Ologies with Alie Ward - Mnemonology (MEMORY) Part 2 with Michael Yassa
Episode Date: December 11, 2024Remembering names! Preventing dementia! Photographic memories! Weed! Goldfish! It’s the thrilling conclusion of Mnemonology with Dr. Michael Yassa, the Director of UC Irvine’s Center for the Neuro...biology of Learning and Memory. We talk long vs. short term memories, how smells can pack a wallop of emotions, prosopagnosia (“facial blindness”), the fog of new parenthood, Alzheimer's and other causes of dementia, and tips to keep your brain in tip-top shape. Let’s make some mems. Listen to Part 1 hereFollow Dr. Yassa on Google Scholar and XVisit the Yassa Translational Neuroscience Laboratory at UCI websiteA donation went to UC Irvine Center for the Neurobiology of Learning and Memory’s graduate student and postdoctoral Junior Scholar FundMore episode sources and linksSmologies (short, classroom-safe) episodesOther episodes you may enjoy: Attention-Deficit Neuropsychology (ADHD), Molecular Neurobiology (BRAIN CHEMICALS), Eudemonology (HAPPINESS), Traumatology (PTSD), Sports & Performance Psychology (ANXIETY & CONFIDENCE), Phonology (LINGUISTICS), Neuropathology (CONCUSSIONS), Quantum Ontology (WHAT IS REAL?), Surgical Angiology (VEINS & ARTERIES), Disability Sociology (DISABILITY PRIDE MONTH), Dolorology (PAIN)Sponsors of OlogiesTranscripts and bleeped episodesBecome a patron of Ologies for as little as a buck a monthOlogiesMerch.com has hats, shirts, hoodies, totes!Follow Ologies on Instagram and BlueskyFollow Alie Ward on Instagram and TikTokEditing by Mercedes Maitland of Maitland Audio Productions and Jake ChaffeeManaging Director: Susan HaleScheduling Producer: Noel DilworthTranscripts by Aveline Malek Website by Kelly R. DwyerTheme song by Nick Thorburn
Transcript
Discussion (0)
Oh hey, it's still the fountain at the mall that's never on.
Allie Ward, you are here for part two of Nemanology, Memory.
Please tell me that you started with part one.
Even if you don't remember it, start with part one because this is the thrilling conclusion.
With Professor, Researcher, and the Director of UC Irvine's Center for the Neurobiology
of Learning and Memory, he is amazing.
We're going to get into it, but first, this is a wall-to-wall episode of Questions from Patrons, and if you'd like to submit some time, you too
can join for as low as a buck a month, and upper tiers can submit audio
questions. Thank you also to everyone getting Ology's merch from Ology's merch.com,
and thanks to everyone leaving us reviews, which boosts the show so much.
And each week I remember to read them all. And I pick a just written one, such as this one
from Sula Singh, who wrote, this podcast saved my life.
It's like spending an hour at the most amazing library ever,
where you find exactly the right book that you didn't
know that you needed.
Sula Singh and everyone who has ever loved a review,
I have read it.
And thank you.
Welcome to the library.
OK, on to part two, where we answer your questions about how to remember names and faces, what causes Alzheimer's, photographic
memory, short-term versus long-term memory, how to prevent dementia, cannabis and memory,
goldfish brains, and why smelling sunscreen makes you want to cry sometimes with neurobiologist,
professor, researcher, and memory expert, nemenologist,
Dr. Michael Yasa.
I have questions from listeners.
Can I ask you one million?
Of course.
You're the best.
Okay.
So this question was on the minds of patrons.
ESO party Elsa T, who's a first time question asker and face name forgetter.
Diana Storesnik, Dean, Lily B and audio question submitter, Summer, wanted to know.
Hi, Allie. Summer from New Zealand here. What I find really interesting is that I remember faces,
but sometimes I can't remember where I've seen that person before, even in what country I've seen
them. And I'm wondering why we're so good at recognizing faces and remembering knowing a face but
knowing none of the details around that face. What is it about remembering
certain aspects of a person or a connection? I know some people have total
face blindness as well. Either evolutionarily or how do we look at
faces where we blank and say, who is that
person?
Yeah, that's a fantastic question, Summer.
So you've already mentioned one version of this, which is face blindness, right?
And I'm not saying that Summer has face blindness at all.
In fact, I think all of us struggle with faces to an extent.
So this condition is medically known as prosopagnosia and a 2023 study in the journal
Cortex titled, What is the prevalence of developmental prosopagnosia? An empirical assessment of
different diagnostic cutoffs found that developmental prosopagnosia, meaning it's lifelong and not caused
by injury to the brain, is more common than previously thought and that it
lies on a spectrum of severity.
I was doing some reading on it from people who have it to try to figure out what it feels
like and I've read it described as seeing a tree and then trying to pick out that specific
tree in a forest or telling the difference between two different cows in a field.
And if this sounds like you, and if you watch movies
wondering, wait, is that the same guy,
or is that a different guy, you may be one in 33 people
who have it, which is cool, but also awful.
So don't let anyone make you feel too bad about it.
Now, faces are a very important piece of information.
When you look at somebody's face,
you can tell by their expression whether they're
a threat to you, whether they're a friendly person.
You can choose to approach or avoid.
You can base a lot of your decision making based on a face.
If somebody's face has an expression of fear and they're looking behind you, they might
warn you to something and you might react accordingly.
So we evolved and not just our species, many social species have evolved to try to always
interpret faces
and facial expressions. But recognizing a face and remembering whether a face belongs
to a certain name is this thing that happened much later in evolution. And I don't think
our brains are just very good at it yet. I think we're developing that, but we're still
sort of half-baked when it comes to connecting names and faces. So number one complaint that I hear from everybody is, I remember faces, but I could never connect
them with when did I see this person, what their name is, or sometimes I'll hear the
name and I can't recall the face, but then I connect when I see it.
So face names, associations like that, they tend to be the most challenging for us humans.
But one of the reasons why faces are really important
is because of this evolutionary survival
sort of significance, things around fear,
things around pleasure, reward, all of that.
We get our cues, a lot of our cues from faces.
I learned a trick once on a film set
where one guy I worked with,
it was Adam Savage from Mythbusters.
He knew everyone's name on this huge set we were on.
I was like, how do you do that?
He was like, oh, okay, I have this trick where that woman's name is Dorothy.
And I think of Dorothy from The Wizard of Oz.
That person's name is Ben and I have a brother-in-law named Ben.
And so as soon as they said their name, you had to pay attention, but then you made a
connection.
Absolutely.
You know, there's lots of folks out there who try to train you on how to make your memory
system better.
And many of them, you might know, they're memory champions out there.
They're sort of memory grandmasters out there.
And they've learned tricks that have been used by the Greeks for a long time, ancient
Greeks, I should say, like mind palaces and so on.
And all of these methods are based on forming strong personal associations.
So if you have never seen The Wizard of Oz, it wouldn't have made sense to you to associate it, right? But if you've seen it and you can picture
the Dorothy in the exact movie, right, that makes it much more personal. So they tell
you to try to be able to remember people's faces and names, make that something that
is emotionally significant to you. But making those connections and making them as vivid
as possible
Seems to be the trick that works for most people
So I hope that helps clay trover Sarah McEacher and Eric kids egg Jennifer fro Ellie's weeple Hannah Nolan Jacqueline church gnomes Laura more Bennett Vanderbush and lean lands Kent urban lists
So leka pevic Kelly Fong Kelly Larson Carlene DH and Connor they them all of whom say that they are garbage at remembering names
It's okay
obviously based on that list you are not alone and patron Jackie G said that a tour guide shared this
tip with them that you introduce yourself first then you can actually listen when other people
say their names instead of mentally rehearsing your own introduction and Jackie G says it's
amazing and has worked wonders for me.
So perhaps that'll make some of you less anxious.
Oh, speaking of anxiety and trauma and emotions in general,
many, many of you, little miss particular, Camilla Gamino,
Susan Singley, Maddie Cakes, popsicle emperor,
Joshua Talzin, Sarah Arguella, Rowan Tree, Michelle Griegos,
Ali Brown, Patricia Evans, Katie Hammond, and Rachel Prostaco
wanted to know how emotions affect retention.
And Nick Alston asked, what's the chemistry behind emotional memories?
And also, patron Ryan asked, Hi, this is Ryan from Los Angeles.
I was wondering if you could speak to the effects that negative emotions such as shame
and guilt have on working memory and long-term memory.
Are people who experience heightened emotions,
either with anxiety or pretty happy people,
do they keep those memories more because there is more of an emotional connection?
There's no doubt that having an emotional connection
strengthens the way that you store a memory.
So certainly if you tend to have much more emotional reactions to things or the experience
that you're having are much more emotional, they will have the capacity to be stored for
longer, to be able to influence your actions and decisions for longer.
And again, there's the evolutionary significance for that.
Of course, more emotional things may be a little bit more involved in your survival,
right?
That said, emotion doesn't always improve your ability to store things.
It colors the experience for sure, but it also kind of zooms in on certain aspects of the
experience and zooms out from others. So you may recall certain details incredibly well,
but there may be other things that are kind of lost on you because of the emotionality.
So it creates this competition between some central features of the experience and then
a lot of the peripheral stuff kind of doesn't win out in that competition and that can be
forgotten.
Many of you wondered about what is normal given our very chaotic and technological world
such as ologist from the caribology episode, Megan Lynch, Amelia Frank, Ginny Bateman,
Katie, Britt Klein, Margaret Anouska, Miranda Panda, Regular K,
Wasp zoologist Eric R. Eaton,
Rachel M., Megan Walker, Tiny Nature,
and first-time question askers Julie Williams,
Chelsea Loves Chocolate, Theo Klein, Tara Villanueva,
and some folks who are distracted by stress,
Dawn Ewald, Holly Cole, Eric Masterson,
and Helen Langiel.
I wonder if our lack of presence, because we were distracted a lot with our phones and
internet stuff, I wonder if that lack of presence is making our memories a little bit more Swiss
cheesy.
I like this, Swiss cheesy.
It's a good way to think about it.
Certainly every time that I've lifted up my phone and tried to record a video of my daughter
playing basketball as opposed to put the phone down and actually watch her play, I feel like I
have Swiss cheese in my brain.
So 100%, I think that you're right when we replace actually experiencing something in
its full glory, three-dimensionality and all of that with some 2D version of what we're
recording on the phone or being distracted
by looking at something at the same time and not being fully aware, fully cognizant of
what's happening, of course it's going to change how these memories are stored and how
they're represented.
And I do worry about that.
I think that there's no doubt that there's value to having the electronics and having
– I mean, look at us.
We have – our devices are out all the time.
And that's just the nature of what we have to do
to be able to deal with the situations around us
and the rapidly kind of evolving world
and all of the stimulation.
But I think it misses something
about experiencing something fully
and truly with all of its four dimensionality,
I should say, and with all the emotional contexts
that come with it,
and to be able to have that genuinely,
you have to be there.
So immersion, I think, is really key
for good memory storage.
So those four dimensions are like a 3D object
on the X, Y, and Z axes,
but with the added dimension of time.
It's interesting because we're experiencing it
less in the moment, but we're able to recall it more with that video footage.
And it's such a weird trade off that it's like, well, I can remember it later as long as I don't fully experience it now.
It is a weird trade off because what you remember based on the video is this weird sort of, you know, two dimensional version on a screen.
And you're never able to piece back being there fully in three dimensions.
But if that was your experience, that never able to piece back being there fully in three dimensions.
But if that was your experience, that's what you piece back.
So it comes back with all of the pleasure, the sensory experience that came with it,
as opposed to if you're looking at the screen, that's your version of that reality.
And that's what comes back.
It's just that two dimensional, more impoverished version.
And that to me, I think is the big concern.
That's funny because I, my
husband and I got married three years ago and a good friend of mine was like
let's watch the video on your first anniversary. My husband and I can't watch
the video. What we remember from it is what we remember and both of us have
like the ick when it comes to watching it for some reason. We're like I don't
want to remember it any differently because we are kind of always like you
said reconstructing those memories based on input, right?
And you don't want to change the version that you have in your brain,
especially if that version was beautiful and something that you want to hold on to.
Now, I have the, anytime anybody tells me to watch a video of myself,
so I just can't do it categorically. That's just something I don't want to do.
But I can totally resonate with what you're saying.
If you've experienced it fully being there,
being able to piece it back together in this two-dimensional version and looking at it
on a video is just never the same. And it kind of alters your, you know, actual recollection
of that experience.
LESLIE KENDRICK And a University of Chicago study recently recruited users of this app
called One Second Every Day to hop in an MRI and look at quick videos of strangers' lives versus one-second clips
that they had recorded themselves using that app.
And the study found that different parts of the brain
light up if it was their own memory versus just
intaking a stranger's video.
And other neurobiologists at the University of Toronto
are exploring how video diaries like that
could help Alzheimer's patients connect more to their own past.
So to patrons Aunt Tiffa and Rosalie de la Foret,
who recall more when prompted by photos and videos,
that's some real science right there for you.
Everyone else, that app was called One Second Every Day
if you want to log little chunks of your whole life.
Now, what if you don't need physical memorabilia or pixels
to jog your memory?
What if your mind is a camera?
So Alia Myers and Erica Perriandri, among others,
had questions about that.
Well, on the topic of photographic memory, many people
asked, Mariah K said, this question has been on my mind
for so long, all caps.
I know there is photographic memory,
but are there different versions or levels of it? Reid Berry wants to know what actually is photographic memory. This question
was asked by Cybermans, Erica, Ariandri, Rachel McGill, or Luke Gramelkin. What is photographic
memory exactly?
Okay, fellas. This is myth busting time.
Okay.
Ready?
Yeah.
Photographic memory does not exist.
How? How? Ready? Photographic memory does not exist.
How? How?
And it's so uncomfortable to hear that. Well, so photographic memory is what we sometimes
refer to as eidetic memory. You know, the way that it's defined, it's that you have
this perfect recall, perfect recollection of something that you've seen potentially only
once, right? And if it's in the context of say reading, you're talking about like remembering page numbers and all of that.
The evidence that that exists historically is slim to none.
We've been lied to.
There's maybe a very small handful of cases of savant syndrome where somebody could legitimately
make the claim for true photographic memory. But aside from that, there's no real evidence that photographic
memory to this sense exists. That said, there's really, really, really good memory and there's
really, really, really bad memory. There's a whole spectrum. And a lot of times when
we're thinking about photographic memory, it's not exactly that, but it's close. It's
really good memory where you're memorizing where things are laid out on the page, where
you may be memorizing things like the page numbers and where the figures were, where
the pictures were, all that kind of stuff.
To the extent that we can tell whether or not that's actually helpful in a day-to-day
learning experience, it's not clear whether or not that actually helps you.
And while a true photographic memory is at this point just flim flam, there is such a
thing as eidetic memory where someone can see a visual and look at it or sometimes hear
something and once it's removed they can recall it in great detail.
It's as if visually they're still looking at it, but it's not 100% faithful and it does
not last a lifetime.
Your brain's like, I don't really need that
fancy of a feature.
Because remember, what we're trying to learn and remember on a day-to-day basis is not
necessarily the details of the exact words on a page or the exact details of what happened.
It's the gist. It's sort of the overall experience kind of abstracted. It's whatever knowledge
you can abstract from that experience and be able to use it to guide my future decision making. So if you start to think about memory
a little bit differently, that it's not really about the past, it's all about the future,
this is no longer uncomfortable. It's okay to sit with that, that there's no photographic
memory because there's no rationale, there's no reason for it to exist.
So though we debunked learning styles in part one, what about people who don't retain things as well visually
but through sound?
Hey, first time question asked was Kimberly,
Kirsten Cornell, as well as E. Jordan, Sean Thomas K.,
Matthew Walcher, Lisa Gorman, Maria K., Josh Walden,
Sonny S., Vanessa Adams, Daniel VanVoren,
Jennifer Frode, Deb Does Science, Alicia Clarkson, and Sam
wanted to know about sound and memory, including song lyrics.
Well, I know because we have obviously a lot of listeners who learn by auditory memory.
Are some people better at recalling things if they hear it?
Yes.
Okay.
Yes.
So, and this takes us back to the conversation about learning styles.
So I want to be very clear, right?
It is a myth.
Learning styles is a myth.
There is no one way to be able to get to your brain and it could be visual
for you or auditory for you. That said, people do have individual differences in how much
they learn visually and how much they learn through their ears auditorily and how much
they learn through other senses. But at the end of the day, the most effective learning is the one that combines the most senses. So as a species, we are far more visual as a species collectively.
If you look at rodents, for example, rats, they have a much stronger sense of smell than
we ever would be motivated by, right? That does not diminish our sense of smell, does
not diminish our other senses, but it says that primarily because we are not nocturnal, we operate usually in daylight and the sunlight,
visual information seems to be really important to us.
We tend to kind of prioritize that.
We have a lot more real estate in our brain dedicated to processing of the visual sense
than to other senses.
So when somebody says, well, I learn better if I hear it, I'll say, I'm willing to bet
that you'd learn better if you heard it and saw it at the same time.
Ah.
So don't just rely on, oh, I just want to hear it.
So for example, if you're reading,
and you say, I'm just not a reader,
well, try reading out loud so you're also hearing it.
And listening to yourself say the words.
And you'll notice that that is a bimodal kind of learning,
and things will stick a lot more.
We did a reading episode recently, and there was a big question as to whether or not audio
books were reading, whether they count, whether you can count them in your book.
Oh, I sure hope they count.
Yeah, I know, me too.
Me too.
So we talked about this in the recent anagnosology episode all about reading and I'll just give
you an excerpt from that with Dr. Adrian Johns who is a professor and historian and who authored
the book The Science of Reading.
It's actually interesting that e-books, or actually
audiobooks more, the idea that you could, as it were,
read a novel or something by having it read to you
by machine.
There are schemes for those going back as far as pretty
much the origin of recording.
So the late 19th, early 20th century,
there were visionary schemes
for having things like, oh, you know, vending machines where you could put your money in
and there would be a speaker's, you know, like a speaking trumpet that would speak a
book to you. It's not like there's something that is that radically new about audiobooks
per se. Having said that, I mean, my own sense of it kind of crudely is that I think with
audiobooks, it's really that you're having something read to you rather than
reading. And part of that has to do with the control of the pace of it. You know,
so you can slow down recordings, you can pause it and all of that kind of thing.
But it's not the same as doing what one does with one's mind's eye all the time
in reading a page where you're constantly shifting the speed and considering things and going back,
you know, without necessarily thinking about it.
You don't have to press a button or something.
Ebooks, on the other hand, I think are just reading.
I mean, I don't have any issue with those at all.
So Dr. Johns is a reading scholar, and I am a lady recording this while not wearing a
bra, but whose entire life revolves around reading to people
So we'll have to arm wrestle for dominance
But I will say that per an ancient study from the late 1960s titled retention and recall
Incidental learning of visual and auditory material in the Journal of Genetic Psychology
That visual memories tend to be better for recall
But there is quote a decline with age for recall of visual material, but virtually no deterioration
in performance on the auditory task. So I'm going to amplify that data in my favor.
Now, what about smell? Cody Burdock, Vanessa Adams, Christine Hurley, The Dork Next Door,
Amanda Regan, Guy Hutchinson, Eating Dark Hair for a Living, Rene Vandenhoven, Jess H. Fiona,
Elizabeth Carrow Young, Anastasia Press,
all wanted to know about smell and memory.
And Susan Singley asked,
why do some aromas bring back such clear
and nostalgic memories, like cut grass,
old books, coconut sunscreen, ocean waves,
and that smell after rain?
But yeah, earlier Mike mentioned that rodents
have a much stronger sense of
smell than us. And I wonder if they feel nostalgic for smelling certain garbage. I bet they do.
Well, you mentioned smell and rats. We have an excellent urban rodentology episode about
rats that made me cry with affection for rats. But Amanda Regan wanted to know, why do smells
or sounds trigger memory sometimes and I have
heard that it's difficult to really know what a smell is you have to have a
memory of that smell that it goes straight to some memory center. Yeah.
What's up with that? Couple things. So let me break this down because you asked a
couple of really really interesting questions but they're a little bit
different from each other. So the first one is maybe whether or not we can sort
of label smells.
You can label a sound and people that have perfect pitch
can tell you exactly what note it is and so on.
And you can certainly label visual things.
We have objects, we have colors, right?
But with smells, it's a little bit different.
And we don't have a great lexicon for smells.
A lot of times we're relegated to kind of like lavender,
kind of like, and fill in the blank,
right? Something that you're familiar with. But that's the rub. You have to go back to
something that you're familiar with. So it reminds you of something else. It reminds
you of something that you might have smelled or sensed before. And that may be just an
evolutionary thing. Like we haven't really evolved to prioritize this kind of information,
smell information. We don't use it typically to navigate around the world, although if you've got a nice
baking cake in the kitchen, you might navigate your way there.
So in some situations, maybe it's helpful, but we tend to navigate mostly based on visual
information.
So our sense of smell isn't keen enough to save us, essentially, so it remains pretty
mid.
And think back to any time you've tried to describe a smell, you've probably said it smells like and thought of the last instance or the strongest
instance of smelling something like that thing. We tend to kind of know what we
need to do based on visual information. So because it was never prioritized, we
never sort of bothered to create robust labels for it. And when you don't have
labels for things, your brains sort of struggle to kind of store it with that fidelity because you don't have that verbal thing that you can
attach to it. I know what a coffee mug is because I have a verbal label for those coffee
mugs, right?
Right, right.
So that's one piece. The other piece, which I think is fascinating, is that especially
smells can trigger memories, sometimes long lost memories. There's certain smells that
will remind me of my grandmother's house certain smells that will remind me of my
grandmother's house, certain smells that remind me of specific people in my past, because
that might have been the perfume or cologne they wear or something like that or similar
to it. And then the experience of going back sort of it like washes over you. You kind
of go back in time, you're immersed, you're exactly in that moment. The sense of smell almost has this
incredibly privileged capacity to do this. And we don't know why this happens, but we
suspect that it's possible part of it is the fact that your sense of smell, unlike all
of the other senses, it has direct access to your hippocampus, your memory bits of the
brain. And it's not clear why that is, but it's sort of co-evolved that way.
The sensory systems in the brain that are outside of smell,
so vision, audition, all have to go through the thalamus,
which is this sort of major hub in the brain
before they get to the memory parts of the brain.
There isn't like this direct access,
whereas everywhere else this happens
for our sense of smell, it doesn't happen.
Our sense of smell doesn't go through the thalamus.
It like directly has this revolving door straight into our memory bits of the brain.
We have no clue why the hell that is.
It's just this weird, quizzical thing, and I don't know to what extent that means it's
truly privileged, and that's the reason why we remember things so vividly.
But it seems plausible that it's at least a contributing factor that you don't have
to gate through somewhere else before you get to the hippocampus right there.
It's like an express train.
Yeah.
So exciting.
What about as someone who has had a nasty concussion, Hope, Lauren Galerio, Addie Capello,
Adam Foote's wife, Anna. A bunch of people wanted to know
how studying concussions or TBI has influenced work
or influences our memory.
You know, it's challenging with concussions
or TBI, traumatic brain injury,
because there's no two injuries that are the same.
So that is a particularly difficult set of conditions.
It depends on the severity of the concussion,
the location, all sorts of things. And we're learning a lot more about this. Clearly, it impacts a variety
of different memory systems, memory being one of the key ones that gets impacted. But
depending on the kind of injury, whether it's a coup, contrecoup kind of injury where there's
sort of stretching and shearing of some of the brain's white matter pathways, the connections
between different regions, All of those things
tend to happen. There's inflammation, there's sometimes frank injury. You can actually see
evidence of that. But it's not clear how much of that is, first of all, common across individuals
because again, the extent of the injury is different. The etiology, the roots of the
injury, the cause, it can be very, very different. But the fact that memory is impacted almost in all concussive injuries is an interesting
phenomenon.
And I always go back to our memory system is, or at least the hippocampal memory system,
is one of the most vulnerable systems in the brain.
It tends to feel the brunt of pretty much anything.
Lyle Ornstein Patrons Rye of the Tiger, Heidi, Brenda Palencia, and Christine Hurley asked about
what is loathsomely called mom brain,
or in Tim Farr's words,
I don't feel like I know things anymore, and it's horrible.
I myself do not have kids,
but I have damaged my brain
with a hospital grade blow to the head, I feel for ya.
And not just concussions,
but even if we're sleep deprived,
even if we're anxious or depressed
or anything that's happening to us tends to impact the system.
It is one of our most primitive systems in the brain.
It's one that we shared with all the mammalian species and many others.
Even though it's tucked in the middle of the brain, it seems like it would be like nice
and enveloped and covered up.
It has kind of weird positioning.
The vasculature around it is quite vulnerable.
The white matter is quite vulnerable.
It's also very close to the most porous parts of the blood-brain barrier.
So toxins can get into those limbic structures much more easily than in other places in the
brain.
So remember from part one that memories are stored all over the brain, kind of like coins
hidden in the sand.
But the hippocampus, that seahorse-shaped organ, acts as a sort of metal detector to
find and retrieve those memories.
So the hippocampus is this wicked combination of really important and very delicate.
Just be careful.
So, it's just a hub of vulnerability and it's one of the
reasons why we studied so extensively in my lab across a variety of different
conditions because we believe that it is very vulnerable and if we develop ways
to be able to protect it or treat it that will generalize across a number of
different conditions. And is it rather new that we even know that the blood
brain barrier is permeable at all?
Yeah, I would think, I mean, we've always known that it's permeable to some things.
We know this because there's a lot of things that are in our blood that get through the blood-brain barrier very quickly and get into our brain easily.
Alcohol is one of those, right? That gets straight from the blood to the brain.
So there are things that we've known about for quite some time that can traverse that barrier with great ease.
But there are new things that we're learning about now that we didn't realize can get through
the blood-brain barrier easily that seem to get through.
And some of these things may be inflammatory in nature, some of them may be toxins.
So when we talk about the connection between the body and the brain, that is real and it's
always been there, but we've only really started to study the sort of mind-body or brain-body connection much more in recent years and trying to understand how our gut, for example, influences our brain, how our brain influences our gut.
This back and forth, which has to be able to kind of get through some of those barriers and the blood-brain barrier being the key one. One of the most interesting things that happened in terms of technology recently is there is
actually an approach, a technique using what's called focused ultrasound to open up the blood-brain
barrier to be able to transmit things through because it's always been a challenge for
us in developing drugs and interventions, sort of pharmacological interventions, how
to package something just right to get it through the blood-brain barrier.
And this may be this other approach.
It seems a little scary, I know, but with focused ultrasounds and what's called micro
bubbles, we can actually open up the blood-brain barrier and get some things that maybe are
larger macromolecules that normally wouldn't get through actually to go through the blood-brain
barrier.
So lots of fun activity these days in research on the blood-brain barrier.
Well, you mentioned something about the gut connection too.
People are talking a lot about the vagus nerve.
Just a quick background on that vagus nerve.
So it's the longest nerve in your body and it plays this key role in your parasympathetic
nervous system, which is the chill side, the rest and digest, as the vagus nerve carries
messages between your heart and your brain and your guts.
And according to this 2017 study, childhood trauma and lifetime syncope burden among older
adults, researchers say that vasovagal syncope, where your heart rate and blood pressure fall
suddenly, which can cause dizziness and sweating or fainting.
According to the study, it's governed by the autonomic nervous system, and it's often precipitated
by a highly salient emotional situation.
And researchers found that the report of childhood abuse
was independently associated with frequent syncopean youth.
So what role does this physiology have?
Does that vagus nerve play a role in memory at all?
It certainly does.
One easy way to think about that is,
and it's been known for quite some time,
is that this is one of the ways by which
the adrenal hormones can
impact the brain. So when we think about cortisol and cortisol release, when we think about
epinephrine, those kinds of things do have a way to be able to impact the brain through
its impact on the vagus nerve. But it's way beyond that now. There's a lot of literature
now suggesting that vagal stimulation, for example, could have some really interesting
effects therapeutic in some ways. And we're starting to understand a little bit more about that vagal stimulation, for example, could have some really interesting effects, therapeutic
in some ways.
And we're starting to understand a little bit more about how that mode of communication
is operating.
But it's certainly there.
And it's, again, one that we tend to learn a lot more about.
I feel like this is all a really good endorsement for meditation and deep breathing.
Well, I mean, that should be endorsed right off the bat all the time.
Yeah, yeah.
You know, you're mentioning toxicity in the brain,
blood-brain barrier, alcohol.
Many people wanted to know.
Matthew Walker, Allie and Julian and Neil Anderson
asked about different opinions of alcohol affecting memory,
but others have more herbal questions,
such as Allie Meyers, Adam, Michael James, Storm,
Ciricity, and Olympia Rempel, and first-time question askers Craig Steinberg
and Zach Gehry. And Chris Bullock asks, why does marijuana make memory so shitty,
asking for a friend? Winky face.
Okay, asking for a friend, I see. Well, answering for a friend, I can't say I've
had firsthand experience of this, but I can say that first of all,
there's different kinds of memory that are made shitty to different extents by marijuana.
So it doesn't impact all kinds of memory.
It might impact your recollection for things that you were doing previously or around that
same time.
Not exactly clear why that happens, but I can tell you that one of the things that's really interesting about marijuana is that when you think about how it impacts the brain, there are particular receptors in the brain, what we call endocannabinoid receptors, that are specifically geared to responding to cannabinoids, which is essentially the active species in marijuana. But the interesting thing is that endocannabinoid receptors are involved in long-term potentiation
in memory, in plasticity.
So in some ways, it's not surprising that it impacts your memory.
It's surprising if it's always shitty because I think there's probably an optimality.
I'm not telling you that you should use it to improve memory, but there may be some realm
in which you can actually improve plasticity as opposed to make it worse.
That's very difficult to get at at an individual level.
But the fact that you have receptors in your brain, and especially in your memory systems,
that are specifically geared to responding to the impact of marijuana, I think is a very,
very cool thing.
And it also tells us that we need to invest a lot more energy and a lot more time and
a lot more resources in understanding exactly how it's impacting the brain, how that changes from individual to individual,
on what background and what context with everything else that's happening in the brain.
So recreational use versus use for depression and anxiety, other things, so more sort of
therapeutic uses.
All of those are really interesting questions.
And now that we are seeing the legalization sort of, you know, across many states and a desire from the National Institutes of Health to really support research
on this front, I'm hopeful that we'll be able to have a lot more answers. A lot of my colleagues
here actually are studying this exact thing.
Would there be a difference between the CBD component and the THC component?
Definitely. So they're different chemicals and they have different potency and different binding properties and so on.
I don't know that there's as much evidence for CBD in terms of brain active kinds of
things or psychoactive kinds of things.
There may be a little bit out there, but certainly the impact of THC has been the one that's
studied much more and there's a lot more literature on that.
So, a 2023 paper in the journal Biomolecules titled Effective Cannabis on Memory Consolidation,
Learning and Retrieval and its current legal status in India acknowledges that, quote,
the role of cannabis on cognitive functions is a matter of long debate, but that generally
THC is responsible for cognition-related deficits while non-psychoactive CBD has been shown to elicit
neuroprotective activity.
However, because it's a restricted substance,
there's not enough research on it, they say.
And contradictions exist.
And some reports showed low THC dose improved learning
in cognition.
So I guess keep an eye out for emerging
studies and as discussed in the recent surgical angiology episode on veins and
arteries, smoking is not the best way to ingest it if you're going to. So remember
to protect your blood plumbing for the long term. Oh, patrons Lila Weller,
Carleen D.H., and Barb Miller had questions about the long and the short of it all.
Well, you know, I feel like some people talk about short-term versus long-term when it
comes to that, but the difference between short-term memory and long-term, when does
it become a long-term memory?
When does it get filed?
Yeah.
Great question, Carlene.
I'm going to get ready to bust another myth here.
When I talk to people about long-term and short-term memory, typically they're complaining about their long-term memory
and they're saying, my short-term memory is okay.
So I can remember things from yesterday or the week before,
but it's like long-term memory that's impaired.
Or my mother, if they're talking about maybe their mother
with developing Alzheimer's or early dementia,
it's the opposite, it's her long-term memory is okay.
She can remember her past, her childhood, but everything in the last few years, her short-term memory
is what's impaired. And I kind of have to stop and say, okay, let me just clear up the
terminology so that when you're talking to a physician or talking to somebody, they understand
what you're talking about. Both of those are long-term memory. You're talking about recent
versus remote memories. Short-term memory is very, very short. We're talking the span
of seconds. So that's at least the way that these things are defined in psychology and
neuroscience. Short-term memory is extremely short. It's what we also call working memory.
If I were to give you a phone number, not that you would have to dial a phone number
these days, everybody just, you know. But if I were to give you a phone number and say,
hey, hold on to this phone number and then you have to dial it, you might sort of rehearse that phone number
to yourself for a few seconds and then you dial it and then what happens to that number?
Poof, goes away. So you stored it very briefly in your short-term memory store, your working
memory, which is there to be able to help you store things for a very short period of
time. You can get distracted out of it very quickly. And also, I can exceed your span very quickly
if I just yell out a whole bunch of numbers at you, things are going to fall out, right?
So it's not intended to store any more than just a few, a handful of items. People used
to say seven plus or minus two, but that number is likely closer to three or four.
Okay, he just threw a whole bunch of numbers at you, but seven plus or minus two means
that brain scientists used to think we can hold five to nine items in our short-term
memory, but turns out we don't even have that much room.
It's like three or four things at a time.
Three or four things.
And the reason why phone numbers work is because we chunk them into three bits.
Yep.
So then everything beyond that is actually long-term memory storage. But when we think about memory for yesterday or last week or the month before versus years
ago, we're talking about recent long-term memories versus remote long-term memories.
And as we get older, our memory for things that happened way in the past could be very
preserved because we talked about how memory over time gets strengthened and kind of linked to a whole bunch of different regions of the brain so
it becomes more robust, more resilient to forgetting. Those are typically the last memories
to go, say for a patient experiencing dementia, they're going to remember those memories
much later in the progression. But memories of the last few months, the last year, the
last few years, they're year, the last few years,
they're going to be the ones that are the earliest to go. Because they have not been
solidified as much. They have not been stored in all of these parallel networks in the brain
and made kind of more resilient to forgetting. They're still somewhat dependent on the hippocampus.
And the hippocampus is kind of the culprit in early dementia, right? It's one of the
places that's changing very early. So as we think about long-term versus short-term, that's
typically what people are thinking about. Why am I not remembering as much of the recent
things but I remember memories in the past for longer? We experience that as we get older
but much more dramatically in the context of dementia. Yeah, so it's just a little
bit of a misnomer but I think, you know, being able to kind of divide it
into recent versus remote covers the same question.
Yeah, oh, I had no idea.
And I do want to ask about dementia and Alzheimer's as well,
but a quick detour with short-term memory.
Many people mentioned having the memory of a goldfish.
Jasmine Patino, first-time question asker,
whose dear, wonderful boyfriend is a goldfish
in that capacity, and Kaitlyn Tindale, who compared themselves to a fish cognitively.
True or false, goldfish remember for two minutes and then they don't know why they're in a
bowl.
You know, I'm trying to remember finding Nemo now and think about whether it was truly two
minutes.
You know, I don't know if it's exactly two minutes, but it is very short.
It is thought to be one of the shortest memory spans.
How do they test that on a goldfish?
It's difficult.
I think that whenever you're testing things with animals,
you have to be clever, right?
So you have to figure out a way that the animal
can kind of indicate to you
whether or not they recognize something.
Maybe it's by the amount of exploration or
the amount of time they spend in the vicinity of that thing. If they're more familiar with
it, maybe they'll navigate to something that's newer. So you can position things in the environment
in a way where you can see how much they explore one over the other and be able to tell, oh,
yeah, their memory is shot or maybe their memory is really good.
I love the idea that like, you know this? And they're like, ah, no.
Yep.
The name is not coming to mind.
I remember the face though, anywhere.
But yeah.
We're in luck because yeah, people study this.
Of course they do.
And according to a recent paper, distance estimation in the Goldfish in the Proceedings
of the Royal Society Bee of Biological Sciences journal, Goldfish can accurately estimate
distance after learning it.
And another study in the journal Animals titled Visual Perception of Photographs of Rotated
3D Objects in Goldfish trained six goldfish to tap either a photo of a frog or a turtle
for a treat.
And researchers report that all the fish had successful performance showing that they were
able to distinguish between the turtle and the frog photographs, which
is evidence of object constancy.
So flim flam, goldfish memories are not trash.
They can be trained to do things.
And it should be noted that in five years of having
my daughter, who's a poodle mix named Gremlin,
I have never successfully trained her to do anything.
So lay off the goldfish and lay off yourselves.
Although when it comes to future fears and caring for loved ones,
many patrons had questions about Alzheimer's and dementia,
including Lisa Gorman, Deb Desai, and Stephanie Halfery,
Maddie Cakes, Two Stones with One Chick,
Miae, Meg McDaniel, Ken Edmondson, Erin White, Kamelia B., Brian Reesinger,
Sarah Crocker, and Stephanie who wrote,
there are cases of dementia on both sides of my family.
Is there actually anything we can do to stop
or slow down this awful disease?
A lot of people obviously concerned about dementia,
concerned about Alzheimer's,
that deserves its own two-part episode.
Certainly.
Can you describe when it comes to memory
the difference between dementia and Alzheimer's? Is Alzheimer's a disease and
dementia is the symptom of it or how does it work exactly what's happening?
Yeah, so the easiest way to think about it is that dementia is a larger umbrella
term and Alzheimer's disease is one of the principal causes of dementia.
You're right that dementia is a set of symptoms and Alzheimer's is a little bit more about
the biology that leads to those symptoms.
And there are many other types of dementia.
So Parkinson's disease can lead to dementia.
There's frontotemporal low bar degeneration or frontotemporal dementia.
Huntington's disease can lead to dementia.
So there's a number of different causes for dementia.
But the most prevalent one, the one that most people are really concerned about is Alzheimer's dementia.
So, yeah, dementia is the umbrella term.
Alzheimer's is the subcategory or the set of causes that lead down the path to dementia,
and it's among many others, but Alzheimer's is the chief one.
When we think about how do we differentiate between dementia and, say, you know, healthy
aging, that's another question that pops up a lot
is as I'm getting to a certain age,
I feel like I am losing my memory.
I'm starting to lose my way when I navigate
or I'm having some memory issues and being forgetful.
And some of that happens as we all get older
and the majority of it is okay, right?
That's just the natural part of a normal aging brain.
But when it becomes pervasive and noticeable to not just to the person but to others around
them and people are sort of missing doctor's appointments or getting lost around their
neighborhoods and they're wandering, then it becomes a real concern.
And when that's happening, already things have changed so much in the brain that now
it's really unable to compensate for it
because we tend to compensate so much for any sort of brain deficit for the longest time. So for many
years a patient with Alzheimer's disease wouldn't technically be a patient because they're not
reporting symptoms, they're not experiencing anything. Neither patient nor doctor can say
anything is wrong with the brain but But already the pathology is changing
the brain. And one of the challenges for us in research is trying to develop ways that
we can detect that pathology, maybe with brain imaging, with brain scans and so on, very
early, even at a time when the patient and the doctor don't really know that anything
is wrong. But when it comes to memory deficits experienced at that older age, you know, the question
is always how do I know when it's really kind of tipped over?
How do I know when it's really problematic?
And a good rule of thumb to think about is that if somebody's forgetting things all natural
and fine, especially if they can remember it later on, if they're reminded and they
go, oh, now I remember, so it's tracked somewhere, it's there, you know that it's okay. Maybe that's a challenge as we're getting older,
that just changes to some extent.
But if they're really never able to piece it back together
and no reminder is helping them,
that they may be kind of over that cliff
and they're going down the path to Alzheimer's disease.
The somewhat more crass example is if you forget
where you placed your car keys, it's okay.
But if you forget that you drove the car, then that may be a challenge.
Is that because of plaques in the brain? Is that parts of the brain atrophying into almost
spaces where there used to be more white or gray matter? What's happening biologically?
We used to have this idea and the idea took the field by storm and actually resulted in,
I think, an over investment of resources into clinical trials that try to get rid of those
plaques.
We used to think that plaques were sort of the evil, right?
And the two pathologies of Alzheimer's disease are plaques and tangles.
Plaques are made up of amyloid protein.
Tangles are made up of amyloid protein, tangles are made up of what's
called tau protein. And for the longest time, people thought if you have amyloid and tau
or plaques and tangles in the brain, that's Alzheimer's disease and we should be trying
to break up those pathologies somehow to restore the brain or prevent it from getting worse.
But the reality is the idea never really fully panned out because just having amyloid in
the brain is not sufficient for you to experience memory problems.
It's not sufficient for you to have dementia.
There's about a third of everyone with amyloid in their brain will likely never experience
dementia.
So, clearly by itself, it's not sufficient.
But something else that you mentioned turns out to be sufficient and really important,
which is neurodegeneration, actual atrophy, actual loss of cells. That doesn't happen naturally as
we age. We lose synapses, we lose connections. As we get older, it becomes more difficult to make
them, difficult to maintain them. That happens for sure. But cell loss in these massive amounts
doesn't really happen unless there's a progressive neurodegenerative illness, which is what Alzheimer's
disease is. So when we find evidence of neurodegenerationative illness, which is what Alzheimer's disease is.
So when we find evidence of neurodegeneration, say for example, in MRI scans, we see a very
close connection, a close link or a relationship between the extent of that neurodegeneration
and memory symptoms or memory loss and cognitive symptoms down the line that are not memory
also.
So executive function gets disrupted, all of our cognitive faculties,
because it's progressive. But in the absence of that neurodegenerative change, it's very
difficult to see associations with actual cognitive or clinical decline, which has created
a bit of a dilemma for the field. Even though the FDA has been approving anti-amyloid therapeutics,
these drugs have some nasty side effects. And it's not clear that clearing amyloid therapeutics, these drugs have some nasty side effects. And it's not clear that clearing amyloid is going to be the solution in the long run.
Coupled with that, also the complexity that Alzheimer's disease is extremely heterogeneous.
So if I were to get it, and hopefully not, but if you were to get it, it may look very
different in your brain than it does in my brain.
We may have a lot of variability.
So that means you may respond to a drug or therapy that I may not respond to and vice
versa.
So the idea of like a one size fits all kind of solution is also kind of falling away by
the wayside and people thinking about the complexity of maybe Alzheimer's disease is
actually diseases and we need to be able to tailor therapeutics to the individual.
Obviously dementia once again deserves its own episode.
It's a complex condition with a lot of emerging research.
But there are some pharmaceutical treatments that can involve modulating neurotransmitters in the brain like glutamate.
There are dietary modifications that can ease some related symptoms,
and psychopharmaceuticals that can help with depression and anxiety related
to the progression of dementia.
And Robert G. O'Day and Isabelle LeClerc
mentioned Lewy body dementia in their questions.
And it is the second most common cause of dementia
after Alzheimer's.
And it's caused by aggregations of proteins
in the brain called Lewy bodies.
And some symptoms of it can include visual hallucinations, trouble with sleep, including sleepwalking, mood changes, and stiffness.
And Isabelle, whose mom had asked why public awareness of Lewy body dementia
was low, but there's a little bit of background on it. And we're gonna get to
more questions about how you can avoid dementia and memory loss in yourself per
an expert. But first let's donate to a cause of his choosing. And he opted to
send it to a fund at his center called the Junior Scholars Fund, which goes to
supporting graduate students and their professional development because he says
they are the next generation of talent and he wants to do whatever he can to
support them. And there's a link in the show notes for more on the Junior
Scholars Fund at the UC Irvine Center for the Neurobiology of Learning and
Memory. So thanks to sponsors of the show for making that donation possible.
Okay, back to the million dollar question here, which honestly I think is eating away at everyone listening.
But it was asked by Stephanie, Christina Manouj, Who Loves Saunas, Margaret Anouska, Karat Singh, Shushanja Gettinger, Nicole DG, Carol Young, and Jenny Hoover. So let's go.
Well, I think, you know, last listener question
that we got so much obviously is,
every day I feel like we're all getting older.
It's nuts.
Wow.
Time.
We're gonna get old eventually if we're lucky, right?
What actually does help us stay sharp
and retain our memories?
Is it Sudoku? Is it going to
Zumba classes? Is it reading? Yeah, you know, the challenge of brain aging and
sort of body aging is an interesting one. I think everybody wants to, you know, live
longer and live happier and healthier lives and so on. And the trick is to make
sure that our brain aging is sort of consistent with our body aging. You really want to kind of maintain health across both of those. So mind
longevity is something that we think about a lot. And there are some answers. So we know,
for example, that maintaining levels of physical activity, regular physical activity, hopefully
not just undertaken when you're 80, right? So starting kind of in mid-life and continuing, regular levels of moderate physical activity
seems to be helpful, seems to be preventative.
They are associated with reduced risk for Alzheimer's disease.
If people get it, they tend to get it later in life, but they're protected from it for
some period of time.
So exercise.
And for more, you can see the paper Exercise and Dementia Prevention, from the journal Practical Neurology, which notes that around a third of dementia cases are attributable to modifiable risk
factors such as physical inactivity, smoking, and hypertension.
They say that with the rising prevalence of dementia, there is a renewed focus on prevention
strategies.
And exercise has emerged as a key intervention
for influencing cognition positively,
including reducing the risk of age-related
cognitive decline and dementia.
So use ologies as walking time, dance around your kitchen,
get a dog to walk, take a break dancing class,
do some arm stretches, just keep it moving, folks.
Okay, what else doc?
The other thing that people have identified in these large scale, not just trials, but also
epidemiological studies, what seems to help? Well, social activity. And this was a challenge in the
pandemic actually. And I got to hear a lot about that from folks who felt that they had social
structures and Zoom was no replacement for it. It just did not help as much.
You really wanted to be around people
and being around people in older age is really key.
Now we see this all the time when people retire.
There's sort of a fork in the road.
You are very socially engaged in your job
and other settings and then when people retire,
in some cases they become very isolated
and they're spending a lot of time
at home, maybe with just a 24-hour news cycle and all that kind of stuff, and not going
out and being around people.
And others have planned appropriately and said, these are going to be my post-retirement
plans, I'm going to spend more time in my community center, in my church, in my whatever
it is to maintain that social level of activity.
And those individuals tend to do better cognitively over time.
Wow.
And of course, not everyone is able to get the same levels of physical activity.
And we have an entire episode about disability sociology that discusses accommodations and
attitudes toward disability.
So talk to a doctor or a physical therapist about what you can do and how your activity
level is.
And we also have an episode on chronic pain
with some biopsychosocial interventions
that have helped some folks.
And as this is being released right before the crush
of holiday travel and flu season,
likely a spike in more COVID cases,
it's always good to take precautionary measures
against infection.
If I'm on a plane, I'm in a mask.
So do what's right for you,
but know that doing physical activity that works for you and staying social is incredibly important
for your health and your brain," she said to herself and aside.
So maintaining a good level of physical activity and a good level of social activity, you know,
a great exercise to do both simultaneously is dancing. So we tend to hear about that
a lot. Like if somebody is in a dance group or does dance classes a couple times a week, they are much happier. You know,
you get a lot of endogenous sort of dopamine boosts that happen when you do that. But the
social exposure and the physical activity seem to be key.
Then we go on to other things that people are really curious about. What about brain
games? What about Sudoku? What about this and this and that? And you know, the evidence
there is a little bit shaky, right?
So there's some evidence to suggest
that maintaining sort of cognitive engagement,
of course, is very helpful,
but most of it is based on cognitive engagement,
again, in a social setting, right?
So if you're playing chess out in the park with somebody
and having discussions and all that,
that seems to be more helpful than playing chess
against a computer avatar at home, right?
You might think, well, cognitively it's the same,
I'm playing chess, but it turns out to be different
if you're doing it with a human, right,
and actually having conversations
and actually being out and about.
So again, I go back to the two things
that are kind of tried and true
and I think are very helpful.
The third thing that seems to be helpful
has to do with diet and being able to have
sort of a heart healthy
diet because we know that heart health is really important for brain health.
So maintaining something that looks close to something like the Mediterranean diet,
smaller amounts of red meat and things like that, high levels of leafy greens and fruits
and vegetables and those kinds of things, that also seems to be associated with better
longevity and higher levels of cognition into that longevity.
Those are massive studies that were done where they randomize people to either the Mediterranean diet or sort of a typical American diet,
and they see some really decent results for something like the Mediterranean diet.
But the brain game stuff is the one that I'm sort of tentative about.
It's like, I don't know if it'll hurt you, but I'm not sure it's helping you all that much. My husband's grandfather played a lot of video games in his later years and he would play
multiplayer.
So he would be on the headset with his grandkids across the country while he was playing like
World of Warcraft or League of Legends or whatever.
We've heard that from a number of folks who are involved even in our studies said, I started
playing video games and I started to, and they felt like even the learning experience was a really good thing for them.
And some others just said, nope, not for me.
I don't know what you're talking.
There's no way I could ever do that.
There was just kind of a block.
So while it's challenging, there's no doubt that incorporating the social aspect into
this has made a huge difference.
This is why I was also reluctant to like tell my kids not to play games and do things because
I felt like if they're doing it with their friends and it's sort of communal, then it's a little bit different
than just droning in front of the computer and playing a game by yourself against the
computer.
Yeah. I love that you are, you're not only researching this on the daily, but also seeing
it as your children are growing up and watching how memory might change. But I imagine there
are some difficult things
about your job or studying it.
I always ask what sucks,
what sucks the most about your job?
Oh, what sucks the most?
Let's see.
I think sometimes the pace by which things move
frustrates me.
And in research, you know, I think in science in general, there's this
notion that we're going to do the best science that we can, we're going to put it out there,
we're going to publish our work, and then hope that somebody else is going to come and
take that work and build on it and then be able to translate things and get them to be
helpful to somebody out in the real world. That takes forever. And it's so frustrating
that it takes forever. And so one of the things that I've started to do in recent years and get my lab more
involved in is say to hell with that, we're not going to wait.
We're actually going to try to do it ourselves.
So I've started to bridge a little bit between sort of the academic environment and more
of the industry.
But for the longest time, it sucked.
It felt like we're so removed as academics doing the science, and there's not enough
of that science that's getting out there and helping people, even though it has the potential
to help people.
And then the other thing that's frustrating, I will say, that you can't change everything.
You always want to do what you feel is scientifically very rigorous and also like morally and ethically
right and you have kind of a code by which you operate.
But there are certain systems in place that are really, really difficult to change.
And some of them you can change and you have to kind of figure out who to work with to
make sure the message is communicated very well outside of academia to work with to make sure the messages communicated very well outside
of academia to be able to influence people.
But I'm very fortunate that I'm surrounded by people who are equally frustrated and also
believe that that sucks.
So at least we can kind of riff off of each other a little bit and commiserate slash come
up with ways that we can try to address it.
Well, there's that community aspect too, right?
Exactly.
That's helping your brain.
That's the thing. Also science, you know, when it started out hundreds of years ago,
when people were doing science, it really
was kind of a solo practice.
When you look at Nobel laureates,
it was always sort of singular winners.
And that's not a thing anymore.
Science is so communal now.
It really requires teams and communities of people
that are dedicated to solving these
big, you know, challenging questions.
They're not very simple at all.
They're extremely challenging.
And I think that's one of the reasons why things have been exponentially growing in
recent years, right?
It's not just that we have better technology.
We have smarter people and hordes of them that are dedicated to answering these questions
and doing it together.
So when you look at the number of authors on a paper or the number of co-investigators
on a grant, those numbers have also shot up.
So that community aspect of it, I think, is what keeps a lot of us at the table and it's
what makes it worthwhile despite the occasional sucking.
Well, if someone wanted to go into this field or just curious, what is the best thing about
your job?
Oh my gosh, there's so many things. I mean, I'm like a kid in a candy store most of the time.
I can tell you that for someone who is naturally very curious and inquisitive, this is like heaven,
because you're learning new things every day and it never stops. And there's no real retirement
also for people like me. Like you retire, but you're still on recall, you consult, you do things. You're constantly continuing to learn. So if that's something
that appeals to you, being a constant student, man, science is like the best place to get
into. The other thing also is being an academic, I have incredible freedom to pursue the questions
of my interest and my lab's interest. If tomorrow I decided I wanted to study fruit flies, I will not lose my job. I can do that. I have to go support that effort
somehow. But no one tells me what to study. No one tells me what science to do. I get
to decide that. And I do it communally with my lab because we're all collectively in it.
So we get to decide on the science that we want to do. We get to write the grants and
papers together. So that's liberating. And I don't know of any other job out there where you can decide what you want to do at
any given day and just go do it.
That's incredibly empowering.
You can continue to do what you want to do and continue to be in love with it for as
long as you want to.
This has been such a journey into my own brain and anyone listening. So thank you
for just inspiring us also to treat our brains a little better. You're very welcome. This was so
much fun. Thank you so much. This is great. So once again, ask neuroscientists neurotic questions,
if you are me, and thank you to Dr. Michael Yasa and everyone down in Irvine for helping arrange
this. There's more links to their lab and hisasa and everyone down in Irvine for helping arrange this.
There's more links to their lab and his work.
They're in the show notes and up on our website
at alleyward.com slash nemanology.
And we're at Ologies on Instagram and now Blue Sky,
where everyone seems to be headed.
See you over there.
We also have Smologies, which are shorter,
kid-friendly episodes that you can find anywhere you get
podcasts.
We put them in a new feed so it's easier for parents
and teachers or anyone who's looking for shorter,
clean language versions of Ologies to find them.
You can look for the new green artwork in the Smology's logo.
Thank you also to Erin Talbert for adminning the Ologies
podcast Facebook group.
Aveline Malik makes our professional transcripts.
Kelly R. Dwyer does the website.
Noelle Dilworth is our birthday girl this past week,
and she's our wonderful scheduling producer.
Susan Hale managing directs at all,
Jake Chafee edits and lead editor
and another great brain is Mercedes Maitland
of Maitland Audio.
With some assists the last couple of weeks
from Jared Sleeper of Mind Jam Media
when I'm late on things.
Nick Thorburn wrote the theme music.
And if you stick around until the end,
I tell you a secret.
And this week it's that I have a theory that if you have a good friend who has a party, like they're close buds,
you should either be the first one there to help set up and just kind of like set the
mood so they're not worried about when people are going to start showing up, or you should
be the last one standing to help tidy up and say, Hey man, great party.
Relatedly, I love ice. I love having any cold beverage with an
absolutely egregious amount of ice. So much so that our freezer could not keep up with my ice
consumption. Our ice maker was like, I don't know what to tell you. Now during early quarantine,
there were a lot of cafes that were closing and Jarrett surprised me when I was out of town. I
was helping my dad and he bought an ice machine from a closing cafe.
We have it in our garage and I use it every single day, even in winter. Now, what does this
have to do with parties? So we have become the friends who show up first with a giant bucket of
ice and it feels heroic. So if you are someone who goes to bed early, be the first to show up at a
party, pick up some bags of ice on the way. Everyone will love you forever. You get to bed early. Be the first to show up at a party. Pick up some bags of
ice on the way. Everyone will love you forever. You get to bed early. Okay. Just in general,
be safe out there. Okay? Bye bye. Meteorology, Fepidology, Nephology, Seriology, Selenology.
Let's make some memories, huh?