No Stupid Questions - 222. What Makes an Idea Interesting?
Episode Date: December 8, 2024What do Karl Marx, Sigmund Freud, and Malcolm Gladwell have in common? Are interesting theories more significant than true ones? And what has been keeping Angela up at night? Plus: an important announ...cement about the show.  SOURCES:Charles Darwin, 19th-century naturalist and biologist.Murray Davis, cultural sociologist.Malcolm Gladwell, journalist and author.Adam Grant, professor of management and psychology at the University of Pennsylvania.B. F. Skinner, 20th-century American psychologist. RESOURCES:Small Fry, by Lisa Brennan-Jobs (2018).Grit: The Power of Passion and Perseverance, by Angela Duckworth (2016)."Why Malcolm Gladwell’s Ideas Are So Interesting, Whether or Not They’re True," by Adam Grant (Quartz, 2015).David and Goliath: Underdogs, Misfits, and the Art of Battling Giants, by Malcolm Gladwell (2013).Curious?: Discover the Missing Ingredient to a Fulfilling Life, by Todd Kashdan (2009)."Interest — The Curious Emotion," by Paul J. Silvia (2008).Exploring the Psychology of Interest, by Paul J. Silvia (2006)."The Ketchup Conundrum," by Malcolm Gladwell (The New Yorker, 2004)."That's Interesting!: Towards a Phenomenology of Sociology and a Sociology of Phenomenology," by Murray S. Davis (Philosophy of the Social Sciences, 1971).The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals, by Charles Darwin (1872). EXTRAS:"How Do You Identify a Narcissist?" by No Stupid Questions (2024)."Is Screen Time as Poisonous as We Think?" by Freakonomics Radio (2024)."Abortion and Crime, Revisited (Update)," by Freakonomics Radio (2024).
Transcript
Discussion (0)
Wait a minute.
I'm Angela Duckworth.
I'm Mike Maughan.
And you're listening to No Stupid Questions.
Today on the show, what makes an idea interesting?
Oh hey, look over here.
What you thought was true is not true. Angela, I am so excited to be with you today.
But first things first, even before we hop into today's conversation, we have an important
announcement to make.
That's right. Mike, after five years and more than 200 episodes, next week will be
the last episode of No Stupid Questions. And I want to say, I have loved doing
this podcast. First, co-hosting with Stephen, now Mike with you. We love our
listeners, we love their questions, but like
all good things, this one must eventually come to an end.
And I've loved doing it with you. And I know we both are so grateful to all of our listeners
for their questions to us over the years, because they've been the catalyst for us to
talk about a lot of interesting ideas on this show, whether it's a piece of research, a philosophical
concept, a think piece from a newspaper or a magazine.
So I thought, Angela, given that this is our second to last episode, that it might be appropriate
to address sort of a meta question.
And I just wanted to talk about why are certain ideas so captivating?
What makes an idea interesting?
What makes an idea interesting versus I guess boring, right?
Yeah. Yeah, I think so, you know
there is an article that I have read and reread and read again and actually I'm not the only dorky scientist who
ritually rereads this article it's by a
Sociologist named Mary Davis and it was published a year after I was born,
1971, and it's called That's Interesting Towards a Phenomenology of Sociology and a Sociology of
Phenomenology. Tell me more. This is this article that I can't even remember where I stumbled across
it because I'm not a sociologist and it's published in this journal called Philosophy of the Social Sciences, which I have never read again. I don't even know if it's still
printed. But I mean, this is like a cult favorite among, you know, anybody who studies human
behavior. And here's the first line of the article. It's long been thought that a theorist
is considered great because his theories are true, but this is false. A theorist is considered great not because his theories are true, but
because they're interesting.
I've heard this before. That's actually quite often quoted not just in scientific circles.
Yeah. Well, you know, Murray Davis was, I think he was the first to at least elaborate this idea.
Like, I think the idea is that if you think about somebody
like, you know, Karl Marx or Sigmund Freud,
I mean, people who have had these very famous theories
of how the world works, how psychology works, et cetera,
they're not remembered and they're not even great
because they were right, but
because they said something that was interesting.
And then he gives you quite literally a formula for all things that are interesting.
And then if you just reverse the formula, you can understand like what is boring also.
Really?
The punchline for Murray Davis is, and I'm quoting, interesting theories are those which deny certain assumptions of their audience,
while non-interesting theories are those which affirm certain assumptions of their audience.
In other words, he thinks that all really interesting propositions have that exact form.
Like, everyone thinks that X is true.
In fact, X is not true. Here's why. Right.
I can give you an example that I don't think he uses, but B.F.
Skinner, the great psychologist who designed these little Skinner boxes for rats, and they
were pressing levers and learning to get the pellet to come out, et cetera.
Like, a lot of people learn about Skinner in intro psych, but a lot of people also learn
that, you know, many things he said were wrong. Like, a lot of people learn about Skinner in intro psych, but a lot of people also learn
that many things he said were wrong, but nobody learns that Skinner was boring.
And the reason why is that Skinner had this really provocative theory.
He was like, oh, most people think that they take action because of their thoughts, right?
Let you have a kind of intention and then you decide to do something,
like turn left or turn right,
or press the lever or don't press the lever.
But he was like, no, there are no thoughts.
That's not true.
You are simply responding to the rewards
and punishments in your environment.
So, even if we don't teach Skinner in intro psych
because he was fully correct in all of his propositions
We have to teach him because it was a theory that up ended
What everybody thought before Skinner came along of course he was up ended too
But like he will remain forever interesting those thing
I think so so well now I'm gonna overuse the word the thing that I think is so interesting about it
Yeah, is interesting more important than true.
I hope that truth is still very, very important.
I mean, it's almost heretical because in science you're supposed to be chasing truth.
But the end of Murray Davis's article, he says,
I contend that the generation of interesting theories ought to be the object of as much attention as the verification of insipid ones.
I don't know how interesting I am.
I will say that I appreciate, though,
the people who go around and look for what we're not looking for,
who go around and think, like, well, what if we're wrong?
Sometimes they're right about our being wrong, sometimes they're not,
but they really move
things forward in a non-incremental way.
And maybe you can hack Mary and say, well, in a conversation where you're talking about
something or just listening and someone says something like, oh, I think so and so is true
or I think we should do this.
I mean, maybe when they say that, you should just flip it on its head and say,
well, what if we're all assuming that this is true?
And in fact, this is false.
Well, truthfully, I think that's where some of the most interesting
books or ideas that we have in the last several decades have come
is because they hit on something that's counterintuitive
and therefore people want to talk about it or argue about it.
Do you think some or all?
Well, I always hesitate.
Now I'm making an always statement.
I was going to say.
I always say sometimes.
I don't want to make a declarative that all things,
but my immediate reaction went back to the book,
Freakonomics, the original Freakonomics book.
I think I was early in my collegiate career
when it came out,
and it introduced this idea that crime reduction was the direct result of the legalization of
abortion. And that was counterintuitive, and it was controversial, and I felt like everyone I knew
was discussing the book, mostly because of that.
Now it had lots of interesting tidbits in it, but that was the thing that I felt got
everybody talking because it was counterintuitive and it was controversial.
Behavioral economics, by the way, is the whole discipline of like everyone in economics thinks
that human beings act like computers, right?
Like they process information and they subtract the cost from the benefits and they figure in the probabilities.
Surprise. That's not always how we make decisions.
Yeah. And then like the provocative, you know, oh, that's not true proposition was like people
are not like computers. They act on emotion and so forth.
Right. Your fellow Wharton professor, Adam Grant, has written an article in courts about why
Malcolm Gladwell's ideas are so interesting, whether or not they're true.
Is that the title of the article?
Yes.
He, by the way, is one of my friends who reads and rereads and rereads this article.
I know that Adam and I share a love of that's interesting, the article.
That's interesting.
See what I did there.
Only if you didn't think that were true before I told you, but go on.
Adam Grant writes about Malcolm Gladwell,
who's the well-known author of many New York Times
bestsellers like David and Goliath,
Tipping Point, Outliers.
Adam writes in this article that a lot of people
give Malcolm Gladwell credit for being a great writer
or storyteller, but Adam would say it's actually his ideas,
not his storytelling that makes it interesting, right?
And one of the things that he talks about
in terms of why Malcolm Gladwell stands out
is this idea that he challenges conventional wisdom.
And just two quick examples from some of his books.
One is this idea that disadvantages can give you advantages.
In one book, David and Goliath, he talks about how dyslexia can actually make people this idea that disadvantages can give you advantages.
In one book, David and Goliath, he talks about how dyslexia can actually make people
more successful because in the absence of one ability,
like reading, people can develop other abilities,
like creative problem solving,
acting, listening, rule-bending, et cetera.
I mean, another example is he writes about things
that appear to be like an individual
accomplishment or an individual failure, but are really part of a collective phenomenon,
something based on society or the situation.
One example that Malcolm Gladwell uses so effectively is about professional hockey and
soccer players.
The data shows that if you're just born a few months earlier than your peers, it has
an impact
on one's ability to play professional sports.
You're like a few months older than your little league co-players.
Right.
And so it's this idea of challenging convention that Adam Grant posits is one of the reasons
that Malcolm Gladwell stuff is so interesting.
You know, if you think about like writing a high school essay, you're like, please write
about Shakespeare's, you know, treatment you think about like writing a high school essay, you're like, please write about Shakespeare's, you know,
treatment of coincidence and like Romeo and Juliet.
Can I just say poor high school teachers who've read the exact same essay
because they're not interesting because people don't have new insights for 30 years.
Well, this is the thing.
Like then if you were trying to craft a really interesting essay in response
to any prompt where like it's not like a personal
story, it's like an idea. You basically have to start with someone's assumptions and then
say that's not true. Like what seems to be true is false and what seems to be false is
true. That's the formula. And I think it can be abused, right? I mean, I don't know if
I should confess to this, but it was very clear to
me when I wrote Grit that what made the idea interesting is that we had a view that what
makes somebody great at what they do is talent. That is the accepted wisdom. But in fact,
it's not. It's sustained effort and interest in what you're doing.
That's grit.
So I had this idea that for grit to be interesting, it had to have a foil.
Like it had to have a, it's not what you think, it's this.
And I have since actually looked at the covers of lots and lots of books.
I've read and endorsed a lot of books. And I don't know if I can say that all of them do this, but I do think and endorsed a lot of books.
And I don't know if I can say that all of them do this, but I do think there is a universal
endeavor to do this, which is to surprise the audience, to upend their assumptions,
because otherwise, you know, why do you pay attention?
And Mary Davis says, like, really, what you're trying to do is grab attention.
The human mind is only able to process a tiny
fraction of reality. So where you allocate that little pen light of attention, where
you direct your engagement, your mind, it has to have a function, it has to have a reason.
And oh hey, look over here. What you thought was true is not true, is an incredibly effective
way to grab attention. Well, to me, it seems like it's so powerful in attracting our attention because it strikes
at our curiosity.
Because then we suddenly think, like, wait a minute.
Still to come on No Stupid Questions, does being self-centered make life more interesting? Everyone thinks the best thing is to be selfless,
but actually it's good to be selfish.
And now back to Mike and Angela's conversation
about what makes an idea interesting.
Can I just confess one thing?
You know I teach this class at BYU
and we bring in so many different speakers
from widely different backgrounds.
So we have actors, we bring in movie directors,
professional coaches, stylist to the stars, on and on.
And the thing that drives me most crazy
is I would say 80 you know, 80%
of the students learn something from everyone and then there's always this
subset. There's a minority of students. Yeah, that are like, hey I'm glad you
brought in such and such an actor but I'm not gonna go into acting so there
wasn't anything for me to gain there. So that bothers you because you're like, why
can't you learn something from this person?
Yes.
Okay, well, your speakers are not saying like everybody thinks that X is true, but in fact,
X is false, right?
That's actually an elaboration of what we're talking about because that's not what makes
your speakers interesting, right?
I find, I will say, that these speakers are always saying something that I find contradictory
to my point of view or what I had thought, regardless of what it is.
And I feel like most really smart people I know, most really interesting people, can
find interesting things from anybody.
It comes down to this curiosity and willingness to learn and pay attention to those things
in order to generate interest.
Is that crazy?
So what you're bringing us to, Mike,
is that Mary Davis's article might be true
of interesting ideas or theories,
or if you have to write an essay
for your high school teacher.
But I think there are other ways that we can be interested.
Like if there's a speaker in class,
and they tell a story about how they got into what
they're doing and they tell you like, oh, and here's something that like I've learned
along the way and I have some advice for you, that can be really interesting even if it's
not a proposition that upends all the logic of your entire life and take something that
you thought was true and makes it false.
I think the broader idea of what makes things interesting, right, not just theories, but things.
I think the key to all of this is that interest is also an emotion.
This is a kind of recent discovery in psychology.
You know, when people were studying emotions at the beginning of the science of emotion,
they studied things like happiness and sadness
and fear.
But it's also true that interest itself fits all the criteria for an emotion.
It has physiological signature, it's a sort of high arousal state.
It also has a valence.
People would prefer to feel interested than the opposite, which is bored.
And emotions all get us to do things.
They motivate us.
You said it yourself, like curiosity or interest, which you can use interchangeably here, those
emotions get you to learn, to pay attention to something that has value for you informationally.
And I think that when you have a really amazing guest speaker,
or I'm reading like Steve Jobs' daughter's memoir, Small Fry,
my gosh, I'm just leaning into every detail.
And it's not because she's saying things that have upended my idea of who Steve Jobs was.
It's just itself new to me, like novelty, and I'm really learning.
Like, I didn't know that.
I had no idea.
She saw her dad a handful of times, all the way up till, you know, age seven, and then
I had no idea that this is what he was like, personally, behind closed doors when nobody
could see.
It wasn't that it defied my assumptions of Steve Jobs.
It was just really novel. And I think that's maybe a broader category.
Things that are new attract our attention
and spark this emotion of interest.
Look, that makes a lot of sense.
I guess I just think that everyone
has something novel to share.
And the more different they are from you,
the more opportunities for novelty.
So everyone's story should be able to spark
that emotion of interest in some way,
no matter how small.
Can I ask you a question about things that,
you know, that pique your curiosity?
Yeah.
When you have a question and you don't know the answer,
how does it feel and what happens next?
And I'll give you just one example.
I think I've talked to you a lot recently about social media and smartphones and whether
it is or is not creating a mental health crisis among teenagers.
This is an interesting question because half the people think quite obviously the answer
is yes, that our young people, Gen Z, they're like incredibly lonely, depressed
and anxious because of social media and smartphones.
And then there are, it seems, an equal number of people on the exact opposite side of the
debate.
So when I came across this question, I'm not kidding, I literally couldn't sleep for days.
I would wake up and try to read more articles and then I would
write in my notebook and then like type some things out and then I'd be like, oh wait,
and I would email people and then I would read another article and then I would have
a conversation with a scientist and then I'd have another conversation with another scientist.
And it was curiosity, but in a way, a kind of a dark form of it because I couldn't shake
the question and I actually never got to resolution. but to me it was not a very positive experience.
And by the way, I'm still a little bit in this like, I want to figure it out.
Urgh, it's like hunger, but it's not a positive state of emotion, unlike most of the research
on interest.
I think there are some questions that won't have answers.
And I think that the too dogged pursuit of them is just an exercise
in frustration. Yours is, I think, different here because you're talking with great scientists
on both sides of what you once told me was a Grand Canyon-sized gulf between the two
sides.
Right.
You've got data, you've got research, you've got scientists trying to answer a question
coming up with vastly different answers. and I think that's really hard.
I think that's different because you're trying
to come to an answer, right?
I am. I'm no closer to it, by the way.
But the dissonance, right?
Like, interest is often a very positive emotion,
and we think of it as the opposite of boredom.
We're like, I will take interest any day of the week.
Right.
But I do think it's sometimes the case where the resolution doesn't come, but you keep
thinking it's going to come.
So you can't write it off as one of those questions that will never have an answer.
The scientists who study the emotion of interest will say that not only does there have to
be a core of novelty, like, I didn't know know that I could learn something, but the I could learn something
has to have a realness to it.
You're like, I could figure out this puzzle.
Like I will solve this riddle.
I don't know whether that has in a way like kept me
in this cognitive limbo.
I mean, I don't know why I don't find this
like a really positive experience.
I find it really like an aversive experience wanting to know.
Maybe I'm just in a bad mood.
By the way, other researchers who studied curiosity do say that like when people talk
about being curious or interested in something, it's not always a kind of like, it's amazing.
I love it.
There is a form of interest, which is like a nagging, gnawing, wanting to know.
Can I give you a much less consequential example, but one that I find interesting?
Absolutely.
I'm going to go back to Malcolm Gladwell.
In his masterclass, he talks about how he learned about this one chef who had kind of
transformed much of the culinary world and started researching this and in the process,
learned how mustard has been changed or iterated on
countless times, but ketchup,
the other kind of primary condiment in the United States,
has not.
Like ketchup is just ketchup.
And so Malcolm says,
why has mustard been iterated on so much and ketchup
almost never? And at the end, he's like, there is no conclusion. I don't know. But it was
just interesting to talk about.
Wait, that's it? That's the end of the article? It's like shoulder shrug?
Yeah, but that was kind of Malcolm's point is that too often-
You want to get to a resolution too often you want like some bottom line answer.
Or you only write about it or talk about it if there is some conclusion.
Whereas sometimes interesting things are just interesting, but we don't have an answer.
By the way, when we're talking about what's interesting, and we're like, oh, it has to be attention grabbing.
And that could be because it's new.
It could be because it's a kind of new, which is defying your original assumptions.
But there is a dimension, which is just how relevant is this to me personally?
And so I think maybe for the things that are really relevant to me, like I really want
to understand teenagers and mental health, that's what I do, I'm a psychologist and this
is my age group that I study.
It's super relevant.
And maybe it's so relevant that the lack of resolution is killing me.
But in general, what is shown in research is that something can be new, it can be assumption
defying even. But if you don't have any connection
to it, right, like if it doesn't feel like it's related to you and your existing projects,
your current goals, that's another way that something can fail to be interesting.
That's another thing that I think about all the time when I teach.
I say to my students like, look, you're in this class.
I'm going to try to make this as interesting as possible.
But there's one thing that you can do to make every minute of this lecture more interesting.
Be selfish.
Be like a complete egomaniac in the following sense.
How does it relate to me?
Because the emotion of interest is going to be enhanced when there is that connection.
Of course, we can do that as speakers too, but it is a trick that I have tried to use
myself.
I'm like, wow, I don't care, but I have to be here.
How do I connect this to something that I already care about?
I love that.
I mean, we've talked about interest now in terms of is it counterintuitive?
Is it novel?
And is it counterintuitive, is it novel, and is it relevant? I think what
I'm taking away from this conversation is that so often, obviously things that you find interesting,
I may not find interesting, and that's okay. And maybe that's because it's not related to my
projects, but it's related to yours. Right. But this is where I love what you've just said,
is this idea of be selfish. First of all, kind of counterintuitive, right?
That's not advice we try to give people all the time.
Right.
Everyone thinks the best thing is to be selfless, but actually it's good to be selfish.
Murray Davis would give me a pat on the back.
And I love your idea because part of, I think, being an interesting person is that you are able to find relevance in
anything or anyone.
I mean, there's the old adage that you can learn from anybody if you're just curious
enough that everyone's an expert in something.
But I actually think it's true.
And so, I asked you what makes something interesting, but I also maybe should have asked, what makes
a person interesting?
And I think part of being able to be interesting is living an interesting life.
And part of that is experiencing things and people and places different than your own.
It's so easy to fall into a bubble of people who think like you, who act like you,
who dress like you, who eat the same foods you do.
Maybe more and more, right?
Right. And I think we'd be in a way better place if we, as humanity,
ventured out a little bit more in terms
of being interested in people who disagree with us,
in people who act differently than us,
whose cultures are different, whose foods are different,
and embrace sort of the counterintuitive thing,
like, OK, I think this, but someone else
thinks that, why?
The novelty of experiencing something completely different than what you're used to.
And then that leads to the idea of we come to understand that things that we didn't initially
think might be relevant to us probably are because we approach it from a selfish, what
can I get out of this?
What can I learn from this?
You know, there is this new theory of happiness that says that,
of course, we can seek happiness through pleasure.
And then, of course, as Aristotle said, we can seek happiness through meaning and purpose.
And the theory says there is another kind of life well lived,
another kind of happy life, and
it's the life of learning.
It's the life of curiosity.
And that's not quite the same thing as pleasure because interest and happiness are not identical
emotions.
Like, it's a different kind of good life.
And I really think this is true.
Like, aligning your life to your values is great.
But that's not the same thing as leading a life that you feel is interesting
to you. So anyway, this idea of the rich life as another life well lived is, I think, timely.
Maybe we're not doing it enough these days. I mean, there's more to learn and there's
more information at our fingertips than at any moment in history. And with each passing millisecond, it seems like there's more and more and more.
And yet it feels to me like we still have a long way to go
to figure out how exactly to craft
this third kind of life well lived,
you know, a truly satisfying life of learning.
And Angela, I just want to add that I hope this podcast
has been a small part of that sort of rich life for our listeners.
I know it has been for me.
And for me too, honestly, a huge part.
Like, I want to do something different now.
I want to break the fourth wall for a moment.
And I want to say thank you on behalf of both of us to all of our listeners for joining
these weekly conversations.
You have been an incredible community and we have loved getting your emails, your insights,
and of course your questions on everything we've discussed on the show.
We have one more opportunity to hear from you.
So here's what we want to do.
In lieu of our regular segment where listeners share their thoughts on the previous week's
episode, Mike and I would love to hear your thoughts about the show as a whole.
Mike, how do you feel about that?
I love the idea.
I think it's perfect.
So you know the drill.
Record a message in a quiet place with your mouth close to the phone and send it to NSQ
at Freakonomics.com and frankly you can share anything you want.
Maybe you want to reminisce about one of your favorite moments from a past episode or share
something important that you've taken away from the show.
We'd love to hear any and all of it.
We are so excited to hear what you have to say about this weird, wonderful, and not so
stupid project. Coming up after the break, a fact check of today's episode and stories from our
NSQ listeners.
And now, here's a fact check of today's conversation. Philosophy of the Social Sciences, the academic journal that published Murray Davis' 1971
article, That's Interesting, is an international interdisciplinary peer-reviewed journal that
is, in fact, still being published.
Mike's framing of Malcolm Gladwell's writing on dyslexia and success could use a bit more
context.
Dyslexia is a learning disability that affects how the brain processes written language that
impacts an estimated 7% of people worldwide.
While it can make language-related tasks more challenging, it doesn't make reading or other linguistic activities impossible.
We should also clarify some details that Mike overlooks in his summary of Malcolm Gladwell's
Ketchup Story, originally published in 2004 in The New Yorker and later explored in Gladwell's
online Masterclass.
The story's central figure, Howard Moskowitz, is not a chef but an American market researcher
and psychophysicist renowned for revolutionizing spaghetti sauce.
As Mike mentions, Gladwell does not pinpoint a single reason as to why mustard has seen
more variation than ketchup, but he does examine several contributing factors.
Experts in the piece highlight Heinz's success in blending all five of the fundamental tastes
sweet, sour, salty, bitter, and umami into a ketchup product with almost universal appeal.
Finally, Angela says that the categorization of interest as an emotion is a fairly recent development in academia.
While it's true that this subject has only become a focus of modern emotion psychology in recent years, we should note that in 1872, Charles Darwin described emotions related
to learning and thinking in his book The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals.
That's it for the fact check.
Before we wrap today's show, let's hear some thoughts about our recent episode on
pessimism. My name is Katie and I run a bike touring organization.
And I've been leading bike trips for teenagers for over a decade now.
And I'm really glad that you guys are having this conversation about
whether the world is becoming more pessimistic, because this is something
I think about often with the work that I do.
I notice it first with parents and the conversations I have with them
about whether or not to send their kids on trips.
They seem to be very risk adverse
and we're often having to like talk to them down
and make them feel like it is okay
to go out on these trips and have these adventures.
I also noticed this with teens themselves.
When adversity comes up, I find kids feel out of control
and that makes them nervous
and then they become pessimistic when plans unravel or when challenges happen like bike
mechanic issues or weather or anything that can kind of happen on an adventure like this.
I often think about like what is the cause of this?
And one of my concerns is whether teens these days are just like more siloed off at home
and connecting more digitally.
So when they're out in the real world having these interactions, they're just not quite able to function.
And lastly, I often wonder if kids have the opportunity to exercise agency in their everyday lives.
I remember a lot about myself being a kid and getting to run around in the neighborhood.
And I got to really exercise my agency and become more confident and independent.
And I don't know if kids have as much of that opportunity living in the fearful world that
we do these days.
Hi, this is Rachel.
I'm a member of Gen Z. And to address this question of pessimism within the generation.
I think part of the issue is social media and news outlets inundating us with negative
information all the time.
Presumably in the past, letters and newspapers would travel a lot slower, and you might not
have such a global grasp of all the terrible things happening in the world. But right now, we're just getting that information all the time.
So it might feel sort of hopeless to make an impact on a global scale
or to address these issues that are huge, looming issues at climate change.
But I find hope in just being able to make an impact within my community
and know that your efforts are fruitful here.
But that's my take on everything.
I'm 38 years old and I've gotten increasingly pessimistic
as I've grown up and realized this is not the reality and adulthood that I was sold as a child.
That was, respectively, Katie Barnhill, Rachel Faga,
and John Cox.
Thanks to them and to everyone
who shared their stories with us.
And remember, we'd love to hear your thoughts
and reflections about this podcast.
Send a voice memo to nsqatfreakonomics.com
and you might hear your voice on our final episode.
We're excited to hear what you have to say.
Coming up next week on No Stupid Questions,
what do we do with unanswered questions?
Wait, we didn't answer all questions about human nature?
No Stupid Questions is part of the Freakonomics Radio Network,
which also includes Freakonomics Radio, People I Mostly Admire,
and The Economics of Everyday Things.
All our shows are produced by Stitcher and Renbud Radio.
The senior producer of the show is me, Rebecca Lee Douglas,
and Lear Kvoutich is our production associate.
This episode was mixed by Greg Rippon.
We had research assistance from Daniel Moritz-Rabson.
Our theme song was composed by Luis Guerra.
You can follow us on Twitter at NSQ underscore show, and you can watch video clips of Mike
and Angela at the Freakonomics Radio Network's YouTube Shorts channel, or on Freakonomics
Radio's TikTok page.
To learn more or to read episode transcripts, visit Freakonomics.com slash NSQ.
Thanks for listening.
I don't know why Curiosity was supposed to kill the cat.
Like what's wrong with curiosity?