StarTalk Radio - Extended Classic: “Are You Out of Your Mind?” with Oliver Sacks
Episode Date: September 6, 2015Join us for Neil deGrasse Tyson’s original interview with his recently departed friend Oliver Sacks, now extended with exclusive, never-before-heard content. Also featuring guest Cara Santa Maria an...d co-host Chuck Nice. Subscribe to SiriusXM Podcasts+ on Apple Podcasts to listen to new episodes ad-free and a whole week early.
Transcript
Discussion (0)
Welcome to StarTalk, your place in the universe where science and pop culture collide.
StarTalk begins right now.
In light of the recent passing of our dear friend and celebrated neurologist Oliver Sacks,
we at StarTalk decided to resurrect this episode featuring
my interview with him at his home in New York City. When I think of Oliver, he was kind of like a
fireside scientist. Someone who would go out, make discoveries, do research in the lab,
and come back and tell you stories about it. And every story he told was interesting. He's kind of like the fun
uncle you wish you had. Everything was interesting. And I think it's because he was at the juxtaposition
of mind, body, and soul. Not only that, we had a shared interest in the periodic table of elements,
all the stories behind how each element was discovered
and where they were discovered
and whether they were poisonous or gaseous or liquid.
I'll not soon forget these short moments we had together.
Welcome back to StarTalk.
I'm Neil deGrasse Tyson, your personal astrophysicist.
This week, we're talking about the brain.
And I had to bring in Chuck Nice for that.
Chuck, welcome back to StarTalk. Hey, Neil. What's happening? Can't get enough of you, man. Hey, I don't
blame you, man. We brought you in for the Super Bowl show and for the brain show. What's left?
Actually, two of my favorite things in the world. I find the brain to be one of the most fascinating
studies that anyone can undertake. That and football, the brain gets bashed in football.
There you go.
So they're totally hooked up for that.
Exactly.
So it's about the brain, because the brain is everything, right?
I think so, yeah.
And we try to define what separates us from other animals.
Right.
And it's language, it's abstract reasoning.
We do art and philosophy and music and science.
Science.
And so we try to sort of say that we are apart from the animals because our brain can accomplish all that.
Actually, I think some other animals are doing calculus on the side.
You think so?
I think so.
What animal would be able to do calculus?
If you could pick one.
If I could pick one, dolphins.
Dolphins probably.
Give them a pen.
If they could ever use it, they would show us what's really going on.
One of my favorite memories is of one of the Gary Larson comic where the farm animals.
Right.
And they're just talking to each other.
The farmer is not there.
Right.
The chicken is talking to the horse.
Right.
The horse is talking to the cow.
And the chicken says, but if you take the mass and divide it by the square root of the speed of light, then you get the same answer.
Right.
And the horse answer,
but you're missing the basic premise of my theory.
And then someone else says,
farmer,
cluck,
cluck,
cluck,
moo.
So for all we know,
this is what's really going on.
But you know,
neither you nor I are an expert on the brain,
and we had to reach out into the ether and find who could help us do this.
Cool.
And so we found,
we found Cara Santamaria.
Cara, welcome to StarTalk Radio.
Thanks.
Yeah, so you're an expert on the brain and brain function.
And you've taught it before.
Did we call you a neuroscience educator?
What's the best title we can use for you?
Well, I've taught in psychology and biology.
I guess you could call me an educator, student of the neurosciences.
So aren't we all?
Because who knows it all?
Nobody knows it all.
So everybody's a student, even those who say they're not.
That's true.
Who profess to say they know it all.
That's true.
They don't know jack.
No.
You never really feel like you know much, I think, when you're studying these things.
You always kind of look back on what you've accomplished and say, really, am I just a
hack here?
How much of this did I really gather?
There's so much more. Well, there's the stuff to actually learn that we know. And then there's the stuff
beyond that that no one has even figured out yet. Exactly. There's a lot of that in neuroscience.
And astrophysics. I think those are two very big frontiers. They are. One is the inner and one is
the outer space. Yes, indeed. And so what I wonder, so you taught where? In New York, if I remember
correctly, from your resume. I was in New York for about a year. And before that, I was in Texas for many years.
In Texas. And where do you call home?
Now I'm in L.A.
In L.A.?
Yeah, I'm in L.A.
No, no. Where are your roots?
My roots are in Texas.
In Texas.
I'm definitely a Texas girl.
Transplant. Okay.
Okay. Well, welcome to StarTalk Radio because we're going to be picking your brain and try to leave you with some left to do your work.
All right. As the non-scientists in the room, may I point out to our listeners two things.
One, Cara is hot as hell.
That's number one.
Number two, she looks about 19.
So those are two things you want to keep in mind when you hear the knowledge drop from
her lips.
Thank you.
That she is hot and looks like she's 19.
I'm not 19, by the way.
Well, of course.
Just so you know.
I'm not 19.
Chuck, why did you pick that age in particular?
Because it's legal.
That's exactly what I thought, Chuck.
There it was.
There's no lying in me, Neil.
That's why we love you, Chuck.
So, Kara, if you were to describe what your particular expertise about the brain is, what would you say?
I think that's still evolving.
I think my interest was in brain damage, has been in brain damage.
That's kind of what I want to continue to study.
That's where you learn because you can't go in and poke somebody's brain and find out what happened.
You got to wait for stuff to happen accidentally.
Yeah.
I mean, you can do models with animals and I've done some of that. Non-human animals. Non-human. Yes.
Exactly. Right. Not just guys that play for the Lakers. But you do, you have to wait until
something happens. And sometimes you have to wait until autopsy, or at least until you can get good
imaging to see what actually did happen to that person. Right. And so a particular injury might
damage a certain part of the brain that you never knew had a particular function.
Yeah.
And then a person behaves crazier than they might have behaved before.
And then you've just nailed the spot of the brain for causes and effect, right?
And brain damage, for a lot of people, it's like fingerprints.
You want to say that we have classified particular areas that do particular things, and you damage this area, and you have this effect.
But the truth is, nobody has the same brain damage.
Right.
Nobody.
As each other. It's a very organic thing. Right. Okay. You could damage the same area and have a totally different. But the truth is nobody has the same brain damage. Right. Nobody. As each other.
It's a very organic thing.
Right, okay.
You could damage the same area and have a totally different outcome.
And have a different outcome?
Yeah, totally different.
And is that just because each individual brain has that particular type of makeup?
I mean, the makeup of our brain is that particular?
Well, it's similar brain to brain, but it's very rare that only a very specialized region
of the brain would be damaged anyway.
You're going to have other effects.
Well, see, that brings me to ask, because how many? We've got 100 billion nerve cells? I mean, brain cells? It's very rare that only a very specialized region of the brain would be damaged anyway. You're going to have other effects.
That brings me to ask, because how many?
We've got 100 billion nerve cells?
I mean, brain cells?
Just the neurons.
Just the neurons.
Not the glia.
I love saying 100 billion.
So let's say that together.
Sounds cool.
100 billion.
Very Carl Sagan.
Take out the billion out there in that low voice.
Like Sagan.
I like that.
Carl Sagan.
Sagan.
Saganomics.
And so 100 billion nerve cells and brain cells, and we typically, the naive thought about the brain is that it stores information like a file cabinet and you go and retrieve it.
But recently we've been learning it's much more complex than that, right, in terms of the storage of information and retrieval. Is that right?
Well, I think we used to think that we could just learn discrete packets of information.
I think we used to think that we could just learn discrete packets of information.
You just see something in the world or you experience something through your senses,
and then it goes to a certain part of your brain,
and it just lives there until you want to pull it back out.
Like a file, like you said.
But really, a lot of it is about connections.
It's all about taking something in, connecting it to something you already knew.
Like Facebook.
Exactly.
It's a web.
It's like a social network. A social network. A neural network.
A neural network.
Can I de-friend certain parts of my brain?
You drink
enough.
We'll get back to that.
How to de-friend part of your memory.
Now Chuck, we put you on assignment
earlier. Yes you did.
Where I work, I'm at the American Museum of Natural History here in New York City.
I run the universe part of the museum, which includes-
Which, by the way, is so cool.
If you're ever in New York, you've got to go there.
Oh, thanks for the commercial.
Seriously.
So I run the Hayden Planetarium.
But now the museum has an exhibit on the brain.
Yes.
And I actually had not had a chance to view it, but we sent you so you could report back
to StarTalk Radio. Right.
On what you found and what
others found. Yes. We made the
mistake, I think, of giving Chuck a microphone.
Yeah. I'll tell you the irony
of this whole piece. It's the
brainless going to a brain exhibit.
Alright. Let's see what Chuck tells
us from the museum, live on
location. Well, he were...
I was live on vacation.
I was live on location. He was live on vacation. At the time.
I was live on location.
He was live on vacation.
Check him out.
What is your favorite part of the brain?
Yeah, the stuff in the brain.
The stuff in the brain.
That's what I need more of.
I need more stuff in my brain
because my brain is kind of empty.
What is empty?
Listen, wait.
Rattle my head.
Look at that.
Blink, blink, blink.
You hear that? Yeah, what's that mean? Yeah, that's kind of nothing. Was it empty? Listen, wait. Rattle my head. Look at that. You hear that?
Yeah, what's that mean? Yeah, that's kind of nothing rolling around in my head.
I'm actually a school psychologist. Really? So now, as a psychologist, do you find that people's
experiences and environment or their brain causes them to have psychological problems? No,
experiential learning
and environmental learning have an impact, I believe, on brain development and the memories
that you retain. So how do you explain my mother telling me that something's wrong with me because
I ain't right in the head? You're trying to get a free session.
No dice? Help me, Pamela. I'm on vacation.
Sorry. No dice?
Help me, Pamela.
I'm on vacation.
Did you learn anything that just totally wowed you?
The fact that your brain tells you everything.
It makes everything in your body go.
Everything.
Everything.
Even the things that you take for granted.
Like you just blinked.
I saw you blinked.
I saw you blink there again.
Your brain told you to do that, right? Yep. Do you know why I'm blinking right now? I'm afraid you just blinked. I saw you blinked. I saw you blink there again. Your brain told you to do that, right?
Yep.
Do you know why I'm blinking right now?
I'm afraid you might hit me.
I won't do that.
Thank you. I can rest easy.
I'll tell my brain not to do that.
As a neurobiologist, I'm interested to know what your favorite part of the brain would be.
Well, right now, at my age, the hippocampus.
How's that?
Ah, okay.
That would be because the hippocampus is responsible for what?
Memory?
Yes, yes.
You have to use your brain in order to keep it going.
So the brain is like a muscle.
You've got to use it?
You have to exercise it.
Use it or lose it.
What was the thing that most impressed you about your own brain that you found out in the exhibit?
I think in relation to short and long-term memory,
that it's sleep that actually transfers memories from short-term to long-term.
So you look well-rested.
No, I'm not. I'm jet-lagged.
Are you really?
Yeah, I am.
Are you from Australia?
I am.
So you've had a long flight.
Yeah.
And did you sleep on that flight?
Only a very little bit. So was've had a long flight. Yeah. And did you sleep on that flight? I need a very little bit.
So was there anything else
that really impressed you?
Something you never thought
you'd know about your brain
that you found out?
No, I can't remember
anything else.
And there you have it.
The short term,
long term,
you need some sleep.
I need some sleep.
Chuck,
just annoying people.
Harassing visitors
to the Hayden,
American Museum of Hayden,
no, actually the brain exhibit
at the American Museum
of Natural History.
Yeah.
So in there,
we heard about the hippocampus.
So, Cara,
tell us about the hippocampus.
All about the hippocampus.
Yeah, in one minute about.
I remember
first learning
about the hippocampus
and then teaching my students
about the hippocampus
and I would imagine
a hippopotamus
walking
All I think of is hippo.
through my college campus remembering
how to get to class. I don't know, that helped me at the time. Yeah, it's a deeper structure of the
brain. It's kind of underneath the cortex and it's involved in memory. And it used to be the case
kind of after a really famous patient, patient H.M. had his hippocampus damaged when he had a surgery for epilepsy.
Scientists all thought, well, this must be the seat of memory in the brain.
This is the only place where memory is.
Once again, because of some accident, then they know this.
Yeah, exactly.
Or they believe it.
They could look at his brain and say, well, he's kind of missing this area, and he can't remember things now.
He can't encode new memories.
What we've found out more recently is that memory is very ubiquitous in
the brain. It's in many parts of the cortex. And like we said before, it-
The memory of a single thing can be spread around.
It can. It can.
Not just different things in different places.
But definitely different things. But the memory of a single thing can too,
because we make associations. Smells help us with memory.
Man, Chuck, I smelled something and I said, man, I remember that 19-
Right.
You know, whatever.
You know, you're right. I mean, I was home today at said, man, I remember that 19, you know, whatever. You know, you're right.
I mean, I was home today at my parents and my mother made some food and it took me back
to being in that home when I was like 11 years old.
Yeah.
That's one of the strongest triggers, actually, because it's a very old part of the brain.
So comfort food is not just that it is, but that it has a smell.
Right.
Yeah.
And that it brings comfort.
Yeah.
That makes sense.
Definitely.
Yeah.
And food.
Flavor. Flavor is smell.
The meatloaf is waiting for you outside.
Smell, taste, all of this plugs into the brain.
Yeah, but then that plugs into the brain and it helps us recall memories and encode memories.
But really the hippocampus we found is more of kind of a way station.
It's the place where memories can kind of first be encoded and then spread out to other parts of the cortex for storage.
So this sounds like there's a risk of misremembering something
if it's got to store one place first and then other places later.
But we get back to that in another segment.
But before we even get there,
do you compare the human brain with other brains?
Because I've been reading about this,
and I learned that the octopus,
which is kind of an extraordinary creature for starters,
can each limb, each of the eight limbs
kind of can operate autonomously
without reference to the brain.
This is what I learned about.
Like there's ganglia, like mini brains.
Yeah, mini brains that can make their own kind of decisions.
This is cool and scary.
Awesome.
You know how dolphins sleep?
I never asked.
Since they have to be underwater,
but they're mammals, they have to breathe.
Oh, yeah, yeah.
It's conscious breathing.
So they sleep by one hemisphere going to sleep at a time while the other remains alert.
Wow.
So they can sleep and still swim to the surface and breathe, but they're sleeping.
Half of their brain is asleep.
So basically they sleepwalk.
They do.
Or sleep swim.
Sleep swim.
They sleep swim.
Sleep swim.
Right.
And how about whales?
I don't know.
I didn't know that.
So they can just show, they got a little switch and just switching left and right.
Just like we have a switch.
Well, we have a switch for our whole brain to go to sleep.
It'd be interesting to see what different behaviors they're capable of as one half turns off and the other turns on.
Definitely.
Because we have specialization of our brain halves, don't we?
Or is that just a fiction from the past?
No, we do.
We definitely have specialization.
Right.
Different hemispheres.
That's always, and other, you know, and sort of creatures that are much smaller.
I learned about flatworms, that they don't even have a brain.
Oh, thank God, because you have scared me.
Are you wondering if flatworms are doing calculus on the side?
And where does Chuck fit into this evolutionary?
Well, we're running down our first segment.
When we get back, my interview with neuroscientist Oliver Sacks. We're back.
Chuck, nice.
Thanks for being on StarTalk.
Let me reintroduce our guest today.
In from Los Angeles, this is Cara Santamaria.
Hello.
Hello.
Did I pronounce your name right, Cara? Yes, you did. Thank you. Thank you for that. Cara Santamaria. Santamaria. Hello. Hello. Did I pronounce your name right, Cara?
Yes, you did.
Thank you.
Thank you for that.
Cara Santamaria.
Santamaria.
That's right.
Wasn't that one of the Columbus ships?
Yes, it was.
It was.
Good.
Many a nickname growing up.
Yeah.
Nina Pinta.
And so we promised you before the break that we would take you to an interview that I conducted
with Oliver Sacks.
He's probably the world's best known neuroscientist.
And if you haven't heard of him,
you may have known the movie Awakenings.
In fact, that movie was about him.
That was about him.
Robin Williams was playing him.
Okay, I was going to say,
I'm guessing he's not the Robert De Niro character.
Because that would be truly extraordinary.
Let me double-check IMDB to make sure.
But yeah, Robin
Williams portrayed him in that
film. It was a semi-autobiographical.
I think it was done up a little for the movies as well.
But basically, it was a story of his
life and breaking in as a
neuroscientist. His latest book, The Mind's
Eye, is about how a brain
helps to understand what the eyes see.
Because your eyes are the organ unto themselves and they just got to hand it
over to the brain then the brain has to make sense of it right so this is what's
what's somebody needed to put write a book on that and there's gonna be a
movie released well there wasn't it was the music never stopped that movie come
out yet that's based on one of his essays called The Last Hippie, which was published in his book An Anthropologist on Mars.
I love that title.
So apparently he's been places, perhaps including Mars.
I don't know.
So that's Oliver Sacks.
And in our first clip, he talks about the things that define your identity and your personality.
your identity and your personality.
And he describes what role the electrical currents in your brain
might play in determining that,
but how a super-duper electrical current
from outside your brain
can really mess with it.
Oh, man.
I think that's what they wanted to do to me.
That's the part of your life
we'll get to at another show, Chuck.
Let's see where...
Let's just see where Oliver Sacks takes us.
A colleague whom I describe in my book, Musicophilia,
he is a surgeon here in New York,
and in 1994, through a freak accident, he was struck by lightning.
And he had a cardiac arrest.
He was dead for half a minute,
or certainly his brain did not get enough blood
or oxygen at that time. He had a sort of strange experience. He was conscious of being flung back
many feet by the thunderbolt, by the lightning which hit him, and then he felt he was floating
forward, and he looked down, and that he saw his own body with people around him and he said I'm dead but then
he seemed to sort of move on and then a sense of ecstasy came on him and he saw a bluish white
light and he felt the most wonderful thing in the world was about to happen and then he regained
full consciousness to find someone doing CPR on him.
But about three weeks after this, he had a strange emotional and musical change.
This man, who had never been interested in music, developed a sudden passion for classical music. First to hear it, and then to play it, and then he wanted to compose it.
And this also went with a mystical feeling.
He felt that God had sent the thunderbolt,
but had also arranged for him to be resuscitated,
and that he now had a mission to bring music to the world.
Was he religious before this?
Not really.
I think there were some seeds of religion,
but these flared up with the experience,
either with the psychological shock of being almost killed,
and who knows what neurological changes might have happened as he was electrocuted
and when his brain didn't have enough blood.
The brain being an organ of electrical current, right?
That's what goes on in the brain.
Yes, absolutely.
There's this chemistry and electricity, and that's it.
And somehow from this comes thought, imagination, spirit, the idea of God, and everything else.
Well, anyhow, this man will put a supernatural explanation on this.
However, he is not ignorant scientifically.
In fact, he has a PhD in neuroscience as well.
As a neurologist, it was up to me to put things in more neurological terms, but without in
any way upsetting him, devaluing his experience. And I said, you know, I'm sure this is what you
experience and what you believe, but will you allow that something might have happened inside
you? For example, it might supernatural intervention make use of existing neurological structures?
And he said, yeah, okay.
And at that point where I suggested that the two were not wholly incongruous,
he said he would be prepared to have subtle forms of brain imagery or whatever
to see whether we might be able to find the parts of his brain
which had perhaps been reorganized somewhat,
pushing him towards religion and towards music.
Wow.
Yeah.
Okay, I got one thing to ask about that whole clip.
What's that?
Was that a beep?
Yeah, that was StarTalk's first beep, yes.
Leave it to a neuroscientist to get the first beep on the show.
We're cool like that. You're cool like thatist To get the first beep on the show We're cool like that You're cool like that
That was the first beep
And we had to beep his quote of someone else
That's not even him
Right, right
So that's what that was
So that's interesting
So what confidence this person must have
To believe that God would strike him dead with lightning
But then rely on someone to resuscitate him
So that he would have these magical musical powers and interest you know i'm just going to say and not to ever devalue someone's religious
beliefs or inclinations you know when normally when god strikes you he's kind of pissed
i'm just saying he didn't hit moses with a bolt of lightning and then say i want you to take these
10 rules down to the people no he said he said, Moses, come on up here.
I want to talk to you. Let's talk.
When God strikes you, there might
be a little problem between you and God.
That you haven't really quite figured out yet.
Right. So,
Kara, so our
identity, it looks like it can be altered by just
sort of an electrical shock.
Do you do experiments with that? It can be altered by a lot of things.
You've heard of Phineas Gage?
No.
No.
Phineas Gage.
No, no, sorry.
A railroad worker.
Certainly.
If he's a railroad worker.
Triply not.
But he was, I mean,
he's a super important story
that most neuroscience hear about.
Is he the one with the spike in the head?
Yeah, he's the one with the spike in the head.
Oh, yeah, yeah, yeah.
He had the tamping iron
and it went through his head
and it blew out part of his frontal lobe.
He stayed alive, right?
He stayed alive and dodged infection, which at the time probably would have killed him with an open head injury.
And he was a different person after.
He was like a womanizer.
He cursed all of the time.
He was a big drunk.
Before that, he was a very straight-laced.
So those people today have spikes in their head.
You just saved my life.
I got a reason now.
It's like, honey, I'm sorry.
I just have a spike in my head.
Don't you see this spike?
So how about memory?
Presumably, it can not only change you.
Can it bring memory into existence or take it out?
Can it make memory sharper or lose it?
What, brain damage?
Yeah.
It could.
Yeah, actually.
We see these movies.
How about The Bourne Identity?
That's a famous one where he just doesn't know who he is.
But I've, well, I was going to say I never met anyone who never knew who they were, but
then.
There are people who don't know who they are.
How many?
I mean, there are people who have like dissociative fugue.
I haven't personally.
Dissociative fugue?
Yeah, it's a psychological condition.
And what's that in English?
It's, they dissociate from themselves and then they fugue.
They go away and they don't know why they're where they are.
So you hear these kind of, you know, true crime stories of people waking up in a parking lot, nine towns over and they'd stolen a car.
Covered in blood.
And they don't remember any of it.
Oh, like the crime committed in that state of mind.
Yeah, sometimes that's, you know, probably not the case.
But even Oliver Sacks did write a story about that.
That does happen.
Because he's especially interested in these kinds of... Bizarre.
Bizarre.
A bizarre thing.
And then I was reading
that memory begins to decline
at age 50.
That's not right.
Tom, that can't be true.
We'll get back to that.
Me and NASA,
we're both losing our memory here.
Let's go back to Oliver Sacks
and my interview with him
in his home office
in Greenwich Village, New York.
And in this clip, I asked him about memories, false or real, and how you get them and how you lose them.
Let's see what he tells us.
If you do functional imaging of the brain, it is relatively easy to tell if someone is telling a lie
because it's quite a complex business to tell the lie.
It's easier to tell the truth.
But you cannot tell if someone has a delusion.
Because they believe they're telling the truth.
Yeah.
Whether the belief is well-founded or not, if they have the belief strongly,
and the emotions and the visualized scenes which go with this,
and when people say they've been abducted by aliens, they truly believe this.
In a book I wrote, the name of the book is Uncle
Tungsten, I mentioned two early memories from 1940 of bombs which had fallen in London. An older
brother of mine confirmed one of the memories. He said, yes, it's exactly the way you describe it,
but as for the other memory, a memory of incendiary bombs in our garden and of my father's attempts to stop it and to douse it with sand and water, which didn't work.
My brother said, you never saw it.
I said, what do you mean I never saw it?
He said, we were away at the time.
And I said, but I can see it in my mind now.
I can hear the crackling of the flames, the shouting.
I see the figures of my father and brother.
It is so clear in my mind.
Why?
He said, because our older brother sent us a letter.
And he said, you were fascinated by the letter and even obsessed.
And obviously not only fascinated by it, but I internalized it and visualized it.
It also helps if your brother is a good writer.
Right, yeah.
So this is a false memory or a secondary memory.
Is that the same as implanted memories we've heard?
Yeah, yeah, in a way.
This is a bit like an implanted memory.
It had been implanted by a good description,
which I think probably appealed to me romantically.
It's sort of exciting.
Though I know now that this is a fictitious or secondary or implanted memory,
it does not seem to me any different in quality from the genuine one.
And if I had functional MRIs and was asked to recollect these two things, I think one would see pretty much the same areas of the brain, both the visual and emotional areas sort of lighting up.
That's cool.
So I think there are two kinds of failures of memory.
One of them is remembering things that never happened.
Right. And the other one is remembering things that never happened. Right.
And the other one is forgetting things that did.
Yes.
When we come back, we're going to explore what effects drugs have on brain functions.
This is StarTalk Radio. We're back.
StarTalk.
We've got Chuck Nice and Cara Santa Maria in the house.
So in this next segment, we want to talk about, on this program on the brain, we want to talk about other ways the brain can malfunction or function in ways that, differently from how nature intended.
Right. So when I think of this, I think of sort of psychoactive drugs,
drugs that people take recreationally or medicinally
to alter their state of mind.
Cool.
And I know our special interview guest,
Oliver Sacks, he has experimented with drugs
and he will tell us about it in some clips coming up.
Cool.
And so, of course, if you're a neuroscientist
and medical doctor, you have access to drugs
and it's just interesting.
But why would anyone want to do this?
I mean, I like my brain.
I like when I have deep thoughts about the universe and almost anything that enters the
brain alters your ability to have those thoughts.
So I'm just wondering.
But it may alter it in a positive direction.
In a way.
Maybe you'll have deeper thoughts. Deeper thoughts. wondering. But it may alter it in a positive direction. In a way. Maybe you'll have deeper thoughts.
Deeper thoughts.
Why would anybody
want to take drugs?
Really?
Let me ask you this.
I don't know anyone
who said,
here's an equation
that I can't solve.
Let me take drugs
so I can be
more intellectually acute
in my ability to solve it.
I've never seen that.
Ever.
Well, I think
it's two prongs.
Are you looking at
solving the equation in a very pragmatic way or in a creative way?
Do you need an external?
Creative mathematics.
Where's my paintbrush?
Really?
Here's the answer to the equation.
You really don't think that physics can be very creative?
It can be creative, but in the end, the adjudicator is nature.
It is not an unlimited, infinite tapestry.
And many drugs are found in nature and we possess
receptors in our brains for some of these drugs is that right like what well most all of them we
have cannabinoid receptors otherwise it would be neutral to us yeah i hear cannabis in that
cannabinoid cannabinoid that's code for uh cannabis receptors refer which is code for
thank you um we also have receptors for pcp in our brain and we have not yet really found the Cannabis receptors. Which is code for? Pot. Melon. Thank you.
We also have receptors for PCP in our brain, and we have not yet really found the endogenous chemical that binds to that.
So that's kind of a quandary.
Oh, interesting.
It's a frontier.
Yeah, it's a frontier.
Let's see what Oliver Sacks tells us about his own time in this exploration.
In the early 1960s, like a lot of people, especially on the West Coast where I lived at the time, I took a lot of drugs.
Uh-oh, now you can't run for office.
No, no, no.
But I really wanted to see what I'd read about other forms of consciousness.
To what extent would the world open for me? Would it reveal domains perhaps of natural or supernatural beauty and meaning?
Well, they certainly opened domains of natural or supernatural beauty and meaning? Well, they certainly opened
domains of natural meaning. On one occasion, since you're an astronomer, I haven't mentioned this,
it was back in 67, I had started seeing patients with migraine. And one weekend, I took an old
book out of the library written in the 1860s. A book was called On
Negrim. And then I loaded up pharmacologically. But instead of giving way to fantasy, I started
reading this book. And the sort of drug ecstasy coupled with what was in the book. And I started
to feel this is a most wonderful book.
I felt that the neurological heavens were opening for me
and that migraine was shining like a constellation.
One of the people quoted in the book was an astronomer,
the younger Herschel, who had migraine.
And describing his migraines, he said he felt like an astronomer of the inward.
As I continued to read the book, I thought this is a wonderful, incredible example of mid-Victorian medicine at its best.
But it was written in the 1860s, and now this is the 1960s.
The author of the book was a man called Living, Edward Living, and I thought, who should be the Living of our time? And there was a
very disingenuous clamor of names came to me, followed by a very loud inner voice which said,
you silly bugger, you're the man. When I came down from that, that sense that I was the man,
and this was my subject, stayed with me. And I wrote my book on migraine, and I never took drugs again.
So in a way, the opening or awakening I had was in that drug experience.
Interesting. So he takes drugs and decides to read a book rather than jump off a balcony.
This guy is really a scientist. That's all I got to say. Anytime you do drugs, and you're just like,
what should I do? Crazy sex by a couple hookers.
I think I should read a book. You are truly a scientist.
Yeah. This. Yeah. That's a whole other state of mind. Exactly. Deep within that.
I was doing some homework on this and I learned that I don't know if you knew this, Cara, that in the 1950s,
the CIA experimented with LSD to see if they could alter the memories and perceptions for espionage purposes.
I don't know if you knew about that.
Did it seem to work?
I don't know.
Outcomes of those experiments?
I don't know the outcome.
The outcome was this.
Everybody they experimented on ended up following the Grateful Dead.
Actually, so that was in the 50s, and that's what birthed the 1960s.
A lot of AWOL soldiers there, I think.
There it is.
And, of course, the psychogenic factors are not unique to Western culture or modern culture.
Right.
Native Americans long ago, well, maybe still, peyotes.
Peyotes, yes.
Wouldn't you get that from a cactus or something?
Yeah, that's cool.
For your vision quest.
Yeah.
Yeah, so I cool. For your vision quest. Yeah. Yeah, so it's all, I find it interesting, but are you suggesting
that the people
who take these inner trips,
that they're somehow,
do they function better
as people interacting
with other people?
Now, there's this movie out now,
what's it called, The Pill?
No, it's with Bradley Cooper,
it might as well be called The Pill.
So it's where,
so he's a parent,
I haven't seen the movie yet,
but I'm told he takes a pill and it actually builds the mental powers and acuity that he has.
I think they're trying to say that once he takes the pill, he's now open to experience everything.
But the truth is, if we didn't have selective attention, we would be less functional.
Oh, okay.
Significantly less functional.
Selective attention that allows you to not be distracted by...
We have to be able to filter what gets into our heads.
So people with ADD, they don't have these filters.
They have less of them.
So it's just like, I'm going to make
the incision right below the A order.
What the hell is that? That's so cute!
That's a cute little bunny over there.
Why is there a bunny in the operating room?
Because I'm on peyote!
So you don't want ADyote. Oh, yeah.
So you don't want ADD surgeons.
Yes, that would be bad.
No, that would be bad.
Unless they're medicated.
Well, yeah.
And so also there are people who have, what I wonder is that people have deep religious experience.
They see Jesus or Muhammad, whatever it might be.
Have people studied what parts of the brain are being excited and what visions they might have and whether that's similar to whatever might be stimulated by drugs?
Or are their bodies producing drugs that give them these visions?
The actual visions?
The actual visions.
Well, I think that there are probably a lot of different parts.
I know that there are scientists out there like Andy Newberg, who has written books about
religious experience,
and he thinks that this is a genetic thing. It's something that's in all of us. We have a god gene
in our brain that allows us to see that. I personally disagree with that view. I don't
think that religious experience is something that's always encoded in our brains. I think
it comes from external sources. But it could be in some people's brains and not others,
right? What's wrong with that? It could be a genetic trait for some people.
But it could be that that's what,
whatever's happening in your brain,
that's how you choose to describe the experiences
in religious terms.
I see, because you have a religious context
in which to interpret it.
And if you don't have the religion,
maybe it's aliens that you're looking at.
Exactly.
Right.
Which is very easy to figure out
because all we have to do is examine your anus.
Is that right?
Seriously.
And if there's no trauma,
you have not been aboard a ship.
Is that how that works?
Thanks.
Next time I'm abducted,
I will carry that.
So getting back to my interview
with Oliver Sacks,
he went on to talk about
how you can have these profound visions
and what might induce them and what they might mean.
So let's see.
This is in his home office, Greenwich Village, New York.
Check it out.
I wondered whether one could imagine something one had never experienced, in particular whether one could imagine a color one had never seen.
And I built up a sort of pharmacological mountain. I won't go
into details. And when I was very loaded, this connected my mind with the seventh color of the
spectrum, indigo, and the fact that no two people will ever quite agree as to what is indigo or
whether there is an indigo. And I said to myself at the peak of my experience, I want to see indigo now.
And suddenly, as if thrown by a paintbrush,
a trembling, pear-shaped blob of indigo appeared on the wall.
And I leant towards it in a sort of ecstasy.
It was a color I'd never seen.
I thought, purely metaphorically, of course,
this is the color of heaven. I thought
this is the color which Giotto had tried to get, but never could. I also thought it's a color which
is no longer in the world. This was the color of the Paleozoic Sea, but has disappeared. And as I
led forward, the blob disappeared. But I somehow felt that blob not only as luminous,
but as numinous.
Numinous. I had to look up that word, Chuck.
Numinous. Numinous.
I thought he said numerous.
Which I believe means of or relating to
spiritual experience. Exactly.
Yes, Chuck, the Ivy League
educated comedian we have here.
That was very poetic,
I think, that interview. It was. It was beautiful. It was very poetic, I think, that interview.
It was.
It was beautiful.
It was.
Well, he's a beautiful man.
I mean, everything about him.
He's soft-spoken and he would never, you know, some people have like the evil side of it.
You can't even picture that.
Then you have to bleep him in interviews.
Well, I bleep him quoting someone else.
That's what that was.
We've got to take a quick break, but more StarTalk Radio.
Welcome back.
During the break, Chuck, you commented that you swig Robitussin for what reason?
No.
Okay, here's the deal.
Because we were talking about, you know, pharmacological drugs being used or inducing hallucinations.
And I know that people actually drink Robitussin,
prescription-strength Robitussin.
You know people who do this?
I know someone who, well, I know somebody who does it.
I've heard that phrasing before.
Seriously.
I have a friend who does it.
Here's why I would never do it.
I'll tell you why very, very quickly.
I'll tell you why.
So this guy and I are hanging out.
He's drinking Lick or Robitussin.
That's the street name for it, okay?
He starts getting so high that he's hung over,
his body's slumped over, and he's calling me Betty.
Now, I'm like two things.
One, I don't know who Betty is, but if she looks anything like me, that's one ugly broad.
Two, I don't ever want to drink anything that makes me think another man is Betty.
Period.
That cured you of your Robitussin.
Right. So, Kara, are there psychometric...
Psychotropic?
Psychotropic drugs that are prescribed for any particular reason?
Not to give people hallucinations on purpose.
Right.
But, of course, there are lots of psychotropics that are prescribed.
Anything from ADHD to schizophrenia to depression, anxiety, all of these disorders require drug treatment, if you have drug treatment, with psychotropic.
So psychopharmacology is all about getting into your head.
Speed.
People take speed on purpose.
On purpose.
To help with attention.
To help it.
Let's get back to my interview with Oliver Sacks and see what he talks about.
Hallucinations.
I don't know if he's ever had one.
I'm very interested in hallucinations.
I don't know if he's ever had one.
I'm very interested in hallucinations.
Some of the hallucinations occur with people who are blind.
And being partly blind myself, I have a few low-level hallucinations myself.
But they're only really of blobs of color and geometrical figures and things like that.
But it puts people on the spot if they have a hallucination.
Hallucination is not like imagery.
It's like perception.
It seems to come from outside.
You have no sense that you are generating it or any part of you. So you hear music.
You run to the window.
You look outside for the source of the music.
It's only when you can't find a source for the music that you perhaps start to think,
has some part of my brain gone on
automatic? Or you may not think that. You may maintain a false belief. One of my old patients
was convinced that the patient next door had a phonograph and was putting on the same record
again and again. When Schumann had some musical hallucinations at one point, he thought it was
divine music. So, Kara, people can have hallucinations.
And if you're religious, you have this sort of inclination to think that it's divine.
Yeah, you may.
If you're not, others might just think you're crazy.
Right.
Or they might think you're crazy in both cases.
You may think you are crazy personally,
or you may not identify those hallucinations as being external.
What is your capacity to judge that you yourself are not of your own mind?
I think it just really depends on the person.
I think that some schizophrenics are aware of their disorder
and they know that they need the Haldol
or whatever drugs they take to get through the day.
And I think some have absolutely no idea
that they're experiencing hallucinations and delusions.
The voices in my head just told me you are correct.
And so how about the kids?
We're prescribing Ritalin for kids.
That's affecting their brain
in some way. Yeah, that's what I was mentioning before. That's speed.
Oh, that is speed. Okay. Well, it's
like speed. Speed-like.
It's a speed-like drug.
And it does. You know, we don't really know the long-term
effects of kids taking these drugs year after
year after year.
Because they're the first experiment in this.
They're not adults yet.
That could be a whole other... We could start another decade like the 60s.
We're priming them for that.
Let's see, I think my one last clip coming up with Oliver Sacks,
and we'd speak a little more about hallucinations and find out what he tells us.
Among the many sorts of hallucination are sorts which one may wake with suddenly in the night. You wake
up suddenly and there's a pterodactyl above your head. These hypnopompic
hallucinations, as they're called, are often of giant figures, sometimes
frightening figures, sometimes an immense spider, although sometimes of a little
man in green, sometimes of an angel. They may be akin to dreams in some ways, but here you
are conscious and the thing is with you in the room. There's a presence in the room. It's not
entirely easy coming to terms with something like this. I've actually recently been hearing about a
10-year-old boy who woke suddenly in the night and saw a figure of a tall woman next to his bed
who told him she was his guardian angel. He turned on the light and the figure didn't disappear. He
ran into his parents when he came back. The figure had disappeared. Now, this little boy was very
disturbed. He had no particular belief in angels or visions, but something had happened which he could not deny, but could not explain and could not integrate into his worldview.
I suspect that visionary experience, whether drug-induced or in dreams or whatever, has played a part in the genesis of everything from folklore to religion.
For example, for some reason, there are physiological reasons for this,
Lilliputian hallucinations, so-called, are rather common, of little people.
And one finds in almost every culture that there are elves, fairies, trolls, little people.
One wants to say they're not at the sort of lofty level
of angels and heavens,
but they do represent another reality.
I think this is almost built into the brain
as well as built into culture.
Before we end the show,
we've added exclusive, never-before-heard content
taken from that very same interview.
He and I got to chatting about mysticism,
religion, and the importance of finding happiness and meaning in scientific truth.
Let's hear what Oliver Sacks had to say about all of that.
I'm here with Oliver Sacks at his home in New York. And Oliver, you're an expert on
the mind and all the curious ways it can malfunction. And as a scientist, when I see people either
ignore evidence or believe things for which there is no evidence, we don't view that as a
malfunction. We view that as just part of how people behave. But it severely interferes with
how the person's ability to understand the natural world. What's your explanation for this, or how do you see this from your lines of research?
I think all of us need to have a sense of meaning, need to have beliefs.
We have emotional needs and intellectual needs.
Certainly when we are very young, these are going to be vested in our parents
and sort of powerful, comforting figures,
and we're not yet having
to deal with the physical world that much. We are so well cared for. Human beings must have come
into the world 100,000 years ago or whatever, and looked at the stars and looked at the mountains
and wondered how this all came to be. And I think there is a very strong craving in all of us for cosmic meaning.
How did this get there? How did this come about?
Cosmologies, of course, are provided in every religion.
Cosmologies usually not only of creation, but often of a caring creator as well.
Certainly this is so for the monotheistic religions.
And it's not easy to give up that way of thinking.
I think it's astounding that in this year, 200 years after Darwin's birth,
150 years after the origin...
The origin of species.
years after Darwin's birth, 150 years after the origin.
Or the origin of species.
The origin of species. The belief in supernaturalism, here in America at least,
is stronger than ever. You know, in the 1880s, Nietzsche wrote about the death of God.
When I was a student 50 years ago, I think there was a strong feeling that we might be moving towards a more secular world.
But it's gone in the other direction. That fact tells us, at least here in the West,
or in America, there's a very strong restoring force back to those ways of thinking. Otherwise,
we would have just moved towards a rational world, you know, step by step without ever looking back.
Yes, there's a very, very strong need, whether I'm lucky or unlucky and not feeling that
particular need strongly, and also being so delighted and enchanted and raptured,
ecstatic in a way by the natural world that I don't feel a craving for any other. I think that the existing world is extremely
wonderful and complete. And I don't, well, I don't feel in myself any disposition to supernaturalism.
In other words, your read of nature is sufficiently fulfilling that there's no other,
you don't have to reach for any other kind of source of enlightenment.
No, I mean, obviously, art and human contact give meaning for me, but I don't feel this,
any metaphysical urge to religion. I was brought up in a fairly orthodox Jewish household, at least
so far as practice was concerned. My parents went to
synagogue. We kept a kosher house. Unleavened bread was destroyed at the Passover and so forth.
However, I don't know what my parents actually believed. Questions of religious belief were
never discussed by them or with them. They didn't necessarily believe anything.
I think for them, religion was mostly a question of practice
and ritual and ethics and remembrance.
However, at quite an early age, there's a particular incident.
My mother was an anatomist and a surgeon and also fond of botany.
And when I was about 10 years old and we were out in the garden, it was a lovely summer day.
And the bees and butterflies were doing their thing.
And my mother explained that they were fertilizing the flowers.
And we had a magnolia tree in the garden.
And this was covered with little beetles.
And I said, what are these beetles doing?
And my mother said, well, they will look after fertilization.
And I said, but why beetles?
And she said, the magnolia is a very ancient flowering plant.
She said they developed 80 or 90 million years ago,
and at that time there weren't any bees or butterflies.
There were only beetles around, and they've stayed with the beetles.
Now, as I think of this, you know, 65 years ago,
I still remember the shock when, as it were,
deep time and a natural process seemed to open out.
The very word 80 million years made me shiver.
The idea that there might have been a time when there weren't birds and butterflies.
And for me, I think there was a sense of epiphany here.
There was something akin to a religious vision.
But the religious vision was in fact one of evolution and natural selection.
So it's not a supernatural awakening.
It was a scientific awakening for you.
Yes, exactly.
So maybe the lesson here is,
for all those who are trying to hit people over the head
for their unscientific ways,
they're not really substituting into that void
that they've just created
the sense of wonder and enchantment
about the natural world.
They just kind of leave them there bleeding in the street. Is that a fair characterization for what some people are doing out there trying to
get people to think? There are a number of evangelistic atheists around, like Dawkins
and Dennett. But I'm not an evangelistic atheist. I think this is mostly people's own business. I am
very worried when faith-based things get into education and politics
and faith-based religion instead of evidence-based religion.
I've certainly, as a medical man, I've seen lives lost and sacrificed
through irrational beliefs in particular.
I'm not sure whether I should mention particular religious sects, but
I've had young patients whose parents refused to have them medicated and say that the Almighty
will look after them when they need penicillin. I don't think the Almighty would have anything
against penicillin, so to speak. Sounds like then people might be in need
of just such an awakening in their own lives.
Maybe not everyone has that kind of life experience,
and so they're less susceptible going into adulthood
believing things that just feel good
rather than are actually true.
You need to find things which feel good and are true.
But it's the nature of the poor, fragile bipeds
in a complex and dangerous environment
that it's very understandable.
We all reach out for power and comfort
of a somewhat irrational soul.
So you're prepared to accept that as a natural part of life,
but you're not on a crusade to rid people of this.
You recognize it as a natural consequence
of our evolutionary trajectory.
Yes, I'd say that, although I would hope that the trajectory might take us past this.
The world is a frightening, it's a huge, perplexing, frightening place.
And it's very understandable to say that one wants to fill it with friendly spirits,
very understandable say that one wants to fill it with friendly spirits to explain it so the woods will have dryads and the streams will have niads. I think there's a powerful need to humanize the
world or anthropomorphize it or put in figures who are intelligible to us who would explain the world
and who will keep an eye on us. So the not knowing is then a fearful state of mind.
Fearful, but it also produces awe, with a capital A. It's a state of wonder as well.
This state of wonder, I think, can propel one in a mystical and religious direction. It will also
propel one in a scientific direction.
The term Darwin uses again and again is wonder and delight. The question as to whether art
needs religion is an interesting one. Or needs the mental states that would give you religion.
My good friend Jonathan Miller, he directed an incredible performance of the Matthew Passion.
He's done this several times at BAM.
And everyone, including the most devout people, are moved to the depths.
After this happened, Jonathan remarked jokingly, he said, not bad for an old Jewish atheist.
However, this old Jewish atheist was able to create or recreate using Bach and his
own imagination. I don't know how much one can have religious imagination or mystical imagination
without belief. There is a great conductor, David Randolph. He's 95 years young. He is the most
amazing man. He leaps on the podium like a 20-year-old.
He has conducted the Sicilia Chorus, which is a secular chorus, for 60 years.
Now, for example, when he conducted a Messiah, Handel's Messiah,
he made a point of saying to the audience beforehand,
he said that he thought there was no such thing as religious music,
and that some of the most beautiful parts of the Messiah Handel had lifted from contemporary,
erotic, and sometimes bawdy Italian songs of his own time. The same music could be fitted
into an erotic context, a solemn religious context, any context, that it was the context and
the association. And he would also bring out, let's say, Berlioz and Brahms and Verdi, who had
written profoundly moving requiems, but they were all atheists. However, I, you know, when one goes
to cathedrals, and you see the power of religion to inspire something like this.
But I think an interesting question is whether there can be any substitute in the creative imagination for these mystical and religious states of mind.
Einstein here talks about the mysterious and that this is the most beautiful experience we have and so the cradle of all true art and science.
But Einstein, I think, seems to regard God as a colleague.
Well, so thank you, Oliver Sacks, for appearing on StarTalk.
Okay. Thank you very much. I've enjoyed it, Neil.
That was Oliver Sacks, friend, scientist, and brilliant thinker.
He will certainly be missed.
For StarTalk Radio, I'm Neil deGrasse Tyson.
And as always, keep looking up.