Ologies with Alie Ward - Oneirology Part 2 (DREAMS) with G. William Domhoff

Episode Date: January 11, 2023

Part 2 is here: Lucid dreaming! Teeth falling out! Medications and dreaming! The source of creativity! Even how to clean your brain. Dr. G. William Domhoff has studied dreams for decades and returns t...o answer an absolute deluge of questions with his wisdom and aplomb. By the end, you’ll know to sleep better, why it’s important, how to relax like a fish, if cheese alters your subconscious, why your dog flaps their paws during naps, and rejoice about a freshly discovered part of the brain. Ooooh, it’s an instant classic.Dr. G. William Domhoff’s book: The Neurocognitive Theory of DreamingHis website: dreambank.netThis episode is wonderfully sponsored by Saatva.com/ologiesDonation will be made to Worldwide Indigenous Science Network and toward dream research at UC Santa Cruz More episode sources and linksOther episodes you may enjoy: Somnology (SLEEP), Chronobiology (CIRCADIAN RHYTHMS), Personality Psychology (PERSONALITY), Molecular Neurobiology (BRAIN CHEMICALS), Attention-Deficit Neuropsychology (ADHD) Part 1 and Part 2, Eudemonology (HAPPINESS), Dendrology (TREES), Thermophysiology (BODY HEAT), Ursinology (BEARS), Cicadology (CICADAS), Opposumology (O/POSSUMS), Chiropterology (BATS)Sponsors of OlogiesTranscripts and bleeped episodesSmologies (short, classroom-safe) episodesBecome a patron of Ologies for as little as a buck a monthOlogiesMerch.com has hats, shirts, masks, totes!Follow @Ologies on Twitter and InstagramFollow @AlieWard on Twitter and InstagramEditing by Mercedes Maitland of Maitland Audio Productions and Jarrett Sleeper of MindJam MediaTranscripts by Emily White of The WordaryWebsite by Kelly R. DwyerTheme song by Nick Thorburn

Transcript
Discussion (0)
Starting point is 00:00:00 Okay, so up top, this episode is exclusively sponsored by Saatva, and this is the first inology's history, the first one we've ever had sponsored by just one company, and it's Saatva Luxury Mattresses, which is perfect because I love Saatvas. If you were looking for a new mattress, trust me, just go straight to saatva.com. They have mattresses with inner springs, they have memory foam, they have adjustable firmness, they have white glove delivery, they have so many good reviews. People love their saatvas, as do I. It's so luxurious, it's so plush. Who knew sleeping can get better? Saatva did. That's s-w-a-t-v-a.com slash oligies. What a dream. Oh, hi. It's still your friend's fiancé who commits everything
Starting point is 00:00:45 to spreadsheets. It's Ally Ward. This is part two of Onorology. We cover so many burning questions in the field of dreams with one of the world's top researchers. Go back to part one and start there. What are you doing? This is part two. Go back. Start with that. Come back here. Okay. Let's get to the good stuff, but really quick, thank you to everyone at patreon.com slash oligies who submitted questions for this. It costs a dollar a month or more to join. Thanks to everyone rating and subscribing and leaving reviews. Also, I read every single one. There were so many lovely ones this week, but I have to shout out Tremendous who shared the show with their husband who looked it up and said that I'm their mom's cousin. Here's the deal. My dad was one of 11 and my mom was one of
Starting point is 00:01:33 six. I have so many cousins who are related to most of Montana, so I'll be honest. I have no idea who Tremendous is. I don't know which cousin's child spouse wrote that, but shoot me a text. People were family. Okay, on neurology, dreams. Tuck yourself in for answers about where creativity comes, neuroimaging the imagination, creepy tooth dreams, foods and dreams, mental health and dreaming, relaxed fish, medications and your sleep, lucid dreaming, a newly discovered part of the brain. What sleep stages are important for cleaning out your brain, your dog's naps and more with author, researcher, psychology and sociology professor, dream expert and onerologist, Dr. G. William Domhoff.
Starting point is 00:02:43 Can I ask a few questions from listeners, from people who listen? Are they listening now? No, not right now, but they know that you're coming on. Oh, I told them. Jesus Christ, I'm sitting here first thing. No, you're right. Oh, up there, hot mic as they say. No, not a hot mic at all. Okay, this inquiry was burning a hole in the minds of Hannah Noose, Dantooine, Maisie Lopez, Corey, Jess Swan and first-time question askers, Andy Cornaccia, Trevor Durning, Marvette Fudge, Jess Nan and Travis Gibson,
Starting point is 00:03:17 who all needed info on whether our dreams have any meaning. What's the point? So yes, everyone needed to know that straight out of the gate. Okay, a ton of people want to know, essentially, Joe Mueller wants to know, do things really have meaning in dreams like seeing certain objects or the recurring dream of teeth falling out? So many people have the dream that their teeth are falling out, which seems weird. Do they have any meaning? First of all, people do have certain common dreams like flying under their own power or teeth falling out, but they're very rare. In other words, in your whole dream life,
Starting point is 00:04:00 and we've studied this by, like we have two-week dream journals from students, and 1% of them, four dreams out of hundreds will have teeth falling out, and certain individuals will have it. But if we all talk about it, this is where the whole not being rigorous and in the lab and really not projecting things on your participants, we call it creating demand characteristics and creating expectations. And also, if you've heard a lot that people have dreams that their teeth fall out, pretty soon you think you have one, or you haven't. But they're very rare, but they are the kind of dreams that we really studied a lot to try to get to this symbolic meaning. But I honestly don't know what the symbolic
Starting point is 00:04:50 meaning of that is in dreams. If there is any symbolism, there could be symbolism for Freudians. It's castration, I think, teeth falling out. Patrons Mallory Skinner, Kat, and first-time question askers, Kristen Robb, Justin Goodheart, and Leanne Murray, as well as me, your father, have all had nightmares of teeth rotting out of our skull and just falling out like overripe peaches from a tree. What does Domhoff say about this? But for your teeth listeners, your teethy listeners, go on dreambank.net. Pick, we've got it limited so the thing doesn't crash, pick two dream series at a time and put in the word teeth. Teeth, put teeth and tooth. Put it in or mode, or so it'll be tooth or teeth, and in
Starting point is 00:05:43 three seconds it will tell you how many dreams have a tooth in it. And it'll give you a tooth column and a teeth column. So if you put in tooth, teeth, gums, tongue, mouth, you'd get a column for each one and you'd get what's called a contingency. Let's take a little jaunt down the teeth dreams timeline of life to get to the bottom of it, shall we? I need to. Okay, so in his 1900 publication, The Interpretation of Dreams, one Dr. Sigmund Freud said that dental dreams are mental dreams and they represent issues with things like castration and repressed sexual urges and the compulsive desire to pleasure oneself. So I went to make a withdrawal from drdomhoffstreambank.net and I looked into his archive to find that yes, the percentage of the 20,000 collected dreams that even mention tooth
Starting point is 00:06:43 or teeth is very low. It's less than a percent. And some of those are just mentioning a toothbrush and not even tooth trauma. I read one dream account from a participant identifying as Barb who dreamt on March 2, 1981, quote, we go to the party. I'm wearing a long, formal gown and I have one high-heeled shoe on and one off. I walk on tiptoes with the right foot and hope no one notices my missing shoe. There is a buffet table, lots of high-class, snotty women. A tooth with a feather on it falls out of my mouth onto the table. I'm embarrassed. I say nonchalantly, oh pardon, very French-like and classy. I pick it up. It reminds me of an engagement ring. I feel the empty place with my tongue. I realized there had been another one that fell out sometime earlier.
Starting point is 00:07:34 Why did Barb dream this? Also, Barb, I would party with you. You sound fun as hell to be a mess with. Now, I've heard that these dreams, these kind of oral embarrassment and horror dreams, mean some fear of a loss of control. But I looked up a 2012 study to get some stats. It's called Dream Motif Scale. It was published in the journal Dreaming. And it provided some numbers on our fucked-up teeth dreams. And despite my assumption that 100% of everyone has these dreams, the researchers report that only 39% of those studied had what they called a TD, which stands for Tooth Dream, at least once. 16% of people reported that their TD were recurrent, and 8% were like, I have these all the damn time, doctor, why, why, why?
Starting point is 00:08:25 And thankfully for us, there is a 2018 study that answers that. And it's called Dreams of Teeth Falling Out, An Empirical Investigation of Physiological and Psychological Correlates. Thank God. Okay, so the authors of this preface by saying teeth dreams are enigmatic, because they don't fall under the rubric of the continuity hypothesis, which just means dreaming about normal shit that happens every day. But again, why? Okay, this is huge. Hold on to your molars. Their findings supported the dental irritation hypothesis, which means you dream of teeth falling out when you have a dental problem. That's it. Sometimes months before it actually gives you any problems. So maybe you're due for a crown replacement, or you should slow down on
Starting point is 00:09:12 the white strips. But let's get to the cooler dreams. But there's a better one that's tantalizing, and that is flying, because up is good in our thinking. When we look at all the work that's been done on meaning in waking life, up is good, down is bad, left is bad, right is good, this kind of stuff. But flying, think of all the metaphors. I'm walking on air. I'm high as a kite. I hope they don't prick my balloon and I fall. So we express elation through height, through flying. I'm walking on air. Over the moon. Over the moon, the whole thing. Right, cloud nine. We're so high right now. And that's the kind of temptation. It just makes sense to think those dreams must be symbolic too. In other words, we have what one researcher calls a waking state bias.
Starting point is 00:10:06 So it lures us into putting more into the dream that's there. Going back to that hypothetical, if I have 150 of your dreams, I think I could know a lot about you. A whole lot. Would I know something you don't know? I don't think so. What I'd know is what I would learn from you if I could sit down with you and say, I would like to have an anonymous interview with you for research purposes. I wish answers honestly as you can. And I ask you what your feelings are towards your mother, what your feelings are towards your two sisters. What are some of your regrets? What are some of the things you worry about? I think that I would then learn from that interview what I've already learned from your dream.
Starting point is 00:10:55 If I wanted to know what I can learn from your dreams, I think I would just interview you for an hour in an anonymous, honest interview. And so that means that dreams have meaning. Dreams have personal meaning. Occasionally they'll have cultural meaning. For example, in societies, they're hunting and gathering societies, they dream more of animal flow and behold. That's a cultural kind of difference. And of course, for every member of every indigenous society all over the world, there is a different relationship with dreaming. But Professor Domhoff also notes in his most recent book, The Neurocognitive Theory of Dreaming, that his research has found unsurprisingly that the dreams of people in indigenous societies
Starting point is 00:11:39 more often feature them as the victims of aggression, which just mirrors the very tragic reality both historically and presently. And a paper on the plane's vision quest paradigm in the journal American Indian noted that in native and indigenous contexts, there's typically, quote, no separation between the world as dreamed and the world as lived. It continues that in non-indigenous culture, the distinction between waking and dreaming is largely a consequence of culturally reinforced theories of mind that have resulted in a bifurcated worldview for most Euro-Americans. So modern Western culture separates them a bunch. Now, the non-profit organization Worldwide Indigenous Science Network has also started
Starting point is 00:12:23 doing what they call dream work, which is collecting dream journals and providing a network to share the role of dreams in their lives. But what about in our futures? So patrons Amy Naramatsu, R.J. Deutsch, Emily Stoffer, Kara B., Bryn M. Peva, Victoria Edding, Zoe, first-time question-askers, Yfana Chimatzko, Taylor Clinton, Nan, F.L. Rabbit, Ariana O'Connor, and Kailin Bennett all wanted to know about that dreamy nostalgia feeling and deja vu and about nocturnal premonitions. What has Professor Damhoff seen in his decades of research? So dreams have meaning, but I don't think they have the profound or symbolic, prophetic, any of those kind of meanings which have been attributed to them in almost every culture,
Starting point is 00:13:12 because there are some cultures actually who really don't give a damn about dreams, but there's a whole lot that do, and they are ones that then we can learn from because they have dreams of prophetic, they use dreams to decide where to go on a hunt, and so on. You have to have certain dreams to enter certain professions like warrior, you have to have certain dreams before you be initiated into manhood, and then of course just like our society then they have ways to induce that dream. And the role of consciousness altering plant medicine goes deep, deep, deep into history, and for more on that I will link a 2020 paper from the Journal of Psychedelic Studies called The Role of Indigenous Knowledge in Psychedelic Science, and that comes hot out of the gates
Starting point is 00:13:58 acknowledging how much colonialism and past scientific research have excluded indigenous knowledge and not given credit where it's due. And essentially it says that in some cultural frameworks psychedelics aren't just for tripping balls, they're a tool used in concert with our brain's own mechanisms to use these altered states of consciousness to our benefit and our growth, so mushrooms, they are not just for enjoying the lights at the next meeting of the juggalos, but our dreams, hallucination, adjacent, first-time question-askers Kevin, Parachan, Sophie Fournier, and Wendy Lockhart wanted to know. What happens is that all gets blurred and people talk about altered states of consciousness. And
Starting point is 00:14:38 so suddenly they're saying dreams are like hypnotic states or like drug states and so on. What the neuroimaging research reveals is that hypnosis is not like psychedelics or hallucinations or like dreaming. In other words, every one of those states has a different network supporting it, and the dreaming network is different from all of those altered states of consciousness networks. And so one of my strong claims, of course, gets me a lot of trouble, is to say you can't learn anything about dreams by studying hallucinations or hypnotizing people or studying psychedelic drug states. That doesn't teach you about dreaming. Dreaming is a normal everyday occurrence in virtually everyone that we know of. There may be a few people that don't dream,
Starting point is 00:15:37 and it'd be great if neuroimaging would study them because I do think there might be a few. But at any rate, I think that we have to understand a dream. We first of all have to be sure that the mental activity is not sleep talking or from a brief arousal or from sleep paralysis. And then study those dreams. Then we could make comparisons. But to try to understand dreams on the basis of psychosis, which is what a famous neurologist of the mid-19th century said, give me the secret of dreams and I will tell you the secret of psychosis. Of course, then Freud quoted that with great approval. And famous philosophers have said that. But important people have said that. Yeah, we believe it, right? What about Professor Domhoff? I don't anymore,
Starting point is 00:16:33 if I ever did. What about dreams and sleep quality? Victoria Edding wants to know, if I remember a lot of my dreams one night or if they're vivid, does that mean I got good or bad sleep? And Jenna Kinalana wanted to know, how do stressful dreams or nightmares affect the quality of the sleep you're getting? Do you have a good cozy sleep? Do you have better dreams? I think I can say with confidence that people that are in really anxiety states that are really highly aroused in some way, whether through drug states or tensions or whatever it may be, that's going to affect sleep quality and it then may affect dreams. It's not the dreams that are, it's not bad dreams that are causing it. It's vice versa. So patron Dominique McDermott asked
Starting point is 00:17:23 why they feel more tired after a night of intense dreams. But according to Dr. Domhoff, it's not your nightmares messing up sleep, it's the tension and discomfort you already have that are giving you the bad dreams. Again, sleep hygiene, coziness, temperature control, stress mitigation can all give you better sleep, which will help you have a better tomorrow. But some types of stress go way deeper than others. Here's where PTSD victims, God love them, it's just a tough thing. But when they're studied with neuroimaging or EEGs, their brains day and night are just really activated. They're on fire. So the brain of a person sleeping, they're not sleeping like a non-PTSD person, their brain is fired up, they're vigilant,
Starting point is 00:18:16 one of the attention networks also has a vigilance aspect to it. And that's the vigilance network, you know, right now we're paying attention, we're in this particular room, we can hear sound, but if we suddenly heard a loud noise or if I noticed out the window that the building was falling, we would totally change. Everything in our body, our digestion would stop, this would stop. Every bit of energy would be mobilized just on that particular focus and I'd be no more imagining or thinking, I'd be totally a kind of focus. That salience part of the attention network is still on in these PTSD victims. And so it really, really does distort their sleep. And the brain is then much more activated. And they're going to have more dreams because they're
Starting point is 00:19:08 more activated. And because they've got incredibly deep and burned in personal concerns, they're going to dream about those things. Now, contrary to the view that though people just keep dreaming the same nightmare, they don't really, we have the dreams on Dream Bank, we have the dreams of Vietnam vet. And he's quite a guy that was a medic in the Cambodian Highlands in 1970 or 71. He saw horrific, unbelievable human slaughter, death. His dreams are full of fright. And he's one of the few people that could write them all down and they kept them all his life. He's probably 70 now. And his dreams are just constantly vigilant, constantly agitated, constantly, you know, somebody comes in there and he's talking to an old friend and all of a sudden
Starting point is 00:20:02 he sees somebody on the corner right turn and just gets fiercely angry and goes at him or they get in a conflict. So the vigilance that's in his dreams. But I would know that if I talk to him because he tells me and he writes stories and he has a website and he puts his poems and thoughts up there. And I once saw a lecture he gave at high school. I'd laugh but it was so sad because he went in and told them what it was like. They, I mean, just scared to be Jesus out of them. I mean, it was overwhelming. And so even the accounts we often give of these things, we soften them for people. If we're really, say what the horrors were like, it's so overwhelming for other people. So you don't, you don't say them. But he said them. And I think PTSD then is the example on this
Starting point is 00:21:01 because it's so different from normative dreaming. And it's more like than the answers to these questions that where these people are temporarily in that kind of agitated state. Well, the interesting thing that we found with people with any kind of diagnosis and it's not totally certain, but the interesting finding which shows how important it is to use a scientific coding system, the main way their dreams differ is there are no friends in their dreams, no friends, no people that they call friends. In one case or two that we studied as well as the individual dream series, it was only their parents and their sister. They had no friends. In another study where you had dreams from, I think, I think made 106 dreams from Schizophrenics and it's on dreamresearch.net
Starting point is 00:22:04 under interesting findings. In their dreams, there's just all strangers. In other words, there's no family and no friend. So if I was reading through a dream series and I got about five, ten dreams into it and nobody has been mentioned as a friend, I immediately, and I tend to go up, I'm wondering, why are there no friends? And then as I'm reading, and now I'm coding too, I'm counting friends, animals, characters, mother, father, or just guy only dreams about his father, never his mother, what's going on. And then we study it more seriously either with our coding system or we create a word string. And that's why I know people basically dream about things they're familiar with 75, 70, 75 percent of the time. But here I should say that I am no
Starting point is 00:22:56 clinician of any kind, whether psychological or medical. And if people do write to me at my website and ask about these things, some of these things, and I say, look, you've got to go to the sleep disorder clinic and you've got to have expert medical attention, ask them, but you shouldn't ask me. You shouldn't get your advice, I don't think, from anybody on the web, but I could be wrong on that. But you sure shouldn't get any advice from me. But a clinician can help reduce the waking anxiety, which can help reduce the nightmares. And some, you know, some kinds of drugs sometimes. And certainly psychotherapies of various kinds can help people. So we all need someone to lean on.
Starting point is 00:23:47 I love him. Well, I wanted to ask about that because we did have a lot of folks who wanted to know about different substances, SSRIs or melatonin or spicy foods, any brain medications, especially things that are sleep aids, do they change how we dream? My statement is on this, is that any time if we go on a drug or if we go off a drug, in other words, we change our neurochemical, our biochemistry, then those things, you know, may happen. And there may then be an adjustment. But again, I'm no medical expert. And, you know, it's one of those things that I look at as a possible window for me into understanding dreams.
Starting point is 00:24:33 So if you're getting some help from medication, you are also not alone. Hi, let me introduce you to my own brain buddy, FXOR. But patrons, Mariah McGregor, Caitlyn Schmitz, Caitlyn Ramirez, and Becky Potruff and first time question asker, Olympia Sylve, wanted to know what is up with antidepressants and vivid dreams. And the deal is they're not necessarily causing the dreams, but they're suppressing REM sleep. And in the case of Lexapros, Olaf's and Balta, Paxil and a few others, it can mean that you might be having these micro awakenings and remembering the dreams more. Also, stress and sadness in your waking hours can mean it's on your mind more in your sleep. So addressing fears or concerns in therapy or with lifestyle changes
Starting point is 00:25:16 that might benefit your mental health may improve things while you're sleeping. Also, those side effects of really vivid dreaming on antidepressants are apparently the worst in the first few weeks. But you can definitely ask a doctor about timing the medication differently, which could help. I just started taking mine at night and it's helping me function better in the morning. So there's that. Also, melatonin may give you bonkers dreams if the dose is too high. So experiment with that. Timothy Wang, who asked, and melatonin is also connected to a neurochemical called vasotosin, which kind of helps erase your memory of dreams so that you don't get confused between them and reality. So if you're on a medication that's blurring those lines, Earl of Grammlekin
Starting point is 00:25:57 asked about Chantex, that might be why. But is your bong bogarting your dreams? Maybe a little bit. And I'm sorry to report the THC has been shown to repress REM sleep. Evan Davis, Ashley Adar, and Ellie Wheeler. So if you have a medication that's affecting your sleep, first time question askers, Laura Rayfield and Katie Gerasovich, talk to your doc about changing your timing, maybe try to cultivate the best sleep hygiene you can manage and treat your brain well. But enough about pills. Let's talk about cheese. So Scott Sheldon, Paul Smith, Lucla Femina, Stephanie Lesky, and Francesca Pirelli wanted to know in Francesca's words, is it true that eating cheese before bed makes dreams more intense?
Starting point is 00:26:36 And I had to look into this for us. I knew you needed to know. Here's the deal. So a 2005 study showed that blue cheese gave people vivid dreams and cheddar made them dream about celebrities. And it's not important that this was a tiny study or that it was funded by the British Cheese Board, which is a great name for a charcuterie-related propaganda machine, the Cheese Board. I love that. But yes, so further studies have disproven that as just delicious flim flam. Now, the reason that you may actually be swept away to dreamland in a tide of fondue is because one, cheese in Europe is usually the last meal of the evening. So it's a scapegoat cheese. And two, eating late at night can cause your temperature to rise and mess up your sleep,
Starting point is 00:27:26 making you wake up more to remember your weird dreams. And then also three, lactose baby. Not all of us can digest that shit. And guess what? Having a 3 a.m. bubbling colon is a nightmare in every fashion. And also, Julia Fisher, the chocolate you're eating before bed, isn't necessarily a dream inducer, but the caffeine may be interrupting your sleep and making you remember your dreams. Sydney Tubes, Rachel Kendrick, Scott Sheldon, Christina Johnson, and Crystal Simons, try eating earlier if your evening meal or fourth meal is causing you some nocturnal stress. Just consider what a 3 a.m. El scorcho gordita does to your butthole. Now imagine that's your brain trying to sleep. You know what I mean? I give a couple lectures back when I taught
Starting point is 00:28:13 on nightmares. And for instance, people have nightmares when they have high fevers. That was going to be my next question. It was your question, actually, Pascal Perron, Miranda Harder, and Ellie's Wable, who asked about cooking up wild brain activity. Let's say with fever dreams, does a different part of your brain activate when the temperature is high or is that a hallucination more? Questions around brain temperature are still not fully understood. And so it's in the realm of not guesswork, but of still work in progress. And I happen to be very interested in it academically because brain temperature is related to level of activation. When we're highly activated, we're metabolizing better. Cells are working more efficiently. So that really then relates to a
Starting point is 00:29:08 lot of energy use. When we're really highly activated, we're metabolizing all ourselves, metabolizing really fast. We're using up a lot of energy. And that gets to then sleep. A few folks wanted to know why physiologically we dream. I'm looking at you, Lauren Cooper, Hannah Johnson, Bethany Barlow and Cygni. Are we defragmenting our drives? Is it like cleaning up after a party? In Sleepy John's words, dreaming is how the brain cleans out the lint filter churrosos. Here's the strange thing about sleep. For decades, centuries, people have looked for sleep as somehow resting something, or it's getting rid of poisons and toxins, or it's time to lay in stores of kind of energy. Those age old concerns are still being studied. But nobody has
Starting point is 00:30:04 done studies that, and this is among a community of serious sleep researcher, where they all come to agree. So folks are working on it, but it takes a lot of research for a scientific community to stand behind something. So we don't know if dreams specifically clean your brain, but we do know that sleep itself does. There have been plenty of studies that during sleep, your body's cerebral spinal fluid gets into your brain's nooks and crannies and washes away beta amyloid and tau proteins that act as plaques, which can lead to the development of Alzheimer's and dementia. So sleepy time can save your mind. But check this out, kiddos. So literally this week, a study titled a mesothelium divides the subarachnoid space into functional compartments, published in the
Starting point is 00:30:53 Journal of Science reported that there's a new part of the brain we didn't know about until this week. This is like finding a hidden room in your basement, or I guess the attic. We didn't know. There's a membrane in your brain. They are now calling the subarachnoidal lymphatic like membrane, or SLYM. I don't know if that's pronounced SLYM or slim or slime. I hope it's slime because the neuroscientists who discovered it think it might help separate the clean cerebral spinal fluid from the dirty stuff and once again help to clean your brain. What else did they say? So they said that deep non-REM sleep is the most important stage for brain cleaning and that sleep is critical to the function of the brain's waste removal system. And this study shows that the deeper the sleep,
Starting point is 00:31:44 the better. Help bonkers, that this is fresh information we literally did not know last week. What an exciting time to be alive and asleep. And so it's a genuine puzzle for them, but there's one thing I think we do know about sleep. Sleep is a state of adaptive inactivity. Adaptive inactivity. What does that mean? Well, seeds, think of the seeds for a tree. They can be dormant for hundreds or even thousands of years. Trees lose their leaves. Insects go underground, 17-year locus. Torpor, which is a kind of really beyond sleep where body temperature goes much lower. Now for more on nature's ability to go offline, you can see the dendrology episodes on trees, the somnology episode on sleep,
Starting point is 00:32:42 we've got a molecular neurobiology episode on brain chemicals, and the thermophysiology episode on body heat, or also the ursinology episodes on hibernating bears. Anyway, what sleep does is allows us to use less energy and at the same time not expend energy. So every animal sleeps differently for differing amounts of time relating to its ecological niche. Let me give you two examples. On the one hand, bush elephants in Africa, they sleep as little as two hours in 24, and they can be moving for two, three days without sleeping. And when they stop, they don't immediately, quote, make up any sleep. So they're really adapted to that little, that niche. On the other hand, opossums sleep 19 hours, and a kind of bat sleeps 20 hours. Now the bat that sleeps
Starting point is 00:33:46 20 hours is really interesting. When does it come out? The dusk. And what's going on at dusk? The moths are flying around. Its food supply is flying around. So it does everything in four hours, whereas the elephant is awake 22 hours, which means that some animals have to fuel themselves for longer stretches just to maintain homeostasis and keep their brain and their body temperature up. But think of it this way. If you had to be awake 24 hours a day, that's a lot of foraging. That's a lot of hunting. That's a lot of farming. That uses up a lot of food. It's far more evolutionarily adaptive. If we take one third of the roughly one third, but it's really less off, there can be more of us, more food supply and so on. So each creature is adapted. We are actually probably adapted,
Starting point is 00:34:45 and this will surprise you, to seven hours a night. Really? How do I know? Yeah, my friend knows. My UCLA sleep expert friend, he studied three different hunting and gathering societies with the whole crew of anthropologists and others that he helped organize. Now for more details, you can see a 2018 study titled Sleep Variability and Nighttime Activity Among Trimani Forager Horticulturists by lead author Dr. Gandhi Yatish, who is doing a postdoc at UCLA's Center for Sleep Research. But researchers over the years studying the sleep habits of hunter-gatherer cultures and agro-pastoralists observed that. They average seven hours a night, but it's actually, they sleep seven hours a night roughly in the winter, but in the summer they sleep six hours.
Starting point is 00:35:38 So, and that's true of like reindeer, now jump, reindeer, they're way up there in a cold, cold Arctic. Hey, they're sleeping a lot less in the summer, and they sleep a lot more in the winter. So this is enormous complexity to sleep that has to do with ecology. And with us, what's striking about us, and where dreams can come in again, is that all, basically all other primates go to sleep when it gets dark, and they wake up when it gets light. Yeah, why don't we do that? Why don't I do that specifically? Yeah, well chimpanzees sleep 11 or 12 hours, whatever it is. We sleep basically seven or eight, because we are not tied to the light-dark cycle. We are tied more to a temperatures, our internal temperature cycles are not tied to the light cycles. So
Starting point is 00:36:37 when it gets dark, everywhere in the world, the human beings huddle together and make a fire, and they shoot the bull, and they talk about dreams, and they talk about their myths, and they hang out, and they have a little fun and dance around, and we wake up, and then our body temperature goes way down during the night, and here's the other part. The brain temperature can't go down, and that's REM sleep. REM sleep is a thermometer. It's most likely, my bet is on the sleep researchers who say REM sleep is a way to reheat the brain periodically during sleep. That's its function, and it makes it so sleep can go on. We cycle between REM and non-REM, we go non-REM, then REM, heat up a little, REM, non-REM, heat up a little, and then towards morning,
Starting point is 00:37:43 the circadian rhythm, we're built into us, our brain temperature starts to go up independent of REM, very likely, as we approach morning. So now we have a dual system going on that's heating our brain. How does all this relate to dreams? If dreams are tied to the level of brain activation, then the point is that when you're an hour or two into sleep, the default network, the parts of the default network that are active during sleep, there's not enough activation for it to stay together. The network breaks apart. Front part breaks off from the back part. It's the simplest language we could talk about, posterior and anterior and so on, but that's not. At that point, if we awaken people, they might have an image or they, oh,
Starting point is 00:38:42 I thought X or say no, I don't remember a thing, but during the REM period, you awaken them, they'll get a dream. Here's what we know. Probably from six, let's say you're an eight o'clock waker-upper, so from six o'clock on, your brain is doing quite a bit of dreaming. What's important about that is that from our studies in the lab, way in the past, we know that the dreams from the first REM period do not differ from the dreams from the second REM period or the third REM period. My dissertation was one small step in that. Two or three better studies came along. They confirmed a lot of what I wrote, luckily, but there were other things where I was wrong on and better data, bigger sample sizes. We can collect a good sample of your dreams
Starting point is 00:39:39 between, I say six and eight in the morning. Furthermore, we can get a lot of your dreams on spontaneous morning awakening. When we do studies where we have people, I did a study in 1963, just as there came to be answering machines on telephone, I bought a couple of those and I had the students phone in any time they remember to dream. Most of them were morning awakenings, 75 or so percent. That's what's predicted in other studies too, but sometimes you're just sitting there, like one woman in the study, she was sitting in the backyard, she was studying, and in the sun, and the sun started to shift and she started to feel a little cold. Her mind drifted off and she remembered a dream about skiing. We have those kinds of things then that
Starting point is 00:40:35 happened. The point is that we can get a good sample of dreams in a variety of ways. For any future dream researchers out there, the last three pages of my new book explain exactly what we need to do, but that nobody is doing, and it has to start with your cell phone. We got to have every, we got to have samples of people from about age nine or 10, and we didn't get into this, but we don't dream often or well until we're nine to 11 years old, which will rattle a lot of cages too. If you have smallgites, more on kids sleep in a bit. But at any rate, if we had people phone in, their dreams anytime day or night, we have the voice to text, and then we have the text in a central place, and we put them on the dream bank and we can search them with word strings. We can
Starting point is 00:41:32 go to town, we can automate this thing and churn out so much. All we need is about a million or two million a year. That's it? For about four years, and we could figure out which theories were right and which were wrong. That's it. That's all you need to get more, what is it, psychopumpologists? Yeah, we'll escort you to the world of dreams. Now, we will escort you through more of your questions in a moment, but first let's donate again to some relevant causes, and this week we're going to send it to two. One is the Worldwide Indigenous Science Network that we mentioned earlier, which creates spaces for ethical collaboration between indigenous and western ways of knowing. We're also going to donate to the research that Dr. Bill Domhoff has dedicated
Starting point is 00:42:22 his whole career to and efforts to further understand who dreams, what, and why. Also, the day after part one aired, I got the sweetest text from Bill just saying that he had a great time chatting, and that I did my homework well. Oh, and then he included an emoji of a bed and said, the emoji above is an honor of Satva as a perfect sponsor. So, it's Domhoff approved, and on that note, those donations were made possible by the one and only sponsor of these episodes, Satva. Today's episode is exclusively sponsored by Satva because one's got to sleep to dream. And Satva luxury mattresses are just like a first-class ticket to Snooztown. Love them. And usually I save my secrets for the end of the episode, but as long as we're on the topic,
Starting point is 00:43:07 this one time I had a dream. Once I arrived to this party and I was wearing like one of those high-necked Victorian dresses that like sad ladies step off of a train in a dusty mining town, and I arrived at this saloon, and like all of my relatives were there, and they were drinking warm beer, and I leaned over to my mom, Fancy Dancy, and I was like, wow, I want to party. What's the occasion? And she was like, it's your wedding. And I looked up and my boyfriend at the time was waiting for me at the top of the aisle, and I was like, shit, I don't even drink beer or like him like that. And I was trying to figure out if there was a way I could like dip out at the back. And then when I woke up in real life, I was on vacation with this boyfriend, and I
Starting point is 00:43:59 unfortunately knew that he was not the one. So thanks, Dreams. You saved this man from dating me any longer. And also you saved us a wedding and a divorce. Years later, I'm now hitched to someone I absolutely adore, but I still don't like beer. Anyway, Dreams, you got to love them. But before you can dream, you got to fall asleep. And the best way to do that is on a Satva luxury mattress. Every Satva is designed for better sleep with features only found on the world's like nicest, most expensive mattresses. They are high, high quality. And what's more, Satva will set up your mattress in the room of your choice and take your old one for no extra charge. And if you think you're going to get that from one of those mattress and a box companies,
Starting point is 00:44:40 you're dreaming. Also, just a side note, I love the people behind Satva. Shout out to all the folks and crafting your mattresses to order in Queens, New York and Austin. Hello. So treat yourself to a mattress worthy of your rest and get a good deal. Thanks to allergies. Right now, save $200 when you purchase $1,000 remort at satva.com slash allergies. That's S-A-T-V-A.com slash allergies. Thank you, Satva. You truly make me sleep easy. Okay, let's get back to explaining the imaging advancements that help us understand the dream world. But the 1990s changed everything because of neuroimaging. And these neuroimaging studies showed for the first time the parts of the brain that were active during dreaming. And it wasn't really expected that parts of
Starting point is 00:45:35 the brain would not be active. For example, there was one very famous theory that said dreams were just a reaction to these random electrical charges called spikes that came from the brain stem. And these electrical charges ended up in the visual cortex. And then our visual cortex saw these spikes and said, what the hell does that mean? And so tried to make sense out of them. That was his theory of dream called activation synthesis. Well, now we know that part of the brain isn't even active. The whole theory was turned out to be wrong anyhow. But the point is, that kind of neuroimaging studies just said goodbye to that theory, which happens a lot of times in science. New technologies come along and things are solved. Professor Domhoff recounts the work of Dr. Ralph
Starting point is 00:46:24 Wrighton, who along with Ward Halstead used neuroimaging in the 1990s on people with brain injuries to develop what is known as the Halstead Wrighton Neuropsychological Battery, which is just another word for a lot of tests. He had a questionnaire and a questionnaire said, has your injury influenced your dreaming? That's all it said. And if they said yes, then said, how's it influenced? And then he would interview them. And some of them say, well, I don't see the pictures anymore. And some would say, I don't dream anymore. He then matched that up with the kind of x-rays we had at that time. And he was able to show there are certain places that if you have a lesion in the brain, you will lose dreaming. And there's two different
Starting point is 00:47:09 places like that. And other places, and this relates back to mental imagery, if you have injuries in these secondary visual cortices, you won't see in dreams, you lose your mental imagery. I lost it. Now, that becomes exciting because we're saying, wow, that's a connection then with, and you also don't see images. You don't have good mental imagery in waking either. And that was done in a lab study by the same great cognitive psychologist, David Fawkes, who did the developmental studies. And then around the turn of this century, a huge development occurred. And it wasn't just having your thong underwear show over your low-rise genes. In 2000, 2001, the world changed, I think, in terms of understanding the human mind. And that is, the neuroimaging studies accidentally,
Starting point is 00:48:01 in a way, discovered the imagination network, which was called the default network. So you've got the person all hooked up, they're participant, they're all ready to go into the neuroimaging machine. You say, okay, we're just fine-tuning things, just relax. Then they notice, what's the record look like when we just told them to relax? And it looked the same in everybody. And it was, you know, different from what you usually see. First, there's the task network. That's where we're getting you all hooked up so you can see whether you see a yellow lemon or a green pig or whatever it may be. There you have to pick this brown thing out of seven different colors. Where our executive network is really common in the visual image, you know, we're always really
Starting point is 00:48:47 focused. But when we're relaxed, we go into a different network. And so they call it, well, that's the default network, the non-task network. Well, it's a crummy name, because it trivializes that it's the inner you, it's the imaginations, the semantic memory bank, it's the self-network, it's the imagination network. What these neuroimaging studies found on patients whose dreams had changed was, if you get a lesion in your primary visual cortex, it doesn't affect your dreaming, nor in anywhere in your executive network, it doesn't affect your dreamy at all. And so when I realized that both the neuroimaging and the lesion network showed the same parts of brain were necessary for dreaming. So there goes, you got dreams
Starting point is 00:49:35 coming from the same network, it creates imagination. How exciting is this? So the part of the brain making all this magic happen, dreams and imagination, it's called the default network. And Bill describes it in the 2015 paper, Dreaming and the Default Network, a review synthesis and counterintuitive research proposal. And the default network doesn't sound like a lot's going on. It sounds like your printer on standby, but do not let the name fool you. So, so many patrons asked something along the lines of creativity and absurdity in dreams. Where is this coming from? I'm looking at you, Gaelic Pearl, MoFo, Sean Thomas, Kane, Mary C, Meg Ahern, Heather D, Van Valkenberg, Emily Stouffer
Starting point is 00:50:18 and first time question askers, Samantha Jackson, Jonah Landau, Katie Pikes, Kylie Chapman, Eliza Miller, Archie Tidelli, Kelsey Kaudabek, Heidi M, Shelby and Brie Stewart, and Maxine Lewis, who in their words wants to know why their mind invented a scenario of trying to put wheels on a kangaroo using pipe cleaners and empty thread rolls. I have a question with that default network. Is that why when we are working, working, working, we're sitting in a laptop, we're trying to come up with something, and then we say, fuck this, I'm going to go for a walk, or I'm going to go take a shower, and then we have like our best idea ever. Is that the switch to the default network? Yes, yes. But that's your example is perfect. One of the people
Starting point is 00:50:58 studies this thing, studies creativity and studies insights. We get insights, and he uses shower in the lecture I heard him, is when you're in a shower, your executive network is attentive, but it doesn't have to be fully attentive. And your attention network is saying, I can fall down, this water could get too hot, but it's cool. I mean, it's relaxed. Yeah. And all of a sudden you're thinking, God damn, that's where that paragraph goes, freaking for me. Wow. How about this for a paper? People are studying creativity. It's a back and forth between the imagination network and this executive network. When they get really good at it, I think, you know, and really refined. And it's hard work that they're doing, getting the image, but then
Starting point is 00:51:47 doing all the counting of all the little pixels. I mean, you know, this is detailed work, but they're going to see this back and forth, but they're going to see you're mostly, this fault networks up. So neuroscientists are looking in painstaking detail at what areas of your brain light up, which probably makes their own brains light up as they do it. But Professor Domhoff delivered a great metaphor. I think of the human brain, including the sleeping as a symphony orchestra. And we think of the symphony orchestra with all the different kinds of musical instruments out there that all have different roles. And we have a conductor. And the conductor is doing certain things and telling this group to sing, you know,
Starting point is 00:52:33 go down, this group to go up, this comes strong. And so you listen to it. And all of a sudden there's just virtually no sound. Other time, boom, boom, boom, boom. But it's in harmony. And it's coming, it's moving around in such a way that it's, that it's very smooth. And always think of that because of a number of reasons from the past in research, you know, the harmonious mind, but also in some detailed work that a great sleep researcher did, where she looked at our brainstem falling asleep. When we fall asleep, it's not just like a burrup or boom, boom. It's just, it is kind of a dance almost, or this harmonious toning down and going silent. When we go to sleep, we have a whole stage called the sleep onset stage. And so when we close our
Starting point is 00:53:23 eyes, then certain networks go down. At the same time, there's neurochemical things that are happening that I liken to that the conductor has looked at the clock and it is a clock that's in us. The orchestra has looked up on the wall and seen or subjectively thought, yes, it's about over. And so the conductor starts to tamp everything down. And as the conductor sort of turns away, then say the horns go down and we're just left with just, you know, the softest part of it. And what happens is then the executive network starts to fall away. The attention networks go down a little bit. And there are studies of this, of how they kind of disconnect from each other. And different brain waves come up. So we're usually in a very fast brain wave state,
Starting point is 00:54:18 let's call it beta. But as we drift into sleep, more alpha and then theta, say it is a little less than alpha, they start to come into the picture and they move from the back of the brain to the front of the brain. So there's this gradual transition. And then this one researcher on sleep onset is a great phrase where he calls the default network the gateway to sleep. It's just the gateway to sleep. And we do, if we awaken you in sleep onset, you're having little mini dreams. Oh, for sure. And that's so great when you start to realize that when you're like, oh, I'm falling asleep. My mom has this trick she taught me where if you need to fall asleep, but your brain is thinking about what you got to do and stress out the way to help her fall asleep, she comes up with a category,
Starting point is 00:55:09 like let's say it's cars or fruit or boys names or whatever. And she'll go, okay, Apple, A, B, Banana, C, Cherry, and she'll go to the alphabet and her name's Nancy, we call it the fancy Nancy. That's great because it's putting you into that more imaginative mode and back and forth and vigilance is going down. Tension networks are now saying, oh, she's messing around. We don't need to watch anymore. And you shift into that. And as you go to sleep, if you awaken people in the first hour, sometimes you'll get a dream from them. But literally, when we're a couple hours into sleep, not an hour and a half to two hours into sleep, this network that's got a front and a back to it, those parts break apart. See, so back in the
Starting point is 00:55:57 back and the front of the brain, there are these important parts and then they're connected through these highways back and forth. But when those highways break apart, then they're isolated and then you can't dream anymore. But that's also the way consciousness works. Consciousness is a property of a very big network. Dreaming is a property of portions of the default network plus the secondary visual and sensory motor cortices. That's why we can see and hear and smell and taste and run and feel exhausted after traipsing back and forth. I traipse back and forth in a convention hall trying to get out of it just a week ago at some meeting and trying to figure out which entrance there's people going everywhere and I go back up the steps. When I
Starting point is 00:57:04 woke up, I thought that was exhausting, you know, the whole set. So, and of course, that's the charm of dreams. Does that charm wear off ever or does it wear on? So patrons Aaron Sorenson, Diane Doty, Christy LaForce, RGIP17, CasePenix, Julie DePri, BastionFelon, Debbie Potts and Darlene Scappura all had questions about dreaming at different ages and Emily Stouffer, Jess Swan and Julie Nobel asked, do babies dream? When do dreams start? One great psychologist, a cognitive psychologist, did longitudinal studies in a lab of children from three to 15 and their results were really exciting because they show that dreaming only develops gradually and it relates, we now know, to the maturation of the default network as well as the cognitive development of imagination,
Starting point is 00:58:00 ability to tell a story, which we call narrative, to generate mental imagery, to have an autobiographical self. All of those things are gradual cognitive achievements. You can do things at five that you couldn't do at four and you can do things at six, you couldn't do at five and so on. Only humans have a default network and it is only functional by age five to seven and not fully mature till nine to 11. What about when you see your dog going and moving his little feet? Because his brain is activated. A lot of us wanted to know about critters in Dreamland, including Spex Owl, Jesse Hurlburt, Alia Myers, Corey Bridget B, Sydney Taupes, Suji, Laurie Fishman, first time askers, Jules Crawford, Sadie Vipe and Allison L and Kate Timms who asked,
Starting point is 00:58:52 I've heard a lot of people saying that animals don't dream. Maybe their brains just do it differently. What do you think? When my tiny little poodle is going, oh, it's the cutest thing in the world. I know. I never usually talk about animals and dreaming because that gets people so upset. I know. I'm just curious. Well, I think that his brain is activated. He's woofing and he's paws are moving. We've all seen it, but that doesn't mean they're dreaming. It doesn't follow from that behavior. That's fair. You're the expert. Well, thank you. And when your goblin is more marking and making flipper paws, that is called heaven and the best, but also myoclonus, which is a type of muscle jerking. And that plus little fluttering eyelids
Starting point is 00:59:43 and eyes are associated with REM sleep, which accounts for about 12% of a dog's life. And if you're thinking, oh, okay, that's only about half as much as a human's 25% of REM sleep, right? No. No, that's 12% of the dog's life. Not their sleep, their life. They're only awake 44% of the time according to a 1977 study on dog sleeping. They're living the life and they're sleeping through it. But are they dreaming? This is where science gets divided. So even though some researchers found that lab rats learning a maze had the same very specific brain activity during sleep leading to the assumption that, yes, the rats were dreaming about the maze. Other scientists say that if you cannot take a report, you don't know who's dreaming.
Starting point is 01:00:34 If only there were a book about animals when they dream. I will actually philosopher David Penu Guzman published the book, When Animals Dream, a few months back. So there you go. And in it, there are accounts of cephalopods changing colors during sleep states and fish who have brain activity during sleep that looks the same as when they're singing songs underwater, which that made me learn that zebrafish sing, I guess, and they're not just performers. I went down a rabbit hole. And it turns out zebrafish are connoisseurs as well. According to the 2018 paper, the effects of auditory enrichment on zebrafish behavior and physiology. Researchers exposed a bunch of adult zebrafish to two hours of Vivaldi music twice a
Starting point is 01:01:21 day for 15 days. And overall, apparently zebrafish exposed to such auditory stimuli were less anxious and they had lower levels of inflammatory cytokines and cortisol. So if a fish can benefit from relaxing, please trust that you deserve peace and self-care as well. If you're like, in my dreams, let's discuss that. So lucid dreaming is when you know you're dreaming and you can get some kind of control over the situations and the characters, kind of like the Star Trek holodeck, but free and it's in your own brain. And tons of you, such as Susan L, Tatum, Michelle Lee, Brenna Lynch, Aaron Hem, Mia, MTB, Kaylee Evans, Toby Tobias, Shay Walker, and Joanne E wanted to know, is lucid dreaming legit and who can do it? What is lucid dreaming? Is it a different part of the
Starting point is 01:02:12 brain? Is it even a real thing or is it just kind of a euphemism for just daydreaming? You're wincing. You're wincing like I just poured lemon juice into a pot. Lucid dreams are a very rare, maybe real thing that had been hyped out of sight, warped everything, hustled beyond belief. It's a painful topic for me, you're right. I thought she could ask me about lucid dreaming because I have foot in two worlds. I certainly get along with all, at least most, of the dream researchers and whatever they're doing and I'm curious, but I have my foot really strongly in the scientific world. If I say there's no reason to believe it, we don't believe it. So most of the studies of lucid dreaming are quite bad. The first serious studies,
Starting point is 01:03:15 I didn't tell you, we're done by a person. We'll go nameless. All people will go nameless. And in this particular dissertation done at a prestigious university where you could at that time in the 70s, they could create their own program and he had created his own program. He wasn't in a department. He wasn't trained in psych or anything else. At any rate, he was both the participant and the experimenter. So 14 of the 17 instances of lucid dreaming were lo and behold, the experimenters. It was also the subject and then he writes a popular book on it and so on. So this is what is mostly going on and in an article about later, he got into the dreams, turned out after he had been a hippie kind of person for a while and always hints at maybe
Starting point is 01:04:10 he had used drugs and so on. Another guy was hilarious. He had insomnia. So he would go to bed and wake up, get up, have coffee, which blew my mind. And he'd finally go to sleep 5, 6 a.m. and they'd have lucid dreams. This woman has had scary dreams as a young kid, 5 to 7 and feared witches. Now, what that creates is a state where you're afraid of sleep. You're scared to go to sleep. You don't want to go to sleep and there are that kind of thing can happen to people. So that person is likely very vigilant during sleep. So once this dissertation based on the person's own dreams was published, then other people tried to do studies using EEG and other participants and they would find that there was more of an EEG state, this alpha state
Starting point is 01:05:05 I mentioned. It's different from our usual waking state, but it's not like what dream sleep looks like or other forms of sleep. So they were very iffy. So that was in the groovy 1970s, but let's skip to the lit 2010s when neuroimaging was more widely available and now this legend of lucid dreaming could be observed. And the researcher had trained a number of participants to have lucid dreams. She used out of these 30-some people, there were five or six that said that they were having, I think it was three or more lucid dreams a week at home. So she gets them in the lab and maybe two have one or two instances. In other words, it's very few. But the interesting thing is that there's a lot more activity in the executive network, a lot more activity. Then comes
Starting point is 01:06:05 finally a neuroimaging study by a guy that was really into lucid dreaming. And he gets, I think it was four guys from in their mid-20s, early 30s, who'd been really working at it on lucid dreaming for a long, long time. They were prolific lucid dreamers. But he's got 15 nights of imaging on these guys. He gets two instances, one from each of two guys. Same thing. He has got some of these areas that are active during waking and very important for consciousness. It's believed. Then we get to another interesting point. In the literature now, the people have tried to study consciousness that are really good. The thing is that there are parts of, it's called the frontal pole or the rostrolateral prefrontal cortex. This particular area in the
Starting point is 01:07:08 brain has been studied. And it's really a key part of the executive network, but it has two parts. One that is sort of more active towards the external world and one that monitors our internal world. And so I just wrote a paper that's going to be published in March in which I, humble dream researchers, have the nerve to write about dreaming and consciousness in which I say dreaming happens in a certain area in the hierarchical network that leads to consciousness. And what differs is in lucid dreaming, I put forth the hypothesis that in lucid dreaming, the internally oriented part of the executive network has been reactivated. Because if our brain is constantly fluctuating in its activation levels, which I believe it is, our networks right
Starting point is 01:08:15 now are changing a million times. If I suddenly notice a sound or if I notice out the window, I notice, my goodness, it stopped raining, it's sunny. That changes our brain all the time. And so coming out of sleep like that momentarily, we could have that particular network. And when it comes to lucid dreaming, it's pretty rare. But some research, like the 2018 paper titled Frequent Lucid Dreaming Associated with Increased Functional Connectivity Between Frontopolar Cortex and Temporo-Parietal Association Areas found that though we don't understand the neurobiological basis of lucid dreaming, evidence shows that there's involvement of these areas called the interior prefrontal cortex and the parietal cortex and people who were able to lucid dream
Starting point is 01:09:13 frequently had significantly higher resting state functional connectivity between these areas. Mumbo jumbo, you're screaming at your windshield. You're like, I don't know what you're talking about. How do I make out with the weird horny green M&M in my dreams? That's all I care about. And I understand you. So I scour the internet to give you these unproven tips. But some people say, good sleep hygiene, a comfortable room and sleeping setup, including temperature. Remember, go cold. Don't get too hot under there. Start dream journaling. During waking hours, check in with yourself to make sure you're in reality. Like look at your hands, poke stuff to make sure it's real so that you do it in your dream and say, holy shit, this is a dream. Also,
Starting point is 01:10:00 before you go to sleep, set an intention and think to yourself, tonight when I dream, I'm going to remember I'm dreaming. I'm going to do it. And you can also try sleeping five or six hours and then getting up and reading or doing something active and then going back to bed for an hour and see if you get that sweet, sweet in between worlds, lucid dreaming reward. So I hope that helps. Tony Vessel, Spexel, Art by D and Liam Morris. But don't get too desperate for it. You're better off just dreaming normally and then playing a video game when you're awake or something. Now what's the heavy part? And maybe we all say, you got a sense of this. What I am uncomfortable with is people go around and say, I can teach you to lucid dream. I can give you techniques to
Starting point is 01:10:49 lucid dream. There's technologies that can help you lucid dream. And I used to have a flyer that I passed around in my class from a guy that was a doctor, I guess, down just a little town below Santa Cruz. He had a machine and he's standing there looking and he said he could come down and he'd have you lucid dream with his machine. I mean, this gets into hustles. This gets into maybe grift even, I don't know. And so it gets uncomfortable for me. But what the problem is, what's difficult to say is, look, if lucid dreaming is atypical and there's a study out there that says none of the technologies and methodologies have improved lucid dreaming for those who do not lucid dream, may I dare ask the question of can we possibly study the life
Starting point is 01:11:50 histories of those people? I mean, it's too much pressure for people. People keep thinking, oh, I can lucid dream if I work hard enough and that's just not true, huh? Yeah, and you can fly if you work hard enough. And, you know, all the things we're told, try harder. You can do it if you try. There was a great book on psychotherapy that talked about that we end up, if we're not careful, and we do this in our society tremendously. It was a great book by this title called Blame the Victim, Blaming the Victim. And so if you didn't prosper in my psychotherapy, what the hell's wrong with you? I mean, it's not my fault. You didn't try far. I told you the instructions. You know, I mean, if things get turned around, a kid comes to school, they are not
Starting point is 01:12:41 nourished. There's no books in their house because their family's been unfairly treated. And then you say to the kid, you're not concentrating. You're not trying hard enough. Yeah. Why don't you develop your vocabulary? I mean, so you're already blaming the victim, you know, and here we're talking about social class and race and gender and etc. But we blame the victims. Yeah. What the hell's the matter with you? I mean, because you can't lucid dream. I mean, so that happens in all of this kind of stuff. Well, that was going to be my very last question. My just had to ask because I've got you here. What would you do if you found out physicists, Caltech, put out a paper next week saying,
Starting point is 01:13:30 holy shit, we figured out multiverses are real. There are parallel universes, time travel is possible. We figured out that dreams are actually a parallel universe. What would you do? If I trusted the physicists, I'd believe it. I've changed my mind before. I did give a lecture that actually got listened to on this. I want to plug in 39,000 views. That's a big time for an academic, especially a dream researcher called the awesome lawfulness of your nightly dreams. And I was in dead serious mode. But at any rate, the interesting thing that I ended with, similar to what you said, is I said, we actually lead two lives. We lead a life that's waking. We lead another life that's dreaming. And I said, they differ because in the waking life,
Starting point is 01:14:32 we pick up right where we left off. But in my dreaming life, it starts over each time. We have a different dream life each night. But if I have lots of dreams from you, and I have actually 4,000 from Izzy, the young woman, I have her dreams from 12 to 25, you see that her life has all these episodes. She's seeing one of these crushes, or she's at a horror movie, or she and her brother arguing in some way. And it can be in a certain sense very thematic. We have the dreams. Dorothea, she named herself, we often would let people give their own pseudonyms, but it got out of hand. And I'm not putting that goofy stuff up there. So at any rate, but Dorothea was something
Starting point is 01:15:31 very educated woman, born probably in the 1890s. But we had six or 700 of her dreams, I forget what it is. But in 72% of those dreams, there's one of seven things happening. This is poignant in her. 20% of her dreams, she's either buying, preparing a meal or eating a meal at, you know, something dealing with meal. So that was her biggest theme. There was also, I'll get to the poignant part in a minute, which is her last dream that she mailed us before the home in Hawaii wrote and said she was dead. She also would often dream of missing the bus or the train. But she also had dreams about 5 to 10% of just which she's trying to go to the bathroom and there's people, people in the bathroom. And it's just, you know, a problem. Hey, who hasn't
Starting point is 01:16:32 been there, right? Renee Q and Ariel Van Sant have, and you're probably just sleeping with a full bladder. And by you, I mean we. Anyway, less toilet, more poignant. So towards the end of her life, she retired to Hawaii and she lived in a real nice place. She swam every day. She seemed to be in fitting health. She would mail periodically these dreams to, in this case, my mentor, Calvin Hall. And then I, you know, inherited all the files and put them on Dream Bay. The last dream before she died, which if you didn't know her, that she 20% of her dreams were about food, you'd say, Oh my God, it's a premonition of her death. But in her last dream, she's sitting around the table and with her siblings, she had several siblings. And she used a phrase,
Starting point is 01:17:24 very proper phrase. She said, Mother had dished too liberally to, you know, my brothers and sisters. And there was nothing really left for me to eat. And then I saw a hand bone sitting at the end of the table or sitting on the floor. That was a dream. So now, you say, my God, you know, premonition, I mean, death, I mean, just only a bone, she's not getting to eat. But no, this is always happening to her in her dreams with these siblings and not getting, you know, there's always this sort of, I get kind of hind tit of all of this, you know, I'm the sort of the left out one in the litter. So she has a separate life. That's the second life. If there's any Rick and Morty fans out there, you might be haunted by the episode
Starting point is 01:18:17 Night Family, in which Rick uses a machine to have his nocturnal self do the chores of his waking self, including getting like a ripped set of abs. But then the ultimate conclusion that they come to is that we, as waking people, are just the servants of our sleeping selves. Creepy. I loved it. I think about it constantly. Well, last questions I always ask is, what is hard about your work? What's the hardest thing about dreams? Well, there's a million things that are difficult for dreams. Because scientists like to observe, you can't observe dreams. You can't make them happen. In other words, you can't do experiments where you do this or that. They don't work. I mean, we try to drop water on people,
Starting point is 01:18:59 whisper in their ear, you know, tape their eyes open and flasks. And you tell them stuff during the day. It doesn't, it really works. It's the mind doing its own thing. I used to call it the spin of the cognitive Rolodex, except nobody has a Rolodex anymore. I remember what they are. It was a desktop address book. You can ask your grandma. Anyway. So semantic memory banks are being stirred up, but it's hard then because we can experiment and we can't observe. And we're at the mercy of you telling the dream. So we've done this elaborate study. We awaken you in the middle of the night and say, would you report your dream? And you say, I can't remember, which is maybe true, but it could not be true. So you're at the mercy
Starting point is 01:19:42 of the participant. If you're like certainty, any scientist wants to control the variables. And if you can't control the variables, you're going to say, that ain't my field. So that's one of our problems. The other problem is that because the work on dreams did not lead to, as was early thought by some people in the late 50s and early 60s, might be a key to mental health. There really was that belief. And some key psychoanalysts of that era who were MDs and very big deals, they helped some of the early dream researchers to get grants. So there were grants for dreaming. But when dreaming turned out to be not going to be useful in terms of medications or studying psychosis or so on, the money did dry up. And I understand it, and I appreciate it,
Starting point is 01:20:34 and I believe it. And in other words, I'm not out scolding anybody. Why aren't you studying dreams? There's Alzheimer's. There's people's attention deficit disorder, as you know. And all of these things of waking command our attention. And there's PTSD. These are things that command the attention of federal government. As far as the foundations, they of course want to make themselves look good. And so they find topics that are relevant. It would make their name even more lustrous than the millions they made. And so dreaming does not fit into that category. But the hardest thing of studying dreams in the past was being up all night. I mean, if you have a sleep dream lamp, somebody's going to have to wake up and wake up those participants, right?
Starting point is 01:21:26 So you have to learn to be what it's like to be a participant. And it ain't fun to be awakened at one or two a.m. and then jostle two hours later and so on. So participants don't necessarily ask. I mean, that's enough of that. That's it. I'm out of here. And if you just get them in our first night, one dream set of dreams, I studied, the guy dreamed that the machine was electrocuting it. Look at all these wires on it. It's called electroencephalogram. So the machine was zapping him, you know. It has its little problems. What about your favorite? Favorite thing about what you do? You've been doing this for years and years and years. What's the best? It's just, I like to discover new things. I didn't want to just add to the details on
Starting point is 01:22:18 vision or learning. I wanted to know the Royal Road to the Unconscious, the meaning of life, why we are so crazy, why we believe the things we believe, why we fight with each other so much. I mean, all those things have been part of my research life in one way or another. So I was interested in using the best methods, the most serious quantitative methods, most objective methods to study the toughest questions. Just the challenge of dreams. And when you have a great mentor that's done so much and has enthusiasm for it, and I might say we've replicated his work again and again, and his coding system has been used all over the world, carrying that on, you know, it had some meaning. And he was also a person that said, I think you're okay. We all need to
Starting point is 01:23:13 have a mentor that says, you know, you're right for this field. When we go to college, we're looking for something that we like that likes us. So if you say, oh, I love psychology, and you haven't done so well in statistics or experimental psych. And then somebody has to say, you know, maybe you ought to go into some more qualitative field. So they like something that didn't like them. It's like finding any match, right? So you find something you really like where it likes you. And so dream research was that for me. You asked me, what keeps you going about dreams? They're for all of their negativity. And they are often negative. There's some real fun in studying dreams and reading dreams. Patron Scott Sheldon asked thoughts on dream journals.
Starting point is 01:24:09 And I think by now we know Professor Domhoff is a fan. But would he read yours? Can they sign up and fill up your dream coffers at the Dream Bank? No, no, no. No, we're very wary. We only use, I mean, the thing is, I don't, you've heard of all the pranks that have been done on researchers, the pilt down man, the fake bones in the British Museum that fooled people. So I am very sensitive to that in terms of, I know people can make up dreams. I know they can change them. So I don't take any, we don't collect any dreams over the web. I'd never take a dream off the web. These are people that write me and then I say, well, send me a photocopy or when did you start
Starting point is 01:24:56 writing them down? Or I got a set of dreams that I got a timestamp on them. So I know that that person wrote those dreams down in the past. Just write them down. Don't look at them. Just write them down every day for a month or two. And that gets you into it. If you stop and you analyze anything and when then you start to project onto it, just write them down. If you can study your own dreams you want to, just write them down. Just keep them. Just put them in the drawer. Don't even look. Just keep them. The more you do it, the more you get into habit. Well, I'm going to start writing mine down. I will. I'll let you know how it's going too. You did start writing them down? No, I'm going to start. I'm going to start.
Starting point is 01:25:36 Why don't you voice them? I'll do voice to text on my phone. That way they're digitized and I can later if I need to analyze them I can. The voice to text works well. I've been trying to tell dream researchers for 10 years to get a voice to text. Well, this is a call to the to the universe. Get with it dream researchers. We got a plan here. Yeah, we got a plan of how to get the dream and we're going to, and I've got, I now know a person who can analyze them. And if I do submit them later to his research, can I pick a cool name like Jiminy Diamond or something? That'll be my student. You can just, yeah, but don't go too far. I won't do Jiminy Diamond. I'll do like Lynette or something. Something Rhonda, something like that. This has been an
Starting point is 01:26:25 absolute dream of mine fulfilled. I have been wanting to interview you for years and this did not disappoint. That was a good time. Thank you so much for doing this. My pleasure. You're the best. I hope it's, I hope it's serious enough. I mean, I hope not. No, this is perfect. So there you have it. Ask smart people, sleepy questions, and just know that that's the best way to learn anything. Bill and I had just such a lovely time chatting. We talked for over three hours on this rainy Tuesday in his office. I'm just so glad I asked him if I could ask him your questions. So his new book is called The Neurocognitive Theory of Dreaming. It's linked in the show notes. If you want to know more, all the details are in there
Starting point is 01:27:11 alongside his research site, dreamresearch.net. I also have a ton of links on my site at alleyward.com slash oligies slash onerology, which is linked in the show notes. We're at oligies on Twitter and Instagram and I'm at alleyward on both. I'm also at alley underscore oligies on TikTok now. I'm giving it a shot, y'all. Thank you, Aaron Talbert for adminning the oligies podcast Facebook group with help from Bonnie Dutch and Shannon Feltis. Thank you, Emily White of The Wordery for making professional transcripts and Caleb Patton for bleeping episodes. Those are up for free at alleyward.com slash oligies dash extras. Susan Hale handles merch and so much else and Noel Dilworth does our scheduling. Thank you to everyone supporting on patreon.com slash oligies.
Starting point is 01:27:57 Thank you, Zeke Rodriguez-Thomas and Mercedes-Maitland for working on kid-friendly, shorter versions of classic episodes. Those are called smologies. They're up at alleyward.com slash smologies. Kellyard Wire makes our website and she can make yours. Nick Thorburn made the theme music and lead editor for this episode was Mercedes-Maitland of Maitland Audio. Thank you so much. You are wonderful. We heart you. Additional editing by Jared Sleeper of Mind Jam Media, who I would marry in a saloon with warm brusquies. And if you stick around, I tell you the secret, but I already told you one in the middle of the episode. So right now I'm going to just go over to Grammy. I'm literally recording this on my bed. I'm going to see if Grammy's going to make any
Starting point is 01:28:45 barking noises in her sleep. Okay, she's awake. She's just breathing at me. That was uneventful, but when she does bark at her sleep, it sounds like this. I'll do my best impression. She sounds kind of like a chicken. Okay, bye. That wasn't a very good secret. No, it wasn't. She was sleeping right before then. Can I do a different secret? That's really funny that it's still recording. My other secret is that it's raining really hard in California right now and I just got out of the shower and I was not wearing clothes and it was raining and I dashed out onto the porch because I've never been nude in the rain, but now I have. So you're armed with that knowledge. Sorry, everyone. Bye-bye.
Starting point is 01:30:14 The dreams, they feel real while we're in them, right? It's only when we wake up that we realize something is actually strange. Today's episode is exclusively sponsored by Sotva. So a final shout out to a dream sponsor, Sotva. We love you. And remember, you can't dream if you can't fall asleep and Sotva is the mattress company dedicated to helping you do just that well and comfortably. And maybe your bed just needs a linen upgrade. Sotva has those two, flannel sheets, satin sheets, percale sheets. But if you have been in the market for a new mattress, go straight to Sotva. And right now, you're in luck because you'll save $200 when you purchase $1,000 or more at Sotva.com. That's S-A-T-V-A.com. Pass it on.

There aren't comments yet for this episode. Click on any sentence in the transcript to leave a comment.