-scientists the data. So there has been, often time, there is a lot of discussion and, you know, excitement and
Starting point is 00:48:29 fights about certain topics, just because of the way they are described. I'll give you an example. If I call the same cellular system, we just talked about a brain organoid. Or if I call it a human mini brain, your reaction is gonna be very different to this. And so the way the systems are described, I mean, we and journalists alike need to be a bit careful that this debate is a real debate and inform very data.
Starting point is 00:49:02 That's all I'm asking. And yeah, the language matters here. So I work on autonomous vehicles and there the use of language could drastically change the interpretation and the way people feel about what is the right way to proceed forward. You are, as I've seen from a presentation, you're a parent.
Starting point is 00:49:22 I saw you show a couple of pictures of your son, is it just the one? Two. Son of a daughter. Son of a daughter. So what have you learned from the human brain by raising two of them? More than I could ever learn in love.
Starting point is 00:49:38 What have I learned? I've learned that children really have these amazing plastic minds, right? That we have an irresponsibility to foster their growth in good, healthy ways that keep them curious, that keeps them adventurous, that doesn't raise them in fear of things. But also respecting who they are, which is in part, coming from the genetics we talked about. My children are very different from each other,
Starting point is 00:50:10 despite the fact that they're the product of the same to parents. I also learned that what you do for them comes back to you. If you're a good parent, you're going to most the time have, you know, perhaps a decent kid at the end. So what do you think just a quick comment? What do you think is the source of that difference? That's often the surprising thing for parents. Yeah, it can't believe that
Starting point is 00:50:39 our kids are there. So they're so different yet they came from the same parents. Well, they are genetically different. Even they came from the same two parents because the mixing of gamets, you know, we know these genetics, creates every time a genetically different individual which will have specific mix of genes that is a different mix every time from the two parents. And so they're not twins, they're genetically different. You just add a little bit of variation. Because you said really, from a biological perspective,
Starting point is 00:51:14 the brains look pretty similar. Well, so let me clarify that. So the genetics you have, the genes that you have, the play that beautiful orchestrated symphony of development, different genes will play it slightly differently. It's like playing the same piece of music, but with the different orchestra and a different director. The music will not come out. It will be still a piece by the same author, but it will come out differently if it's played
Starting point is 00:51:44 by the high school orchestra instead of the... Instead of the scale in Milan. And so you are born superficially with the same brain. It has the same cell types, similar patterns of connectivity, but the properties of the cells, and how the cells then react to the environment as you experience your word will be also shaped by poo genetically you are. Speaking just as a parent, this is not something that comes from my work, I think you can tell a birth that it's a different, that they have a different personality in
Starting point is 00:52:19 a way, right? So both is needed, the genetics, as well as the nurturing afterwards. So you are one human with a brain, sort of living through the whole mess of it, the human condition, full of love, maybe fear, ultimately mortal. How has studying the brain changed the way you see yourself? When you look in the mirror, when you think about your life, the fears, the love, when you see your own life, your own mortality? Yeah, that's a very good question. It's almost impossible to dissociate some time for me, some of the things we do or some of the things that other people do
Starting point is 00:53:03 from, oh, that's because that part of the things we do or some of the things that other people do from, oh, that's because that part of the brain is working in a certain way. Or thinking about a teenager, you know, going through teenage years and being a time funny in the way they think. And impossible for me not to think it's because they're going through this period of time called critical periods of elasticity where their synapses are being eliminated here and they're just confused. And so from that comes perhaps a different take on the behavior or maybe I can justify scientifically in some sort of way. I also look at humanity in general and I am amazed by what we can do and the kind of ideas
Starting point is 00:53:54 that we can come up with and I cannot stop thinking about how the brain is continuing to evolve. I don't know if you do this, but I think about the next brain sometimes. Where are we going with this? Like what are the features of this brain that evolution is really playing with to get us in the future, the new brain?
Starting point is 00:54:18 It's not over, right? It's a working progress. So let me just a quick comment on that. Do you see, do you think there's a, there's a, a lot of fascination and hope for artificial intelligence of creating artificial brains? You said the next brain, when you imagine over a period of a thousand years, the evolution of the human brain, do you sometimes envisioning that future, see an artificial one? Artificial intelligence as it is hoped by many, not hoped, thought by many people, would be actually the next evolutionary step in the development of humans.
Starting point is 00:54:56 Yeah, I think in a way that will happen, right? It's almost like a part of the way we evolve. that will happen, right? It's almost like a part of the way we evolve. We evolve in the world that we created, that we interact with, that shape as we grow up and so on and so forth. Sometimes I think about something that may sound silly, but think about the use of cell phones. Part of me thinks that somehow in their brain there will be a region of the cortex that is a tuned to that tool. And this comes from a lot of studies in in in in modern organisms where really the the cortex especially adapts to the kind of things you have to do. So if we need to move our fingers in a very specific way, we have a part of our cortex that allows us to do. So if we need to move our fingers in a very specific way, we have a part of our cortex that allows us to do this kind of very precise movement. And an owl that has to
Starting point is 00:55:51 see very, very far away with big eyes, the visual cortex, very big. The brain attunes to your environment. So the brain will attune to the technologies that we will have and will be shaped by it. So the cortex very well may be shaped by it. In artificial intelligence, they may merge with it, they may get an envelope it and adjust it. Even if it's not a merge of the kind of, oh, let's have a synthetic element together with a biological one. The very space around us, the fact, for example, think about we put on some goggles of virtual reality and we physically are surfing the ocean, right?
Starting point is 00:56:35 Like I've done it and you have all these emotions that come to you. Your brain placed you in that reality. And it was able to do it like that just by putting the goggles on. It didn't take thousands of years of adapting to this. The brain is plastic, so adapts to new technologies. So you could do it from the outside by simply hijacking some sensory capacities that we have. So clearly over recent evolution, the cerebral cortex has been a part of the brain that has known the most evolution.
Starting point is 00:57:12 So we have put a lot of chips on evolving this specific part of the brain. And the evolution of cortex is plasticity. It's this ability to change in response to things. So yes, they will integrate, that we want it or not. Well, there's no better way to end it, Paulo. Thank you so much for talking to them. You're very well.
Starting point is 00:57:36 This is very exciting. Thank you.

There aren't comments yet for this episode. Click on any sentence in the transcript to leave a comment.