Modern Wisdom - #834 - Lyman Stone - Why Is Everyone Having Fewer Children?
Episode Date: September 5, 2024Lyman Stone is a demographer, researcher, and a writer. It wasn't long ago that everyone was worried about the population bomb and within a few short decades global birth rates are now declining. What...'s going on? What is driving such a rapid change in the number of children people are having and should we do anything about it? Expect to learn the best explanations for why birth rates are declining, whether declining birthrates are downstream from declining marriage rates, what winning the lottery does to marriages for both men and women, Lyman's controversial perspective on the impact of sperm count and testosterone levels on fertility and much more…. Sponsors: See discounts for all the products I use and recommend: https://chriswillx.com/deals Get a 20% discount on the best supplements from Momentous at https://livemomentous.com/modernwisdom (automatically applied at checkout) Get the Whoop 4.0 for free and get your first month for free at https://join.whoop.com/modernwisdom (automatically applied at checkout) Sign up for a one-dollar-per-month trial period from Shopify at https://shopify.com/modernwisdom (automatically applied at checkout) Extra Stuff: Get my free reading list of 100 books to read before you die: https://chriswillx.com/books Try my productivity energy drink Neutonic: https://neutonic.com/modernwisdom Episodes You Might Enjoy: #577 - David Goggins - This Is How To Master Your Life: https://tinyurl.com/43hv6y59 #712 - Dr Jordan Peterson - How To Destroy Your Negative Beliefs: https://tinyurl.com/2rtz7avf #700 - Dr Andrew Huberman - The Secret Tools To Hack Your Brain: https://tinyurl.com/3ccn5vkp - Get In Touch: Instagram: https://www.instagram.com/chriswillx Twitter: https://www.twitter.com/chriswillx YouTube: https://www.youtube.com/modernwisdompodcast Email: https://chriswillx.com/contact - Learn more about your ad choices. Visit megaphone.fm/adchoices
Transcript
Discussion (0)
Hello everybody, welcome back to the show. My guest today is Lyman Stone. He's a demographer,
researcher and a writer. It wasn't long ago that everyone was worried about the population bomb,
and within a few short decades global birth rates are now declining. What's going on? What is driving
such a rapid change in the number of children people are having? And should we do anything
about it? I expect to learn the best explanations for why birth rates are declining,
whether declining birth rates are downstream from declining marriage rates,
what winning the lottery does to marriages for both men and women,
Lyman's controversial perspective on the impact of sperm count and testosterone levels on fertility,
and much more.
Trust really is everything when it comes to supplements.
A lot of brands may say that they're top quality, but few can actually prove it, which is why
I partnered with Momentus.
They make the highest quality supplements on the planet.
If you've been struggling to fall asleep on a night time, stay asleep throughout the night.
Or if you're waking up not feeling rested or revitalized in the morning, their sleep
packs are one of my favorite products, which I use every single night before bed.
They contain only the most evidence-based ingredients
at perfect doses to help you get the best night's sleep
of your life.
And there is a 30-day money back guarantee
so you can buy it completely risk-free, use it,
and if you do not like it for any reason,
they will give you your money back.
Plus, they ship internationally.
Right now, you can get a 20% discount by going to the link in the show notes below This episode is brought to you by Whoop. Whoop is a 24 seven health and fitness coach
that tracks your sleep, strain, recovery, stress,
and more to provide personalized insights
that help you reach your goals.
Each morning, Whoop gives you a recovery score
that acts as your daily guide
for how much you should exert yourself.
At the end of the day, you get a recommendation
for your ideal bedtime.
You can also track over 140 different habits and behaviors to see how they
impact your overall health.
It basically means you can stop guessing about what's happening inside of your
body by wearing a small thing on your wrist that tracks absolutely everything.
Plus you can join for free, pay nothing for the brand new whoop 4.0 strap.
Plus you get your first month for free and there's a 30 day money back guarantee
so you can buy it for free, try it for free.
If you do not like it after 29 days, they will give you your money back right now.
You can get that free whoop 4.0 strap and that 30 day money back guarantee by
going to join.whoop.com slash modern wisdom.
That's join.whoop.com slash modern wisdom.
This episode is brought to you by Shopify.
Businesses that sell more sell on Shopify, which is why they are the global force behind
Gymshark, Skims, Allo and Newtonic.
When it comes to converting browsers into buyers, they are best in class.
Their checkout is 36% better on average compared to other leading e-commerce platforms and
their shop pay means that you can boost conversions by up to 50%.
Best of all, their award-winning support is there to help you every step of the way.
Look, you're not going into business to learn about how to code or build a website or do back-end
inventory management. Shopify takes all of that off your hands and allows you to focus on the job
that you came here to do, which is designing and selling an awesome product. Upgrade your business
and get the same checkout that we use at Nutanix with Shopify sign up for a $1
per month trial period at Shopify.com slash modern wisdom or lowercase
that's Shopify.com slash modern wisdom to upgrade your selling today.
But now ladies and gentlemen, please welcome Lyman Stone.
What is the story of fertility rates in America over the last few years? They're going down.
As in every country, essentially.
So yeah, I mean, in 2007, the average woman had 2.1 kids per woman.
Today it's closer to 1.6.
So half a child missing per woman though.
Uh, so it was a weird thing to say which half of the child is missing.
Um, no, realistically that means every other woman, um, is missing a child that
she would have had if birth rates were stable at 2007 approximate levels.
What happens if we go much further back?
What was happening with fertility rates in America
in like the 1800s and the early 1900s?
So pretty much everywhere it used to be
that women had lots more babies.
I say pretty much everywhere
because there are exception cases.
They're sort of unusual ones.
Tokugawa era Japan had pretty low fertility.
There was low fertility in some parts of the Roman Empire in the first and second centuries.
But pretty much everywhere, I mean the US had I think six children per woman in 1800,
with the exception of like Massachusetts and Connecticut that were lower, maybe around four.
But that number is kind of an optical illusion. We say, oh, they had six children per woman.
But the first thing you have to keep in mind is most women didn't survive to
the end of their reproductive years.
Right.
So if life expectancy is 30 and we calculate a total fertility rate up to age 50,
well, half of women never even make it to the end of their reproductive years.
Oh, so that was a projected rate, not an actual number. Yeah. Well, half of women never even make it to the end of their reproductive years.
Oh, so that was a projected rate, not an actual number.
Right. Well, it might be true for the women who actually survive,
but a lot of women don't survive.
I don't know why I'm laughing. That's not funny. That's horrible.
But it's a statistical problem, right?
That the actual, the actual number of children born per woman
is gonna be much lower
because many of them are not gonna survive
long enough to have six kids.
And then secondly, a lot of those children didn't survive.
So surviving fertility per woman
in the US was quite high still, probably 2.7 to 4-ish
in that range depending on the place and the time.
But in many societies in Europe it was much lower, in some places below two, um, because, uh, death rates for
children and for women were so high that the average woman hitting puberty really
could only couldn't expect to have, um, that many kids.
It's wild to think that you could get between two and a half and four with all
of the hurdles that you need to get over risk of mother dying during
childbirth, risk of child not making it to maturity, risk of mother just dying
outside of childbirth and still you get close to two and a half or four.
Yeah, it's, I mean, humans have overcome extraordinary adversity.
Just before you, before I got on this podcast, I was writing about the
massive population oscillations in
like the Yucatan Peninsula over the last several millennia. And I mean, the result is,
I mean, literally the stories are embedded in our genes, right? Like you've got subpopulations that
they have meaningfully different genes related to how they digest certain foods or how they interact with certain viruses and bacteria
because of horrible things that have happened in their past.
And the survivors come out of it fundamentally changed.
And so we as a species have a variety of adaptations
species have a variety of adaptations that help us continue to have children despite all these things.
The most important one is that we have very low variation in our reproductive systems.
That is to say, pretty much any human can be fertile with almost any other human.
Is that not common in other animals?
We have a lot of genetic variation, um, that we are still fertile across that
in some animals, um, uh, would be more challenging.
What do you mean?
Be, be, can you be more specific?
Uh, yeah.
So, I mean, in some species, um, you've got groups that are isolated for thousands of years, and when
you bring them back to other branches of the species, they basically can no longer produce
fertile offspring.
Whereas, human populations that were separated for tens of thousands of years, when they
get back together, still make babies.
What's going on is we have very, so to speak,
harmonized reproductive systems.
We have very low variation in genetically
what's going on with reproduction.
We pretty much all do it the same way.
There's variation in some of our hormones
and a lot of, there's some variation in human body size,
but even then humans have less variation in body size
than most animals.
I mean, if you look at other species
that have wide distributions,
they just have more variation than we do.
So the upshot of all this is,
you can survive a lot more things
if you're working with a very standardized,
or rather your genes can survive a lot more things. If you you're working with a very standardized or rather your genes can survive a lot more things.
If you're all working with a very standardized
toolkit and whoever the survivors are, uh, they
can, they can make babies.
Looking at the 1900s, there must be some
interesting oscillations and lessons in terms of
fertility rate, in terms of desired number of
children versus actualized, et cetera, et cetera.
As you have people dying, different economic situations, fast crashes, and then
rebounds and sex ratio imbalances and stuff like that.
What are some of the lessons to take from the 1900s?
So definitely one of the lessons we've learned is when people, when people are less likely to die,
they are in less of a hurry to start having kids.
And so they start later.
And furthermore, when people believe
more of their children are gonna survive,
they have fewer children, which is to say,
people are implicitly targeting surviving fertility.
And that explains most of the fertility decline
up through like 1950 around the world.
And even today, a lot of the fertility decline up through like 1950 around the world. And even today, a lot of the fertility decline in developing countries is essentially explained
by assuming, if you just assume that women are targeting or families are targeting a
stable surviving number of children, that explains most fertility decline.
It is the case that surviving fertility rates are actually rising in some African countries,
for example, even as overall fertility rates are declining.
But in general, surviving fertility is a lot more stable, particularly in that period where
you're coming off like early 1900s and things like that, or today for some African countries.
But that doesn't explain what's happened in industrialized
countries over the last 40, 50 years because almost all children survive to puberty. It's
like 99 point something percent in our societies. So there's not really any leverage there and
yet fertility keeps falling. So that's not going to be about child survival. We also know it's not
about rising standards of living. I have some articles
about this coming. They might be out before this podcast, so
then people can look them up. But essentially, we can see
what happens when people suddenly become a lot wealthier
but without cultural change. And we can see what happens when whole countries
suddenly become much wealthier,
but without dramatic cultural change.
So like for countries, we're often looking at like
natural resource discoveries, right?
Like they find oil and suddenly they're no longer
in destitute poverty, they're like wildly wealthy.
For individuals, we look at things like lottery winnings
or money transfers. and we can see that
when you make people richer, they don't have fewer babies, and in fact, typically they have more
babies. So what happens is not that societies became richer. We know now that fertility began
to fall in many countries long before they were industrialized. And in many early industrialized countries like the UK,
fertility did not fall for a very, very long time,
and it didn't fall until other conditions were met.
So basically, one of the key takeaways that we learn from,
particularly the last century,
is that fertility decline is not about just becoming a high income society.
We know a lot about fertility enough now to know that fertility declines largely in
response in the long run to cultural factors, not to just becoming a wealthy society.
There was an interesting study. I saw you tweet.
I actually looked at it off the back of your Twitter about fertility rates
from lottery winners and, uh, Swedish people when they win the lottery.
Yeah.
We have several studies, uh, that leverage lottery winnings.
Um, the findings are not all in perfect harmony, as studies never are.
But in general, when men win the lottery, they definitely have more kids.
That's true across many different contexts.
When women win the lottery, there's a little bit more variety.
But even then, it does generally seem like there's a sort of a pro
family effect.
In the Swedish case, it was one of the bigger gaps.
That is, there was definitely a positive effect for men.
And I forget if it was a neutral or slightly negative effect.
I think women get divorced a little bit more likely.
Yeah, there was definitely a divorce effect.
I forget what the fertility effect was for women.
I will say in Africa, for example, we have a collection of studies recently that showed
that when African women have higher incomes, they have more children.
And we also know that from studies of American women and American men.
So there's definitely some cultural contextualization there.
But just in general, I mean, think about any product. Like if you have more money,
will you consume more hamburgers? Very likely. If you have more money, will you
consume more airplane tickets? Very likely. Most goods are kind of, you know,
consumption of them is going to be approximately related to income.
So kids are not different in that regard. If you give people more money, it does tend to be the case that they have a bit more.
Yeah, I think it's a interesting or difficult circle for me to square with
modern living standards at the moment, compared with how they would have been in
1900 or in 1800 or whatever,
the fact that people a lot of the time feel or say,
anecdotally, I can't afford to have a kid.
Look, the cost-
Even though they're living in the richest society
in the world.
So, and they feel like it's true, right?
They feel like they do not have the money
to be able to raise a child.
How could I raise a child? I can barely pay for myself. I can't get on the property ladder
and cost of living is going up as ever before and the cost of fuel and the cost of housing
and the cost of groceries, inflation through the roof and unemployment and all the rest
of it. So that is genuinely how they feel. Even if that isn't perhaps, um, accurate, uh, quantitatively when
you compare it with our ancestors.
So where's that either estimation of the cost of having children or
estimation of the cost of how much, uh, life is, is going to come in as an expense.
What's going on here?
So one thing to keep in mind is what I said about mortality,
that when you actually look at surviving fertility rates, um,
modern people have more kids than people in the past. I mean,
human populations in the past were largely stable for a very long time.
Now there was a lot of oscillation for hundreds of years of time,
but like there were long periods where for centuries humans had below
replacement rate fertility. Okay. In the past, not below 2.1, they were having six kids, but five of them were dying. So, for
certain periods. So, it's important to keep in mind that like the ancestral
primordial human environment that sort of structures the hardware we're working
within our brain was an environment where human population was not growing. Um,
it was not a growth environment for human population. Um, so,
um,
the 20th century saw massive human population growth.
That's unusual in human history. Um, not quite,
not totally unique, but it's not the dominant experience. Rapid population growth is not the most common human experience.
And what that should tell us is that like three or more surviving children
is not the usual human experience through most of human history.
So right off the bat, we just have to get out of our head that
people in poor societies were having tons of kids, right? They were having tons of babies,
but rather few of them became kids. Okay? So we just have to keep in mind that humans are targeting
the number of children they're going to raise. And so on that sense, yeah, actually the number of children people were
raising did rise over the course of the 20th century as, um, particularly the,
the early 20th century as global income started to rise.
Right.
And that's no longer the case.
Child mortality improved.
So just, I really want to dig into.
You get things like the baby boom, right?
Like the baby boom, there's this massive increase in standards of living with
rapid technological innovation in the mid century that led to dramatic increases
in wealth, particularly housing wealth. And that's both size of housing and quality of
housing. People are living in way better houses. I mean, look at the things people lived in.
I don't mean rich people. I mean like normal people like in the late 1800s and like compare
that to what they were living in, you know, 1948. And it's just seismic improvement.
And they had way more kids.
So just like I said, I've never heard of this before, but it makes, it makes
complete sense.
Are you saying that there is somewhere sort of internally inside of either
men and women or just women, men and women alike, you are targeting
surviving fertility.
So for example, we know in modern and ancient societies, if a kid dies,
the odds of having another kid rise. Okay.
Demographers sort of macaque in a macabre way called this replacement
fertility, not replacement rate, um, mortality or replacement fertility.
Um, so basically the idea is that humans are shooting for surviving family size. We don't think about it much in our society because almost all of our kids live.
But this was very, very, very present in the past.
And you see this in the past in societies where people had six or seven kids, you
routinely, there's a problem in genealogical data.
You find a kid, their name is John, they die when they're a month old.
The next kid also gets named John.
When he dies at two, the next kid is also named John.
They just keep reusing names
because they're just shooting for a John.
You know why they want a John?
Because dad's name is John
and there needs to be a John after the John, okay?
So like name repetition is like a really strong indicator
psychologically of what's going on here.
Oh, wow.
That's such an interesting way that you've got this census
data or whatever type of data you've got.
You get tax record data, all kinds of things.
You've got genealogical data going back a long way.
And you've got four kids all named the same.
There's no way that you would name four kids that survived
into adulthood the same name.
It would be absolutely nightmare. This is, yeah.
And again, the names that get repeated are likely to be family names particularly,
right? Because you really want an inheritor for the name.
So that's the first thing to keep in mind.
Okay.
Is that it really is the case that like modern people are actually raising bigger
families often than people of the past.
It's not they're burying more children, but they're raising more of them often. The second thing that's going on is we have to
distinguish between standard of living and standard of living. Okay? There's the
standard of living in the sense of what you earn and like we have a better
standard of living. We are more productive. We earn more. We are richer.
And then there's the standard of living that is
the standard that if you don't meet it,
you feel bad about your position in life.
Okay?
So if you raise that first one,
you can think of it as like income level.
Fertility doesn't tend to fall and it often rises.
But if you raise that other one, that is your expectations about what you should have or
what a child should have, fertility tends to fall.
What's happening in modern society is that income has gone up.
But particularly over the last 60, I would say especially the last 40 years, expectations have
also dramatically risen, particularly in terms of what you will provide to children. There's a bit
of an arbitrary example of this, but I love it. If you look up the frequency of the word parenting
in English language texts over time, it's like, it really is quite rare before like 1975.
And if you look up like other substitute terms like child rearing and things like that, they're
also uncommon.
But after 1980, the share of English language texts, and this is true in English English,
American English, English fiction, it's true across all textual categories. Legal, it's true in legal cases like corpuses of legal
texts. Frequency of parenting vocab just explodes in the 80s, 90s, and 2000s.
We became way more preoccupied with parenting. This, this idea of parenting, this notion of like all the things you have to
provide your kids. Um, now I think this actually arguably may have begun a bit
earlier than the linguistic shift. Um,
but it is the case that there has never been an era as preoccupied with
parenting strategies and the things you have to do to raise your
children as our current one.
We are fixated.
What's that an indication of?
I think it's an indication.
I think it's two things.
One, it's just kind of, it's just a cultural meme.
You know, there are some societies that did human sacrifice.
There are some societies that tattoo their whole bodies.
And there are some societies that are obsessed with parenting.
So on some level, it's just...
Societies just do things for reasons that are hard to explain.
On the other hand, I do think there's a sense in which
as children became rare, or as childbearing became rare, and as we all developed the expectation
that all children survive to maturity, it kind of raised the question of like, so what
should parents do?
Like what are we supposed to do?
And I think for like the baby boomer generation, the answer was like nothing.
Like, I mean, we'll put you in a school, put you in front of a TV when you need
to be distracted, but like parents didn't have to do much in a nice little
indicator of this, you've looked at like the share of mothers who breastfed
their children, it reaches like rock bottom lows in like the forties, okay.
Forties and fifties.
And then it's been rising since then. So in
like the 40s and 50s, I think it's something like 25% of American moms were breastfeeding
their children and the vast majority of kids were formula fed. And it's not that that's
bad. Like full disclosure, I'm a botan paid for consultant for the formula industry. Like
I'm not saying formula is bad, but I think this testifies to people's views of like,
of parenting as an endeavor, right?
In the middle of the century, it was very outsourced, right?
Outsource the feeding, outsource the education,
just trust whatever the school is offering for your kid,
I'm sure it's fine.
Whatever's on TV, I'm sure it's fine,
just put the kid in front of the TV.
Well, you know, whatever's at church, I'm sure it's fine. They'll do religion.
Parent doesn't need to handle religion. That's church's job.
Outsource everything, right?
So you get this very outsourced model of parenting in the mid 20th century. Um,
as people just sort of assumed that like, if you keep the kid alive,
everything's fine.
Cause that's the goal because they could remember a time when kids didn't all live, right? And they could see that they were entering a period where if you
really tried, you could keep your kid alive. And so that they entered a period where it was really
possible to keep almost all the kids alive and they hadn't thought about all the new difficulties
that might create. And I think the generation, the baby boomer generation that grew up that way, and then also the kind of
the Gen X generation, kind of experienced
this very hands-off parenting,
and it didn't actually go very well for them.
That is, they saw that culture started,
intergenerational cultural transmission was breaking down,
right, that gaps between generations and social values, that intergenerational cultural transmission was breaking down, right?
That gaps between generations and social values,
attitudes and behaviors got much bigger.
This is empirically true that this did happen.
So we see a much faster pace of social change
that caused a lot of people to be like,
whoa, whoa, maybe we don't wanna do that.
And also you get things like rising discourse about fatherlessness, um, single parents, um,
you know, obviously you get things with drug abuse, whatever. Um,
what was the birth rates during, uh, baby boomer generation?
It's a bit over three per woman, I believe in the U S maybe even almost four
at one point.
I wonder whether just adding in, uh in another element here, I wonder whether, because
living standards were rising so much into generational competition theory, they
were just so much further ahead.
So that discussion we had before about expectations versus reality, living
standards versus living expectations.
Yeah.
Let's say the kids are doing great.
We don't need to do any parenting.
Like look how much farther ahead they are than us.
We went on a holiday twice this year.
I didn't even get to go on holiday at all when my
parents were growing up because you were growing
up during world war two.
Yeah, exactly.
So, I mean, that's exactly what's going on.
Is that that generation actually, the metrics that
they were used to measuring against were met.
Okay. Um, and then you get in the eighties, you get the kids who came out of this looking around
to being like, okay, um, that didn't actually go great. Like we, we broke pieces of society.
We didn't mean to. And also just feeling like their childhood maybe
wasn't actually quite what they wanted it to be.
Um, or that they didn't have a close attachment to their
parents or like, like, or also as the outsource things got
more and more expensive, like, wow, it costs a lot to put your
kid in childcare all the time.
Um, and also what happened as a result of this is because
children became in some sense, less similar to the parent generation on average.
It meant that there was more heterogeneity in society, like basically
more diversity and I don't just mean like racial ethnic, I just mean like
more diversity in how you think you should fold your laundry.
Okay.
Like just like, it's just more likely that like children and their parents or their
grandparents just didn't share a view on, you know, what kind of yard maintenance you
should do.
Just these little things like within a family are a little bit irksome.
And as a result, you get a generation coming up that's more aware, wow, the world is really
diverse.
I can't just trust that whatever's on TV is good, right?
It might not replicate my values.
I can't just trust that whatever's in the schools is correct.
It might not replicate my values.
Right.
And so you get the rise of parenting, right?
The idea that like, actually we have to protect our children
from becoming drug abusers.
You're talking about, uh, parenting is kind of stepping into the
natural flow of children just finding it out on their own. Right, exactly. You're
stepping in and you're saying, okay, I can't outsource everything. I have to do it.
I can't just put my kid in school. I have to make sure they get a good education.
Now, initially that means choosing more carefully about outsourcing. Okay, I'm
not just gonna put my kid in whatever school. I'm gonna make sure they get a
good school. I'm gonna make sure they get a good school. I'm gonna make sure they get good childcare.
I'm gonna make sure they get, you know,
good formula for them or whatever.
You know, it's this quality idea.
But I think what we're seeing now
is even more that branded parenting,
things like free range parenting,
helicopter parenting, gentle parenting,
all these like types of parenting that people do. Okay.
I think what's going on is this people realize how diverse society actually is
and how they really do want to see certain
of their values replicated in their children.
They are looking for parenting strategies
that they never learned as a child, right?
Because they just kind of were put in childcare and that was it.
They're looking for strategies to replicate their values in the next generation.
And the result is that they have a very costly vision of parenting, right?
Their vision of parenting involves doing all these things exactly right,
these strategies, these disciplinary modes, like whatever it is, it's like a very long to-do list
of how to be a good parent. Okay? So I think basically what's going on here is,
yes, rising expectations of what your kid is going to have. And then also as society became more
heterogeneous in various ways, more and more parents realized, oh, to get what I want out of
raising a child, that is raising someone who's like me in certain valuable ways,
I have to do a lot of effort. So that raises the cost.
Yes.
So that therefore means even if living standards are higher, even if real world money is more
available, even if all of the things that
economists look at and say, why is it that
people think that having children is so much
more expensive?
They were poor in the past and they had more
children.
There is this degree of investment, this sort
of expectation, investment expectation.
Going back to what you said before, to do with this pivoting
in living standards, it was a fascinating study about UBI that just came out recently, what for the people that didn't get to see that, can you just
give a 30,000 foot view?
Yeah, there's, um, there's a growing interest in universal basic income,
basically just giving people a monthly check for nothing.
Um, and some of the big donors interested in it, um, put some
money into a pilot project.
Okay.
Basically they got a bunch of people, gave them a thousand bucks a month
and survey and kind of did collected follow-up data on them.
And what they found is, um, for every dollar you gave them, um, their
earnings declined, I think it was like 25 cents, so to speak.
Um, their spending rose like 30 or 40 cents, 45 cents maybe.
And it rose kind of across categories.
It wasn't like it all went to one thing.
It rose in like necessaries like food,
and it also rose in like luxury categories like vacations.
Actually, the biggest growth was in gifts.
People gave more gifts to other people when they had a UBI.
And all the UBI recipients, by the way, were poor. They People gave more gifts to other people when they had a UBI.
And all the UBI recipients, by the way, were poor.
They were all low-income to begin with.
So there was, if you think about it,
for every dollar you give, there was a reduction in income,
an increase in spending,
but not enough to count for that whole dollar.
So you would expect that net worth should have gone up.
Like people should have had more savings then by, you know,
25 cents per dollar, 20 cents per dollar.
In fact, their net worth declined by about six cents per,
you can think of it as like by six cents per dollar
kind of network is a little more complicated because it's not
it's a stock, not a flow. So for every, you know, $1,000 of
monthly income, they ended up with about 60 bucks less of
net worth at the end. So there's two problems here. One is just like, that seems bad. We got success.
We got people to work less, spend more, and also like not have any wealth building despite
higher income. So that's unfortunate. But two, it really raised a question about even the
credibility of some of these results, right? Because you add
up, you know, 25 cents less of earnings, 40 cents more of
income, a bit less of net worth. And there's a huge resist,
there's like a 35 cent residual of like, where did that money go?
Like, did they just like, did they just light it on fire?
Like, did they buy something they didn't wanna report?
Like something illegal?
Did they cut their work by more than they reported?
Did they increase spending on some other category more?
So it not only, and they also found like there
was not a big improvement in housing conditions. There was
not, there really were not improvements in, there were not
big improvements in almost any category that would be some,
like obviously if you give people money, they'll spend
more. That's not an improvement. That's just like a thing that happens.
So we didn't see improvements in, we didn't see big improvements in like objective
things that we might all agree are a good thing. And so that should really concern us about UBI, that it's probably not going to result in great
liberation for people.
It might just result in like more gambling.
Well, a place that people might go to is they might say, well,
you know, you that's right.
Perhaps they haven't got that much to show for it financially
at the end of the day, but what happened with regards to an
alleviation of stress.
And they also looked at that as well.
The cash transfer resulted in large
but short-lived improvements in stress and food security,
greater use of hospital and emergency department care
and increased medical spending of about $20 per month
in the treatment relative to the control group,
suggested that the use of other office-based care,
deep dental care, blah, blah, blah.
However, we found no effect of the transfer
across several measures of
physical health as captured by multiple well-validated survey measures and
biomarkers we drive from blood draws can rule out even very small improvements
in physical health and the effect that will be implied by cross-sectional
correlation between income and health lies well outside of our confidence
into the, so people weren't happier.
They weren't less stressed.
They didn't have, uh, improvements
again, on any of these measures that are like things we would all universally
want, like if we found a transfer that just made everyone happier and healthier,
it'd be like, great, cool.
Like on these measures that are like clearly like good outcomes.
Yeah, they found nothing.
It didn't help people at all.
There was, there was another study, a different study that looked specifically at home that gave a UBI specifically to homeless people. And the goal was we will give them a
thousand bucks a month to find housing. And they found in the long run, there was no across,
I think it was three year follow up. After three years, there was no difference in the homelessness rate of people who got like
zero dollars or a thousand dollars a month.
Um, simple reality was that giving homeless people money did absolutely
nothing to get them in house and a lot of money, a thousand bucks a month.
You can pay rent for that for one person.
Is that not something to do with the culture or the lifestyle or the substance
dependency that these people have kind of being a lingering overhead that doesn't something to do with the culture or the lifestyle or the substance dependency
that these people have kind of being a lingering overhead that doesn't allow
them to get the exit velocity to get out of the bottom of homelessness?
Yeah, there's, it would be easy to look at these UBI studies and say what these
tell us is that nothing helps, the poor are permanently poor. That is in fact
not what they show because we actually have a lot of interventions we do know work. Focused substance abuse
interventions like getting people in rehab, getting them support, stuff like
that, does cause people to be less poor. High quality therapeutic interventions
like based on cognitive behavioral therapy, not low quality random talk
therapy things but like high quality therapeutic interventions
can actually boost people's like long run income and health.
Certain religious interventions are actually known
to boost long run income and health.
There are a lot of things that we can do
that do lift people out of poverty.
I mean, education, obviously.
Handing them cash turns out not to be one.
It just isn't. So yeah, it is the case that like if you are a donor and you have like 20 million dollars that you want to spend on helping
poor people in the US, let's say, you would be way better off investing that in like focused substance abuse treatments.
Okay.
Um, then a UBI, okay.
Giving cash doesn't do a thing for long run outcomes.
It actually does.
It does briefly, briefly it does, right?
A change in income is a better predictor of happiness
than absolute income.
Like, eventually everything normalizes
until you disequilibrate it again.
Yeah, so, but I mean, the point is like,
if you actually want to alleviate poverty,
not just for the next six months,
but, you know, actually lift people out of poverty,
like, UBI is just not the way to do it.
It just isn't.
And at this point, we have decades of
experiments and pilot studies and all these things. Looking at this, we know that this doesn't work
for long-term poverty alleviation. Now, there are things that are somewhat similar to UBI's
that do work. So conditional cash transfers. So for example, child
allowances. Sometimes when people really want to argue
against child allowances, they will sometimes call them UBI's.
They are not UBI's. Not everybody gets them. You don't
get them forever. You get them conditional on something. That
is having custody of a child.
And it's important to mention having custody, okay? Like people who are the genetic parent
of a child, but not actually like in the same tax unit as them, don't get a child allowance,
right? So when we give people a child allowance that rewards them for actually raising
their child co-residentially, that's good. Okay? It generates slightly more children,
it generates slightly less divorce, and it generates slightly more spousal or
co-residence of partners. We know this, we have decades of studies showing this.
So UBI, where you give people money for nothing,
not a good policy.
Conditional transfers where you give people money
for something can be a decent policy,
depending on what outcome you're targeting
and what you're trying to do.
It's still expensive, but it's important to distinguish between UBI, which
now I think we really should be able to put this to bed, that it's not a good idea, and
conditional cash transfers, which may in fact be very worthwhile.
So when we get back to the fertility rates and where we're at at the moment, what are people saying,
modern people right now about the number of kids that they do or don't want and their
reasons for wanting or not wanting children?
In surveys, like in the US, for example, people report that their ideal number of children is 2.3. If instead of
ideal you ask them to rate how happy they would be with different family
outcomes like 0, 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, whatever, people's sort of weighted average happiness tends to be about 2.3.
If you ask people what the ideal family size for a family is, their average tends to be 2.3.
Any way you ask that's getting at desires, wants, ideals for the US, it's 2.3.
If you ask about intentions, how many kids you intend to have, that's a very different
question. That's not asking what people want. It's asking, okay, down to brass tacks, thinking
about your life as it really is. It's almost like asking, down to brass tacks, thinking about your life as it
really is. It's almost like asking people how many kids do you think you can afford to have?
Okay, all of us would recognize how many kids can you afford to have is not the same thing as how
many kids do you want. Okay, how many kids do you intend to have depending on the population subgroup and the exact survey and all it ranges from like 1.85
to 2.1 in the US actual fertility in the US is 1.6 so people are way short
they're about 0.7 kids short of what they want and And they are about point two to point four kids,
point two to point five kids,
short of even what they think
they're really realistically gonna have.
They're not even meeting what they think of
as like their realistic goals.
And just presumably the number of, when you look at actual
fertility rates, that's people who've had kids so far.
When you ask people what they intend to, that's future
focused and given different ways.
Yeah.
But given, given that we're on a decline in terms of
fertility, we can assume that the number of actual children
is going to continue to move away from that number of
intended children.
So I, I get that, but that Pew research that came out recently, like 10 percentage
point increase in the number of people under 50 saying that they didn't want to
have kids 37% to 47% between 2018 and 2023.
What do you make of that?
That's nearly 50% of people under the age of 50 saying that they don't want to
have kids unlikely to ever have kids.
So the actual prevalence of young people who say they don't want to have kids was 20
something percent.
Um, the 50 something was like the share of the share of childless young people who say the reason for being childless is because they just don't want kids.
So say, okay, among childless people, what share just don't want kids.
The actual share who say they don't want kids is like maybe 20, 20, 25%.
That sounds high.
25%. That sounds high. It's similar to what I find in my surveys. Let's say we do inhabit a society where the average
person wants 2.3 kids and also where, you know, 15 to 25% of
people say they want zero kids. We are inhabiting a society of
increasingly polarized desires, where there's people more and more clustering at zero and at three or four.
Not a lot of people saying like six.
So yeah, it is what it is. Long run for fertility, you know, you can have a society where everybody has two.
You can have a society where half the people have zero and half the people have four.
They both have a fertility rate of two on average.
So what we're seeing is, yeah, there is some rise in intended childlessness.
It's largely offset by rising desires for big families.
On average, desires haven't changed a lot.
And what we can see is that the fertility decline
we've had over the last 15 years,
it's bigger for unintended births.
That is unintended births have fallen like, I think it's like 40%.
What's unintended births?
Births people were not trying to have.
So like they were not planning to have a kid and they did. Whereas intended births have fallen
like I think a bit less than 20%.
So yeah, there's been a bigger decline
in unintended births, meaning that,
meaning you can think of that as like,
people avoiding having kids that they didn't wanna have,
that would be consistent with the idea
that there's more people wanting to avoid have kids and successfully avoiding. But we've also
seen a decline in intended births. That is people who do intend to have kids but are having fewer of
them. So I don't worry too much about like a growing share that say they want zero because,
okay, that's fine. You can want zero. people want four other people want to, on average, it
doesn't seem to be changing a whole lot.
You seem, uh, surprisingly, uh, positive or at least non-apocalyptic, uh, given
most of the demographers and the people sort of in this world that I'm speaking
to have this like cataclysmic, uh, perspective, it doesn seem to, the alarm bells don't seem to be going off quite as aggressively,
or maybe you're just holding it together well.
You know, falling fertility is a very serious problem.
It is. It's a problem. To me, my view, the biggest problem of falling fertility is I want people to have the lives they want to have.
I want to live in a free society where people are free to achieve reasonable, normal and acceptable life goals.
Two kids, that's a totally reasonable life goal. People should be able to achieve that.
If they can't achieve it, that's a serious problem in society, in my view. It also happens that low fertility has really
bad economic effects in the long run, it has bad political effects, it has all
sorts of bad effects in the long run. Those are less of a concern to me because
first of all I think it's kind of gross to use babies as like your instrumental solution to GDP.
Like have a baby for the GDP is like not a great pitch or like have a baby so we can recruit them into the army.
Like not a great pitch.
So to me, the biggest issue is just people not getting what they want.
So if people genuinely want zero, like that's not my
problem. I don't want zero. I want five. So I want to be able
to hit five. I want society to not put obstacles in front of
that. Because we are below desires on average, I'm very,
very strongly in support of pro natal policy. I think we
should be doing a lot more to help families hit their goals
and offset some of that standard of living
issue. But, and this is what I think is really important, you're right. There are a lot of people
who like me are pro-natalists. That is they want to see more kids born and they do adopt this really
apocalyptic tone. It's a fertility crisis, demographic winter, Koreans are going extinct. And I think that that tone is just a
massive mistake. The best predictor of people not
wanting kids is depression and anxiety.
Okay, if you want to know if someone wants to have kids,
ask them to rate one to 10 how happy they are.
The number they give you is going to be wildly correlated
with the number of kids they want to have.
The strongest predictors of low birth rates
at the individual level are fertility preferences and measures of mental health
People who are
depressed anxious uncertain sad
Worried about the future. They don't have kids
Why because the reason you have kids is to raise them and watch them become fruitful adults
And if you think that the future is scary and bad and awful
adults. And if you think that the future is scary and bad and awful, then you're not anticipating the joy of
children. You're anticipating watching your children suffer
in a bad world. Okay. So my view is that if you actually want
people to have kids shut up about a crisis. Okay, so the
only way to get more kids is to convince people that the future isn't awful.
That life-
So you're saying that the birth rate decline is downstream from a mental health crisis?
It's definitely related to it.
So, I mean, yeah, this is, you know, there's, there's these like broad structural
things about like the standard of living people expect, the standard of care they expect for kids,
all these things.
But there is also very concretely, yes, a mental health crisis.
I mean, pick your condition.
I mean, depression, anxiety, autism, schizophrenia.
I mean, these are very different conditions.
Or just like survey reports of sadness, survey reports of uncertainty, survey reports of beliefs about if the future is going to get better or worse.
Um, negative affect across all of these indicators associated with lower
fertility preferences and lower fertility behaviors.
Um, so like categorically, my concern is that some very well-intentioned people talk a lot about the fertility crisis,
and they are sabotaging efforts to address the problem of low fertility and the problem of people's fertility not meeting what they want.
Because by talking crisis, crisis, crisis, crisis, we're telling people, look, if there aren't more babies, the future will
be very bad. Okay. But think about what that says. Somebody says, okay, so then I shouldn't
have a baby. You should bring your children into this world that's reliably good.
Yeah. Because think about it. And also there's no way I can guarantee other people will have
babies. I can't change the social fertility rate. I can only change my own fertility rate.
So if I see that other people aren't having babies, I'm not going to have babies
because a world of low fertility is a world of bad outcomes. And I don't want to bring my baby.
It's like, it's this like vicious cycle where like, because people are so
convinced that like of doomerism, they don't have kids.
And then the pronatal people come out and tell even more doomer-y stories.
When what we should say is like, look, if you individually have kids, And then the pronatal people come out and tell even more do-mer-y stories.
When what we should say is like, look, if you individually have kids,
like desired children, this isn't as true for like unintended children. Okay. But people who want kids when they have kids is associated with very positive outcomes.
They have higher happiness in the short run. They have higher happiness in the long run. Ten years out they're happier than otherwise similar people who want to
get and didn't have them. People who want kids and don't have them have higher rates
of prescription of anti-psychotics, anti-anxiety, anti-depressants. They are more likely to
have quite adverse mental health outcomes. People who want kids and have them report just a
whole range of better life outcomes and people who want kids and don't have them report considerably
worse subjective well-being in the long run. Now for people who don't want kids, it's a
different thing. Okay. I'm not trying to bully anybody to having kids that they don't want to have.
But there are so many people that want kids and they're not having them because they're
worried about this, that, and the other.
And we just need to say, look, first of all, we know in longitudinal surveys that when
people have kids, they increase the number of kids they think is ideal. So think about what that says.
Once you have the experience of having kids, you decide you want more of them on average.
Okay? So like my, my encouragement to people is like, kids are going to be way more fun than you
think. Like they're going to be way more interesting than you think. They're going to be full of way
more value than you think. Um, and once you have them, you're way more likely to be like, I kind of like to have more of
these than being like, dang, I would have preferred fewer.
That latter response, it does happen.
People do say that in surveys.
There are people who say, you know, I love my kids, but if I had it to do over, I might
have one less.
There are people who say that. It's not,
it's not never, I'm not going to say that never happens. It does. But on average,
people who say, you know what, I had these kids and wow, yeah, I'm totally raising my number now,
way more numerous. Have you had a look at retrospective regret of childless people?
What is there to learn from that? How common is it? So actually the Pew survey you mentioned has some surveys on, has some stuff about this. They didn't
ask it exactly. But yeah, I mean, childless people do report various difficulties arising from
childlessness. Crucially, the Pew survey asked young people,
do you expect such and such a thing to be a challenge
with childlessness?
And then they asked older childless people,
was such and such a thing a challenge?
And they found that across numerous categories,
the challenges were more common
than the young people expected.
What sort of challenges?
Feeling disconnected from the next generation, feeling like you lost friends
because you didn't keep up, you didn't have kids, feeling like you were lonely, feeling
like I forget the list they had, they had a bunch of these different things.
And across the vast majority of them, current people who don't want kids
were underestimating just how big the difficulties, challenges, and regrets can be.
That doesn't mean that childless people all regretted it. Many of them didn't.
But it is the case that young people are greatly overestimating the benefits of childlessness, so to speak, or the costs of having children.
Or maybe they're underestimating the benefits of having children.
However you want to put it.
It is the case that when people get to retirement, it's not that free solo life is just not going
to be as good as they think it is.
What is the relationship that you see between marriage rates, desire to get married, mating
crisis, desire to get into a relationship and desire for children and stuff. Does this piece together in a nice, neat
fashion, does it make a cohesive narrative or
is it individual things going on?
So fertility of married people has not declined
nearly as much as the fertility of unmarried
people.
Um, and crucially, the main thing that's happening in fertility is just that people are not getting
married as much.
Okay.
They're getting married later or not at all.
But are people not getting married because they don't want kids before they get into
a relationship?
Um, no, because we know that desire for children is approximately stable, right?
So we know that.
We know desire for children is approximately stable.
We also know desire for marriage is approximately stable.
About 90% of people say they want to get married, most of them in their mid to late 20s.
That's the typical age, which is a bit younger than actual marriage ages right now.
So we know that that's what people want. And yet marriage
ages are rising and rising and a growing share of people will never marry. So
we're actually seeing a shortfall in marriage versus desires. Okay. People who
get married, their fertility rates are not declining that much, not declining as
much. And if everyone got married at the same rates they did in
2007, fertility would not have declined nearly as much. Most of the decline is just because
of less marriage. Okay? So yeah, a mating problem is a huge part of the story. And there
are so many reasons people are not marrying as much as they used to.
I mean, one of the biggest ones is just an economic change.
It is the case that if you look at like data from dating apps,
women swipe on men's incomes at the same rate that men swipe on women's conventional physical attractiveness.
I'm not sure you can say that says something bad about us as a species if you like.
It's facts. It hasn't changed in, you know, 2,000 years.
Actually, let me take that back. It hasn't changed in 600,000 years. Actually, let me take that back. It hasn't changed in 600,000 years. So status will always,
men's status has always been fertility correlated. It just always has been. It's always been the case.
There's virtually no exceptions that high status men, high income men in money societies have more
kids than low status men. That being the case, you have to ask what,
not what has happened to men's income generally.
You have to ask what happened to men's income
at the marriage ages that if you get married in those ages,
it's easy to have two or three kids.
So that's gonna be marriages in your 20s.
If you're not married in your 20s,
odds of getting to two and especially three kids decline.
So what has happened to the income of men in their 20s?
In the US at least, it has actually declined over the last 20 years, 30 years.
Men in their 20s are actually getting poorer in inflation adjusted terms. This is not true for men generally.
This is not true for older men. This is not true for the population generally.
People are getting richer. Incomes are going up. But for men in their 20s, that's the group for whom
income is sort of setting the bar for marriage. Because I'm assuming that women in their 20s are like not getting less attractive or something. Okay. And again, these are not the only things that
guide marriage. Obviously what really guides individual marriage is like love and shared
values and shared life orientation. But funny enough, people tend to fall in love with people
who are within a certain socioeconomic band
of their expectations and within a certain range of, it sounds so callous to say, but
like physical attractiveness. Yes, individually people marry for love. Collectively humans don't, they marry for other things. So,
it is the case that men's marriageability in the key marriage ages for supporting fertility has
declined really, really sharply. That is, their
incomes have fallen, particularly compared to women. That being the case,
there's less marriage. And in fact, marriage rates and particularly fertility
rates have declined at exactly the ages where men's incomes are declining. So,
early and mid-20s. And at the ages where men's income is rising in the 30s and 40s, fertility rates have actually risen.
There actually has been an increase in fertility at those ages.
Not enough to offset the decline in earlier ages, but a little bit.
So basically what's going on with the marriage market on a deep level is that
young men are poor.
They cannot support a family, particularly
a family whose consumption expectations are set by the joint incomes of two single people
with no kids. So if you spend your whole 20s setting your consumption expectations based
on zero care responsibilities, your parents are still young enough
that you're not having to take care of them in old age.
You don't have kids, you don't have a spouse or something like that.
And then you come into marriage and that's how your consumption
expectations have been set.
Well, it's really hard to have kids.
You mentioned before about this decline in unintended kids. You mentioned before about this decline in unintended kids.
I know that you've looked recently and I've been doing quite a bit of
research into this too, about the attitudes toward contraception in the US.
I think if I was to put a couple of bets on a roulette table for upcoming public
health, few rules, uh, things that people are going to be really, really concerned about. couple of bets on a roulette table for upcoming public health
furors, things that people are going to be really, really concerned about. I think we're already sort of starting to see
a lot of women talk about the psychological challenges that
come from hormonal birth control, physiological changes,
things that you can lock in for the rest of your life if you
take it during puberty and periods where the brain is in development.
I also think microplastics in water,
I think toxic environments, molds, et cetera,
is also gonna be another one.
But it's surprising to me that it seems to be this,
it seems to be two things at the same time.
It seems to be both a very pro contraception
and a very anti contraception culture.
I don't know whether that's just two different cohorts and I get to like hear the
most vociferous of one and the most vociferous of the other.
But do you know what I mean?
There's this sort of superposition I'm in.
Yeah.
Everything natural except reproduction.
Right.
That's, that's the vibe, right?
All natural except reproduction.
Um, so, uh, I think that's a real thing.
Um, on the fertility front, um, you know, you
sometimes hear things about falling sperm
counts or falling testosterone counts.
Um, this is basically fake.
Um, sperm counts are not falling in
scientifically studied cohorts.
Um, and anyways, the actual effect of the kinds of variation in sperm counts we observe,
the implication would just be you need to have sex for an extra two or three months to have a child,
not that you would become infertile.
Testosterone changes are a bit more complicated because there's just measurement of testosterone is extremely problematic.
It's problematic since like it's very difficult and it changes over the course of the day
and it changes in different parts of your different types of tissue samples or cell
samples.
But either way, these kinds of things don't appear to be affecting fertility. Microplastics, there are some BPA, there are some
plastics that could impact fecundability. So far, a couple
studies have gone looking and have not found anything, or at
least not anything significant. But it could be, I mean, these
things last a long time. So it could be that takes us a few
generations for them to build up in sufficient levels to do damage. In general, and in contraception, the best evidence suggests that hormonal
contraception can increase time to pregnancy after cessation by a few months. That is, it can take a
few months longer to become pregnant. And for some methods, it can be a lot longer.
I think long acting reversible contraceptions like implants can increase time to conception
by quite a few months.
There doesn't appear to be long term identifiable damage to fecundity.
The problem with all of these studies is that people who choose to use more intense contraception are also people who want fewer children.
So even when they go off contraception, they have fewer children because the
reason they were using contraception is because they're people who want fewer
children.
Have you, have you, have you found any research looking at how, uh, women's
desire for children, desired number of children, intended children changes
when they're on or off birth control?
I haven't, I know that there's some stuff that's,
I don't know the state of the research on this,
but I've seen headlines saying that like when women
are on certain forms of hormonal birth control,
that like the type of like body type that they will swipe on
on dating apps is different or something.
I don't know how credible that is.
In general, I'm skeptical of most of these types of findings
for a variety of reasons.
One, we have really good controlled studies
that look at time to pregnancy.
And we can see that for couples
who are not using contraception of a given age and actively trying to conceive,
time to pregnancy hasn't changed in decades, as far back as we
can study. It just isn't the case that time to pregnancy has
changed. People's ability to conceive hasn't changed a bit.
So, and time to live birth hasn't either that is implied miscarriage rate has not changed.
So that tells me that whatever's going on under the surface, it's not dramatically
altering fertility.
There could be things on mate selection.
I would totally believe that the rise in, but honestly contraception wouldn't be the
main thing I'd point to. I would point to the rise of like SSRIs, um, and other, um, you know,
antidepressants, anti anti anxiety, anti-psychotics, um,
more directly impact the brain, the share of the population, or I mean,
frankly also just drugs, um, like pot and opioids and like,
there are so many other substances that would be like my go-to on like,
if something is messing with mate preferences,
what could it be?
Maybe the really like quite powerful drugs
that are designed to alter your neurochemistry.
Like, let's not say those things are bad.
Like, I don't know if anyone's ever studied
what they do to mate preference.
They do alter libido, obviously, but I don't know if anyone's ever studied what they do to make preference. Um, they do alter libido obviously. Um, but I don't know what they do to make
preference and things like that or views of commitment. Um, uh, and also like I
said earlier that like depression is itself, um, sort of anti-natal. Um, so
like you could find that like antidepressants are anti-natal, but they're
less anti-natatal than the depression
that they get.
Oh my God, that's so funny.
Right?
So you could get these kinds of really interesting challenges.
And so in general, my view is like, we live in a chemical soup.
We just do.
We always have. In ancient times, they weren't chemical soup. We just do. We always have. In ancient times,
they weren't chemical soup. It was just that humans were so
hungry. They were just I mean, you look at old cookbooks and
they were eating stuff that today, you know, 20% of the
population is allergic to and it smells like death and like it's
awful. But they were eating it like twice a month. Or like, I
mean, you look at the,
it's there's something called the Eastern agricultural complex,
which is pre corn agriculture among, um,
native American tribes in the Eastern U S. Um, and like,
you can look it up. There's like all these crops that you're like,
what is this crop? And I kept telling my wife, like, I really wish,
I really wish there was like a restaurant that would like,
like a hipster restaurant that would like, like a hipster
restaurant that would like use only pre Colombian
crops and animals and like have a whole menu on it.
It was like, this would be a cool idea.
And then I looked up the crops and I was like,
okay, so like this one has like an aroma that can
be smelled down the street when it's cooked.
Like this one has great proteins.
It's like better than quinoa.
Also, like everybody's allergic to it.
Like so many people are allergic to it.
Like they're awful plants.
Like there's a reason that as soon
as Native Americans got corn, they were like,
yeah, I want corn, corn's amazing.
So like one of the things we don't talk about
with like the great diversity of crops
that people used to eat is that like so many of them were really bad and like bad for you
and unpleasant or maybe they're good for you, but they're unpleasant or they're pleasant,
but they're bad for you.
That's not to say declining crop diversity isn't a bad thing.
It is like we should preserve rare varietals.
Not the ones that stink the street out there.
Yeah, exactly.
Like I would be okay if dirt. Do you know about dairy?
Do you are familiar with Darien?
Yes. Yeah. That's stinky fruit.
Yeah. I'd be okay if it went extinct.
Like I know like all of Asia cries out in horror at what I just said,
but like that one I wouldn't mourn. Um, but, uh, but by and large,
um, like humans have always been putting just weird stuff into their bodies.
And so I think we are less fragile than we think we are.
And we talked at the beginning about how humans lived in environments where so
many people were dying and yet they still carried on.
And I think we're less fragile than we think we are.
And to be honest, this is actually part of what I think is part of the cultural
change we need to promote fertility is like kids are not as fragile as you think they are.
That standard you think they have to have, they might not need that standard.
They might be tougher than you think they are.
They might can just be okay playing outside zone.
You might not need to intensively do every little thing. And beyond that, you are less fragile than you think you are.
Children are not going to break you.
You're going to be okay.
Some people won't be.
Most people will be like, you are going to be okay.
You are less fragile than you think you are.
You can do it.
You can raise kids.
You can parent.
You will pull it off.
you think you are, you can do it.
You can raise kids, you can parent, you will pull it off.
Does political polarization play a role here?
You know, we recently came out of the DNC where they were offering free vasectomies and abortions outside, I think.
Uh, but you know, what about the role of, yeah, polarization in
political beliefs, parents wanting their kids to date
somebody from within their own political class, the increasing importance of politics in mate
selection and in people's lives than perhaps it used to previously?
Yeah.
So politics plays a role in a couple of ways. One, political cultures are sets of ideas and values
and memes and norms and behaviors and social ties.
The fact that one side of politics in the US at least
has adopted particularly
anti-natal social values.
That is, I mean, a great example of this is like
the average fertility rate of Republican elected officials
at the federal level is something like 2.6 kids.
The average fertility rate of Democratic ones is like 1.4.
So like there's like microcosm right there, right?
Interestingly, that means that Democrats are closer to the societal average.
So Republicans are the weirdos, but they're like the weirdos continuing humanity.
But so I do think this is unfortunate that we kind of have this perpetuating political culture
on one side that promotes norms of anxiety, fear, and uncertainty about children and presents
children as burdens primarily. Let's not say Republicans are innocent. There's a lot of ideas
and values on the Republican side that I think are antinatal in a little
bit more of an oblique way.
Particularly norms around work.
For Republicans, everything is about jobs.
If it makes jobs, it's good.
But I'm like, if you employed a mom earning minimum wage, and her alternative was staying home with kids or a dad. It could be a dad
as well, though in practice, statistically, it's usually mom. And she has two kids. It's probably
more socially valuable for her to invest in her kids than flip burgers. So I'm actually not
interested in creating make work jobs, just to pull
people away from their families because I want to buff labor force participation.
That's just not what I value. In general, just the super work focus that
Republicans tend to have, I think is a cultural mistake. Work is rivalrous with family on some level.
So at the same time, I do think it's worth remembering
that political culture comes from somewhere, right?
We know that depression is not equally distributed
across the parties.
We know that certain types of fears about the future are not equally distributed across parties. We know that certain types of fears about the future
are not equally distributed across parties.
And as a result, I don't know how much we can blame
partisanship for what might actually be a difference
in religiosity or family background
or mental health
or a million other variables, right?
Is partisanship causing this
or is partisanship just reflecting
that this is the kind of divided society we live in?
Wow.
Yeah, well, I mean, increasingly fewer people
are disbelieving about the heritability
of sort of psychological dispositions,
whether that be introversion or extroversion or conscientiousness or
willpower or fucking hunger or political ideology, some things that are more
complex, which are an entire sort of suite of dispositions that all come
together.
But this is the, every time that I see it, every single time that I see an anti-natalist movement that intends on continuing, it blows my
mind.
It's like what you, this is mating seppuku.
Do you know how things work?
I just, dude, the thing outside of the DNC for the, I'm just going to make this
plain for the people that haven't seen the previous episodes.
Your political affiliation is heritable.
That means that if you are a card carrying, died in the wall Democrat, it is more likely
that your children will be Democrats as well, because there is a particular suite of traits,
personality types and dispositions that have led you toward that.
You can take personality tests for both Republicans and for Democrats, left leaning, right leaning,
Labour, Conservative, whatever it is.
And people will veer in that kind of a direction.
Given that your children are made up of the raw genetic
material that you're about to give to them,
if you are someone who wants your particular political
ideology to keep moving forward,
and you do not have children,
that is you basically seeding ground to the side that decides to
have children.
And it just, every single time that I see this, I'm like, is it, do they not know that
this is the case?
Are they, do they know and not care?
Are they so short, short term minded that they think, yeah, but we're going to, you
know, buff Kamala's votes by 0.1% in a key battleground state by doing the vasectomy
outside of the DNC truck?
I don't know.
I think there's a couple of things going on.
One is they're betting on the power of conversion, right?
Because most people, you have to think about the dynamic here. Because more socially conservative people have more children, a disproportionate share
of children are raised in conservative environments, and yet we end up with relatively even politics.
How?
Because conversion favors Democrats.
Okay?
So the Democratic Party is disproportionately made up of converts, so to speak.
People raised Republican and became Democrat.
And as a result, it's going to be people who selectively believe in the cultural power
of conversion.
And I would say they're not actually entirely wrong.
So I've done a fair amount of writing about this recently that heritability of fertility turns out to be very complicated.
It's true if you have a norm that leads to zero fertility, okay,
that weeds itself out relatively quickly.
Unless it has a reason for people to select it for fitness in the time being.
Right?
has a reason for people to select it for fitness in the time being. Right? So if there's a norm that,
you know, gives you zero fertility, but also it just hacks the part of your brain that handles dopamine, you'll choose it every time. And eventually the species will just go extinct.
Like species can just go extinct. Like it not inevitable that fertility is 2.1.
We could just stop having babies and go extinct.
And there are pieces of your brain that can be hacked
through drugs, through medicine, through congenital defects,
through whatever.
Yes, dopamine receptors can get fried.
People can become addicted to things that kill them. through whatever, like yes, dopamine receptors can get fried.
People can become addicted to things that kill them.
People can, it is possible that we could just stop breeding.
There's no reason that fertility has to return to two.
And I've actually run a lot of simulation models on this
that like, let's say you have like the Amish, okay?
Are the Amish ever going to become a majority of the population?
The answer is absolutely not.
They never will.
If all you do is you take actual real world Amish fertility
over the last 50 years and trend it forward
and use real world rates of conversion
in and out of the Amish,
you find that the Amish are going to peak
at a maximum, a maximum of like 7% of American
population and they're never actually going to get there. The simple reality is conversion is
really powerful and assimilation is really powerful. There is not, for simple monogenetic traits that have certain types of fitness profiles,
genetic drift can be really powerful.
But most traits aren't like that, particularly if they're polygenic.
For example, politics, I mean height is quite heritable.
Okay, like height's very heritable.
Like it's like 80% heritability. And some behavioral traits are quite heritable. Okay, like heights very heritable like it's like 80% heritability
and some behavioral traits are quite heritable. There's debate about schizophrenia, but it appears to be relatively heritable. Okay. And it has behavioral implications and stuff like that.
Social values as a family set, which includes both politics and religion, is like one of the
least heritable traits sets that we know of, along with reproduction,
ironically. I mean, religion is actually highly environmentally sensitive, right?
The reason kids born in Saudi Arabia become Muslim is not because they have a Muslim gene,
right? It's because they're in a context that encourages Islam. Okay
And we know this because kids get adopted across countries and they're
Astronomically more likely to end up in their adoptive parents religion than their birth countries religion like Korean kids adopted in America Don't become Buddhist by and large. They become like, you know Presbyterian. Oh, there's a lot of Korean Presbyterians, too
so although there's a lot of Korean Presbyterians too. So in general, social values, they do have a,
I'm not gonna say they don't have a heritable component,
they absolutely do, depending on kind of how you define
heredity, because that's complicated,
and what social value you're talking about,
it ranges from like 3% heritable to like 40% heritable.
That's kind of the range you're talking about.
There aren't a lot of social values that are over 40% heritable to like 40% heritable. That's kind of the range you're talking about. There aren't a lot of social values
that are over 40% heritable.
Whereas there's a lot of physical traits
that are considerably over 40% heritable.
And then you get like traits that are kind of like,
like a medley of the two.
So you think about things like sexuality,
the super controversial one, intelligence.
And there's huge debates about heritability of these.
Sexuality is highly non-heritable, for example.
There's some genes that do relate to it,
but probably through gene environment interactions,
not directly gene.
The upshot of all of this, I'm kind of meandering,
the upshot of all of this is that like
conservative fertility advantage
does drive some
electoral advantage, but way less than you would think
Way less than you would think from just like a naive sort of breeders equation type of population genetic model
it really is the case that
The heritability rates are sufficiently low and
conversion rates sufficiently high
that they're, um, that conservatives don't get nearly as much electoral bang as you would expect from their fertility buck and their fertility buck is large. It's a very large fertility gap
between Republicans and Democrats. And in a pure hereditary environment, like this would be the last generation
of Democrats to have an electoral shot.
Yeah.
But we all know that's not what's going to happen.
We all know that politics are going to remain highly competitive for, and
Democrats have the same thing where they're like, look at how conservative
young people are, or look how liberal young people are. This is the last time conservatives will have a shot. And yet here we are. They think that every
generation, and it's still the case that politics are competitive on the same issues that they
always have been. So I would strongly caution against trusting to fertility to give your
cultural group a long, a longterm
majority, you really do need to persuade other people.
You mean that you can't shag your way out of some ideological cul-de-sac?
Probably not.
You, you actually need to make persuasive arguments.
So, so, you know, I think, you know, vasectomizing everybody who walks into
the DNC, whatever it's net social benefits or costs, it probably doesn't
have a huge effect on long run political equal equilibrium.
Okay.
Um, what are your predictions for the sort of future of Western fertility rates and stuff at
the moment?
Like, just can it's going to continue?
I mean, does it bottom out?
It has to presumably at some point it's going to be carried
by certain small subgroups that would maintain.
I mean, it could bottom out at zero.
Um, like, I mean, if you think about like, let's, let's imagine,
okay, let's just do like doomsday scenario.
And it turns out that like microplastics totally sterilize people.
Okay. Microplastics are also totally inescapable. They're like literally in
the air. Okay? Like unless you are wearing like a hazmat suit, you are getting
microplastics in considerable volumes. So if microplastics cause sterility, then Western fertility will bottom out at zero. Okay? On the other hand,
if low fertility is caused by, if falling fertility is being caused primarily by,
I don't know, pick your pet theory. If it's caused by like particular parenting norms, parenting norms are something that
probably could weed out fairly quickly in just a few generations.
Okay?
They're going to be highly volatile.
We know that they can be broken across generations because that's what has happened recently.
And we know that people with parenting,
specifically parenting norms that encourage more children,
that seems of values, that seems more likely
to have high heritability, like 0.3 or something,
than low, like 0.1, okay?
So like, if parenting norms are what's driving low fertility,
then maybe in 150 years, we're like back up to two.
Okay. People's expectations about the amount of
investment that's required in a child.
Yeah.
Yeah.
And people who are raised in like lower expectation
environments will, will, will be, will make up a larger
share of the generation.
And I don't think there's a compelling reason to think
that like super high expectations, parenting is necessarily
always can be super persuasive as a conversion prospect.
So, on the other hand, you can get things like if it's really just that like young men's incomes are crap,
that's not likely to go away because skills, bias, technical change in the economy is what it is.
You need skills. It takes a while to get in the economy is what it is. You need skills, it takes a while to get them,
it is what it is.
So that might not necessarily go away,
but that also wouldn't cause us to flatline at zero.
That might cause us to flatline at one,
or 1.2 or something.
So, you know, I tend to think that stuff like
skills, bias, technical change is a decent
explanation for those types of kind of structural economic factors and also things like parenting
or decent explanations for what's going on with fertility.
And I don't think the more apocalyptic explanations are reasonable.
And so I think that we're likely to see continuing decline for
quite a while. I think we'll bottom out somewhere between 0.4 and 1.3. And then
maybe there will be social subgroups that find ways to achieve higher fertility.
Whether that will be heritable will remain to be seen.
Fertility heredity requires stable transmission of the same traits across lots of generations to do the work. There's only one gene that's known to have long run positive
selection, um, across a long time span.
Um, so I'm, I'm a little, I'm just skeptical that we're going
to suddenly find a new gene that like drives super strong
positive selection.
Maybe we will. Um, right now we can see strong positive selection. Maybe we will.
Right now we can see that positive selection
for fertility across cultures is happening across
50 or so gene genetic loci,
but it's not the same ones in different cultures.
It's not like all Western,
and this is just among European societies.
Among Western European and American societies,
there's 50 or so genetic loci
that are among active selection,
but there's only one genetic loci
that's under active selection among like all groups.
And it's been under active selection for 30,000 years,
and it's still only 66% of the population.
So like, give it another 500 years,
it's still gonna be like 66% of the population.
So other than that, like there is selection
on fertility happening in different societies,
but like Dutch people are breeding for,
are like selecting on different genes than American people
and different than Estonian people
and different than Swedish people.
And like, it's just, it's different.
We're not gravitating towards like,
when it's not like we're finding a gene
that across populations, it's like predicting
higher fertility.
So I'm skeptical we're gonna find a new one.
What is a medium term future
with an upside down demographic table look like?
What will it be like to exist over the next going from wherever we're at now,
1.6 to somewhere between 0.4 and 1.3?
Yeah, it's going to be a lot of disappointment and a whole lot of cope.
So disappointment, there's going to be a lot of people not having the families they wanted to have. That will lead to various coping mechanisms.
Trying to encourage the younger generation not to make the same
mistakes, which might be an endogenous method of fertility increase. Or just
being angry at the next generation because you feel no attachment to
them, which could be an endogenous
mechanism for continued fertility decrease, um, uh, disconnection between
generations, sadness, insisting that you're not actually sad.
You never really wanted those kids anyway.
Um, people have different coping strategies, but it's going to be a sadder
and more disappointed society.
There will be reduced entrepreneurship, reduced pace of technological innovation.
There will be reduced economic growth rates. I mean, we've lived in societies with zero
population growth before. They were called feudalism. So we will see more inequality, greater concentration
of wealth across generations, because estates will be divided among fewer
people. Quite, I mean it's unpleasant. This is why people talk about a
fertility crisis, as we've already discussed, I really dislike that language.
And I think humans have survived unpleasant environments before.
Life in a low fertility future will be worth living.
It will be so worth living as human life has been for hundreds of thousands of years. So, and crucially,
the main obstacles to having kids are likely to be psychological. That is, if you can get
over the burden of like being married younger than your friends, not going on quite as many
international trips as your friends, and strategies, conducive for mental wellbeing, you'll probably have plenty of
kids.
It's going to be an interesting period.
I, the cope thing, I think is going to occur on an individual
level, but it's also going to occur on a macro level as well.
You know, it is so, I don't know because I live in this weird,
like degenerate internet
subculture niche, as opposed to sort of normie world.
Uh, but you know, people still talk about an overpopulation problem.
They talk about population boom, about limited resources, about
us running out of space, about us running out of energy, about all of this stuff.
And there are going to be all of these things that come along for the ride.
about all of this stuff and there are going to be all of these things that come along for the ride that you population decrease is going to result
unless we have some unbelievable automation and AI robotics stuff that
comes in to save the economy.
You're going to have to have a reduction in economic growth.
Okay.
So what's that going to cause?
It's going to be a lot of people that are very unhappy and start pointing
fingers all over the place and you go, well, the policies that you were putting
in place only not so long ago, or the, these are the fruits of those exact
suggestions and now it's something else.
It's something else is going on for you to complain about.
So yeah, I, uh, I'm, I'm.
Concerned quietly concerned.
I'm not as equanimous as you are,
although I'm actually feeling a little bit better
after today's conversation.
What's next?
When it comes to studies, where is your focus going to be
over the next year or two?
So right now, I'm the director of the Pronatalism Initiative
at the Institute for Family Studies.
And what we're focusing on right now is housing.
Housing is a major milestone for young people. It's one of those, one of those things that people tend to think like you shouldn't have
kids until your housing situation is kind of sorted out. And young people are
getting less and less likely, they're living in smaller and smaller houses and
they're less and less likely to own their houses.
And so we are exploring what can be done about that.
How can we make housing more affordable?
How can we make it a more useful method of long-term planning for young people?
How can we make it more accessible to them?
And what types of housing are most supportive of transition to family life?
So we're doing a lot of original research there and hoping to release some of that over
the next few months.
Dude, that's cool.
That's cool.
I'm a big fan of the Institute for Family Studies.
Brad's a fascinating writer.
And so why should people go?
They want to keep up to date with all of the work that you're doing. So IFS is a great place to look for a lot of the, um, the population work.
Um, if you want all my truly unfiltered opinions on this stuff, um, uh, you can
follow me on Twitter at Lymanstone KY.
Um, and, uh, and you can, you can get all my opinions there on, on all the sorts
of topics that, uh, that, that sometimes I should actually shut up about.
Hell yeah.
Lyman.
I appreciate you.
Thank you, man.
Good talking to you.