StarTalk Radio - Are You Out of Your Mind?
Episode Date: March 28, 2013How do we know what we know? The brain is an amazing product of evolutionary engineering, but many influences can play with our perceptions. Subscribe to SiriusXM Podcasts+ on Apple Podcasts to liste...n to new episodes ad-free and a whole week early.
Transcript
Discussion (0)
Welcome to StarTalk, your place in the universe where science and pop culture collide.
StarTalk begins right now.
Welcome back to StarTalk. I'm Neil deGrasse Tyson, your personal astrophysicist.
This week, we're talking about the brain.
And I had to bring in Chuck Nice for that.
Chuck, welcome back to Star Talk. Hey, Neil. What's happening? Can't get enough of you, man.
Hey, I don't blame you, man. We brought you in for the Super Bowl show and for the brain show.
What's left? Actually, two of my favorite things in the world. I find the brain to be one of the
most fascinating studies that anyone can undertake. That and football. The brain gets
fascinating studies that anyone can undertake.
That and football, the brain gets bashed in football.
There you go.
So they're totally hooked up for that.
Exactly.
So it's about the brain, because the brain is everything, right?
I think so, yeah.
And we try to define what separates us from other animals.
Right.
And it's language, it's abstract reasoning. We do art and philosophy and music and science.
Science.
And so we try to sort of say that we are apart from the animals because our brain can accomplish all that.
Actually, I think some other animals are doing calculus on the side.
You think so?
I think so.
What animal would be able to do calculus?
If you could pick one.
If I could pick one, dolphins.
Dolphins probably.
You know, give them a pen.
If they could ever use it, they would show
us what's really going on. One of my favorite
memories is of one of, it was a Gary
Larson comic where the farm animals
and they're just talking to
each other. The farmer is not there.
The chicken is talking to the horse, he's talking to the cow
and the chicken says,
but if you take the mass and divide it by the
square root of the speed of light
then you get the same answer.
Right.
And the horse answer, but you're missing the basic premise of my theory.
And then someone else says, farmer, cluck, cluck, cluck, moo.
So for all we know, this is what's really going on.
But, you know, neither you nor I are an expert on the brain.
And we had to reach out into the ether and find who could help us do this.
Cool.
And so we found, we found Cara Santamaria.
Cara, welcome to StarTalk Radio.
Thanks.
Yeah, so you're an expert on the brain and brain function,
and you've taught it before.
Do we call you a neuroscience educator?
What's the best title we can use for you?
Well, I've taught in psychology and biology.
I guess you could call me an educator, student of the neurosciences.
So aren't we all? Because who knows it all? Nobody knows it all. So everybody's a student,
even those who say they're not.
That's true.
Who profess to say they know it all.
That's true.
They don't know jack.
No. You never really feel like you know much. I think when you're studying these things,
you always kind of look back on what you've accomplished and say,
really, am I just a hack here? How much of this did I really gather? There's so much more.
Well, there's the stuff to actually learn that we know, and then there's the stuff beyond that
that no one has even figured out yet. Exactly. There's a lot of that in neuroscience. And
astrophysics. I think those are two very big frontiers. They are. One is the inner and one
is the outer space. Yes, indeed. And so what I wonder, so you taught where? In New York,
if I remember correctly, from your resume. I was in New York for about a year. And before that, I was in Texas for many years.
In Texas.
And where do you call home?
Now I'm in L.A.
In L.A.?
Yeah, I'm in L.A.
No, no.
Where are your roots?
My roots are in Texas.
In Texas.
Definitely a Texas girl.
Transplant.
Okay.
Okay.
Well, welcome to StarTalk Radio, because we're going to be picking your brain and try to
leave you with some left to do your work.
Right.
As the non-scientist in the room, may I point out to our listeners two things.
One, Cara is hot as hell.
That's number one.
Number two, she looks about 19.
So those are two things you want to keep in mind when you hear the knowledge drop from her lips.
Thank you.
That she is hot and looks like she's 19.
I'm not 19, by the way.
Well, of course.
Just so you know.
I'm not 19.
Chuck, why did you pick that age in particular?
Because it's legal.
That's what I thought.
That's exactly what I thought, Chuck.
There it was.
There's no lying in me, Neil.
That's why we love you, Chuck.
So, Kara, if you were to describe what your particular expertise about the brain is, what would you say?
I think that's still evolving.
I think my interest was in brain damage, has been in brain damage.
That's kind of what I want to continue to study.
That's where you learn because you can't go in and poke somebody's brain and find out what happened.
You've got to wait for stuff to happen accidentally.
Yeah.
I mean, you can do models with animals, and I've done some of that.
Non-human animals. Non-human animals. Exactly. Right. Not just guys to wait for stuff to happen accidentally. Yeah. I mean, you can do models with animals, and I've done some of that. Non-human animals.
Non-human animals, exactly.
Not just guys that play for the Lakers.
But you do. You have to wait
until something happens, and sometimes you have to wait
until autopsy, or at least until you can get good imaging
to see what actually did happen.
Right. And so a particular
injury might damage a certain
part of the brain that you never knew had a particular
function. And then a person behaves crazier than they might have behaved before.
And then you've just nailed a spot of the brain for causes and effect, right?
And brain damage, for a lot of people, it's like fingerprints.
You want to say that we have classified particular areas that do particular things,
and you damage this area, and you have this effect.
But the truth is, nobody has the same brain damage.
Right.
Nobody.
As each other. It's a very organic thing.
You could damage the same area and have a
totally different outcome.
And is that just because each individual brain has
that particular type of makeup?
I mean, the makeup of our brain is that particular?
Well, it's similar
brain to brain, but it's
very rare that only a very specialized region
of the brain would be damaged anyway. You're going to have
other effects. Well, see, that brings me to ask, because how many?
We've got 100 billion nerve cells?
I mean, brain cells?
Yeah, just the neurons.
Just the neurons.
Not the glia.
I love saying 100 billion.
So let's say that together.
Sounds cool.
100 billion.
Very Carl Sagan.
Take out the billion out there in that low voice.
Like Sagan.
Carl Sagan, yeah.
Sagan.
Saganomics.
And so 100 billion nerve cells and brain cells, and we typically, the naive thought about the brain is that it stores information like a file cabinet and you go and retrieve it.
But recently we've been learning it's much more complex than that, right?
In terms of the storage of information and retrieval.
Is that right?
Well, I think we used to think that we could just learn discrete packets of information. You just see something in the world or you experience something through your senses,
and then it goes to a certain part of your brain,
and it just lives there until you want to pull it back out.
Like a file, like you said.
But really, a lot of it is about connections.
It's all about taking something in, connecting it to something you already knew.
Like Facebook.
Exactly.
It's a web.
It's like a social network.
A social network. A neural network.
A neural network. Exactly.
Can I de-friend certain parts of my brain?
You drink
enough. We'll get back
to that.
How to de-friend part of your memory.
Now Chuck, we put you on assignment earlier.
Yes you did.
Where I work, I'm at the American Museum of Natural History here in New York City.
I run the universe part of the museum, which includes-
Which, by the way, is so cool.
If you're ever in New York, you've got to go there.
Oh, thanks for the commercial.
Seriously.
Yeah, so I run the Hayden Planetarium.
But now we, the museum, has an exhibit on the brain.
Yes.
And I actually had not had a chance to view it, but we sent you so you could report back to StarTalk Radio.
Right. On what you found and what others found.
Yes.
We made the mistake, I think, of giving Chuck a microphone.
Yeah.
I'll tell you the irony of this whole piece.
It's the brainless going to a brain exhibit.
All right.
Let's see what Chuck tells us from the museum live on location.
Well, he were...
I was live on vacation.
At the time.
I was live on location. He was live on vacation. At the time. I was live on location.
He was live on vacation.
Check him out.
What is your favorite part of the brain?
The stuff in the brain.
The stuff in the brain.
That's what I need more of.
I need more stuff in my brain.
Because my brain is kind of empty.
What is empty?
Listen, wait.
Rattle my head.
Listen to that.
Blink, blink, blink.
You hear that?
Yeah, what does that mean? It's kind of nothing. What is empty? Listen, wait. Rattle my head. Look at that. Blink, blink, blink. You hear that? Yeah, what does that mean?
Yeah, that's kind of nothing rolling around in my head.
I'm actually a school psychologist.
Really?
So, now, as a psychologist, do you find that people's experiences and environment or their brain causes them to have psychological problems?
or their brain causes them to have psychological problems?
No, experiential learning and environmental learning have an impact, I believe,
on brain development and the memories that you retain.
So how do you explain my mother telling me that something's wrong with me because I ain't right in the head?
You're trying to get a free session.
No dice?
Help me, Pamela.
I'm on vacation.
Sorry.
No dice?
Help me, Pamela.
I'm on vacation.
Did you learn anything that just totally wowed you?
The fact that your brain tells you everything.
It makes everything in your body go.
Everything.
Everything.
Even the things that you take for granted.
Like you just blinked.
I saw you blinked.
I saw you blink there again.
Your brain told you to do that, right?
Yep.
Do you know why I'm blinking right now?
I'm afraid you might hit me.
I won't do that.
Thank you.
I'll tell my brain not to do that.
As a neurobiologist, I'm interested to know what your favorite part of the brain would be.
Well, right now, at my age, the hippocampus.
How's that?
Ah, okay.
That would be because the hippocampus is responsible for what?
Memory?
Yes, yes.
You have to use your brain in order to keep it going.
So the brain is like a muscle.
You've got to use it? You have to exercise it.
Use it or lose it.
What was the thing that most impressed you about your own brain that you found out in the exhibit?
I think in relation to short and long-term memory,
that it's sleep that actually transfers memories from short-term to long-term.
So you look well-rested.
No, I'm not. I'm jet-lagged.
Are you really?
Yeah, I am.
Are you from Australia?
I am.
So you've had a long flight.
Yeah.
And did you sleep on that flight?
Only a very little bit.
So was there anything else that really impressed you?
Something you never thought you'd know about your brain that you found out?
No, I can't remember anything else.
And there you have it.
The short term, long term, you need some sleep.
I need some sleep.
Chuck, just harassing visitors to the American Museum of Hayden Point.
No, actually the brain exhibit at the American Museum of Natural History.
Yeah.
So in there we heard about the hippocampus.
So, Cara, tell us about the hippocampus.
All about the hippocampus.
Yeah, in one minute about.
I remember first learning about the hippocampus and then teaching my students about the hippocampus.
And I would imagine a hippopotamus walking through my college campus, remembering how to get to class.
I don't know, that helped me at the time.
Yeah, it's a deeper structure of the brain.
It's kind of underneath the cortex.
And it's involved in memory.
And it used to be the case kind of after a really famous patient, patient H.M., had his hippocampus damaged when he had a surgery for epilepsy.
Scientists all thought, well, this must be the seat of memory in the brain.
This is the only place where memory is.
Once again, because of some accident, then they know this.
Yeah, exactly.
Or they believe it.
They could look at his brain and say, well, he's kind of missing this area,
and he can't remember things now.
He can't encode new memories.
What we've found out more recently is that memory is very ubiquitous in the brain it's
in many parts of the cortex and like we said before it the memory of a single thing can be
spread around it can it can not just different things but definitely different things but the
memory of a single thing can too because we make associations you know smells help us totally
man chuck i smell something and i said man i remember that 19 you know whatever you know you
know you're right.
I mean, I was home today at my parents', and my mother made some food,
and it took me back to being in that home when I was like 11 years old.
Yeah, that's one of the strongest triggers, actually, because it's a very old part of the brain.
So comfort food is not just that it is, but that it has a smell.
Right, and that it brings comfort.
Yeah, that makes sense.
Definitely.
Yeah, and food.
Flavor.
Flavor is smell. The meatloaf is waiting for you outside. Smell, Yeah, that makes sense. Definitely. Yeah, and food. Flavor. Flavor is smell.
The meatloaf is waiting for you outside.
Smell, taste, all of this plugs into the brain.
Yeah, but then that plugs into the brain
and it helps us recall memories and encode memories.
But really the hippocampus we found
is more of kind of a way station.
It's the place where memories can kind of first be encoded
and then spread out to other parts of the cortex for storage.
So this sounds like there's a risk of misremembering something if it's got to store one place first and then other places later.
But we get back to that in another segment.
But before we even get there, do you compare the human brain with other brains?
Yeah.
I've been reading about this, and I learned that the octopus, which is kind of an extraordinary creature for starters,
can each limb, each of the eight limbs kind of
can operate autonomously without reference to the brain.
This is what I learned about.
Like there's ganglia, like mini brains.
Yeah, mini brains that can make their own kind of decisions.
This is cool and scary.
Awesome.
You know how dolphins sleep?
I never asked.
Since they have to be underwater, but they're mammals, they have to breathe.
Oh, yeah, yeah.
It's conscious breathing.
So they sleep by one hemisphere going to sleep at a time while the other remains alert.
Wow.
So they can sleep and still swim to the surface and breathe.
Yeah.
But they're sleeping.
Half of their brain is asleep.
So basically they sleepwalk.
They do.
Or sleep swim.
They have to sleepwalk.
Sleep swim.
They sleep swim.
Sleep swim.
Right.
And how about whales?
I don't know.
I didn't know that.
So they can just show, they got a little switch and just switching left and right.
Just like we have a switch.
Well, we have a switch for our whole brain to go to sleep.
It'd be interesting to see what different behaviors they're capable of as one half turns off and the other turns on.
Definitely.
Because we have specialization of our brain halves, don't we?
Or is that just a fiction from the past?
No, we do.
We definitely have specialization.
Right.
Different hemispheres.
That's always, and other, you know, and sort of creatures that are much smaller that i learned about flatworms that they don't even have a brain
oh thank god because you have scared me are you wondering if flatworms doing calculus
and where does chuck fit into this evolutionary
well we're running running down our first segment when we get back
my interview with neuroscientist Oliver Sack. We're back.
Chuck, nice.
Thanks for being on StarTalk.
Let me reintroduce our guest today.
In from Los Angeles, this is Cara Santamaria.
Hello.
Hello.
Did I pronounce your name right, Cara?
Yes, you did. Thank you. Thank you for that. Cara Santamaria. Santamaria.
That was not one of the Columbus ships? Yes, it was. It was. Good.
Many a nickname growing up. Yeah. Nina Pinta. And so we promised you before the break that we
would take you to an interview that I conducted with Oliver Sacks. He's probably the world's
best known neuroscientist. And if you haven't heard of him,
you may have known the movie Awakenings.
In fact, that movie was about him.
That was about him.
Robin Williams was playing him.
Okay, I was going to say,
I'm guessing he's not the Robert De Niro character.
Because that would be truly extraordinary.
Let me double-check IMDB to make sure.
But yeah, Robin Williams portrayed him in that film.
It was a semi-autobiographical.
I think it was done up a little for the movies as well.
But basically, it was a story of his life
and breaking in as a neuroscientist.
His latest book, The Mind's Eye,
is about how our brain helps to understand what the eyes see
because your eyes are the organ unto themselves.
They just got to hand it over to the brain.
Then the brain has to make sense of it.
So this is what somebody needed to write a book on that.
And there's going to be a movie released.
The music never stopped.
Did that movie come out yet?
I think it did.
It already did.
It never stopped.
It did come out.
That's based on one of his essays called The Last Hippie,
which was published in his book An Anthropologist on Mars. I love it did. It already did. It never stopped. It did come out. That's based on one of his essays called The Last Hippie, which was published in his book
An Anthropologist on Mars.
I love that title.
Groovy, man.
So apparently he's been places, perhaps including Mars.
I don't know.
So that's Oliver Sacks.
And in our first clip, he talks about the things that define your identity and your
personality.
And he describes what role the electrical currents in your brain
might play in determining that,
but how a super-duper electrical current from outside your brain
can really mess with it.
Oh, man.
I think that's what they wanted to do to me.
That's the part of your life we'll get to at another show, Chuck.
Let's see where...
Let's just see where Oliver Sacks takes us.
A colleague whom I describe in my book, Musicophilia, he is a surgeon here in New York,
and in 1994, through a freak accident, he was struck by lightning, and he had a cardiac arrest.
He was dead for half a minute, or certainly his brain did not get enough blood or oxygen at
that time. He had a sort of strange experience. He was conscious of being flung back many feet
by the thunderbolt, by the lightning which hit him, and then he felt he was floating forward,
and he looked down, and that he saw his own body with people around him. And he said,
and that he saw his own body with people around him.
And he said, I'm dead.
But then he seemed to sort of move on. And then a sense of ecstasy came on him.
And he saw a bluish white light.
And he felt the most wonderful thing in the world was about to happen.
And then he regained full consciousness to find someone doing CPR on him.
He regained full consciousness to find someone doing CPR on him.
But about three weeks after this, he had a strange emotional and musical change.
This man, who had never been interested in music, developed a sudden passion for classical music. First to hear it, and then to play it, and then he wanted to compose it.
And this also went with a mystical feeling.
He felt that God had sent the thunderbolt,
but had also arranged for him to be resuscitated,
and that he now had a mission to bring music to the world.
Was he religious before this?
Not really.
I think there were some seeds of religion,
but these flared up with the experience,
either with the psychological
shock of being almost killed, and who knows what neurological changes might have happened
as he was electrocuted and when his brain didn't have enough blood.
The brain being an organ of electrical current, right? That's what goes on in the brain.
Yes, absolutely. There's this chemistry and electricity, and that's it. And somehow from this comes thought, imagination, spirit, the idea of God and everything else.
Well, anyhow, this man will put a supernatural explanation on this.
However, he is not ignorant scientifically.
In fact, he has a PhD in neuroscience as well.
As a neurologist, it was up to me to put things in more neurological terms,
but without in any way upsetting him, devaluing his experience.
And I said, you know, I'm sure this is what you experience and what you believe, but will you allow that something might have happened inside you?
For example, it might supernatural intervention make use of existing neurological structures?
And he said, yeah, okay.
And at that point where I suggested that the two were not wholly incongruous, he said he would be prepared to have subtle forms of brain imagery or whatever
to see whether we might be able to find the parts of his brain
which had perhaps been reorganized
somewhat, pushing him towards
religion and towards music.
Wow.
Yeah. Okay, I got one thing
to ask about that whole clip. What's that?
Was that a beep? Yeah, that was
StarTalk's first beep, yes.
Leave it to a neuroscientist
to get the first beep on the show.
We're cool like that. We're cool like that.
You're cool like that.
That was the first beep.
And we had to beep his quote of someone else.
That's not even him.
Right, right.
So that's what that was.
So that's interesting.
So what confidence this person must have to believe that God would strike him dead with lightning,
but then rely on someone to resuscitate him so that he would have these magical musical powers and interest?
but then rely on someone to resuscitate him so that he would have these magical musical powers and interest.
You know, I'm just going to say, and not to ever devalue someone's religious beliefs or inclinations,
you know, normally when God strikes you, he's kind of pissed.
I'm just saying.
He didn't hit Moses with a bolt of lightning and then say,
I want you to take these ten rules down to the people.
No, he said, Moses, come on up here.
I want to talk to you.
Let's talk.
When God strikes you, there might be a little problem with you and God.
That you haven't really quite figured out yet.
Right.
So, Kara, our identity, it looks like it can be altered by just sort of an electrical shock.
Do you do experiments with that? It can be altered by a lot of things.
You've heard of Phineas Gage? No?
No. Phineas Gage?
No, sorry. He was a railroad worker.
Certainly. If he's a railroad worker,
triply not.
But he's a super important story
that most neuroscience hear about. Is he the one with the spike
in the head? Yeah, he's the one with the spike in the head.
The tamping iron and it went through his head and it
blew out part of his frontal lobe. He stayed alive.
He stayed alive and dodged infection, which at the time probably would have killed him with an open head injury. And he was a stamping iron and it went through his head and it blew out part of his frontal lobe. He stayed alive. He stayed alive and dodged infection, which at the time probably would have killed him with an open head injury.
And he was a different person after.
He was like a womanizer.
He cursed all of the time.
He was a big drunk.
Before that, he was a very straight-laced.
So those people today have spikes in their head.
You just saved my life.
I got a reason now.
It's like, honey, I'm sorry.
I just have a spike in my head
don't you see this spike
so how about memory
presumably
it can not only
change you
can it bring memory
into existence
or take it out
can it make memory
sharper or lose it
what brain damage
yeah
it could
yeah actually
we see these movies
how about the born identity
that's a famous one
where he just doesn't know who he is.
But I've, well, I was going to say I never met anyone who never knew who they were, but then.
There are people who don't know who they are.
How many?
I mean, there are people who have like dissociative fugue.
I haven't personally.
Dissociative fugue?
Yeah, it's a psychological condition.
And what's that in English?
They dissociate from themselves and then they fugue.
They go away and they don't know why they're where they are.
So you hear these kind of, you know, true crime stories of people waking up in a parking lot, nine towns over and they'd stolen a car.
Covered in blood.
And they don't remember any of it.
Oh, like the crime committed in that state of mind.
Yeah, sometimes that's, you know, probably not the case.
But even Oliver Sacks did write a story about that.
That does happen.
Because he's especially interested in these kinds of bizarre, bizarre,
bizarre. And then I was reading that
memory begins to decline at
age 50. That's not right.
That can't be true.
We'll get back
to that. Me and NASA, we're both losing our memory
here. Let's go back
to Oliver Sacks and my interview with him in
his home office
in Greenwich Village, New York.
And in this clip, I asked him about memories, false or real, and how you get them and how you
lose them. Let's see what he tells us. If you do functional imaging of the brain,
it is relatively easy to tell if someone is telling a lie, because it's quite a complex
business to tell the lie. It's easier to tell the truth. But you cannot tell if someone has a delusion. Because they believe they're telling the truth. Yeah, whether the
belief is well founded or not, if they have the belief strongly, and the emotions and the
visualized scenes which go with this. And when people say they've been abducted by aliens, they
truly believe this. In a book I wrote, and the name of the book is Uncle Tungsten,
I mentioned two early memories from 1940 of bombs which had fallen in London. An older brother of
mine confirmed one of the memories. He said, yes, it's exactly the way you describe it. But as for
the other memory, a memory of incendiary bombs in our garden and of my father's attempts
to stop it and to douse it with sand and water, which didn't work. My brother said, you never saw
it. I said, what do you mean I never saw it? He said, we were away at the time. And I said, but I
can see it in my mind now. I can hear the crackling of the flames, the shouting. I see the figures of
my father and brother. It is so clear in my mind. Why? He said, because our older brother sent us a letter. And he said,
you were fascinated by the letter and even obsessed. And obviously not only fascinated by it,
but I internalized it and visualized it. It also helps if your brother is a good writer.
So this is a false memory or a secondary memory.
Is that the same as implanted memories we've heard?
Yeah, yeah, in a way.
This is a bit like an implanted memory.
It had been implanted by a good description,
which I think probably appealed to me romantically.
It's sort of exciting.
Though I know now that this is a fictitious
or secondary or implanted memory,
it does not seem to me any different
in quality from the genuine one.
And if I had functional MRIs
and was asked to recollect these two things,
I think one would see pretty much
the same areas of the brain, both
the visual and emotional areas, sort of
lighting up.
That's cool. So I think there are two kinds of failures
of memory. One of them is remembering things that never
happened. And the other one is forgetting things that did.
Yes.
When we come back, we're going to explore what effects drugs have on brain functions.
This is StarTalk Radio. We're back.
StarTalk.
We've got Chuck Nice and Cara Santa Maria in the house.
So in this next segment, we want to talk about, on this program on the brain,
we want to talk about other ways the brain can malfunction or function in ways that,
differently from how nature intended.
Right.
So when I think of this, I think of sort of psychoactive drugs,
drugs that people take
recreationally or medicinally
to alter their state of mind.
Cool.
And I know
our special interview guest,
Oliver Sacks,
he has experimented with drugs
and he will tell us about it
in some clips coming up.
Cool.
And so, of course,
if you're a neuroscientist
and medical doctor, you have access
to drugs, and it's just
interesting. But why would anyone want to do this?
I mean, I like my brain.
I like when I have deep thoughts about the universe.
And almost anything that
enters the brain
alters your ability
to have those thoughts. So I'm just wondering, Carol.
But it may alter it in a positive direction.
In a way. Maybe you'll have deeper thoughts.
Deeper thoughts.
Why would anybody want to take drugs?
Really?
Let me ask you this.
I don't know anyone who said, here's an equation that I can't solve.
Let me take drugs so I can be more intellectually acute in my ability to solve it.
I've never seen that.
Well, I think there's two prongs.
Are you looking at solving the equation in a very pragmatic way
or in a creative way?
Do you need an external...
Where's my paintbrush? Here's the answer
to the equation. You really
don't think that physics can be very creative.
It can be creative, but in the end
the adjudicator is nature.
It is not an unlimited
infinite tapestry. And many
drugs are found in nature,
and we possess receptors in our brains for some of these drugs.
Is that right? Like what?
Most all of them.
We have cannabinoid receptors in our brains.
Oh, otherwise it would be neutral to us.
Cannabinoid.
I hear cannabis in that.
Cannabinoid.
Cannabinoid.
That's code for?
Cannabis receptors.
Refer.
Which is code for?
Pot.
Melanin.
Melanin.
Thank you.
We also have receptors for pcp in our brain and we have not
yet really found the endogenous chemical that binds to that so that's kind of a quandary
oh interesting it's a frontier yeah it's a frontier let's see what oliver sacks tells us
about his own time in this exploration in the early 1960s like a lot of people especially on
the west coast where i lived at the time, I took a lot of drugs.
Uh-oh, now you can't run for office.
No, no, no.
But I really wanted to see what I'd read about other forms of consciousness.
To what extent would the world open for me?
Would it reveal domains perhaps of natural or supernatural beauty and meaning?
Well, they certainly opened domains of natural meaning.
On one occasion, since you're an astronomer,
I haven't mentioned this, it was back in 67,
I had started seeing patients with migraine.
And one weekend, I took an old book out of the library
written in the 1860s, a book was called On Megrim.
And then I loaded up pharmacologically.
But instead of giving way to fantasy, I started reading this book.
And the sort of drug ecstasy coupled with what was in the book.
And I started to feel this is a most wonderful book.
I felt that the neurological
heavens were opening for me, and that migraine was shining like a constellation. One of the
people quoted in the book was an astronomer, the younger Herschel, who had migraine,
and describing his migraines, he said he felt like an astronomer of the inward.
As I continued to read the book, I thought this is a wonderful, incredible example of mid-Victorian medicine at its best.
But it was written in the 1860s, and now this is the 1960s.
The author of the book was a man called Living, Edward Living.
And I thought, who should be the Living of our time?
And there was a very disingenuous clamour of names came to me,
followed by a very loud inner voice which said, you silly bugger, you're the man.
When I came down from that, that sense that I was the man and this was my subject stayed with me,
and I wrote my book on migraine, and I never took drugs again.
So in a way, the opening or awakening I had was in that drug experience.
Interesting. So he takes drugs and decides to read a book rather than jump off a balcony.
This guy is really a scientist. That's all I gotta say. Anytime you do drugs, and you're just like,
what should I do? Crazy sex? Buy a couple hookers?
I think I should read a book.
That's right.
You are truly a scientist.
Yeah, that's a whole other state of mind.
Exactly.
Deep within that.
I was doing some homework on this, and I learned that, I don't know if you knew this, Cara, that in the 1950s, the CIA experimented with LSD to see if they could alter the memories
and perceptions for espionage purposes.
I don't know if you knew about that.
Did it seem to work?
I don't know.
I don't know the outcome.
The outcome was this.
Everybody that experimented on ended up following the Grateful Dead.
Actually, so that was in the 50s, and that's what birthed the 1960s.
A lot of E-Wall soldiers there, I think.
There it is.
And, of course, the psychogenic factors are not unique to Western culture or modern culture.
Native Americans long ago, maybe still, peyotes.
Where do you get that from?
A cactus or something?
For your vision quest.
Yeah.
So I find it interesting, but are you suggesting
that the people
who take these inner trips,
that they're somehow,
do they function better
as people interacting
with other people?
Now, there's this movie out now,
what's it called, The Pill?
No, it's with Bradley Cooper,
it might as well be called The Pill.
So it's where,
so he's a parent,
I haven't seen the movie yet,
but I'm told he takes a pill
and it actually
Builds the mental powers
And acuity
That he has
I think they're trying to say
That once he takes the pill
He's now open to experience
Everything
And pull it
But the truth is
If we didn't have
Selective attention
We would be less functional
Oh
Okay
Significantly less functional
Selective attention
That allows you to
Not be distracted by-
Yeah, we have to be able to filter what gets into our heads and stays there.
So people with ADD, they don't have these filters.
Is that-
They have less of them.
Less of them.
Less of them.
So it's just like, I'm going to make the incision right below the A order.
What the hell is that?
That's so cute.
Well, that's a cute little bunny over in the-
Look at that.
Why is there a bunny in the operating room?
Because I'm on peyote.
Oh, yeah.
So you don't want ADD surgeons.
Yes, that would be bad.
Unless they're medicated.
Well, yeah.
And so also there are people who have, what I wonder is that people have deep religious experience.
They see Jesus or Muhammad, whatever it might be.
Have people studied what parts of the brain are being excited And what visions they might have
And whether that's similar to whatever might be stimulated by drugs
Or are their bodies producing drugs that give them these visions?
The actual visions
The actual visions
Well, I think that there are probably a lot of different parts
I know that there are scientists out there like Andy Newberg
Who has written books about religious experience
And he thinks that this is a genetic thing It's something that's in all of us there like Andy Newberg who has written books about religious experience and he
thinks that this is a genetic thing it's something that's in all of us it's we
have a god gene in our brain that allows us to see that I personally disagree
with that view I don't think that religious experience is something that's
always encoded in our brains I think it comes from external sources but it could
be in some people's brains and not others right what's wrong with that so
it could be it could be a genetic trait for some people but it could be that
that's what, whatever's
happening in your brain, that's how you choose to
describe the experiences in
religious terms. I see, because you have a religious
context in which to interpret it. And if you don't have the religion,
maybe it's aliens that you're looking at. Exactly.
Right. Which is very easy to figure out
because all we have to do is examine
your anus. Is that right?
Seriously. And if there's no trauma,
you have not been aboard a ship. Is that how that right? Seriously. And if there's no trauma, you have not been aboard a ship.
Is that how that works?
Thanks.
Next time I'm abducted, I will
carry that.
So, getting back to my interview
with Oliver Sacks,
he went
on to talk about how you can have these
profound visions and
what might induce them and what they
might mean. So let's see, this is in his home office, Greenwich Village, New York. Check it out.
I wondered whether one could imagine something one had never experienced,
in particular, whether one could imagine a color one had never seen. And I built up a sort of pharmacological mountain.
I won't go into details.
And when I was very loaded,
this connected my mind with the seventh color of the spectrum, indigo,
and the fact that no two people will ever quite agree as to what is indigo
or whether there is an indigo.
And I said to myself, at the peak of my experience,
I want to see indigo now. And
suddenly, as if thrown by a paintbrush, a trembling pear-shaped blob of indigo appeared on the wall.
And I leant towards it in a sort of ecstasy. It was a color I'd never seen. I thought,
purely metaphorically, of course, this is the color of heaven.
I thought this is the color which Giotto had tried to get, but never could.
I also thought it's a color which is no longer in the world.
This was the color of the Paleozoic Sea, but it's disappeared.
And as I led forward, the blob disappeared.
But I somehow felt that blob not only as luminous,
but as numinous.
Numinous.
I had to look up that word, Chuck.
Numinous.
Numinous.
Numinous.
I thought he said numerous.
No, numinous,
which I believe means
of or relating to spiritual experience.
Exactly.
Yes, Chuck,
the Ivy League educated comedian we have here.
That was very poetic,
I think, that interview.
It was.
It was beautiful. It was. Well, he's, that interview. It was. It was beautiful.
It was.
Well, he's a beautiful man.
I mean, everything about him.
He's soft-spoken
and he would never,
you know,
some people have like
the evil side of it.
You can't even picture that.
Because then you have to
bleep him in interviews.
Well, I bleep him
quoting someone else.
That's what that was.
We've got to take a quick break,
but more stocking as we return. This is StarTalk Radio.
Welcome back.
During the break, Chuck, you commented that you swig Robitussin for what reason?
Okay, here's the deal.
Because we were talking about, you know, pharmacological drugs being used or inducing hallucinations.
And I know that people actually drink Robitussin,
prescription-strength Robitussin.
You know people who do this?
I know someone who, well, I know somebody who does.
I've heard that phrasing before.
Seriously.
I have a friend who does this.
Here's why I would never do it.
I'll tell you why very, very quickly.
I'll tell you why.
So this guy and I are hanging out.
He's drinking Lick or Robitussin.
That's the street name for it, okay?
He starts getting so high that he's hung over,
his body's slumped over, and he's calling
me Betty.
Now, I'm like, two things.
One, I don't know who Betty is, but if she looks anything like me, that's one ugly broad.
Two, I don't ever want to drink anything that makes me think another man is Betty.
Period.
That cured you of your Robitussin.
Right.
That cured you of your Robitussin.
Right.
So, Kara, are there psychometric... Psychotropic?
Psychotropic drugs that are prescribed for any particular reason?
Not to give people hallucinations on purpose.
Right.
But, of course, there are lots of psychotropics that are prescribed.
Anything from ADHD to schizophrenia to depression, anxiety, all of these disorders require drug treatment, if you have drug treatment, with psychotropic.
So psychopharmacology is all about getting into your head.
Speed.
People take speed on purpose.
On purpose.
To help with attention.
To help it.
Let's get back to my interview with Oliver Sacks and see what he talks about.
Hallucinations.
I don't know if he's ever had one.
I'm very interested in hallucinations.
Some of the hallucinations occur with people who are blind.
And being partly blind myself, I have a few low-level hallucinations myself.
But they're only really of blobs of color and geometrical figures and things like that.
But it puts people on the spot if they have a hallucination.
Hallucination is not like imagery.
It's like perception.
It seems to come from outside.
You have no sense that you are generating it or any part of you.
So you hear music.
You run to the window.
You look outside for the source of the music.
It's only when you can't find a source for the music that you perhaps start to think,
has some part of my brain gone on automatic?
Or you may not think that. You may maintain a false belief. One of my old patients was convinced
that the patient next door had a phonograph and was putting on the same record again and again.
When Schumann had some musical hallucinations at one point, he thought it was divine music.
So, Kara, people can have hallucinations. And if you're religious, you have this,
this sort of inclination to think that it's divine. If you're not, you just think others
might just think you're crazy or they might think you're crazy in both cases.
You may think you are crazy personally, or you may not identify those hallucinations as being
external. What is your capacity to judge that you yourself are not of your own mind? I think it's
it just really depends on the person. I think that some schizophrenics are aware of their disorder
and they know that they need the Haldol and or whatever drugs they take to get through the day
and I think some have absolutely no idea that they're experiencing hallucinations and delusions.
The voices in my head just told me you are correct. And so how about the the kids we're
prescribing Ritalin for kids that That's affecting their brain in some way.
Yeah, that's what I was mentioning before.
That's speed.
Oh, that is speed.
Okay.
Well, it's like speed.
Speed-like.
It's a speed-like drug.
And it does.
You know, we don't really know the long-term effects of kids taking these drugs year after year after year.
Because they're the first experiment in this.
Exactly.
They're not adults yet.
That could be a whole other.
We could start another decade like the 60s. We're They're not adults yet. That could be a whole other, we could start another decade
like the 60s.
We're priming them for that.
Let's see,
I think my one last clip
coming up with Oliver Sacks
and we'd speak a little more
about hallucinations
and find out what he tells us.
Among the many sorts
of hallucination
are sorts which one may wake with
suddenly in the night. You wake up suddenly and
there's a pterodactyl above your head. These hypnopompic hallucinations, as they're called,
are often of giant figures, sometimes frightening figures, sometimes an immense spider, although
sometimes of a little man in green, sometimes of an angel. They may be akin to dreams in some ways, but here you are conscious
and the thing is with you in the room. There's a presence in the room. It's not entirely easy
coming to terms with something like this. I've actually recently been hearing about a 10-year-old
boy who woke suddenly in the night and saw a figure of a tall woman next to his bed
who told him she was his guardian angel. He turned on the light and the figure didn't disappear. He
ran into his parents when he came back. The figure had disappeared. Now this little boy was very
disturbed. He had no particular belief in angels or visions,
but something had happened which he could not deny,
but could not explain and could not integrate into his worldview.
I suspect that visionary experience,
whether drug-induced or in dreams or whatever,
has played a part in the genesis of everything from folklore to religion.
For example, for some reason, there are physiological reasons for this,
Lilliputian hallucinations, so-called, are rather common, of little people.
And one finds in almost every culture that there are elves, fairies, trolls, little people.
One wants to say they're not at the sort of lofty level of angels and heavens,
but they do represent another reality.
I think this is almost built into the brain as well as built into culture.
Is he telling us that it's built into my brain that I'll imagine little people when I'm asleep?
Possibly.
Gremlins. Gremlins are built into our brain.
He's saying he's seeing it across cultures,
just the same way that we see faces and things that aren't really faces.
Right.
We may be predisposed genetically or evolutionarily.
You mean when I look at clouds, I see Abraham Lincoln?
Yeah.
Yeah.
And you don't see an alien.
You see a human.
I see a human.
Because that's important for your evolution.
I don't see a lobster.
No.
You may if you're Dali.
No, but if it is a lobster, I probably won't think lobster.
I'll think it's, if it really does look like a lobster. Lobster man. I don't think it's lobster man. No, I'm it is a lobster, I probably won't think lobster. I'll think if it really does look like a lobster.
Lobster man.
I'm more likely to say it looks like something familiar to me.
Yeah.
Right?
Exactly.
And so what's the limit of the brain?
I can imagine a day where we plug a USB port into your neck and upload or download.
We kind of did that with Watson on Jeopardy a few weeks ago.
Carol, are you afraid of Watson's rising up? Not at all.
Watson knows things.
He has a memory bank.
Like we said, but ours is very different.
So why can't he ever be us?
One thing a computer still can't do
is recognize an object that's partially
occluded. If you put a picture of a cat
that's hiding behind a tree, a computer will never
know it's a cat. Because the tree is in the way.
What do you mean?
You can see its ears, you can see its tail. We know it's a cat. And this is a huge project. Because the tree is in the way. What do you mean? The tree is in the way.
No, you can see its ears. You can see its tail.
We know it's a cat.
A computer really has a hard time
synthesizing a whole from part.
And it's easy for us.
This is a human perceptual thing
that's so easy
and we don't know how it works.
Okay.
We've got to end the show there.
You've been listening to StarTalk.
I'm Neil deGrasse Tyson.
And as always,
keep looking up.