Modern Wisdom - #041 - William Von Hippel - The Social Leap; How We Evolved From Tree Swinging to Human Being
Episode Date: December 2, 2018William Von Hippel is a Professor of Psychology at the University of Queensland and Author of The Social Leap. Today is nothing short of fascinating as we learn the story of how our ancestors went fro...m being chimps in trees to bipedal apes out on the plains and why we took that dangerous step in our development. We discover why our brains more than trebled in size and how that benefited us, what tools and tactics enabled these early humans to survive in a perilous new environment and what the implications are for our modern day minds dealing with primitive motivations. Further Reading: The Social Leap - http://amzn.eu/d/iFGKgsg Check out everything I recommend from books to products and help support the podcast at no extra cost to you by shopping through this link - https://www.amazon.co.uk/shop/modernwisdom - Get in touch. Join the discussion with me and other like minded listeners in the episode comments on the MW YouTube Channel or message me... Instagram: https://www.instagram.com/chriswillx Twitter: https://www.twitter.com/chriswillx YouTube: https://www.youtube.com/ModernWisdomPodcast Email: https://www.chriswillx.com/contact Learn more about your ad choices. Visit megaphone.fm/adchoices
Transcript
Discussion (0)
Hello, friends. This week I'm sitting down with William von Hippel, who is a professor of
psychology at the University of Queensland in Australia. His work's been covered in the
Australian New York Times, the economist Harvard Business Review, Time Magazine, and the
Sydney Morning Herald amongst many of the publications. And on top of that, William von
Hippel sounds like a barren from medieval times, which is it's the sickest name ever
So I first came across Bill's work when I heard him on Joe Rogan's podcast a couple of weeks ago
And I was absolutely fascinated very fortunately
We've managed to find a slot in Bill's book tour, which he's currently undergoing in America
And I managed to sit down with him to go through his new book, The Social
Leap, The New Evolutionary Science of Who We Are, Where We Come From, and What Makes Us Happy.
If you have ever wondered about how great apes and trees became bipedal beings out on the planes
and then evolved into the humans that you see before you today. This podcast is really for you.
There's a beautiful story element and a narrative behind what Bill talks about,
plus there's loads of implications for how we operate today.
Again, the same as with my podcast I did with Professor Robin Hanson,
the elephant in the brain, why we are the way we are nowadays
is a lot due to the environment that we evolved in.
But in other news,
I have got some messages about why there wasn't a podcast up
on audio platforms last week.
And if you do not form your own social media,
you may not know why.
However, we didn't know the YouTube exclusive,
what it's really like starring on Take Me Out.
After the what it's really like on the violin podcast,
we decided to double down if you don't broke, don't fix it. Sure enough,'s really like on Love Island podcast, we decided to double down
if you don't broke, don't fix it. Sure enough, the podcast has taken off really quickly and it's
been covered by some of the UK press, which is always good news, despite them misquoting me and
one of them misspelling my name, but whatever, it's fine. But we're back on a schedule. I've got
Johnny and Yusuf coming up very, very soon
to do some special edition Christmas podcasts.
I've also got the fattest, thickest, longest section
of recording that I've ever had to do
in the build up to Christmas.
So you may even get to a week
over the Christmas holidays, but don't hold me to it.
In the meantime, we're gonna find out
why we are the way we are and where we came from.
Oh yeah, PS. The first minute and a half might sound slightly different to normal. I was still
fighting with Skype at this point, but don't worry, I was victorious. Normal audio services I'm going to have a look at the video. I'm going to have a look at the video.
I'm going to have a look at the video.
I'm going to have a look at the video.
I'm going to have a look at the video.
I'm going to have a look at the video.
I'm going to have a look at the video.
I'm going to have a look at the video.
I'm going to have a look at the video.
I'm going to have a look at the video.
I'm going to have a look at the video.
I'm going to have a look at the video.
I'm going to have a look at the video. I'm going to have a look at the video. I'm going to have a look at the video. I'm going to have a look at the video. I'm going to have a look at the video. I'm going to have a look at the video.
I'm going to have a look at the video. I'm going to have a look at the video. I'm going to have a look at the video. I'm going to have a look at the video. I'm going to have a look at the video. I'm going to have a look at the video. I'm going to have a look at the video. I'm going to have a look at the video. I'm going to have a look at the video. I'm going to have a look at the video. I'm going to have a look at the video. I'm going to have a on. It's been a technological, catastrophy, so far, but it would appear that we've managed to tame all of the electronics that we're using to get this call to work,
which I'm really, really happy about. So thank you for that.
I totally my pleasure. I've got high hopes.
So the social leap you are currently on your book tour, that's right.
That's right. Amazing. So, the socially, what is it?
So that's the term I use for the kind of solution that our ancestors came to when they left
the rainforest and were forced to move to the savannah.
And so, you know, here they were, these sort of chimp-like creatures who were really at
the top of the food chain when they were in the canopy, but were very vulnerable on the
ground.
And so, how did they solve that problem?
Well, I think it took a few million years of sort of skulking around the edge of the Savannah,
but eventually, and I believe by the time we got to Australopithecus,
they had learned to cooperate and band together in their mutual defense.
And so I call this the, this leap from the trees to the Savannah
and then this increasing sociality that came with that leap and that in fact I believe started off
our entire evolutionary process that brought us to where we are now
that's what I call the social leap. Amazing. What was the process of researching the book
before we get into it? What was the... Sure. Is it a lot of deductive reasoning? Is it
analysis of... Well there's a lot of really fabulous archaeology and paleoanthropology and other forms of anthropology
that people have done to try to make inferences about where we came from, what were the cognitive
capabilities of our ancestors and their social lives like.
And there's not much to work with when you're looking at fossils that are millions of years old.
But these people have done a fabulous job of trying to infer cognitive abilities in the
deep past, and all I did was really come along and read this literature. I've got a wonderful
colleague, set of colleagues at UQ who are expert in these areas, and we have these
fortnightly meetings where we hash it out. We've been doing that for about the past dozen years.
I've finally got to the point where I felt like,
okay, I think I've got the story figured out.
And it makes sense to me how it fits with our modern psychology.
I think it's time to try to tell that story.
Fantastic.
So, let's start the story then.
Why does it begin?
So it begins six or seven million years ago.
We don't know exactly, of course, but basically the story
is as the tectonic activity in the Great African Rift Valley
increased, you got a lot of upwelling on the east side
of the Rift Valley.
As I gather it's a bit of thinness in the crust
or a hot spot or something, that's a result of the two plates
tearing apart, or the single plate becoming two that's a result of the two plates tearing apart
or the single plate becoming two plates,
starting up at the Red Sea and working its way down
to the coast of Mozambique.
So the whole right side of that Ethiopia, Kenya, Tanzania,
vast areas are big air, today about a mile up.
And what that appears to have done
is caused the rainforest to slowly dry out with the end
result that our ancestors had very little choice but to move to this savannah because the
simply wasn't enough forest left for them to make a living.
Right.
Okay.
So that move, it must have been very biologically expensive and very vulnerable for a while.
Absolutely.
I mean, I suspect that it was devastating
and that we, if you replayed that a dozen times,
I bet you've had 11 times out of 12,
all you get are a bunch of dead chimp-like animals,
but somehow we got lucky.
And you can look at cues of how we got lucky
by looking, for example, at the chimpanzees in Senegal
who live on the savannah,
and by looking, of course, at savannah baboons
and other primates who've chosen to live on the ground.
I suspect that the chimps in Senegal are a great choice,
because if you look at what they do,
they basically do the kinds of things
that I would imagine our ancestors would have done,
which is sort of skulk around the margins,
try to keep a tree inside at all times,
and make a break for it whenever a predator comes along,
because leopards, lions, hyenas, all those
would now be an enormous threat,
despite the fact that when they were in the trees,
they were not a threat at all.
Yeah, I get that completely.
So is there any crossover of chimps
moving towards those chimps, actually moving towards
the development that we had?
Well, I don't think so.
And they don't, they do well, I shouldn't say that.
They're showing some very interesting early signs that look a little bit like what I suspect that we did.
So, for example, they tend to share a little bit better than chimps typically do.
They're the only chimps that we know of that sharpen sticks into spears by biting them and then use those spears to
stab and retrieve monkeys that they eat, these bush babies out of tree hollows and
And they even sleep in caves. So there's some very interesting things that they do
But I actually think that the the shift that mattered was one that probably took a few million years. And that was an animal that was now exclusively living in the savanna and had become bipedal.
And bipedality in my mind, we can talk about where that came from, but in my mind it played
a critical role in leading to us because it changed the body of our ancestors from one
that wasn't not a very effective body, it body throwing to one that was now very effective at throwing.
By virtue of the longer waist,
by virtue of the muscles no longer being so vertically oriented
in the chest and shoulders and being more lateral,
and by virtue of greater flexibility in the wrist
because they weren't using it their hands
as a platform to climb trees as much.
All of those things would have enabled
much more effective throwing.
Okay, why is throwing so important?
So throwing is probably the most important military invention of all time.
That's throwing per se. I know that sounds outlandish, but not throwing per se, but rather
the capacity to kill at a distance. Right. So no other animal has that capacity,
and that capacity is mission critical to enable a larger force of weaker
Individuals to overcome a smaller force of stronger ones. So if you and I were and our friends were on the
Savannah and a lion came along and we want to do a attack and imagine we're all armed with knives. And there's enough of us.
We could kill the thing, but we know full well that whoever goes in first is going to be lying to them, right?
We'd all be going, you I'm after you, Chris.
You go ahead and I'm right behind you.
But you can go first.
Exactly.
And so, but once we could throw, now we could attack a lion or defend ourselves from a lion
at a safe distance.
And especially if there's enough of us all throwing stones at the same time, now we're
in a position to actually do great damage to the lion without suffering any damage to
ourselves. Right. Okay. So, that sounds a position to actually do great damage to the line without suffering any damage to ourselves.
Right, okay. So that sounds a lot to me. I deserve a show on Netflix in the UK called The Evolution of Us. Have you seen this?
I heard of this. I haven't. It sounds very interesting though.
So it sounds an awful lot like the story that you're going through at the moment. And on it, they had got monkeys.
They'd attached different microscopic dots that can be tracked by a computer.
And then they'd tracked their walking gate when they walked on a treadmill.
And one of the big differences that they highlighted between those monkeys and us
was the size of our glutes,
the monkeys essentially don't have any glutes. In comparison with us, it's the biggest
muscle in our body, right? And that capacity for us to be able to stand up straight and
to be able to run long distances, they highlighted this really big difference because when you
watch a monkey walk on two legs, it's pretty unwieldy, isn't it? It moves, it moves super weird. It doesn't
look like it was meant to do that. And I guess that's because it's not.
Yeah, that's right. So, um, chimps and apes, you know, and monkeys in general can't lock
their knees. They have no need to be able to do that. And so their whole body isn't shaped
properly for walking on two legs. Of course, they can do it. And especially if when you see
them in water, they tend to do it. And occasionally, they can do it, and especially if when you see them in water, they tend to do it.
In occasion, they just do it for other reasons.
But by the time we get to Australopithecus,
we were bipedal, and so there's those famous footprints,
quite an out of pronounceable,
lay of toly footprints, that Mary Leaky,
if I remember, I'd found, and when you computer model
of those footprints,
you can see that the footstrike is a vananimal
that had locked its knee.
And therefore, its full weight was coming down on its heel,
something that a chimpanzee can't do.
And so that makes, the good reason the wire antestases
would have become bipedal,
that obviously would have taken a while.
One of those reasons is probably this notion
that you were just discussing the idea of persistence hunting.
That it requires fewer calories to run long distances on two legs at a slower pace to chase
down antelope and other animals that will sprint as hard as they can and then soon enough to
become exhausted and can be killed.
So many people have argued that that's part of the reason why we became bipedal.
And I suspect it's true.
Any change that's that big is highly likely to have
multiple causes. I think that that was an advantage of bipedality. My personal view is that's not
what caused it. What caused is probably psychological factors. But the outcome of those of that change
was now an animal that's much more effective at throwing. And of course, the key to throwing is,
if you imagine one Australopithecus throwing rocks at a line, well,, the key to throwing is, if you imagine one
Australopithecus throwing rocks at a lion, well, it's going to end up in the belly of a slightly
bruised and annoyed lion.
But if you imagine 50 Australopithecuses throwing rocks at lions, now you get a world where you
could conceivably drive it away.
And so there would have been enormous evolutionary pressure on these ancestors to learn to work
together in a way that chimpanzees
simply can't do.
And that increased sociality and cooperativeness then would have kicked into a gear, all sorts
of forces that led to our brain expansion and led to where we are today.
Okay, so you mentioned psychological factors that you thought that was the reason that we
moved to bipodality.
What are those?
So if you look at chimpanzees, they're incapable of planning for unfelt needs. So, if it's hungry
now, it can plan to, like, maybe grab a branch, it'll go over to a shrub, it'll grab a branch,
it'll strip the leaves off, and then walk over to the termite mound and dip it in and fish
out for termites. And so, in that sense, it's planning for a felt need.
It's hunger and it's desire to eat termites.
But once it's finished fishing for termites,
it can't imagine that it'll ever be hungry again.
And so it literally throws this thick away
and off it goes.
And of course, there's lots of experiments
that do that in a more controlled fashion
where you find that exact effect,
that it's simply using capable of considering the fact
that it might be hungry
again or anything else. Now, if you look at our ancestors like Australopithecines,
there's no sign that they either could plan for unfelt needs. Now, of course, that's super
hard to know, right? They've been gone for millions of years. But the one thing that chimpanzees
couldn't do it, and then when we get after Australopithecus,
we get to homo habilis and the tools that they made,
these old-awwn tools which are very slightly sharpened stones.
And some people argue that Australopithecuses
were sharpening stones as well.
There's some controversy,
a evidence about that.
But either way, the tools that either Australopithecus made
or that homo habilis made have never been found
at any great distances from where they're acquired and made.
And what that suggests is an animal that's making a tool for its immediate use and then literally
leaving it behind.
Now, it's possible, of course, that it's easier to make a new one than it is to schlep it
with you.
We don't know, but by the time we get to homo erectus, they're making a much nicer tool.
It's bifacial.
It's really, the assualing tools are really quite lovely. And they're making a much nicer tool. It's bifacial. It's really,
the assualing tools are really quite lovely. And they're carrying a great distances. So we know,
at least by the time we get to the next species down the chain from homo habilis,
we know that we've got an animal that can envision unfelt needs. You know, I've used this tool now,
I'm going to want it again tomorrow. So then the question is how is that tied to bipedality?
Well, if you're a, and also if you're a template creature in the Savannah and you're setting out across the grass for food or for whatever purpose,
you and you cannot envision unfelt needs, some people said, well, maybe they developed bipedality in order to carry a weapon or to carry food.
Well, that's not super plausible in the sense that they're not trying to prepare for
the future.
If an osteopithecus was hungry, it would eat the food.
If it's not hungry, it would leave it behind because it won't envision being hungry again.
So then you have to ask yourself, well, what might an osteopithecus feel?
What would be its felt need every time it's set out across the savannah?
And I think it's super clear that you imagine yourself this, you know, three and a half
foot tall guy, setting out across the open grass, I think you're going to feel fear because
any, you're just, you're just available for any major predator, any large cat or dog
that's out there.
And so if you felt need, every time you set out across the savannah was fear, then what you would probably want with you
is some sort of weapon.
So if chimpanzees today who live on the savannah can chew a stick
and make a sharp point, I'm sure our Australopithecus ancestor
could do the same thing.
They had slightly larger brains than a chim.
And so my guess is that they're fashioning a crude spear or
club or even stone, and they want to carry it with them every time
they set out across the savannah.
Now, they may need to make a new one every morning
because when they get home,
they don't think they'll ever need it again.
But every time they set out,
they're gonna look for something like that.
And that, of course, will be a psychological pressure
toward bipodality because it's much easier to carry
a spear or some other sort of weapon
if you can walk on two legs.
I get that, I get that. So So why go out on the plains at all?
Well, because the trees are disappearing, you're in the no choice condition.
You've got like five trees that you're sitting in, but there's nothing to eat there.
Yeah. And so if you want to find food, if you want to get to another standard trees where there
might be other safety and other things to eat, you got to move along.
Right. Totally. You mentioned brain size differences in brain size there.
And also, obviously, there'll be big differences
in physiology.
What are the differences between the closest relative
that we've got and the Australopithecus in terms of brain
size and then in terms of structure?
So the Australopithecus has about a 450 gram brain
and the chimpanzees about 380.
So you've got about 3 million years on the savanna has given you about 70 grams of brain
now.
Not a whole lot.
Big investment for not a lot of returns.
Right, right.
Well, I think that that's, I think there's good reason for that.
And I think that good reason is is that brain's they're super expensive.
We just think of them as an unmitigated good because they can do so much for us.
But until we live in a world where you have a purpose for your brain, you're paying a big
price in calories.
Our brain uses 20% of our metabolic energy at all time, so that's a big cost, and what
return are you getting on that investment?
Well as a chimpanzee, what are you going to do with a little more brain?
It's not clear when you don't cooperate with each other, when you don't work together
well, it's not entirely clear that you're going to gain something to offset the losses. Part
of the reason for that, of course, is that they don't have control of fire, and so they
can't release nearly as many nutrients from their food as we can. So, Richard Rangham
talks about that and his wonderful book, Catching Fire, basically, in order to develop the
big brain that we have that's supported by such a small gut, you need to be eating a lot of meat and you need to be releasing as
many calories from it as possible, as many nutrients as possible, which is achieved by cooking.
Because raw food simply is, I mean, you can tell just by this sense of smell, if you
sniff a steak that's cooking up nicely, it smells beautiful, assuming you like to taste
to me, if you sniff a raw steak,
it just doesn't smell beautiful at all.
No.
And the same holds for potato.
A raw potato is inedible.
A cooked potato smells and tastes delicious.
And so that's our, that's our evolved understanding
of what's nutritious for us and what's not.
Yeah, I really wanted to touch upon this.
The way that I think a lot of modern humans,
if you don't look into evolutionary biology, sweet things taste sweet because they're sweet
is kind of like the sentence, right?
But that's not the case.
Well, no, because our ancestors were constantly in search of salt, sugar, and fat.
Those are the three things that allow us to
survive and that are in short supply on the savannah in the jungle anywhere else. But we lived in a
world where there was never enough of those things. And so we didn't devolve a psychology that says,
well, I only want a little bit. We evolved the psychology. I want to get as much of it as I can.
And so it's super hard to stop eating that stuff because our ancestors never worried about eating too much of it.
So the McDonald's hacking of sweet food and salty food
and all this sort of stuff, the binging,
when you're binging away on some fast food,
it's actually a primitive brain that's kicking in saying,
this is good for you, you should have more of this
because there's not going to be much around.
Right, it is that, but I would actually caveat that ever so slightly by saying that we have evolved
to get full when we eat a lot of meat because protein is the one
signal that we, it's the one food source that we, the macronutrient, that we really search out
because our bodies are built to protein and we need to get enough protein in our diet
in order to survive.
And so humans and crickets and all the other animals that have been tested basically use
protein as a guide to indicate when they're full.
This is really beginning with wonderful research by Steve Simpson, who's a biologist,
I spent much of his career at Oxford and is now at Sydney University.
And Simpson has shown, he's got a wonderful book, The Nature and Nutrition.
He's shown very clearly that if you vary the protein levels in food, animals will weigh
over eat carbohydrates in order to get enough protein.
But they won't weigh over eat proteins in order to get enough carbs.
Protein seems to be the limiting factor.
And I do believe that the way it works is that it makes you feel full.
And so when you eat a hamburger, McDonald's,
or a steak, or something like that, you go,
oh, I feel so full.
Whereas if you sit down with a box of cereal,
I mean, I could literally eat a box of cereal
while I'm watching television,
and not even feel it, not even know.
Oh, absolutely.
But I'll have consumed way more calories
than I would get out of a big Mac,
or a steak, or something like that.
Why is there this up-a-ceiling on the protein intake,
do you think?
Well, so think about our ancestors.
They would have, so today they get lucky
and they kill a giraffe.
They're going to stuff as much giraffe down their pile
as they can, but they're going to get to a point
where, all right, I can't eat anymore
of this, I'm feeling super full.
They would have only very rarely been in a situation
where there was so much carbohydrate that A, tasted very good and B, they wanted to eat.
And so there's just going to be little evolutionary pressure on them to detect that they're full
from carbs, but a great deal of evolutionary pressure to be able to say, all right, well
now I'm say that I've had enough protein.
Amazing.
And interestingly, the data also suggests that part of that is driven by lack of variety.
And so if you serve this interesting work with Amnesics, people who have such dense amnesia,
they won't even remember five minutes ago. And they'll interestingly, they'll eat a second lunch.
If you serve them lunch and then leave and come back and say lunchtime, they'll eat it again.
It's such a mean experiment if someone's trying to lose weight.
It is. It is a mean experiment. But it's interesting because what's happening there is that
they part of our sense of being full is actually our knowledge that we ate. And because they
don't have that knowledge, they don't have that sense as strong of a sense of being full.
But what's interesting is that if you give them less variety, like you say, well, here's
the one food that you'll leave for lunch, like you say, well, here's the one food
that you'll leave for lunch, these sandwiches,
they don't want those sandwiches anymore.
Again, suggesting that we've evolved,
Sarah, I've had enough giraffe,
I'm not gaining anything from this,
I don't feel like eating it anymore.
I get that.
There's a competitive eaters that are in ice cream competitions.
They offset the ice cream with french fries.
Because they want to do something to allow yourself to keep on, although I personally can't
imagine doing that, but it makes good evolutionary sense.
Fine. If the evolutionary psychologists and biologists say that it's okay, then the competitive
eaters have got it right. So, Australopithecus is now on the edge of the plains, maybe developed some tools and
level of cohesion within the group, and they can really rudimentary tactics to take down prey.
Where do we move from there? What do we roll forwards on to from there?
Great question. So now here we are with this animal that's barely gained any brain power,
but is bipedal and has a capacity to throw and probably has the psychological understanding that we need to work together and a tendency,
you know, a preference to cooperate because of course evolution works by what you want to do,
more so than by a knowledge GI off to do this and that knowledge would have been hard for such an
animal anyway. So you now've got a more cooperative animal that is sort of working together on this savannah. And that actually opens up all sorts of potential benefits to get smarter.
Because once we're working together, well, we could imagine division of labor.
Now, we can be a much more effective squad. If you go left, I go right, and
someone else attacks down the middle, we could bring down this animal, and we could
have it for dinner. And so all sorts of emergent properties come from groups
that can envision what each other are thinking,
that can find a way to work together,
that can plan for the future,
that can do all these sorts of things.
So, whereas in the past,
getting smarter would have been very little use,
now getting smarter is of great benefit.
So, there's some really interesting work
that came out recently on Notch 2NL gene, which
is a gene for brain expansion.
And what the day, it's new data, but what they suggest is that this gene likely appeared
in our line maybe 12 million years ago.
And the way it did is that it accidentally duplicated itself, but then this new version
was turned off, so to speak.
Now evolution often works this way because if you've duplicated a gene unintentionally,
you know, in some, in the process of myosis or wherever,
if there's an accidental duplicate copy,
now you can do things without duplicate,
without disrupting the function that the original did
because it's an extra,
so you can just play around with it, so to speak.
And so this extra gene seems to have emerged to 12 million years ago or so in our line,
and about 3 million years ago, around the time of Australopithecus,
it seems to have duplicated itself again and turned back on.
My guess is that it turned back on many times randomly between 12
million years ago and 3 million years ago, but whoever got that extra gene turned back on
Didn't gain much from it the cost of the of the extra brain was an outweighed by the benefits
But now you get to these social animals these Australopithecines who are working together and now the benefits of being smarter
Can outweigh the cost because we can gain emerging properties from our group if we can work together better and so
because we can gain emerging properties from our group if we can work together better.
And so you see rapid brain expansion.
So remember, the first three million years gained us
maybe 70 grams of brain.
Now you go from Australopithecus to homorectus.
You're moving about one million years
and you go from about four or 50 to about nine, 60 grams.
So more than doubling of brain size.
Now, admittedly, homorectus is bigger,
so it's got, it's more of a relative issue, but it's still a big change. And then admittedly, homorectuses bigger, so it's more of a relative
issue, but it's still a big change. And then you go from homorectus to us, and we're now
at about 1350. So they've essentially added an entire chimp brain onto homorectus in the last
million and a half years or so. So massive brain expansion that accompanied the
greater, the sociality that we gained on the savannah, and that enabled
us to suddenly become much more effective than we had been before.
So is brain size equal to power? Is that the way that it works?
Yeah, basically. I mean, it's complicated, of course, because elephants have ginormous brains,
and to the best of our knowledge, they haven't built great cities or done any of the things,
of course, they don't have hands either, but they do have fabulous trunk.
great cities or down into the things course, they don't have hands either, but they do have fabulous trunk.
But so it's an issue, it's an issue we don't fully understand, but there's no question
that larger brain gives you much more computing power.
Right.
And then you've said, as you've mentioned, a really clever giraffe, or a really clever zebra
has an upper limit on what the use is, and it's actually a very expensive cost to have that
brain, to have to consume all of those extra calories, but you are given more capacity to utilize
that brain when you are able to work together in a group and you said, Homeorectus 2 million years
ago, is that right? Yeah, a little less. Okay. And then where did they go from there? How do we
think that they move forward? Well, so Homeorectus, we can see signs that they actually can plan for the future.
So remember, I talked about how they carry their tools around.
We also see some really interesting evidence that they work together really well with division
of labor.
So for example, in making those tools, there's a site in India that's 1.2 million years
ago that Kerry Shipton has written about.
And he talks about how, as you look at the site
You can see that the original
Pieces of rock that they want to fashion into a tool are not loose in one location and then maybe 10 meters over to the right
They're originally worked on for a little bit to get them into the right basic form and then maybe 10 more meters over
They're now doing the final sharpening. Well
maybe 10 more meters over, they're now doing the final sharpening.
Well, if one, if one homorectus is making the tool,
why would it break it loose here?
Walk 10 meters work on a little bit more,
systematically walk another 10 meters of work on it more.
It probably wouldn't break it loose and sit down.
But if, but if people are working together,
if the homorectus knocks it loose, hands it to his mate,
who's better at the, at the second stage,
and he hands it to a friend, who's better at the second stage, and he hands it to a friend, who's better at that final stage.
Well, it would make sense that it's systematically being done at certain spatial locations around
the site.
So it's pretty good evidence for division of labor.
It's a primitive Henry Ford.
Yeah, that's exactly right.
It's like a little factory, right?
And then we see evidence that, so for example, up by the sea of Galilee,
there's a wonderful, just an anecdote, but it's an interesting one where an elephant has
either been killed or scavenged, and the homo erectus who are working there, we see a
bunch of hand axes, there are shoelace in tools lying around, and we see the head has been
broken loose from the neck and turned over. Brains are a wonderful source of fat because the myelin sheath around the neurons.
Brains are very, very high in fat and cholesterol, but remember that's what everybody's seeking
all the time.
Of course, it'd be a lot easier to get into a mammoth's brains through the base of its
skull than trying to break the skull through the top because they're such massive item.
Well, you could imagine a thousand chimpanzees crawling all over a mammoth skull. They're never going to effectively turn it over
because they can't work together. But homorectus, you know, everybody heav on three kind of thing,
you could imagine that's working together effectively to achieve something that they couldn't do
individually. And so examples like these suggested by the time we're at home or rectus, we're now super effective.
We're using our brain power to accomplish things that we could never
accomplish individually because we can work together as a group.
We can divide up tasks and we can even plan for these future
activities like a hunt.
You guys come over here.
We'll try to get the horses to run down this way here and
we'll dig a trap and we'll capture and kill them.
And there's evidence that they were eating horses and elephants, even massive elephants,
much bigger than today's throughout Europe. So mega-fowner elephants were being
felled by humans who had a brain that was two-thirds the size of hours, but they were so much,
they were so much more capable of working together. That's right. We don't know for sure that
they're bringing them down, right?
All we can see is maybe they're scavenging them.
But I think the data suggested they were hunting them.
If you look at the marks of the tools on the bones,
you can see lots of marks where, if they're scavenging,
like up high on the leg near the torso,
when animals hunt other animals,
they always eat that region first.
And yet we see lots of marks on the bones
up in the upper thigh bone,
where you wouldn't be working with your stone tools
if you were scavenging someone else's kill
because they'd be not there to eat.
That's already gone.
Yeah, so I think the data suggests pretty strongly,
I know this controversy also not everybody would agree,
but I think they suggest pretty strongly
that home erectus is now moving back to the top of the food chain and
they're doing that via their capacity to cooperate and work together. Fascinating.
That's so interesting. What are the what are the tribe sizes that we think at this sort
of stage? Ah, that's a great question. I don't know. The they're probably much smaller
than what we see today.
Now, there's a lot of argument about what's the optimal tribe size in humans even today.
And people have given numbers like 150.
And it is the case that humans are quite capable of processing the interrelationships of 150 other humans.
And Robin Dunbar is lots of nice evidence showing that.
But in actuality, when you look at hunter-gatherer groups, they very rarely are anywhere near
that size.
The preferred size is much closer to 20 or 30, because when you get groups too large, everybody
starts to bigger.
We've got a world with all sorts of laws and rules and all sorts of expectations that
are formally laid down that prevent that kind of
arguing and bickering and we're less interdependent.
So if I don't get along with my neighbor, it's not ideal, but I don't have to interact with
them all the time.
Whereas if we're hunting gathers and we're actually working together every day on the hunt,
I don't want to be with someone I don't get along with.
And so groups tend to splinter and break off into smaller groups when that happens.
And I suspect you need one bad apple, I suppose, don't you?
Exactly.
Well, they have good ways of getting rid of one bad apple, right?
Because there's no laws against it.
And so bad apples don't wake up one morning or when they do wake up, everyone's gone.
But if they're bad enough, right?
But it is the case that Homerus probably had the exact same issues.
And so they're probably traveling around in small groups that are a combination of family
and close friends.
And they're probably doing chimps and humans, both are what they call the sort of fission
fusion groups where larger groups come together and then they break off and go their separate
ways and they re-come together.
So long as their members are the same overarching group.
And so,
I don't know what homorectuses' language capabilities were if they had any.
It may have been all gestural, it may have been spoken, we don't yet know. But there would have been, even if it's gestural, there'll be meanings in some places that aren't quite the same as others.
And so there's probably in groups and out groups among homo erectus, just like there are in chimpanzees and just like there are in humans.
That's fascinating that there would be essentially different languages and dialects of these
gestural communication tools.
You could potentially have someone from another tribe that's really, really far away that
comes in and you essentially can't communicate.
I know that sounds stupid considering I'm next to France and I can't speak to a French person. But it could be very similar. I mean there's going to
be some basic gestures that everyone understands. But you know it's always the case that when you're
traveling in front country, we will say, don't use that gesture. That's a rude one here, even
though it's a positive one where you're from. And I see no reason why that hasn't always been the case.
That's fascinating. I recently did a podcast with Professor Robin Hanson
who wrote the elephant in the brain.
Yeah, it was a great podcast.
I enjoyed that.
Thank you.
Well, yeah, it was, I found that so fascinating.
What I really loved, I wanted to dip in there
if any of the listeners haven't heard,
that he had an evolutionary justification for gossip.
And you mentioned about the one bad apple
that gets left every evening.
And I found it completely fascinating
that he spoke about that bad apple
that has the social norms of the group
and the fair play gets,
it's enforced via the fear that the gossip
is going to oust you as being the bad apple.
So you want to adhere to what
the rules and the norms are, because if you don't, you know everyone's going to find out about it.
I thought it was so interesting to hear that there was an evolutionary justification for the water cooler.
Sure, and you know, if you think about it, it makes perfect sense because we all gossip.
We kind of look down on it when other people do it, but we are so happy to engage in it ourselves. Oh, really? Did you hear about so-and-so? And why is it so universally beloved to do it?
Because it plays such an important role. It's a way of managing reputations. And so if I live in a
very small group where everybody is in everybody's business, and I start misbehaving, well, the group
can bring me in line via gossip. And importantly, they can do it in a way that's very safe.
So imagine that I'm the big tough guy in our group and you and someone else are worried
about me in the way I'm throwing my weight around.
And you can bring it up in a very off-hand way, you know, sort of along the lines of,
what do you think of Bill?
Or not, that would be too direct.
You'd say, oh boy, boy, boy, did you hear what Bill did yesterday?
And if the other person then responds with, I must have been a wonderful thing. He's a great guy. You'd say, oh boy, boy, boy, did you hear what bill did yesterday? And if the other person then responds with,
must have been a wonderful thing.
He's a great guy, you know, you shut your mouth.
But if he says, oh, what was bill up to?
You'd say, well, I'm not totally sure what he meant,
but he did this.
And then you can see if the other person ratchets it up
and says, oh, what a jerk.
Or if he said, oh, I'm never liked him.
I mean, no harm.
Yeah.
And so Steve Pinker has this wonderful talk
where he talks about our indirect uses of language,
where, for example, when a police officer stops you and you want to bribe him, you don't say,
hey, can I bribe you? You say, you know, is there any way we could solve this here and now?
And you hope you'll say, well, if you give me the $50 I'll take care of it for you
and you give me the $50 drive off. But of course, if he says, no, you have to pay the ticket,
you go, okay, sorry, I thought maybe that we could do with it here. Now, you can,
that kind of indirect language could be used very easily in gossip if you're
unsure about the feelings of the person you're talking to and unsure whether they're going
to join your coalition.
And so gossip is super important for managed general reputations and it's much safer way
of doing so than if you couldn't communicate with the kind of complexity that we're capable
of.
There's only two forms, broadly, two real forms of communication, isn't there's words or written communication
or body language, and there's violence. That's it. That's right. That's right.
If one fails, you've got to do the other. Yeah. We've got this amazing communication
system that the other animals just don't have. And so, you know, we know that all animals can communicate.
But if you look at, for example, when chimps try to communicate what's going wrong or
what's going on, right, they're very limited in what they're able to get across to each
other.
And there's interesting cases of female chimps trying to communicate the danger they're
in to their male friends or partners and they just look at them online.
What are you talking about?
Whereas human beings, you know, one of the most amazing things
about being human is the cumulative nature of our knowledge.
And so because we can talk to each other,
I can tell you about what happened
when that lion nearly ate me out by the water hole.
And now you know how to handle it
without ever going there yourself.
And every generation we can build on the knowledge
of the past, which enables, we don't have to reinvent the wheel.
And children can learn what only geniuses knew just a few generations ago,
because it becomes part of our law, part of our way of doing things.
Totally right. I guess that combined with the ability to anticipate felt needs is a super,
super powerful, unfelt needs. Yes, sorry. It's a super powerful combination.
That's absolutely right. And so our extraordinary communicative abilities facilitate our groups and their capacity to work
together.
Our capacity, we call theory of mind, which is our understanding that the contents of
your mind are not equivalent to the contents of my own.
So chimpanzees can't do that bad.
They're partially aware, but not fully aware of the fact that what you know is different from what I know, because it requires the stability to put myself
in your shoes that kind of perspective taking, which is actually very difficult. Humans
don't even get it till they're about four years old. But once you get that, now, not only
can you work together more effectively, because I know what you know, but you can communicate
much more effectively as well.
If you look at the language of small children, even when they have the words, sometimes they're very hard to follow because they don't know what you know and don't know and they start a story in the middle.
But once you've got theory in mind, once I know what you know and don't know as a function of what you saw and what you heard and all the rest,
I can much more effectively communicate with you, we can divide up our tests much more effectively, and I can also teach you much better because
I know what you know and what you don't know, so I know where to start when I'm trying
to teach you that which our group does to kill the elephant or whatever it is.
That must also tie into lying that I'm going to guess and deceit that theory of mind.
Can you explain how that works together?
Yes, absolutely. Other animals are deceptive all across the animal kingdom and even the planking them and
animals and plants to see each other by trying to look like poisonous ones when they're not by trying to disguise themselves etc
But when I say trying I don't mean intentionally. I just mean that's what they evolved to do
Humans are the only ones who can lie in the sense of
mean that's what they evolved to do. Humans are the only ones who can lie in the sense of
intentionally planting falsehoods in someone else's mind. And so you can imagine that when you got theory of mind, you're the first organism to get it, nobody else has it. You suddenly realize,
well, gee, this is what everybody else knows, and here's what I know, I can benefit myself if I
try to plant some falsehoods in your mind that make me look better than I really am.
And when no one else knows that, and no one else has that capacity, it would give you
an enormous advantage.
So of course, everybody else involves that capacity because it's so effective.
And now what we see is children start to lie as soon as they get to the point where they
have proper theory of mind.
Now they'll tell us some very simple lies prior, like you say, did you do this,
and they know there's trouble, though, no.
But that's just, that's a very simple kind of lie
that doesn't really require me to know the contents of your mind.
It's just me trying to get out of it.
More reactionary, isn't it?
Yeah, exactly.
And famously, Coco the Gorilla,
who Penny worked with the assigned language,
famously told her once when she came into their trailer
or whatever, wherever he was living.
And he had torn Coco, I guess she had torn the sink right off the wall and then claimed
the kitten did it.
And so it's a great example of, you know, very simple.
I know I didn't do that.
You see a cat, he did it, but wildly implausible, right?
And so the little kids can do that, but once you get theory of mind, now you can start
to plant some very impressive lies.
And so the small children, there's some wonderful experiments where they'll teach them theory
of mind, and then the kids start to lie, if it benefits them, to their advantage.
And so until you get there, you can't effectively lie
because you don't realize that what I know
is different from what you know.
So why would you try to plant something in my mind
if you assume that our minds are completely identical?
Yeah, lying's are very complicated.
When you break it down, the fact that we do it
so naturally on a daily basis is more a byproduct
of the way that we operate
as opposed to the fact that it's simple. It's incredibly complicated task to achieve.
So, we're, home orrector, we've moved onto the planes where maybe you think tribes 20 to 30,
something like that with some primitive communication tools. Where are we going from there?
What's next?
Yeah, so now the question is, what happens now and then how does it lead to where we are today?
And one of the important things that would have happened with home erectus is now they've moved to the top of the food chain.
And so no longer is it the case that say we're two tigers or lions or even mammoths are important predators of theirs anymore, because as a group,
they're so effective that, although occasionally individuals will be killed by these large
animals, they're not really a threat to the group writ large.
But there is an important threat to the group writ large, and that is other groups of homo
rectus.
And so once you've got the vision of labor and all these capacities, really, the only
threat to you is other groups who have the same capacity and who might be competing for the same resources.
And so what that suggests is that we would evolve than a psychology where we cooperate really well with each other, right? Because that's what enabled us to start defending ourselves in the savannah, but we would not extend that cooperative nature to other groups. And so we certainly know this holds for Humpal Sapiens.
And I suspect it goes back to Homo erectus
that they've now evolved the tendency to be very cooperative within their group,
but a group from the outside may or may not be friendly.
It may or may not be on their side or against them.
And so they withhold judgment and they don't necessarily cooperate with them,
certainly not automatically.
It's a potential existential threat, right?
That's right.
And so that would have led to tribalism, ethnocentrism, and all the kind of genocidal tendencies that
we see in people today.
And the important thing to remember is that the Savannah made us co-operative and friendly
to each other.
But it did that in order to make us more effective killers.
So it's not a lovely, friendly thing without a negative side,
at least from a moral standpoint.
What it is is, by becoming cooperative and kind
to each other, our group works better
and can more effectively kill other animals
and potentially other members of our own species as well.
That's so fascinating that the basis for tribalism,
everyone talks about tribalism
and this bipartisan politics and this group
versus that group. To actually find a justification for why we are the way we are, it makes so much
sense. You wouldn't want to absolutely. What about the tribe from the other valley? What
if they're ill? What if they carry some some disease or some pathogen or what if they,
you know, I guess that's, yeah, there's so many reasons as to why we would be
wary. Right, exactly right. And in fact, you,
the pathogen examples a great one, that's separate from our
possibility of competing over resources and wanting to kill each other.
That's where we're accidentally killing each other. And as you get closer to the equator, there are more pathogens and tribes tend to stay
apart more for that very reason, because you know, a few and I live in Sweden and I've
never, you live a thousand miles away.
Chances are you and I have been exposed to the same very few pathogens that can survive
in that environment.
But if you and I live near the equator, well, even if you're only a quarter mile away,
you may well have encountered pathogens
and have a resistance to them that I've never seen.
And so if you and I mingle,
you could make me second kill me
without ever meaning to do so.
And as a consequence, I'm an evolved tendency
to stay away from you.
And of course, that's exactly what we see.
That's so fascinating.
So what areas are we talking about
that are mostly occupied by homo erectus
at this time? I think he said 1.2 million, 1 million years ago, something like that. Where
are we on the planet at the moment?
Okay, so probably by 1.7 million years ago, homo erectus have both stayed in Africa and
left. And so you see, homo erectus moving out of, you know, through Arabia into Asia and into Europe,
and they've basically occupied beginning around then and then extending until Neanderthals,
where they're offspring, they've occupied the lower half of Asia and they've occupied
almost all of Europe.
And of course, they also have colonized all of Africa.
And so those homorectus who stayed in Africa are the ancestors of homo sapiens.
Those homorectus who moved out of Africa are the eventual ancestors of Neanderthals. And
so when homo sapiens leaves Africa, we encounter Neanderthals. And as we now know, we interbred
with them a few times. And so we carry a fair few Neanderthal genes as well as some of
the genes of other offshoots
of homo-ractus who had left Africa and lived in Asia at least.
It's like two to five percent, right, of our genes.
That's right. Neanderthals.
And it's actually higher in white populations, I think, is that right as well?
Right. Well, there's no evidence that Africans
interbred with Neanderthals because, of of course Neanderthals didn't live there.
And so everyone who left,
we're Caucasians and Asians,
the every non-African in today's world
is a small subset of the groups that left Africa
beginning maybe 85,000 years ago.
And so those people appear to have left
and some took a right turn and worked
their way toward East Asia, some took a left turn and worked their way toward Europe.
And all of those people seem to have interbred with the Neanderthals. Some of them also seem
to have interbred with other subpopulations that came from homeructous like the Denisovans
from that cave in Siberia. We know that some humans have some of their genes.
But we haven't tested so many Africans yet to say with any confidence that they don't
have Neendothal genes, but so far, there hasn't seen to be any, and of course it would make
perfect sense that they don't, because they didn't leave Africa.
Well, Neanderthals, as well, would be very unsuited to being in that environment, right?
That's right.
And given that we wiped them out or killed them via disease or whatever, when we encountered Neanderthals as well would be very unsuited to being in that environment, right? That's right. That's right.
And given that we wiped them out or killed them
via disease or whatever, when we encountered them,
one can imagine that those who did work the way
toward Africa didn't meet a friendly reception
and that didn't work out very well, if it ever happened.
Absolutely.
In the Netflix series that I was discussing earlier on,
I found it really fascinating
where they explained why we have different colored skin.
They were talking about the melanin
that protects from the sun
and that when you close it to the equator,
that's more important.
But as you get further north,
that actually doesn't keep you as warm.
And it seemed so bizarre
that I'd never thought about it,
but the distribution of body shapes
and of physiological makeup, the inuit tribes and the people that
are in the north, they tend to be smaller with high levels of fat and then you've got the
best runners from the world, all come from Kenya.
This isn't a surprise, right?
Right, I mean, local ethnic groups adapt to the place they live.
It's not a very sensible thing to think about race, which is such a broad category that
it covers too many ethnic groups.
But if you think about different ethnicities, they make perfect sense that they have to
find a way to make a living where they are.
So my younger brother is a biologist who works up in the Bering Sea with a bunch of Inuit
groups trying to help them study the consequences of these perclerates
and stuff that have been left by the military that might be cancer-causing.
And so he was up there one summer and way up in Northern Alaska off the barren sea, and
the water is like one or two degrees Celsius in the ocean, and the local kids are running
in and playing.
Oh my god.
And if you went up to your ankles,
you nearly have a cardiac arrest
because it's just so cold and unbelievable.
But they've got this sort of thin layer of subcutaneous fat
that protects them in a way that people who aren't in,
you just don't have.
Yeah, that's so interesting.
So when we're talking about these tribes and stuff,
I really wanted to get onto discussing sexual relationships
and how partners and child rearing worked throughout the timeline that we're talking here.
Would you be able to explain some theories behind that to us?
Sure.
So the child rearing and partnering is complicated because human beings are so behaviorally
flexible that we've got lots of ways that we do it.
But there are some simple, not simple, there are some underlying rules that apply to all
of us.
And basically, the underlying rules stem from sexual selection, the notion that the Darwin
proposed that both have to find a way to survive, which is kind of what we think of with natural
selection, not dying when you get attacked by a predator finding enough to eat, but you also have to find a way to mate.
And if you don't find a way to mate, it doesn't matter how fabulous you are, those whatever
traits you have are not going to exist in the next generation.
And so sexual selection is that process of evolution that is dependent on both our ability
to attract a member of the opposite sex and our ability to compete with members of our own sex in order to do so.
So that applies across the board and all humans have been shaped by sexual selection because
we all have these traits that we try hard to have these traits that others, opposite
sex finds desirable and that we that facilitate competition with our own sex.
One of the interesting psychological consequences of that
is that we end up with this really unfortunate circumstance
whereby everything is relative.
And what I mean by that is that it doesn't really matter
how good of a guy I am.
What matters is how do I compare to the other guys in my group?
So if all the men in my group are sort of worthless, lazy,
stupid, and mean, well, it'll be pretty easy for me to get a girl
because I don't have to be very special. I can be pretty awful, but I'm still the best choice she has.
Right.
Whereas if all the guys in my group are really fabulous, I'm stuffed because she's never gonna choose me.
And so in the end, what people really care about, they're constantly looking around and asking how they stack up compared to others.
And the reason they do that is it's super important for me not to fall behind
everybody else, because if everybody else in my group is better, it doesn't matter how
good I am, I'm going to get left out of the mating game. And that's an unfortunate part
of our psychology, because it virtually guarantees in the modern world that it's harder to be
happy. I could work really hard to make a lot of money. And now I got more money than my, this neighbor,
but I then turn on the tele or I meet somebody
who's got more money than I do.
And I'm all upset all over again.
Yeah, totally.
It puts us on this hedonic treadmill
to be constantly comparing ourselves to others
rather than just having a set standard
of what we want to achieve.
Yeah, Alan DeBotten from the School of Life talks
about confidence in people being the highest
identifier of confidence in someone based on their profession is the
Incomand profession of their same sex parent
I'm interested and that level of that level of comparison
But it's it's so it's so well known. It's cliche? Keeping up with the Joneses. What is that?
Exactly.
Exactly.
And so the thing is that if I imagine that I invented a new pill and it made you twice
a smart and I gave it to you, well instantly you'd feel like you're some kind of genius,
right?
Because stuff used to struggle with it would be easy for you now.
But imagine that you then left your office and you found out that I gave everybody two
of them.
And so you only had one.
Everybody else had two. And now you'd feel that I gave everybody two of them. And so you only had one, everybody else had two.
And now you feel like an idiot, literally seconds later, because everybody else is discussing
things you can't understand.
And so sometimes comparisons make perfect sense.
You need to be at the same level of everybody else's.
But ironically, the same thing also happens because of sexual selection in domains that
don't really matter.
So imagine I told you, look, today is your lucky day,
I'm gonna give you just for being you $100,000.
Well, you'd be totally chuffed
until you left your office to brag about it,
and you found out given everybody else a million.
And then suddenly you'd be pissed off.
Why did he only give me a hundred grand?
Well, it's ridiculous to feel that way,
because your 100,000 still buys you
whatever 100,000 can buy
no matter what everybody else has.
But the logic of sexual selection now makes you think, oh, well, if everybody else got
a million, I'm going to be left behind and the girl's not going to choose me.
Is that an artifact of our sexual selection processing that's hacking our psychology for
a whole lot of unrelated other processes, right?
Well, it is.
It's an artifact that has both these unfortunate consequences and also a kernel of truth.
Because if I do give you $100,000, you are more attractive, but you're not more attractive
if everybody else got a million.
Yeah, that's totally right.
Is this unique to humans?
That's a great question.
I mean, the thing is that animal fairness is something that cuts across all the animals
that actually have a social system that can track these sorts of things.
So the most famous example is a study by Sarah Brosnan and Franz DeWall.
And what they did is they trained these capuchin monkeys to play this game where they would
give the monkey a pebble.
It would return the pebble and they would give it a slice of cucumber. And so we know that the monkey regarded payment of a cucumber
slice as fair because it actually learned the behavior and returned for a cucumber.
But now what they do in the critical point in the experiment is they have another monkey in the
cage next to them. And when that monkey returns the pebble, instead of giving it a cucumber like
they were like the first monkey got, they reward it with a grape.
Well monkeys, they far prefer grapes over cucumbers, and so then the question is what is our original monkey do now that it sees another one getting a grape for the very same behavior in which it's being paid in cucumbers.
Well when they then try to reward that animal with cucumbers, it gets really upset.
And often rejects the cucumber refuses to take it, and this is a great video that Duol has where he's giving a TED talk and you can see the
monkey literally throw the cucumber at the expressioner as he's like, as he's just totally
outraged.
And so it's a wonderful example of an animal sense of fairness.
Now I don't think that they think of fairness quite the same way that we do.
I think that would be anthropomorphizing, but what I do think is happening is that they're going, well, I cannot accept a reward, this less than the reward,
the other one gets around being left behind. And of course, it's not doing that consciously,
but that's nonetheless the psychology that underlies its rejection of a reward that it previously
thought was just fine. Yeah, I guess it's a very nuanced thing to be able to understand
comparing different, you know, your car versus my thing to be able to understand comparing different,
you know, your car versus my motorbike, your house versus my boat.
You know, that's very, very nuanced and that's subtle that humans do.
Whereas, you know, great, great versus cucumber, I think, yeah,
that's basic. Yeah, absolutely.
So I know that I discussed this with Robin Hansen and he had, he knows that this is such a contested
area about how monogamous the particular individuals were around about this sort of time.
Where does your current opinion lie with regards to monogamy within these developmental tribes?
So if you, there's lots of variability, you've got, you've got groups of
people who clearly show very little signs of monogamy and then you've got
groups of people where you've got basically just monogamy. And of course you've
got polygyny as well. My intuition, I don't know this but I believe it, is that
basically our ancestors were on average serially monogamous with a little bit of shenanigans
along the way.
And so what we see quite commonly in hunter-gatherers
is monogamous pairing that lasts for anywhere
from very briefly to seven or eight years even,
or sometimes even for life, but usually not forever,
and they sort of eventually go their own ways
and then repair up with somebody else.
I think that system was super common
because it gains you advantage of not putting
all your genetic eggs in one basket.
You know, when times change, maybe somebody else's genes
would be a better fit for the new world.
And it's consistent with our modern psychology
that it's easier to maintain passion for somebody
when they're new than when you've been with them
for a long time.
But I don't think that polygene was a super common system or any kind of polyamorous system.
And the best evidence for that I think is literally the signs of our testicles.
If you look at the testicles of all the great apes, you know, gorillas have very small testicles,
because they maintain a harem physically with their body strength.
They don't compete with no other male
as having sex with their females.
And it does actually take that much sperm to inseminate a female
so they can have small testicles.
Chimpanzees have enormous testicles
because everybody's having sex with her when she becomes fertile.
And so they basically need to wash out the guy before them
and have sperm competition where their sperm
gets there first. And then human beings, our testicles are closer to a gorilla than a chimp, but they're way larger than a gorilla. And that tells me that we're using where there is some
sperm competition in our ancestral history, but nothing like the level that we see in chimpanzees.
There's an argument to be made as well about penis shape, right? Did it create a vacuum?
Is that correct?
Yeah, that's a great question.
I don't totally know.
I've seen the experiment and I've seen this sort of penis plunger system.
It's super possible, right?
It's all these things evolve.
And in our case, our penises are way larger than the other gray dapes.
And of course, we copulate for much longer than most of the primates do.
You know, exceptions with bonobos who have these constant orgies.
But human beings, when they copulate, do so for a long period of time.
They do so when the female is not currently fertile because, of course,
she's hidden her fertility signals.
Obulation is cryptic in humans.
We can't tell.
And so, and that gets human males to be interested in human females across the cycle because
otherwise they might be left out.
And of course that makes both sides engage in greater pair bonding, which makes good
sense when you look at the difficulty of raising human babies to adulthood.
They take a lot of work by both parents.
And so, I suspect that the penis may well be designed to try to help suction out
the guy ahead of them, but probably the primary purpose is for bonding so that they have a good
time together and copulation can take quite a while. And so I think the reason our penis is so
large is sexual selection that that's what human females prefer. And so that's what human
males are like. And that's why of of course, we're so obsessed with it.
And the ideal outcome or the outcome
that evolution was wanting us to arrive at
was great pair bonding here, right?
Was a high level of investment between each other,
is that correct?
Yeah, so when you have a system like ours where
it takes so much effort to raise a human,
to a dozen years, minimally, to, you know, a dozen years,
minimally, to get them to be a reasonably effective unit
who can forge for itself.
That takes a lot of care and takes two parents
to achieve that.
And it is the case that relatives also often work together.
So particularly her family will play a big role
in helping raise the kids.
And we see this in all sorts of places.
But I do believe that part of what also
underlies that is pair bonding and his willingness
to look out for her and help her in return for her fidelity.
And so I think that's a deal that humans made
somewhere along the way.
It's not a mission critical deal for our intelligence
or our social intelligence, because I do think
that's more happening at the group level. But it does appear to me to be the deal that we did strike.
Well, it makes the most sense that the man cannot raise the child.
The man can't get pregnant, so they have to focus on what they can do.
Right.
Both sides, what can you bring to the table?
And you have to remember that females, the obligatory investment of a male, the minimal investment he can put into a child, is a teaspoon of sperm. The minimal investment
she can put into a child is nine months of gestation and ancestral societies, two years of
lactation. Well, that's a huge investment compared to his. And as Trimmers has shown us in
the early 1970s in his parental investment model, that means
that on average men will compete very fiercely for females because of course they're competing
for her investment.
Yes, yeah, I totally get that.
So, would there any other discoveries that you came across while you were writing the
book and researching it that you found that were really surprising?
Well, what's surprising about it to me
is how many implications it has.
So I'd be working on the book,
and then I would get a friend or a colleague
who would say, hey, can you come give a talk
about leadership or about happiness or about innovation?
And I would say, well, I don't work on that.
And they would say, well, but surely,
what you're doing must have implications for it.
And so I'd say, huh, let me think about it. And it always did. Every time somebody came with one of those requests,
I would give it a little bit of thought. And I'd say, well, wait, it really does have implications
for when and why we innovate, for how we lead, for what makes us happy. And so for me, the surprise
was just how socially, psychologically rich it is to understand and to think a lot about where
we came from because it tells us think a lot about where we came from
because it tells us a fair bit about why we are the way we are today.
Yeah, I think one of the definite conclusions that I drew from my conversation with Professor
Robin Hansen was that the world around us has moved an awful lot quicker than our evolution
can catch up with.
And I think we kid ourselves
into believing that we're a lot more sophisticated and a lot more in control, a lot more in control
of our unconscious mind than we would care to believe. And you know, when you're talking about
you say 80,000 years ago was when there was a fairly big split off and there was only 10,000 years ago, I think in and around the Philippines and stuff where there was the final last non-Sapiens
homo species.
Is that right?
The miniature, diminutive size.
Yeah, I see.
In Florida, I don't know exactly when they were, when their last stand was, I have to admit,
I'm not sure about that, but I suspect you're right.
Let's respect that numbers correct. Certainly, we were interacting with Neanderthals 20 and 30,000 years ago,
and probably for quite a long time, right? So for example, the first Australians arrived 65,000 years ago.
Well, if we got to Australia 65,000 years ago, we'd covered a fair bit of Asia and Europe by then too.
We'd had a lot of interaction with Neanderthals by then, and presumably with Denisovans
and others.
What those others might be, of course, we don't know.
But there probably was a lot of intermixing and a lot of interesting blending, because
really, we're interacting with cousins, so to speak.
Our ancestors, some of them left, some stayed. Sapiens happens to be a product of those who stayed,
but we're an inter-blended product with those who left.
Yeah. Do you ever imagine what it would be like if we'd...
if there was still megafauna around, megafauna animals,
or if Neanderthals had managed to hold on,
or if there was multiple different homo subspecies floating around.
Now do you ever fantasize about that or think about what it would be like in the modern world?
I do, and I worry that the story would not be a positive one, because we're so tribal
already when it's just other groups of humans who basically are the same as us, but with
some very slight appearance differences, it's easy for me to imagine we could be really awful
with people who really are distinctly different species.
And it's easy, given that there aren't any left,
I suspect we were awful with them,
that we're super effective.
And when we're not positively disposed
or others, we don't use that super effectiveness for good,
we use it to exterminate them.
We're genocidal toward each other. And so, if maybe if there
were still lawless sorts of cousin species in the world, it would be an easier
pleasant, lovely place, but I have the bad feeling it wouldn't.
Yeah, I'm tempted to agree, but I do think oddly enough, the two conversations I've
had with yourself and Professor Hanson, very strangely,
I feel liberated when I hear them. I think a lot of the time we beat ourselves up about
being less in control of our unconscious mind and our actions and our motivations than we
wish that we were. We get frustrated when we're in traffic and we get scared when we hear
loud noises. You know, all of the emotions that we feel, loneliness and depression and anxiety and everything,
both positive and negative, just artifact of a time that we evolved in that no longer exists.
And we're kind of trying to make this primitive brain fitter modern world.
Now, that's absolutely right because the greatest invention that we ever had was this idea of cumulative culture,
of learning from each other, of communicating that information over great times and distances,
and that moves so quickly, and we're such journalists that we have the capacity to do that.
But as you point out, that doesn't mean that our brain changes it anywhere near that speed, it just doesn't.
And so the things that make us happy, the things that scare us,
those early things are still in there. And I guess what I would argue is the important thing to remember is that we're not totally at their mercy, right? They don't control us, but they're an
important nudge that pushes us to feel jealous sometimes, to misbehave other times. And the key is
to stop and ask yourself, well, why am I feeling this way and why am I being so
aggressive towards so and so? And why am I not happy with what I've got here when I used to be and
ask yourself those questions? And I do think that we can retake some of that control simply by
virtue of the power of our frontal cortex, so to speak, and trying to say, well, I can't let
myself just be buffeted around by my unconscious mind,
which really evolved a long time ago and is much better suited to a world that doesn't
exist anymore.
I totally get that.
The race to the bottom of the brainstem, so to speak, needs to be counteracted.
I did a book with meditation expert, Corialin, and he's got this wonderful term that I love
yous and called the Mindfulness G gap. And he talks about in between
the action occurring which you need to react to and your reaction is this mindfulness gap.
And that one breath, that two breaths in between, it can make such a profound change. And you know,
that probably wouldn't be that useful if you're out in the planes. You want to be like a boxer,
you want to react before you even think. Whereas now, obviously, being able to step in between ourselves and our reactions is actually quite
a useful skill.
That's absolutely right, because of course, we no longer solve our problems physically
where speed is of the essence. We now solve our problems much more by negotiation and
verbally. And so, if given that, you're better off not necessarily flying off the handle
right away and giving your immediate response, but saying, well, hold on. And I love that
turn of mindfulness gap. I've not heard that. But yeah, Cory is super smart. So I'm not
surprised. It's absolutely perfect, I think. So the final question that I wanted to ask,
I'm aware that you're unable to answer this with any real certainty. But at the moment,
we're moving forward towards a world of AI development and you know, the world is changing so quickly that I suppose
any genetic mutations which were, which did provide any particular advantages would have
to be so generally applicable that they're probably not going to stick because of how fast
the environment's moving. The environment now is different to it was 150 years ago
and so on and so forth.
So if you were to put your money away your mouth is,
have you got any idea about whether we're going to see
a different homo species before,
is it going to be 500,000 years?
Is it going to be a million years?
Can you predict what would happen
if you were to put your money down?
Well, that's a great question.
And I'd be happy to predict it because I know neither of us will be here to find out
if I'm wrong.
And so I'll be able to stay with great confidence.
But in truth, our species is a new one.
Homo erectus lasted for almost 2 million years.
And we've only been around for two or three hundred thousand.
And it's hard to make an exact beginning point because of course it was a continuous process. And then the question is, is our incredible
effectiveness going to be our undoing or is it going to make us a huge success? And literally
you can't tell right now, right? It could easily blow us all up. We could develop a wonderful
AI system that some random psychopath reprograms to kill us all. There's a million ways it could go wrong, and only a few ways it could go right.
And we're trying to trick this sort of, as you call it, this primitive brain of ours
into living in this super complicated, super diverse, modern world.
So my suspicion is that if you fast-forwarded a few hundred thousand years,
we've got a 50% chance of having been really lucky,
and we're now basically biologically the same that we are now,
although we've done all sorts of tweaks to make ourselves live longer,
and we're probably a little cyborg-like,
machine-enhanced and all that kind of stuff.
But I also worry a lot that we don't get anywhere near that far because we just do ourselves
in either with our own inventions that we're about to come up with that it will then take
over or just blow ourselves up because that risk is always there and it's come close
many times.
Yeah, I think I did a podcast with Professor Adam Frankie, who wrote Light of Us Stars,
and he identified that one of the byproducts
of any world-gurdling civilization is going to be
the level of global warming that we have
because you can't have the energy you need
for the civilization without the byproduct
of the greenhouse gases and so on and so forth.
And then if you read super intelligent, fantastic book
on AI by Professor Nick Bostrom
from the Future Humanities Institute of Oxford,
you know, there's so many different ways that we can fuck this up.
Like so many different ways.
And it's nearly infinite.
I know, and it's bizarre.
It's so bizarre to think that if evolution hadn't created that mutation that took us from
a homo erectus to homo sapiens. The homo species may have
lasted an awful lot longer. It's so bizarre to think that you can overshoot brain capacity
to the point where it becomes dangerously proficient and that can actually be your downfall.
Right, and the irony is that it's the combination of individual brains and our incredible capacity
to connect and do each other, because I don't think a human on the planet could make an iPhone. But a lot of us obviously can make iPhones. And when we all
tweak it together, which we, that's what made us so successful in the Savannah, was our capacity
to work together. And that capacity could be the irony is that that was saved as could easily be
our undoing. Yeah, that's so fascinating. I have got a final question before we go. One of my
friends mentioned it before.
He mentioned that a lot of people enjoy the smell of fresh grass and
wondered whether or not that was harking back to the fact that being out on the plains was a natural habitat for us.
It's going to be very very difficult to work it out, but I thought that was an interesting thought that you know, it's yeah
That was an interesting thought. That, you know, it's, yeah.
Yeah, it's a great question because the, what are our natural proclivities?
You know, I argue that we all enjoy throwing rocks because, even from very young age, because
throwing stones was so important for our ancestors.
And even into homo sapiens, lots of modern, lots of human cultures, until very recent times,
still used stone throwing very effectively.
And so the smell of grass is another example
where that's a natural habitat for us.
It there's some very interesting work
that talks about what makes a scene beautiful
and what makes a place stand out
as a lovely place to live.
And the evolutionary arguments are that things
that afford us a wide view, we like that
because we could see enemies coming at us, things that are forest that are likely to have lots of game.
All those kinds of things are preferences that if our ancestors had it would have made them more successful.
Now you raised an interesting one, this idea of freshly cut grass or just grass in general. And I suppose that that makes good sense, because of course, grasses are a very unique species
in the sense that it can dry out.
The roots are still alive, but their animals won't be there.
But once grass goes, once it rains,
and the grass goes green again, animals tend together.
And so we may have evolved to proclivity
to light the color and the smell of fresh growing grass,
because it's a good sign that dinner must be around the corner.
Yeah, that's so fascinating.
So what you're saying is that in terms of creating a nice landscape or a scenery potentially
out of the back of a fancy, five-star hotel that the interior designers or the landscape
designers need to be calling an evolutionary biologists and psychologists to then come
and consult and they can say,
well, there wouldn't be enough buffalo over the far side, so I don't really think that we
can have that lake in that area.
Right.
Exactly, and if they pay us a lot, it'll work out better for everybody.
Fantastic.
Well, that's brilliant.
Bill, would you be able to tell the listeners where they can find you online, please?
Certainly.
So, at this point, the easiest way to find me is simply the Google Bill or William
Von Hipple, the ONHIPPEL. My book, The Social Leap, is available now in the States and will
be available soon in the UK and is becoming available in lots of other places as well.
So googling either of those two things is the best bet. I've set up a website
for the book, but it's so rudimentary at this point, I would ignore it.
That's fine. Well, I'll make sure that the link to all of your content is in the show notes
below. I'll make sure it's the social leap is available in the UK on the 27th of December.
So as I'm lounging around next to the Christmas tree, eating the remainder of my chocolates,
I know what I'm going to be reading.
I can't thank you enough for coming on bill. It's been absolutely fascinating.
I feel like we could have gone on for hours and hours.
So we may need to come back and give it another round too in the new year.
But I hope that your book tour continues to be very successful in the US.
Will you be back in, will you be back in ours in time for Christmas?
No, I won't. I won't get back to Australia until February because we're covering a lot of ground.
But I will get to the UK briefly, my sister lives in London and so my daughter goes to school in
Wales and so it's my hope to be noodling around there a bit as well. And it's been lots of fun
chatting with you and I would look forward to doing so again. Amazing. You'll have to drop me a message
once you're in the UK and if I can come down
and meet you for a coffee, that'll be fantastic.
Perfect.
Cheers Bill. Thank you very much for your time.
Good to talk to you.
Bye bye.
you