Radiolab - Touch at a Distance

Episode Date: September 1, 2023

In this episode from 2007, we take you on a tour of language, music, and the properties of sound. We look at what sound does to our bodies, our brains, our feelings… and we go back to the reason we ...at Radiolab tell you stories the way we do.  First, we look at Diana Deutsch’s work on language and music, and how certain languages seem to promote musicality in humans. Then we meet Psychologist Anne Fernald and listen to parents as they talk to their babies across languages and cultures. Last, we go to 1913 Paris and sneak into the premiere of Igor Stravinsky’s score of The Rite of Spring.  Check out Diana Deutsch's 'Audio Illusions' here (https://deutsch.ucsd.edu/psychology/pages.php?i=201).  Our newsletter comes out every Wednesday. It includes short essays, recommendations, and details about other ways to interact with the show. Sign up (https://radiolab.org/newsletter)! Radiolab is supported by listeners like you. Support Radiolab by becoming a member of The Lab (https://members.radiolab.org/) today. Follow our show on Instagram, Twitter and Facebook @radiolab, and share your thoughts with us by emailing radiolab@wnyc.org Leadership support for Radiolab’s science programming is provided by the Gordon and Betty Moore Foundation, Science Sandbox, a Simons Foundation Initiative, and the John Templeton Foundation. Foundational support for Radiolab was provided by the Alfred P. Sloan Foundation.

Transcript
Discussion (0)
Starting point is 00:00:00 Hey, it's Lulu. To start today, I want to play you a sentence that I heard 20 years ago that changed the course of my life. Sound is kind of touch at a distance. I was 21 years old, living in Queens, working at a coffee shop, more than a little lonely, unsure what I was doing with my life, and boom! Those six eerie words floated out of the radio. I didn't know it yet, but it was this brand new show called Radio Lab that was doing something with sound and layering and music that almost no one was yet bringing to journalism. That episode would then go on to do this very meta thing of explaining how the words
Starting point is 00:00:51 that were being spoken right then were just waves of vibrating air traveling across space and time into my ear drum. which vibrates a few very small bones. And the little bones transmit the vibration into this salty sea, where the hairs are. And the hair cells are literally bent by weight. They bend like trees and breeze. And when these hair cells bend, charged molecules, blood inwards, and activate the cell. And it was like, I felt it. This stranger's voice had just made contact, literal contact with me. And in that moment, I knew that was my path.
Starting point is 00:01:45 I wanted to get into that world and use that medium to do that thing. 20 years later, I still feel so lucky to be part of this show where every week, we send out sounds. Y'all, y'all! And voices. Hey, no.
Starting point is 00:02:04 I feel like screaming, scream. Is it like crying, crying? Yaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaa I play about monsters every night. That, you know, actually touch you. And look, I think in a moment like right now, when so many of us feel divided across space, across geography, across ideology, I think sounds potential to sneak in and connect. I think it really matters. But the truth is, and the reason I am here yammering about all this is that it's a really dire time in public media right now. And to be able to keep bringing you stories like this that
Starting point is 00:03:06 are packed lovingly with sounds, sounds that we hope can help you not just understand a story, but feel it. We really need your support. We've run the numbers and found that less than 2% of you support Radio Lab. And if we could get that number up just a hair, just a little bit, it would make a huge difference. The best way to do that is to join our membership program, The Lab, where you commit to tossing in just a few bucks a month. It is super easy to sign up. To check that out, you just go to radiolab.org slash join. And to entice you, we just made a brand new t-shirt that every single new member will get this month.
Starting point is 00:03:52 And it has a great design. It is a reference to the episode you're about to hear, some more on that in a little bit. And if you join, you also get all kinds of other perks, special event invitations, extra interviews you can listen to, but really it is just about supporting the show, keeping us going. So again, if you want to check out what the t-shirt looks like, see how easy it is to join few bucks, like a coffee a month, you can just go to radialab.org slash join and pick the amount that's right for you. All right, thank you for listening to all that on with today's episode,
Starting point is 00:04:35 which is actually that very episode that I heard 20 years ago. I hope you enjoy. listening to Radio Lab. Radio from WNYC. Three, why? Now? Now? Okay. You're listening to radio.
Starting point is 00:05:07 I'll continue. We're here to report the first large scale study comparing the prevalence of absolute pitch into normal populations by means of... This is Professor Diana Deutsch. Diana Deutsch? Well, yeah. I'm going to turn down my headphone, I'm going to turn down my headphone, and I'm a professor's psychology at the University of California San Diego. Can you still hear me, Diana? Okay. Hello. Diana's studies sound. How humans perceive sound.
Starting point is 00:05:38 She's a scientist, she has a lab, but every so often she will also release CDs. Right. These CDs of audio demonstrations that she uses in her research and that's why we called because it was in the production of her second CD that she stumbled onto the weirdest phenomena. Well, I'll tell you what happened is that when you do post-production as you know of speech, you loop things, loop things, loop things so that you can zero in on piece, put that sound too loud, you need to unpop or SS that sounded too sharp and so on. So you put things on loops in order to fine tune the way the speech sounds. So I had this particular
Starting point is 00:06:21 phrase on a loop and forgot about it. What phrase was this? It's a phrase that occurs at the beginning of the CD in which I say, the sounds as they appear to you are not only different from those that are really present, but they sometimes behave so strangely as to seem quite impossible. To seem quite impossible. Now, I had sometimes behave so strangely looped.
Starting point is 00:06:45 The sounds as they appear to you are not only different from those that are really present, but they sometimes behave so strangely, sometimes behave so strangely, just those few words, sometimes behave so strangely, and forgot about, sometimes behave so strangely, sometimes behave so strangely. So here's what happened. Diana leaves her studio. She closes the door, goes into the kitchen to make some tea. All the while, this loop is worrying away in the background. As she's sipping her tea, she thinks, is someone singing? Who's singing? I heard what sounded like song in the background. She realized, wait a second. That's not singing. That's me. Talking.
Starting point is 00:07:25 That very phrase. Strangely. But at this point, sometimes behave so strangely. It's a peer to be sung rather than spoken strangely. Sometimes behave so strangely. This is sometimes behave so strangely, right? Yeah. You still hear the words, but they song words, rather than spoken words.
Starting point is 00:07:45 It's weird, like it just switches at a certain point. Three or four repetitions in. Right. It's going, it's going, and then... Pow! If it comes music. And then now, now none of us can get it out of our head. Like the whole office is like, sometimes VH, so strange, sometimes VH, so strangely.
Starting point is 00:07:59 Sometimes VH, so strangely. Sometimes VH, so strangely. And you know what, if you do this demo and then you go back to the original sentence, it sounds like speech to begin with and when you come to that very phrase, I seem to be bursting into song. The sounds as they appear to you are not only different from those that are really present, but they sometimes behave so strangely as to seem quite impossible. I have to say this can continue for months and months,
Starting point is 00:08:28 it's sort of like your brain gets altered for that particular phrase, and it continues to sound like singing. Sometimes it makes those sounds. For a very, very long time. Sometimes it makes those sounds strangely. Alright, so here we have just one small indication that music is... Well it behaves very strangely. I mean, think about this.
Starting point is 00:08:50 We started with some basic speech. Repeated it a few times, somewhere along the way, it leapt into song. How did it change like that? And if that's all it takes to turn something into music, then what exactly is music? Really? Sometimes we hate so strange, deep. This is Radio Lab. Today's program is about answering that question.
Starting point is 00:09:16 We're trying to, in any case, some chat up in the mod here with me. It is your average growl, which my partner in crime. It's a little hard to get out of your head. I know. It is really weird. No get out of your head. I know. It is really, we can. Oh I know. Yeah. So this hour, what are we doing?
Starting point is 00:09:29 We are going to try, and we will probably fail. Yes, we will fail, but we will make an earnest effort to try to find the ingredients of music, both its bassist and language, its bassist and physics, its bassist in your brain. We'll look everywhere we can, software, trying to find out what music is made of, and why it touches us so intimately. Then touches us sometimes not in a good way. If you've ever had this experience of going to a concert, hearing some music and it just made you upset, for some reason, like irrationally upset, almost like you wanted to hurt someone. If that rings a bell, there's a segment later in the show you will not want to miss.
Starting point is 00:10:08 Sometimes. This is radio lab. Stick around. Sometimes baby has so straight. SREEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEEE Maybe hence so. We'll hear more of them later. So let's explore a little bit more closely. This connection between language and music. Yes. You think of them as separate. The thing is, they're really closely related.
Starting point is 00:10:52 Says Neuroscientist Mark Jude Tremel. When we speak, we sing. You know how to use the pitch of your voice to convey emotion and meaning. Like, I went to the store just because I raised the pitch, the note, if you will. You interpret that as an interrogative. A monotonic speech, you know, talking at the same rate and rhythm in the same pitch and loudness. I mean, that is not how humans talk. But humans talk in all kinds of different ways. In different languages, each language
Starting point is 00:11:28 has its own musical personality. German is different than French, is different than Swahili, and if you look at those differences closely, there are all kinds of things we can learn about music. Take Diana Deutsch. Okay. She's recently been looking at tone languages. Just published her results. And the results are startling. Diana, before we start, what exactly is a tone language? Okay.
Starting point is 00:11:52 In tone languages, words take on different meanings, depending upon the tones in which they are enunciated. For example, Mandarin has four tones, and the word mar in Mandarin means mother in the first tone, hemp in the second tone, horse in the third tone, and a reproach in the fourth tone. Could you say them? Would you like me to? Yeah, could you give them a string? I thought you were. Well, I have them on CD, but I'd rather hear you say them. Well, okay, so excuse my bad pronunciation without trying.
Starting point is 00:12:31 Ma means mother. Ma means hemp. Ma means horse. And ma is a reproach. So conceivably, if you screwed up the tones, you could call your mom a horse. Yes indeed, in fact,
Starting point is 00:12:50 there are quite a lot of jokes where, westerners who don't speak the tones right, say terrible things. 進主白金漢宮, you have to be very careful. 以及為於溫殺的其他 皇家家園進行研究
Starting point is 00:13:03 See, this is a basic difference in English. We don is working with some Mandarin speakers, and she notices something. There were these words. These words that they would say, where they would all hit, precisely and then they would just say, that they would just say, that they would just say, that they would just say, that they would just say,
Starting point is 00:13:22 that they would just say, that they would just say, that they would just say, Day Diana is working with some Mandarin speakers and she notices something. There were these words. These words that they would say where they would all hit precisely the same note. With their voices not just close to one another, either. Exactly, precisely, and consistently the same pitch. Even on different days. In fact, would you like me to play for you one person reciting a list of 12 Mandarin tones on two different days? Yeah, definitely.
Starting point is 00:13:52 First you have the first word spoken on day one, followed by the same word spoken in day two, then you have the second word spoken on day one, followed by the same word spoken on day one, jaw, followed by the same word, spoken day two, jaw, and so on. And that way, you can see the consistency. Tai. It's going to appear as though the words are being repeated immediately, but in fact, the repetitions are common in totally different days.
Starting point is 00:14:21 So each of those word pairs came out of the mouth of one person separated by like 24 hours?所以每個字的字都被一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人一人 Well, I concluded that basically this was a form of perfect pitch. I've never quite understood what perfect pitch is to be honest with you. You don't know what that is? No. Should I? I mean, I know I should. Well, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's, it's just, it's, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's just, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, it's, color as a brown and you knew that how to my eyes right you didn't need me to put this brown coffee cup next to my blue jeans no I didn't know it just see the brown I mean it's absolute brown perfect pitch people have that with pitch they hear a pitch they know exactly
Starting point is 00:15:36 what note it is the rest of us have to run to the piano so if they hear a from an elevator can they name the note yeah that's exactly's exactly it. Anything with a pitch, like a horn honk. They could tell you that horn is enough, or those church bells, they're alternating between B, flat, and B. And if the faucet were dripping, they could say that faucet is dripping in a D sharp. They don't even have to think about it. They just know. It used to be that the note names would jump out at me. Diana Doich is actually one of these lucky people. To the extent that it would even be a nuisance.
Starting point is 00:16:10 And why is that good? Well, it's really rare. It only happens like once every 10,000 people here in America, Europe. Yeah, but so does it is a turning your tongue into a hula. Yeah, you're a hula, Hula, Hula. And of the people who have it. Yeah. Well, let's see how much I say this. If you look in your music history text, which you will see that every famous composer, like the really big ones, like Mozart, Park, Beethoven, I'm a Beethoven. They all had it. Really?
Starting point is 00:16:50 Mendelssohn, the list goes on and on. So if you have perfect pitch, on some level, you are closer to them. You've got the gift. Ganyhow. You've got the gift. Get it out. Let's get back to Diana Deutsch. Okay. Okay, let's talk about your latest experiment.
Starting point is 00:17:13 That's the one really interested in. Okay, so you compare Chinese kids to American kids. To see who has perfect pitch more. So explain how this works. You had a group of Chinese music students, a group of American music students, a group of American music students at the Eastman School of Music here in New York. You played them a bunch of notes, I imagine,
Starting point is 00:17:30 in a room and asked them to guess what those notes were. Right. How did that work exactly? Well, the test consisted of piano tones, which began on the C below middle C, that's the snow, and extended up three octaves all the way up to that note. That's a big range.
Starting point is 00:17:47 Yeah, 36 notes. Can you demonstrate? Sure, yes. Here are six tones. Such as were given in the test. So you would have played those notes to both sets of kids and asked them to name the notes without going to the piano. What were the notes really?
Starting point is 00:18:09 What these notes were? D, E, G-Shar, C-Shar, D-Shar, and G. What were the results? Well it turned out the Chinese group far outperformed the original group. Of those students who started musical training at ages 4 and 5, 74% of the Chinese group show perfect pitch, but 14% of the US non-Toran countries. Wow, 74%.
Starting point is 00:18:36 The Beijing group was nine times roughly, more likely to show perfect pitch than the American. Jesus, that's a split-ins. Daggering difference. It's a staggering difference. And it's your hunch that the difference is because they speak a tone language. That's my hunch.
Starting point is 00:18:55 I mean, it's known that in the first year of life, save for made six months up to, you know, a little past a year, infants learn features of their native language. This is a very, very important stage. Let's suppose that tone and the absolute pitch of tones is a feature which is potentially available to anyone. Babies who are exposed only to an international language such as English are not given the opportunity to acquire tones. Then they're going to be at a real disadvantage
Starting point is 00:19:37 when they come later on to learn to take music lessons. So you think that as they're, as they're learning their language, which includes inherently music, which is some degree, they are essentially learning two languages as they learn one. Is that right? It's a matter of fact, if you take the first tone, ma, it's a flat tone, it's really sung. Compared with English speech, it's really more like sung. That's always been sort of the stereotype of the Chinese language, it's very sort of sing-songy. Yes. Wang Gongli, all the English-speaking, Lianbao, Luibao,
Starting point is 00:20:20 for example, the third tone in Mandarin, ma, it's sort of like a J-type pattern. The second tone, which is a general output, gliss, maa, the fourth tone, which is a rapid downward gliss, maa. I mean, these are all kind of musical relationships. Yeah. Given the evidence on absolute pitch, one could speculate further and say,
Starting point is 00:20:40 well, maybe other features of music are also enhanced for individuals who start off learning tone language. So then here's my big question. Could this explain the experience that I had? And I think a lot of people have this experience when they're taking music lessons and playing little piddly pieces like Fruirajaka.
Starting point is 00:21:04 And here are these Chinese girls, right? Her playing rock mononof. Yeah, right. You know, they're brilliant. Is this why? Well, I think it's a viable hypothesis. I mean, evidently, it could be something else. It could be something else going on.
Starting point is 00:21:21 Like what? I mean, what could argue that instead it might be genetic and so on, but they're... They seem to be such a boring theory. It's a boring theory, and furthermore, we don't have to assume that knowing what we do about exposure to tone language in very early childhood as a... Yeah, it's just not fair.
Starting point is 00:21:39 And I think we can look at it another way around. Here we have a faculty that had been thought to be confined to a few rare individuals who were just extraordinarily gifted. Right. That might, in fact, be available to any individual provided they're given the right exposure at a critical period. And that raises the question of what other sorts of abilities could be brought out if we only knew just what to do.
Starting point is 00:22:17 There may be much more human potential than we had realized. BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO! BOO music psychology music psychology and as I mentioned earlier she's also the releaseer of two CDs yeah two CDs one's called musical illusions and paradoxes and the other one is called phantom words and other curiosities what would you put on a CD exactly she puts these little audio pieces that she uses in her research the stuff I guess that she will play it as subjects as she tests them and she puts these on CDs because they're kind of fun to listen to. This is like an ear test or an... Yeah, sort of. We've actually put a couple on our website.
Starting point is 00:23:10 What do they sound like? It's just a little sample. Alright, I'll do some samples. There is the chromatic illusion. Kind of as a carnival feel to it. There's also the kambyata illusion. Oh, the kambiata illusion. And of course,
Starting point is 00:23:29 the Phantom Word experiments. Ah, Phantom Word experiments. None of those pieces are going to make any kind of sense, unless you visit our website radiolab.org, where all will be explained. Sometimes, they behave so smart, where all will be explained. Sometimes they behave so strangely. We will hear more radio lab in a moment. We will hear that phrase, sound, is touch at a distance, learn about what that means, learn
Starting point is 00:24:22 about how it can be such a powerful sound. It will make old French ladies throw things. But first, I wanted to just say that that song, da da da da da da da da da da da da da da da da da da da da da da da da da da da of the hookiest earworms I've ever encountered in my life. Duh, duh, duh, duh, duh, duh, duh, duh, duh, duh, duh, duh, and I have finally used my powers as host to get it on a t-shirt. That's right, we just made a brand new t-shirt that says sometimes behaves so strangely. I think that phrase is kind of a motto for what we are doing here, the magic of sound,
Starting point is 00:25:01 the power of sound, and you know, if you wear it, it might be talking about how sound works or it might be talking about the person inside the shirt who sometimes behaves so strangely. Anyway, it's really cool. Comes in four different colors. I'm so excited about it. And it can be yours if you join the lab this month
Starting point is 00:25:17 and support us. So this is your moment. Get a great shirt, support our little operation here of Sonic investigation and Tom Foulery. It would mean so much. The way you can do that, you can check out the shirt and consider joining. You just go to radialab.org slash join. Join.
Starting point is 00:25:37 Radiolab.org slash join. Join the party. Thanks, more radio lab in a moment. This is RadioLab, I'm Chad Abumarhan. And I'm Robert Krell, which our program today is about music. What it is, how it works. And what we want to do next is we want to stay on the subject, where we're going to explore this a little more deeply. Take a closer look at the connection between language and music, and we're going to add
Starting point is 00:26:03 touch. Touch. And that will take us to the ear, the ear, and then into the brain, the brain, and then to the big question, the really big question. Why does music or how does music become a feeling, the feeling? Why do we get such deep pleasure or deep pain? We will have pain coming up too. All simply because of air pressing against your ear. All right, where first? All right, well, there is a psychology professor I want you to meet at Stanford who directs the Center for Infant Studies there and for an old is her name. And she got it into her head that there is a kind of deep universal music inside language. And she discovered it actually at a hospital.
Starting point is 00:26:46 inside language. And she discovered it actually at a hospital. The Max Planck Institute in Munich has an obstetrics unit which is very popular among expectant mothers. These mothers came from the wards of this German hospital and so they were Turkish, they were Greek, they were Sicilian, they were the so-called guest workers in the German society. Of course I didn't understand a word of what they spoke. As soon as they put the baby down and no longer had the physical contact, bodily contact with the child, they started to sing almost. In one language after another, a book of on the babies. I heard these, I heard them use these melodies. Russian?
Starting point is 00:27:26 Oh, to reach the child to remain in touch with the baby. F-A-P-L. Yiddish? F-A-P-L. So next day I brought my tape recorder. Hanford old took her tape recorder from that hospital and traveled all over the world recording parents as they talked to their very little babies. And it didn't matter whether the parents spoke a romance language or a tone language everywhere she went.
Starting point is 00:27:52 Below the words she heard consistently the same melodies. For example. I'll start with approval. When a parent wanted to praise a child. We would ask the parents to show the baby they were happy. Good boy, Now you got it. Just using their voice show them you're happy with that. Das. Achatari Gadi, give it a goady. Hindi. I don't know. Kvalu Kvalu. Portuguese.
Starting point is 00:28:18 Yes, it's a Kvalu. And what these things had in common was that the melody was a kind of a of a rise fall. Good girl. Good girl. I got it. Good girl, sweetie. So it doesn't matter what words the parents are saying, it's always really about this melody. And why that particular melody? All she knows is it works. There's something about this melody that that keeps the child doing something. There are she says other categories she discovered. Now with a prohibition in contrast, your goal is to stop the child from doing something. The category that says
Starting point is 00:29:03 stop. Quite a different melody. It's short. It's sharp. And musical terms, it's staccato. Better bet. There is the category of, look, pay attention to that. Now, this frequently used rising pitch. Shara, look, look, sweetie.
Starting point is 00:29:18 They frequently use higher pitch. A unicorn? A unicorn? So far, Anne Fernt has found four universal melodies that praise, that stop, that call attention, and of course, the melody that comforts. And while this may seem obvious to you, if you think about it, this is music that is understood by infants who are just new in the world. But we all know what it means. But we all know these songs.
Starting point is 00:29:48 We're used to thinking of sounds as being about something. Speeches always about something. But it feels to me more like touch. Touch isn't about something. If you whack me on the arm in a sudden sharp way, I'm going to be startled. Or a gentle touch has a different effect. And I think, you know, actually, sound is kind of touch at a distance.
Starting point is 00:30:21 I was Anne Frenol director of the Center for Infant Studies at Stanford. And when An says, sound is more like touch. That turns out to be literally the case. This is something I learned from friend of mine, Jonah Lehrer. My name's Jonah Lehrer, who at this very moment is working on a book, an upcoming book on Art and Science, on the connection between Art and Science. Lulu here real quick, a few years after this aired, serious problems with Jonah Lares. Journalism would come to light.
Starting point is 00:30:47 Everything in this episode has been fact checked and stands. Hans has Jonah thinking about sound as a touch. More like touch. I asked him, how does sound get into or touch your brain? Take us on that journey. It's just waves of vibrating air. It's just your voice. Touch at an edit.
Starting point is 00:31:14 Beginning your voice box compresses air, and that air travels through space and time. Into my ear, the little tunnel waves of diffuse vibrating air, focused and channeled. Into my ear, the little tunnel waves of diffuse vibrating air focused and channeled. Into my ear drum, which vibrates a few very small bones. And the little bones transmit the vibration into this salty sea, where the hairs are. And the hair cells are fasting, the hair cells become active when they are literally bent by way. They bend like trees and breeze. And when these hair cells bend, charged molecules flood inwards and activate the cell. So the sound triggers the bones, the bones disturbs the fluid, the fluid rocks the hairs. And then the hairs set off essentially electricity?
Starting point is 00:32:14 Yes. Huh. That's the language of neurons. All those changes from waves to bones to electricity, all those things were a trip on their way to being heard. It's only when the electricity finally forms a pattern in your brain. Only when it's deep inside, that's when you hear something. It feels to me more like touch. Sound is kind of touch at a distance. All right, now that we have gotten a sound and any sound into our heads, let me ask you the next really big question. Okay.
Starting point is 00:33:14 Why do some sounds, let's make it music, okay? Why does music make so many of us so often feel so strongly? Yeah, like in terms of what we were just listening to, like how does all that electricity from the ear, going up to the brain in the next millisecond, become a feeling. Yeah. Well, let me introduce you to someone.
Starting point is 00:33:35 Mark Trutremo. Actually, we heard from him earlier. He's a neuroscientist. Dumb in the Department of Neurology and Harvard Medical School. And Mark can at least begin to answer this question, this feeling question. He's done something really interesting.
Starting point is 00:33:49 He's able to listen to the electricity as it pulls in the ear and shoots up this big fat nerve to the brain. It's kind of a popping sound. He can actually listen to that nerve, to the electricity. So faster than I'm able to do here with my fingers. Is that the sound? That's what it sounds like.
Starting point is 00:34:12 How do they get this sound? I actually have no idea. I guess they sort of tap into the nerve. This is the sound of sound entering a brain. Yes, this is the sound of sound entering the brain as electricity, little pulses. And as you can hear, the electricity has a meter. What Mark has discovered is that when the electricity entering your brain is even and regular, which is this regular?
Starting point is 00:34:36 Yes, this is regular. Sure, let me hold this sweet. Yeah, that's regular. Right. When the meter of the electricity is regular and rhythmic, it will arrive in our mind and be heard by us as a sound that we generally like. Like this. Nice sound.
Starting point is 00:34:56 That in music is known as a perfect fifth. The input's coming from a perfect fifth is very, very regular. Like a metronome. However, and here's where it gets interesting. When the meter going from the ear to the brain is irregular, jagged, a with make unpredictable. Straighten it. Let me hear it. This is jagged. This is jagged. Wait. Oh, here it is. Yeah, and what Mark has discovered is that when electrical impulses like that travel from the ear to the brain, they will become heard by us by our mind as a sound that we generally don't like.
Starting point is 00:35:33 Like that. Don't care for that one. That's a minor second. The input coming from a minor second is very, very chaotic. Okay, so let me just sum up here what I think you're saying. If a sound entering my brain is disorderly and unexpected, electrically speaking, then that would make me feel uncomfortable. Yeah. And if it comes in in a familiar and orderly way that will make me feel comfortable.
Starting point is 00:36:09 There does seem to be a relationship between the kind of electricity a sound produces and how we feel about that sound. Do they have fancy names for this? Well, that's a minor second. That thing you just heard. But do the scientists have names for pleasant and unpleasant? Consonant is pleasant. Dissonant is unpleasant.
Starting point is 00:36:28 That's not a science name. That's a music name. Oh, okay. And these are fixed positions in your ear. Well, it may be fixed for scientists, but maybe, let me just propose this to you, that what people find pleasing, what people find painful, is malleable.
Starting point is 00:36:44 I'll tell you why. I'm going to tell you a story now, a true story. It involves a musician. E. Grostrovinsky, who is now considered to be one of the great composers of the 20th century, if not the most important composer of the 20th century. That's Jonah Leraghan. And Jonah tells the story of two concerts, one year apart, in the same city, the exact same piece of music. The audience that heard it first, and the audience that heard it second, heard totally different things. So let's begin first, Jonah. How does this sum just set the scene? This is May 1913. It's a spring night? It's a it's a balmy summer night.
Starting point is 00:37:27 Black Thai costumes, the women have their fedoras. This was evening clothes. Yeah, well, this was the Russian ballet. This was high art. And the program said this is a concert about spring time. But as they settled into their seats, it turns out that what Stravinsky had in mind was not spring like Cunybees. No. The spring Stravinsky had in mind was about change, about radical change. Ritual murder. Literally, that's what the story of the play is. It's a pagan ritual where the end of Virgin gets massacred. Oh dear.
Starting point is 00:38:01 Oh dear. But the music itself is fascinating. The beginning is this very charming bassoon. It's a classic Lithuanian folk tune, and it does sound like the earth is warming. And that lasts for about a minute. And then we get some tutti of flutes, and since lovely, it's getting a little more disturbing. And then about three minutes into it. Everything changes. There's just an earthquake.
Starting point is 00:38:52 Stravinsky plays this chord. There's a great story that when Diagolab, who's the head of the ballet ruse, first heard this chord, and Stravinsky was playing on piano form. He asked Ravinsky how long will it go on like that? And Svinsky looked at him, said, to the end, my dear. And it literally does. That chord structures the music. It's one of the most difficult sounds you've ever heard.
Starting point is 00:39:19 It is just the stereotype of dissonance. It is, it hurts you. Huh. Well Well what happened? Well after about three minutes they rioted. They what? They rioted. Meaning what? Like they screamed or through? They screamed. There was blood. Old ladies were hitting each other with kings. Why were all ladies? Oh ladies should have gone and hit Stravinsky with a king. But once they started screaming Stravinsky ran backstage
Starting point is 00:39:51 and by some accounts was crying. The Jinski was off on the side of the stage screaming to his dancers to keep the beat. Wow. Quite the fiasco. And the question is why. But this is the feeling question. Why so much feeling about a piece of music?
Starting point is 00:40:10 Why did they write? You would think that they write it because they were hot because they didn't like those sounds because they thought those dances were making strange and odd gestures. Well Jonah offers a different theory. Well let me put it this way. This ride has been talked about and written about for forever. But to the best of our knowledge, no one has ever tried to explain what happened that night through the lens of brain chemistry.
Starting point is 00:40:39 Brain chemistry. Yeah, what music can do to a brain? You know, if you try to imagine yourself where all you've heard is Wagner and the great romanticism of 19th century music. And then all of a sudden you get this. I mean, these are noises you've never heard before. No, it's all very new, but scientists are beginning to figure out what happens in our brain when we hear noises we've never heard before, especially dissonant noises. We find that chords, musical chords that are typically judged to be dissonant, elicit these wild fluctuations in brain activity.
Starting point is 00:41:28 This is Jan Fischman, he is a neuroscientist, and he studies those wild fluctuations in the brain. On an area of the brain called the auditory cortex. Let's zoom in to the auditory cortex for a moment, because this is basically hearing central. And when you're listening to music, there are all kinds of neurons during all kinds of things. One gang in particular, that Yon is interested in, that's right. A gang that he suspects gets very agitated
Starting point is 00:41:56 when it hears sounds like these. The new noise! These neurons might be the new noise department. Because he thinks their job is to take every new, strange, unordered, unpredictable noise that comes into the brain and figure it out. Find the pattern. There are groups of neurons whose sole job it is. This is how Jonah puts it.
Starting point is 00:42:22 To turn that dissonant note, dissect it, take it apart, and try to understand it. We are pattern-searching animals. And this is how Yaelan Fishman puts it. And so at the level of the auditory cortex, the brain has this daunting task of having to be able to disentangle this complex mixture of sounds. Most of the time, those neurons in the auditory cortex succeed in finding
Starting point is 00:42:49 the pattern. But every so often, maybe inside the brains, inside the heads of the people in the audience listening to the right of spring that night were all of these neurons, trying to make sense of the new sounds and failing. Not just failing once or twice, but over and over and over. Yeah, because the right of spring keeps being dissonant all the way through, so they can never get any rest. And when those neurons fail, repeatedly, there are consequences. Chemical consequences. What happens is our neurons scored out a bit of dopamine. And what does the dopamine do?
Starting point is 00:43:45 Well, dopamine makes us feel. A little dopamine makes you feel happy. That's why sex and drugs make you feel euphoric. But a little too much. The night euphoria turns into literally schizophrenia. Really? Yes. I don't want to oversimplify schizophrenia in any way, shape, or form.
Starting point is 00:44:04 But some of our most effective treatments for schizophrenia work by suppressing dopamine release in the brain. So there's some kind of relationship. Too much dopamine has been shown clinically to make people feel crazy. Yes. Maybe that's what happened that night on May 19, 1913. Music erupted, neurons revolted. Right.
Starting point is 00:44:22 Dopamine flooded through this to their brains. And people went mad. Literally mad. Let's goed. Right. Dopamine flooded through this is to their brains. And people went mad. Literally mad. Let's go to the second note. This piece comes back to Paris, does it not? Yes. How much later after the riot? From May to March.
Starting point is 00:44:36 Oh. So it's almost a year later. Yes. And this time it doesn't come with the ballet. This time it's just being performed as a work of music. So does anyone buy tickets? Oh yeah it's it's it's it's it's kind of sell out. They do cause a few nights of violent riots. Can you set up the situation now the audience? Is it a different audience? I actually don't know if the audience is
Starting point is 00:44:56 different. But we can at least say that the audience is coming to it with a different set of information. Exactly they've been warned. So for the first time they can actually sit back and really try to pay attention to the notes. By being willing to listen, they could hear the orders and patterns that Stravinsky had hidden in this work. They were able to hear the music and find the orders hidden underneath this noise. Was there a riot this time? Second round? Oh no, quite the opposite.
Starting point is 00:45:34 Stravinsky was a hero. They carried him out on their shoulders. Really? Literally he was, literally he was carried carrying the shoulders and the press was glowing. In one year, in one year. In just one year, Stravinsky had gone from villainous monster to hipster icon. To the extent that police had to escort him from the concert hall to keep him safe from adoring fans. And that was just the beginning.
Starting point is 00:46:06 The third story, if you wanted to tell a third story, it became children's music, it became Disney music in 1940. 27 years after Stravinsky caused a violent bloody riot. He was negotiating with Mickey Mouse over the rights to use his music in Fantasia. Which Fantasia? Is it starring a hippopotus in a little tutu? Is that the one? Is it the mushrooms, John?
Starting point is 00:46:27 Yeah, I think it's the mushrooms. It's mushrooms. So how does this happen? How do you go so quickly from being the most outrageous thing that literally maddens people to a triumph to kids' music? Yes. I mean, the right of spring is perfect evidence of the brain's astonishing plasticity.
Starting point is 00:46:45 See, this is the really cool part of it for me. If you remember just one bit of science from this whole thing, remember this. Those neurons we met earlier. The ones with little voices? I like them, yes. It turns out those neurons learn. And they learn fast. Because they're actually part of a larger network of brain cells with a very technical name.
Starting point is 00:47:08 Called the Coticofugal network. And what this network does is it's always sort of monitoring listening to the sounds that are coming into the brain and tuning those neurons. It's a better hear those sounds. We're trying to get the station on the radio just to getting it just right. And we're going to get the station on the radio just to getting it just right. So our neurons literally adjust. Literally, we're talking the biochemical engineering sense. So if on that first night you just hear the right as pure noise all the way through from
Starting point is 00:47:38 beginning to end, if you're listening, if you're letting your cortical fugal network do its job, it can actually re-sculpt your brain and let you hear the patterns better as the symphony evolves. Is it fair to say that this is a sort of tug of war that an artist comes, creates something that is new and unpredictable and strange and maybe noise-ish at first hearing. And the artist is thrilled to be new in that way. And then the brain ruins it all slowly but surely by making it familiar. Well, the brain uphorses to know.
Starting point is 00:48:19 The brain constantly wants to assimilate every experience we ever had and to every other experience. And I think Stravinsky realized it was the purpose of the artist to challenge the brain, to break the brain out of its conservative cycle. The astonishing thing to me is here you have an artist like Igor Stravinsky, who comes to town intentionally trying to get people to sit in their seats
Starting point is 00:48:44 and really listen to music. And the strategy he chooses is instead of pleasing them, he wants to put them in a little bit of discomfort or real pain even. And indeed, they not only listen, they riot. But within a year, and this is the sad part to me, within a year, it's easier to hear, suddenly it's pleasant, suddenly they like it, and suddenly U.S. Trvinsky is robbed of his newness. Why is that sad for you? Well, because it kind of, I don't know,
Starting point is 00:49:11 I never thought of the brain as the enemy of the artist before. Yeah, but I can give you a different interpretation on this. Here comes a guy who offers up the most dissonant, stabbing, percussive, painful music to anyone had heard to that point. And we learned to love it.
Starting point is 00:49:28 Doesn't that make you sort of feel pride? No, I know. We like our brains can decode anything. I, we learn to love it only because it's well made. Yeah, but we just random car honks. I don't think you could really appreciate that. I disagree. Are you thinking it would be like just...
Starting point is 00:49:44 Have you heard the music that was written after Stravinsky? It's even worse than what you just did. Yeah, but my brain has never accommodated that. But some people love that stuff. And my only point is that if there are these fixed poles in our ear between consonants and dissonance, which is how we started this whole thing. And now we end up learning that our brains can override that to such an astonishing degree.
Starting point is 00:50:12 Well, then culture wins. Culture beats biology. It's true, but to me, it's sad. Sad for the artist, it's not sad for us. It's sort of like the artists and the brains are it's not sad for us. It's sort of like the artists and the brains are in the kind of eternal struggle. That'll do it for today. Additional production by Sarah Cari.
Starting point is 00:50:37 And we will throw some new sounds at ya next week. Radio Lab was created by Chad Abum and is edited by Soren Wheeler. Lulu Miller and Lahten Nasser are our co-host. Dylan Keef is our director of sound design. Our staff includes Simon Adler, Jeremy Bloom, Becca Bressler, Rachel Kusik, Akati Foster Keys, W. Harry Fortuna, David Gable, Maria Paz Gutierrez, Sondu Yannam Sambanan, Matt Kielte, Annie McEwan, Alex Nisen, Sara Kari, Anna Ruskweppaz, Alyssa Drunk Perry, Sarah Sandback, Perian Wack, Pat Walters, and Molly Webster.
Starting point is 00:51:21 Our fact-chunkers are a diet, Kelly, Emily Krieger, and Natalie Middleton. Hi, this is Finn Calling Through Stores, Connecticut. Leadership Support for Radio Lab Science programming is provided by the Gordon and Betty Moore Foundation. Science Sandbox, Assignments Foundation Initiative, and the John Templeton Foundation. Foundational Support for Radio Lab was provided by the Alfred B. Sloan Foundation.

There aren't comments yet for this episode. Click on any sentence in the transcript to leave a comment.