Making Sense with Sam Harris - #149 — The Problem of Addiction
Episode Date: March 4, 2019Sam Harris speaks with Sally Satel about addiction. They discuss whether addiction should be considered a disease, the opiate epidemic in the U.S., the unique danger of fentanyl, the politicization of... medicine, PTSD, and other topics. If the Making Sense podcast logo in your player is BLACK, you can SUBSCRIBE to gain access to all full-length episodes at samharris.org/subscribe.
Transcript
Discussion (0)
Thank you. of the Making Sense Podcast, you'll need to subscribe at SamHarris.org. There you'll find our private RSS feed to add to your favorite podcatcher, along with other subscriber-only
content. We don't run ads on the podcast, and therefore it's made possible entirely
through the support of our subscribers. So if you enjoy what we're doing here,
please consider becoming one.
Welcome to the Making Sense Podcast. This is Sam Harris. Okay, very brief housekeeping here.
Once again, my meditation app is available at wakingup.com.
And if you're using the app and finding it valuable, Your reviews are incredibly helpful. Please leave those in
the App Store or in the Android Store. And any reports about bugs, please send directly to
wakingup.com. We are continually fixing those and pushing new updates, so please make sure you're
using the latest version. My major priority for this year is to make the waking up course as good as
it can be, so thank you for all the feedback. Today I'm speaking with Sally Sattel. Sally is
a practicing psychiatrist and lecturer at the Yale School of Medicine. She's an expert on addiction,
and she focuses on mental health policy as well as political trends in medicine and psychiatry.
And her most recent book is Brainwashed, The Seductive Appeal of Mindless Neuroscience,
which she wrote with Scott Lilienfeld. Anyway, we talk about addiction. We discuss the opiate
epidemic and the significance of fentanyl. We talk about PTSD. We cover the intrusion of politics into medicine.
We'll also talk about the ethics of organ markets, the buying and selling of organs.
Anyway, fascinating conversation. This is one that I hope will be of practical use
to anyone who either has suffered from addiction or knows someone suffering. There were a few connection and latency issues that you'll hear,
but nothing too terrible. This is what happens when you do these interviews remotely. In any
case, I hope you enjoy the conversation as much as I did. And now I bring you Sally Sattel.
So, Sally, you were recommended to me by our mutual friend Steve Pinker.
It was a fulsome recommendation of your expertise on many topics that we're going to touch.
And here you are.
So thanks for coming on the podcast.
Oh, well, thank you.
And thanks to Steve Pinker.
Obviously a great fan of his.
And yours.
I'm a longtime podcast listener.
So, yeah, so you were reminding me we met at one of those Beyond Belief conferences at the Salk Institute back in 2006 or so.
Yeah, I think it was more like 2009.
It was quite a while ago.
And it was very interesting. I think I was writing a book at that time with Scott Lilienfeld on the promise and peril
of neuroscience in the public square.
So that was a very important meeting for me, actually.
I learned about a lot of people's work there.
And I was familiar with yours, but I heard your talk.
And I remember I spoke on, since I'm a clinician, I'm a psychiatrist,
so I try to stick with clinical matters and see most things through that lens, you know, how
brain science, how junk science all refracts through a clinical lens. So I spoke about
post-traumatic stress disorder and how it is both, how it's both a product of brain and mind.
In other words, you know, mechanism, which is brain function, but, and meaning. And that in
my field, I think we've tended to be a little reductionist about it and see it largely through the lens of anxiety, of a fear
response that hasn't extinguished after the stressor has gone away, which is to me the essence
of continuing fear. And that's very highly legitimate. And of course, one of the best
therapies is exposure therapy, which touches on that mechanism. But there is so much more to post-traumatic stress disorder in
terms of what keeps it alive for people. And that often has to do with meaning. So that was my talk.
Yeah, I want to talk about that. I mean, there are many intersecting issues here with addiction and
the opioid epidemic and PTSD. And so I want to dive into all that.
But first, more generally, how do you view your work as a psychiatrist? Because you're sort of at
the nexus of clinical work on these various fronts, but also you comment on the politicization
of science and medicine. and you have been,
there's kind of a, to some degree,
a culture war component to what you've been doing.
So how do you summarize your approach to psychiatry?
Yeah, well, very much there is a culture war component.
In fact, I wrote a book back in 2001
called PCMD, How Political Correctness is Corrupting Medicine.
And then I collaborated with Christina Off Summers in 2005 on a book called One Nation
Under Therapy.
And both of those books had a very thick thread of politicized science or even junk medicine.
or even junk medicine. And in fact, in a way, so much of it comes down to,
the critiques often came down to explanatory reductionism. And as an addiction psychiatrist, that's my main field. And I do work part-time in a methadone clinic. I've done that for about 20
years. And this year, I'm actually spending the year in a small town in Ohio trying to
understand the, I even call it an addiction epidemic at this point, not just an opioid epidemic,
in a small town compared to an urban area. And there are lots of interesting differences we can
talk about. But the overarching, I guess, almost everything I've written has to do with some sort of perversion of the data or some
sort of questionable interpretation.
And so I would just give you an example.
Take, for example, post-traumatic stress disorder, since I brought that up.
A reductionist approach, and not an incorrect one, but just one explanatory level, would
be at the level of the amatory level would be at the level of
the amygdala, at the level of neuroscience. And I'm not saying it's illegitimate at all.
It's very real. It's very true, but it's just one level. And when you reduce things to one level,
we do that in addiction as well. Now, the dominant view of addiction is that it's
a brain disease. And anytime you reduce things to one level, it's obviously a precursor to
oversimplification. And when you're in the clinical world and policy world, that's usually a recipe
for a bad policy. And it's also a recipe for politicization because it can foster a victim narrative because someone, if there is a certain level of explanation that can be traced back to a perpetrator, then it becomes a victim narrative.
And any time, again, there's someone to blame.
And in the case of the opioid crisis, there's been much focus on, of course, the
pharmaceutical companies. And I do think they bear some responsibility, don't get me wrong,
but it also very much fits into litigation. But of course, as a clinician, I'm most concerned with
how it may undermine the best kind of care. So pretty much everything I've written about,
yeah, it goes to these kinds of oversimplifications and what's being left out. Now we have to be more
nuanced. Right. So let's start with addiction because it is obviously an enormous problem and
many people listening to this podcast will either have some firsthand experience with it themselves or know somebody suffering with
some version of it. What should we understand about addiction at this point? I should reference
another podcast I did, which I don't know, you may have heard. Do you know Johan Hari, the
journalist? So he's written a couple of books, one on the war on drugs and addiction,
Chasing the Scream, and the other on depression. Yeah, Lost Connections. Yeah. You know, he came
on the podcast and, you know, he's a great speaker and a very interesting guy. But, you know, he's
taken a line through both of those topics that seems to de-emphasize the role of biochemistry and the disease model,
certainly, of addiction, and puts the blame far more on the lack of meaning and lack of
connectedness that someone may experience in their life. And he draws a lot of motivation
from a few experiments. One is famously described as the Rat Park experiment,
which you probably know about. So in the aftermath of that podcast, I received some
angry pushback from people who didn't like that line at all. And I mean, in Johan's defense,
he doesn't actually discount the role of biochemistry, but if you get him talking,
he can certainly seem to. One question off the top is, is there much daylight between your view of addiction and the one he's putting forward? And whatever your view is, what do you think
people should understand at this moment about addiction? Yeah, I think there's a little daylight.
I agree with you. I think one could walk away from his excellent work. I admire him very much, but you
could walk away from that with perhaps an undue emphasis on the cultural, social, or psychological
dimensions. However, I think that, and I would say my profession or the addiction field, has over-medicalized addiction. And I don't say that
as someone who is not in thrall to the technology of brain imaging, but I think we have over-medicalized
it to the point where we put too much emphasis on the, I'll call them anti-addiction medications,
people call it MAT, and I'm referring there to methadone, buprenorphine, and then there's
another medication, naltrexone, which is an opioid blocker. These are all excellent medications,
and I use them every day. I prescribe them. And occasionally, there is a patient who gets on
methadone, and I would say he would fit the classic medical model, which is to say that addiction is something almost imposed on you.
Even we call it a person with substance use disorder.
And I realize in medicine we have to we have to give things shorthand names.
But I even cringe sometimes when I hear that because it makes it sound as if it's something that happened to you.
And addiction is a very intricate and deeply personal kind of affliction.
So, for example, basically, I see things on a large spectrum.
And as a clinician, you take people as individuals.
But occasionally, I'll see a person who says, all I need is the methadone
and I'll be fine. And usually that's not the case. They're on the methadone. So what does
that mean? Methadone, of course, is an opioid replacement. It's a synthetic opioid.
Actually, can you remind people, why is the transition from heroin or another opiate
to methadone advantageous at all? And why is the transition from heroin or another opiate to methadone
advantageous at all?
Why is it given as a treatment?
Well, if one is abruptly withdrawn or one loses supply to opioids and they've been on
it chronically and on a substantial dose for a while, even though some people, low doses
can even precipitate withdrawal when they stop it abruptly.
And that's basically just your body, which is already adapted. There's been neuroadaptation
to the chronic exposure. And so there's a withdrawal syndrome. And it can be very intense,
extremely intense, to the point where some people will continue using drugs just to avert the withdrawal.
People feel extremely ill. It's been called the worst flu you've ever had. Nausea, vomiting,
shakes last about 72 hours at its worst, and then it's over in about a week. Some people have
documented what's called a protracted withdrawal syndrome, which is sort of a low-grade
withdrawal, which could go on even for months. And so it's highly destabilizing. And you can't
break the cycle. A lot of people can't break the cycle on their own. I should add, many people do,
and clinicians don't see them. But the folks we see, obviously, have a very hard time stopping
drugs on their own. So to suppress the withdrawal symptom,
there's this replacement opioid. It's called methadone. And buprenorphine, which is a partial
agonist, methadone is a full agonist of the mu receptor, will suppress the withdrawal. And it
also suppresses craving. So as you can imagine, that's an excellent way to break the cycle and stabilize someone.
For most people, it's not enough.
It's necessary, but it's far from sufficient.
They have so much repair work to do.
Not only do they have to repair all the damage to their life that was done while they were
addicted, you know, all the bridges they've burned, all the relationships they've destroyed,
all the jobs they may have lost, the reputation, the health. There is also the problem of what predisposed
them to using in the first place. And this is where I'm very much with Johan in saying that
most, but not all, but most people I've treated, most addiction memoirs, well, all addiction memoirs I've read,
talk to a kind of psychic, profound psychic distress.
It often takes the form of self-loathing as one of the most prominent themes, but
other people want to repress painful memories.
Some people, I think, just should have been on a better dose of Prozac or something else
because they're using it to deal with anxiety and depression. And sometimes a conventional medication can be what they need, but other times it's a more
existential kind of lostness. And these drugs really help. They really do. And sometimes
they're a very good, you know, they're just a good numbing agent. In fact, I refer to them as
oblivion. I mean, you've heard of stimulants and depressants.
I have a new class called oblivion.
And that's what in fact, that's what morphine is.
Right. It's Morpheus.
I mean, from the god of Morpheus, who who lived by the river.
I'm going to mispronounce this because I'm not a Greek scholar, but left.
And that's the river of oblivion.
So these drugs, of course, have a profound history.
So that's what replacement opioids do. And that's huge, but it's rarely enough. Now,
occasionally there's a person for whom it is enough. This is a person, let's say,
for whom the withdrawal was so, or the avoidance of withdrawal was such a powerful engine for continued use that once you
took care of, as a clinician, you know, once we basically treated the withdrawal, the person had
enough social capital, enough, hadn't, you know, just had enough of a social network to be able to
get back on his or her feet just with a medication. That happens to be rare in my
experience, but it would happen. And in that case, I would say the person fit the medical model
more snuggly. But in most cases, in fact, we think of addiction, or we, I say we, because
Scott Lilienfeld and I have written about this quite a bit, we think about it as operating on many different levels simultaneously, obviously on
the neurobiological plane, but on the psychological one, on the behavioral one. It's incredible how
important cues can be, how important conditioning is in perpetuating drug use and also in treating drug use.
Because, of course, one of the first things you try to get patients to work on is identifying the kinds of situations, the kind of internal mood states, the kind of people that are around that get them craving. And that's a pure Pavlovian
phenomenon. And that's part of cognitive behavioral therapy for addiction is to get people
to recognize these things. And sometimes they're obvious. You don't drive by your dealer's house.
I knew of a school teacher once who had to get a, what do they call those things,
had to get a, what do they call those things, like a marker board as opposed to a chalkboard because the chalk dust reminded him of cocaine. Right, right. Wow. So what is the role of AA
in kind of framing our beliefs around addiction? Because there's this model that specifically an alcoholic is somebody who is irretrievably suffering from a kind of disease. And once an alcoholic, always an alcoholic. I actually don't have direct experience with AA or addiction, so I may be getting it slightly wrong in terms of just how they place emphasis on this,
but what's your view of the role AA has played in all of this? And in what sense is addiction
a disease? And in what sense does that analogy break down?
I actually don't consider AA the source of what I think is a problematic medicalization. I attribute that to the National
Institute on Drug Abuse, and we can get back to that. But as far as AA is concerned, interestingly,
in the early 30s, it did not use the word disease. But in any case, if you look at the 12 steps,
But in any case, if you look at the 12 steps, there's so much about, they do have a spiritual dimension to them.
There's a big emphasis on the so-called moral inventory, not moral as in you're a morally
flawed person, that addicts are morally flawed people, just that in many cases, so many,
I'll use their word, amends need to be made.
Just that in many cases, so many, I'll use their word, amends need to be made. And going back to, I suppose, what Johan would, where I agree with him is that so much addiction flows from so much personal unhappiness that you want to also go back to the origins of why you even became addicted in the first place. So I find AA, I personally have trouble with a higher power. I don't quite understand that. And I don't understand the surrender when, in fact, you're doing all the work.
So that because they have that and what are the steps, I surrender my will, I believe. In any case,
there seem to be paradoxes. But the point is so many millions of people have found it useful.
But the point is, so many millions of people have found it useful. But as far as it being a disease, I mean, I think if you took a poll, the majority of Americans see it that way. And I try not to debate it. And I do make a different distinction between disease, which is somewhat metaphorical, and a brain disease, which reifies it much more as a physiological problem and a physiological problem almost only.
But when people say to me, and I found this very interesting in this small town I'm working in in Ohio, that a few of the nurses and social workers have said to me,
they kind of lower their voice because they know they're being a little politically incorrect here,
but, you know, do I really think that you're a psychiatrist? Is addiction really a disease?
And I like the fact that they asked me that question. Now, if there were some crusty old
sheriff, you know, who just wanted to lock people up and didn't want his deputies to be
administering naloxone, you know, the overdose reversal drug, and didn't want to be bothered with these folks,
that would be a whole different discussion. And there I'd say, yes, it's a disease.
Because my usual response to that question is, what are my choices? Because my choices is that
it's a moral failure or it's a sin. Well, then I'm going with disease. But I'd like to be able to be more nuanced about it.
So when I've had these conversations, I'll just stipulate, for some people, it's very important to embrace that disease model. For others, less so. But I just say, so if addiction is a disease,
then it's most important for us to say, well, what kind of disease is it? Because unlike
one of the many slogans one hears lately, addiction is not a disease like any other.
And that's important to know. And I'll get into that in a minute. But I would like to say that
I acknowledge why the National Institute of Drug Abuse, which is responsible for this brain
disease formulation, and so many other advocacy groups endorse that, that I do see the virtues
in it. I understand that they were trying to, you know, wrest it out of the realm of criminal
justice. And I'm all, of course, I endorse that. They wanted more funding for treatment and research, and those are completely laudable goals. They think it can erase stigma. I don't believe it can. and cognitive behaviorists and others who have shown that the more you medicalize a
behavior problem, actually, the more you increase social desire for social distance on the part
of others and the more it induces a sense of therapeutic nihilism.
And there's also research showing that patients who endorse a brain, a disease model for themselves
actually don't
quite do as well because there's a sense of a loss of self-efficacy that goes along with that.
But again, these are studies and as a clinician, you deal with everyone on a personal level.
To be honest, Sam, it never comes up when you're treating someone. These concepts just never come
up. You just deal with how do you put one foot in front of the other?
And, you know, what are the skills you have to use to stay, you know, to stay clean?
And then at some point, people get enough sober time, absence time, where they can start
exploring if they're interested, what are some of the kinds of problems that preceded
their drug use in the
first place, because some of those vulnerabilities still exist and put them at risk. But we don't do,
you know, classic depth psychotherapy. We're not getting into childhood traumas or
primitive events because those are anxiety provoking. And that's the last thing you want
to do for a person whose habit has been to look for
a drug when they feel anxious. So for many years of therapy, I'm not saying people have to be in
therapy for many years. Hopefully they internalize a lot of these skills for themselves. But the
effort is very much pragmatic and I would say cognitive, behaviorally based in terms of therapy therapy,
and then in terms of rebuilding their lives, you know, again, vocationally,
getting their kids back if they've lost them, getting jobs back, regaining trust,
establishing a healthy social network, these kinds of things.
But that's interesting. So the classical talk therapy, you're saying in this case,
So that's interesting. So the classical talk therapy, you're saying in this case,
certainly in the acute stage after cessation of drug use, is counterproductive because just kind of endlessly taking an inventory of all of your past suffering that may or may not explain
how you got here just produces the negative mental states that people want to self-medicate
away from in the first place. Exactly. And some patients have said to me, well, you know, shouldn't I be talking about,
because I see a psychiatrist and they have this, because most people in the addiction world are not
treated by psychiatrists. They're treated by counselors or social workers. But I'm the
psychiatrist. So they, you know, maybe they have Freudian images, I don't know. But, and they say,
maybe I should be talking about my childhood. And then I explain just what I said to you, I explained to them and
they say, you know, that makes a lot of sense. And I say, that's a luxury you will have after
you've been, you know, after you've, after you've, you're stable for quite a while, if you still feel
that's important to you, then you, you know, can pursue it. And luckily they, they seem to,
you know, accept that. And of, they're free to go to someone else
who will do that with them. Although I think most people who are sophisticated about working with
people with drug problems would not do that kind of exploratory work in an early stage.
Hmm. Is it simply an empirical fact that people who kind of cross some line into a clear substance abuse pattern can't then go back and,
let's take alcohol as the normal social lubricant. Is it possible for someone to become a,
quote, healthy social drinker after having had a problem with alcohol? Or is the AA model of
once an alcoholic,
always an alcoholic, a fair description of the pattern?
I would not say that's fair, although it's very common. Certainly, it is probably not a good idea
for someone who's had a severe alcohol problem to attempt moderate drinking. Presumably they tried that along the way. However, there is a group and
it's, I think it's legitimate. It's called moderation management and it does have membership.
And then it has, there certainly is a subpopulation of individuals who can return to
control drinking. As a clinician, by the time they get, see, by the time someone gets to a clinician,
you have to remember there's so many layers at which people have peeled themselves off.
I mean, let's take this situation of two people who go, they seem to be matched on almost every
variable. And they're both curious about like what, let's say cocaine, because most people have,
I suppose, experienced alcohol, but they're going to a party and they know there's
going to be cocaine there. And they both say, look, we'll make a pact. We'll both try it,
see what it's like. And one of them tries it. And his reaction is, eh, which is actually most
people's reaction the first time they try cocaine. And most people's reaction the first time they use
a heroin is they throw up. Then the, but the other friend tries it and says, oh, my God, this is fantastic.
Now, that's very interesting.
And that's why I think more biologically oriented folks stop.
And frankly, you could build a whole career on figuring out why are those reactions different?
And I think they're mediated, you know, through neurochemistry differently.
But now here's another scenario where these two friends, two more friends, go to a party.
They know there's going to be cocaine.
One of them tries it and says, oh, my God, this is fantastic.
Give me more.
And the other one says, oh, my God, this is fantastic.
Get it the hell away from me.
And that's very interesting.
So that's someone who peeled off at the very first step. Then you have people who peel off in terms
of quitting use after they've used a few times and they came home late and their wife gives them a
dirty look and says, what have you been doing? And they don't think, okay, I don't want to go down
this road. Well, you can see where this is going. Then there are people who lose their job
or about to lose their job. And they think, wow, I better get it together. And then there are the
people I see who, despite so many of these consequences, didn't quite get it together.
Now, there's always one consequence that brings them in. And why that one and not the one before is the alchemy of addiction.
I don't know why.
There are too many variables.
Because everyone who's walked into our clinic practically is there because a spouse is going
to leave them, a boss is going to fire them, or a probation officer is going to violate
them.
And that goes to one of the reasons why I find the brain disease formulation, which
privileges so much the neurobiological level, why I find it problematic.
Because it takes our eye off several other levels of explanation.
One of them being that addiction is a behavior that responds to consequences.
It responds to sanctions and incentives.
And so if you read the early papers, in fact, the brain disease was officially
unveiled in 1997 in an article in Science. The definition why it was a brain disease is,
I kid you not, because addiction changes the brain. Well, this conversation changes the brain,
so that's absurd. But, this conversation changes the brain, so that's
absurd. But you could then more generously say, well, okay, in what way does it change the brain?
Does it change the brain in which people have no choice but to use or but to continue to use?
And we know that's true just because of what I told you, because people, because there's an enormous literature on contingency management, which is how you manipulate the incentives and sanctions to help
people stop. And one of the most fascinating, I'd say if I had to sum up all of addiction science
in one, in one vignette, it would be the Vietnam veterans experience, which I'll tell you. This was 1971. And I remember the New York Times in the
spring of 71 reported on the Department of Defense research on all the veterans, all the GIs
in Vietnam that were addicted, addicted, not just using, but addicted to opium and heroin and really good,
high-grade Southeast Asian stuff. And that's no surprise in a way, because what is war?
It's terror and boredom. And what are drugs good for? Terror and boredom. Plus, this was towards the end of the war,
and there was so much demoralization and such a sense of betrayal by so many that there was just
a simmering rage that a lot of these men had. So drugs worked for that. Drugs were totally
normalized in the military at that point, in Vietnam.
They were abundant.
So every possible variable that lowers the threshold for using a drug was there.
They had access.
It was normalized.
It was good quality.
And they had a reason for using it.
Well, Nixon was terrified.
And there was already a heroin problem in the
urban centers, and he was afraid that these men would come back and just seed that population
even further of heroin users. So he instituted a program which just has the best name in the world,
Operation Golden Flow. And as you might guess, basically it said, you know, for those of you whose year is up,
whose tour of duty is up, you will not be allowed back in the States until you pee in a cup and
there's nothing in it but your pee. And actually, once they were told this, the folks who were,
you know, about to leave, the vast majority of them were able to stop using on their own.
They did offer some treatment in Vietnam for those who had more trouble.
And then they left.
GI, well now veterans, were followed by Lee Robbins of Washington University, who wrote a paper in which she said this has blown, the title of it or the subtitle was something like,
the data I'm about to present now blows out of the water this once addicted, always addicted
meme. And what she found following these guys for three years was that very few of them resumed use
of heroin, 12% over a three-year period. The majority of those who resumed use had a prior
use, in other words, a use that predated their deployment. And that the reason, and she interviewed
many of them, a subset, and they said, well, you know, we had lives to
live now. We're back in the States. We have families. We have responsibilities. If we wanted
to continue to use heroin, we'd have to go into these terrible neighborhoods. Now it's easier.
People will deliver. But, you know, and of course it was totally stigmatized. And that, to me, is the full spectrum of so many of the dynamics
that are involved with addiction. Yeah. Well, the context clearly matters to a remarkable degree.
What do we know, though, about the behavioral genetics here? I mean, is it well understood that there is a gene or genes that govern a person's susceptibility to falling into addiction regardless of context?
I'm not a behavioral geneticist, but I'm going to say that whenever you're in the realm of behavior in humans, it's rare that one gene is responsible.
So everything and most things in psychiatry are
highly polygenic, but I have no doubt that there are some people whose circuitry is genetically
built so that they find their reward system is more sensitive, that their locus coeruleus is much more attuned to the
withdrawal phenomenon so that it's much less tolerable. And we have impulse. Of course,
there's the issue of impulse control. I mean, one becomes a highly steep discounter in the course
of being an addict. Some people are
steep discounters before they become one, and that probably predisposes them. But it's usually
a combination of many, many, many things. We know that, you know, so-called adverse childhood
experiences predispose, but they're all predisposing. And one could argue, for example,
that if everyone in your family were
an alcoholic, to the extent that anyone might use that as a justification for why they became
an alcoholic, one could just as easily say, well, you saw what it was like, you know, then it was
your job to not drink at all, something like that. So that can also go both ways.
Right.
So on the spectrum of difficulty in kicking an addiction, where do these various drugs
and substances lie?
I mean, can you generalize about how hard it is to get off of heroin versus the pharmaceutical
opioids that people are having problems with now versus alcohol and anything else?
Well, as far as opioids, a lot of this is obviously dose-dependent and often a route of
administration-dependent. But conceivably, it could be as hard to get off prescription opioids,
especially if you've crushed them up and snorted or injecting
them as heroin. There should be probably no difference. Interestingly, nicotine is considered
the most addictive drug, but that is highly conflated with the fact that smoking itself
as a behavior is addictive, highly addictive, arguably more so than nicotine itself.
The ritual of it and the social aspect of it?
Yes, the social aspect of it. But also the fact that there's talk about context. Of course,
it's hard to, of course, the uptake, it's called capture. The capture rate for nicotine is about
one in four. In other words, if you start smoking with some regularity,
you will probably continue to smoke with regularity,
whereas with heroin and other drugs, it's more like, you know, one in ten.
Why would that be?
Why would it be?
It looks like when you hear a capture rate is one in four,
you think, wow, that must be highly addictive.
But think about the context.
Nicotine is legal.
I mean, in the forum of cigarettes, nicotine is ubiquitous. Admittedly, cigarettes are much more maligned,
you know, nowadays and for good reason than they were, but still. And nicotine, and this perhaps
is one of the most important aspects, it's not an intoxicant. It doesn't affect your performance. If anything, it might
enhance it in some ways. So that the consequences for using, for smoking are so much less and so
much less immediate. And that's very important too, because of course you can get lung cancer
and devastating diseases, but they're so delayed. Whereas the consequences for intoxicants come much sooner. So all these
play in to the fact that someone would sustain their use, but that's over and above the base
addictiveness of nicotine itself. And that's also why cigarettes are so hard to quit. And that's
been misconstrued as nicotine being one of the most addictive drugs in the world, but that's not true. And where does marijuana fit in here? Is there an addictive component to it, or is there some
other category of compulsive use that shouldn't be categorized as addiction?
Actually, the physical addiction, the physical withdrawal that I explained before, that you'll
get from opioids, that you would get from alcohol, you would get from barbiturates, you would get from benzodiazepines like Xanax or withdrawal.
Those were considered the hallmark of withdrawal.
But ever since cocaine, ever since the 80s, that's been downplayed as an indicator of addiction because cocaine and the stimulants don't have that kind of
physiological picture. I mean, they have their own discontinuation syndrome, there's no question.
But so some drugs have that and some drugs don't. I have to say, I'm not that expert in marijuana.
I do know that because the potency is so much greater now than it was when, I'm sure we were maybe a
little older than you, but I think we were both, you know, when we were in college. We probably
had the same marijuana though. Yeah. So much of this, back to the concept of capture.
If you'd like to continue listening to this conversation, you'll need to subscribe at SamHarris.org.
Once you do, you'll get access to all full-length episodes
of the Making Sense podcast,
along with other subscriber-only content,
including bonus episodes and AMAs
and the conversations I've been having on the Waking Up app.
The Making Sense podcast is ad-free
and relies entirely on listener support.
And you can subscribe now at SamHarris.org.