Modern Wisdom - #923 - Matt Ridley - Why Evolution Favours Beauty Over Survival
Episode Date: April 3, 2025Matt Ridley is a science writer, journalist, and author. Evolution is a strange theory. If survival is all that matters, why do we find things beautiful? Why does beauty exist at all? And if aesthetic...s are so important, how do some species thrive without it? Expect to learn what Darwin’s strangest ideas were, the fundamental mystery of sexual selection, why females choose certain males based on beauty and performance rather than obvious survival traits, if females actually have as much agency in mate selection as we assume, or if other forces dictate choice, the alternative explanations for beauty and why aesthetics are so important and much more… Sponsors: See discounts for all the products I use and recommend: https://chriswillx.com/deals Get 10% discount on all Gymshark’s products at https://gym.sh/modernwisdom (use code MODERNWISDOM10) Get the Whoop 4.0 for free and get your first month for free at https://join.whoop.com/modernwisdom Get a Free Sample Pack of all LMNT Flavours with your first purchase at https://drinklmnt.com/modernwisdom Extra Stuff: Get my free reading list of 100 books to read before you die: https://chriswillx.com/books Try my productivity energy drink Neutonic: https://neutonic.com/modernwisdom Episodes You Might Enjoy: #577 - David Goggins - This Is How To Master Your Life: https://tinyurl.com/43hv6y59 #712 - Dr Jordan Peterson - How To Destroy Your Negative Beliefs: https://tinyurl.com/2rtz7avf #700 - Dr Andrew Huberman - The Secret Tools To Hack Your Brain: https://tinyurl.com/3ccn5vkp - Get In Touch: Instagram: https://www.instagram.com/chriswillx Twitter: https://www.twitter.com/chriswillx YouTube: https://www.youtube.com/modernwisdompodcast Email: https://chriswillx.com/contact - Learn more about your ad choices. Visit megaphone.fm/adchoices
Transcript
Discussion (0)
What was Darwin's strangest idea?
Sexual selection by mate choice is the idea that Darwin had alongside natural selection
and which he maintained was a very different process.
Almost nobody agreed with him in his lifetime.
It was a failure in the sense that he couldn't persuade people that this was an important
thing.
And when people did agree with him, they thought, well, yeah, but it's just a small niche thing in the corner of biology. And I don't think that's right. I think he was onto
something that actually when mates are selective, which they are in many species, it drives a huge
amount of evolution in the other sex. And it's a very different process from natural selection.
I call it the fun version of evolution because it produces songs and things like that.
the fun version of eagolution because it produces songs and things like that.
It's less utilitarian.
Hmm.
Yeah.
Uh, what was the reaction when Darwin first proposed sexual selection?
Well, he mentioned the idea in the origin of species very briefly.
And he said, I think that he had a friend called Sir John Sebright, who'd been breeding rather beautiful bantam, new varieties of bantams.
And he said, if a man can produce a beautiful bantam in a short time, then why can't a
female produce a beautiful male in over a thousand generations?
And he was ridiculed for it.
And by the time of the fourth edition of The Origin of Species, he felt it necessary to put in a sentence saying, yeah look they are beautiful, these male birds,
to us, but that doesn't mean they were put on earth to please us. They could have been put on
earth to please females and this made things worse because everyone else said, I'm sorry, are you suggesting that female birds are capable of aesthetic discrimination?
Give me a break.
And Wallace in particular deserted him on this topic.
So did Thomas Henry Huxley, Herbert Spencer, all his normal defenders were not prepared
to defend this idea.
Partly these crusty old Victorians were a bit uncomfortable with the
idea of women having sexual agency at all, of course, let alone lust. So, you know, one
has to take into account that. But I then I'm very fond of a person who features in
my book called Edmund Seleuze who was an amateur
naturalist who watched the same species as me, the black grouse, as well as a
number of other species. And he said, you know, Darwin was right! The evidence
speaks trumpet-tongued in his favor, which is such a nice phrase, I think,
because it's clear when you watch some of these birds that the
females are being very selective and are in charge of whether or not mating happens.
Yeah.
I can imagine that Victorian England wasn't superbly keen on the idea of flipping the
gender hierarchy upside down and saying, well, maybe, you know, maybe the males were
shaped by female preferences. And that also sort of has in it a sense of almost sort of promiscuity in a way,
a degree of female sexual agency, which again,
Victorian England, probably not superbly popular.
Yeah. And we don't need to be all that smug about Victorian because we too tend to say,
well, hang on, isn't female beauty to males more
important than male beauty to females in our species? And it might well be the case. I mean,
that's true in some bird species, but actually in our species, both sexes are highly selective when
they choose long-term partners. And so there's going to be, you know, different criteria, but similarly choosing,
similar choosiness in both sexes.
But yeah, no people, people find it instinctively odd that women should be
choosing that females should be choosing males on the basis of appearance.
What is the fundamental mystery when it comes to sexual selection?
fundamental mystery when it comes to sexual selection? The fundamental mystery is why so many species indulge in growing and displaying features
that hinder their own survival, take a lot of energy, and can be amazingly flamboyant.
If you look at some of the birds of paradise that do a sort of shape shifting display where
they disappear into a sort of black hole and project a iridescent smiley face on it, what
on earth is going on? It's such an eccentric outcome to come from evolutionary biology that it still doesn't…
Where's the rhyme or reason is another way of putting it. Actually, I see evolutionary biologists
arguments over the last 150 years as being a series of last-ditch attempts to put rhyme or
reason back into this process. There might not be rhyme or reason.
It might, it might be just be extravagant for your own sake, because females are
going to go for the most extravagant thing you can do.
Um, and I'll explain why I think that works as a technique.
Yeah.
I, I can imagine, I can see, especially in a civilization which still has the sort of conceptual inertia of
intelligent design, of beauty being sort of divinely bestowed from above, that you observe
these birds doing crazy dances and making themselves into smiley faces and hopping around
and pecking and doing all of this stuff and think, well, how lovely that God has made
these birds do this dance for our benefit.
This is beauty incarnate. I get to observe and enjoy. Maybe he wasn't for you.
Right. And the bird that Wallace and Darwin ended up arguing about most in 1868 when their
dispute over this came to a head was a bird called the Argus pheasant, which is a sort
of peacock-sized bird in the jungles of Southeast Asia, newly discovered at the time, which has
enormous wings, very, very long wing feathers. These wing feathers have a series of objects
depicted on them that are clearly intended to be three-dimensional optical illusions. In other words,
they look like little spheres because they've got highlights at the top and shading at the bottom. So somebody's gone to great
trouble to make these things look as if they're actually three-dimensional. They're sticking out
of the feather like a sort of pebble or a jewel. Genuinely, Darwin's critics, including a guy called Wood, who was doing the pictures
actually for his book, said, look, I'm sorry, for this to be created by females, female
birds, you've then got to posit that females have an aesthetic sense. But the idea that a bird with a
brain the size of a walnut is capable of appreciating and enjoying three
dimensional optical illusions is for the birds. I mean, I didn't use that
phrase, but that was the implication of what he said. So
there's real, you know, people like Sir Joshua Reynolds have been writing books about
aesthetics at this stage and saying, you need to have been to Oxford to really understand
aesthetics.
But sorry, can I go back to one thing you said, which intrigues me, and that's the idea of intelligent design.
Because in some ways, Darwin is flirting with something that looks a bit more like intelligent
design here. And it's been pointed out by Evelyn Richards and others, who's a historian
of this period, that his interest in natural selection almost seems
to dry up after the origin of species. He doesn't spend a lot of time talking about
it. His next books are about things like the domestication of animals. Well, that's not
natural selection, that's artificial selection. And then sexual selection, which again is females driving the selective process.
And Wallace, his friend and rival, reacts against this in exactly the way that you might,
where he sort of says, look, I'm now more Catholic than the Pope. I really believe in this bottom-up
natural selection, survival of the fittest thing. And I
think bird beauty is just for some reason, something that helps the species survive or
the individual survive. And it's a part of natural selection. And I don't like the way Darwin is
flirting with conscious beings, which female birds are, choosing what males should look like.
Now Darwin isn't going that far.
He's not, you know, you're not literally saying that, that, that, that females are
sitting down and planning what they want peacocks to look like.
Um, but there is, there's a little bit of, um, uh, you know, that he's prepared to accept that evolution can be directed in a way that looks a little dangerous
to people like Wallace.
Isn't it interesting that Darwin, someone whose proposals were recently heretical to
the previous dominant ideology, inside of his own new ideology becomes a heretic.
You know what I mean?
That's a lovely way of putting it.
Thank you.
Yes.
Um, absolutely.
And, and there's a plaintive quotation from him at one of his last meetings at
the Linnaean society before he died, where he says, I still think I'm right.
I know all you guys tell me I'm wrong.
By this point, he's pleading with them.
As he's being, I mean, again, look, I, um, maybe he doesn't have the spear in the
side and the crown of thorns on the head, but it does feel a little bit like a guy
who's being like prostrated a little bit.
He sort of begging for a bit of like, guys,'s being like prostrated a little bit, sort of begging for a
bit of like, guys, please, like it's ultimately this is going to hurt you more than it's going
to hurt me on judgment day, you know, like he does have this like Messiah thing going on.
Although as far as I've read, Robert Wright's book was the first one, Moral Animals, what got
me into evolutionary psychology. Again, for wonderful Wonderful book. I mean, that's, that book is 30 years old now.
More than 30 years old.
It's like 92 or something it came out.
And for anyone that wants a good kind of half biographical look at Darwin's life
with framing of evolutionary psychology, there's some stuff in there that's a
little outdated, obviously it's three decades of a relatively new field.
So some stuff's moved on, but it's so great.
But in that, um, Darwin seemed to be pretty sort of racked by self-doubt, uncertainty.
He had like a little bit of a disposition toward low mood sometimes.
And, uh, I imagine he doesn't have the, he doesn't get mad.
He gets sad and he doesn't have mad, he gets sad,
and he doesn't have the big sort of fuck you energy
that a renegade, rebellious, anarchist thinker would have.
So I think he's actually kind of an unlikely individual to go so hard
against the dominant sort of mainstream hegemon that was whatever came before him.
And I do wonder what would have happened,
how much further his work could have got
if he didn't have to get over not only himself,
but then the additional pressure of
everybody else saying he was wrong and then
his own self-doubt being reinforced
by what people were saying from outside of him.
It must have been really tough for him to
navigate because he didn't have,
I think I'm right in saying this,
by the time that he died, he still didn't have a fully perfect explanation of the peacock's tail.
It was this sort of, ah, it's kind of there and I think I've got this inclination, but I don't have
something that's concrete. And then if all of your peers are saying, yeah, mate, you, I mean, you hit
the lottery once with that thing, but you don't get to run it. You can't wheel it up and run it
back another time. It's just isn't going to work.
Yeah, that's all true.
He is a cautious, conservative establishment figure.
He's wealthy and mixes in upper middle class circles and he's not a boat rocker in the
sense, Wallace is a socialist and a feminist
and all sorts of, and a man of humble background and things like that.
So in that sense, Darwin is an unlikely revolutionary.
But in another sense, I don't think you're right to say that the self-doubt held him
back.
Once he'd committed to writing The Origin of Species, which took a big leap,
took 20 years of angst, as you say, before he did.
Once he did, he very rarely gave an inch.
Well, no, that's not true.
He compromised.
Actually, the later editions of The Origin of Species are much less convincing
than the early ones because he is trying to compromise with his critics.
And he's obviously, you know, feeling the pain of some of the
criticisms. But he then plows on finding all these stories about animals and plants and
details that can buttress his ideas. And, you know, there's no sense in which he sort
of wants a quiet life. Well, he does. He doesn't want to get involved in the controversies
himself, but he wants to keep pushing the ideas out there. So he's a magnificent person. But
Robert Wright was the one who pointed out, and I'd never thought this before until I read Robert's point on it, that the way in which Wallace's letter from Papua New Guinea, or from New Guinea,
was handled was quite cunning on Darwin's part and that quite selfish actually. We tend to think of
him as being magnificently generous and saying, look, this chap is a scooped me.
Um, but why don't we both present our ideas at the Linnaean society together?
Um, yeah, but when it came to it, Wallace was off in New
Guinea, didn't know this was going on.
They didn't have time to tell him.
Lyle says, look, look, um, you poor chap, Darwin, don't get too head-tumped about it.
We'll have a meeting and we'll present your paper first and then Wallace's and
you'll get the credit.
And so in a sense, Wallace does get shafted by this process.
Um, uh, and, uh, and Darwin for all his politeness.
He's got a ruthless streak in him.
He's got a ruthless streak and he wants his priority on this topic.
But back to sexual selection, Wallace wins the argument in their lifetimes.
And continues to really, in many ways up till today, actually, the versions of
Wallace's theory are still pretty popular.
We can come back to the details of that if you like.
of Wallace's theory are still pretty popular. We can come back to the details of that if you like.
Some of the things Darwin says in his dispute with Wallace are quite stupid, actually. For example, Wallace said, look, female birds are mottled brown because they want protection
on the nest. They don't use the word camouflage because it hasn't been coined yet, but that's what they mean. And the reason I know that is because female birds that breed in holes
are often quite brightly colored, things like parrots or kingfishers or woodpeckers. And
Darwin says, no, no, no, no, I don't believe that females are camouflaged. And you think, why not?
And it's because he's, he's desperate not to give an inch on the idea that, uh,
that sexual selection is driving bird color.
Why are birds so useful to use for this study?
What is it about birds?
Why is it not dogs?
Why is it not cows? Why is it not cows?
Why are we not using sheep for this study?
And birds are a bit more like us than many mammals.
They like song, they like color, they like visual things.
And we are, we've got pretty good color vision for mammals.
And most mammals have only got two color channels.
We've got three as have other primates.
Um, so we see a much more colorful world, a rather like the world the birds see,
not nearly as colorful as they see.
They've got at least four channels.
They've got a trevalid vision, all sorts of things.
So to some extent we can sort of empathize with birds. But in terms of the study of sexual selection, birds really do stand out because there has
been an explosion of dramatic shapes, crests, plumes, colors, displays, dances, and songs
in the birds that dwarfs other species.
So if you just take song, for example, I was out this morning when the sun came up and the bird song was fantastic.
It's springtime.
There was no mammal noise at all.
Maybe I heard a sheep at some point.
Maybe a dog barked in the distance, but that was it.
You know, if we didn't have birds, think how silent it would be.
And song is quite a useful thing to study, actually, if you want to understand what's going on. distance, but that was it. If we didn't have birds, think how silent it would be. Song
is quite a useful thing to study, actually, if you want to understand what's going on here.
Birdwatching gets a lot of human beings into natural history and then into biological sciences. Me, I was a
birdwatcher before I ever thought of being a scientist, and that's true of a lot of people.
Jim Watson, who co-discovered the structure of DNA, he was a birdwatcher as a teenager, and that's what got him interested in biology, et cetera. So I think,
now you could say butterflies, dragonflies,
lots of sexual ornamented colors,
fish, lots of bright colors,
but they're not as easy to study.
They're either too smaller or they're harder to observe
or they're underwater or something.
Birds are the obvious ones to go for.
Mammals, mammals are brown with very few exceptions.
I mean, there's a black one and a gray one and few monkeys have colorful faces,
but apart from that, they're really grim to look at and the noises they make are
terrible really.
Um, uh, and also they do a lot more sexual coercion than birds.
Um, there's another way in which we're similar to birds and that is forming
pair bonds to bring up offspring.
Birds do a lot of that.
Most birds, black grouse are an exception, peacocks are an exception, but most
birds, the male and female collaborate to rear the young.
Um, and again, we empathize with that in an awful lot of mammals, all the work is
done by the, by the mother, um, both gestating and lactating obviously, and,
and nurturing, uh, the offspring.
So there's a sense in which we are honorary birds.
This episode is brought to you by Gymshark.
Gymshark makes the best gym wear on the planet.
Their hybrid shorts in onyx grey and navy are a complete game changer. They're the best
men's training shorts in the world. They've got a crest hoodie, which is what I'm flying
in anytime I'm traveling in their Geo seamless t-shirts or what I train in pretty much every
single day. All of these are unbelievably lightweight. They're sweat wicking and easy to wash and
dry and the fit and quality of the fabric is phenomenal. It's a one-stop shop for all
of the gym kit that you need and that's why it's kind of pretty much all I wear. And if
you're still on the fence, they offer 30 day free returns so you can buy it and try it
for 29 days. And if you don't like it, you can just send it back. Plus they ship internationally.
Right now you can get 10% off everything from Gymshark sitewide by going to the link in the description below or heading to gym.sh slash modern wisdom and using the
code modern wisdom 10 a checkout. That's gym.sh slash modern wisdom and modern wisdom 10 a
checkout. Okay. Okay. So getting into the meat of it, why do females choose certain
males based on beauty and performance rather than obvious
survival traits?
Right.
So why not just choose a strong male who will give you strong children?
And the answer is that you've got to, there's a seduction going on.
It's a charm, it's a a persuasion it's not a coercion
that's the first point. The second point is yes but why let yourself be charmed by a
flamboyant tale or bright colors or whatever and the argument that Wallace raised and that
has reverberated since through the topic is that it's a proxy for fitness of some kind.
It's telling you if you can grow that peacock's tail and keep it in good nick
and display it frequently then you must be quite healthy. You must have good disease resistance
genes or something like that. And that's the kind of version of sexual selection we always hear from natural history programs
and that is generally pursued by most biologists.
And it's probably not wrong, but there's another thing going on that I think is usually
more important, particularly when you get these exaggerated flamboyant plumages.
And that is the idea that Ronald Fisher first thought of in 1930 and was later mathematically
proved by Russell Landy and Mark Kirkpatrick in 1980.
And that is that the fitness the females are after may not be just whether their offspring survive,
but whether their offspring seduce. The thing that really matters to them may be having offspring
that can persuade members of the opposite sex to mate, particularly male offspring,
particularly male offspring, and that it's no use choosing an ugly male partner that is particularly strong and disease resistant, so that you can have strong and disease resistant sons, if those sons can't persuade other females to mate with them because they haven't got flamboyant tails.
Otherwise known as the sexy sun hypothesis.
The sexy sun hypothesis.
And that's a sort of runaway effect.
Seduction of the hottest versus survival of the fittest is another way I put it.
I didn't think of that till after I'd finished the book, but.
Oh, what a shame.
That was good.
That's good.
Okay.
So we have this sort of Fisherian runaway selection thing going on that.
this sort of Fisherian runaway selection thing going on that traits that are sexually attractive are selected over time that causes sons to become sexier, but eventually you end up with
a risk and a trade-off for the males.
Even before we get onto risks and trade-offs, why is it that there is such a thing as sexiness that isn't just utility of survivalness?
Why is it not that maximized survivalness is sexiness?
Why is there this other pathway, this other attribute?
Right.
Well the answer to that, I think the clue to it, and I can't prove this, and this is
the problem with this version of sexual selection, there is very hard to devise experiments that prove it. I can't prove this. This is the problem with this version of sexual dyslexia.
There is very hard to devise experiments that prove it. I will mention one in a minute.
The answer, I think, is that the smallest bias in the females in a random direction
will get exaggerated. It doesn't really matter which direction it's in,
it will run away. You can't stop it. And so the clue is the fact that you get such extraordinary
diversity of sexually selected ornaments in birds and other animals. In other words, there is no
pattern, there's no general practice that they tend to have
eyes.
Right.
It's not always the biggest tails.
It's not always the brightest front.
It's not always the tails.
It's not always the wings.
It's not always the crest.
It's not always the breast.
It's not always the back.
It's not always red.
It's not always yellow.
Do you see what I mean?
And once you start looking at the extraordinary diversity of ways in which sexual
selection has gone mad in the birds of paradise, in the pheasants, in the mannequins, in species
like that, why do puffins have red and blue stripes on their beak?
That's a sort of completely different way of doing things.
There's a bird called the tragepan, which pops out from behind a log when
he's trying to seduce a female and lowers from his throat an electric blue apron with red patterns
on it. I've seen that. Of skin. Why? So it's the very arbitrary nature of the features that I think argues for this process. Now you can still say,
yeah, but why, you know, why would it matter? And of course, probably what's going on is that to
start with being a bit brighter than another male does mean your immune systems in better order,
or you haven't been infected with malaria
or something like that.
So at the end of the book, I say, hang on, we're constantly trying to choose between
these two theories, fitness and hotness, if you like, and we shouldn't have to.
They're obviously both going to end up assisting each other.
Well, if you assume that the reason that you have fitness is to survive in order to be
able to reproduce and hotness allows you to reproduce more quickly, they end up netting
out at the same outcome even if they sort of get there in different paths.
Yes, but it might be worth mentioning that what I think is the best experiment I describe
in my book, it
doesn't feature birds, unfortunately, it features a small insect.
It was done by Andrew Bownford and one of his students on a sort of Brazilian fly.
What he did was he allowed them to mate, in the, this is, and she actually, it was she who
did the work and I'm trying to remember her name, but Andrew's student.
He took the unsuccessful males and put them on one side and the successful males and he
bred a lineage from one and he bred a lineage from the other.
So he's now got the failures and the successes fathering the next generation and he does
that for several generations.
And then he says, what's the difference between these flies at the end of several generations?
Are they less able to
survive because they've been bred from the failures and the answer is no.
Are they less able to persuade other flies to make with them?
Yes.
So that's quite a nice, that's the best experiment for teasing out these
two hypotheses that, that, that I've come across.
That's really cool to understand that there is one dial for fitness and one
dial for hotness and they're maybe interrelated upstream before them.
There is something that causes them both to happen and maybe they do on average
tend to happen sort of synchronously that fitter tend to be hotter.
I would also imagine that that's the case, but that they are distinct and
they are interpreted in different ways. So that's, that's, that's cool.
So just to kind of round out the Fisherian runaway thing, any minor advantage
in terms of sexual selection, trait display that a male has.
If it's even, you know, 5149 over time, that will be selected for sufficiently
that it continues to get more and it continues to get brighter and it continues
to get more elaborate.
And that's where you end up with after a few million years of evolution, you just
end up with these sort of very, very extravagant displays.
Yes.
Although if the runaway process is as accelerating as Fisher thought, then it
not might not be a million years.
It might be one of these things that happens really very quickly in a few thousand years.
And, you know, the peacock might've gone from having a short tail to having a huge tail in the
sort of blink of an evolutionary eye. And one of the ideas I tie with in the book is can we,
can we catch a species in the moment when it suddenly starts having a runaway selection.
And come back in a thousand years and see what's happened.
You need to, I don't know, I feel like you need to sow the seeds with, uh,
girly daytime magazines, you know, that have got the new trend, what to look for in this, this summer's new boyfriend or whatever.
And, uh, that's how you sort of inject it socially
and from there the runaway begins.
Well, this is why I went to and sat for two nights running
on top of a mountain in Norway,
not allowed out of my little canvas blind
from 8 p.m. to 8 a.m.
watching a bird that displays at midnight, through the middle
of the night, called the great snipe. Of course it's not dark in Norway at that
time of the year and the great snipe looks like any other snipe. There are 17
species of snipe on the planet and they are all sort of really well camouflaged
in marsh vegetation including this one one doesn't look much different.
But this one does it, this lecking males gather together
and competitively display.
Um, and at the height of its display, it flashes it, the white feathers in its
tail very, and it's like turning on a light, it's very bright, little flash,
very brief, but the tail feathers are not very exaggerated and they're not much whiter in the male than
the female a little bit.
If you tippex some of the male's tails, so young people don't know what tippex is, but
if you put white paint on some of the male's tails, um, uh, you can improve their mating success.
According to some Scandinavian biologists who are very ingenious.
Oh, it's like a, it's like a snipe aesthetician giving them some
odd beauty beautification augmenting.
So it's a boob job.
It's a boob job for Snipes.
Exactly.
Um, and so, but my point is the male snipe and female snipe look almost identical.
In fact, you can't really tell the difference and the tail is a bit different, but you
can't see it very well most of the time.
So, so maybe this is a species that's only just started having highly skewed sexual,
uh, mating success so that one male gets to mate with 10 females, which is roughly
what happens.
It hasn't had time for the tail to get huge and white and dramatic, and that might be
about to happen.
The conventional explanation for the great snipe is that it's because it's often displaying in very poor light
there's no point in being brightly colored and a lot of the display
involves making clicking noises and maybe it's an auditory lek rather than
a visual lek but other birds make noises on the lek too so I kind of like my idea
best that this is a species that's only just begun to leck. There's another bird called
the buff-breasted sandpiper, which sort of lecks, but sort of doesn't. I'd like to watch
that species for a thousand years and see what happens.
What's the leck paradox?
The leck paradox is that the black grouse, which leck, live next door to the red grouse, which like live next door to the red grouse, which don't, they pair
up.
So one male, one female, and they both bring up the kids.
Therefore, because the one male gets to mate with 10 to 20 females in the black grouse,
but the other 19 or nine or 19 males on his leg don't get to mate at all that year.
The bird will have less genetic diversity in its population than the red grouse.
It will be more genetically monotonous.
It will be more inbred, not to the stage where it's a sort of health problem because
the males usually only get one year at the top and the females disperse and so you know the species
is fine in that sense. But it must be the case that there is less genetic diversity in a leking
species like the black grouse than a a leking species like the black grouse
than a monogamous species like the red grouse.
In which case there's less point in being choosy because the genes are going to be more
similar.
I mean, when you go onto a lek, you're bound to be looking at some half brothers because
they tend to recruit to a lek near where they were born.
And if they were born in the same
year then the chances are they had the same father even though they might have had different mothers.
So if they're half brothers and they look the same and by the way they do look very similar to us,
then what's the point of being so choosy? The species that are most choosy about making sure
you get the very, very best male and not settling for second
best, which the redgrass do all the time.
They say, look, I just want a bloke who's going to look after kids.
I don't care what he looks like.
I'm anthropomorphizing, but you get the point.
The species that are most choosy have least reason to be choosy.
That is the lect paradox.
I think the
Fisher theory shows you a way out of it. It doesn't matter how little variation
there is you've still got to follow the fashion but it not really I mean you
know I'm struggling with it too so it is a paradox and it's a it's an intriguing
one. This episode is brought to you by Whoop. I've won Whoop for over five years
now since way before they were a partner on the show and it's the only wearable I've ever stuck with because
it is the best. I've actually tracked over 1600 days of my life with it, according to
the app, which is pretty crazy. Whoop is super innocuous. You don't even remember that you've
got it on and yet it tracks everything 24 seven via a little device on your wrist. It
tracks your heart rate, your sleep, your recovery, all of your workouts,
your resting heart rate, heart rate variability,
how much you're breathing throughout the night and now can even track your steps.
But all of this into an app and spits out very simple to understand
and fantastically usable data.
It's phenomenal.
I'm a massive, massive fan of them.
And best of all, you can join for free, pay nothing for the brand new whoop 4.0 strap, plus you get your first month for free. And there's a 30 day money back guarantee.
So you can buy it for free, try it for free. If you don't like it after 29 days, I'll just give
you your money back. You can get the brand new Whoop 4.0 and that 30 day free trial by going to
the link in the description below or heading to join.whoop.com slash modern wisdom. That's join.whoop.com
or heading to join.whoop.com slash modern wisdom. That's join.whoop.com slash modern wisdom.
Right.
So birds that have these, um, speed dating, which is kind of bird speed
dating is what sort of like the, the, the lacking is in a way.
Yeah.
But speed dating where they all end up meeting with the same male.
Remember?
Yeah.
Yeah.
Okay.
Good.
That doesn't happen to you and me.
Non-monogamous speed dating for birds.
In those situations, you have a lot of the reproductive rewards
accruing to a few at the top.
Yeah.
You also have to assume that that would mean the more sexually
selective the women are being, the more that
they're skewing toward that single male or small number of males at the top.
There's going to be less of a chance of survival for that next generation that
comes along just due to some of the inevitable reduced genetic diversity.
So you think, okay, these two things kind of do come into
conflict a little bit with each other, the hotness and the
fitness can actually start to, it feels like they can fight
against each other.
Yes.
Well, the conspicuous plumage for a start is a threat to
survival.
Um, the, uh, the dancing and fighting that you do for months on end is a threat to your survival.
So yeah, males are putting themselves at risk to present themselves.
But one way of looking at it is that the black grouse and the red grouse, the males are putting
in an awful lot of effort in both species, but the red grouse, the effort is going into escorting the female, defending
the territory, escorting the chicks, helping the chicks, sheltering the chicks, being vigilant
over the family, things like that.
Whereas the black grouse, the effort is going into endless displays, fights, competitive dances
and so on.
So you end up deciding which way to push your effort.
And when you push all your male effort into display, you are wasting it as far as the lineage is concerned, as far as the chicks are concerned.
So if I go out in June in the Pennines, I can find a pair of redgrouse in which the male is
standing up looking around and the female is down in the heather with the chicks and he's got his
eyes out and if he sees a hawk coming, he gives an alarm call and they all hide. He's very valuable in that sense. If
I find a black grass with chicks there's no sign of the male. He's miles away. He's had
his two seconds of fun two months ago or whatever. The female is entirely on her own and having
a one parent looking after the offspring as opposed to two is bound to be a disadvantage.
And sure enough, black grass seem to have lower chicks survival through that period
of where chicks are small and indeed they have smaller broods actually.
So the species as a whole is not going to do as well.
And that's a rather intriguing thought, I think, that sometimes these sexual selection
arms races end up making a species more likely to go extinct.
That's fascinating.
Sexual selection could actually be a maladaptive force that pushes species toward an unsustainable
extreme.
Yes.
This idea has been around for a long time time and there was a sort of rather cartoonish
version of it that was in vogue for a while.
Do you remember the ancient Irish elk, the species that went extinct at the end of the
Ice Age, which was an enormous deer bigger than a moose and with huge antlers, much bigger
than a moose's antlers, but similar much bigger than a moose's antlers, but similar in shape
to a moose's antlers.
And how on earth these poor deer managed to carry these vast antlers around is sort of
a bit of a mystery.
And what were they for?
Were they for fighting or were they for displaying?
And actually there's some quite good evidence that they might have been more about display than
fighting based on how good they would have been as weapons, if you like. But the question of why
that species went extinct used to be dominated by the theory that the antlers got too big
extinct, used to be dominated by the theory that the antlers got too big and the deer couldn't fit between the trees when your and my ancestor was running after them with
a spear.
And so they caught them.
Now, nobody thinks that's why it went extinct.
It was a large animal.
Our ancestors were very good at wiping out large animals, which were slow breeding and
easy to find. They wiped out mammoths
and woolly rhinoceroses and stepped bison and things as well. So at the end of the ice
age, it was doomed because it got predated by human beings, or because climate changed
or something, not because their antlers were too big. besides, if you look in some of the best bogs in Ireland
that have lots of these animals in them where they got stuck in the mud, there was higher
mortality among young than old deer, as you'd expect in any species. the, you can take these arguments about section selection being a handicap a little too far
if you're trying to use them to explain the extinction of a species, but maybe it does
play some role. How extreme can these traits become then?
Well, there's a little bird called the clubwinged mannequin, which has a display in which it makes a sort of resonant twanging noise with its wings, which carries a long way through
the Ecuadorian cloud forest where it lives. And in order to make this noise, the bird has had to
redesign not just the feathers of its wings, which are contorted in a sort of strange way,
but the wing bones themselves. Wing bones are generally the same in all birds. I mean,
obviously there's a scale difference, big birds and small birds, but the shape of a wing bone is generally pretty well defined as being, you know, the best
strength to weight ratio and things like that. Not in this species. It's got a sort of weird,
heavy club shaped wing bone in its body. I mean, in its wing, which is there purely to enable it to
make a clanging noise in the
springtime or the breeding season.
They don't have spring in the equator.
And Richard Prum has written about this in his book, The Evolution of Beauty.
And it's quite a good example of just the lengths.
I mean, this must make it harder to fly for a start.
The lengths to which actual selection can go, a peacock's tail, there's a bird
called the bullwaz pheasant, which lives in Borneo, where the male when
he displays disappears into an enormous sort of white disc, which actually comes from his
tail and his head is then hidden by fleshy inflated blue tubes that stretch before and
after the head.
So he looks like a sort of plate with a blue knife on it.
That's not a very good description, but do you see what I mean? And you get to think,
poor creature, what have the females done to this species to make it to these ordeals. But that gets to another point which I'm intrigued by, which is
that sexual selection can be possibly a more creative force than natural selection. Because
instead of just saying in a utilitarian way, I just want to enable you to survive, it says,
let's try something really wacky and see what we end up with.
And Richard Prum has this theory, he's the guy who worked out what color the feathers
were on dinosaurs, by the way.
And he has this theory that feathers were invented for display before they were ever
used for flight and that we wouldn't have had flight if we hadn't had sexual display.
Wow.
That is cool.
Yeah. I suppose if you're just rolling the dice in so many ways, it's like, Hey,
they might be attracted to this, try it on, you know, here's a new outfit.
Here's a new fashion.
Have a crack.
Exactly.
Yeah.
And I haven't mentioned the bowerbirds, but I've got to get them in at some point.
Australia and New Zealand, sorry, Australia and New Guinea, where bowerbirds live.
These are birds that have basically invented art.
They build complicated structures not to nest in, but to seduce females in. It's the males that
build them and they decorate them with colorful objects arranged in ways to enhance perspective and ways to look decorative and
sorted by color and all sorts of things.
I watched a great bower bird at his bower trying to seduce a female with a red chili
pepper which he was displaying to her on the edge of a cemetery in Queensland in Australia. But his main art installation was a
huge patch of grey and white objects, which snails, shells, and bones and things like that,
but also bits of plastic and bottle tops and bits of broken glass, et cetera, because we were in the edge of a town.
And this art installation included not only a plastic hand grenade, but a tiara, a toy
tiara.
I think it was a toy, maybe it was a real diamond tiara.
What about seemingly tiny traits, very sort of minuscule things that for us to look
at, we wouldn't realize that it was actually a different, but that is something that's
sexually selected for as well.
Yes.
Um, uh, I mean, some of the, some of the song things are very, very obscure.
A lot of the, a lot of seabirds, things like puffins, the male and female look identical.
You really can't tell the difference between them.
They can, but we can't.
And they're both brightly colored. So there's a bird called
the crested orclit, which is a cousin of the puffin, which lives in the Pacific Ocean. And
there was a very neat experiment done on that in the 1990s where they said they grow just a tiny little black sort of forward-pointing crest
on the top of the head and their beak gets much redder in the breeding season.
So they took some birds, caught some birds, and they lengthened the top knot on the head
or shortened it and then measured how long it took
for that bird to acquire a mate and by lengthening the top knot you shorten the time that the bird
takes to acquire a mate. The bird is more attractive and that was true for both sexes.
So that's rather intriguing that proved what we had suspected for a long time that
that you can get mutual sexual selection.
You can get choosiness in both sexes in some species for the same criterion.
And then there's a bird in New Zealand called the Paradise Shell Duck where the male and
female are both smart, but they look very different.
The male has a black head and a gray patterned body and white wings.
The female has an orange body and a white head.
They're both striking birds, but they look quite different. Now, clearly, the females are saying,
I want the male with the blackest head. The males are saying, I want the female with the whitest head.
Well, does that ring a bell? Do human beings have mutual sexual selection? Yes, we're both very choosy when we pick long term partners, but we don't have the same
criteria, do we?
Male beauty and female beauty are different things, both on the outside of the body and
possibly on the inside of the brain.
In other news, you've probably heard me talk about element before, and that's because frankly,
I'm dependent on it.
For the last three years I've started my morning every single day the same way with Element
in a cold glass of water.
Element is a tasty electrolyte drink mix with everything that you need and nothing that
you don't.
Each Grab and Go stick pack contains a science-backed electrolyte ratio of sodium, potassium and
magnesium with no coloring, no sugar, no artificial ingredients or any other junk.
It plays a critical role in reducing muscle cramps and fatigue while optimizing brain health,
regulating appetite and curbing cravings. Their orange salt in a cold glass of water is
gorgeous. It's a beautiful salty sweet orangey nectar and it's how I've started my day every
single day for as long as I can remember and I genuinely
feel the difference when I take it versus when I don't.
Best of all, they've got a no questions asked refund policy with an unlimited duration so
you can buy it totally risk free.
If you don't like it, you don't even need to return the box.
That's how confident they are that you'll love it.
Right now you can get a free sample pack of all eight flavors with any purchase by going
to the link in the description below or heading to drink lmnt.com slash modern
wisdom.
That's drink lmnt.com slash modern wisdom.
Okay.
So could sexual selection have shaped the human mind?
We talked a lot about birds so far.
Let's bring it a little bit closer to home for what it could have done to us.
Yeah. Yeah, my book isn't about one ugly African ape, but inevitably, you know, one feels obliged
to put a chapter in at the end about this.
And I'm absolutely sure that sexual selection is going on in our species.
I'm also pretty sure it's mutual and not like the black grass in which it's female selectivity that's driving male
appearance. I think both sexes are very selective. We're a monogamous species, at least socially
monogamous. That doesn't mean we're necessarily faithful and we can be much less choosy when it
comes to short-term sexual encounters. But for long-term pair bonds, both sexes are pretty damn choosy about who they settle down with.
That, after all, is the plot of every romantic comedy ever made.
So what's going on in human beings?
What are we selecting for?
Well, clearly there are sexually selected features of bodies like breasts or beards or something that may be
involved in beauty.
But I think it's more interesting to look at the inside of the head because the human
brain did something very odd.
It exploded in size over a relatively short period of about a million, two million years,
maybe three, I don't know, but not
a very long period.
It accelerated.
The increase in brain size was very steady until around Homo erectus, it suddenly takes
off.
And actually, it's got slightly smaller again in the last 50,000 years, we think.
It reached its maximum size about 50,000 years ago on average.
And that might be something to do with, you know, agriculture enabling us to live on
meat, more meager diets or something like that.
But it was very costly.
I mean, the human brain is a huge user of energy.
It takes a lot of energy to build it, takes a lot of energy to build it, it takes a lot of energy to run it.
Why?
What's the purpose of growing such a big brain?
No other species needed it to survive on the savanna.
If you say, right, well, it helped us get through the ice age on the savanna when the
climate was very variable, plenty of other species managed to survive on the savanna, you know, buffaloes and gazelles and baboons and chimpanzees in similar habitats and so on.
They didn't need 1200 cc brains.
So maybe it wasn't all about survival.
Maybe it was about something else.
Now there's two other possibilities.
One is that it was a social thing, that we needed big brains to understand the
groups of people we were living in. We lived in big groups, we were plotting and
scheming and deceiving each other, so we needed big brains to figure out what
other people were up to and that kind of thing. And that's a very popular thing
called the social brain hypothesis and that's obviously to some extent true as well. But there's a third possibility
which almost never gets discussed, but which was laid out in a very good book by Jeffrey
Miller 25 years ago called The Mating Mind, in which he says actually, this looks awfully
like a sexually selected feature. It's a mental peacock's tail.
The sudden takeoff, the fact that it didn't happen to other species, and the fact that
the things we use it for are not just solving practical problems or understanding how to
get on with each other in society. We also use it very conspicuously for things like
wit and humor music and song
Verbal dexterity
Poetry all these kinds of things
Toolmaking as well, you know, I mean practical things as well
Some of which looks awfully like showing off
to members of the opposite sex. So maybe, and you know, it's not at all difficult to see that people with great minds are attractive
to members of the opposite sex in human beings. People, you know, with the verbal dexterity of George Clooney or the singing ability of
Mick Jagger, you know, these guys don't do badly in the attractiveness stakes.
I've chosen male examples, but I genuinely want to keep stressing that I think in our
species it's going both ways.
Humor is a very good example.
If you ask people, Helen Fisher did this, how important is humor to you in choosing
a sexual partner?
It scores very highly.
The personal columns where people advertise for, well, I guess they don't do it anymore.
They do it on site online, but you know, good sense of humor, GSOH is, is a very important
part of it.
And what, what's the point of humor otherwise, you know, and watch what people do with humor.
They show off with it.
You know, they're, they're not doing it to find out information from other people, they're doing it to impress
other people. And that looks awfully like sexual display. So Miller says, and I think he's right,
that this isn't a slam dunk, this isn't a proven idea, but to spend the whole of the 20th century thinking about Freud and Marx
and Piaget and all the other theories of mind, behaviorism and all these things without taking
into account that the organ we're doing all this behavior with was probably subject to sexual
selection and was probably being used to seduce as well as to
survive. To do all that without taking that into account is a mistake. And we might have left an
enormous hole within a lot of our social science, within psychology and sociology and economics and
all these other disciplines, the whole being sex.
And we need to put it back in there.
It's mating all the way down.
It was always mating.
It's turtles all the way down.
It is.
So, you know, one of the things that you've mentioned there
is this, I guess, bi-directional sexual selection
that traits happen both not just male to female,
but female to male as well.
What determines whether it is unidirectional or bidirectional?
What does that say about the environment and the child rearing and the expectations of
that particular species?
How does that all fit together?
Yes.
And the person who solved that problem was a brilliant evolutionary psychologist
called Robert Trivers, who said something that's blindingly obvious, but none of us
had thought of it before, and he said it in the early seventies, he said, the
species where the sex that invests most in rearing the offspring will be competed for by the sex that invests
least.
So it's called parental investment.
But it's a vicious circle because, as I say, the redgrouse, they both invest a lot in looking
after the kids.
The blackgrouse, they don't.
The female does it all.
So the blackgrouse, you get huge amount of male-male competition to try to mate with
females and a lot of sexual selection, less in the redgrouse.
But which came first, the chicken or the egg?
You know, did the parental investment come first or the sexual selection come first?
And the sort of exception that proves the rule here is those species of birds where
it's reversed, where the brightly colored forward and aggressive females compete for dull colored males because the males sit on
the eggs.
I studied one of these species, it's called the gray fallow rope, it lives in the Arctic.
Fallow ropes, jucanas, doterals, there's a number of species that do this.
It's not very common, but it's not all that rare either. The female
is much more conspicuous, much more boldly colored, spends much more time displaying,
and much more inclined to fight with other females.
So that kind of proves Triva's parental investment theory right.
Now in human beings, you can say that women do more of the work.
And of course they do.
They do gestation and lactation, which men can't contribute to at all.
But compared with gorillas or chimpanzees, males do contribute an awful lot more parenting
than most other great apes. We have less sexual dimorphism than most other great apes. I mean, a male gorilla weighs twice as much, if not more, three times as much as a female
gorilla.
And he has a harem of six or seven females.
In chimpanzees, they have a multi-male system where each female mates with lots of males,
partly to frustrate the tendency of
males to commit infanticide, which they do in a lot of mammals to bring females back
into fertility, probably in human beings too.
Look at the number of stepchildren that get killed compared with biological children.
The murder rate is much higher.
The Cinderella effect as it's known.
The Cinderella effect, exactly.
So it's unfortunate that there's only four great apes, orangutans, chimpanzees, gorillas
and us, plus the gibbons are similar species.
Because if there were 30 or 40 species of apes, then we could really do some good comparative
analysis and see how we ended up with the mating system that we did.
But I would argue that the need for fathers to be involved in provisioning and protecting offspring as
well as mothers has been a feature of hunter gatherer life for a very long time.
And it has made us into a species in which females are going to be pretty
choosy about males as well as males being pretty choosy about males,
as well as males being pretty choosy about females.
Are there any parallels between bird mating behaviors
or whatever and human romantic displays
or social structures?
Well, it's hard not to watch some of these bird displays and not draw parallels with
nightclubs and other things.
There's a strutting that both species do, I suppose.
But I think that's mostly anthropomorphism. We,
you know, we human beings. Well, I think song is actually the
most intriguing one, because there's no other mammal that is
as interested in singing as we are with the
possible exception of gibbons and one of maybe howler monkeys but you know we song and language
are very unique and remarkable human features and they feature heavily in seduction and display.
That's true of many birds as well.
And the complexity of song in birds is truly extraordinary.
The number of different phrases and different motifs.
Oh, sorry.
I've left out whales.
Haven't I?
Whales really sing as much as we do.
So they're another example.
When you try and teach a chimpanzee to speak, it's really tough and you can get up to a
few hundred words, you can't get grammar, you can get up to a few hundred words.
You can't get grammar.
You can't get syntax really to speak of same for a gorilla.
When you try and teach a parrot to speak, and this has been done.
There was a famous African parrot called Alex, who was an enormous vocabulary
and really seemed to understand grammar in a way that, um, and any other, most
other animals can't, um, you know, the word order or whatever matters, you
know, in terms of what it means.
Um, uh, in that sense, there are similarities between us and, and, and birds.
Okay.
Do you, another similarity question, I guess, is, is, is, is, is, is, is In that sense, there are similarities between us and birds.
Okay, another similarity question, I guess.
Do birds and humans have an innate appreciation for beauty?
Is the drive for aesthetic pleasure some evolutionary force?
There's a rather good quote from Darwin on this, which I'm rather fond of, which is,
birds appear to be the most aesthetic of all animals, excepting, of course, man, and they
have nearly the same taste for the beautiful as we have.
And he's really flirting with a dangerous idea there that there's something uncannily similar
about us and birds here because there's no reason why.
It's convergent evolution if we and birds have this similar taste for the beautiful and one of the things that I've been thinking about is
it's unlikely to have been inherited from a common ancestor this taste for the beautiful
because our common ancestor with birds we now know lived about 400 million years ago.
That's an awfully long time ago and we know what that common ancestor looked like, roughly.
It was a lumbering reptile that lived in a swamp.
It gave rise to both the dinosaurs, which gave rise to the birds, and to the so-called
mammal-like reptiles, which gave rise to the mammals.
So we're not close cousins, descended from a creature that had a sense of the beautiful,
probably.
Maybe we are, but it doesn't seem likely.
It seems more likely that we have ended up with an appreciation of color and tune and song and melody and fashion and all these kind of things,
and so have quite a lot of bird species. And it just so happens that those have ended up with similar outcomes.
Now, why might that be?
Well, notice that on the whole section of selection goes for pure colors,
not browns and grays.
So it goes for limited number of wavelengths,
you know, limited number of frequencies, pure hues. Not, you know, if you've got
every hue you can think of then you end up looking brown. And it's the same with
song. If you just want to make a noise, a click or a roar or something,
it's got every sort of frequency in it.
But if you go for just specific frequencies, you get a whistle or a, or a
tone or a, or a tune.
Um, and that's of course much harder to do.
I mean, you can make a boring noise by dropping a rock or you can paint something brown just by mixing lots of materials together, but to
actually create something that has a pure color or a pure sound is much more
improbable, much more unlikely, much more conspicuous, much rarer. And that's why we use it in our sexual displays and that's why birds
use it in their sexual displays. And so there's a sort of almost a thermodynamic idea at the root
of this. But as you can see, I'm beginning to wave my hands a bit and I haven't thought this one through properly.
I like it.
I mean, definitely the refined nature of it not being everything suggests that you're
purposefully doing this one thing.
If you're brown, you didn't mean to be brown, you just are brown.
But if you're such a pure color, if you're such a pure note or tone or sound
or whatever, that suggests that there's been some thought put into it,
some pressure selected for it.
Yes.
There's a sort of watchmaker aspect to it.
What do you think?
So, you know, taking a broader picture here, lots of past failures
in evolutionary theory, trying to work out why things were the way that they were.
What do you think we should learn about biases in interpreting our nature, what we should
consider, where things come from, given the replete history of us putting off both of
our feet in our mouths and getting stuff wrong all the time?
Well, for me, the history of science always teaches the importance of humility.
Overconfident rejection of maverick ideas is the constant theme of all science.
But that doesn't mean that every maverick who comes along waving a new theory is Galileo.
Quite a lot of the time he's not, or she's not.
For me, that's the big puzzle of my life is how do I know when to listen to a maverick and when to tell them to get lost.
Because, you know, there are many, many scientific debates where you just want to say,
oh, for God's sake, get real, that idea can't be right.
And 95% of the time you're right to have that attitude, but 5% of the time you're being
like Catholics and being dogmatic and telling a perfectly sensible chap to get lost when
you shouldn't.
This was true of Darwin generally, in that he was a maverick and a heretic and he had
to work really hard to get taken seriously.
Evolution was rejected and still is by many people.
And it's true of his sexual selection idea where he did, he was rejected as a nut
nutcase in his lifetime and for quite a long time afterwards and wrongly so.
But since then, lots of people have put forward fresh ideas about why birds are colorful. For example, to go back to this, there was a theory in the 1980s that it was all about
warning predators that you were in good health and therefore there was no point in chasing
you.
Well, I don't really see why a kingfisher needs to do that more than a sparrow, but
you know, maybe there's an idea there.
And in general, I'm more frustrated by science being too dogmatic than being too open to new ideas. Yes, if you're too open to new ideas,
if you open your mind too much, your brain falls out, as someone must put it. But I would like,
generally, to teach the lesson that we need to be more tolerant of disagreement, of heresy, of mavericks,
and give them at least the privilege of testing their ideas. That said,
you often get told by people, I've got this new theory. And the, the, the line I always come back with was how are you going to test it?
And that often shuts people up.
So it's lazy to come up with an idea.
It takes work to test it.
Awesome.
Matt Ridley, ladies and gentlemen, Matt, I'm a massive fan of your work.
I think this is really, really interesting.
I didn't realize I was going to become such a garage ornithologist for the afternoon.
Um, where should people go?
Do they want to keep up to date with your work and what you've got coming out?
Um, well, I'm, I have a website, which I mostly keep, keep up to date.
I'm, I'm, it's called matridley.co.uk.
Um, I, uh, I'm just about to get on Substack, I think too.
I can churn my stuff out there.
I am just about to get on Substack, I think, too. I can churn my stuff out there.
Um, uh, but, uh, um, and I'm on, well, I'm on Twitter.
I'm not very active on Facebook and LinkedIn, but I try and be, um, and I
write, uh, books and journalism as well.
And the book is, the book is called, I should say, Bird, Sex and Beauty.
The strain, the, um, oh, what's the subtitle?
The implications of Charles Darwin's Strangest Idea.
Heck yeah.
Matt, I appreciate you.
Thank you.
Thank you so much for having me on and allowing me to rabid on at such length.
If you are looking for new reading suggestions, look no further than the Thanks so much for having me on and allowing me to rabid on at such length. it right now for free by going to chriswillx.com slash books.
That's chriswillx.com slash books.