Modern Wisdom - #939 - Dr Charan Ranganath - The Neuroscience Of How To Improve Your Memory & Focus

Episode Date: May 10, 2025

Dr. Charan Ranganath is a cognitive neuroscientist, professor, and an author. What are memories? Our brains are shaped by countless experiences, but how exactly do we store these stories? Learn what ...makes some memories stick, why others fade, and how our minds handle the ones we'd rather forget. Expect to learn why memory is important, what the difference is between the experiencing self and the remembering self, How human memory works, why we remember some events so clearly and others vaguely or not at all, how we can make ourselves forget, the best memory improvement techniques, what the relationship between memory and novel experiences is, how our memories shaped by our social interactions, and much more… Sponsors: See discounts for all the products I use and recommend: https://chriswillx.com/deals Get up to $50 off the RP Hypertrophy App at https://rpstrength.com/modernwisdom Get 35% off your first subscription on the best supplements from Momentous at https://livemomentous.com/modernwisdom Get 5 Free Travel Packs, Free Liquid Vitamin D, and more from AG1 at https://ag1.info/modernwisdom Extra Stuff: Get my free reading list of 100 books to read before you die: https://chriswillx.com/books Try my productivity energy drink Neutonic: https://neutonic.com/modernwisdom Episodes You Might Enjoy: #577 - David Goggins - This Is How To Master Your Life: https://tinyurl.com/43hv6y59 #712 - Dr Jordan Peterson - How To Destroy Your Negative Beliefs: https://tinyurl.com/2rtz7avf #700 - Dr Andrew Huberman - The Secret Tools To Hack Your Brain: https://tinyurl.com/3ccn5vkp - Get In Touch: Instagram: https://www.instagram.com/chriswillx Twitter: https://www.twitter.com/chriswillx YouTube: https://www.youtube.com/modernwisdompodcast Email: https://chriswillx.com/contact - Learn more about your ad choices. Visit megaphone.fm/adchoices

Transcript
Discussion (0)
Starting point is 00:00:00 Why is memory important? Well, memory is probably not important for the reason people think it is, right? So we always think, well memory is important when we can't remember, then we get frustrated about it. We say, oh, why can't I remember this person's name? Why can't I remember the name of that guy who is that thing? Why memory is really important is because it's absolutely central to helping us understand the present, where we are in space, when we are at a time, and to be able to plan and imagine possible futures.
Starting point is 00:00:37 So if you look at people with memory disorders, their problem in life is not that they can't remember the past per se, it's that their inability't remember the past per se. It's that their inability to remember the past makes it hard for them to remember whether they've eaten recently, or they end up repeating themselves over and over again, or they just don't have much foresight into what they will do in the future. They have all of these deficits that keep them from living independently, not because they can't tell you what happened, you know, an hour ago or something. It's because that inability leads them to just not be able to do almost anything that healthy people do in society in a day to day basis. Yeah. So you've sort of touched on something there. You've got a self that experiences stuff and you've got a self that
Starting point is 00:01:29 remembers you experiencing stuff. What is the relationship, the difference, the tension between these two selves? Well, one of the things that we know from memory research is that the overwhelming majority of what we experience will be forgotten, right? So if your listeners end up telling somebody, Hey, I heard this great interview on this podcast, the interviewer was on fire, you know? And so then they, they describe it to one of their friends. If they spend 10 minutes describing this long form conversation that we're
Starting point is 00:02:02 having, that would be a huge success memory wise, right? There's no way anyone's going to regurgitate every word of what we said. And many of the important points we talk about, people will probably forget, right? So here's the thing. Now I want to make a decision about my life. Now I want to make a decision about whether to take vacation. Well, what do I do?
Starting point is 00:02:22 I think about all the past vacations I've taken, where I went and whether I like them or not. And if I do that, I'm going to be relying not on what I experienced, but on memory, which is much, much more, much less complete, right? It's this tiny fraction of what we actually experience. And so, Danny Kahneman, the Nobel Prize winning psychologist, referred to this idea that we have an experiencing self that lives through all of these things that we do, but the remembering self only has access to a tiny bit of that information. And that's the basis on which we decide, am I happy right now? What do I want to do in the future? Where am I? And so forth. We're really in the situation where we make most of our decisions based on the remembering
Starting point is 00:03:13 self. It's almost as if they're two different people because of the fact that the experiencing self is in the present and the remembering self is in the past. It's like, if you're a fan of that show, Severance, it's a bit like that. It sort of suggests to me that the, if there was a way for the remembering self to remember everything, the experiencing self experienced, that this would give us a fuller picture, maybe we would be choosing from a deeper, more rich data set to be able to make our decision about the vacation, whatever it is. Or, well, we once had a conversation with somebody about going to Tulum, but I forgot
Starting point is 00:03:56 about that conversation about Tulum. We went to Tulum and Tulum sucked or it was great or whatever. Yeah. Yeah, it's a great question. And Kahneman felt like it was in fact quite irrational that we don't have this capability to access the experiencing self when we remember. And to me, it's actually quite the opposite. If you talk to people who have what's called a highly superior autobiographical
Starting point is 00:04:19 memory, these are people who have just, uh, this incredible database of information that they've experienced. They could tell you on a particular date, seven years ago, exactly what they had for breakfast, what the weather was, who won the sports matches that day. Unbelievable. And if you read descriptions, many of these people talk about it as torture. They say it's something that they wouldn't wish on their worst enemy. I mean, they are often plagued by memories.
Starting point is 00:04:50 At best, it's irrelevant stuff, and at worst, it's the stuff that is minor and negative, but it just comes back to them over and over again. So one way I like to think about it is, is that we're blessed with this incomplete memory, because what we remember tends to be what we need.
Starting point is 00:05:09 So just as an analogy, imagine you're taking, you are taking a trip, right? Since we're in vacation mode, imagine you are taking a trip, you'll pack your suitcase. Now, will you pack everything you own? Probably not. Probably not. Probably not. Okay. Good. So I don't know, maybe you're a minimalist, but most of us would not.
Starting point is 00:05:29 Right. So when you're making a decision, what do you, about what to pack? What are you thinking of? How do you choose? Probably think about similar trips you took in the past, uh, might imagine what this trip might be like and try to work out what it could be that would be required. Exactly. So you're going to work out what you think you'll need on this trip, right?
Starting point is 00:05:51 And you'll carry with you only the stuff that you think you'll need. Sometimes you'll carry stuff that turns out you don't use. Sometimes you'll pack some things and it turns out that you're missing something. Right. And you forgot this jacket that you desperately need, and then you'll kick yourself for not having packed everything. But here's the bit on average, you do pretty well by packing just what you need. And that's how memory works.
Starting point is 00:06:17 It's kind of an analogy for memory and the journey of life that we take, right? Our brains are trying to figure out through millennia of evolution, what's the best information to carry with us? What's the information that we're going to need in the future? Is it going to be every word of every conversation or is it going to be the stuff that was important in some way? The stuff that we paid attention to, the stuff that was surprising or new, the stuff that got us emotionally aroused because it's rewarding or something that is scary and a threat that we want to remember later on, right? And if you look back at the memories of your life, those will be the kinds of things that
Starting point is 00:06:58 we tend to remember better. Just lingering on the super-remembers for a second, what's happening inside of the brains of these people? Are their brains structurally, functionally different? Do we know why it happens? Yeah, it's kind of surprising how little we know about these individual differences in memory overall. There are some hints that there are some differences. I believe it was in an area of brain called the striatum. But you know what surprised me in reading that research is how subtle and small the differences really are.
Starting point is 00:07:35 They're not all that big. And you can look at people with severely deficient autobiographical memory, people who you ask them, you know, what they did yesterday, and they'll give you a very minimal answer and they won't be able to remember hardly anything in detail. What they tell you will sound like they're reading from a book, not something that they actually lived, right? Yet you look in real life and the highly superior and the severely deficient people, they're functioning at equal levels. It's not like the highly superior people
Starting point is 00:08:12 are limitless and they're rich and famous. And yeah, you can find some rich and famous people with H-SAT, but for the most part, they're just like everybody else in terms of their daily life. They didn't perform better in school. They didn't do, you know, live out some amazing experiences that nobody else would have. So we don't know enough about what makes the brains different, but it may be something related in my opinion, and this is just purely opinion. It may be something to do with the way people think about what's interesting to them and the way they build their knowledge. So for instance, there's all this data, not all this data, there's all these videos of LeBron James. I mentioned him in my book, and I also did an interview with the NBA about this. And basically,
Starting point is 00:09:03 And I also did an interview with the NBA about this and basically. LeBron has this extraordinary memory for basketball. He could talk about a game that he played in long ago and described plays in that game. And the plays go point by point with a video that you could sync up to what you say. I mean, it's just, and you think about how quickly that information is going by him and how hard it would be to construct some kind of a detailed memory from it. But the guy has studied the game and played the game for so long. What seems like this incredibly confusing array of people running around at super high speeds and all of the stuff happening, he's able to just grab it and put it in a little compartment because he's already seen it before basically.
Starting point is 00:09:50 And so he's not even looking at what's happening now. He's looking at what's going to happen 30 seconds ahead, a minute ahead. He's looking at where the ball is going and anticipating three plays ahead, right? And it's that expertise that really gives him this extraordinary ability to remember it detail. And that's a case where it's not about remembering more in the sense of just remembering everything willy-nilly. It's about remembering better in the sense of being able to grab the patterns, the information that you need, and being able to pull it out as quickly as possible. Okay. Let's say we're talking about a normal human now, not a never remember and not an
Starting point is 00:10:28 everything remember. How does human memory work typically in our brains? Oh, that's such a big, big question. I think the way human memory works is it's often spontaneous in the sense that we will, you know, in our research, that's actually happening right now. But in some of our very recent research, we've found that we're not actually encoding rich memories all the time, because much of the world is very predictable. And in fact, it looks like our brains are really kind of forming these detailed snapshot memories at these moments when or when we're struggling a little bit because something's new or surprising or at times where this is just motivationally very important to us. We just achieved a goal of some kind, right? And so these moments are kind of these snapshots that we have.
Starting point is 00:11:26 And then later on, they can often spontaneously pop into our heads. Like if you hear a song that you haven't heard in a long time, or if you go back to a place you haven't been to since you were a child, all of a sudden these memories pop up in your head, right? But what happens is you get these little bits and pieces and then you reconstruct that into a story. What's critically interesting about that piece is that when we're remembering, we're never really replaying the past.
Starting point is 00:11:56 We're imagining how the past could have been. Just like an archaeologist imagines how an ancient civilization might have been based on some fragments of pottery that they take up, right? This episode is brought to you by the RP hypertrophy app. This app has made a massive impact on my gains and enjoyment in going to the gym over the last year. It's designed by Dr. Mike Isretel and comes with over 45 pre-made training programs and more than 250 technique videos. It takes all the guesswork out of crafting the ideal lifting routine by literally spoon-feeding
Starting point is 00:12:27 you a step-by-step plan for every workout. It'll guide you through the exact sets, reps and weight to use and most importantly, how to perfect your form so that every rep is optimized for maximum gains. It even adjusts every week based on your progress and there is a 30-day money back guarantee so you can buy it and train with it for 29 days. If you don't like it, they'll just give you your money back. That's interesting. So why do we remember some events so clearly and other ones that we've seen in the past that we've seen in the past that we've seen in the past that we've seen in the past that we've seen in the past that we've seen in the past that we've seen in the past that we've seen in the past that we've seen in the
Starting point is 00:12:55 past that we've seen in the past that we've seen in the past that we've seen in the past that we've seen in the past that we've seen in the past that we've seen in the past that we've seen in the past that we've seen in the past that we've seen in the past that we've seen in the past that we've seen in the slash modern wisdom and modern wisdom at checkout. That's interesting. So why do we remember some events so clearly and other ones vaguely or not at all? What, what predicts whether something will enter memory? There's many factors. Um, I actually came up with an acronym for this after talking about memory for a long time and it took me forever to realize that I could come up with an acronym for this after talking about memory for a long time. And it took me forever to realize that I could come up with a mnemonic to help me
Starting point is 00:13:28 remember these things, which is medic. Um, and so it stands for meaning error. Uh, let's see, it was meaning error. Now I'm trying to remember it all. Uh, you've got to remember the mnemonic. Distinctiveness. That's right. Meaning, error, distinctiveness, importance, and context.
Starting point is 00:13:50 It did come to me, the mnemonic hell. There it is. So, uh, so let's go through that. Right. Meaning is just like what I was telling you in the Lebron James example, when you have a body of knowledge about something, it makes it really easy to memorize new information based on what you already know. So for instance, if you're a football fan, I'll say European football or whatever,
Starting point is 00:14:13 everybody in America outside of America, what they would call football. Right. So now I give you a, a hypothetical player who doesn't exist at all. And I give you the statistics on their performance i say here's all the statistics you need to know what this player is good at what they've done in the past and so forth. If you're a soccer expert you can grab that information immediately even though it's just a bunch of random numbers i just made up a name of something. Because you have a structure in your mind for locking in that information. And so meaning is a really good way to lock in new information by being able to tie what you're trying to learn with stuff you already know. So a lot of mnemonic devices work that way. I also mentioned error and this is kind of a counterintuitive one. I usually like to talk about this last, but it's the second letter in medic.
Starting point is 00:15:06 So, so error is basically the fact that the more we struggle to try to pull up a memory and if we get the right feedback, what will happen is, is it gives our brain a chance to actually stabilize the memory and repair the problems that would happen. What I mean by this is let's say you memorize how to pronounce my name and then later on, an hour later, you just ask yourself, okay, how did I pronounce this name again? Let me try it and you just say it. And then you play back what you actually said, which you did it perfectly by the way. What you'll find is you hear
Starting point is 00:15:45 that and now your brain will go, wait, I made a little bit of a mistake. It wasn't quite what I said before. Let me fix that memory. And we can see this in action in the brain. And we've shown that you get activity in areas like the hip campus when you're actually trying to retrieve information. And then you repair these memories relative to just trying to memorize it, just looking. Right? That's why you learn a layout of a place better if you drive yourself than if you sit in an Uber.
Starting point is 00:16:16 Because you're actively trying to generate stuff for memory and then finding, oh wait, I was wrong. And those errors allow the brain to build a more detailed map, so to speak. So error is one, the more you struggle, the better it is. Information will stick and you'll be able to retain it later on. If, if you don't give yourself a chance to struggle, like you just read the lines and play without rehearsing them from memory. You're never going to memorize it and retain it. Well, right. So that's another example. So I went to error. There was also now distinctiveness. So distinctiveness means
Starting point is 00:17:00 that essentially our memories are competing with each other. And so this is a little bit counterintuitive. You might think memories are just like your phone. You store photos and every photo is independent of each other. But that's not the way the brain does it. This is why we don't run out of capacity like your phone does, because we're using the same neurons to code multiple memories. And so what happens is, is that there's a pattern of activity that's uniquely associated with my memory for you.
Starting point is 00:17:30 And a pattern of activity that's uniquely associated with other people I know. Now, to the extent that you look like people I know, those memories will be confusable to me. But if there's some way I can attend to the features that make you different from anyone else I've met, now I will have a distinctive memory. And so my brain, when it's trying to pull up a memory
Starting point is 00:17:55 of you, it won't be as hard because you stand out relative to all the other faces of people I've met, right? So where this comes into play, for instance, is when people will take photos at Instagram walls when they go to places or they're at a concert and they're taking videos. And what you find is that people actually have a poorer memory for these experiences when they do that than when they're actually just trying to immerse themselves in the experience. And the reason is that when you're mindlessly taking pictures, you're not actually immersed in the details that will give you a distinctive memory.
Starting point is 00:18:34 You're just kind of like floating around, trying to grab as much information as you can, and what we lose is the memory for that experience and all we get out of it is a recording that most of us never go back to. So distinctiveness is usually important. Now you could use the camera and say, I'm going to use this camera to take a picture of something that's so uniquely associated with this place or so uniquely associated with this moment that I will not be able to forget it. And that's where you're using the camera as a way of giving you a distinctive memory as opposed to depriving you of it. So we've gone through M, E, and D now, right? So I is the big one, which is importance.
Starting point is 00:19:17 And when I say importance, I'm not saying what you think is important in terms of your higher order self, but rather what your brain thinks is important based on evolution. The things that are important, as I mentioned before, tend to be things that are emotionally evocative in some way or another, or arousing in some way. Being in a new place, being surprised, but also being scared. Traumas are enormously memorable, regardless of whether we want them to be. Things that are like being in a state of desire, those moments are very memorable to us.
Starting point is 00:19:56 And so you can look at those states and what you'll find is that there's these chemicals in the brain that are released pretty intensively during these states. Chemicals like dopamine, noradrenaline, serotonin, cortisol, and many of your listeners, I'm sure, have heard some of these things. Dopamine, everybody talks about nowadays, right?
Starting point is 00:20:16 And what dopamine does, for instance, is it gets you revved up. It doesn't actually give you a sense of pleasure, but it does get you revved up and it promotes plasticity. It allows these memories to rapidly consolidate, so to speak, so that they're much more resilient. So importance is not necessarily what I think, oh yeah, I have to make sure that I remember this doctor appointment next week. But rather it's something more along the lines of, oh my God, I was in this cave and I got attacked by a bear.
Starting point is 00:20:52 I want to remember that experience and I want to remember how I got there, how to avoid that in the future. Right. That's the kind of thing I'm talking about. So importance is a big one. And that's why we often remember these emotional experiences. The last one, and I know I've gone on for a long time, but you wanted, you asked for it. I did indeed.
Starting point is 00:21:11 So another one is context. That we'll see in medic. So context is the place, the time that these events took place. So when we remember events or episodic memories, these are memories that are glued to a place and time there's brain area called the hippocampus and what we know is the hippocampus if you if it has any kind of a filing scheme, it files things according to time and place. So that's kind of why for instance, if you hear a song, and this is a song that
Starting point is 00:21:44 you haven't listened to since you're a teenager. Well, now what happens is it brings you back to that time because it's a cue that pulls up memories from that time period. And so that time period now has all sorts of memories associated with it and might bring you back to, you know, sneaking beers with your friends in high school, or might bring you back to days where you went out to the beach or whatever it was, right? And that experience is something we would call mental time travel, where a memory can bring you back to a past context, a time and a place. And these memories might seem like they're forgotten, but they can be pulled up if you're in the right context. And conversely, if you're in the wrong context, it's really bad.
Starting point is 00:22:31 So for instance, just as a simple example, you walk into the kitchen and you can't remember where you are or why you went to the kitchen. Sorry. You walk into the kitchen, you can't remember why you went there. Then you walk back to whatever room you went from. And now it all of a sudden pops in your head. And the reason is, is that my memory, uh, for what I wanted to do was in say this room, which is my home office, but then when I get to the kitchen, I'm in a different context, my mind is switched over to a different place.
Starting point is 00:23:03 And so these boundaries between the rooms act as shifts in our context that make it harder to remember things like that. So that's the last one. And all of these factors will determine life or death, you know, in terms of memories. It seems to me, I've had it in my head as you've been explaining so far, that memory feels a little bit like the breath, the breath being one of the few windows that we have into controlling the autonomic nervous system that you actually do it unconsciously, but can step in consciously and make some amendments to it too.
Starting point is 00:23:39 You can kind of tinker a little bit. Is that a fair analogy in this regard that there is a, there is memory is going on, we don't necessarily get to choose. And then we also have this degree of conscious control over how much stuff gets, um, put into memory at the time, how much we pull it back out, et cetera. Absolutely. In fact, I would go farther and say we both have a lot of under the hood processes of memory. We have, but we also have a ton of control with memory, although we often don't use it, or at least we often don't use it properly.
Starting point is 00:24:17 So what I mean is on the one hand, you have these automatic processes that happen under the hood, like the brain is constantly tuning itself up to learn from experience so that you can process things faster and more efficiently. So you're trying to learn Chinese, let's say. Initially, these characters will be very hard to read, but if you keep reading the same characters over and over, your brain will start to tune up so that you can process that information faster. A lot of that happens without or even really feeling the changes that are happening in
Starting point is 00:24:51 the brain. It's just happening. Now, on the bad side, you hear fake news and you hear it from 10 different sources and all of a sudden it starts to feel more believable. The reason is our brain has tuned itself up to process those messages. And in the process, it seems easier to think about. And if it seems easier to think about, we often believe it's true. Right?
Starting point is 00:25:15 So that's an example of some of these automatic influences of memory that I talked about in the book. Um, but there's also gobs of control we have. So for instance, there's research showing that when we remember an event, we have a lot of control over the narrative that we use to put together that effect. Right. So there's great stories about people where they have fans of two different sports teams, they watch the same football match.
Starting point is 00:25:44 Then later on you ask them for their memories of it. And their memories are just completely different from each other because they're watching it from the perspective of the team that they're liking, right? Um, and there's other research showing that you can change your perspective. You can look at the same event from a different point of view. And all of a sudden remember things that you didn't remember before. And so we have control over that narrative to some degree. And this is really important when you talk about things like trauma, right?
Starting point is 00:26:15 I used to work with, uh, veterans who had PTSD and one of the big challenges that they face was they had all the shame about their trauma and they never talked about it with anyone. And you could see this massive transformation in these patients when they would get hooked in with a group and they're talking to other people who experienced similar traumas and they share their trauma and all of a sudden they realize, well, they're not alone. There are other people who face similar things and they're getting support from these people from a completely different perspective. They're getting support from people who don't blame them,
Starting point is 00:26:57 who aren't telling them to feel guilty. They're telling them that they're cowards. And all of a sudden they look at the same experience they had from a completely different perspective. And now what happens is you're modifying and updating that memory. So we have a lot of agency over the way we use memory. And I think that is important because often people feel like their memories are, you know, a literal set in stone record of what happened. And in fact, it's like the sands are always shifting.
Starting point is 00:27:29 There's always change that's happening. Um, sometimes when we do it without knowing it, and we just make a lot of mistakes because errors start to accumulate in our memories, but, uh, sometimes we can do it properly and sometimes we can use that to our advantage. Before we continue, if your workouts feel flat, your recovery is slow or you've just been feeling off, it might not be your training plan or your diet. It might be something a bit more boring like zinc.
Starting point is 00:27:53 And while most supplements like Tonkat Ali can help, zinc quietly plays a huge role in testosterone production, strength, recovery and energy. And most people are chronically low on it, which is why I'm such a huge fan of momentous is zinc because it supports testosterone boost vitality and it helps keep everything running like it should. Plus it is NSF certified for sport,
Starting point is 00:28:13 which means it's been independently tested and approved for purity, safety and zero shady ingredients, just clean, effective science backed goodness. And if you're still unsure, momentous offers a 30 day money back guarantee. So you can try it for 29 days. If you don't like it, I'll give you a money back. Plus they ship internationally.
Starting point is 00:28:29 Right now you can get 35% off your first subscription and that 30 day money back guarantee by going to the link in the description below or heading to livemomentous.com slash modern wisdom using the code modern wisdom. A checkout that's L I V E M O M E N T O U S dot com slash modern wisdom and modern wisdom a checkout. Why's liveemomentous.com slash modern wisdom and modern wisdom, a checkout. Why do we forget things? Is it just a real estate demand problem inside of our brain? There are two schools of thought, both of which are probably right. One is that you forget because a memory just disappears from the brain that these connections that you have between neurons that allow the memory to be pulled up. Those connections start to become,
Starting point is 00:29:13 start to decay and become wiped out and then you lose access to the memory because it's just gone. Another school of thought is that you can't find the memory you're looking for, but if you had the right cue, you would be able to pull it up, right? So you're trying to remember the name of the guy who was in that thing and you can't pull it up. And then an hour later, it just pops into your head, but it's too late, your conversation is over, right?
Starting point is 00:29:38 So in one case, you were in the wrong context, you couldn't pull it up. But now in a different context, you can pull it up very easily. So we have, it's absolutely unequivocal that we have access to more memory. We have more memories that we could pull up than we can, than what we can actually pull up at any given time. So some of forgetting is just not being able to find. There is some evidence
Starting point is 00:30:06 though to suggest that not everything's completely capped and you know I fully believe that too based on the biology of it. So it's a little of both but I think we don't give ourselves enough of an opportunity to find those memories sometimes when they are there. How can we make ourselves forget? Is that possible if there's something where we think, I don't want to keep that in my mind anymore. I've been able to dispense with my 16 digit pin across the front of my card that I've been desperately trying to remember for the last three years. Is there such a thing as making ourselves forget? There is. It's controversial as to how effective it is. Uh, but a good friend of mine, Mike Anderson has done a lot of research
Starting point is 00:30:51 on what he calls voluntary forgetting, which is basically you cue people to recall something and then you tell the person, just suppress it. Don't think about it. And what you find is, is that the better people get at this, the more likely it is that they can voluntarily forget something. Now these effects aren't gigantic and it's not clear. Is this something that will help you? Probably it would be something that would help you forget, you know, um, a temporary
Starting point is 00:31:24 password or something like that. I would be surprised if it doesn would help you forget, you know, a temporary password or something like that. I would be surprised if it doesn't help you with that. You know, maybe a traumatic experience, unlikely, but who knows? If you do it enough, maybe it would lead you to forget it. If nothing else, I think it would make it harder to retrieve those memories. I get the sense, and I certainly know this for myself myself that, you know, if I look back across a trip that I've taken or a time, an evening that I've spent with friends or an episode or a book that I've read or whatever, and I don't feel like I've recalled enough.
Starting point is 00:31:58 There's this sort of odd sense of guilt. I think, well, what was the point of doing it? What was the point of, what was the point of doing it? What was the point of going through all of this if it was just the passage of time and I've got no memory dividend that my future self is able to tap into so that my remembering self can actually sort of extract it. Is there an issue with this attempt to try and memorize more, this sort of a never ending overdraft of wishing that you could recall more of the things that you did? Yes and no.
Starting point is 00:32:28 I mean, what you're talking about is, I think, one of those kind of points where you want to be able to remember the things that are important. So I always say remember better, not more. Try to remember the things that matter. So if you're with your family or close friends or a partner and you go on a holiday, those are the things that we typically like to remember. I mean, to some extent, a holiday is an investment in memories, right? On the other hand, there's a lot that we forget that you can look and ask yourself, you know, 10 years later, will my future self care that I
Starting point is 00:33:00 forgot this? No, okay. So not that important. And I think that if you go back to the examples of the things that you are remembering with it, maybe you just need the right remind. Maybe you need to mentally go back there because sometimes what happens is when you take a holiday, you're in such a different state of mind, you're in a different place, then you come back home and you lose that part of yourself because you're a different person when you're in a different context, so to speak.
Starting point is 00:33:32 So I think about this where Hawaii is my happy place. When I'm in Hawaii, I can get into a mode that I can't get in my daily experience at work. So sometimes it's a matter of you go back there and you are in the humidity and you're smelling these tropical flowers, birds are going and boom, you remember all these things that you couldn't remember before.
Starting point is 00:33:57 And so maybe that's the thing that you wanna be able to do is try to call back some of those memories either with photos or with you know just at the end of the day while you're on these holidays. If you just take a moment and reflect on trying to just remember one positive thing from the day right what you'll find is is that one positive thing will come to mind and then that'll make another positive thing come to mind. And all of a sudden you've recalled all of these experiences that you had. And the act of recalling those experiences will make it easier to bring those experiences to mind later on
Starting point is 00:34:35 so that you can incorporate it into your remembering self in a more rich way. What are the fundamentals of training memory? You know, we've spoken about kind of how the environment cues our brain to tell us the things that we want to remember. This is how you breathe and why you breathe when you're not thinking about breathing. But if I was to start thinking about breathing and I wanted to breathe better, how should people come to think about training their memory? What should they do if they want to remember an experience while it's happening? What are the fundamentals of that? Okay, so there's different things. So a lot of people when they talk about memory training is like memory athletes or something where they're trying to memorize like the thousandth digit of pie. And that stuff is not going to be particularly helpful in remembering this moment that you want to
Starting point is 00:35:25 hold in mind as pristinely as possible. Right. Um, what I would say is the biggest thing that helps with remembering in a way that will keep, get you back into that moment is the sensory details, whatever they are. Now, some people don't have a great ability to remember the sensory details, whatever they are. Now, some people don't have a great ability to remember the sensory details in certain ways. There's people who have what's called aphantasia,
Starting point is 00:35:53 who can't visually image things, for instance. But to the extent that you can immerse yourself in whatever makes this moment unique, that will make it more memorable to you because it's going to be distinctive. The D in medic that sticks out, it'll give you a context that's very unique and specific, the C in medic. And so basically we can do that in part by controlling,
Starting point is 00:36:19 managing our, you know, basically just keeping ourselves from sabotaging ourselves for the most part. So, you know, there's just certain things that are just memory blockers like stress, fatigue, illness, depression, multitasking. Multitasking is probably the major malady of the modern age, right? If you want to remember this moment, don't keep looking at your phone. Don't turn off, put your watch in do not disturb mode. Because the more tempted you are to do these things, even thinking about it can be enough to sap your attention and get you out of that moment that you won't remember later on.
Starting point is 00:37:01 Right. Okay. Avoiding doing the things that get in the way of you recalling the things is a good place to start. Yeah. Another thing is just setting the intention of what you want to take away in the first place. So, and this is, I think a great analogy to the breath, right? Because I think a lot of people think that it should just be natural, should be, Because I think a lot of people think that it should just be natural, should be, we just get everything for free. And that's not really how it works, right? So if you know memories, your memory is going to be incomplete. Ask yourself what you want to take away from this experience in the first place. Because it's a lot easier to focus on the information that you want to take away in the first place
Starting point is 00:37:46 than it is to try to pull it up later on if you did not focus on that information. Now I know that sounds a little like obvious in some ways, but how often do we really ask ourselves what's the memory I want to take away from this experience? I would bet you almost never. I don't do it nearly as much as I should. Although I try to be kinder to my remembering self now than I used to before I wrote my book. Mmm Yeah, that's interesting. Okay, so error driven learning some people kind of hinted at earlier on yeah, explain that to me Well, this is kind of a counterintuitive concept, but basically when our brains form memories, they're a bit incomplete.
Starting point is 00:38:31 And like I said, they're very tied to a context. And so what can be a problem with that is when you're trying to remember certain things, like let's say you're trying to remember, let's say you're trying to learn a new language, right? And you're trying to remember the words that you learned and use it in a conversation when you go on holiday. Well, if you're trying to do that, you want to be able to pull it up in a whole lot of different contexts. So one of the things we've found in our computer models of memory is that when
Starting point is 00:39:02 people try to pull up these memories, the brain won't really do a perfect job of reconstructing what you pulled up. But then if you get the right answer and you have it in front of you, now the brain can tweak the memory, you can update it in a way so that it's going to be better at pulling up that information later on. So imagine, uh, maybe I'll give you another example. Like I'm trying to memorize, uh, your name, right? So now I, uh, leave, I'm done with this podcast and I try to visualize you and pull up your name.
Starting point is 00:39:40 Then I actually look at the video of our conversation and then, oh, yeah, that's Chris. And I remember his face and everything, right? Now my brain has tweaked my memory so that I've got a more accurate memory, a more distinctive memory of who you are later on. But it's also from a different, it's now been associated with a different context. And so the more places and times that we bring up this memory, the more resilient it will be and the less tied it will be to one unique cue that we would have, right? Does that make sense? Yeah. Yeah, yeah, yeah. But on the other hand, if we don't give ourselves a chance to pull up the memory
Starting point is 00:40:21 in the first place, like if we just try to, I tell myself your name over and over and over again, that's not going to be very helpful because I haven't really given my brain a chance to pull up the crummy memory that it has. I'm just kind of like trying to read this word over and over and over again, but I always have the right answer. So I'm not relying on that in the first place. So you know, you imagine you're trying to learn how to play basketball. You're obviously bet your best bet is to actually shoot baskets, not to think about it, right. And, uh, like, and you're not going to get them all, you know, not going to
Starting point is 00:40:59 get them all the time, but it's through that struggle that your brain can tune up and say, I had this model of what I was supposed to do and it's through that struggle that your brain can tune up and say, I had this model of what I was supposed to do and it's wrong and so I need to fix it. And that's what we call error-driven learning. And we can see this, this is actually, if you look at AI models like chat, GPT, and so forth, they work because of this principle of error-driven learning where the model tries to predict, let's say the next word that's going to come up. And if it gets it wrong, then it tweaks just the connections in the model
Starting point is 00:41:31 that are needed so that you can pull up that word later on again. Right. So it's learning, not trying to memorize everything, but rather learning what it needs to do to correct the mistake. And that's where error- driven learning really is key. Does that suggest that we learn more from pain and difficult life circumstances than enjoyable ones? Do we have the negativity bias present in our memory as well as in our sort of noticing our reticular activating system?
Starting point is 00:42:01 Well, what I'll say is that the pain that I'm talking about is the pain of struggling to remember something. And so if we adopt a strategy of saying that essentially memorizing should be hard, learning should be hard, as opposed to learning should be easy and I should be getting easy A's all the time, then it's not so painful. It's just part of the process. Just like you would be if you're trying to learn how to play tennis, you just do it because you like it. Right? And so the more we can kind of get people from thinking about memory as kind of a shortcoming to thinking about learning as a journey of curiosity,
Starting point is 00:42:41 then it's no longer painful. It's kind of the whole process of learning, right? So that's separate from the point that you brought up about the negativity bias, but maybe we could, I'll let you react to that. No, no, no, you're on the money, keep going. Okay, so separate from this is this negativity bias. And this is another thing I think that people
Starting point is 00:42:59 don't appreciate is that our memories for the past are incredibly biased by what's going on around us in the present. So what I mean by that is if I'm feeling bad, I will have a bias to pull up negative memories. And whatever memories that pull up, I will reconstruct them in kind of a negative way. Now, if I have positive, if I'm in a positive frame of mind, I will
Starting point is 00:43:27 manifest that opposite bias where I'll be able to pull up memories that are more positive and reconstruct them in a more positive way. Um, so you tend to see both kinds of biases. In fact, oddly enough, as people get, you know, really older, uh, they tend to have more of a positive bias in their memory for whatever reason. This is of course just on average, but everybody has ups and downs, right? And that's going to affect our memories. And just as an example of this, let's say I had an ex-girlfriend who was dating and she dumped me for a couple of months.
Starting point is 00:44:03 I'm remembering all of these terrible things, either things that I could have done, that I did that might have led to the breakup or things that she did that made me think that she was such a terrible person. Then later on, I meet the woman of my dreams who I ended up marrying, and it was just like six months later. Now I can look back at my memories from that first relationship in a completely different perspective where it's neither good nor bad.
Starting point is 00:44:31 It's just like, if anything, I dodged a bullet and now I can look back at the positive experiences of what I got out of that relationship as well as the negative experiences. And so that's what I mean is that our present beliefs, as well as our emotional states affect how we reconstruct things. This episode is brought to you by AG1. I've been drinking AG1 every morning for years and it just got better. AG1 NextGen keeps the same simplicity, one scoop, one today, but now comes
Starting point is 00:45:02 with four clinical trials backing it. In those trials, AG1 Next Gen was clinically shown to fill common nutrient gaps, improve nutrient levels within three months, and increase healthy gut bacteria by 10 times even in healthy adults. Basically, they've upgraded the formula with better probiotics, more bioavailable nutrients, and clinical validation, and it's still NSF certified for sport, so you know that the quality is legit. They genuinely care about holistic health, which is why I've got my mom to take it,
Starting point is 00:45:30 my dad to take it, and tons of my friends too, and it's why I put it inside of my body every single day. And if I found something better, I would switch, but I haven't, which is why I still use it. Best of all, there is a 90-day money back guarantee, so you can buy it and try it every single day for three months, and if you don't like it, they'll give you your money back for all of it. Right now you can get a year's free supply of vitamin D3 K2 and five free AG1 travel packs by going
Starting point is 00:45:52 to the link in the description below or heading to www.drinkag1.com slash modern wisdom. That's www.drinkag1.com slash modern wisdom. Oh, that's fascinating. So this must create a kind of recursive loop for people whose lives are going well or lives are going badly, for people who are depressed or people who are happy, because your current state creates a context and that context predisposes your brain to viewing and interpreting and recalling and focusing on past experiences that match with where you are right now as an experiencing self. So sad people think about more sad things and happy people think about more happy things.
Starting point is 00:46:36 Is that a fair assessment? Yeah. And this is why depression is such a vicious cycle because when you're depressed, you tend to ruminate. And rumination is almost by definition, regurgitating memories that are negative, that will reinforce your feelings at the moment. Right?
Starting point is 00:46:55 So now you recall more negative memories. What happens? You feel worse. The worse you feel, the easier it is to recall negative memories, but even more importantly, the worse you feel, the harder it is to recall negative memories. But even more importantly, the worse you feel, the harder it is to pull up memories that counteract your view of the world at a given time. A big, big part of cognitive therapy that we would do is get people to overcome those biases by actually pulling up memories that contradicted their sense of the world at a given
Starting point is 00:47:26 moment, because it's so easy to fall into this trap of, you know, having a negative state of mind and then reconstructing the past in the worst way possible, and then using that to confirm your beliefs about what's happening in the moment. Right. And so, you know, we do have the capability to pull ourselves out of it, at least not necessarily in cases of clinical depression, but I think, you know,
Starting point is 00:47:52 when you're having a bad day, if you're not clinically depressed, it's a lot easier to pull yourself out of it by pushing yourself to remember just one positive thing, as minor as it could be at the end of your day. And I've done, I don't have the discipline to do things consistently, but when I do this, it takes me a while and I'll pull up something dumb like, Oh, that, you know, uh, I made a good sandwich at lunch or something like that. I like that sandwich. And all of a sudden I get access to more stuff.
Starting point is 00:48:26 It changes the way I feel and the change in feeling makes it easier to pull up other memories that make me think maybe today wasn't such a bad day after all. So funny, man. I've been spending a lot of time this year really trying to get into neurobiology a lot more, Rick Hansen, if you're familiar with Rick from Hardwiring Happiness, I think he's just so great. You know, he's got this unique intersection of Dharma, come Buddhist mindfulness stuff, and then, you know, the hardcore science behind
Starting point is 00:48:54 the neuroscience, the things sort of your work as well. Anna, the more that I realize we have this fascinating window into the structure of our own brain. And you do have some conscious control over it, even though you can't predict the next thought that's going to come careening into view. You do have some conscious control over it. But unlike going to the gym, you can't see if you're actually doing it. You can't really fully tell whether or not you're doing gratitude right. Did I do 10 reps of gratitude there? You can't say that with the same level of certainty that you know if you did 10 repetitions of a bicep curl.
Starting point is 00:49:36 I mean, you don't know, is this actually making me better? You get a pump, you get a preview in the gym. There's no brain pump. All you do is just have a degree of uncertainty about whether or not that thing was the way that you were supposed to do it. And I think it was, and I'll just keep on doing it. But it is really interesting that we do have some control, not entire control, but we do have some control and this control gets tuned up over time. Just on your point about choosing the good sandwich, Oliver Berkman has the idea of a
Starting point is 00:50:06 done list, which is the opposite of a to-do list as he goes through the day and he crosses things off like clean shoes and walked dog and stuff. He likes to do that. And I adapted it to a well done list, which is basically what you're talking about. So toward the end of the day or as stuff goes on throughout the day, I think, oh, fuck, like that was a really good walk. I had a really good walk earlier on today and the sun was shining and I got to see a dog. That was nice. The dog was pretty cool.
Starting point is 00:50:31 And, you know, I was listening to this song and that song makes me feel happy. And that was good. And it's, you know, it's little things. But yeah, I just, I'm kind of fascinated about what you were saying there, this sort of interjection, maybe for people with low mood or a catalyst for people It's little things, but yeah, I just, I'm kind of fascinated about what you were saying there, this sort of interjection maybe for people with low mood or a catalyst for people that are in good mood, there's two worlds colliding here and one of them is top down, right, which is reframing
Starting point is 00:50:56 things as they're happening. Do I want to really be present as present as possible, allowing this to sink in really, and then a little space repetition, which I'm sure we'll get onto. Uh, but then there's also the bottom up, which is, well, go and find good experiences, you know, go and spend time doing things that are worthy of memory. Um, and it, yeah, for people that are in, uh, good moods, they want to maintain or bad moods they want to get out of. It feels like those, those two things need to work simpatico together.
Starting point is 00:51:26 It's, it's not enough to just think your way out of a living problem or think your way out of a feeling problem. But you also are missing out on some of the gains if you never think about your good experiencing experiences. Oh, absolutely. Yeah. I mean, you want memory to be kind of your copilot, but you don't want it to be in the driver's seat, right?
Starting point is 00:51:48 What do you mean? When you say that? Well, what I mean is, is that you want to actually experience things that you want to, um, ideally have them work hand in hand, ask yourself, okay, what, what do I want to remember at the end of the day? Right. Well, it's the memory that I want to carry to remember at the end of the day? Right. What's the memory that I want to carry with me at the end of the week? And often that will help us avoid getting into traps that are things that we often find ourselves doing that are not particularly things we will want to remember later on.
Starting point is 00:52:20 Like, um, I have checked email way too many times today. I went on Facebook more than I would have liked today already and those are things that are not the stuff that I want to remember later on. Those are the things that, you know, except for the work stuff that I have to deal with, for the most part, that's wasted time in the sense that it generated no memories that I will want to carry with me, right?
Starting point is 00:52:48 So I guess investing in the right experiences will give you the right memories. I don't know if that relates to where you were going. No, for sure. I, there's certainly an issue of being taken out of the moment, trying to bank it, to remember it whilst also trying to bank it, to remember it whilst also trying to experience it. You know, Oliver has this beautiful, uh, really interesting blog post where
Starting point is 00:53:13 he talks about one morning he lives in New Yorkshire Dales in the UK countryside. And, uh, he talks about one morning, beautiful snow, the sun's risen and the birds are chirping and he is experiencing this thing and loving it. As he's experiencing it, he's thinking, you really should remember this. Like this is really the sort of experience that you should be remembering more of. And in the act of trying to enjoy something, he's whipping himself about not remembering it in future, which causes him to not even enjoy it in the moment. And I think lots of people have that, this is so beautiful.
Starting point is 00:53:49 I better hold onto this. I better be able to remember this in future. If I can't remember this, I'll feel guilty. It's like, dude, you're making yourself feel guilty for not remembering a thing that hasn't fucking finished yet. Like it hasn't even happened. Yeah, I do. I actually do the opposite for that very reason.
Starting point is 00:54:03 I actually will, if I say, I want to remember this later on, I say, what do I want to immerse myself in right now that I really want to take away from this? Maybe I'll focus on the sky and what it looks like right now, or the smells or the sounds of this moment. Uh, maybe I'll just kind of take a moment or really check in with my
Starting point is 00:54:29 feelings maybe and just kind of get a sense of like, well, this is how I felt at this moment. And so, yeah, I mean, I think it's perfectly natural to be at moments where you're like, this is so good, I have to hold onto it. But rather than kicking yourself, you can be proactive and indulge yourself in the very things that you want to remember even more. You mentioned that about emotions. Yeah. You mentioned that about emotions.
Starting point is 00:54:56 What's, what's the relationship between emotions and memory? Well, so we already talked about the idea that emotions can be a context, right? That emotions can bias the things that we remember and the way that we remember them. But emotions are also associated with these motivational circuits in our brain, right? So dopamine is a good example. Dopamine helps us. It's not about, it's not rewarding per se, but it helps us learn about rewards, learn where they are and how to get them. Right. So, where that comes into play is when we're in a new place, you will tend to
Starting point is 00:55:39 have more dopamine activity when you're encountering something that is surprising, you know, somebody comes in out of the blue that you haven't met before. Well, that will be associated with more dopamine release. When you're curious, when you're in a state of curiosity, when it's like, you go, I thought I read this in his book, but he's telling me something completely different. Now you go back to the book. We've shown that you get activity throughout the dopaminergic circuits of the brain. Those moments tend to be memorable in part because they're producing plasticity. But there is a kind of a catch to this, which is that emotions will often... But there is a kind of a catch to this, which is that emotions will often, so these emotionally intense moments where you get these chemicals released in your brain, they don't turn up the
Starting point is 00:56:33 volume in memory or the brightness in memory so much as they turn up the contrast. So they help you remember certain things more than others. And they give us a sense of vividness, but not necessarily that context. So in other words, when we're emotionally having an emotionally intense experience, we will remember that experience better, but we won't remember all of the aspects of that experience better, right? So you have a traumatic experience. You'll remember the things that were especially traumatic about it, as opposed to the color of the carpet or, you know, the rabbit that was in the background when this happened. That's interesting.
Starting point is 00:57:19 What, uh, let's say that it's something good. Let's say that it's something good. What are the areas that an emotionally strong memory is going to focus on? Does it focus on more good things or is it just negativity bias all the way down? No, no. So let's say if it's a good experience that you have, it's, so what's kind of interesting is, is a lot of the work has focused on information that's the experiences that are arousing. So say you go snowboarding or something and you like snowboarding or you like surfing and you go surfing.
Starting point is 00:57:53 Those are good experiences that are pleasant experiences often that are arousing. And what happens again is you'll remember the most significant parts of those experiences, the most attention graphic parts, maybe you're surfing and it's that particular wave that you caught that was the most exhilarating part of your set, as opposed to the other things. Now, when you have positive experiences that are kind of calming, we don't know as much about them. So for instance, like you come back home and you hug your partner or you kind of like
Starting point is 00:58:29 your dog starts licking your hand or something like that. Not necessarily arousing, maybe, but maybe if it's relaxing, it's not going to be the same chemicals released in the brain that are gonna promote these memories. And we don't know as much about whether there is an advantage for those calming experiences. It may be that the sort of more hard coded biological responses that give you these emotional memory enhancements tend to be for more arousing experiences.
Starting point is 00:59:03 What's the difference between remembering and imagining? Not much. So if you actually scan people's brains while they're, they are imagining something, you will find that those brain scans are for the most part indistinguishable, um, from, uh, a brain scan of somebody actually, let's say, watching a movie or listening to a story. And those brain scans are fairly indistinguishable from somebody remembering the movie or remembering the story or remembering an experience that they actually have. We are most likely imagining how the past could have been, imagining what's happening in the
Starting point is 00:59:46 present, and using that same core system to imagine what could happen in the future. The differences are that we do have this episodic memory system that allows us to ground our experiences in a place in a time. The other thing is that our imagination typically is not as vivid as reality. For most of us, when we imagine something, it's not going to be associated with as intense a smell on the site and so forth. It's going to be more focused on the things that we thought about and the emotions we had. And so that's where we have this ability to tell the difference
Starting point is 01:00:28 between imagination and memory. Um, but you have to take the time that you have to engage this area of the brain called the prefrontal cortex, uh, and actually take pause and use. This kind of more, uh, reasoning approach to be able to say, this is something that I didn't actually experience. I just thought about it. I thought about responding to that email, but I didn't really do it. And this happens to me all the time.
Starting point is 01:00:59 Most of these problems that I write about in the book are my daily experiences. Is it possible to learn something if we don't remember it happening? You know, you go through, you talked about LeBron James, knowing games. If he can't remember shooting that particular three-pointer, how does that particular three-pointer get added to the corpus of experience that he can draw on to become a better basketball player? I can't remember this very move from when I was 12, but it has contributed to his capacity. Is there different, is there embodied learning?
Starting point is 01:01:35 Is this such a thing? How does this work? Yeah, yeah. So basically, the kind of memory that we've been largely focused on is the kind of memory I study, which is episodic memory, that ability to travel to a particular time and place. But the brain has all sorts of capabilities for learning that are not episodic memory based. So you can have somebody even who has a pretty significant memory disorder. Say if you got into a car accident and you had some, uh, uh, you know, damaged your hippocampus and you had amnesia, you could still potentially learn how to play the piano or learn how to become a better basketball player or, uh, learn
Starting point is 01:02:15 all of these skills because there's learning that anytime you have a bunch of neurons connected to each other, to each other there's that capability for plasticity and reforming those connections and the brain's constantly doing that it's constantly changing its structure based on our experiences so that principle of error driven learning happens we know in the motor system for instance and even if it's not tied to any particular context if i'm shooting that basketball my, there's a new brain area called the cerebellum that generates an internal model of what I just did and a prediction of where that ball is going to go into. And the ball doesn't go into the right place. Or if it doesn't feel right as I'm shooting the ball, my brain will tweak the memory so that I will be better at.
Starting point is 01:03:06 In fact, actually just saw a talk yesterday suggesting that sleep is a big part of that whole dynamic. That during REM sleep, when we're dreaming, for instance, what may be going on is at least one of the things that goes on is that the motor system is really tuning itself up so that you get these better movements and better hand-eye coordination based on the skills you've tried to learn early in the day. In fact, that seems to be why dogs, for instance, like move their legs at night when they're asleep. It's not necessarily that they're having some conscious dream as much as necessarily that they're having subconscious dream as much as their brain is like basically just tuning up the motor system. And in fact, those movements come from a very primitive area of the brain is the trance
Starting point is 01:03:53 side. And your dog's going to wake up tomorrow and be 2% faster because it's practice at during its sleep. You need to be raised to that. Possibly, yeah. Yeah. Yeah. Give me the, so it seems to me like novel experiences are pretty reliably placed into memory. This is new.
Starting point is 01:04:12 This is something that's different and intense experiences as well. This is, you know, this is really, again, which I guess intensity is kind of like volume novelty. And what we think of as typical novelty is categorical newness, I suppose. Can you say the difference between the two kinds of novelty again? Sorry, I missed that. Well, if intensity is something I may have done before, but in a manner that I haven't experienced it previously, which I suppose is a kind of novelty, but I think when people think about novelty, they think about something that's categorically new.
Starting point is 01:04:52 I haven't been to this particular holiday destination before. I haven't skied on this particular mountain before, let's say. So let's use skiing as an example. So one might be, I've been down this mountain a hundred times. I know the route, but this one time there's an avalanche behind me. Okay. So I've been there previously, but the level of intensity or this time I go faster or this time I do a different, I do a different trick.
Starting point is 01:05:18 Yeah. It's a kind of novelty, but I think experientially for most people, we need to sort of work within the confines that most people's lives are rather routine. You know, you can say, hey, maybe if you want to make your day go a little bit slower, take a different route to work. And you go, yeah, but there's only five routes I can take to work. You know what I mean? So, and the same thing goes for skiing down a mountain and so on and so forth.
Starting point is 01:05:41 So yeah, novelty and intensity just seem to be sort of two levers that, common threads that have come through today. Well, you know, so it's a great point. I think that novelty doesn't have to be something that is new per se. So the brain's constantly trying to generate predictions about what's going to happen. And this is just based on the way the brain's wired it. I mean, I will bore people with the details of thalamocortical interactions, but the basic gist of it is that when you're in a completely new place, your brain just has no predictions it can make, right? Or it's making very weak predictions based on similar places you've been. But let's say you take the example of you're going down the mountain and you're skiing,
Starting point is 01:06:29 and you've been down this mountain a hundred times. There are still potential prediction errors there that you can make that can drive learning in the sense that you can focus your attention on the minutiae. Because I mean, the world is always changing, our brains are always changing and everything changes. But often we just don't notice it. If you take meditative practices like mindfulness, a big part of these practices is attuning
Starting point is 01:07:01 yourself to what's new and that creates these prediction errors in your brain. It stimulates curiosity and will give you better memories. It creates more distinctive memories. So I guarantee you that almost anything you do can be associated with some novelty, but you have to be curious and look for it and not assume, not get so caught up in your predictions about the world that you just assume that your predictions are right. What's the relationship between memory and sort of subjective passage of time, the sense that we have of how quickly or how slowly time is going or time did go in the past?
Starting point is 01:07:45 Yeah, there is a relationship. People argue about the relationship. But one thing that we've noticed is that shifts in our context affect our memories. And that, especially if you look on a long enough time scale, dramatically affects our ability to tell the passage of time. So during the pandemic, people would sit around their computers all day, like all the students who were in my classes. And what I would ask them, one time I just had the idea to just ask them in class just to keep their attention, is do you feel like the days
Starting point is 01:08:22 are going faster or slower since the lockdowns happened than before? And so I'll ask you, you know, maybe if you were in another place that was locked down, did the days go by faster or slower for you? Yeah, I think they, I think they probably went by pretty quickly. Okay. So this is interesting, but let's, let's go back to it. So during the like, so basically if I said just the last 24 hours, did they go by faster or slower, overwhelming majority of people said that they went
Starting point is 01:08:53 by slower on, I think only two out of 120 people said it was going by faster. Most people said their days were going by slower, but then speaking to your point. And I said, okay, how about the weeks you get to the end of the week? Are they going by faster or slower and everybody said that the weeks were going by faster? So what's going on this no law of physics can explain that right? It's like How does time move more slowly in a day but disappear in a week? And the reason that's due with memory. You're staying in the same context and so you just have these memories that aren't very different from each other. And so as a result, it feels like the day has just been going really
Starting point is 01:09:39 slowly because you haven't been accumulating all these memories that would give you a sense of time passing, but then you reach the end of the week and you can't remember anything because you had all of these blurry memories that interfered with each other so much that you look back and it just feels like the time went by and disappeared because you have effectively lost that time because you can't remember it, right? effectively lost that time because you can't remember. Right. And so it, memories can really warp our sense of time, uh, uh, as a result.
Starting point is 01:10:15 Uh, but the opposite is also true that if you don't have memories, you will be floating in time and space. That is if you ask people with memory disorders, what date is it? They won't be able to tell you because their last memory of a time and a date was the time that they had, you know, a good memory. But, you know, they'll be, if this, they had a stroke or a cardiac arrest that gave them brain damage in, you know, 1997, they will, every day is 1997 to them. And they look at the mirror and they're like, what's happening? I don't, I don't understand this.
Starting point is 01:10:51 What else, what is it that you, you wish more people knew about memory? If there was something that, uh, is regularly complained to you about how the human memory system works or about some misunderstanding, some common myth that people have around it. What is it that you wish that you could dispel? I think that I would, I mean, there's so many, we've talked about a number of them already, but I, probably the biggest one is that memory should be free and easy. And that's just not true. I mean, all memory research shows that it's hard and it's, it's not, it's not easy and it's not free for sure. And so a bit of intention goes a long way in helping you remember what you need. As opposed to trying to remember everything and expecting it should be there.
Starting point is 01:11:45 So that's a really big one. I guess the other one is that memory is supposed to be about the past, and it's really about the present and the future. Why? Well, let's just take the present, for instance, right? For you just to keep up with this conversation, you're constantly referring back in memory to things that I previously said.
Starting point is 01:12:09 If you did not have memory, you'd just be repeating the same question over and over and over again, right? And so, and you see this in people with memory disorders, they will repeat themselves every 10 minutes because they don't remember it's one of the biggest signs, if you have a relative who's entering dementia, if they say the themselves every 10 minutes because they don't remember it. It's one of the biggest signs. If you have a relative who's entering dementia, if they say the same thing over and over again and don't remember that they've repeated themselves,
Starting point is 01:12:34 that's a big sign that they have a memory problem. So that's just one example, but you know, others are just your ability to recall memories and use it to make sense of what's going on right now. Well, I've been here before and therefore here's what I can expect is going to happen now. I've done this podcast a number of times and I know I can do this and I'm going to do a great job of interviewing because I've interviewed a lot of people before, right? And that's also your expectations for the future and your
Starting point is 01:13:07 predictions about the future. And so, your ability to remember to, you know, you want to be able to buy groceries when you get to the store, you're using memory to project into the future what you will be buying from the store, right? And then later on, when you get to the store, you have to rely on memory to buy those things. So, I mean, there's, I can give you a billion examples. I mean, these are just ones that just popped up off my head, but there's no
Starting point is 01:13:35 shortage of examples in terms of how memories about the present and the future. And I mean, if it were just about the past, it would be useless because past is over. There's absolutely, we survived the past. So there's nothing in the past that we need to hold on to except the stuff that matters for the present and the future. Hmm. Yeah. Crazy.
Starting point is 01:13:58 Charan Ranganath, ladies and gentlemen, it's, uh, it's fascinating. The, the, the idea of how human memory works is something that I've been really interested in for a long time. So it's nice that you've got to do a Prima today. Where should people go? They want to check out all of the stuff that you do online. Well, so I have a website called charanranganath.com and people can sign up for a sub stack. I haven't actually started writing them yet, but now that I'm done with teaching for the year, I can, uh, I'll start to use that to create a mailing list free.
Starting point is 01:14:30 I'm not going to sell anything to anyone. I just cause it's not what I do for a living. So, uh, so people could just get more information that way. Uh, you can also look up information about my events. Also have an Instagram that's pretty active. And people can follow me there at the memory doc, the memory DOC. And that's a good one. And a little less active on LinkedIn, but I have LinkedIn and blue sky as well. Off Twitter now because it's a hellscape. That's another conversation. Sure. A talk for another time.
Starting point is 01:15:07 Sharon, I appreciate you. Thank you, mate. All right. Thank you very much. Take care.

There aren't comments yet for this episode. Click on any sentence in the transcript to leave a comment.