Modern Wisdom - #874 - Michael Morris - Why Are We More Divided Than Ever?
Episode Date: December 7, 2024Michael Morris is a professor of cultural psychology at Columbia University and an author. Why are humans so tribal? Despite our capacity for empathy and inclusion, why do we always gravitate toward g...roups of similar individuals? And is there such a thing as good tribalism? Expect to learn why tribalism exists and how it evolved in humans, why we can hate people outside of our group and why we become hostile, if the modern world has worsened tribal instincts, whether tribalism is actually a good thing for our society, why so many people identify as not the opposition instead of as for their own group and much more… Sponsors: See discounts for all the products I use and recommend: https://chriswillx.com/deals Get up to $600 off the Pod 4 Ultra at https://eightsleep.com/modernwisdom (use code MODERNWISDOM) Get a 20% discount & free shipping on your Chairman Pro at https://manscaped.com/modernwisdom (use code MODERNWISDOM20) Get 5 Free Travel Packs, Free Liquid Vitamin D, and more from AG1 at https://drinkag1.com/modernwisdom Extra Stuff: Get my free reading list of 100 books to read before you die: https://chriswillx.com/books Try my productivity energy drink Neutonic: https://neutonic.com/modernwisdom Episodes You Might Enjoy: #577 - David Goggins - This Is How To Master Your Life: https://tinyurl.com/43hv6y59 #712 - Dr Jordan Peterson - How To Destroy Your Negative Beliefs: https://tinyurl.com/2rtz7avf #700 - Dr Andrew Huberman - The Secret Tools To Hack Your Brain: https://tinyurl.com/3ccn5vkp - Get In Touch: Instagram: https://www.instagram.com/chriswillx Twitter: https://www.twitter.com/chriswillx YouTube: https://www.youtube.com/modernwisdompodcast Email: https://chriswillx.com/contact - Learn more about your ad choices. Visit megaphone.fm/adchoices
Transcript
Discussion (0)
Hello friends, welcome back to the show. My guest today is Michael Morris. He's a professor of cultural psychology at Columbia University and
an author.
Why are humans so tribal? Despite our capacity for empathy and inclusion,
why do we always gravitate toward groups of similar individuals? And is there such a thing as good tribalism?
Expect to learn why tribalism exists and how it evolved in humans,
why we can hate people outside of our group
and why we become hostile so quickly
if the modern world has worsened tribal instincts,
whether tribalism is actually a good thing for our society,
why so many people identify as not the opposition
instead of as for their own group, and much more.
Given that tribalism and polarization are two of the most
used words in all of 2024, feels like a timely opportunity to have this conversation and there
is an awful lot to take away from this one. Sleep isn't just about how long you rest but how well
your body stays in its optimal temperature range throughout the night and this is where 8Sleep
comes in. Just add their brand new Pod4 ultra to your mattress, like a fitted sheet.
And it will automatically cool down or warm up each side of your bed up to 20
degrees. It's got integrated sensors that track your sleep time,
your sleep phases, your HRV, your snoring,
and your heart rate with 99% accuracy.
It'll even start cooling or heating your bed for you an hour before you get into it.
That's why 8Sleep has been clinically proven
to increase total sleep by up to one hour every night.
Best of all, they have a 30 night sleep trial
so you can buy it and sleep on it for 29 days.
If you don't like it, they'll give you your money back.
Plus they ship internationally.
8Sleep is offering their biggest discount of the year.
Right now you can get up to $600 off the Pod 4 Ultra by going to the link in the description
below or heading to 8sleep.com slash modern wisdom and using the code MODERNWISDOM at
checkout.
That's E-I-G-H-T sleep dot com slash modern wisdom and modern wisdom at checkout.
This episode is brought to you by Manscaped.
If you're still using an old face shaver from
three Christmases ago, please join us in the modern world. Manscaped brand new Chairman
Pro brings a professional shave directly to your home. It comes with two interchangeable
skin safe blade heads, whether you want to go completely clean or leave some stubble.
Plus both heads are designed to help reduce razor burn and irritation. So your skin feels smooth and comfortable after every shave.
Best of all, it can run for up to 75 minutes on a single charge and it's
waterproof. So you can use it in the shower or if you've got a particularly
hairy face right now,
you can get 20% off and free shipping worldwide by going to the link in the
description below or heading to manscaped.com slash modern wisdom and using
the code modernwisdom20
a checkout. That's manscaped.com slash modern wisdom and modernwisdom20 a checkout. This episode is
brought to you by AG1. Over the span of about a year I tried pretty much every green string that
I could find trying to work out which one was best and I came across AG1 which I've stuck with
for over three years because it's the best, It's the most comprehensive, it's the most highly
tested and rigorously formulated. They genuinely care about holistic health and that is why
I've got my mum and my dad to start taking it and my friends as well. And if I found
something better I would switch but I haven't which is why I still use it. If you're on
the fence you're not sure how it's going to taste or if it's going to work
for you, there is a 90-day money back guarantee so you can buy it and use it every single
day for three months and if you do not like it, they will give you your money back.
So if you want to replace your multivitamin and more risk-free, start with AG1.
Right now you can get a year's free supply of Vitamin D3 and K2, 5 free AG1 travel packs plus that 90-day money-back guarantee by going to
www.drinkag1.com slash modern wisdom. That's www.drinkag1.com slash modern wisdom.
But now ladies and gentlemen please welcome Michael Morris.
Why does tribalism exist?
Why did it evolve?
Well, tribalism is what got us out of the Stone Age.
It's what led to our human specific form of social life,
which is different from the social life of other social species,
including our cousins, the chimpanzees.
They live in minimally collaborative troops that can never get larger than about 50 individuals
or they turn into a bloodbath.
And we evolved some social quirks that enable us to live in culture sharing groups.
And these culture sharing groups allow for a level of collaboration and common
faith and common concern that is not present in any other social species.
So tribes are large groups united by shared culture and our tribal instincts were, you know,
adaptations or mutations that changed our psychology slightly to enable us to live in
this kind of group. And it just turned out to be the ultimate killer app of evolution, because once
we were in these culture sharing groups, it snowballed. You know, the, the cultures started getting more complex and more
adapted to the local ecology with each generation.
And then humans without becoming any brainier, we're more capable of surviving
and thriving because they could tap into these better cultures, each generation.
And they just left all the other species in their
dust, you know, so that's the basic story about
tribalism and we're stuck with it because it's in
our wiring.
You know, it doesn't always lead us to do the
right thing, but I still believe that it's mostly
adaptive, that our tribal instincts enable us to do
most of the things that we
are proud of and that we benefit from.
We notice it more when it leads us to do things that are dysfunctional.
And certainly there are examples of that in the world today.
So tribalism is predicated on culture, that without culture there can be no tribalism.
Is that a fair?
Yeah, that's a fair way to say it.
And that's not always, that's not always a prominent theme.
I mean, most of the talk about tribalism is by the sort of political pundits, you
know, and they're just kind of grasping for a catchall explanation to understand,
you know, the red blue rift and the record racial protests
and religious conflicts. And, you know, it's, it's an easy, quite facile thing to say,
you know, oh, it's our resurgent tribalism, you know, our tribal instincts have reappeared,
you know, and we're, we're, we're descending into tribalism and our democracy will never
be the same.
That's what we've been hearing.
I call it the trope of toxic tribalism.
It's a pretty despairing theme because the idea is that somehow the genie got out of the bottle and there's no way to get it back inside again.
I don't really think that's what's going on.
We have some bad conflicts in the world today, but that's true.
That's true. Every generation, every generation thinks they're presiding over the end times.
You know, what's new is this way of talking about the conflicts as though they
reflect some evolutionary curse, you know, some drive to hate other groups that is always going to be undermining us.
And I don't think that's true. I think that the tribal instincts are instincts that evolution
sculpted in order to help us be culture sharing animals. And that enabled us to live in very large collaborative groups.
And a side effect of those tribal instincts is that we sometimes get into conflicts with
other groups.
But they're not instincts for hostility, they're instincts for solidarity.
All of our instincts will lead us astray in some situations. We evolved to be attracted to sweet tastes because
fruit has a lot of nutrition. Now, if you live on a block with two donut shops, that wiring might
lead you to eat in an unhealthy way. It doesn't mean that it's an instinct for gluttony. It's an
instinct for fruit. And if we understand that, we have a better way of coping with the problem
than if we think that we're cursed with some flawed wiring, which is, you know,
a way of thinking about human nature that is kind of attractive in a tragic way.
You know, like that.
Why do you think it is?
Cause I, I've noticed this too.
And look, I, I'm going to fight the fight for the, it seems like people hate out
groups more than they love in groups today.
I'm going to try and, I'm going to try and stress test it as much as possible.
Okay.
Thank you.
But what do you think is so alluring about this sort of myth of martyrdom?
The, the, this woe is everything.
It's all sort of bro.
Why is it that that's all sort of broken.
Why is it that that's a tempting,
seductive talk at the point?
Well, it's this kind of Manichaean message, right?
That the world is coming to an end because,
along with our evolutionary blessing of intelligence
and familial loyalty has come
this, uh, curse of genocide.
And, you know, it's like, it's, you know, the person delivering the
message feels very important, you know, and the people listening are spellbound.
At least in the short term, it gives everybody the opportunity to be Cassandra.
Exactly.
Yeah.
And, you know, it's, it's yeah.
Tragic plays don't always, don't always talk about the end of the world, but, um,
even a tragic play, which talks about the end of a particular person's life or
success is, is kind of, um, riveting, you know, and always has been riveting.
Uh, but yeah, I think, i think i think that are the problem.
The problem in the media or the problem with thought leaders you know and you and i are probably you know.
Prone to this is that are we get rewarded for dramatic statements.
The more dramatic the more worrisome the more clicks clicks, you know, the more, you know.
So I think that there has been a cascade of Cassandras, you know, everybody wants to out
Cassandra the other Cassandras and become the pundit de jure.
So I think that some of that has been going on.
And what makes for good, what makes for good op-eds doesn't necessarily make for good Uh, did you or so I think that some of that has been going on and, um, yeah, yeah, yeah.
What makes for good, what makes for good op eds doesn't necessarily make for good
policies and that's, that's where I think, you know, a little bit more grounding in
science when we talk about tribalism would elevate the discussion a little bit.
The, uh, perverting incentives of clicks.
It's, I wish I had a more sexy answer because it just seems so cliche and so
obvious for me to say, well, people need to get attention and this is how the
attention economy works and blah, blah, blah.
And then downstream from what people say, other people believe because
people believe what others say, especially people who have been chosen by the
media or whoever to be the proselytizers about our current level of culture.
media or whoever to be the proselytizers about our current level of culture.
So I think an interesting framing is, is tribalism an us thing or a them thing?
Like how much of tribalism is in-group favoritism versus out-group persecution?
That's, that's really well put. I would say 95% of our tribal wiring is about us, not about them.
Our, our evolutionary forebearers did not have that much contact with other tribes.
You know, the, the population density and in a Stone Age Europe or, you know,
Africa before that was very low.
There were very few homo sapiens total.
The entire population was like a small city today.
So they didn't have that much contact with other tribes.
What they had a lot of contact with was each other.
They had daily contact with each other.
And what paid off evolutionarily was having traits and having
capacities that enabled you to collaborate effectively with the
fellow members of your tribe.
And, um, evolutionary scholars can, you know, slice the salami very thinly
when they talk about these adaptations.
I'm someone who, I've been a business school professor and a sort of political consultant,
organizational consultant for 20 years and also a researcher of cultural psychology.
So I tend to distinguish tribal instincts in three major waves, because I think it corresponds to three
major systems in our group psychology that we can still recognize in ourselves today,
and that, you know, effective leaders or activists or managers or coaches draw upon and harness
today.
So I can go into those, but I think I would say an answer to your question is that it's 95% us instincts.
They're not them instincts because them instincts just wouldn't have been adaptive, right?
You know, it wouldn't have been adaptive to go looking for other tribes to fight with.
Yeah, understood.
So just to linger on that, I want to get into a peer, ancestor,
et cetera, instinct in a bit, but what, what does it mean to have an us instinct
in absence of a them instinct?
What does that mean?
Surely only by there being a them can we define an us.
Well, I'm I'll agree partly with that.
I think that an out group is often a foil that allows for a more precise
definition of the in-group and a stronger feeling of distinctiveness in the in-group.
And part of in-group identity is usually trying to find some way to feel slightly
better than another group.
And it's interesting when you look at studies all around the world, it's not
like every group feels like they're more technologically advanced than everyone
else, and it's not that everybody feels like they're better looking or that they
are better athletes than everybody else.
But almost every group in the world feels that somehow they're better looking or that they are better athletes than everybody else. But almost every group in the world feels
that somehow they're more humane.
You know, they're sort of more human than the other group.
In a lot of indigenous groups,
the name for their own group is also the name for human.
So the implication being that, uh,
other people are slightly less human than, uh, than the end group.
So, um, yeah, so it is once, you know,
I can imagine that groups that had almost no
contact with other human groups, They had contact with animals.
They had contact with other kinds of things.
They had some basis for a comparison.
But in the modern times where we have a lot of contact
with other groups, it does become a salient part
of how we define the innards.
Interesting.
So I'm thinking about, again, ancestrally, let's think about some of the stories
that would have been told about what the weather is, about the moon being a god
or a goddess, about the sun being some sort of either benign or malevolent
or assistant force in some way.
You know, we have all of these personified stories that we have, which created them.
That them may be different species, different astral plane, different dimension, different whatever.
Um, but we have a them and an us.
Is it your opinion then?
I'm jumping way ahead, but this is just too interesting to ask.
What has been so perverting about the modern world to cause tribalism to
happen in the way that it is, is it simply our exposure to the number of different ways
that we can slice and dice and fracture and fragment society?
Yeah.
Well, I guess, you know, it depends on what we mean
by the modern world.
Do we mean like contemporary times?
I think that's what you mean.
Like, why do we have political tribalism?
So much worse than just a few generations ago.
political tribalism so much worse than just a few generations ago. And I think you can tell very clear stories about these particular historical developments.
So in the case of the political tribalism in the states and I think in many other Western democracies as well, you have political parties that
didn't used to be a salient basis of individual identity.
Two generations ago, my parents, they didn't know whether their neighbors
were Democrats or Republicans.
They didn't know if their work colleagues were Democrats or Republicans.
You know, there's the occasional Zlot who puts out a lawn sign, but that was,
that was it. But what has happened in the United States,
at least over the last two generations is you first had an enormous wave of
residential sorting, uh, as technology changed and transportation changed,
you know, you, you,
you were freer to live in any part of the country that you wanted to.
And liberals moved to the coasts and to the college towns.
Conservatives moved to the heartland and to the exurbs.
And that meant that you weren't going to the Norman Rockwell town meeting and
listening to a wide range of opinions and having to reconcile your beliefs
to your neighbor's beliefs. You were living in these ideologically inbred communities where when
you went to the grocery store or the softball game or whatever, you kind of heard opinions very
similar to the ones that you already held. And then starting in, I think the nineties, you know, you had this fracturing of the media landscape where
previously there were three network television shows that were required by the FEC to provide
very boring, bland, balanced coverage of every issue. So everybody was listening to Walter
Cronkite. Everyone was getting their news from the same place. And everyone knew that everyone else was doing
that, which contributes to that sense of common,
common knowledge, right?
Like I know you watched Walter Cronkite last night too.
So I feel a lot of commonality with you.
And what happened is you started to have cable news
stations that were 24 seven news instead of one hour a night.
And they were partisan, you know, you had Fox, you had MSNBC, and then
the next generation were websites that were even more partisan, like
talking points bulletin or, you know, the Hill.
And then you have social media feed, which is even more of an echo chamber
for reasons that, you know, we've all talked a lot about in the last years, you know,
that it's not just that I am connected to people
who share my politics,
but that I have ready opportunities to spout off
on my politics and get massively reinforced for it.
The costs of virtue signaling have gone way down
and the rewards have gone way up,
compared to the old days where you had to actually
go to a political rally and shout in someone's face
and maybe get shoved.
So yeah, so we've had this first, this fracturing
or this sort of change in our residential landscape
and then a change in our media landscape where we get our news from.
One of the fundamental tribal instincts is what I call the peer instinct and it's something we all
know about. It's our tendency to conform. It's our tendency to imitate what we see around us
more than we realize. It happens unconsciously. And that's become the primary way that we form
political beliefs. We sort of learn by osmosis from what we're seeing on TV and what we're
hearing across the neighbor's fence. And so we have these political beliefs that we think are
well-informed, but they come from a relatively narrow range
of the full opinion spectrum.
But because we consume more news than ever before,
we're confident in our beliefs.
And then we hear a politician from the other party
on television and we're just dumbfounded.
We're baffled.
We don't understand how they could possibly believe what they're saying.
And so then the attribution is, well, maybe they don't believe it and they're not sincere
and they're just saying it because it's in their self-interest, even though they know it's wrong.
Or they have some cognitive problems.
That accusation was being made about, you know, Biden, about
Trump, then about Harris, right?
You know, we had so many accusations of IQ deficiencies.
Apparently everybody's got senility or dementia now.
Yeah, of some kind, right?
So whether you think that they're being insincere or you think that they lack
cognitive acuity, it's, you know, it's not a very charitable attribution. And so there are negative
feelings. And so I think that's how this sort of antipathy for the opposite party and this
polarization has become so much more salient in the last two generations than it was in our parents or grandparents time. And it's not
something that came from an innate drive to hate. It came from this conformist instinct,
which is an us instinct. It's an affinity for the in-group. It's a desire to mesh with the
in-group that operates pretty subconsciously. And we're not aware of how much it drives us. And we, we, we kind of not naively think that we have an
accurate view of reality, but in fact, you know, our view of reality is conformist
and the other side's view of reality is conformist, but we don't realize our own
bias.
So their bias looks so extreme to us that we then start to attribute all
sorts of negative things to them.
So that's, that's what I think.
And what I think a lot of behavioral scientists think about
the political tribalism. And then there is also tribalism ethnically and racially in the United
States that has escalated in the last 10, 20 years after a long period of progress.
And, you know, I think that we can explain that in terms of the us
instincts, not in terms of hate.
And that's an important correction because there's this enormous, you
know, DEI industry that is not exclusively, but it largely makes use of bias training workshops where people are told
that they have a sort of unconscious hate for the other side. And it makes people feel falsely
accused. And there's a lot of evidence that it polarizes groups. It makes people more inhibited
about interacting or mentoring or hiring across ethnic lines. So
it can have counterproductive effects. Yeah, I think it's good to move on from just focusing
on sort of political divides only a week or so after the election, still in the blast radius of
that. But yeah, when I think about tribalism, and I keep on getting it in my head, it's not just tribalism, it's polarization.
And I think that the word is being used interchangeably.
It seems like you maybe have a more sort of scientifically grounded definition of what
tribalism is and that what the media is using the word tribalism for is maybe to just highlight
polarization, groups that don't agree with each other.
Yeah, sometimes yes. to just highlight polarization groups that don't agree with each other. Sometimes, yes, but I think often they're trying to say, what is the psychology that
is driving this?
They have different theories about it.
It's a funny thing because they often want to say it's an evolved psychology.
I think they're right about that, that our evolved psychology is contributing to this, but then they, they sometimes can't really explain why now, why is it
worse now if it's our age old psychology?
And I think if you go into the science of tribal instincts, you can, you can start
to understand why certain things have become more salient, uh, and at certain
points in time.
Yes.
I think, you know, the, again, to fight for the, hang on.
I thought everybody hated each other.
Yeah.
Yeah.
No, you're being a good devil's advocate.
A conversation needs that.
I'm trying my best.
Um, I, you know, thinking about breaking it down.
Yes.
Racial lines, you know, we S we've seen an awful lot of movements.
A good example of this BLM, a lot of the time,
a movement called Black Lives Matter,
defining itself around the in-group.
But what did much of that movement,
what did much of the communication end up being,
it was a demonization of the out-group.
A lot of that discussion was around
pointing the finger outside.
The Me Too movement, right, was around protecting women.
Title IX, around protecting women. But how was the communication? What was the sort of language that was being
used? A lot of the time it seems to me, and again, that might be the more nefarious edges,
the spiky extremes of these movements almost certainly will be the newsworthy ones that
capture the sort of crazy Twitter repost and whatnot. But still, it seems to me like a
lot of even in-group movements defined around an in-group
communicate themselves as being not an out-group. And I've seen the stats that said in 2012,
people stopped voting for the in-party and started voting against the out-party.
The messaging that was come around, was it 30% of Trump's campaign budget in some states were only spent on that one Kamala
Harris is for they, them, Trump is for you thing. Like it's all-
Apparently it was the most effective ad in this election. Yeah. So I think you're right. And
oddly enough, it was about pronouns, you know, or it was about, you know, at least sort of about
pronouns. So yeah, it was very effective. It was pithy and it captured that.
So I think you're right.
I would push back, you know, the Me Too movement,
it wasn't just a woman's empowerment movement.
It was a movement of saying,
you were sexually abused by your boss, me too.
I was sexually abused by my boss.
I kept quiet for a variety of reasons,
but now there's strength in numbers because we know it happened to so many people and so we can
all speak out and there's the possibility for change. So I think that movement, it wasn't
something that metastasized from a us movement into a your bad movement. It was, it kind of started from, you know,
the other group, other groups misbehavior. But Black Lives Matter, I think you're, you're
right that it, but again, it was, it was implicitly about police brutality. And, you know, and
then people can argue about the statistics, you know,
exactly. It is certainly the case that African-Americans had a higher, um,
statistical likelihood of, of having interactions with police and then of
having interactions that, that, um, were harmful, um, in one way or the other.
harmful in one way or the other.
Um, but so, yeah, so I agree with you that, uh, these movements are, um, uh,
these movements, uh, are adversarial or oppositional, uh, at some, at some level.
Um, I wonder whether that's a natural, a natural outgrowth of just the pushback of anyone disagreeing
with your in-group proposition.
You make a proposition around us,
a group that isn't you pushes back against that.
So what do you do?
You lean in against the argument.
So I guess, we've spoken about a number of different ways
that people can get split up in terms of tribes. What do people become tribal around mostly?
Is it accent, appearance, familiarity, gene pool? Like what is it? What are the core characteristics
that compose tribes? It's a very interesting question.
So, and again, I think it varies with these three basic tribal instincts,
but I'll start with the base one that I've mentioned, the peer instinct.
I think what's really interesting about the peer instinct is that many studies show that race is not one of the primary triggers.
It becomes a trigger that people learn to use as a group marker if they live in a society like the
United States where race, where physiognomy is correlated with cultural groups.
But in places like Israel, you know, you can't always tell from somebody's face,
whether they're Palestinian or Israeli in the Ukraine, you can't tell whether
someone's Russian or Ukrainian from their face, you know, if you get them to talk,
then you can tell you can sometimes tell from their clothing.
So the, these, uh So these other cues are,
there's a lot of evidence that we are wired more
to use language as our basis of sorting.
I'm so glad.
I'm so glad you said that because I've been spouting
this people are more racist against accents
than they are against skin colors thing for a while.
It may very well be some of your work from a long time ago
that I've been harping on about,
but if that's true, then I'm-
No, it's totally true.
Like kids don't use race.
They don't like preferentially socialize
with someone, a stranger of the same race
until they're like six.
But the language thing starts when they're infants, because even when,
even in the womb, the kid is hearing their mother's language
and even their mother's dialect.
So even like neonates, they'll have a preference, you know, if you kind of put
them in front of, you know, two screens and there's someone talking with their
mother's dialect and there's someone talking with a different dialect,
they'll reach for the food in front of the screen.
No way.
It's not their mother, but it's their mother's dialect.
So the mother tongue is a super important and fundamental thing.
There's also some funny studies that show that children are wired to start
assuming that people who speak the same language as them will prefer the same
food as them.
And the way they do these studies is that they've got like a baby who can't
talk yet, but the baby can listen, right?
And the baby sees one adult, you know, maybe speaking French and one
adult speaking English, and then there's two kinds of food. And they've seen, you know, they've seen
like an English speaker eat this food and they've seen a French speaker eat this food. And then if
the French person speaks, reaches for the English food, you know. The baby shows a surprise reaction, like startled, like, Oh my God, the French
person was saying, so what's really interesting is that babies are not racist.
They don't judge you based on your race, but they already judge you based on your
accent, judge you on your accent and on what you eat.
You know, they're already watching what you eat and your accent.
So they're, they're not, you know, they're not little Buddhas, you know, they're not
racist, but they're not little Buddhas.
They're judgmental already.
Fantastic.
Well, dig into, we've got these three instincts, peer instinct,
hero instinct, ancestor instinct.
Let's run through those.
They seem to be.
So I've mentioned the peer instinct that, you know, corresponds to what we might
call conformist impulses or the herd instinct, the bandwagon
instinct. We're wired to sort of unconsciously learn what the people in our group do. We just
form like a register of what's normal in our group without even trying to. And then we feel a sense of satisfaction when we mesh, when we match, when we fit in with
what other people do.
And this evolved its thought to enable coordination.
Even like a million years ago, our forebearers, Homo erectus, we have evidence now that they
started hunting as a sort of collaborative group,
you know, and gathering as a collaborative group in a way that other species can't do,
like working from a common plant. And that is something we derive today, but it does limit
independent thinking sometimes, but it is something that enables all of our
collective thinking and enables our collective work. Even in fields like art and science,
the great contributions, they build on the work of other people. It's not being done
completely in a vacuum by one person. So I think while we are wise to be wary of conformity,
we should understand that this ability to mind meld with other people
and this impulse to mesh actions and this ease that we have
at collaborating with people in our group,
it empowers most of what we do.
Yeah, what is a peer instinct at its best, peer instinct at its worst? The peer instinct at its best is the kind of, um, think of the seamless interaction
between a basketball team that plays together, you know, behind the back, no look passes,
you know, like I, I, or in, in, in football or soccer, you know, like, I know you so well
that I know that, you know, that I'm going to be here.
Like we can read minds and we can do these amazing things together.
Uh, where it's at, it's worse is when, you know, I'm an engineer and I know
that the airbag design is unsafe, but the other 11 people around this boardroom
are all saying it's good enough.
And so I censor myself and I go along with the group and then, you know,
a customer dies in a, in an airbag explosion, right?
That's, that's where conformity is at its worst.
And I think all of us have probably had an incident in our life like that, where
we, we kind of went along with the group because we didn't
want to get in the way of progress. Um, but we knew we were right and we should have spoken up,
you know, so that's, that's the danger. Um,
it does a one point here, I guess you've mentioned as this sort of taxonomy,
this breakup of different types of instincts, but I can see a world in which
this breakup of different types of instincts, but I can see a world in which peer instinct would be restricting because conformity limits creativity.
If you're sort of following a lot of the time, I imagine that breaking out of the box is
something that then becomes more difficult.
Yes, but I think part of creativity is having a point of departure, which is often somebody else's work.
And some creativity, think of a jazz quartet, right?
It's not one individual's work.
It's playing off of each other
because of reading each other's mind
and knowing where somebody's gonna go
and then doing something that compliments it.
So there, I agree with you, but I think we have a kind of stereotype of creativity that
involves the lone genius. And I would submit that most creativity, even in the arts or in the
sciences, is collaborative creativity. Yeah, that's interesting. Okay. Hero instinct.
So the hero instinct, it's something that was born about a half a million years ago,
when our ancestor Homo heidelbergensis began doing things that no humans had been doing before.
Like at this time, we start to see in the fossil record skeletons of people with congenital
deformities that survived to the age of adulthood.
What does that tell us?
Someone was taking care of a person who probably couldn't pay them back.
Somebody was doing something pro-social.
They were doing something good.
We also see around this time they started hunting much larger animals like woolly mammoths
and rhinos and stuff, which in some cases indicates an individual was willing to take
some personal risk, be the lead hunter so that the group could then rush in and take
down a much larger prey than was possible before.
You also see at this time, the tools,
much more sophisticated tools that required a lot more work
are showing up at this time.
And the idea is that people started to have
a new motivation, not just a motivation to be normal, which is the peer instinct, but a motivation to be normative, to be a
contributor, to be, to be more respected than the average person in the group.
Um, and how do you, how do you do that?
Well, you, you have to make sacrifices for the group.
You kind of take a personal hit to benefit somebody else.
Um, but you also have to know what the group values and that's not always trivial.
And so a cognitive quirk that came along with the hero instinct is this idea of
emulating people in the group with status. So we look to the sort of cultural heroes in our community as beacons of what does the
group value as a contribution.
And we look for distinctive quirks or behaviors of those people and we tend to emulate them.
And this is also something that we often deride today because it is this kind of superficial
status seeking. Like I saw, you know, LeBron James wears these sneakers. So I'm wearing these
sneakers. It doesn't, I still can't dunk, you know, it doesn't really help, you know, but I'm,
I'm trying to be like LeBron, right? That there's, there's a silly side of it. It leads to
superstitious learning in some cases, but in general, it provided a sort
of engine of innovation and adaptive cultural change because imagine an early agricultural
group where everybody was planting one kind of peas and then somebody starts to plant
corn or something or yams and then that tends to grow well,
because maybe the climate changed,
or maybe the group migrated to a different ecology.
And the younger generation sees that,
and they will emulate that.
And then you'll have a gradual shift of the culture
towards what's working currently, know, currently or working in the
new environment. So while we can deride status seeking, this hero instinct,
it was a way for individuals to become rewarded by the group, to have status and tribute, you know?
And it's funny, you know, anthropologists, there's a certain group of anthropologists who were
really invested in the idea that in hunter gatherer groups, there was absolutely no hierarchy,
that everybody was totally equal and that it was like classical communism, like food
was distributed according to need.
Everyone starved.
Yeah. You know, like food was distributed according to need and, and everyone's, everyone's starved.
Yeah.
And there are reasons for this, like in the, in the Bushmen, you know, the Bushmen of, uh, which for a
while you weren't supposed to say the word Bushmen.
Now you're supposed to say Bushmen again, apparently from what I, what were you briefly
supposed to say that you're not supposed to say anymore?
You're supposed to call them like the, the San people or the Kung people.
Uh, but now, now I've read some things that they actually, that, you know, some
people actually think Bushman is a better word.
I don't know.
But, uh, so I've said all three words now, so, uh, I should be safe.
Equally safe or equally canceled.
Yeah.
Yeah.
So, um, so, uh, they have this wonderful ritual in their group that's called insulting the meat. And the way it
works is when the young hunters come back from hunting, most of them probably got nothing and
one of them got like a little rabbit and then one of them got an antelope. And so the old men and
women of the village, when the hunters come back, they start insulting
the hunter who got the antelope.
And they're like, look at that scrawny thing.
There's practically no meat on it.
What is that a mouse?
You know, and they kind of go on like that, you know, in the way that you could, you could
picture uncles in Brooklyn on their stoop.
They're shit talking.
They're shit talking, essentially.
And they call it insulting the meat.
And so anthropologists said, oh look,
they obviously don't want to have
any status differentiation.
But sort of modern anthropologists
who use more biological methods,
they're really into measuring fertility.
So they'll measure for, over time, living with a group, they'll measure for, you know, over time, you know, living with a group,
they'll measure, okay, which hunters were successful most often?
Like what's the batting average of these different hunters?
And then 10 years later, okay, which of these hunters has the most children?
You know, and surprise, surprise, the correlation between status measured
by successful hunts and, um, and reproductive,
uh, reproductive, uh, success, um, is just as high in the hunter gatherer group as it
is in the kingdoms, you know, in the groups that, that celebrate inequality.
You can insult the meat all you want, but what can I say?
That's a, that's an attractive, that's an attractive man carrying a good antelope and
taking my bed.
It's it's, um, there would be a serious incentive problem if the hunters genuinely felt insulted,
you know, it's a status leveling thing, right?
You don't want the hunter to get too big ahead because we all have to live together.
But also if there was, think about it this way.
If there was no reward for working that hard
beyond the caloric input that you get, like
you're working, you have this excess output,
there's additional risk.
You know, if you're the one that takes down
the antelope or the whatever, it's you that's
the one that's potentially going to be killed by it.
You've got to drag it back. Presumably you're carrying it for a good chunk of the way, it's you that's the one that's potentially going to be killed by it.
You've got to drag it back.
Presumably you're carrying it for a good chunk of the way, if not all of the way.
Yeah.
Why?
Why are you doing that?
Is it just because you want to bestow your caloric excess surplus onto the group or is
it because you think you're going to get laid?
Or is it because you think that you're going to be, your children are going to be better
protected or that people are going to revere you or that all of the downstream benefits that come from status are going to be bestowed
on you.
Obviously that's going to be the case.
Yeah.
I think that what's really interesting about it is that pro-social behavior is socially
rewarded, right?
We can see in this group, it's rewarded through social opportunities.
In other groups that are hunter gatherer groups, like the Aceh, they distribute food in a completely
socialist way, but the productive hunters get better medical care.
If they are injured, the chief sees to it that they get the medicine.
You've got to prioritize the person that's providing all the food.
Exactly.
It's not surprising. Uh, but what's interesting is that evolution didn't just wire us to be
calculating and to be revenue maximizing, even though our, my economist
colleagues tend to think that the world works that way, it also wired us to
care about esteem as an end in
itself. You know, the standing in which we are held and the internal esteem that we feel when
we know that we've done the right thing. And I think one of the reasons that we became wired with that sort of pride shame system
is because social rewards tend to come probabilistically and with considerable delay. So if I was a person who was only going to be pro-social, like I was only going to try to hunt
hard if I was sure that there was a reward waiting for me.
I wouldn't do it very often, you know, but if I'm wired that I want the feeling, you know,
I want the feeling of being celebrated and the feeling of knowing I'm the contributor, then I
probably will get more rewards because I'll do I'll do the pro social thing more often.
So, uh, I think we're wired for both.
We're wired to, to contribute in order to be rewarded and we're wired to contribute
as an end in itself.
Hmm.
You know, I mean, that's what the conscience is, right?
I've been thinking about this a lot more recently.
I did a ton of, ton of therapy over the last year.
And if you do tons of psychotherapy and you're talking to someone face to face,
your conscience, apparently just the volume gets turned up and you can't
hide things from yourself anymore.
So, um, you've been really thinking about that.
You have this sort of, I don't know.
It's like a representative of the group sat on your shoulder, your better self.
Sometimes, uh, just judging you.
You should, should you have done that? Should you have said that?
Is that a, is that a virtuous behavior?
Is that a non-virtuous behavior?
Like what is that?
If not this sort of sense of social obligation to the rest of the world.
Now, if you're religious, maybe, you know, it's, it's your virtue speaking up on high.
It's something a little bit more transcendent than that, but at least
functionally adaptively, why would it work?
Well, it's to ensure that you know what other people would have thought about the thing
that you just did or said or thought about doing.
Yeah.
Yeah.
Freud, you know, Freud talks about the super ego as your sort of internalized voice of
authority, but I really think it's more like it's the internalized voice of the,
of the, the respected members of the community. You know, it's like your board of advisors,
your internal board of advisors is suggesting that what you're about to do is unwise. And that's,
that's the emotion of pride shame is kind of like a good PR agency. It encourages you to do the things that are thought well of,
and it encourages you to decline the temptations
to do things that are not thought well of.
And then it also encourages you to publicize
when you've done a good thing.
So in every culture, when Olympic athletes win a gold medal,
they go like this, they, you know, they cower, they, they disappear, they become smaller.
So we, we not only have these emotions that are driving us to, to do what the
group rewards, but we are also wired to showboat when we have done something well.
And so we, we, we, we, we, we, we, we, we, we, we, we, we, we, we, we, we, we, we, we, we, we, we, we, we, we, we, we, we, we, we, we, we, we, we, we,, but we are also wired to showboat when we
have done something well and to hide it when we have not done something well.
So just to kind of round out the hero instinct bit, in a more socially complex group structure
where status bestows on certain people, the people that achieve it, benefits.
That requires a degree of experimentation, adventure, risk taking, divergence from the
peer instinct, which would be more conformist. So someone's going to go and do this thing.
We then have benefits bestowed to that person, which is the reward for them,
taking the risk effectively and, and well, and pulling it off, doing the dance,
killing the antelope, whatever.
And then downstream from that, because you now have this kind of, uh, social
capital capitalist system where everybody's trying to accrue as much
social equity as they can, I look at the person that's done really well.
And I think, okay, well, he did that.
What are some of the principles that I can take from that?
Maybe specifically, or maybe even sort of more philosophically.
Well, he did something different or he was courageous or he was, you know,
honorable or whatever it might be.
Is that a good framing for hero instinct?
Yeah.
Yeah.
And I think, I dunno, I, when I was writing about this and, you know, obviously Is that a good framing for hero instinct? Exactly. Yeah. Yeah.
And I think, I don't know, when I was writing about this and, you know, obviously I've been
reading a lot and I do research in the area, but I often reflect back on when I was an
adolescent, like when I was in high school.
And I feel like a lot of my mental life in those days when I was unfolding as a person was these Walter Mitty like fantasies of heroic
action and glory. Like, oh, the high school caught on fire. Oh, I'm going to save the pretty girl.
Or, oh, my parents are in a car crash. Well, I'm going to go run and get, get medical attention. You know, it was all this constant scenarios about
things that I might do, which, you know, I would regard as, you know, I think a lot of our fantasies,
whether they're sexual fantasies or fantasies about achievement, they reflect, you know,
these programs that are partially evolved programs and then take on a particular cultural,
that are partially evolved programs and then take on a particular cultural instantiation, but they're programs for action. And so I think that probably these hero scripts that go through
our heads are gendered and they may take a different form in different subcultures, but
they may take a different form and different subcultures, but they, uh, they certainly seem to be things that are essential parts of your mental life, especially when you're
young, you know, when you get older, then you're actually doing things.
But uh, you don't have time to dream.
You're too busy with tasks.
Yeah.
Yeah.
And then there are people like, you know, I don't know, Walter Mitty might be a reference
where I'm aging myself, but it's a story by James
Thurber about this guy who goes through his life fantasizing about acting heroically, but never
doing it. And it was sort of a commentary on modernity. But I think that there's also some
But there's also some interesting studies that help us understand who are the Walter Middies and who are the people who actually step forward and do contribute to groups at
a sacrifice to their own safety or at a sacrifice to their own comfort and then gain the actual
glory rather than just the fantasized glory.
Yeah. I'm, uh, I'm going to have to ask you if I imagine that you do know, I want to have a
conversation with someone about the psychology of courage and bravery.
Yeah.
I'm sure that there's somebody out there that's done it, but it's something I've
been pretty fascinated by recently.
And it sounds like that, you know, you posit an ideal, you find yourself falling
short from that ideal and it's all, it's not due to something structural, it's simply due to your own commission, your own
lack of willingness of effort. Yeah. Well, in my book, Tribal, I'm not sure if I'm supposed to
mention it, but I write about this one study that I found fascinating. There was this program called the Freedom Summer, which was designed to
help register African American voters in the state of Mississippi in the 1960s when a very
small percentage of African Americans were registered to vote because, you know, arbitrary
inconveniences were put in place to stop them. So this group said, okay, we will train idealistic
college students to go for the summer to Mississippi and they'll go to every community
and they'll sort of build trust with people and then help people fill out the paperwork
so that we have a much higher rate
of registered African-American voters.
And applications were taken
at all different colleges across the country
and people answered all sorts of questions
and sent in their applications.
And then the best people were selected
and they were brought to a university in Ohio,
not in the South, for some initial training.
And then just when the first volunteers were sent down
to Mississippi, several of them were murdered in a very brutal
way by the KKK.
And that created a crisis in the program,
as you might expect, where people thought this was going to be a difficult summer, but they didn't think they were going to get murdered.
And so a third of the people, I'm trying to remember, I think a third of the people stayed and two thirds of the people decided, probably don't want to do this in the end.
And then, you know, they ran the program at a slightly smaller scale than they
expected and there were no more killings, but there were some difficult
circumstances.
And after the fact, this, uh, sociologist realized all those applications, all
those applications are still on record.
All those applications are still on record. So I want to go back and look at those applications
and see if I can distinguish who were the stayers
and who were the quitters, right?
Who were the people who stayed,
even though suddenly it was looking like a much tougher gig.
You know, it was looking like something
that was going to be dangerous.
And what was interesting is that these people had written essays about their
political values and they had talked about their experience being involved in
political activism and that sort of thing.
And none of those things matter.
None of those things differed between the stayers and the quitters.
The only thing that differed was that the stayers were people who happened to have close personal friends who were also in the
program and were people who were part of organizations that were involved with the program. They
belonged to a political organization that was one of the sponsors of the program. So the conclusion was that these people were embedded in relationships where
they couldn't quit without really disappointing some people that they
definitely were going to see again, right?
They were going to, they were definitely going to see the, the political
club that they were a part of back in their,
in their hometown. And they definitely were going to see their friends who are also part of this
group. So these being in a network of people who are committed to a cause, it sort of anchors you
to that cause so that you stay with it. even when the going gets tough. He's saying that the key to bravery and courage is social pressure.
Well, social pressure and social incentives, right?
Correct.
It's a sense of support if you do it well and a sense of-
Studies of, studies of militaries have found that, you know, why do, why do
people risk their life on the battlefield?
Is it for their country?
Is it because they care about democracy?
No, it's because of their buddies from boot camp.
It's their buddies from boot camp who are out there
and they're not gonna run if their buddy's out there.
Even in insurgent groups,
or you might call them terrorist groups,
there was a theory that
terrorists were mentally ill, crazy people. But then when people actually did studies,
they typically found the inconvenient finding that these terrorists were above average in
mental health and were not these weird deviant loners. They were popular people, well connected.
It's because for the same reason, the people who are central members of the
community, like the mosque or whatever, if the mosque gets radicalized, those are the
people who are going to throw stones or throw Molotov cocktails.
I had Edward Slingland on the show.
Oh yeah.
He's one of the researchers of this material.
It's phenomenal.
So, um, he did that book about alcohol, uh, over time.
And he told me this really great story about how it's a very common.
Initiation slash bonding ritual for army's troops to drink together.
Maybe not the night before the battle, but at some point, not too far off.
And, uh, it's the impact of alcohol fascinating.
It makes you worse at lying because it down regulates your PFC, but it makes
you better as a lying detector.
So it's this sort of, uh, truth serum milieu that gets thrown through everything.
But even more important than all of that is the fact that you're going
to feel like shit the next day.
It's the suffering, the shared suffering that you go through.
So when you're on the battlefield with your buddies in a week's time, and there's
a ton of horses charging at you and you've got your pikes in the ground or
whatever, there is this sense of shared camaraderie because you have been
through something already that was difficult, albeit maybe just a rough
hangover, but yeah, maybe just a rough hangover.
But yeah, I just really love that idea.
I love, you know, we often, and I think this is something that's potentially robbed from
modern generations who get too coddled, too cotton wool helicopter, snowplough parented,
that if you don't, not only do people not create
the ability to be anti-fragile and resilient against life
individually, but they don't get the opportunity to go through
the really important formative bonding processes of sharing in
that suffering, sharing in that discomfort with other people
too. And, you know, as somebody who has spent a lot of time in
solitude, I'm an only child, this business and everything
that I've done is in one form or another being on my own.
Uh, the more that I'm able to let other people in, the guys that work
with me now on the podcast, the people that I worked with in my events
company previously, the more that I can do that, the more I feel like I've
got sort of, uh, stabilizing wheels to sort of weather the ups and downs,
the turbulence, uh, that, that comes
along with stuff. Cause I'm like, ah, fuck, we're in it together. Like we're in it together. They're
doing it for me. I'm doing it for them. Isn't this cool? And, um, yeah, it's, uh, it's, it's good.
It's noble. It's definitely functional as well. Yeah. I, I, um, in, I'm in a pretty solitary profession myself, you know, being a behavioral scientist
and a professor. And I found that even I tend to like courses that are team taught, you know,
the core courses where we have to have a team of professors who all go and deliver the same material
on the same day to different groups. Even though they're more work, there's something wonderful
about that experience of being in a
shared battle. Then you regroup at the end of the day and how did it go? Oh yeah, my afternoon
session was a nightmare. Oh, couldn't have been worse than mine. The commiseration and working
together. Yeah, it's a wonderful bonding experience. In a lot of traditional cultures,
particularly ones that are cultures
where people have to hunt or people are in battles
where people are warriors,
you have rites of passage that adolescents go through
that are really dysphoric,
that involve painful and frightening experiences that you typically
go through with your age mates, you know, with the group of people within one or two
years of you. And you are for the rest of your life bonded with those people because
you've gone through this terrifying painful experience with them.
Hmm. Okay. And then we've got the ancestor instinct.
Yes. The ancestor instinct is the most recently evolved
wave of adaptations that contributes to our ability to live in tribes.
And in some ways it sounds like the most primitive of them.
It's, it's the urge to replicate the ways of past generations,
to maintain traditions.
We can recognize it in ourselves.
It's our curiosity about past generations of our family
or the original family recipe for this meal.
We sort of fetishize antiques, you know,
objects that come from the past. We want to hear about founders,
not just the founders of our nations, but the founders of
religions, the founders of the organizations that we are a part of.
So we have this kind of irrational curiosity, this kind of irresistible curiosity about
the past and this impulse to maintain these ways.
We feel really good when we maintain a tradition because we feel connected not just to the
current community but to the past generations of the community. And many anthropologists think
that it has something to do with our fundamental fear of mortality, that because we are the animal
that knows that we will die, we have a sort of latent terror about our mortality. But if we feel like we are part of
this enduring tradition, then we can feel that we are part of something that will also endure
for many generations into the future so that it's less terrifying. We have a kind of indirect
immortality from our membership in a culture. So the Ancestor Instinct, the reason it was adaptive is that it allowed early human groups
to hang on to the discoveries and the inventions of past generations, even ones that weren't
immediately needed.
So what you see at this time is you start seeing that not just like the, it was not just
the case that people were replicating the tools, like the spears that were being made by the prior
generation, but they were also replicating the cave art and the, you know, the little Venus figurines
and the bone flutes, things that didn't have an immediate instrumental purpose, but it was something the past generation was
doing, so therefore let's treat it with reverence.
Let's continue it.
We're not sure why we're doing it, but
yeah, give me the, give me the adaptive reason.
Cause this just seems like superfluous archaic bullshit.
Well, the great thing about it, about, you know, it sort of corresponds to like myths and rituals, right?
Why did we have this ritual learning modality where we, we, we sort of
learned something by rote and then we repeat it, even though we don't understand it.
Well, it allows us to learn things that kind of go beyond our understanding.
You know, maybe, um, the past generation figured out some way of making a fishing
net and we don't, we don't really understand why it
works. But if we have this kind of sacrosanct attitude toward
it, we will just copy it the same way it was. And then it
allows us to benefit from the technology, even if we wouldn't
be able to invent it. Or imagine that,
imagine that we're a group that lives near the ocean and there's a tsunami
that happens maybe every 60 years, you know?
If there's a myth about the tsunami, and we repeat that myth,
even though generations go, generations go by without a tsunami, but we still repeat the myth. The myth is there
to protect us when the tsunami does come. And it's not a hypothetical example. There's a group
that are sometimes called sea gypsies that live pretty much on the water in Thailand. And they
were not, they did not lose many people in the great tsunami of 2006,
even though many people in the coastal villages died
because they have all these songs and myths
about how the ocean looks when there's a tsunami.
It's tricky, you know, the ocean actually recedes
before a tsunami, which is like a invitation
to go look in the tidal pools.
But if you have a lore in your culture of like, if the ocean ever
recedes head for the hills, you know, that's, that's an adaptive, um,
it's an adaptive cultural, uh,
are there any other, uh, seemingly superfluous, uh, cultural artifacts
that you've fallen in love with during research for this that ended up being quite adaptive
or just had a kind of interesting use?
Well, there are many examples where I was like,
oh my God, I didn't know that, I wouldn't have suspected that.
They're not necessarily, not all of them are things
that have been adaptive for me personally,
but we have this myth about a primordial flood, you know, the Moses story,
right? And many things about the story, even as a child, when you hear it, you're like,
this is made up nonsense, right? Like, yeah, he built a boat just by himself, and then he had all these animals. It doesn't, but it turns out that groups
all around the world have myths about a primordial flood.
And around, I guess around 8,000 years ago,
there was a major rise in the oceans
that corresponded to the end of
some kind of mini ice age or something. I can't remember the exact geological details, but the
ocean level rose all around the world. And the Aboriginal peoples of Australia,
they all have myths about a primordial flood. They have different kinds of
myths about like what the ocean did. And a few years ago, some anthropologists who had collected
the flood myths all around the country, they teamed up with some geologists who built simulations
based on the topography of different parts of Australia. And what they found is that the myths of these groups
correspond pretty well to what we can simulate happened
7,000 years ago in that part of Australia.
So, you know, these really traumatic events,
they get really well preserved by myths because we treat myths in such a sacrosanct way,
where you're not allowed to tell your version of the Noah story. You're supposed to tell
the exact version that's in the Bible. And similarly, in these aboriginal groups,
you're supposed to tell the exact story of the primordial
flood myth.
So, the Moses story, sorry, I keep saying Moses, it's the Noah story.
The Noah story is our primordial flood myth, but it likely is the remnant of this event
that happened, that really happened.
To you, it may just seem like a whimsical story, but it's actually geology and
meteorology masquerading as a tale from the past. Yeah. And it lasted in the oral tradition for
thousands of years before it was written down. This is one of the fascinating things. I had a
great conversation with Alex O'Connor, who is an atheist, skeptic type person out of Oxford,
but very open to the idea of religion.
And he was arguing for the side of religion and saying that basically in the modern world,
what we told was for people to let go of the thing that they found most easy to believe,
which was story.
It was a persona and personification.
It was narrative.
And to start to believe in the thing which is the least believable, which
is data and stats.
And it's very sterile.
And we have to sort of do, we have to put ourselves into this very different type of
world, this very different type of mindset in order to be able to go.
And this many people will be saved by a mosquito net or et cetera, et cetera, as opposed to
what we lived through for our entire human history.
And yes, the problem with that is that you can't, it's unfalsifiable.
It allows all sorts of fuckery to be slipped in here and there.
But yeah, I always think about that frame.
I always think about things that are figuratively true, but literally false, or things that are functionally true,
but literally false and things that are literally true, but functionally false.
And that myth story, you know, very well be maybe one of those things that's literally
not that true, but functionally is like perfect.
Right, right. In the sense that it, you know, it provides a warning, you know,
In a sense that it provides a warning. The person who lived to tell the story probably had some sort of boat.
I was someone who grew up as a very individualistic and very rationalistic person when I was young.
I studied the humanities and I was told that what
distinguished humans from the beasts was our rationality, our morality, and maybe our aesthetics,
right? That's the glorified notion of what makes humans humans. And in the process of doing the
research for this book, I came to see that as very incomplete,
that some of the things that make us human and that enabled us to build these wonderful,
comfortable civilizations that we live in today are conformity, status seeking, and
kind of nostalgia about the past, sentimentality about the ways of the past. And these are the kinds of things I always used to critique my parents for, you know, like,
so yeah, I think that I've, I have come to a different understanding of the world, you know,
through thinking a lot about how, how the mighty have fallen.
thinking a lot about how the mighty have fallen. Okay.
So what about, what are the levers that pull on tribalism?
What causes culture or tribalism to ossify more or for it to change or become more contagious?
Great question.
Yeah.
One of the themes that I try to express is that there's a myth that
cultures are kind of like permanent fixtures, you know, that the, the red and
the blue party that we see today in the United States will be around forever.
And always have been false.
You know, like I'm old enough to know that it was, it was completely
different when I was a kid.
The, the, there was as much variation within the Democratic Party as there was
between, you know, there was Northern Democrats and Southern Democrats and so cultures are in flux
and the cultures that individuals and small groups express are in flux in an even more rapid way
because we all internalize multiple cultures and the situations that
we go into trigger different cultures.
So I'm meeting you in your podcast self, but I'm sure if I knew you from the gym or if
I knew you from, you know, church or whatever else you do in your life, you know, I would
see a different Chris, right?
I would see a different Chris, right? I would see a different person. So we
have short-term fluctuations based on situations, and then we have long-term evolution of cultures,
and there are levers of those short-term changes and levers of the long-term changes. So
in the short-term, these three instincts, these three levels of tribal motivation, they
are triggered by slightly different things. So the peer instinct, this kind of conformity,
this kind of set of shared habits that we just jump into, it's triggered by the more than anything
else by the audiences around us, by the ways that they speak, by the ways that they dress.
What we call code switching is an example of this.
So when Barack Obama used to speak in a slightly different register when he was in front of an African American group, as opposed to a group of farmers from Kansas, that wasn't something that he was doing intentionally.
That was just a reflex that came from the fact that he grew up with a mixed family and with
different parts of his life living in each of those communities. And when he's in front of people
who look a certain way, dress a certain way, talk a certain way, and yes, have certain ethnic
characteristics, although that is less fundamental to it. He clicks into one set of speech habits or
another set of speech habits. Some of my research have pushed a little deeper on that and showed
that it's not just speech habits, it's your basic biases in making sense of the world,
making sense of ambiguous events. Cultures have different biases. And when you are around an
audience from one of your cultures, you start thinking with that worldview. And when you're
around people from another one. So when I'm around my fellow professors,
you know, I'm thinking in terms of data,
you know, I'm thinking in terms of economic theory,
that kind of thing.
When I go to my hometown and I'm with my buddies
in a dive bar, you know, I'm thinking in terms
of different templates, different scripts,
you know, ones that help me bond with them
and help me be understood by them, but that wouldn't work as well with my
colleagues at Columbia.
So it's audiences, the tribes that we're around that trigger those peer instincts.
And then for hero instincts, the impulses to contribute, the impulses to sacrifice
and do something exemplary for the group.
Some very potent triggers of that are cultural symbols.
So for the longest time,
armies would follow a national flag into battle.
Crusaders followed the cross, they took the cross.
Sports teams, the mascot runs out on
the field and people go crazy.
Uh, or them be something similar.
Was that an anthem?
Exactly.
The song that they come out to.
Exactly.
An anthem.
Um, you know, whether it's a national anthem or whether it's, you know, the
fans will chant a song in the stadium.
So it's, it's a kind of a set of images or a set of words or, you know, phrases like,
you know, in the United States if you say all men are created equal, you know, you trigger a certain
political creed, you know, which is why Martin Luther King, you know, quoted the Declaration of
Independence when he was trying to build a broad coalition for civil rights. And so these symbols, these cultural icons are potent triggers of hero instincts.
If you want to get people to be pro-social, to take risks, to make sacrifices, surround
them with the symbols of their tribe.
And then the one that is maybe least obvious is the ancestor
instinct. What is it that causes people to start thinking in terms of tradition and letting tradition
guide them? One of the kinds of cultural cues that is most important is ceremonies. So ceremonies are public events that involve symbols, but
that often involve synchronous movement. So you're talking in unison, you're moving in
unison, you're often marching or you're in church, you're getting up and down in a yoga
class, you're going through these positions together. And there's even a neuroscience literature on this that synchronous behavior,
it lulls people into a different mental state where their self-concept as an individual is
reduced. They become more open to unity experience. Their critical thinking is somewhat reduced.
their critical thinking is somewhat reduced. And so they become more open to this idea of accepting tradition. So if you have a moment in a yoga class or in a religious service where suddenly you feel
part of a tradition, that's because you've been in a ceremony and ceremonies bring that out in us.
And ceremonies bring that out in us.
I, uh, I suppose that's the, uh, one of the vectors of weakness that cults cult leaders take advantage of too.
Exactly.
People more suggestible.
You are a part of the bigger thing.
Do you not, do you not feel this sense of connection to the wider world?
Yeah.
Yeah.
In cults, I mean, there are daily ceremonies, you know, where you're,
you're kind of made to feel part of a broader system, your individual
self-consciousness gets dampened. And I think people join cults because this unity experience
is a wonderful thing to experience. A lot of us will experience it on a sports team or if you're
part of a political campaign and you're throwing yourself into it, you feel part of
something larger and you're making sacrifices for that group and you feel connected to prior
generations of people in the same community. So it's a wonderful experience that people get when
they start to join a cult. And then I think cults differ from just a strong culture in some of their recruitment techniques.
One thing that really characterizes cults is what's called network isolation, where they will, you know,
they'll find somebody who maybe is not tightly embedded in a community, like maybe a transfer student who is new on a large campus or someone who's recently gotten out of the
army and hasn't really started work yet. And they will invite that person to some social
activity, maybe a volleyball game or a dinner. And they love bomb them. They surround them
with people who hang on their every word and tell them they have spiritual potential
and make them feel wonderful.
And if they respond well,
then they get an invitation to come to our group's retreat.
It's in this beautiful mountain setting
where cell phones don't work
and where there are no newspapers and no television.
And then you have long days of getting up early,
eating a low protein diet,
maybe meditating, chanting, hiking together,
listening to charismatic lectures,
having sort of confession sessions.
And this is the same routine that seminaries use
to recruit priests.
It's the same routine that the Muslim Brotherhood
uses to recruit priests. It's the same routine that the Muslim Brotherhood uses to recruit.
Cults don't have a monopoly on this set of recruitment techniques, but basically what
you're doing is you're pulling somebody out of the mixed social network that they ordinarily live in.
And then after the retreat, usually 10% or 20% of the people are willing to move into a cult residence. And then once they're
in the cult residence, then they're told, you know, you should probably cut off some of these
old friends because they don't really get it about the important work we're doing here in the cult,
or at least give them a break. And maybe don't talk to your family so much because a lot of
families, they may seem like good people, but they're a little bit hostile to the church. And then of course, the family gets angry because they
haven't heard from you and they do things like trying to kidnap you or trying to convince you
that the cult is bad. And that ends up corroborating what the cult has been saying. And then you get
into a world where you're living in a day-to-day routine where
you're completely surrounded by fellow cult members and they look up to the cult leaders and the cult leaders basically have this monopoly on status. Nobody else in your world has status.
You're not seeing deference to anybody else. And that's when it gets really dangerous because
it's abnormal. In normal life, even if you're a very religious person, you know, you,
um, you know, you, you may think the Pope is a wonderful person, but you're,
you're also a big fan of messy and you like, you know, the music of a Mick Jagger.
And so no, no one of these heroes can dominate you completely.
Uh, but when you're in a cult and you're not allowed to listen to Mick Jagger,
you're not allowed to watch messy.
And it's all day long ceremonies involving the cult leader.
That gets dangerous.
What about factors or situations in the environment that cause people to focus
more on either collaboration or on competition.
I kind of have it in my head around warfare,
peace time, war time, a threat, et cetera.
Surely those sorts of things,
but there must be a ton of these.
You're right.
In addition to these sort of social triggers,
or who you're around, or who you, around or whether you're in a ceremony or you're
seeing symbols or you're seeing audiences, particular emotional states also contribute
to these things. And so threat tends to be something, particularly existential threat,
like the fear of death, a brush with death, or the fear of
collective threat, like there's some sort of threat to your organization or threat to your country.
That leads people to cling to traditions in ways that they don't do otherwise. So that can be very tricky in terms of setting off dysfunctional traditionalism because in
a corporation, there's a tendency to think that your own traditions are wonderful and
that the competition, that their traditions are silly.
But then imagine it's a time of threat, like your
business is not doing well. Well, the tribal reflex is to cling to your traditions even more,
but that's not adaptive because that's really a time when you want to be learning from the
competition. You want to be open-minded. Similarly, in warfare, if you're trying to negotiate a peace treaty, but there's a risk of death
because there's been some killing, well, that makes it harder for people to take the perspective
of the other side. So threat leads people to anchor on their own traditions, on their own group.
How much of that do you think is playing into the modern world, the
polarization side of tribalism, lots of headlines, very scary news out there, threat?
Yeah.
I don't know. You know, like, I mean, when COVID happened, you know, a lot of people were dying. But I don't know that it led to partisan polarization. Um, I do think that in the, you know, in, we see a lot of tribalism with
regard to the Israel Gaza conflict.
You know, the campus that I teach in has been, it's, it's sort of like getting
into an airport to get into the main campus because, uh, because there's been,
you know, protests that were very disruptive.
And now there's really strong management of who can enter the campus.
And I think that that was a conflict that escalated and became very acrimonious
because of the brutality of not just the people died, but
the people just died in a horrible way, both on October 7th and in Gaza. So I do think in a case
like that, people start to, even though they're fellow college students are marching and chanting
and calling their classmates Nazis.
And it's bizarre because, yeah, there is a big problem in the world, but your fellow
Columbia undergraduates are not the problem. But there's this tendency for the, you know,
these protests started as vigils,
vigils expressing solidarity for vulnerable civilians
in Israel and in Gaza.
But then a few months later, the protests were accusations,
you know, accusing the opposite faction of students of being Nazis and being genocidal.
So I do think in that case, because people are aware of the mortality and the bloodshed, there's this way in which people take on this exaggerated sense of being vulnerable themselves. Some of my Israeli
colleagues who are leaders in the protests, they would say things like, my children would
be more safe in Israel or in the Gaza Strip than they are at the Columbia campus.
And I'm like, okay, there's not a lot of threat
to physical safety.
There's a threat, there's been a threat
to people's peace of mind
because there's been some harassment,
but there are not people being killed.
Well, even that is this threat
to your like ideological mental model purity, because you're going to brush up
against and push up against other people that say
you're wrong, that thing's not right.
Yeah.
I mean, this conflation between emotional or
intellectual insults with physical ones and even,
you know, the, the, the ratcheting up of emotional
insults as being, as being something that you're never supposed to encounter
is, is a really interesting, uh, pivot.
So I guess, you know, we've laid out a nice paradigm.
So I want to kind of come back to where we started, which was, uh, your compelling
science that proves that we don't hate outsiders, that it is an us thing, not a
them thing, uh, like I say, you know, the internet, unfortunately, and again, you are, you've
chosen a hell of a time to release a book given, uh, the, uh, the, the recent election.
But a lot of people will just think this can't be the case.
I've heard all of these stories about how people aren't voting for their own side.
They're voting against the other.
Look at the messaging, look at the fear, look at the, et cetera.
There is hostility. I'm not arguing that there's no hostility in these conflicts. wrong side devoting against the other, look at the messaging, look at the fear, look at the etc.
Yeah.
There is hostility.
I'm not arguing that there's no hostility in these conflicts.
I'm just saying it doesn't start from hostility.
It doesn't start from a drive to derogate and fear outsiders and that solidarity within one's group does not imply or necessitate antagonism towards other
groups. Now, that may sound like semantic hair splitting, but it's not because the diagnosis
that there's this discourse that we talked about of people saying like,
a deeply buried drive to hate outsiders has somehow atavistically reawoken to doom us
to a future of internecine.
It's like very grandiose rhetoric
and it doesn't suggest ameliorative policies
because well, if we're cursed by some drive to hate and to be hostile, that is not something that we can really work with.
But if, for example, we believe that the root of the increased partisan conflict has to do with this conformity instinct, which got
into a feedback loop because it created residential sorting and then news media sorting.
And then once you're in these inbred environments, then being conformists created, you know, different political worlds. If that's
your diagnosis, well, there are things you can do about it. You can break out of your bubble.
I live on the Upper West Side of Manhattan where there are not a lot of Trump signs and there are
a lot of Harris signs. And then even the businesses are like Lululemon and Whole Foods, which
And then even the businesses are like Lululemon and Whole Foods, which basically don't, they don't operate in Republican districts.
You know, these are, these are blue tribe symbols, blue tribe icons.
And, um, I've been quite busy the last couple of days because I'm buying a house
upstate and upstate is different, you know, upstate, if I'm going to be spending
more time up there, I'm not going to be constantly reinforced and
constantly triggered to think about the world through these
blue tribe lenses, because I won't be in an ideologically
inbred environment all the time. And there are things we can do
beyond ourselves personally, you know, there's been a lot of efforts to create
dialogue across the red, blue factions of society.
Some of it's going on at universities or, you know, town gown, you know, like bringing
the university students to talk to the people in the university town who may not share their
view or we bring some of our students to industrial towns in the university town who may not share their view, or we bring some of
our students to industrial towns in the Midwest that have suffered from globalization.
The first wave of these programs, very well-intentioned, but a lot of them had names like Red Meets blue or town gown encounter or hello from the other side. And it sort of accentuated that you
are about to be confronted with one of them, you know, one of these people from the other side.
And that raises defenses. That's not a form of interaction that people tend to learn more
moderate views from. And the kinds of programs that the research suggests are more effective
are they're named things like Coffee Party USA, Make America Dinner Again,
Open Lands Discussions.
And the logic here is you bring together, you deliberately bring together
people that you know are registered
Republicans and Democrats, but not to talk about divisive political issues, to talk about things,
passions they have in common, like they all like coffee, or they're all foodies, or they're all
Christian believers, or they're all people who are outdoors people. And the idea is you start talking and then you move from this conversation to that conversation
and you maybe eventually get to politics.
But the conversation is one that is bonding and one that is more likely to last than if
you ask people to discuss global warming or if you ask people to discuss abortion or something
like that.
It doesn't lead to usually a conversation that lasts very long.
So I think the diagnosis does matter
when it comes to trying to think about what can we do
to get ourselves out of this bind that we're in.
It's not like we've always been in this bind,
it's something that happened in the last couple
of generations for particular reasons that involve the interaction of some of our tribal psychology with some technological changes and
demographic changes. And we can find our way out of it. And the doomsayers often will say things
like, America has never been more divided politically. That is absolute nonsense if you know your history.
Abraham Lincoln took over with less than 40%
of the popular vote.
Seven states seceded before his inauguration.
Civil War started a few weeks afterwards.
And that's what we call a political rift.
Your country is in a civil war.
You are not considered to be a legitimate president by a lot of people.
And that's a real struggle.
But it's really interesting to me what he thought was the answer to that.
In his first inauguration, he said, he talked about the daunting prospect of what the country faced.
And then he said, I'm not going to get this perfect, but something like the mystic cords
of memory will yet swell the chorus of the union.
And what he meant by that is that the best resource we have for healing the rift is our
common heritage, our common history, our common ancestors.
And if we think about them, we will feel like our current divide is not such a big deal.
And the other thing that Abraham Lincoln did, which is kind of a seasonally topical
point, is that he is the one who instituted the holiday of Thanksgiving. Most Americans have the
notion that Thanksgiving has been practiced ever since the pilgrims landed on these shores. That's
a myth that was created in part by Abraham Lincoln and the people around the thought leaders of the day
who believed that this holiday would be an effective way
to unify the country.
They made reference to the precedence of the pilgrims
and the precedence of George Washington
holding a Thanksgiving event.
In order to make people feel like this new holiday was not some alien thing, but was
something that was already a time-honored tradition, already something that was part
of the American tradition.
And it became an instant tradition within a decade or so of Abraham Lincoln announcing
that we're going to have this new holiday Thanksgiving.
It was a sacred national tradition tradition and it's something that helped
bring the country back together.
So I think we talk a lot about references to the past
involved in divisive populism,
like talking about the good old days
as a way to blame immigrants for a problem.
But talking about history has also been useful
for inclusive populism, for creating a
broader group identity, for reminding people that we all share certain ancestors. So I'm someone who
I don't believe that we're seeing the end of democracy. I think that the problems that we face
are not larger than the problems that our country know, our country has come through before, or that other democracies have come through
before and that, you know, it's helpful to have a, a diagnosis that is realistic
rather than a diagnosis that is, you know, Manichaean that talks about sort of,
you know, good versus evil, the end of the world, you know, this kind of really
grandiose, grandiose ways of talking about our situation.
kind of really grandiose, grandiose ways of talking about our situation.
Yes, maybe less sexy, but hopefully a lot more inspiring and, and more accurate as well. Dr. Michael Morris, ladies and gentlemen, I really love this orthogonal
approach to tribalism. I think that we need to try and draw back some of that.
And it's, it's so interesting to think about the, the positive outcomes that
you get from having this, from having access to teamwork, from having access
to aiming up, from having access to your, your history, the trends,
archetypes from the past.
It's a, that's cool.
It's a, it's a, I mean,
archetypes from the past.
It's a, that's cool.
It's a, it's a, I mean, tribal, tribal psychology is, is what made us human and underlies all of our proudest accomplishments.
It goes awry sometimes.
Yes.
Like every instinct does, you know, but if we understand the instinct, then
we can, we can manage that.
Okay.
Michael, where should people go to keep up to date with all of your work?
Well, uh, I have a book website that's called tribal book.org.
That is a good place to go to learn more about my book.
I also have a site called Michael W Morris.com where you can learn a lot about my research and the kind of courses that I teach as well as
learning about the book and other things that I've written.
That's right. Michael, I appreciate you. Thank you.
Thanks so much for having me.