Modern Wisdom - #456 - David Robson - The Psychological Power Of Expectations

Episode Date: April 4, 2022

David Robson is an award-winning science writer specialising in the extremes of the human brain, body and behaviour. Our expectations have a profound effect on the outcomes we get in life. This isn't ...positive vibes from The Secret, this is one of the most replicable, robust effects that impacts pretty much everything we care about to do with our lives and health. Expect to learn how people with no gluten intolerance can have a gluten reaction after eating a meal with no gluten in it, why the drones at Gatwick were probably imagined, why French people eat worse than Americans but live longer, how your thoughts are more important than your genes when it comes to athletic performance and much more... Sponsors: Join the Modern Wisdom Community to connect with me & other listeners - https://modernwisdom.locals.com/ Get 30% discount on your at-home testosterone test at https://trylgc.com/modern (use code: MODERN30) Get over 37% discount on all products site-wide from MyProtein at https://bit.ly/proteinwisdom (use code: MODERNWISDOM) Get 15% discount on Craftd London’s jewellery at https://bit.ly/cdwisdom (use code MW15) Extra Stuff: Buy The Expectation Effect - https://amzn.to/3NzBIKh Check out David's website - https://davidrobson.me/  Get my free Reading List of 100 books to read before you die → https://chriswillx.com/books/ To support me on Patreon (thank you): https://www.patreon.com/modernwisdom - Get in touch. Instagram: https://www.instagram.com/chriswillx Twitter: https://www.twitter.com/chriswillx YouTube: https://www.youtube.com/modernwisdompodcast Email: https://chriswillx.com/contact/  Learn more about your ad choices. Visit megaphone.fm/adchoices

Transcript
Discussion (0)
Starting point is 00:00:00 Hello friends, welcome back to the show. My guest today is David Robson. He's an award-winning science writer specializing in the extremes of the human brain, body, and behavior. Our expectations have a profound effect on the outcomes that we get in life. This isn't positive vibes from the secret. This is one of the most replicable, robust effects that impact pretty much everything that we care about to do with our lives and our health. Expect to learn how people with no gluten intolerance can have a gluten reaction after eating a meal with no gluten in it. Why the drones at Gatwick were probably imagined,
Starting point is 00:00:35 why French people eat worse than Americans but live longer, how your thoughts are more important than your genes when it comes to athletic performance and much more. This book and David's work about the expectation effect has been completely consuming me since I had this conversation with him. I've spoken about it on a bunch of other podcasts that I've guested on. Every so often I do a podcast that completely sort of changes my view around something that were all very familiar with how we frame events and the sort of impact that this has. And this is a real, a very, very shocking episode
Starting point is 00:01:12 in the nicest way possible. There is so much to take away from today. I really hope that you enjoy it. It's genuinely changed my world view on a lot of things. So if you do enjoy it, then share it with a friend. This can help a lot of people. And if nothing else, it's really, really interesting. So yes, make sure that you do sit down and take some notes with the very least focus, okay? But now please welcome David Robson, look at the show. Yeah, thanks so much for having me.
Starting point is 00:02:01 How did you get interested in the way the expectations can impact our lives? Like I guess like as a medical writer I kind of know about the placebo effect for ages but then I just like over the last five years I've just seen like such an abundance of research that had kind of moved beyond like you know the effects in hospitals but to like all other areas of our lives so you know like So, you know, like there is some great research on these kind of weird psychogenic illnesses that pass between people just through expectations, but then also, like research looking at the way our expectations shape our performance at the gym, like how we respond to sleep loss, even like how quickly we age.
Starting point is 00:02:41 And it was actually that finding I just bought like I have to write this book now because it's like it's actually cutting people's lives by like seven and a half years. Like if you have this negative view of aging, if you see it as this kind of period of inevitable decline. And that just seems so profound, but like actually, you know, you could do a lot of good by telling this story basically. What's the difference between the expectation effect and the placebo effect? What's the difference between the expectation effect and the placebo effect? So the placebo effect is like one type of expectation effect. And so that is like, I guess most people are familiar with it, but that's very much like if you have high expectations that a treatment is going to be effective,
Starting point is 00:03:16 even if it's a dummy treatment, then you will see some kind of alleviation of your symptoms. So that's been researched for like five decades, you know, like there's no longer any controversy over that. But the expectation effect is kind of much more general. So this is looking at how we create self-fulfilling prophecies from our beliefs through like various mechanisms. So changes to our behavior, changes to our perception and changes to our physiology. And that happens both positively and negatively, right? This isn't us just making things better, we can also make things worse. Yeah, and I think that's how it often manifests in our lives like day to day,
Starting point is 00:03:53 is that when we have these kind of negative expectations, we're like needlessly hampering our performance. We're like kind of limiting our potential essentially. So, so yeah, a lot of what I writing the book is actually just like asking people to reassess those negative expectations and just kind of bring it up to something that's a bit more objective or open-minded and even that can have a huge effect on your life. The problem is we have a negativity bias, right? We have a leaning generally towards
Starting point is 00:04:20 negativity, we're vigilant for whatever might be lurking in the bushes of existence. And now the expectation effect is something that we know about. That negativity bias bleeds into a reality, negativity bias, right? We become a self-fulfilling prophecy of the fears that we had previously. Yeah, that's exactly it. So it's like the, one of the researchers told me, and I totally believe that our expectations that we have today are kind of shaping our reality tomorrow.
Starting point is 00:04:47 So, you know, like they are actually shaping the way we perceive the world, but also kind of physically what's happening to our bodies. But like you said, we have this negativity bias. I think like in our culture, we often see like being pessimistic as being inherently more rational or kind of smarty, you know, smart to be cynical. But often we're just needlessly negative, actually our kind of pessimistic expectations are no more rational than if we were like a kind of polyanna figure who was always being like totally optimistic about everything, you know. So yeah, that's why I think we have to find that sweet spot where we're not like raising our expectations to be kind of wishful thinking, but we're just kind of asking objectively could I see this situation kind of more positively essentially you looked at the expectation effect and diet right
Starting point is 00:05:33 yeah exactly so that's one of my favorite examples and like this is another way to kind of differentiate the expectation effect from just like positive thinking you know like effect from just like positive thinking, you know, like, it bollocks like the secret where you kind of, you're told to just imagine yourself being like super skinny and like you will just become super skinny. Like that's totally not going to happen. But what the scientific research shows is that it's much more about our kind of specific expectations of the food we're eating. And so when we're kind of dieting,
Starting point is 00:06:02 we're so focused on like the kind of sense of deprivation and like, you know, all the calories we're missing, that that actually kind of sets up the body for starvation. So it does things like increases the levels of the hunger hormone grailin that stimulates appetite. And grailin also kind of slows your metabolism, so you burn fat more slowly, essentially. So if you're on a diet, you've got this mindset of deprivation, you've got high levels of grailin that are kind of making you have hunger pangs and like reducing your metabolism, as I can recipe for disaster, it's like going to make your diet so much harder. So yeah, that was revelatory
Starting point is 00:06:40 for me to just realise that actually, you know, very simply kind of the kind of packaging we see around our diet foods are actually changing the way our body responds to the food. Is that language like low calorie snack and stuff like that? Yeah, all of that kind of thing and you know, like they, the scientists did this kind of linguistic analysis of kind of diet foods and it's not just like focusing on the low calories, it's also like they miss out on all of the vocabulary that might help you to get pleasure out of food. So, you know, like if you have a high fat food, it's going to be like luxurious, decadent, you know, like, you know, creamy and spicy. Yeah, creamy, tasty. They're going to focus on all of the kind of sensory aspects. If you have a diet food, it's like sensible, which is like such a planned, horrible word.
Starting point is 00:07:26 And then like that actually changes your hormonal response to the food, just by having that kind of language that sets you up to feel like you're depriving yourself. Interesting. So there is a balance, I suppose, for marketers that are doing food marketing between the, I guess, the conversion in terms of whether people are going to pick it up, and then the enjoyment or the effectiveness of whether or not they'd actually satiate to people a bit more. What was the, didn't you look at something to do with French people? Aren't French people supposed to be really fat and they're not? Yeah, right, exactly, because they eat a lot of food that's kind of high in saturated fats. So they should have higher levels of cardiovascular disease because of this. And yeah, they don't. They actually have lower BMI's, but also just lower rates of heart attacks compared to America and the UK even though we
Starting point is 00:08:19 eat fewer centurated fats. So that's kind of this mystery, but then you look at the French kind of whole attitude to eating. And they just don't feel's kind of this mystery, but then you look at the French kind of whole attitude to eating and they just don't feel this kind of guilt around what they eat that we have in the UK or US. So it's like there was the survey that asked, it was like word association and like, what do you associate with cream cake? And in France it was like celebration and in the UK it was guilt. Regret. Yeah. Yeah, exactly. And so the idea here is that actually if you're feeling like totally stressed about the food you're eating all the time and you're missing out on all of that pleasure, that's like a major source of stress and we know that stress itself can kind of contribute to
Starting point is 00:08:56 ill health. So like, you know, day to day you're not going to notice the difference, but over a lifetime, it can help to explain the differences between those cultures in terms of the kind of illnesses that they experience. What was that thing that you looked at to do with gluten and gluten intolerances? Yeah, I mean this maybe is a bit controversial. So there are some people that definitely have a gluten intolerance. There's just no question about that. So the body is responding to it. It's got a true allergic reaction. So, I'm not questioning that. But, like, I think recently we've seen a lot of media coverage where people don't have like a celiac disease, but they're just kind of gluten-sensitive. So, about 30%
Starting point is 00:09:38 of people now report that, and it's, you know, that has kind of increased from a couple of percent to 30% in like 10 years. It would be crazy if like our bodies were changing that quickly. And then, said the scientists have just kind of done these experiments where they gave these people these foods that didn't contain any gluten or wheat, but they told them it did contain the gluten and wheat, and they still experienced all of the symptoms. So they were still having like the bloating or the diarrhea like all of this stuff. So like that shows us that their symptoms aren't imagined like they really were experiencing them, but it's through an expectation effect. It's like the kind of opposite of the placebo effect is like the expectation that they will become ill makes them feel the symptoms and that probably can explain the rise. Like the
Starting point is 00:10:26 essentially all the media coverage about wheat sensitivity is kind of making a lot of people ill. Wow. It's like a psychological contagion in a way. And if you're hearing a lot of new stories about this is how bad gluten is for you and this is how you're going to react and this is how you're going to respond and maybe you've got a friend who's got it as well. And it doesn't surprise me that that's stuff happened, but it's so hilarious. It's such an interesting intersection, right? Because people don't have a gluten intolerance or they might not have a severe of a gluten intolerance as their body is responding to. However, they are suffering with the effects of a gluten intolerance. So it is not quite the same as a placebo effect, right?
Starting point is 00:11:12 It's in some sort of no man's land in between the two. They don't have the problem, but they do have the impact, but they don't have it for the reason that they thought that they did. Yeah, that's exactly it. And I think, actually, like a lot of people find it quite offensive if you suggest it's kind of the origin as psychological rather than like purely biological. But actually, I don't think that distinction makes sense anymore when we understand the mind-body connection because we know that like our expectations can change off physiology.
Starting point is 00:11:42 So, you know, loads of other experiments have shown that's like if you're expecting to kind of have a headache, that expectation then actually changes the release of like different chemicals in your brain that change the vasculature of your brain. So it's actually like changing the pressure that causes the headache. So if you take a kind of tablet and you expect to have a headache,. It's not the biological action that's going to give you that headache, but you're still going to fill the pain and it's still actually caused to change within the brain itself. It's like the words of biologically active rather than the pill you're taking essentially. We're saying exactly the same thing with the wheat intolerance. It's like the expectation is biologically active. It's not the food you're eating that's actually causing that problem. I think the reason, and I could totally imagine if I had some illness or allergy of some kind that I've been dealing with for a long time and then somebody
Starting point is 00:12:37 comes in and says, well, this is potentially contributed to or worsened by your psychology, potentially contributed to or worsened by your psychology, that makes you feel culpable. That makes you feel like it's your fault almost and like you're in control. The reason that people like the idea of medicalizing or at least it makes them feel more secure is that by giving something a term, by branding it as something, it kind of makes it feel more realistic, more real, more medicalised, more scientific, and probably a little bit more like it's not their fault. Yeah, totally. Yeah, and I think like that's something that has been contributed to by the medical profession, in that like I think a lot of doctors have this kind of assumption actually, that if something's psychogenic, it's kind of not real, it's imaginary and they have kind of assumed
Starting point is 00:13:30 it's kind of malingering so often like doctors can be quite rude when they present this diagnosis that something's psychogenic in origin. Well, the psychogenic in me. I said that's just like if it's psychological in origin essentially. So if it comes from like the brain, not from like a biological direct biological action. Yeah, and that, I mean, it's so damaging. And then like said, these people then feel very resistant to the diagnosis, they're not really going to be very positive about, say, having like a cognitive behavioral therapy to try to treat the psychogenic illness. So they like stuck in the sleep, it's actually much harder to treat them than it would be often to treat someone with a purely biological
Starting point is 00:14:11 illness. No way. So somebody that's got a psychogenic gluten intolerance is potentially more difficult to treat than somebody who has a biologically driven gluten intolerance. That's so interesting. Yeah, it's crazy. I mean, and because there's like loads of other psychogenic illnesses, there are like even more severe. So, some people have psychogenic blindness where they, their brain has kind of fooled them into thinking that their eyes can't see. And then, so they actually can't see, like it's actually, their brain is kind of fooled them into thinking that their eyes can't see. And then so they actually can't see like it's actually the brain is just blocking that sensory information from being processed consciously. And again, or paralysis even like some people have like paralyzed limbs, they're caused by like the expectation that it's been paralyzed somehow
Starting point is 00:14:59 without a kind of biological damage and atomical damage. And those people, yeah, like it's really difficult then to try to get them to respond to treatment because it feels so insulting and there's such stigma attached to that, even though it shouldn't be because actually, you know, all of us are kind of experiencing these expectation effects to a greater or lesser degree. It's just like, you know, they're having a more extreme reaction than those people. One of the places that I noticed this in my own life was with fasting. So It's just like, you know, they're having a more extreme reaction than those people. One of the places that I noticed this in my own life was with fasting. So everybody knows the experience of being hungry and not having eaten it's 2pm and you still
Starting point is 00:15:32 haven't had any food in you or whatever. And you've got those hunger pangs. But after a bunch of different episodes with longevity doctors, like Dr. David Sinclair and his colleagues, I realized that maybe that hunger signal is something that I'm actually seeking. Maybe that's something that's good for me. It's a Hormises stressor. It's going to activate telomeres and all of the other stuff that I can't remember.
Starting point is 00:15:55 And that then reframed the experience of being hungry to me. I thought, actually, this is a signal that something's going right here. This is the input that I'm after, and yet, okay, it's not wildly enjoyable, but it's not that bad. Just the sensation of being hungry without the narrative and the story around, oh my God, I need to eat.
Starting point is 00:16:18 My energy's going to be low, blah, blah, blah. I actually find that my energy goes up while I'm hungry. I find that I'm more alert. I find that my thoughts are quicker. And I think, oh, this is good for me. This is something that I'm supposed to be doing. And I'm going to look forward to having my food in, and I was time or whatever. Yeah. I mean, that's just like what the expectation effect is all about is this kind of process of reframing. So it's not about denying the feelings. It's not like you're telling yourself, like, I'm not hungry, but you're just, you're reinterpreting those feelings and you're kind of telling yourself like based on the science that
Starting point is 00:16:49 is actually like a good thing for you. And so we see that with a lot of the other expectation events that I discuss like when people are working out at the gym, you know, like some people really catastrophize like when they're kind of feeling tired, like during an endurance exercise. And they see it as a sign of that their own fitness is, you know, kind of really poor, that maybe they're going to suffer a heart attack or something. And what you want to do is just kind of change that interpretation and be like actually, like, you know, no pain, no gain, like if it's, if I'm feeling uncomfortable, that's when I'm doing my body, like the most good. And you can see that when people do interpret that,
Starting point is 00:17:26 like it changes a bunch of things in the body, but especially like it, it kind of helps to encourage the release of the endogenous opioids that we have in the brain. So like the endorphins essentially that give you that run as high. Like when you reinterpret your feelings during the workout and realize that actually like the feeling of pain can almost be like a reward, a reward, you get the release of those endorphins that makes you feel good and that makes you want to come back to the gym afterwards. What is quite powerful?
Starting point is 00:17:54 What are some of the examples of this being used in sports? Yeah, I mean, there's like low-sophonic total evidence of like coaches kind of giving, um, telling their athletes that they're taking this kind of band substance, but they're actually just giving them an injection of like glucose or whatever. I like, um, but because the athletes kind of believe they've taken this performance and answer, they do, you know, like they have much greater strength for endurance. So, you know, it's being used kind of, I think that's ethically dubious, like, necessarily leading the athletes and like, getting them to feel that they're taking a band substance is like, you know, definitely like some questions and alarm bells there. But I think more generally
Starting point is 00:18:35 like, there's a lot of research just showing that you a lot of the sports supplements that we take, like the legal ones were also kind of better, benefiting from a placebo effect. So things like caffeine that we might take as a muscle stimulant, actually the research shows that if you give people decaf but tell them that it's like high intensity caffeine, they like get a lot stronger. And if you give them like proper like high dose of caffeine and tell them that it's decaf, they actually get weaker because of that. So it's like, yeah, it's crazy. So actually most of the benefits from that supplement is coming from our expectations, not from the chemical effect.
Starting point is 00:19:12 It's good. So another implication here for marketers and general branding is the story that you tell people around the things that they're taking, largely determined their experiences of it. And I'm in Austin at the moment, and there is a strong psychedelic community here. And I had, do you know Hamilton Morris? For Hamilton's pharmacopier guy, you used to work for Vice? Ah, yeah, yeah, I do. So long stringy kind of chemist guy that goes and takes drugs all over the world.
Starting point is 00:19:46 I was talking to him about Bufel-Alvarious, which was the Toad psychedelic Toad, and people used to sort of rub it on a piece of glass and then they scrape it off and then they smoke the Toad venom. And he is adamant that you're able to recreate that chemically one-to-one. It is the same molecular structure and yet people still went about using the toad
Starting point is 00:20:10 and there is a whole story around the fact that it's sacred and he had the exact same thought of what you're mentioning now with regards to the supplements that almost none of it is, in fact, literally none of it is to do with the fact that it's come from a toad. And everything is to do with the fact that there's this story around the fact
Starting point is 00:20:31 that the toad is sacred and you go on a big walk to go and find them and you see them and you spend time with them. And the same thing when people are talking about psychedelics, set and setting, those are the two things that you try and control. What's your intention coming into this? What's the experience being around this? If you're going away to do an Iowaska retreat, you're fasting for days beforehand
Starting point is 00:20:49 You make sure that you don't swear. You're in this beautiful surrounding It you've got you know indigenous music being played and you've got a headdress on and people are dousing you in flower petal water and stuff What is this? It's priming. It's story. It's narrative, it's creating that expectation that something magical and positive is about to happen. Yeah, I mean, that's totally it. The ritual is just so important, but it's like, I almost find it's surprising that anyone could think you could separate the two, because I would say, yeah, in those experiences, like maybe 80% of what people are feeling is going to be from the ritual, from the story, like you said, from the setting, and like, very small part is going to come from the drug.
Starting point is 00:21:29 I mean, we see this in medicine, we're like people who are receiving more feed after an operation. So they could receive that in their intravenous strip, it's just delivered automatically. Or you can have a doctor give you an injection and tell you, you know, this is pain relief, you're going to feel better. And actually actually to get the same level of pain relief, you need twice as big a dose when
Starting point is 00:21:50 it's the morphine is administered surreptitiously without the ritual of the doctor giving you that morphine. So it's like the placebo effect alone is like, yeah, like half of the power of morphine, which is quite crazy. I remember reading a story in one of Johann Hari's books about a guy who had a magic electric wand thing in the 1800s or the 1900s. Do you know this story? He needs to wave it over people and then over time they took different things away.
Starting point is 00:22:20 Do you know this? I don't, no. Okay, so these patients were catatonic for one reason or another. Maybe it was chronic pain or back pain or depression or something. And this guy said, I've created this special new device and I'm going to wave it over you and it's going to make everything better. And it was all complex and maybe it used electricity and it was wood wrapped in metal with wires and other stuff, used to wave it over people. And these people that had been catatonic for years
Starting point is 00:22:47 had sometimes got up and started moving around. All right, it's a miracle. And over time, I can't remember if it was this guy or if it was somebody else that was trying to disprove it or whatever, a stress test it, started recreated their own and started taking bits away from it. So first they unplugged the electricity
Starting point is 00:23:03 and then they removed the wires and then they took away the metal sheet and then they took bits away from it. So first they unplugged the electricity and then they removed the wires and then they took away the metal sheet and then they took away the wood and then it was just someone waving the hand over people and saying the same thing and you end up with exactly the same outcome. Yeah, I mean it's incredible. And so I think a lot of,
Starting point is 00:23:18 we're like part of the placebo effect is just that feeling that someone is kind of caring for you. That can be quite powerful in itself and we know that that can do things like reduce the inflammation that you feel within your body. So it's almost like if you're kind of feeling isolated and ill, your body's like sending loads of inflammatory markers like that can kind of they attack pathogens by like brute force. But when you know that you've been cared for or that you have kind of some kind of security, markers that can attack pathogens by brute force. But when you know that you've been cared for or that you have some kind of security, the body can remove that inflammation
Starting point is 00:23:52 and allow the more specific parts of your immune system to kick into action, so the production of antibodies. So yeah, I feel like the healing ritual in itself is super important. And then on top of that we have the specific expectations of what the treatment is going to kind of give us, like what benefits we're going to get. And then that can have more specific effects on, you know, whether you have like pain relief,
Starting point is 00:24:13 whether it reduces your blood pressure, like all of that kind of stuff. You said that our beliefs are more powerful than our genes when it comes to exercise. Why is that? more powerful than our genes when it comes to exercise? Why is that? Yeah, I mean, so there's this experiment. It was just like the scientists like asked these people to come into the lab. They gave them a genetic test. It was looking at the CREB-1 gene. So that's just, we know that that is involved in like loads of things to do with like endurance exercise. So kind of how efficiently your lungs kind of can exchange the carbon dioxide for oxygen. Your body temperature as you exercise, if you have like the bad variant, you just start to feel like hotter and more uncomfortable, like a bit more sweaty as you exercise. So, you know, like it is important, but the scientists gave sham feedback to these
Starting point is 00:25:05 participants. So they, you know, regardless of what they actually had, they told some, like, you have this amazing variant, you're going to be brilliant, and the office were told, like, you're really not cut out for exercise, like, don't expect much. So that changed, like, the endurance on the treadmill, like it did have an immediate effect on performance, but, um, eventually, then it changed the physiological measures too. So actually, if you expected to have this great version of the gene, it actually changed the efficiency of the gas exchange.
Starting point is 00:25:33 So that's kind of incredible. Also, things like the efficiency of their movements seem to have been different. But what's even more incredible to me was actually they then compared the size of the expectation effect to the size of the actual genetic effect. And often the expectations were having more of an effect than the genes itself. So whether you think your cutout for exercises is probably more important than the kind of
Starting point is 00:25:57 genome you were born with. Wild. What did you, you looked at Michael Phelps for some reason? Yeah. What do you learn from him? So he'd always said like he didn't think like his secret was in his body, but it was more like in his amazing ability to visualize exercise. So like in addition to doing all of the physical training, he would like imagine every race
Starting point is 00:26:21 in like my new detail, all of the turns, every twist of his body he would plan out. And then he said it was like his visualization was so good that when he actually got into the pool it was almost as if he had done like an extra kind of training practice. And then so that's how it really intriguing, but it's kind of anecdotal, but then actually this scientific research shows that visualizing exercise can be really powerful on its own. So there's like this study, they just got people to kind of imagine like lifting heavy objects every day for six weeks. They weren't actually doing any weight lifting, they were just imagining it. And then like compared to the beginning of that period, they were like 10% stronger.
Starting point is 00:27:03 Compared to participants who hadn't done any kind of imaginary lifting at all and they were actually a bit weaker. It sounds incredible and like I think it's important to emphasize here that like the scientists aren't claiming that it was actually building the muscles themselves, that they didn't have more kind of muscle mass, but what the brain was doing was kind of recalibrating what it thought the muscles could achieve, and then that was causing it to recruit more muscle fibers. So when we exercise, even if we're really exerting ourselves, we only recruit about 50% of our muscle fibers.
Starting point is 00:27:37 That's to stop us from exhausting ourselves and giving us more energy reserves if we need it later. Probably when they were doing this visualization exercise, it was just persuading the brain to maybe recruit a few more of those muscle fibers, which then made them like physically stronger, even though they didn't have bigger muscles. A big chunk of what people are doing when they train in the gym is drilling that CNS, right?
Starting point is 00:27:58 Getting them, they're called movement engrams, making sure that you're moving efficiently, that you're doing things right. And I remember hearing about, I ruptured my Achilles a year and a half ago and my doctor had said to me, look, throughout the process of this, before you can get onto doing calf raises, you can restrict the atrophy that you're going to get in your calf if you imagine yourself doing calf raises. So although it was difficult for me to do, because the only thing more boring
Starting point is 00:28:27 than doing car phrases is only imagining that you're doing car phrases. But I would do a little working set on the days that I could be bothered where I would imagine that I was doing that. I have no idea whether or not it had an impact, but at least it seems like that atrophy of the CNS would have probably been down-regulated. Yeah, exactly. I think that's like, I mean, that's really where this kind of technique
Starting point is 00:28:53 is so promising is for rehabilitation. When people break limbs, they do experience a lot of strength loss, and then this research has shown that they can kind of cut the amount of strength that they lose by about 50% if they imagine doing the exercises. So it's like, you know, it's just helping them to, it's like giving them a head start when they do get back to kind of doing the normal workouts. What did you look at to do with sleep? Yeah, this was totally fascinating to me. So, like, scientists can just like get people into their labs and get them to sleep for a night and they can measure like the brain activity to see if they've actually been asleep or if it's been really disturbed. And then you get people to kind of report how much
Starting point is 00:29:36 sleep they had. And in general, people's subjective assessment of their sleep is just really poor. So, like, almost everyone, like, over-or-under estimates how much they've slept. So you get this group who are like the complaining good sleepers. So they sleep about eight hours a night. You know, no problem there at all, but they might, I guess you know, they wake up like in the middle of the night for like a couple of minutes, but in their head it was like they were awake for hours and they are totally convinced that they're going to have the effects of insomnia the next day. And what you can see is that that becomes the self-fulfilling prophecy. So they suffer from like poor concentration, you know, fatigue, irritability, all of these
Starting point is 00:30:20 things just because of their expectations, not because they've suffered any kind of sleep loss. And the reverse presumably as well for the people who under sleep, but convinced themselves that they've probably done okay. Yeah, the non-complaining bad sleepers at the total opposite. So they actually don't have any of the effects of insomnia, not even like things like high blood pressure, which often comes from bad sleep loss. They're just completely immune to all of that. blood pressure, which often comes from bad sleep loss. They're just completely immune to all of that. So I actually personally identify with that. I do think I do have disruptive sleep,
Starting point is 00:30:52 but I also just try to focus on how much sleep I have got. Because I know that even if it feels quite disturbed, if you're just lying there dosing, that's better than not getting any sleep at all. And yes, I don't get like a bad like effects of insomnia. Whereas my partner's the total opposite. Like he will like, he sleeps for like nine hours a night. But if he wakes up once, he will be like,
Starting point is 00:31:15 in a really bad mood the next day. Did you look at people who consider themselves or think that they're not going to get to sleep very easily. And people, a lot, everyone's familiar with this. You've got a big event tomorrow, or you're going away in holiday in the flights early. So you get to bed a little bit earlier than usual, and you end up falling asleep three hours after your usual bedtime, lying awake and bed thinking I need to get to sleep. I'm going to guess that the expectation effect must...
Starting point is 00:31:41 Why, are you expecting to go to sleep? I guess that you're fulfilling the prophecy of fearing that you won't. Yeah, that's exactly it. It's like you're raising the anxiety that you won't. And then when you're not getting sleep, you're kind of catastrophizing that and focusing on all of the kind of bad effects that are going to arise from that sleep plus. So yeah, that's like, I mean, it is, you know, I've experienced that a lot, and it's like a terrible thing. But actually, counterintuitively, the best way to avoid that is to try to stay awake. If you're just like, if you're just like, well, I'm not going to sleep, so I'm just going to like stay awake here,
Starting point is 00:32:19 and like, you will yourself to stay awake. You kind of cut through all of that anxiety, and ironically, then you get to sleep like more quickly. So yeah. That's interesting. What about aging then? You said at the start that the expectation effect can reduce life by seven years? Yeah, yeah exactly. So researches in the US kind of bloked these huge like longitudinal studies. So tracking people's kind of lives from you know like teen at their teen ages right to their kind of deaths in their 70s and 80s. And at some point in that you know like in their 30s they kind of measured like their expectations of what was going to happen as they got older. So they asked them you know is it going to be
Starting point is 00:33:02 a time of decline and disability going to kind of lose your independence or do you see it as this kind of time of growth, you know, time of wisdom, like, you know, they weren't kind of asking them, like, do you deny that there's any problems that come of aging, but just some people kind of see the good as well as the bad. And they found that those, those kind of views that people had in midlife then predicted how long they would live by a difference of about seven and a half years. So like big difference in longevity. And then how big the study was. Yeah, that would have been thousands of participants. So this is a really representative sample. Yeah, exactly. And then it's been like repeated again and again with loads of different
Starting point is 00:33:45 samples. And they found the expectations of aging can predict things like, like, your risk of getting Alzheimer's disease, like if you have the positive use, it cuts your risk about Alzheimer's by about 50%. Even if you have the genetic predisposition to get Alzheimer's, it still cuts it by 50%. Make this is wild. This is so crazy. So I was like totally skeptical of that. But then, you know, like we were saying, like this study is well conducted and they were like big numbers.
Starting point is 00:34:18 And then the scientists have also kind of joined the dots to kind of find the mechanism for how that would happen. And so like one is just kind of behavioral. It's like if your defeat is about getting old, you're not going to do so much exercise, you're not going to look after your health, you know, that's going to have an effect. But there's also kind of physiological effect here too. And that's like if you feel vulnerable as you get old, every challenge in your life is going to feel like super stressful. So that's going to raise levels of cortisol. Erases levels of inflammation. We know that both of those things can cause bodily wear and tear. And then you can see the effects right down to
Starting point is 00:34:57 the kind of differences within the cells themselves. So it kind of, the people with the negative effects have like shorter telomeres, like you, I'm, you know this, but yeah, like those protective caps that kind of protect our DNA from kind of damage, and the kind of epigenetic markers within their cells. It's like the cells are just ticking at like a quicker rate if you have the negative beliefs compared to the positive beliefs. Why do you think that this effect exists? So there's some mechanistic elements of this, right? Like you said, the person, and this is not just raging, but the whole effect overall,
Starting point is 00:35:38 I'm trying to work out why this is adaptive. I'm trying to work out why it's a universal, why it seems to work across a million different mechanisms. Why have we got this? Yeah, I mean, it's like, I think it's just so fundamental to how the brain works. And what it does is it gives us kind of flexibility. So there's like this new idea in kind of the study of consciousness that the brain works
Starting point is 00:36:03 as this kind of prediction machine. So it's always like building simulations of what's going on around us. That helps us to kind of process the sensory data. But also just as importantly, it's kind of it's allowing the brain to kind of prepare the body for the challenges that we're going to face. So you know, just like adjusting the levels of your hormones, like so that if we're going to face. So, you know, just that adjusting the levels of your hormones, like so that if you're going to face like a kind of danger, like you're kind of pumped up and like physically ready to attend to the threat, and you know, your psychology is going to change as well,
Starting point is 00:36:35 so that you are kind of more on the ball and like more capable of kind of, like focusing on the danger and not kind of thinking about other stuff, like not being too dosy. So it's actually this prediction machine is giving us a lot of flexibility. It's allowing us to respond like second by second to what's going on around us. The thing is, like those predictions, like sometimes it's purely based on things like the context of the situation that you're in, or you know, how much you've eaten, how much you've slept, you know, there's definitely like an objective element to that that the body's kind of getting from all of it, it's kind of senses like within the body and like on the skin.
Starting point is 00:37:13 But also it's just drawing on all of its previous experiences to kind of calibrate those predictions. And that's where like our conscious thinking thinking and our expectations and our interpretations of the events, like the way we choose to frame them, that's shaping the predictions and then it's shaping how the body responds as well. What do you see as the difference between creating a positive expectation and being delusional? Yeah, I mean, that's something like I really wanted to get clear in my book because it's like so much of the positive thinking literature is kind of about being delusional in a way. So it's like, you know, we were talking about like your expectations of the gym.
Starting point is 00:37:56 Like, if someone is like a real couch potato, like going to the gym and telling themselves that they're like an Olympic athlete, it's only going to lead to disappointment. Like you cannot be that delusional that you're really going to fool yourself. So I think like it's really important to set like realistic expectations, even if you're improving your expectations, it's like always trying to be kind of honest yourself and authentic. So like at the gym, like I think like don't tell yourself you're going to be, you know, like a world class athlete straight away, but like just reinterpret those feelings that
Starting point is 00:38:30 you're having at the gym. Like if you're being like overly negative about like the fatigue that you're feeling and you're kind of catastrophizing that and you're telling yourself that there's something like fundamentally wrong with your body, like you just need to re-apprise those feelings and actually look at it much more objectively and kind of just tell yourself that actually if you're feeling fatigue, like that is not only normal but it's desirable, it's like you are fasting, it's like you're telling yourself that yeah, like I'm hungry, but that is why I'm doing this, like two years for it. Yes, exactly. So I think that's how we want to reframe our feelings is just to be and our expectations
Starting point is 00:39:05 is just to kind of yet to be honest and to just avoid catastrophizing stuff when actually we can just see it in a more positive light. So you've got the proximal zone of development for skill acquisition and we've got the proximal zone of expectation for the expectation effect not too far, not too little, certainly not negative. Yeah, exactly. I love that. Yeah, it definitely has to be a kind of proximal zone.
Starting point is 00:39:31 And actually, like if we look at the expectation effect around stress and anxiety, like it's exactly what you're describing there, in that our culture has always been like really down on anxiety and seeing it as being like purely kind of detrimental to your performance and debilitating. But actually, the science shows us that, you know, those stress responses can be really useful and adaptive. Like, you know, cortisol is kind of sharpening your mind. It's actually helping you to release kind of glucose into your blood.
Starting point is 00:40:00 When your heart is pumping like that can, like, really hard. That can feel quite uncomfortable. But it's actually like gangled the oxygen around your body into your brain. when your heart is pumping like that can like really hard, that can feel quite uncomfortable, but it's actually like gang all the oxygen around your body into your brain, so it's like preparing you for like a big challenge. And what the scientists like, they kind of told some people like, suppress your feelings, like just, you know, tell yourself, I'm calm, not anxious. And they told the other people just like, um, just accept your anxiety, but recognise that it might be having these benefits and that you actually evolved to fill this for a reason. And then they got them to do things like difficult public speaking or like, um, doing like a tough
Starting point is 00:40:37 kind of graduate exam. And they found that actually the kind of honest reappraisal of the feelings, like not suppressing them, but just like, reinterpreting what they meant and like just questioning, like, are they actually detrimental or could they be useful? That that in itself was enough to improve their performance, you know, on the public speaking, on the exam, kind of making them more creative and like improving their numerical skills, reducing their anxiety. And it also just helped them to kind of recover from their anxiety afterwards. So, you know them to kind of recover from anxiety afterwards. So, you know, like, if you've had a stressful event, like sometimes you can just feel exhausted for the whole day afterwards. You're still kind of charged. But for these people, they just went
Starting point is 00:41:15 back to the normal life, the body started adjusting their food properly again, the heart rate returned to normal. And that's really important for the long-term effects of stress. Because if you have a momentary feeling of stress that then recover quickly afterwards, that's not the damaging for your body in the long term. It's the chronic stress that we're seeing that's super, super damaging to people. I have a friend comedian Bridget Fettisie
Starting point is 00:41:40 and she still gets very, very nervous before she goes on stage and she's got a little mantra that she says to herself, she says, I'm not nervous, I'm excited. I'm not nervous, I'm excited. I'm not nervous, I'm excited. And I have another friend, very well known DJ who frequently throws up before he goes to do a set because that's how much he cares because the nerves are in there. And he now, he's now told me that if he doesn't throw up before a set, he gets concerned. He's more concerned if he doesn't vomit before he goes on than he does because he's not sure if he's in
Starting point is 00:42:15 the zone. Well, I mean, that is extreme, but I totally can identify with that. And it's like, like you're saying about kind of, I'm not nervous. I'm excited. Because what's happening in your body is so similar, they're actually indistinguishable, it's silent. It's very much the story that you're telling yourself that is determining whether it is nerves or excitement. Yeah, exactly. I think for most people, it's a combination, you're anxious, but you're also excited.
Starting point is 00:42:42 Both of you are feeling both of those things because it's important to you. it's almost just kind of recognizing that fact that like you're doing this for a purpose like most often people aren't forcing you to do this you're doing it because you're ambitious or because you love to entertain or whatever and just recognizing that fact that you're kind of feeling these things for a reason that can be really powerful. What was that study to do with people pretending to be fighter pilots? Ah yes, this was to improve like vision, which is kind of crazy. Yeah, so they took like people with kind of normal vision and some people who are a bit kind of short-sighted and they just kind of told them to pretend that you're a proper fighter pilot. So they gave
Starting point is 00:43:25 them a site test first of all and then put them in this, you know, hyper realistic kind of flight simulation. And while they were doing that simulation, they had to kind of read off some codes from the wings of the planes that they were kind of flying around. And what they found was that the people who were doing that kind of hyper realistic simulation, but they actually like that eyesight physically improved. So they were able to see characters that were much smaller than they had been able to see on the kind of real sight test. And then they repeated this experiment in other settings that they are one of them was just like they took the normal sight test, but you know it goes from like big letters to small, they just reversed it. And the idea there was that we have this kind of association that the further down the test
Starting point is 00:44:09 you go the harder it is to see, but the top you should be able to see. And again they were able to distinguish characters that they hadn't been able to see before. You looked at Willpower as well and I'd heard this, I'd seen a story to do with, was it Roy Balmeister that did the original Willpower degradation studies? Yeah. Yeah, so I remember seeing, how would you say, a prelude to the expectation effect a few years ago, when I was reading about the fact that, it's not Willpower degradation, what's it called? Ego depletion. Ego depletion fact that, it's not willpower degradation, what's it called?
Starting point is 00:44:45 Ego depletion. Ego depletion, that's it. Ego depletion seems to be present only in the people that know about ego depletion. So tell it, what's the red pill that people need to understand about willpower? Yeah, I mean, that is totally an upper expectation effect. So yeah, what's interesting is that in Western culture as a whole, we've kind of had this
Starting point is 00:45:08 assumption that like when you practice kind of mental focus or kind of self-control, like when you're resisting temptation, that that is tiring and that it's going to, the more you do it, like the harder it's going to be, essentially. So, you know, and that's exactly what people like Roy Bammeister heard recorded, you know, like it's, it is like a muscle that you're kind of tiring out and the longer you do it, like the more likely you are to kind of lose your focus or give into temptation. But then they do some studies in India where people don't share that cultural belief. So actually in India, it's much more common for people to think that will power is self perpetuating.
Starting point is 00:45:50 It's almost like they think like, once you get into the zone, it's like easier to kind of continue practicing your will power is. Yeah, it's like, which is equally plausible. Yeah, totally, yeah. It's just like anything like once you get into
Starting point is 00:46:03 the kind of momentum going going it kind of can continue And so then they did the ego depletion experiments with those people and they found the total opposite actually Like they didn't experience ego depletion at all. The willpower did indeed crease increase the more they practiced it So their focus was much better on a second task compared to the first task because I'd'd had this time to warm up. So that was a really strong evidence that like, yeah, it's totally due to our underlying beliefs about what's going to happen to the brain. It's purely an expectation effect. There's no kind of biological cause for ego depletion. Does knowing about the expectation effect or the placebo effect change the effect of it. Because I don't know if we have created an information hazard for everybody who's listening now
Starting point is 00:46:50 that could have blissfully stumbled upon the expectation effect and benefited from it, but now they know the fact that it's only a blah blah. No, yeah, I mean, the great thing is, is actually the opposite. Like, um, knowing about the expectation effect can actually make it more powerful in some way. And so we know this from studies of placebo. So like in a typical placebo experiment, you give someone a painkiller, you give someone a dummy pill, but you tell them it's a painkiller. So it's like, you have to be deceptive. But then scientists started, like wondering, well, what happens if we actually just give people a jar of placebo pills, labelled as like placebo pills take to a day, can that actually improve
Starting point is 00:47:31 their pain just as well as the, kind of, surreptitious, placebo? And they found that actually it can. So in this study from Portugal, they were looking at people with chronic pain, they gave them these pills and they also gave them this kind of explanation of the expectation effect. So just a kind of grounding in like what happens with the mind body connection, how the brain can produce its own pain killers, you know, all of that information. And then they just gave people these pills and told them to kind of take them to a stay. And they were really clear like, you must take the pills even though they're dummy pills.
Starting point is 00:48:05 Then five days later, they had seen a reduction in their symptoms already. There was about 30% on the kind of standard score of pain. So that's the clinical threshold for any new treatment. Like if a treatment's effective, you have to have a 30% reduction. And these people were experiencing that from open label to see placey those. But let's sell it. Yeah, exactly. We need more and actually you can buy like placebo pills on Amazon.
Starting point is 00:48:31 So like it's already available if you want it. But actually like, you know, that's fine. Like and if I had chronic pain, I probably would take like open label placey pose. But actually, there's just loads of other ways you can like increase expectations without kind of the sham treatments in medicine. So, you know, just like giving people psychological therapy where you kind of explain the importance of expectations. And then, so if they've had heart surgery, you kind of just help them to kind of set out
Starting point is 00:48:59 a kind of framework for like how they hope they will improve over the next six months. That can also bring about this kind of placebo-like response. So you still see kind of improvements in biological measures, like the level of information that they're experiencing off the operation, how quickly they leave hospital, how quickly they return to work. All of that just through honestly helping people to change their expectations. Wasn't there something to do with medicines where people began to use genuine medicines and then there were either weaned off or the dose got reduced or something. Because I imagine that must be, let's say that you have a drug that's got a
Starting point is 00:49:40 relatively toxic side effect. Perhaps there's a reason why you wouldn't want to be on it longer term. You introduce it, you taper it down, that would be, you get both sides right you've been introduced to the experience you've got that. Set and setting almost and then you you die it back and people still get to benefit. Yeah I mean that's a really big hope for like treating addiction, essentially. So there have been studies looking at people in rehab from terrible injuries. And you do start out by giving them an opioid drug, but very quickly you give them the drug and then you give them the placebo pill as well. So they take both together.
Starting point is 00:50:21 And you can strengthen it even further by associ of associating both with like a really strong smell, so like the smell of cardamom, for example. So, so you'd have like you'd sniff the cardamom and you'd take the placebo and you'd take the drug and then after a few days you just stop or encourage them, you don't force them but you encourage them like you don't need to take the drug anymore, just take the placebo pill and the smell. Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás Nicolás So that can really reduce the amount of the active drugs that people take. And there have been case studies where people have completely weaned themselves off the opioids quite quickly just by using this technique. What was the thing to do with peanut allergies? Oh yeah, yeah.
Starting point is 00:51:17 I mean, that was one of my favorite studies because I think again it shows just how we're talking about honestly reframing stuff rather than like being delusional. So like when kids have peanut allergies you can actually train the immune system to stop responding to the peanut protein and it's just like exposure therapy so you start off with like a tiny dose of the protein and then you build up over six months to hold peanut. So by the end you know know, for kid, is that a party in there, it's certainly like a Snickers borrower, they're not gonna be in danger having eaten that because they've trained their body to deal with it.
Starting point is 00:51:51 But when the kids do this therapy, it's like really uncomfortable, you know, because the body is starting to have like a mild allergic reaction as they increase the dose. So they get hives, you know, uncomfortable feelings in their mouth, they might feel sick, you know, uncomfortable feelings in their mouths. They might feel sick. You know, all of these things that are frightening
Starting point is 00:52:07 and kind of seem to be putting them in danger. And the researchers like just try to get them to reinterpret how they saw those that discomfort. So they just said, this is rather than being dangerous, this is actually a sign that the treatment is being effective. It's like in the same way when you're doing strength training, your muscles are gonna start aching. Like, you know, this is a sign that you're stimulating the immune system
Starting point is 00:52:33 and it's kind of learning and responding. So at the end of the trial, those patients had had that kind of training and reframing. They just reported fewer side effects overall. So it had reduced their discomfort, but even more impressively, like it had also changed the efficacy of the treatment. So those kids were actually showing high levels of this friendly antibody
Starting point is 00:53:00 that can protect them from the overall, like noxious allergic reaction, it kind of just stops the immune system from overreacting. And they had much higher levels than the kids who hand learned to reframe these symptoms in that way. Have you considered whether or not this in part accounts for some of the side effects we've seen with the recent vaccine rollout and the fact that people have, I mean, has there ever been as widespread of a medication with as many disputed, disputable side effects and then people reporting them and all sorts of stuff?
Starting point is 00:53:35 Yeah, yeah, I mean, totally, it definitely has had an effect. Like, and you know, like I think on social media, you can see everyone kind of sharing their kind of war stories, but having the vaccine and then like being knocked out for a couple of days or, you know, having a bad headache or whatever. So it was very much like contagious these expectations. So the studies so far showed that actually, you know, these vaccines are quite, they definitely stimulate the immune system. So they do cause like for some people or about 25% of people, they do lead to those kinds of mild side effects. So things like fatigue, maybe having a bit of a fever, having a headache, you know, that's all kind of to be expected for like half of 25% of
Starting point is 00:54:19 the people having the vaccines. But actually in the in the trials, you saw that the people receiving the placebo injections were also quite likely to have those side effects too. So about half as many people had those side effects in the placebo trials. So we know from that that yeah, like the vaccines themselves were causing some of these like slightly uncomfortable side effects, but the expectations were causing it for a large number of people too. And there was that thing at Gatwick Airport, which I remember from a few years ago where it was all over the press and the entire airport was shut down and people were saying that
Starting point is 00:54:57 there was drones absolutely everywhere, but you reckon that might be an expectation effect as well. Yeah, I mean, that was, say, yeah, like in 2018, you know, I think just one person reported a drone and then like within a couple of days, like hundreds of people said they've seen them. But like the police and the army were using like, you know, radar, like they were, you know, using all their technology to kind of detect where these drones were. And like, they couldn't find a single one and no one took a photo of a drone that could be verified. That's it. There was no footage. It didn't appear on any radar. It wasn't on any of the equipment. And yet you had this cascade of reports coming in.
Starting point is 00:55:35 Yeah. So it just seems like it was, yeah, like a contagious expectation effect. And so we know that the expectations can shape perceptions. And that it's like, it was priming people to see a drone when like there was nothing in the sky. This would be interesting to look at if there was a similar sort of event happening again. What you would presumably expect in terms of the number of reports would be like a normal distribution curve. So you would have one, a few, a few more, a few more, a few more, a peak, and then it would begin to tail off. I wonder if you mapped those onto a distribution about whether or not it would come back like that. That would be interesting. Yeah, I mean, I think anecdotally from what the police had reported, it did seem like that
Starting point is 00:56:20 we've got working, that you did start out with just one and then within a couple of hours, so it was maybe another person seeing it. But then as it kind of got into the media, you know, and was spread around the airport like, yeah, you saw rapid increase. And like, there can't have been that many drones around like it would have just been impossible. So, I mean, you can never roll out the possibility that maybe the first report was genuine, but definitely like the airport didn't need to be closed for like three days and like disrupt all of those holidays. Like yeah. One thing that you said earlier is something I've been thinking about recently, which is this sort of culture of generalized cynicism that we've got at the moment. And when you realize,
Starting point is 00:56:59 you are right, there's something people think of it as more rational, more cool, almost to be cynical and sort of blunt about most things. You see this, like in its extreme on the internet, no one is excitable on the internet. Everyone's playing like a Louis Theroux persona. And yeah, when you think about the fact that on the internet, everyone's playing like a Louis Theroux persona. And yeah, when you think about the fact that that cynicism
Starting point is 00:57:31 and the sort of public messaging that we have, and then the social, like top down public messaging, and then the bottom up social messaging that people see, literally could be making people more sick, more fat, less healthy, stupider with side effects from, you know, it's everything. Sinicism, cynicism genuinely is kind of like a disease or a virus. Yeah, it totally is. And like, what really annoys me is it's like, if you're being like, like, people think it's like super smart to be skeptical,
Starting point is 00:58:01 but actually being reflexively skeptical and just not believing anything good, like to me, that is just as bad as being. Oh, it's just as stupid. Yeah, yeah, exactly. As being totally gullible, it's like you're doing exactly the same thing. It's just you're taking the opposite viewpoint. So yeah, that drives me crazy.
Starting point is 00:58:21 And especially, like I see it a lot with like, you know, like as a science writer, like amongst my colleagues, actually, it's kind of quite fashionable to always be like super skeptical of any new and exciting results. It's like, that's not, that's not being like a better journalist or like a more skeptical thinker. That's actually just like kind of virtually signaling all my store. Yeah, like trying to prove your intelligence without applying your intelligence. The programming is just as simplistic. It's just moving in the opposite direction. You haven't thought about it either. You haven't considered it either. You're just taking a heterodox position in the hopes that this makes you seem cool. So what about the ethics then? Do you think that there's
Starting point is 00:59:00 an ethical concern around deceiving somebody or deceiving other people, even if it's in the service of making their outcomes in life better. Yeah, and I do see it as like a big ethical issue to like, um, deceive people. Um, and I think like in medicine, you know, like, um, it's why these open label placebos, like the honest placebos are so important because I think if you start deceiving patients and then they find out they've been deceived, then they're gonna lose a lot of faith in the medical profession
Starting point is 00:59:31 and then that's gonna cause problems further down the line. So I think you just have to be honest for the ethical reasons, but then I actually think like when we know that you can apply the expectation effect honestly without any kind of problems anyway, there's no need to be deceptive really like you. It is enough often just like it looks so many of these studies, it's enough just to kind
Starting point is 00:59:53 of like tell people about the science, like you just give them like a kind of you know popular science article that kind of explains how like stress can be beneficial to you or you know how you can reframe the feelings of exercise and that can improve your performance. Very plain, not deceptive at all. Just that information is enough. The knowledge is power in these cases. I don't think we need to worry about deception anymore. Are there any effects that run counter to the expectation effect? Are there any things that you found? I think you mentioned something to do with constantly following happiness
Starting point is 01:00:31 as a actually can make people less happy, which seems to kind of run in the opposite direction. Were there any others? Yeah, I mean, that's the main one, I think. And yeah, it's kind of like it makes sense. I mean, so it's like counterintuitive, but it's like if you're always trying to be happy, like the kind of converse of that
Starting point is 01:00:51 is that you're actually then demonizing the negative feelings you might be having, or the uncomfortable feelings you might be having. So it kind of is like an expectation effect in that it's like, if you want to be so happy that you think that any time you feel frustration or anxiety it's a disaster, an expectation effect and that is like, if you want to be so happy that you think that any time you fill a frustration or anxiety it's a disaster,
Starting point is 01:01:08 you're kind of then setting yourself up to like experience those emotions a lot more negatively and to kind of, for them to have a bigger impact on your life. But yeah, it is, I think like the simplistic view of the expectation effect would just be it's like, tell yourself to be happy and you will be happy. But yeah, that doesn't happen. So So yeah I think we have to be careful to kind of have the to kind of outline the limits of the expectation effect and the nuance. It's not always as simple as just kind of hoping for
Starting point is 01:01:37 the best. I also just think we have to be honest about the fact that like say in medicine like say you've got like a terminal illness or you know like just improving your expectations isn't going to help you to get better like sometimes the expectation effect just there's no physiological mechanism by which it could help you to kind of benefit from the expectation effect essentially. So we we just have to be careful to be honest about when it does work and when you're not going to expect to see the full benefit. Is one of the most important things here, information or people's understanding or knowledge around whatever the experience is that they're going through. So for instance, talking about
Starting point is 01:02:23 going to the gym, the understanding that one of the byproducts of going to the gym is going to be you sweating and you're lifting, you're feeling aches in your muscles and stuff like that, that knowledge enables the reframing, right? My point is, is there a, it seems like one of the first steps to becoming effective at the expectation effect is. A degree of understanding your knowledge around the specifics of whatever it is that you're trying to get the effect to work on is that right. Yeah, it totally is. Yeah, exactly. So it's like, yeah, we've like exercise it helps you to understand like the exercise physiology there and to understand why you're feeling these things, because otherwise it's really easy for you to be like to catastrophise those feelings
Starting point is 01:03:09 if you don't understand the science of why they might be beneficial to you. At the same with stress, you feel the discomfort, and if you don't understand some of the basics of the science of why we have the cortisol spike at all,, yeah, all you're going to focus on is the negative feelings and not necessarily on why it might be useful. So, yeah, I really do think, actually, it's just a bit of scientific literacy and all of these areas can go a long way to helping you to reframe your expectations. That's one part of it, right? What I want to try and get out of you is, what are the common strategies if people want to effectively apply the expectation effect, which aren't contingent on specific
Starting point is 01:03:54 knowledge about the thing that they're trying to use it on, you know, reframing and all of that stuff, what are your, what's the best framework that you've come up with that people can take away? Yeah, I mean, there's definitely some things that I think you can do that, like whatever challenge you're facing, they're going to help you to form better or more objective expectations. My favourite one is really this process called self-distancing, and so it was developed by Ethan Cross at the University of Michigan kind of independently from the expectation effect. But what he had noticed is that if you, the way you talk to yourself is often really different from the way you would talk to a friend.
Starting point is 01:04:36 So you're much more self-critical and much more negative when you talk to yourself compared to if you're advising someone who's in exactly the same position. And so his solution to that is just to imagine that you're advising your friend like when you're talking to yourself. And you can even use like the third person kind of language so you can be like, I could be like, you know, it sounds stupid, it sounds like Elmo from Sesame Street or to the big, could be like, you know, like David's worried about, I don't know, like I'm getting older, David feels that he's going to be like vulnerable and, you know, suffer from decline, you know, all of that. But the fact is, like, if you were talking to yourself, you might be like, look in the mirror, see some wrinkles and be like,
Starting point is 01:05:21 oh God, I look hideous, this is awful, like, it's only going to get worse. If you were talking to your friend, you would be like, no, you're still fine. There's still lots to look forward to. Don't be so defeated. You can still grow. You can still... There's still stuff that you can do and achieve as you're getting older. So yeah, I think that's just something that I found super helpful. Whenever I am wrestling with needlessly negative expectations, it's just a shift perspective. I think, would I actually talk to other people and tell them the things that I'm telling myself? That's used the third person in a monologue, and also imagine that you were saying it to somebody else. Yeah, exactly. And treat yourself with the kind of compassion
Starting point is 01:06:06 that you would treat someone else, basically. So. David, this is great. Now, this is a really, really cool effect. I'm very, very glad that you've written the book. If people want to see what you do, where should they go online? So my website is DavidRobson.me.
Starting point is 01:06:22 I'm D underscore A, underscore Robson on Twitter. I'm on LinkedIn. Yeah, but I mean, basically Twitter on my website is the best place. Dude, I really appreciate you coming on today. Thank you. Well, yes, it's been my pleasure. Thank you.
Starting point is 01:06:43 Yeah, I'm fed.

There aren't comments yet for this episode. Click on any sentence in the transcript to leave a comment.