No Stupid Questions - Why Do People Love Horror Movies? (Replay)

Episode Date: October 3, 2024

When are negative emotions enjoyable? Are we all a little masochistic? And do pigs like hot sauce? SOURCES:Carol Dweck, professor of psychology at Stanford University.Sigmund Freud, neurologist and f...ather of psychoanalysis.Paul Rozin, professor of psychology at the University of Pennsylvania.Robert Sapolsky, professor of biology, neurology, and neurosurgery at Stanford University.George Vaillant, professor of psychiatry at Harvard Medical School and Mass General Research Institute. RESOURCES:"The 10 Scariest Horror Movies Ever," by RT Staff (Rotten Tomatoes, 2022)."Box Office History for Horror," (The Numbers, 2022)."Around the World, Adolescence Is a Time of Heightened Sensation Seeking and Immature Self-Regulation," by Laurence Steinberg, Grace Icenogle, Hanan M. S. Takash, et al. (Developmental Science, 2018)."Why Taste Buds Dull As We Age," by Natalie Jacewicz (The Salt, 2017).Horror Literature Through History, edited by Matt Cardin (2017)."Why We Love the Pain of Spicy Food," by John McQuaid (The Wall Street Journal, 2014)."Glad to Be Sad, and Other Examples of Benign Masochism," by Paul Rozin, Lily Guillot, Katrina Fincher, Alexander Rozin, and Eli Tsukayama (Judgment and Decision Making, 2013)."The Ignorant and the Furious: Video and Catharsis," by the Association for Psychological Science (2010).Mindset: The New Psychology of Success, by Carol S. Dweck (2006)."Adaptive Mental Mechanisms: Their Role in a Positive Psychology," by George E. Vaillant (American Psychologist, 2000). EXTRAS:Terrifier 2, film (2022)."How to Change Your Mind (Update)," by Freakonomics Radio (2022)."Why Is U.S. Media So Negative?" by Freakonomics Radio (2021)."Why Is Academic Writing So Bad?" by No Stupid Questions (2021).Han Dynasty restaurant.

Transcript
Discussion (0)
Starting point is 00:00:00 Hi, NSQ listeners. If you've been following us for a while, you know that Stephen Dubner used to co-host the show. To kick off the spooky season, we thought you might enjoy this classic episode in which he and Angela break down why certain people love horror movies. On Saturday, we'll be back with a brand new episode featuring Mike and Angela. I know everyone says this is dumb, but I'm gonna do it anyway. I'm Angela Duckworth.
Starting point is 00:00:32 I'm Stephen Dubner. And you're listening to No Stupid Questions. Today on the show, why do some people love horror movies? It is so gory, it has caused audience members to pass out and vomit. Angela, we have a listener email from Skyo who writes to say, why are people into horror movies? I couldn't for the life of me bring myself to watch one from start to finish, by the way.
Starting point is 00:01:06 Skyo, I am so on your side. Anyway, Skyo wants to know, what is the psychology behind fear seeking when there exist many other ways to experience stimulation? Angela, this is a good question. It's such a good question. I want your answer. I can tell you about horror movies I've seen.
Starting point is 00:01:25 You've seen at least one, right? Can I just give you a blanket no? What? I actually looked up on Rotten Tomatoes the 10 scariest horror movies ever. OK, tell them to me. I want to hear. All right, we'll see how many you've seen. And then each one, I guess each one,
Starting point is 00:01:38 you're going to say you haven't seen any of these? It's a waste of breath, because I've not seen any of them. OK, you're zero for 10. I'm going to do my score. All right, here we go. Number one, The Exorcist. Check. Hereditary.
Starting point is 00:01:50 No. That's from 2018. The Conjuring from 2013. No. The Shining. Yes. Texas Chainsaw Massacre. No.
Starting point is 00:01:58 The Ring. I did not see that. Halloween. Yes. Insidious. No. And let's see, this is capital IT. I can't tell if that's supposed to be it or if it's actually IT.
Starting point is 00:02:10 It's it. Stephen King. I mean, I think IT can be pretty scary. No, this is not your system crashing. That I would watch, Angela. Okay, so you've seen about what, four or so? Was that the list? Was that all?
Starting point is 00:02:22 Yeah, that's the list. That's 10. I was also going to say like Poltergeist Carrie silence of the lambs Okay, so with a different list you would have had a higher hit rate, but Skyo and I are on team Do not watch horror films. Okay, it's complicated Stephen. I just want to tell you I'm now on your team What do you mean? I realized listening to that list that any of the more recent? horror movies really there's almost zero chance that I've seen them because I'm in my 50s now and I do not watch horror movies now.
Starting point is 00:02:51 I don't think I watched them in my 40s and I don't think I watched them in my 30s either. So it was really like a teenager, young 20s thing. So I have to say you fit the data really really nicely Horror film numbers are up lately over the past five or six years But they're still relatively low. So as a share of all ticket sales I guess this is assuming that people actually go to theaters to watch movies So we may actually be dealing with a different population too Because it may be that there are people who watch a lot of horror films at home and not in theaters.
Starting point is 00:03:26 But it looks as though horror films represent 10% of all tickets sold. Now, it could also be that only 10% of films made are horror films. That may be fairly representative. But if you look at age, it's definitely a younger person's game. So it's probably not so surprising that neither you
Starting point is 00:03:44 or I are seeing any Lately, I think it's also not surprising because the feature of Behavior called sensation seeking like hey, let me try that. I've never done that before I know everyone says this is dumb, but I'm gonna do it anyway that tends to peak during Adolescence late adolescence early adulthood, you know, late teens, early twenties. So that makes sense. Horror films are one example, let's say, of sensation seeking. But what else might there be? There might be eating spicy foods or might be jumping
Starting point is 00:04:18 out of a parachute. What do you count as that? Well, this is all what Paul Rozin, who is one of my favorite psychologists, he also is at my university, University of Pennsylvania, he calls all of these things benign masochism. Good phrase. Isn't that so good? Can I just say, as a non-academic who's read a lot of academic work, I am so appreciative when someone, whether it's a psychologist, an economist, anthropologist, whatever, has come up with a phrase to describe something that makes the lay brain really get it.
Starting point is 00:04:50 But I feel that there are so many brilliant people out there doing research, and they want to communicate their findings to the public and their peers, of course, but they don't have the ability or maybe the desire, or maybe they don't wanna seem as though they're trying to dress it up or dumb it down or something. But I find that it's incredibly useful when there is a name.
Starting point is 00:05:11 Okay, I have to digress for a minute here. You're going to digress on my digression? I am, just for a second. We'll come back to horror movies. But growth mindset is a pretty sexy phrase, right? Absolutely. And now, if you ask a CEO or a head of a PTA committee at your kid's school, they probably know what a growth mindset is.
Starting point is 00:05:31 But Carol Dweck only recently, relatively, started using that phrase. She used to call it implicit theories of incremental, I know, right? I can't even remember. It was like incremental intelligence theory. There was some multi-syllabic, multi-word phrase and I believe she had to come up with some terminology when she was writing her first, last, only, at least for now, popular book for a lay audience. And you know, implicit theories of intelligence and incremental verses, whatever.
Starting point is 00:06:05 These were not going to fly, and so growth mindset ended up being the terminology we all know and love today. So I agree with you. And can I just say, I don't mean to disparage any academics who don't come up with a phrase to describe an idea or a theory or their work overall. And I realize that for the most part, people like you, Angela, academic researchers are writing primarily for each other in journals. Now we've talked about how journal writing typically is, what's the word, horrible? Maybe. Yeah, I think we use the word horrible. Yes.
Starting point is 00:06:38 It's not writing that the average person would forget about enjoy, but even comprehend. person would forget about enjoy but even comprehend. So I just do want to say to all of our academic listeners out there that I as a medium informed lay person really do appreciate when you take time to essentially label your idea in a descriptive way because I think it leads to a lot more interest and therefore understanding and therefore theoretically application of that idea into fruitful policy and so on. Well, not only have you just sent Paul Rosen and his collaborators a little Stephen Dubner thank you note, you're going to want to keep that stationary out because this is the title
Starting point is 00:07:19 of their paper. I love this title. Glad to be Sad and Other Examples of Benign Masochism. That is good. Which was published in the not so sexily named Judgment and Decision Making Journal. But you know, isn't that great? Glad to be sad. So what Paul Rosen wants to argue is that he can explain not only going to watch The
Starting point is 00:07:40 Exorcist, but also why you would get on a roller coaster, why we would listen to like sad music, why you would eat really spicy hot sauce. The list goes on. In fact, he had people rate on a scale from I think zero to a hundred how much they would want to do these things that are, you know, on the face of them, things that you shouldn't want to do because all of these emotions that you know these activities produce are negative emotions. So I want to tell you what the subscales are on this. Let's start with fear. That's one subscale. Sadness is another. Sad movies, sad novels, sad music, etc.
Starting point is 00:08:21 The sensation of burning, particularly food. So, food, so spicy food and then all the things that it makes you do, like cry and sweat and so forth. There's a disgust subscale. Of course, Paul Rosen is beloved for his— The king of disgust. The pioneer of the psychology of disgust, but you know, like pinching pimples. Ugh. Or like looking at like, distressing images. You're like, oh my God, come here.
Starting point is 00:08:48 You have to check this out. And then you keep scrolling through like more. You keep scrolling through. I don't keep scrolling through. Oh, come on. You've never done that. I don't. You've never gone on YouTube and watched people like pop these like huge zits. Believe it or not, I haven't.
Starting point is 00:09:03 It was just me. I'm getting a whole different image of what you do in your spare time. I thought you were just reading a bunch of academic journals, maybe seeing your family once in a while, but this is all about the zip popping. I contain multitudes.
Starting point is 00:09:16 You know, separately, we got another listener email asking a very similar question about horror movies, but this one was more specific. So this was from a listener named Stephanie who wrote to say, "'Recently a few friends of mine heard about a horror movie being shown in theaters. It is so gory,
Starting point is 00:09:33 it has caused audience members to pass out and vomit. Although I know movies often make these claims to generate buzz, this still repulsed me enough to never want to see the film. My friends, however, upon hearing these claims, immediately decided they had to see it. When they went, someone sitting a few seats away from them did throw up about 15 minutes in, followed by at least
Starting point is 00:09:52 one other person later in the movie. So my question, Stephanie writes, is why are many people drawn to consume media that they are pretty certain will horrify, disgust, and possibly traumatize them on a deep level. I was so intrigued by this email that I wrote back to Stephanie and asked her what was the movie? The movie is called, Terrifier 2, she says. And then she added, please don't watch it.
Starting point is 00:10:16 I feel guilty for putting you onto it. And whatever you do, don't show it to Angela. She must be protected. Wait, I wanna see Terrifier 1 and 2. What are you talking about? Of course you do. Gory movies, by the way, on the Paul Rosson scale, for whatever reason, did not, quote unquote, load with the horror movies.
Starting point is 00:10:38 And what I mean by that is, you know, I told you that he was asking people, like, on a scale from zero to 100, like, how much do you like these activities, even though the emotion that they produce is negative? What loading means is like, does that item, in this case, gory movies, does that correlate with the other items on the fear scale? And you're saying gory and fear don't correlate?
Starting point is 00:10:59 Or they don't correlate enough. They probably correlate, but they don't hang together as much as you think. So I interrupted you. You gave us four categories. There was fear, sadness, burning hot food, and disgust. What else? Yeah, and I have four more. Okay, so there's pain. So do you enjoy, say, getting a really hard massage or, you know, the flash of pain when you experience your hand in ice water. There's the alcohol subscale, and this is in particular beer and scotch. I think I can get on board with this subscale.
Starting point is 00:11:30 An attraction to those represents what? I think in this case, the argument is that these are bitter or burning, right? I think scotch has like- Unless you think they're just delicious. Well, you know, one way you can think about these things is that in many cases, babies would not like these things, right? Like babies don't like pain, they don't like fear.
Starting point is 00:11:48 I tried so hard to get my babies to drink scotch and they were just terrible at it. Yeah, well, so there you figured it out. You didn't need a random assignment study for that one. Okay, just two more. One is the feeling of physical exhaustion. And then the last subscale was bitterness. I guess like coffee is on this scale. So maybe when I was talking about whiskey just then, which was on the alcohol
Starting point is 00:12:10 subscale, maybe there's like a different feeling. But all of these things, they're all what Paul Rosen calls benign masochism. Still to come on No Stupid Questions, would Aristotle have been a fan of horror movies? Such productions, the great philosopher argued, serve to purge viewers of their pent-up emotions. Kind of like a pimple being squeezed. Now, back to Stephen and Angela's conversation about horror movies and benign masochism. So glad to be sad and other examples of benign masochism. The title alone suggests to me that many, many, many people are attracted to these difficult or weird or bitter things
Starting point is 00:13:06 because it must trigger some positive response. Is that what we're being told? I actually asked Paul about this relatively recently. You know, the paper is almost a decade old. And he now believes that the reason why we like to do things that are suffering experiences, the reason why we put ourselves through pain is because we have this kind of mind over body sense of control when we're able to withstand it. So he thinks that it's like this sense of, I've got this. I'm in a state of fear, but I'm making myself do this. And so it's kind of a sense of control.
Starting point is 00:13:43 So you challenge yourself and you meet this challenge and feel better for that. That's right. You don't really think that you're in danger when you're in a horror movie. Right. You don't really think you're gonna have a heart attack when you're eating like a spicy taco. So this kind of sense that you're subjecting yourself to something which is aversive, which is negative, but they're all safe threats. Also for example, when people go and exercise really hard, the body produces endorphins. That then creates a kind of analgesic and even euphoric effect in large amounts.
Starting point is 00:14:20 So you could also argue that there's another mechanism at play in addition to this sense of control that we get when we Engage in these safe threats. There could be these like oppositional defenses that the body has against these threats and then we get to enjoy them right or even just the Cessation like when you get out of a horror movie, you're like, oh god. Thank god That's over and so the relief, the absence of pain. Those all make a lot of sense to me and I appreciate the explanation. But let me just say I don't have that desire.
Starting point is 00:14:55 None. Well, you did use the phrase suffering experience to describe all of these. I would say Scotch is not a suffering experience. Oh, yeah, you would have had a little blip for the Scotch and whiskey. And then bitterness too. Unsweetened coffee, which is you. And even hot food. Do you eat spicy food?
Starting point is 00:15:13 You know, I didn't used to. And then I read two things that changed my idea about this. One was Robert Sapolsky writing about how many people get so fixed in their appetites and habits of everything, food, music, places they go, the people they see, by the time they're roughly 35, that they just sort of calcify. When I read that, I thought, oh crap. Not I, said Stephen Dubner. Yeah. And so I'm a person who I think is relatively eager to try out new things, but that sort
Starting point is 00:15:43 of gave me even more motivation to do so. Additionally, then I read something about how as you get older, your taste buds sort of dull. And so I read that many older people therefore start eating spicy food even when they didn't like it before. And I thought, oh, that's interesting. I'm going to try. And in the last like five, 10 years, I've been eating a lot more of it.
Starting point is 00:16:03 And it turns out that I just like it. And I don't think that has anything to do with taste buds being duller. I think I just was scared of it for some reason. I grew up eating a kind of, you know, meat and potatoes-ish rural farmer bland diet and spicy things seemed a little bit strange and foreign. And now I eat a lot and I like it. I want to double click on this. Believe it or not, Paul Rosanen has done a spicy food study with pigs.
Starting point is 00:16:28 And so I'm going to tell you about it, but before I do, what spicy foods do you eat? So the primary spicy food I eat is from one restaurant in New York, which is just a really good Sichuan Chinese restaurant called Han Dynasty. Wait, is it the same one in Philly? It is! Oh my gosh! Philadelphia gave New York Han Dynasty. I go there all the time. It's very good. Very spicy. I love, love, love, love it. And every dish at Han Dynasty has a spice scale on it.
Starting point is 00:17:03 Zero to ten. So what do you get there? I mean, my parents, well, my mother is from Sichuan province. That's like famously, you know, like the province in China with the hottest food. My grandmother used to make food for my father who is not from that part of China, and he would weep. It was so spicy. And my grandma, whatever, she was like 75 and cooking this incredibly hot food.
Starting point is 00:17:26 Anyway, my point is that at Han Dynasty, I am not a ten person. I am more of a three or four person. I recommend the garlic chicken, which I think is a three. Yeah, the garlic sauce is for wimps. I know, it's like the equivalent of chicken breast. But I'm venturing up and just today, I put some, you know, that sauce that comes out of the rooster container, that like hot sauce that everybody uses on every, what's it called? It's like a clear bottle and a green top.
Starting point is 00:17:55 Anyway, I put it on my food just today. Congratulations. So what have we established? Spicy food is not unpopular among the two of us. That took us a while to do this. I was like, no, no, I wanted to tell you about this Paul Rosin study. Oh, about pigs. Yes, about pigs. But here's what Paul Rosin did. And I think it underscores this idea
Starting point is 00:18:14 that benign masochism, like so many other things, has to be multiply determined. It can't just be the pleasure of escaping a safe threat or relief from pain. So Paul wondered how it is that we end up eating spicy food because around the world, even in the province of Sichuan or the country of Mexico where there's this long proud tradition of adults eating spicy food, babies and very young children don't, and they don't like it. No baby likes Sichuan peppercorns. Or scotch, as we've established.
Starting point is 00:18:47 Or scotch, as Stephen has established with his own children. So what Paul was inspired to do, because I think at the time he was in Mexico, but I'm not 100% sure, and he was living in this rural village, and he noticed that the families would feed their pigs by just throwing out the slop of what was left over.
Starting point is 00:19:07 Spicy slop. And then he thought to himself, well, how did a piglet learn to eat hot food? Do they like the hot food or are they just hungry? So what he did, and I think he did this with Cheetos. So he would take a Cheeto and he would put hot sauce on it. You know, basically, Paul made up his own flaming hot Cheeto. He should get credit for this. In the control condition, he just had the plain one.
Starting point is 00:19:30 And then he would put them next to each other in the yard where the pigs were. He wanted to see which one it preferred, which one it would eat first. And to his surprise, over and over again, these pigs preferred the non-spicy Cheeto. They had lived for a long time on the spicy food but clearly they didn't like it. So if the pig could talk the pig would have said, hey human handler, enough I've been eating this spicy slop you've been giving me. I've never enjoyed it. My eyes are tearing, I'm sweating here, come on. So then Paul says, okay, now that's really interesting. Like babies, pigs don't like spicy food.
Starting point is 00:20:11 How the heck is it that kids do eventually turn into adults who eat spicy food? So then he started just sitting in on these meals and he told me the story of this particular family. There was like three kids in the family. So there's little kid, middle kid, older kid. I guess they would have these like tortillas and there wasn't actually much to put on them, but you added spicy sauce to make it palatable. And then just as you would go down in age,
Starting point is 00:20:38 the pattern was that the child would eat less and less of the spicy food. And what he saw was that the littlest kid would just like take their tortilla and then like all little kids look to the older ones and just see what they were doing. But he speculated that when we have a role model, when we have a higher status person in our midst
Starting point is 00:20:58 who does something like eat spicy food, watch a horror movie, go on the internet and look at disgusting pimple pictures, whatever it is, we can learn that that is something that we should like. And this generally gets to a topic that Paul has been obsessed with for his whole life, which is like, how do we come to like what we like? To me, the dividing line is the things that are put in front of us, like the pigs and the kids, and the things that we seek out, like the horror films. I can see how we're adaptable, so that when things are put in front of us, and when we have no choice set, really,
Starting point is 00:21:32 we habituate to things, even though the pigs were probably not very happy about it. But like to go to the trouble to make plans to go to a movie that I know I'm going to hate, that would make no sense for me. So then I realized, well, there are people who plainly really love that, to a movie that I know I'm going to hate, that would make no sense for me. So then I realized, well, there are people who plainly really love that,
Starting point is 00:21:48 enough to go out of their way to do it. When I think about the appeal, what makes me different from that person or that person different from me, I try to think through how I would have to be to wanna do that. So I could see two different directions. One is that I'm a very secure, settled, unafraid person,
Starting point is 00:22:08 and I can watch a really horrible, scary film for what it is and have fun with it. Like a tourist visiting a country that I wouldn't want to live in, but it's okay, because I know I'll be going home. So that's one. The other is really opposite, which is I feel the world itself is so chaotic
Starting point is 00:22:28 and scary and weird that it's nice to have that perception upheld to some degree in this film version that I can see the darkest elements of human behavior. Oh, interesting. Like dystopian movies and TV series which are all the rage right now because we seem to be living in dystopia itself. Exactly.
Starting point is 00:22:49 And it makes me feel like I'm not wrong for seeing the world that way. And so it could actually uphold my perception of the world. I know that people these days are watching all kinds of dystopian miniseries and movies. I hate them. I'm like, oh my gosh, life is hard and the world is melting. The last thing I want to do is sit on the couch and watch disaster unfolding on the screen. So let me ask you this. If you avoid that kind of, we'll call it horror, dystopia, whatever,
Starting point is 00:23:23 do you also avoid news about terrible things? I think I do. I would be very curious to know if the fans of horror films also consume a lot of social media and other media, because you know, media generally veers toward the negative. I believe we've talked about that on this show before, the power of bad and all that, but also it just sells. And when I look at social media, I think even more in the last year or two
Starting point is 00:23:52 than it used to be, it can be remarkably nasty, negative, designed to enrage. And I do wonder if that is scratching a similar sort of itch as the horror film is scratching, especially keeping in mind what I mentioned earlier that the demand for horror films has gone up in the last five years. So I do wonder if that and social media and bad real news in the world may be somehow connected.
Starting point is 00:24:19 Yeah, I don't know the answer to that. At least I haven't seen any study that I thought really teased these things apart well, because anything correlational like that, you're like, yeah, but could it be this? Could it be that? Like, what else could be going on? Now, I've only glanced at this Paul Rauzen et al paper, and I did see there was a quick reference to Aristotle's notion of catharsis. And I see here that Aristotle did put forth a theory of horror as catharsis. So when talking about Greek tragic plays,
Starting point is 00:24:54 which are full of very, very dark things, betrayals and stabbings and the murder of family members and others, so this piece is saying that such productions, the great philosopher argued, serve to purge viewers of their pent-up emotions. KINNEY LECINSKI-WILKINSON-TOMPSON-WILKINSON-WILKINSON-TOMPSON-WILKINSON-TOMPSON-WILKINSON-TOMPSON-WILKINSON-TOMPSON-WILKINSON-TOMPSON-WILKINSON-TOMPSON-WILKINSON-TOMPSON-WILKINSON-TOMPSON-WILKINSON-TOMPSON-WILKINSON-TOMPSON-WILKINSON-TOMPSON-WILKINSON-TOMPSON-WILKINSON-TOMPSON-WILKINSON-TOMPSON-WILKINSON-TOMPSON-WILKINSON-TOMPSON-WILKINSON-TOMPSON-WILKINSON-TOMPSON-WILKINSON-TOMPSON-WILKINSON-TOMPSON-WILKINSON-TOMPSON-WILKINSON-TOMPSON-WILKINSON-TOMPSON-WILKINSON-TOMPSON-WILKINSON-TOMPSON-WILKINSON-TOMPSON-WILKINSON-TOMPSON-WILKINSON-TOMPSON-WILKINSON-TOMPSON-WILKINSON-TOMPSON-WILKINSON-TOMPSON-WILKINSON-TOMPSON-WILKINSON-TOMPSON-WILKINSON-TOMPSON-WIL with the anxieties of real life. Do you buy that? You know, the idea of catharsis, which may have started as early as Aristotle, but was a central part of Freudian psychoanalysis, has pretty much been debunked. So even though it's intuitive that we should release our emotions, like, let me get the anger out. Let me get it out of my system. It turns out that the modern science of emotion regulation
Starting point is 00:25:47 and psychotherapy suggests that in most cases we just kindle and it's worse to vent, to kind of like be cathartic and it's much better to take some psychological distance, some space between you and the emotion. So it's a charming concept, like get it all out, but it turns out not to be true. So let's go back to Skyo's original question, to the last part of Skyo's question. What is the psychology behind fear seeking when there exists many other ways to experience stimulation?
Starting point is 00:26:18 And let me add one last question onto that. I think I understand why we experience fear. We want to be scared of things that may endanger us. But why, when there is no real danger, why would we want to seek out the same emotion like fear in a horror movie? How does that make sense to you? Well, I said that on behalf of Paul, there is this idea of experiencing fear and other negative emotions but knowing that it's imaginary or an unreal threat. So there's this feeling of mind over matter, this feeling of control.
Starting point is 00:26:55 That's one thing that we talked about. We also talked about, you know, maybe there's the relief after the fear passes, you get the pleasure of not having the fear anymore, right? The second reason. Third is maybe there's some social peer and modeling influences. We watch other people and they go on roller coasters and watch horror movies and that encourages us to do it as well. But there is just one additional thing that I'm now thinking about because you said catharsis
Starting point is 00:27:25 and that brought up Freud. So I spent some time with George Valiant, the Harvard psychiatrist who was, as most psychiatrists of his generation were, trained as a Freudian. And he pointed out to me Freud's idea of defenses, these ego defenses, things that we do to help us through life, and especially the pain of having what Freud thought were these deep unconscious conflicts. So one of the defenses is called anticipation. And in anticipation, you pretend in your mind that something truly horrible has happened.
Starting point is 00:28:00 You play out the worst case scenario. And I'm not even going to ask you to utter it in this conversation, but you can just imagine, like, what would be your worst fear come to pass? For me, it absolutely has to do with the loss of loved ones, but I can only use abstract language because I can't even bring myself to say it. When you play that out, when you imagine it happening, you're kind of habituating to it, you're like kind of coming to tolerate it. And so if and when that does happen or some version of that happens, you will be prepared. So that's another layer of this. We're kind of preparing ourselves. You know, we're practicing what it would be like and what we should do.
Starting point is 00:28:40 So Angela, let me ask you one last question. Has this conversation made you more or less likely to watch a horror film in the near future and why? It has not changed my aversion to horror movies. Has this conversation made you more or less likely or neither to engage in some form of benign masochism in the near future? This conversation has made me want to order hotter food from Han dynasty in the very short-term future, possibly tonight. What are the odds that you're going to see Terrifier 2 and vomit?
Starting point is 00:29:15 Oh, that? I'll get at least 20 percent. Maybe while I'm watching Terrifier 2, I'll have some spicy dan dan noodles. Coming up after the break, a fact check of today's conversation. And now here's a fact check of today's conversation. In the first half of the show, Stephen reads Angela the media review website Rotten Tomatoes' list of the top 10 scariest movies of all time. He actually skipped number nine, the 2012 movie Sinister, directed by Scott Derrickson and starring Ethan Hawke. As with the other modern horror movies, Angela has not seen this film.
Starting point is 00:30:05 Later, Angela forgets the terminology that Stanford psychologist Carol Dweck originally used to describe the idea that she ultimately called growth mindset. The phrase Angela was looking for is an incremental theory of intelligence. This is the opposite of a fixed mindset, which Dweck originally called an entity theory of intelligence. This is the opposite of a fixed mindset, which Dweck originally called an entity theory of intelligence. Also, Angela has difficulty differentiating Paul Rosen's alcohol subscale from the bitterness subscale. Rosen and his colleagues write that the flavor of alcoholic beverages isn't bitter per se, but rather, quote, innately negative. Stephen would obviously disagree.
Starting point is 00:30:47 Next, Angela cannot remember the name of the hot sauce that she's been experimenting with. She was thinking of Sriracha, a condiment named after the town of Sriracha in Thailand, where it was first created by home cook Tanam Chakkapok. It was later introduced to the United States in the 1980s by Vietnamese immigrant David Tran, who began selling his own version of the sauce through his company Hui Fong Foods. Finally, Angela describes University of Pennsylvania psychologist
Starting point is 00:31:18 and King of Disgust Paul Rosson's research on whether pigs enjoy spicy food. The experiment took place in the 1970s in Oaxaca and involved both pigs and dogs. Rosen did not, as Angela recalled, use Cheetos. Instead, he offered the animals a plain cheesecracker and one laced with hot sauce. Rosen said that the animals would eat both snacks, but always chose the mild cracker first. That's it for the Fact Check. No Stupid Questions is part of the Freakonomics Radio Network, which also includes Freakonomics Radio, People I Mostly Admire, and The Economics of Everyday Things.
Starting point is 00:32:02 All our shows are produced by Stitcher and Renbud Radio. The senior producer of the show is me, Rebecca Lee Douglas, and Lierk Vaudic is our production associate. This episode was mixed by Eleanor Osborne with help from Jeremy Johnston and Greg Rippon. We had research assistants from Catherine Moncure. Our theme song was composed by Louis Scara. You can follow us on Twitter at NSQ underscore show.
Starting point is 00:32:26 If you have a question for a future episode, please email it to nsq at Freakonomics.com. To learn more or to read episode transcripts, visit Freakonomics.com slash nsq. Thanks for listening. Oh my God, I'm looking at images of Terrifier 2. I have no desire to look. I'm not even going to listen to any words you say. I'm going to hang up now. The Freakonomics Radio Network, the hidden side of everything. Stitcher.

There aren't comments yet for this episode. Click on any sentence in the transcript to leave a comment.