Something You Should Know - Weird Factors That Distort Your Thinking & How Animals Perceive the World
Episode Date: May 5, 2022Holding on to a grudge can feel righteous. After all, you’ve been wronged. You are entitled to hold that grudge. However, there are a lot more reasons - and better ones - to let that grudge go. This... episode begins with an explanation and a great quote to remember about grudges. Source: Judith Kuriansky author of The Complete Idiot’s Guide to a Healthy Relationship (https://amzn.to/3KQEetb) So many things can alter your thinking in ways you don’t realize. For example, the human brain has trouble making sense of fractions. We are also less open minded to ideas that conflict with what we already believe even if the evidence is obvious. And there are other ways your thinking gets distorted. Joining me to discuss this is James Zimring, Professor of Experimental Pathology at the University of Virginia School of Medicine and author of the book Partial Truths: How Fractions Distort our Thinking (https://amzn.to/3LxUj85). The world animals live in is very different than ours. Mainly, that’s because the way they sense their surroundings is so much different. Some animals see what we can’t, others hear what we can’t while others taste what we can’t. And the more we know about this, the better we can understand our own senses. Listen as my guest Jackie Higgins explains. Jackie is a writer, producer and director of films about animals and she is author of a book called Sentient: How Animals Illuminate the Wonder of Our Human Senses (https://amzn.to/3OUqY9S). If you struggle with your weight, your allergy medicine may be partially to blame. Some common antihistamines have been linked to weight gain in some people. Listen as I explain what the research says. https://www.verywellhealth.com/do-antihistamines-cause-weight-gain-83094 PLEASE SUPPORT OUR SPONSORS! Go to https://Indeed.com/Something to claim your sponsored job $75 credit to upgrade your job post! Offer good for a limited time. With Avast One, https://avast.com you can confidently take control of your online world without worrying about viruses, phishing attacks, ransomware, hacking attempts, & other cybercrimes! With prices soaring at the pump, Discover has your back with cash back! Use the Discover Card & earn 5% cash back at Gas Stations and Target, now through June, when you activate. Get up to $75 cash back this quarter with Discover it® card. Learn more at https://discover.com/rewards Small Businesses are ready to thrive again and looking for resources to rise to the challenge. That’s why Dell Technologies has assembled an all-star lineup of podcasters (and we're one of them!) for the third year in a row to create a virtual conference to share advice and inspiration for Small Businesses. Search Dell Technologies Small Business Podference on Audacy.com, Spotify or Apple podcasts starting May 10th! Today is made for Thrill! Style, Power, Discovery, Adventure, however you do thrill, Nissan has a vehicle to make it happen at https://nissanusa.com Use SheetzGo on the Sheetz app! Just open the app, scan your snacks, tap your payment method and go! Learn more about your ad choices. Visit megaphone.fm/adchoices
Transcript
Discussion (0)
Metro Links and Cross Links are reminding everyone to be careful as Eglinton Crosstown LRT train testing is in progress.
Please be alert as trains can pass at any time on the tracks.
Remember to follow all traffic signals, be careful along our tracks, and only make left turns where it's safe to do so.
Be alert, be aware, and stay safe.
Today on Something You Should Know, why if you're holding a grudge, you really need to let it go.
Then, some interesting ways your thinking gets distorted, which can lead to some bad outcomes.
For example, there's this thing called the primacy effect,
which is what people learn early on,
they will hold on to despite evidence to the contrary.
And so this is probably the biggest,
baddest cognitive property that we have.
Also how allergy medicines can make you fat
and the world animals live in is so different than ours
because of how they sense and perceive the world. For example, catfish, they sense their world
through taste and they don't have a tongue. Their whole body is their tongue. All the flanks of
their body are covered in taste buds. All this today on Something You Should Know.
Bumble knows it's hard to start conversations.
Hey.
No, too basic.
Hi there.
Still no.
What about hello, handsome?
Who knew you could give yourself the ick?
That's why Bumble is changing how you start conversations.
You can now make the first move or not. With opening moves, you simply choose a question to be automatically
sent to your matches. Then sit back and let your matches start the chat. Download Bumble and try it
for yourself. Something you should know. Fascinating intel. The world's top experts and practical advice you can use in your life
today something you should know with mike carothers hi welcome to something you should know
i suspect all of us have at some point in our lives held a grudge against somebody and it's
probably a good idea not to hold on to that grudge very long.
I remember hearing this quote a long time ago. It was from Dr. Judith Kuriansky, who wrote a book
called The Complete Idiot's Guide to a Healthy Relationship. And she says that holding a grudge
is like drinking poison yourself and hoping it makes the other person sick.
I always like that.
The resentment you feel by holding a grudge can do some real damage to you emotionally and physically.
Not only does it interfere with your present pleasure and keep you focused on the pains of the past,
it could also destroy a salvageable
relationship. The physical pain can include a tight chest, tense muscles, upset stomach, headaches,
and poor sleep. Letting go of a grudge isn't always easy, but it may be worth the effort.
You might just try speaking up and let the other person know what they did that
bothers you and try to offer a solution. And if an apology from the other person is in order,
you'll probably get one. And then you'll feel a whole lot better. And that is something you should
know. Fractions distort your thinking.
Now, marketers know this and use this.
Perhaps the simplest example is gas prices.
They're always expressed as a number and then nine-tenths of a cent
to help make you think that it's actually costing less than it does.
That's just one example.
It does seem that the brain doesn't really
grasp fractions very well without some considerable effort, and that can cause some trouble for
all of us, according to my guest, James Zimring. He's professor of experimental pathology at
the University of Virginia School of Medicine and author of the book, Partial Truths, How
Fractions Distort Our Thinking.
Hey, James, welcome.
Hi there.
Thanks for having me.
So I think a good way to explain this rather than trying to explain the concept, just give
an example of how fractions distort our thinking. A comical example is that in the 1980s,
A&W fast food chain took on a gargantuan task, which was they were trying to unseat the Big Mac
as the king of the burgers in America, which had been dominant since 1971.
So they put together what they thought would be a much better burger. And by blinded tests,
it tasted better, it was cheaper, and it was more meat.
So they thought, wow, this is really going to do the trick. And when they released it on the market,
it did not sell well at all. They had to pull it eventually. And when they did a market analysis
after the fact, what they discovered was that many people thought a third pound was less than a quarter pound because three is less than four.
Well, what seems weird to me about that is anybody who took arithmetic in grade school
learned that a quarter of a pound is less than a third of a pound.
A quarter of a cup is less than a third of a cup.
I guess people just reflexively think four is more than
three. So what could A&W have done to avoid the confusion? Well, that's a good question that I'm
probably not qualified to answer. I guess they could have said the bigger burger, you know,
or just explicitly stated that as a fact. Yeah, right. There's an obvious solution.
They could have said 30% of a pound instead of 25% of a pound. But in this case, which is funny,
A&W wasn't trying to deceive people. Actually, just the opposite. It's when people are trying
to deceive us that it probably becomes much more problematic. For example, and maybe this is intentional or not, if you look at mutual funds
when they're first released for us to invest money in, if we have money to invest, that's a pretty
serious thing. And mutual funds, people don't usually invest in them unless there's some track
record of how they did, because who's the manager? What are they charging? How do they do in the stock market? Well, companies that release mutual funds often, not always, do something
that's called pre-incubating their funds. So they'll kind of have their funds running without
public investment for a couple of years. And the ones that do really well, then they make available
to the public. The ones that do really poorly, you never hear about.
They never release them. They destroy them. Now, on the surface, that seems almost like good
consumer advocacy. Why would you release to the public a product that you had not yet tested?
But what is really going on here, at least to some extent, is that imagine if none of the
funds, their strategy mattered.
If none of the funds had the ability to really outperform the market average, but just by
chance some would and some wouldn't.
They're only showing you then the ones that just happen to do well.
They're only showing you the top of the fraction and and they're selling you kind of a false product.
Now, how would we know if that was true? If that was true, then pre-incubated funds,
as soon as they were released to the public, would stop performing well, or as well as they had.
And that is the case, that once pre-incubated funds that really outperformed the market are
released, they usually hit the market average. That may or may not be intentional, but that's a rather serious manifestation of how
subtle fractional differentiations can have huge effects on people's livelihoods.
Well, I sort of get what you're saying, but that is a little difficult to get your head around
because it does seem that basically they're just testing out their
strategy ahead of time before they release it to the public. What's maybe an easier to understand
example of how this works? So let's consider for a minute self-help books or books that are going
to show you how to become rich, you know, and they'll often have a title of something like the five habits of self-made millionaires. And the implication is if you find
the things that self-made millionaires do, and then you do those things, you're more likely
to become a millionaire. Well, I can promise you that self-made millionaires have the following
habits. They eat, they breathe, they sleep,
they yawn. Obviously, you're not going to become a millionaire by doing those things.
But the point I'm trying to make is, it's not what do self-made millionaires do,
it's what do self-made millionaires do that other people don't do. And that second part,
that's the bottom of the fraction, It's seldom, if ever, contained
in these seminars and books that you might buy. Ask that question. Go to a self-help seminar,
and they'll say, well, these are the habits that highly disruptive startup companies do.
And if you ask the question, okay, that's interesting, but of the companies that
fail, of the companies that aren't disruptive, that don't do well, do they have the same properties or not? And you usually won't get an answer. So by just presenting part of what has
the form of a fraction, you can distort the message you're making. Now, remember, nothing
that's coming out of your mouth is untrue here. None of the numbers you're giving are incorrect.
You're just presenting part of what has an underlying fractional form,
and that can be very deceptive.
Yeah, I think that is so true.
You have to be so on guard about what people say
and then the evidence they use to support what they say
because on the surface, it might look right,
but if you dig deeper, you go, wait, no, wait, no,
wait a minute. Wait a minute. What's another example of that?
If you talk about claims that are made about products having some benefit to you,
crystals having curative properties, it's not a question of whether somebody tried medicinal
crystals and got better. Some people will try
medicinal crystals and they will get better. And that is evidence that they might work.
The question is, do people who use medicinal crystals get better at a more frequently or at
a higher rate than people would have just gotten better on their own, right? So until you juxtapose with and without this thing, you can't tell.
All you can tell is that you're kind of cherry picking an association. And again, this isn't
necessarily some sinister plan to manipulate people because we do it to ourselves all the time.
It's called the availability heuristic, where we replace what comes to mind easily for what we think is most likely or most probable.
But again, it's also a way that people can purposely manipulate us if they want to.
You talk about, and I remember when this was a story about the Dungeons and Dragons and
teenagers killing themselves and all that. Can you tell that story? Yeah. So in 1979, there was a gentleman, a young
man named James Dallas Egbert. He went by the name Dallas. He was a child prodigy, brilliant kid.
And he was at Michigan State University. And he was fairly troubled. And one of the things that
he did was play Dungeons and Dragons. Now at that time,
D&D was a relatively new game. It was confusing to parents, right? There's this game, but there's
no board. People sit around and they role play. It has demonic tones to it. And there was just a
lot of confusion about it. And Dallas, as he went by, disappeared from Michigan State University.
He left a note, which seemed like a suicide note, disappeared from Michigan State University.
He left a note, which seemed like a suicide note, but they weren't sure.
And they launched an investigation to find him.
And one of the detectives who was looking for him came up with the theory that he had killed himself in the steam tunnels under the Michigan State as part of a Dungeons and
Dragons game that got out of control.
Well, they searched the steam tunnels.
They searched all around. The newspapers had headlines stating as a matter of fact that that
was why he disappeared. Even 60 Minutes got involved in a segment with Gary Gygax, the
inventor of Dungeons and Dragons. And it turns out that Dallas ran away because he was so troubled and wound up.
They found him about a month later in Morgan City, Louisiana, you know, 1100 miles away or so in an apartment where he was just kind of being despondent.
And eventually he called the detectives. They didn't find him. He said, please come get me.
And they they took him back to his parents. And this caused the light to shine on Dungeons and Dragons. And over the next five years or so, there were 28 cases of murder or suicide by adolescents
who were playing D&D.
And the idea entered people's heads that this is a dangerous game and it's causing kids
to kill themselves.
And to be fair, that was a very appropriate concern.
You have 28 suicides associated with this game.
It's something that needs to be investigated.
But when they investigated it,
they found that at that time, there were already 3 million people playing Dungeons and Dragons
across the country, basically 3 million adolescents, because that's where it was most
popular. Now, the suicide rate at that time was 120 for every million adolescents, which is basically 360 suicides a year you would expect
without any outside influence. And so when you say, wow, there were 28 suicides playing Dungeons
and Dragons, that seems like a really dangerous game. But when you consider that there were 28
suicides out of 3 million people playing, and the rate of suicide should have been 360 out of 3 million anyway. If anything,
the game had a protective effect instead of a damaging effect. And so this is what happens,
again, if you look just at the numerator of the fraction and not the denominator. And this
continues to play forward with these horrible, tragic mass shootings that we've had by adolescents
in recent decades and the claims that, well, they're playing violent video games or they're listening to certain types of music.
They are.
This is true.
But that's not the question.
The question is, are kids that carry out horrible acts exposed to these things more frequently
than your average kid?
Is there any association?
Well, I want to dig a little deeper into this Dungeons and Dragons thing in just a moment.
First, my guest is James
Zimmering, and he is author of the book Partial Truths, How Fractions Distort Our Thinking.
This is an ad for better help. Welcome to the world. Please read your personal owner's manual
thoroughly. In it, you'll find simple instructions for how to interact with your fellow human beings
and how to find happiness and peace of mind. Thank you and have a nice life.
Unfortunately, life doesn't come with an owner's manual.
That's why there's BetterHelp Online Therapy.
Connect with a credentialed therapist by phone, video, or online chat.
Visit betterhelp.com to learn more.
That's betterhelp.com.
This winter, take a trip to Tampa on Porter Airlines.
Enjoy the warm Tampa Bay temperatures and warm Porter hospitality on your way there.
All Porter fares include beer, wine, and snacks, and free fast-streaming Wi-Fi on planes with no middle seats.
And your Tampa Bay vacation includes good times, relaxation, and great Gulf Coast weather.
Visit FlyPorter.com and actually enjoy economy.
So, James, it's almost human nature, it seems, that when there's a problem, we want to find the cause.
We want to find the villain.
In this case, it was the game Dungeons & Dragons.
And let's just blame that and and then you
know we can get on with our lives like it just it just seems right even though there's no evidence
to support it it really does seem like right because it catches our attention it's what comes
to mind you know um people who want to go swimming in the ocean, especially in my generation,
often at times say to themselves, I'm not sure I want to go in the ocean. I think I might get
eaten by a shark. And the whole series of Jaws movies came out when I was a kid. And every time
someone gets eaten by a shark, it's all over the news and it's all over the papers and we see it
and it's available to our mind. And now the truth is a lot more people die in the ocean drowning from currents or rip
tides than ever get eaten by sharks, many more.
But I don't remember Hollywood ever making a movie called Riptide.
And I don't remember the news saying, unless it's a celebrity that drowned, oh, yep, another
person drowned in a riptide today.
And so what becomes available to
our mind as the most likely thing is whatever was sensationalized or caught our attention.
But it causes us to make really bad choices all the time when we're trying to assess risk.
Driving your car is hands down more dangerous than flying by every analysis. But not many people are afraid of
driving. A lot of people are afraid of flying, in part because you're not in control and it's
unfamiliar perhaps, but also because when plane crashes go down, it's such a big deal. And
Hollywood has made horror films about it and all this kind of stuff. So don't get me wrong.
This thing, the availability heuristicistic is very advantageous to us in many
cases, right?
I mean, a lot of times you see dangerous things and you know, hmm, I shouldn't be doing that.
But the problem is, is they distort the real probabilities out there once you begin to
investigate and they do lead us to make quite bad choices.
Let's talk about how confirmation bias fits into all of this and maybe start off by
explaining what confirmation bias is. So confirmation bias has several nuanced
definitions, but in short, it is that once you believe something, as you have new experiences, you will filter those experiences based upon your belief
such that you will notice or emphasize things that support your belief, and you will discount
or ignore entirely things that refute your belief such that it's not really that seeing is believing,
it's believing is seeing. And why do we do that? I mean, is that just
human wiring? We're just built to do that? Yeah, we always have to consider anthropological,
you know, cultural influences, but this seems to be everywhere. And it's really kind of,
you know, confusing when you think about why would a creature have this tendency that seems kind of
odd or not terribly clever. And there are some advantages to having it to be sure,
but the disadvantages are overwhelming. There was a wonderful study done where at a department of
psychology at a university. So you always have to say, well, are psychology students representative
of all of us? But we're always stuck with that when they do psychology studies. And they took two groups of
students who had opposite beliefs on whether the death penalty is a deterrent or not, right? Which
is a common debate. And they mixed them together. And then they presented a bunch of evidence to
them based upon studies of this question. And then they separated them apart and talked to them. And what they found was
that people with opposite beliefs who saw the very same evidence presented, both sides
increased the belief in what they had believed at the beginning. So the same evidence polarized
the beliefs even further, which if you look at some of the struggles of our society today,
you can kind of see this happening real time. And when they interviewed the people after the fact,
they realized that what had happened was they all saw the same data, but the people who saw
data that supported their belief rated that as high quality data and the data against their
belief as poor quality data. And so they're stuck, all of us, in a certain belief construct
based upon how we experience the world.
Well, that certainly seems problematic
because it almost doesn't matter what the truth is.
You believe what you believe, and even if you're wrong,
and I show you evidence that you're wrong,
you'll still believe it's true. So that doesn't seem like
a good thing. Correct. Now there's this thing called the primacy effect, which is whatever
you happen to believe first, then immediately attaches and even gives you evidence that
persists if your original evidence is removed. So there was this great study
done by Bruner and Potter where they showed people pictures that were out of focus so that you could
not discern what the picture was and ask people to guess what it was a picture of. And then they
showed them a series of the same picture slowly coming into more and more focus. And people stuck with their original guesses as to what the
picture was, even when it became so much in focus that it was obviously something else that people
who had never made a guess could easily discern. So, you know, we talk about debates of what are
in our primary education textbooks, what our children are exposed to when they're young. And we talk about it for a
number of reasons, but this is a big part of that because what people learn early on, they will hold
on to despite evidence to the contrary. And so this is probably the biggest, baddest cognitive
property. I'm going to call it a property because it's not always a defect that we have. And it explains a lot of what human society holds.
But people do change their minds. And one thing I've noticed, and I think it frustrates people,
they'll argue hoping to change the other person's mind. People, when they do change their mind,
change their mind over time.
It takes time.
It isn't like, oh, God, now I see what an idiot I was.
I'll believe you now. It really takes time to change your mind.
It is going to take time and reflection.
One of the most interesting developments in education around these areas, at least most interesting to me that I've read
recently, is the difference between justifying a position with reasons as opposed to mechanism.
And what I mean by that is if you say to someone, give me 10 reasons why you believe this,
they will usually give you 10 reasons and it will enforce their belief even more because look at all
the reasons they have. But if you ask to say to someone, okay, the reasons you have to believe
this, how exactly would that work? What is the mechanism by which your reasons justify your
claim? Then usually their belief becomes a little bit less extreme it doesn't you know it doesn't go
away but it forces them to think through the internal implications of their belief construct
as opposed to just you know repeating over and over again a matter that they hold as fact and
so when you ask is there hopes here yes i think there. I think that when we engage reflective debate about mechanisms and
justifications, people become much less polarized and can understand the nuances of an issue
better. When we just pound, you should believe this for this reason, louder and louder and
louder and with great repetition, things become pretty extreme. So how you ask the question, how you approach the
problem can have a big effect. Well, not only is this conversation interesting, but it helps you
think differently. I think it makes you a more critical thinker and only good can come from that.
James Zimmering has been my guest. He is a professor of experimental pathology at the
University of Virginia School of Medicine
and author of the book Partial Truths, How Fractions Distort Our Thinking.
And there's a link to the book at Amazon in the show notes.
Thanks, James.
I'm grateful for your time and your help, and thanks for doing your podcast.
People who listen to Something You Should Know are curious about the world,
looking to hear new ideas and perspectives.
So I want to tell you about a podcast that is full of new ideas and perspectives,
and one I've started listening to, called Intelligence Squared.
It's the podcast where great minds meet.
Listen in for some great talks on science, tech, politics, creativity, wellness, and a lot more. A couple of
recent examples, Mustafa Suleiman, the CEO of Microsoft AI, discussing the future of technology.
That's pretty cool. And writer, podcaster, and filmmaker John Ronson, discussing the rise of
conspiracies and culture wars. Intelligence Squared is the kind of podcast that gets you thinking a little more openly
about the important conversations going on today.
Being curious, you're probably just the type of person Intelligence Squared is meant for.
Check out Intelligence Squared wherever you get your podcasts.
Since I host a podcast, it's pretty common for me to be asked to recommend
a podcast. And I tell people, if you like something you should know, you're going to like
The Jordan Harbinger Show. Every episode is a conversation with a fascinating guest.
Of course, a lot of podcasts are conversations with guests, but Jordan does it better than most.
Recently, he had a fascinating
conversation with a British woman who was recruited and radicalized by ISIS and went to prison for
three years. She now works to raise awareness on this issue. It's a great conversation. And he
spoke with Dr. Sarah Hill about how taking birth control not only prevents pregnancy, it can
influence a woman's partner preferences,
career choices, and overall behavior
due to the hormonal changes it causes.
Apple named The Jordan Harbinger Show
one of the best podcasts a few years back,
and in a nutshell, the show is aimed at making you
a better, more informed, critical thinker.
Check out The Jordan Harbinger Show.
There's so much for you in this
podcast. The Jordan Harbinger Show on Apple Podcasts, Spotify, or wherever you get your podcasts.
Just as we humans have our senses to help us navigate and perceive the world,
other creatures have their senses, and they don't necessarily work the same as ours do.
Some of them have senses that work a whole lot better than ours.
And perhaps there are things we can learn from those creatures
that can help us better understand how we function.
Jackie Higgins is a writer, producer, and director of films about animals,
and she has a book out called Sentient,
How Animals Illuminate the Wonder of Our Human Senses.
Hi, Jackie, welcome.
Thanks for having me on, Mike.
So to give people a sense of what we're talking about here,
let's start by discussing the peacock mantis shrimp
and their sense of sight and their sense of color.
Yes. So this creature has a dizzying array of color sensors in its eyeball. So when scientists
looked at its eyeball, they found 12 different color sensors in their eyes. And to put things
in perspective, we have three different color sensors. So we're known
as trichromats. And from that, we see millions of colors, whereas they're dodecachromats. So
scientists envisioned or imagined that these creatures could see a million times a million
times a million, et cetera, et cetera, septillion. I mean, it's such a large number of colors with so many zeros on the end that we
can't quite compute how many colors they see the thing with with our senses is that they
circumscribe every waking moment so they're the most mundane we forget about them we take them
for granted there's this quote i use i go from aristotle to da vinci leonardo da vinci he had
this amazing quote
saying that the typical person looks without seeing and listens without hearing, touches
without feeling, eats without tasting, inhales without awareness of odor or fragrance. So the
idea of looking at the shrimp and understanding what it might be able to see gives us some
distance on ourselves for us to realize how extraordinary our color site is and that we do see millions of colors.
We see millions of colors, but they're all shades of a couple.
I mean, there's really only a handful of colors.
Does that shrimp actually see like a color that we couldn't even imagine?
Well, that was the notion, yes.
And then through that, I introduce a scientist who was searching
for the first human tetrachromat. So, there is evidence to suggest that some women are walking
around with four different types of color cones in their eyes. And so, they are able to see colors
that most of us can't see. I suppose the one easy way of thinking about it is if you think of color blindness
so there are some people who only have two different types of cone and they see red green
you've heard of red green color blindness so they they cannot see colors that I know that I can see
so yes this shrimp the idea would be that this shrimp can see many more colors than we could imagine.
So our sense of taste, and you talk about the Goliath catfish, how does this all come together?
So catfish, basically, they sense their world through taste.
And they don't have a tongue, as you and I understand a tongue.
Their body, their whole body is their tongue. The flanks and the feelers of their little whiskers, their catfish whiskers, but all the flanks of their body are covered in
taste buds. And so I chose the Goliath catfish because this fish is enormous,
possibly more than a meter, many meters long, swims through soupy Amazonian waters where there isn't much light.
And the taste buds on the outside of its skin enable it to track its prey.
So it sees the world through taste. But what's extraordinary, or I find kind of extraordinary,
well, I understand it, but I love all these connections, is that the catfish, the taste buds that are on the outside of the catfish are incredibly similar to the taste buds on our tongue.
Proof yet again of this deep evolutionary past we share with fish.
All life or vertebrate life on this planet comes originally from fish and the Devonian seas when the first tetrapod crawled out the land millions of years ago.
By studying these fish and these taste buds, we can better understand our sense of taste and how
it works. And so let's talk about the sense of hearing and the great grey owl. I find this
interesting. Can you talk about that? Yes. We all think of owls as having incredible eyesight, which they do.
But the great grey owl does this rather wonderful thing.
From 30 metres away, it can hear a mouse or a vole underneath a mound of snow.
So it's not seeing them.
It's not sensing their heat.
It's hearing them.
And the way that it does this is it uses both its ears.
So like we have stereoscopic vision using two eyes to get
a sense of depth and position. These owls use stereoscopic hearing and they use these two ears
to map their environment. This is an aspect of hearing that I was fascinated by, this idea that
sound creates a landscape around us. And it's something that we as humans use all the time.
John Hull wrote a wonderful book called Notes on Blindness about his experience of losing his eyesight.
And he talks about this idea of geographic sound.
He has these wonderful, he's got an amazing turn of phrase.
And he talks about thunder putting a roof over his head.
Or when it rains, suddenly the contours of the world become apparent to him.
He can grasp topography again.
Things beyond what he can feel with his fingers, which is another way in which he could grasp topography, but he can hear the topography of a landscape in the distance that he can't see.
It's an aspect of sound that we don't necessarily
notice. And I talk about echolocation and how we're all using echolocation to some degree.
So when you say echolocation, I think of bats like sonar. Is that what you mean, echolocation?
So echolocation, we always think of bats who send out ultrasonic peeps that then
basically bounce off the surface and they then listen to the reflections of these ultrasonic
sounds. And that enables them to map the topography of their world, map their landscape.
I was walking down the corridor with a scientist talking about this idea that we
use this sense of echolocation subliminally without really noticing it.
And I could hear our feet kind of hit the floor surface.
And the scientist said to me, you probably noticed that we're about to run into a wall was coming up because of the reflections of the echoes that my feet were making, because we're attuned to using and hearing these kinds
of sounds. You see, that's really interesting to me, because I know exactly what you mean
about how sound guides us like it did you in the hallway there. And yet we never think about it we never consciously say oh well
i can hear that the wall's coming up you just yes you just know it is and you you turn right
you turn yeah absolutely and there are many of these senses that work like that our sense of
body um our sense of proprioception this sense that if you close your eyes you i'm i hope mike
you know where your body is to the point that you can take your finger with your eyes closed and touch your nose.
So this, again, is one of these senses.
Without this sense, it's impossible to move.
It's impossible to sit up and to walk.
It's one of these senses that is so familiar and automatic that we just don't notice it.
Often the way one notices these things is if
you lose them. And I met an extraordinary man called Ian Waterman, who back in the 1970s
contracted a nasty virus. And it completely interfered with the senses that enabled this
sense to exist, the senses in his muscle, all the nerves back to his brain were
essentially cooked by this virus. They were wiped out. And so he had lost his sense of body. So when
he came around from his feverish dreams in the hospital, he could not feel his body from the
neck down. So it was as if when he first was grappling with this new way of being, he said he felt disembodied.
So if he closed his eyes, it wasn't like his body had been anesthetized.
You know, we still have a sense of our flesh and our presence if we're anesthetized.
If you're going to the dentist and your gum's been anesthetized or your side of your cheek, you still feel these things.
You still feel its presence. He felt nothing. It was like everything had disappeared.
It's back to that Leonardo da Vinci quote, you know, we're using them all the time without
appreciating them. And some of them are working secretly beneath our conscious radar, which makes
it even more difficult to appreciate them. Was he paralyzed or he just couldn't sense his body, but he could use his body?
So he couldn't sense his body and he couldn't use his body because he couldn't control it,
but he wasn't paralyzed. So he found his arms would be kind of off moving, but he didn't know
that they were moving. I mean, he joked that one was kind of, you know, saying hello to a nurse in an inappropriate way, but he didn't know. He didn't know that that's
what his arm was doing. He could only gather control and master his body again by allowing
his sense of vision to step in for this lost sense of proprioception. So if he looked at his hand and knew where it was, then he could control it.
And so Ian, more than any other person perhaps, had to break down every single motion. He had to
relearn how to master his body and relearn how to move. He had to break down the simple action of
sitting up into the many different components, which, you know, you talk about things that happen automatically.
It wasn't happening automatically for him.
So he had to break it down and basically sew his body back together with his sense of sight.
But if you touched him, would he feel it?
No, he also is touch blind. He's not,
and this is where touch, I think touch was one of the most interesting senses. Often when people
say, what's the sense you'd least like to use? We often say vision, but touch. Touch is so
complicated. There are many different senses in our skin. And if you stroke his arm, he can't feel
it, but he can feel hot and cold. That's because temperature sensors are different sensors within
our skin to the ones that your body registers if you're being stroked or the sensors that you're
using. He also can't feel the topography of the world. So you've got many
different senses, for example, in your fingertips and in your skin that enable you to detect
texture, shape, size. He's lost all of that. So there are certain dimensions of his touch that
he has and very few temperature essentially, but everything else was wiped out alongside this muscular sense,
this deeper sense of body, the sense of proprioception. I've never heard of that.
That is so strange. That is so- Well, he's completely, he's unique,
almost. There's been a couple of other cases like him, but he's such a rarity, such a rarity. And the fact that he was able then to walk again
and to master his body, I think makes him exceptional and unique. He's a properly amazing
man. And so now that he can walk again and he figured out how to walk again, is it very
deliberate where he has to watch himself walking or does he now do it automatically?
No, every moment he has to watch himself. He talks about his daily marathon.
If the lights go out and he's unable to see his body, he falls like a rag doll. He even,
if he's watching fireworks and you know that momentary blackness you get after bright lights just gone off in front of your eyes, even that short loss of contact between his body, he staggers.
So when power cuts have happened in his house, he's been at the mercy of just he can't move.
He can only move when he watches himself and he engages with that movement.
So for him, a walk in the countryside is not enjoying the view, listening to the bird song.
He has to really focus on what it is he's doing. And he might be able to continue
the conversation at the same time, but not with the same level of
concentration of focus that you and I could give it because it's such a mental feat just to walk.
So talk about our sense of pleasure and pain because I don't think people really
think of those things as senses, but you talk about the common vampire bat.
So talk about that.
Yes.
The vampire bat has these little sensors in its nose that enable it to detect the tiniest variations in temperature so that if it's landed on its prey's hide, it can detect where the vein is pulsing beneath the skin from the heat that emanates.
So these little trip sensors are much the same as what is found in our skin. David Julius, who's this extraordinary scientist who was
awarded the Nobel Prize for Physiology and Medicine this year, is in the book and he
studied the vampire bat and he also studies trip sensors in us. And these trip sensors in us give
us a sliding scale of temperature and a sliding scale of, you know, sometimes boiling hot water will trigger pain,
or incredibly cold things will also be painful. So sliding scale of temperature alongside a sliding
scale of pain. And the vampire bat on pleasure and pain, there were so many sensors within our
skin that scientists are just beginning to understand, not simply the trip sensors,
but this incredible sensor that they
found in our skin a few decades ago, but they're only just beginning to kind of map out. And this
is a sensor that fires if you lightly touch your skin, so with very little pressure, and then you
move it at a speed between three and five centimeters a second so that's essentially a stroke and it
will ping only if your um whatever is touching it is bodily temperature so the scientists have
called it the caress sensor so we've just discovered a sensor in our skin that is
activated when we're stroked quickly because we're almost out of time here, but I wanted to talk about
our sense of direction. Because again, I don't think people think of that as a sense. They think
of it as really almost an ability, an innate ability. You're either good at your sense of
direction or you're not, but it doesn't feel like a sense. And so we know that birds are able to sense the magnetic field.
So they have this kind of compass sense that enables them to navigate.
So they navigate by looking at the stars
and they navigate by listening to infrasound and it bouncing off mountains.
But when it's dark at night and there are no stars because it's cloudy,
these birds use the magnetic field
that surrounds the earth to navigate. And Joe Kirschvink at Caltech is convinced that humans
are also able to use this magnetic field. Now, the mystery at the moment is figuring out where
these senses are. And there's some extraordinary science being done. And there are two theories. One is the quantum compass,
whereby quantum changes happen in these little cryptochromes in eyes, in the bird's eyes,
for example, that enable them perhaps even to see that magnetic field. But Joe thinks that
what is the sensor are these tiny magnetite crystals that basically swivel like the little needles of the compass
that enable us to know where North is. So it's a really fascinating possibility that humans
might be able to have this sense of direction. And he's hoping, being funded by NASA, he's hoping to
go out to Australia and study the indigenous tribes there who do have
an exceptional sense of direction and to find out whether they are using somehow this magnetic field.
Well, I always wondered why there's a difference between, what's the difference between people who
really do have a good sense of direction and those who don't, because there are people that have a very good sense of direction.
Sense of direction.
Or maybe people, if this sense, if we are,
he's done these experiments where he has in his basement at Caltech,
he's let people sit in a chair and he's removed all sensory information.
So people are relaxed.
Their eyes are closed.
They're not hearing anything.
They're not feeling things. And he's changed the magnetic field about their heads and looked for
the brain, looked at the brain and found differences in brainwave patterns. So if we
are sensing these changes, maybe these people that you talk about are just more tuned to that
somehow. But it's an if.
It's an if.
It would seem that our sense of balance is one of those senses that you don't really notice it unless you don't have it.
And if you lost it and you couldn't stand up and balance yourself, that you would really notice that.
But, I mean, imagine that.
Imagine if you lost your sense of balance.
So there was this wonderful quote from Brian Day, who studied how we walk, for example.
And he talks of walking as one of the most daring balancing acts in the animal kingdom,
which is wonderful. We're inherently unstable on two feet. And each time we lift our leg and step
forward, that's a kind of tip into
the unknown. So yes, I use, so it is very much a sense and our body is adjusting according to what
information is being sensed in terms of gravity. And we use our vision to balance ourselves. And
we use that sense I talked earlier about proprioception, all of them key and all of them senses.
Well, what's interesting about this discussion is it makes you realize that your world, what you perceive as your world, the colors, the tastes, the sights, the things that you see
may be very different than another person, certainly other creatures.
Even though we're all in the same world, the way we take it in is all unique.
I've been talking with Jackie Higgins,
who is a writer, producer, and director of films about animals,
and she is author of the book,
Sentient, How Animals Illuminate the Wonder of Our Human Senses.
And you will find a link to that book in the show notes.
Thank you for being here, Jackie.
The pleasure, Mike.
And thank you very much for having me on.
One way to deal with allergy symptoms
is to take antihistamines.
But what you may not realize
is that those types of medications
can promote weight gain in a lot of people.
There have been a couple of studies that have shown that people who took common allergy medications
like Allegra or Zyrtec or Claritin showed a roughly 10% increase in body weight.
Now, the connection between antihistamines and body weight is not new.
Some older antihistamines have actually been prescribed in the past
to help underweight children increase their appetite
so they would eat more and put on some pounds.
But how it works exactly to cause weight gain, no one's really sure.
But be aware that if you take these drugs to make allergy symptoms more manageable,
your weight may suffer
if you're not careful.
And that is something you should know.
If you found something useful
or interesting in this podcast,
you know, like the fish
whose whole body is covered in taste buds,
come on, that was pretty
interesting. Tell someone you know
about something you should know so they can hear it too.
I'm Mike Carruthers. Thanks for listening today to Something You Should Know.
Welcome to the small town of Chinook, where faith runs deep and secrets run deeper.
In this new thriller, religion and crime collide when a gruesome murder rocks the isolated Montana community.
Everyone is quick to
point their fingers at a drug-addicted teenager, but local deputy Ruth Vogel isn't convinced.
She suspects connections to a powerful religious group. Enter federal agent V.B. Loro, who has been
investigating a local church for possible criminal activity. The pair form an unlikely partnership to
catch the killer, unearthing secrets that leave Ruth torn between her duty to the law, her religious convictions, and her very own family.
But something more sinister than murder is afoot, and someone is watching Ruth.
Chinook, starring Kelly Marie Tran and Sanaa Lathan.
Listen to Chinook wherever you get your podcasts. for The Silver Lining, a fantasy adventure series about a spirited young girl named Isla who time travels
to the mythical land
of Camelot.
Look for The Search
for The Silver Lining
on Spotify, Apple,
or wherever you get
your podcasts.