Science Vs - Antidepressants

Episode Date: October 28, 2016

There’s an intriguing body of research that suggests the power of antidepressants doesn’t come from chemicals in the drugs, but from the power of placebo. Not everyone agrees, though. We speak to ...researchers and medical professionals on either side of the debate, and some wedged in-between -- Prof. Peter Kramer, psychiatrist and author of Ordinarily Well: the Case for Antidepressants; Prof. Irving Kirsch, psychologist and author of The Emperor’s New Drugs: Exploding the Antidepressant Myth; psychiatrist and radiologist Prof. Helen Mayberg; and psychiatrist Prof. Gregory Simon. Crisis Hotlines:US National Suicide Prevention Lifeline 1-800-273-TALK (2755)US Crisis Text Line Text “GO” to 741741Australian Lifeline 13 11 14Canadian Association for Suicide PreventionOur SponsorsCasper - Get $50 towards any mattress by visiting casper.com/sciencevs and use the promo code SCIENCEVSThird Love - Go to thirdlove.com/sciencevs to start your free trialWealthsimple – Investing made easy. Get your first $10,000 managed for free.Credits This episode has been produced by Wendy Zukerman, Heather Rogers, Shruti Ravindran, and Diane Wu. Our senior producer is Kaitlyn Sawrey. Edited by Annie-Rose Strasser and Caitlin Kenney. Fact checking by Michelle Harris. Sound design and music production by Matthew Boll, mixed by Martin Peralta and Matthew Boll. Music written by Bobby Lord and Martin Peralta. Selected References2008 study suggesting that antidepressants are not much better than placebo for people suffering in severe depression.2016 study suggesting that antidepressants were way better than placebo in treating people suffering from severe depression. 2016 study on how drug companies under-report side effects in clinical trials. 2003 round-up of the most common side-effects of antidepressants. 2013 study which uses brain imaging to try to pinpoint whether patients would respond better to medication or psychotherapy.  Learn more about your ad choices. Visit podcastchoices.com/adchoices

Transcript
Discussion (0)
Starting point is 00:00:00 After decades of shaky hands caused by debilitating tremors, Sunnybrook was the only hospital in Canada who could provide Andy with something special. Three neurosurgeons, two scientists, one movement disorders coordinator, 58 answered questions, two focused ultrasound procedures, one specially developed helmet, thousands of high-intensity focused ultrasound waves, zero incisions, and that very same day, two steady hands. From innovation to action, Sunnybrook is special. Learn more at sunnybrook.ca slash special. What does the AI revolution mean for jobs, for getting things done? Who are the people creating this technology?
Starting point is 00:00:39 And what do they think? I'm Rana El-Khelyoubi, an AI scientist, entrepreneur, investor, and now host of the new podcast, Pioneers of AI. Think of it as your guide for all things AI, with the most human issues at the center. Join me every Wednesday for Pioneers of AI. And don't forget to subscribe wherever you tune in. It's season three of The Joy of Why, and I still have a lot of questions. Like, what is this thing we call time? Why does altruism exist? And where is Jan Eleven? I'm here, astrophysicist and co-host, ready for anything.
Starting point is 00:01:18 That's right. I'm bringing in the A-team. So brace yourselves. Get ready to learn. I'm Jan Eleven. I'm Steve Strogatz. And this is Quantum Magazine's podcast, The Joy of Why. New episodes drop every other Thursday, starting February 1st. Hi, I'm Wendy Zuckerman, and you're listening to Gimlet Media's Science
Starting point is 00:01:39 Verses. This is the show where we pit facts against pharmaceuticals. Today we're tackling one of the biggest mental health questions of the decade. Do antidepressants really work? Now, if you're feeling depressed or you just want to talk to someone, in the US you can call the National Suicide Prevention Lifeline at 1800 27800-273-8255. That number will be on our website along with other resources. Currently, I'm on trazodone. Prozac. Prozac.
Starting point is 00:02:15 Venlafaxine. Welbutrin. Citalopram. Bupropion. Lexapro. We are well and truly living in the age of antidepressants. More than one in every 10 Americans over the age of 12 take antidepressant medication.
Starting point is 00:02:33 That's according to the Centers for Disease Control. And over the last two decades, the number of Americans on the meds has jumped nearly 400%. But it's not just Americans who are popping these pills. Globally, the number of people on antidepressants has increased over the decade. Since the late 1980s, these pills have been considered a major breakthrough in the treatment of depression.
Starting point is 00:02:58 But in 2008, a study came out that cracked the industry wide open. It said that antidepressants work about as well as a sugar pill, and because of that, there is little reason to give these pills to most people suffering from depression. Headlines came out screaming, antidepressants don't work, and a media frenzy began. For the millions of patients struggling with depression, putting their faith in medications to feel better, this won't come as happy news. The fight is about
Starting point is 00:03:30 antidepressants. The medical community is at war. And the war rages on today, with academics continuing to pump out papers and books with titles like Ordinarily Well, The Case for Antidepressants, and then those on the other side calling antidepressants the emperor's new drugs. Meanwhile, patients are left in the middle, not knowing whether they should take the drugs or not. So today we are going to wade through the studies and the counter-studies to find out, do the drugs work?
Starting point is 00:04:04 When it comes to antidepressants, there are lots of prescriptions. But then, there's science. Before we get into the debate about the drugs, let's talk about depression. The National Institutes of Mental Health describes it as a serious mood disorder that lasts for two weeks or more. But serious mood disorder can manifest in many, many different ways.
Starting point is 00:04:30 So to give you a tiny sense of what it feels like to have depression, we spoke to a group of people that are willing to spill their guts out to a microphone. New Yorkers. The way that I often describe depression is it's not really an emotional thing, it's more of a lack of emotion. Suicide didn't even make sense because there was no pleasure in the darkness either. There's just zero. I had a really short temper, I was really impatient,
Starting point is 00:04:56 and that's not typical. I was having difficulty sleeping, eating, I had like outbursts of random crying. I couldn't see the future being better than the present was. Oh, my God, that's, like, the worst feeling ever. Do I want to die? Do I not want to die? Like, it is very hard. Depression can feel like an attack on the inside. The people we spoke to said that something small could set off a cascade of internal abuse
Starting point is 00:05:25 that hijacks the entire day or even the entire week. It's an intense experience that can hold you prisoner, they said. So could a pill really be the key to escaping that internal prison? There are several different types of antidepressants on the market and many of them boost levels of particular chemicals in your brain that are known to help regulate your mood. Now, some drugs, for example, will pump up levels of serotonin, while newer drugs give your brain access to more serotonin as well as another chemical called norepinephrine. On the website of one of the country's best-selling antidepressants,
Starting point is 00:06:06 it says, quote, experts believe that depression results when certain chemicals in the brain are out of balance, end quote. So an injection of these certain missing chemicals should be making people happy. But what if the chemicals in these drugs aren't actually doing anything? There's an intriguing body of research that suggests the power of these drugs doesn't come from any chemical inside the medication, but rather from the power of placebo. The idea of placebo is basically that your thoughts can change your biology. So with depression, you could be taking a pill that is actually just a sugar pill,
Starting point is 00:06:49 but if you believe it's the real medicine, then you'll feel better, just because you expect to. And the placebo effect can change how we respond to all kinds of things, from pain relief to hypnosis to the treatment of irritable bowel syndrome. And it's this thing that Irving Kirsch, a psychologist and researcher, has devoted his career to. And he's the one that did that study in 2008 on antidepressants, the study that sparked this whole debate over whether these drugs work or not. My name is Irving Kirsch. I'm the Associate Director of the program in placebo
Starting point is 00:07:26 studies at the Harvard Medical School. And my favorite color is, believe it or not, black. Maybe I like black and red, the anarchist colors. Interesting for a man who's trying to tear down the way that we treat people with depression. Now, Irving has studied the placebo effect in all sorts of things. But in the 1990s, he started to get curious about the placebo effect in people with depression, because he thought that depression would be exactly the kind of condition that would have a big placebo effect. One reason for that is that a core feature of depression is hopelessness, a sense of hopelessness. One of the things they're hopeless about is ever getting better. And so now they've got a new, there's a new kind of drug coming out that hasn't been tested yet.
Starting point is 00:08:16 It's being tested in this new clinical trial. Maybe that will work. That's something that might instil a sense of hope. Irving went about studying antidepressants by getting clinical trial data from the Food and Drug Administration. These are the guys that approve where the drugs are allowed to be on the market. Because the thing is that the placebo effect is such a powerful effect
Starting point is 00:08:41 that to get a drug approved by the FDA, drug companies have to consider it in their trials. Typically, what drug companies will do is they'll get a group of people, they'll give them a particular drug that they're testing, and then they'll get another group of people and they'll put them on a placebo. Then the company pretty much has to show that the drug worked better than the placebo. But here's the thing. Drug companies don't have to publish all of their studies, so they can pick and choose what they release to make their drugs look better than perhaps they are.
Starting point is 00:09:13 Now, this is currently changing, but at the time that all of the antidepressants on the market were approved, that was the system. So Irving did some detective work, got all of the data, the published and the unpublished stuff, and what did he find? The surprising thing that we found was that the people got better when they were taking the drug, but they also got better when they were taking the placebo, and the difference
Starting point is 00:09:41 between the response to the placebo and the response to the active drug was much smaller than one would expect from a treatment that had been heralded as constituting a revolution in the treatment of depression. To him, this discovery was a real breakthrough, something that could change the way that depressed people are treated. But many of his colleagues didn't feel that way. The reaction was of many people, well, this can't be true. Of course, antidepressants work.
Starting point is 00:10:15 Then came even more research suggesting that Irving was onto something. Around the same time as Irving's 2008 paper came out, scientists were discovering that the claim that the drug companies were making about people with depression needing to boost certain chemicals in their brain wasn't right. In fact, it turned out that people with depression weren't necessarily low on certain chemicals and didn't need them to be topped up. Now there are other theories about how these drugs
Starting point is 00:10:45 work, but the truth is we still don't know what these drugs are doing. So could it really be all placebo? Irving published more and more papers in 2012, 2014 and 2015, all finding that antidepressants really aren't that much better than a dummy pill. But doctors keep writing prescriptions for antidepressants. Irving says it's because they too are being fooled by the placebo effect. And he says there is a rich history of mistaking placebo for effective medicine. That's why for so many centuries, bloodletting was thought to be an effective treatment. You have to go back and you'll see the kinds of treatments that they used to use successfully in the history of medicine.
Starting point is 00:11:37 It included things like frog sperm and feathers and fur and spiders that were given as treatments that people would ingest. And if it didn't kill them, it might make them better. And that's part of the history of the placebo effect. And how did they get the frog sperm? I have no idea. Good question. Now, to be fair, the psychiatrists that we spoke to said, yeah, we get it.
Starting point is 00:12:04 We know that not everyone who is depressed should be given these pills. And lots of psychiatrists, in fact, won't prescribe drugs for some of their depressed patients. The National Institute for Health and Care Excellence, a leading health agency in the UK, specifically says that people with mild depression usually shouldn't be offered antidepressants. They suggest other things, like therapy. But Irving and his research are taking this debate one step further and saying that antidepressants are essentially frog sperm
Starting point is 00:12:37 and that no one, not even those who are suicidal, should be on the meds. Not even the very most depressed patients. No one shows that much difference between drug and placebo. Not even close. Irving doesn't see patients anymore. But at the time his research started coming out, he did. And he was so confident in his findings
Starting point is 00:13:00 that he changed the way he treated his patients. When I started seeing the data, I stopped referring clients that I was seeing for antidepressant medication. So even if he's right and antidepressants are all placebo, who cares? What's the problem with giving people sugar pills if it'll make them feel better, if they can get out of bed and go to work. Who cares what's doing that? Just give the people their frog sperm. Well, not so fast, says Irving. There's something else to consider here. The frogs. No, there's actually something else to consider here. Lots of something else's.
Starting point is 00:13:45 These drugs have many side effects. I started to realise that I was becoming zombitised. It's like really sleepy all the time. Yeah, I lost weight. I gained a lot of weight at one point. Like I was like three steps behind my actual body. I had loss of sex drive and I gained some weight. At the beginning, I had a shockingly low libido.
Starting point is 00:14:07 It affects you sexually. Stuff down there does not work when you're on it. A 2014 analysis of a bunch of studies found that on average, around 30 to 50% of adults treated with antidepressants had some kind of sexual problem from taking their meds. There are also rare but noted side effects, like having heart problems or being at an increased risk of gastrointestinal bleeding.
Starting point is 00:14:31 And Irving says that this list is even longer. He points out that drug companies don't always publish their side effects. A 2014 study backed that up. It looked at more than 100 trials of antidepressants and antipsychotics and compared the numbers of side effects in the published and unpublished documents. On average, the drug companies left out more than 40% of the side effects from their published studies. Bottom line, Irving says that the risks of antidepressants are just too high
Starting point is 00:15:03 and the benefit is just too small. Continuing to tout antidepressants as a first-line treatment, given data that placebos work almost as well, and that antidepressants have the greatest risks in terms of side effects and health risks, to tout that as the first-line treatment is irresponsible and doing harm to depressed people. But while Irving may be confident in his work, it is by no means scientific consensus. There are a lot of doctors and researchers out there who disagree with him. Strongly. But I want to talk a little about what he just told you, because I think the case for antidepressants as placebo is a house built of fantasy bricks. Dr. Peter Kramer is one of the loudest critics of Irving's work.
Starting point is 00:15:59 That was him at a debate with Irving earlier this year. Peter is a psychiatrist and professor emeritus at Brown University. He's written books on the benefits of antidepressants. And to give you a sense of how much Irving and Peter disagree on the importance of antidepressants, Peter's first book was called Listening to Prozac. You might have heard of it. And Irving wrote a paper soon afterwards called
Starting point is 00:16:22 Listening to Prozac but Hearing Placebo. Yikes. Peter has been treating patients with depression since the late 1970s and he remembers the world before antidepressant drugs were widely prescribed. You saw people who were seriously depressed at a level you just don't see that often today. There were people who looked like a Durer etching or a Blue Period Picasso painting. I mean, thin, wracked with pain, wringing their hands, repeating depressive phrases. And in those days, it was treated with psychotherapy.
Starting point is 00:17:02 Peter believes that the age of antidepressants that we're living in today isn't driven by powerful placebo, but rather by doctors like him seeing how powerful these drugs really are. You know, you give these medicines to people, they start feeling substantially better within weeks. I think when doctors saw how well these medicines worked, you know, often they were impressed and adopted them wholeheartedly. And the thing is that Peter relies on a raft of studies to back up his belief that antidepressants are better than dummy pills. Studies like a 2008 analysis of 74 clinical trials looking at the published and unpublished data
Starting point is 00:17:43 and found that the drugs were, quote, shown to be superior to placebo, end quote. Well, there's just a host of evidence that antidepressants do outperform placebo. And, you know, I think the debunking case is just bad science. So, what is going on here? And how can everyone have their own studies to back up the exact opposite conclusion?
Starting point is 00:18:14 Especially when, get this, they are all using clinical trial data that they got from the FDA. How can they get the exact opposite answer? Is it magic? No. Madness? No. It's because of this. Hamilton. We'll explain after the break. Welcome back. We've just introduced you to two professors with very, very different ideas about antidepressants. They both have studies to back up the claims that they are right.
Starting point is 00:18:57 What magic are they using to get their results? No magic, except for the magic of statistics. It all has to do with the tool that you use to crunch the numbers and see whether a drug works. So children, snuggle up and let me tell you a tale of how depression drugs get on the market and how scientists use the same data to make their own arguments.
Starting point is 00:19:29 It all starts when you measure how sick someone is. You have to do this in a clinical trial so then you can see whether someone improves or not after they've taken a pill or after they've popped a placebo. Now, while doctors can check your blood pressure for hypertension or do a test to see if you've had an infection, researchers are yet to find an objective test for depression. So to figure out if someone has improved after taking antidepressants, they have to ask all these questions about how you're feeling, and then they tally up the points, and then they use those results to rank their patient's symptoms on a scale to see just how depressed they were
Starting point is 00:20:05 before and after taking the drugs. And there are a few different scales that you can use for depression, but one of the most popular is called the Hamilton Scale. Yes, the Hamilton Scale. And it's been used for more than 50 years. This survey includes questions like, do you think that life is not worth living? And if you do, you get one point on the scale. But here's another question on the same scale. Do you have heavy feelings in your abdomen? Like, are you feeling constipated?
Starting point is 00:20:39 If you do, you get one point. Have you lost your appetite? Do you feel like people have let you down? Do you get gassy? Do you lost your appetite? Do you feel like people have let you down? Do you get gassy? Do you have mild headaches? Do you feel restless at night? If you do, for each one, you get one point on the Hamilton scale. Those with the higher points have the more severe depression.
Starting point is 00:20:57 Lower scores mean milder depression. Now, in a drug trial, researchers will take the patient's scores at the beginning and end of a trial, looking to see what's changed. And they'll be specifically looking for the difference between how people's scores changed if they were put on the placebo and how people's scores changed if they were put on the drugs. The goal here is for researchers to see if there is a significant difference between the effects of placebo and the effects of the drugs.
Starting point is 00:21:31 And this, this is important. Because it's where Irving, Dr. Placebo, and where Peter, Dr. Prozac, diverge. I didn't want to be Dr. Prozac, diverge. I didn't want to be Dr. Prozac. Okay, okay. So it's here where Dr. Placebo and Dr. I'm not Dr. Prozac diverge. You see, like we said before, it's all about how you crunch the numbers. There's statistical significance,
Starting point is 00:22:01 which is what Peter points to to help make his case that antidepressants work, it's been the gold standard of measuring drug effectiveness for decades. For all drugs, not just antidepressants. And statistical significance measures whether the results of your clinical trial were not due to random chance. That is, they weren't just a fluke. But there is also something called clinical significance, which is a higher bar to jump.
Starting point is 00:22:32 And this is what Irving, Dr Placebo, uses. And this is a test that's supposed to more accurately reflect the patient experience, that is, whether the patients feel like the drug is actually working. So when Irving says, Even with the most severely depressed, you don't get a meaningful difference. What he really means is this. There's a statistically significant difference between drug and placebo, but it's not a clinically meaningful difference.
Starting point is 00:23:05 So statistical significance. This is what we will call the low bar, because it's simply asking whether the difference found between the placebo and the drug was not due to random chance. And clinical significance, that's the high bar. Do people feel better on the drugs? So this is what the debate is all about. And it's why it's such a hot media topic. Because it's all about statistics. So, now that we're all together on this one, low bar, high bar, let me tell you a little bit more about the high bar.
Starting point is 00:23:46 The high bar was determined by a leading public health body in the UK and it requires that people taking the antidepressants improve by at least three points on the Hamilton scale compared to those on placebo. It's worth pointing out that Irving is not alone in wanting to use the high bar. Even beyond depression research, in many areas of medicine, there has been a long push to use higher benchmarks than statistical significance. Now, in theory, it's a great idea to use a high bar to judge a drug's effectiveness, right? To show that the changes you see are going to be
Starting point is 00:24:25 important to the patient. But there are real questions about whether the scale used to set this bar, the Hamilton scale, can actually do that. Remember those questions on the Hamilton scale where saying you have a heavy abdomen will get you the same amount of points as feeling like life is not worth living? Or to Peter, that's just wrong and misleading. And it can make placebos look more effective in clinical trials than they really are. People in the placebo arm of the trial, you know, may do better in terms of their constipation or sleep without really doing better in their core depression.
Starting point is 00:25:03 Plus, antidepressants can actually increase your score on the scale because they have side effects that actually cause sleep disruption and headaches, making someone's depression on the Hamilton scale look worse, even while the drugs actually might be helping their depression. And Peter isn't the only one that has a problem with Irving's high bar. A 2008 paper called it, quote, downright arbitrary, end quote. And on that, here's another thing you have to know about Dr. Placebo, Irving. His studies actually find that people with extremely severe depression
Starting point is 00:25:45 do jump his high bar on the drugs. But get this, that three-point difference between drug and placebo needed to clear the high bar, that's not good enough for him anymore. Now we have found out that even that would be an overly liberal and overly lenient criterion, and you would need a seven-point change in order for somebody to be rated by their clinician as minimally improved.
Starting point is 00:26:19 That should be someone, if you're getting any benefit, it should at least be minimal improvement. But while Irving keeps moving the bar, the arguments from antidepressant proponents also run into trouble. Because if the scale is broken and we can't properly measure depression, then a lot of clinical trials that show that the drugs work, the ones that rely on that low bar, they're wrong too.
Starting point is 00:26:49 Because here's the thing. They also use the Hamilton scale. Hamilton! And there are other scales that you can use that focus on things like whether you can hold down a job or they try to look at your overall quality of life. But the thing is, these scales don't consistently show that the drugs are that much better than placebo either.
Starting point is 00:27:08 All that's to say, we don't know how to accurately measure whether someone's depression is getting better. And that's not all we don't know. Like we said in the first half of the show, we don't know exactly what these drugs do in the brain. Plus, a lot of this debate about placebo versus antidepressants is based off short-term studies. Many antidepressant clinical trials only go for six or eight weeks,
Starting point is 00:27:34 so we don't have much research on what these drugs are doing in the long term or what placebo does in the long term. And another thing we don't know, how depression works in the brain. Helen Mayberg is a professor of neurology and psychiatry at Emory University. She studies brain scans of highly depressed people and she thinks about all the unknowns in quite a beautiful way. It's like an orchestra.
Starting point is 00:28:00 Within a brain area, you know, all the violins have to play together. They have to be synchronised properly with the bassoon and the basses and the flutes. You know, we can come up with all kinds of clever metaphors about brain choreography, but at the end of the day, what is a system that emotional pain can lock you down to not even get away? No one knows. So the issue is, it's a mystery. Given all of these mysteries and unknowns here, Helen says a drug that clears the low bar, statistical significance, might actually be important. It might just be enough of a lift for some people. How can anyone judge statistically
Starting point is 00:28:51 what's a meaningful clinical change? It's like if you're trying to get up the side of a mountain and the placebo gets you part of the way, but it's that extra kick that gets you over the ledge from the active drug. One doesn't want a discount that needed extra boost. And I think that until we really have a way to quantify what that extra part is, I'll still take getting over the ledge if I need the extra help to get there. Still though, the fact that this whole debate is about a little lift kind of tells you that on average, the drugs aren't that great. The problem is, is that our drugs aren't as good
Starting point is 00:29:44 as we want, which doesn't mean that they're worthless. And this was something that we heard from a few doctors. Gregory Simon is a clinical psychiatrist and chair of the Scientific Advisory Board of the Depression and Bipolar Support Alliance. Whew, what a title. I don't think anyone, or at least anyone who practices much or pays much attention, would
Starting point is 00:30:05 say that the current generation of antidepressant drugs is remarkably effective or terribly successful. His clinical work has given him a chance to see these drugs work in practice. And he says that, yeah, it's been a bit of a bummer. They're actually quite disappointing. You know, they are modestly effective, highly variable in their effects from person to person, and lots of them have side effects. And that's not a very good situation. I would certainly, in my clinical practice, I make do with what I have. So I certainly prescribe those medicines, and I've seen them help a lot of people.
Starting point is 00:30:39 But would I wish for something better? Absolutely. That high variability that Gregory is talking about might be explained by the fact that different people respond to antidepressant drugs in different ways. The results from drug trials are an average. So people who might be responding really well versus people that might be responding really poorly just get washed out in the mix. Helen Mayberg, who talked about the orchestra in people's minds, is researching these different responses.
Starting point is 00:31:09 Her work using brain scans is showing that there does seem to be different types of brains that need different types of treatment. We will learn along the way that when we start to get people the treatment that matches their brain type, then we'll be able to see that if you get drug and you should have gotten drug, do you stay well?
Starting point is 00:31:32 Because that is the treatment needed. And even though this is a live research question, who should get the drugs? Helen says it's an important part of the equation. I don't think that anyone can deny that there is a placebo response with antidepressants. The question becomes is that because there is an effect above the placebo, is that difference worth it at the individual patient?
Starting point is 00:32:01 Helen, who regularly sees people with severe depression as part of her research, says that because you don't know how different brains are going to react, it's worth giving the drugs the benefit of the doubt. And Gregory, who treats patients with depression, agrees. Irving, though, he doesn't see patients anymore. Your studies and your words are very strong about antidepressants not being effective. And then when you imagine someone with a patient right in front of them
Starting point is 00:32:33 and there's nothing that's working and they're suicidal. Then you might reach for, be tempted certainly to reach for anything. I don't know what I would do at this point. I'm glad I'm not in that position. One's heart goes out to the suffering. And it's hard when you haven't found anything else to not look for something that might work. So when it comes to science versus antidepressants,
Starting point is 00:33:03 do they stack up? Do the drugs work better than a placebo? Well, it depends. If you use the most scientifically accepted standard, statistical significance, then yes, they do. But if you raise the bar to try to see a clinical significance, then, for the most part, they don't. But there are problems with this higher bar because the way that we measure depression is kind of crude
Starting point is 00:33:35 and it doesn't take in all the nuances of the disease. Either way, you count it, though, and this is important, for people with very severe depression, the drugs help more than placebo. But given the unknowns here, Dr. Helen Mayberg and other doctors that we spoke to do hope that patients will try to get all of the treatment that they can. If you can get better on placebo, that's great, because that means your brain has a capacity to repair itself. But if someone does take a sugar pill and continues to do badly, I would certainly hope that the fear of side effects wouldn't stop people from actually entertaining the idea that an actual antidepressant medication or other treatment might be in their best interest. The thing is, when it comes to depression, sometimes you need to try a few things to find out what works.
Starting point is 00:34:34 Because at the moment, the science isn't good enough to say whether the pills will or won't work for you. For now, though, what we can say is there is no evidence that frog sperm is useful. That's science versus antidepressants. And by the way, this is our last episode of the season, but don't worry, we'll be back in the spring of 2017. In the US, that is. We'll also be occasionally sending out interesting little tidbits, so keep your ears open. And hang around after the ads to hear some of our favourite moments that didn't make it into this season of Science Versus. Sometimes you need to kill your darlings.
Starting point is 00:35:18 Not you guys. We love you guys. This episode has been produced by Heather Rogers, Diane Wu and Truti Ravindran. Our senior producer is Caitlin Sorey. Edited by Annie Rose Strasser and Caitlin Kenny. Fact-checking by Michelle Harris. A big thanks to Carly Schwartz for sharing her story with us and to all the New Yorkers who are happy to share their very personal stories
Starting point is 00:35:39 with a complete stranger holding a microphone. That's why we love New York. Sound design and music production by Matthew Boll, mixed by Martin Peralta and Bobby Lord. Music written by Martin Peralta and Bobby Lord. You can always keep in touch on Twitter. We're at Science Versus, that's Science VS, or on Facebook. Yes, we are on the faces. We are also doing our very first live show in Toronto for the Hot Docs Podcast Festival on November the 19th. So go to Toronto, have a visit. Toronto. Sorry, I meant Toronto. You don't pronounce the T. Learning. Thanks to everyone who helped get our new season of Science Versus off the ground. The engineering team at Gimlet, Alex Bloomberg, Caitlin Kenny,
Starting point is 00:36:26 Austin Mitchell, Annie Rose Strasser, a big thanks to Leopold Rogers and the whole Rogers clan and the Ludwig family, the Sorry fam, Joseph Lavelle Wilson and all the Zuckerman family, the original Science vs Fact Checkers. We couldn't have done it without you. I was aiming for the target. Yes! F***!
Starting point is 00:36:58 People can come at you with a bat. They can come at you with a tomahawk, with a car, with a pointy stick. Tomahawk was your second one? Yeah, I just happened to buy one tonight. No bug spray. No bug spray. The guy's like, you go to Sam Houston? I'm like, no, I'm a journalist.
Starting point is 00:37:17 I'm going to look at dead bodies. We need bug spray. And he's like, oh. They fling themselves out of the maggot mass. they'll go flying like ping And then yeah like popcorn Yeah they're just they're pinging out The idea that A kind of Hollywood style Thrusting
Starting point is 00:37:34 Into the vagina was going to give you Sexual pleasure And the harder thrusting was going to give you more Ay mami tiene tan cuervas Y yo soy sin freno. Like, you've got so many curves and I don't have any brakes on my car. That's a cute one. A little farther away.
Starting point is 00:37:52 What is your name? My name is Heather. What's your name? Leo. You are becoming more and more convinced that Joe is in love with you. And just in case, can you just tell people they're not in love with Joe? You're not in love with Joe. You don't even know who Joe is in love with you. And just in case, can you just tell people they're not in love with Joe? You're not in love with Joe. You don't even know who Joe is. Just so happens my boyfriend's name is Joe.
Starting point is 00:38:12 You're in love with Joe. You're in love with Joe. I'm Wendy Zuckerman. Fact you next time.

There aren't comments yet for this episode. Click on any sentence in the transcript to leave a comment.