StarTalk Radio - StarTalk Live: Big Brains at BAM (Part 3)
Episode Date: August 2, 2014Our exploration of the human mind concludes with Neil deGrasse Tyson, Eugene Mirman, neuroscientists Mayim Bialik and Dr. Heather Berlin, and guests Michael Ian Black, Paul Rudd and Bill Nye.Read more... and listen to the full show:http://www.startalkradio.net/show/startalk-live-big-brains-at-bam-part-3 Subscribe to SiriusXM Podcasts+ on Apple Podcasts to listen to new episodes ad-free and a whole week early.
Transcript
Discussion (0)
Welcome to StarTalk, your place in the universe where science and pop culture collide.
StarTalk begins right now.
Welcome to StarTalk Radio. I'm your host, Neil deGrasse Tyson.
I'm an astrophysicist and director of New York City's Hayden Planetarium at the American Museum of Natural History.
The following show is the third part of a live show we recorded on February 24th, 2014.
Because that show went on for a long time and all of it was great, we had to create a third part.
This is without
precedent in the archives of StarTalk. Our show was recorded at the Brooklyn Academy of Music,
known as BAM to the locals. And in addition to my co-host, comedian Eugene Merman, we were joined
on stage that night by neuroscientist Heather Berlin, Bill Nye the Science Guy, comedian Michael
Ian Black, the actor Paul Rudd, and star of the hit TV show The Big Bang Theory, Mayim Bialik.
Some great science fiction storytelling involves power of the mind.
Telekinesis, you know, mind control, mind reading.
Is anyone working on this? Is it in the future? Or is it just a pipe dream?
Well, there are studies, let's say something like mind reading. There are studies now where you
put someone in a scanner and you show them different pictures or even movies and you see
what the sort of neural signature is of that. And then you again put them in the scanner. The
experimenter doesn't know what they're looking at. They're only reading out their brain activation
and they could very reliably predict what the person is seeing. And then if
you take that a step back and you can even look at what a person is imagining. So over time,
the more precise this gets, I can definitely envision someone just reading someone's
neural sort of activation and predicting what it is they're thinking about.
So you'd have to walk around with some kind of neuro map visor so that you can detect if
someone is being turned on
or turned off
or highly useful socially.
Yeah, if you're autistic.
You're describing like
super extreme guesstimation.
Yeah, you could be wrong.
The more we understand about the brain,
the closer you will be to getting right.
But successful charlatans
or so-called mentalists or magicians,
they have this ability
at some very reasonable level,
probably through facials. Go ahead.
That's not mind-reading, right?
That's a very subtle fine-tuning into body language,
gesturing, shifting of basically the air around people.
But there's nothing magical, mystical about it.
Your grandmother is here, and she agrees.
LAUGHTER
Getting something? But I'm sorry, if we take what you're talking about, Heather, maybe 10 steps further, is
it possible in the future, it would be possible to kind of record your consciousness and then
market that as I would be able to live as you for a day through VR virtual reality playback.
I don't know if you could ever.
That's a very philosophical question.
I'll tell you what.
I want to be a lady for a day.
What would you do?
I would buy that URL right now, man.
Ground floor.
But actually another study that I'm involved in,
we're doing something called hyperscanning or dual scanning
where we can scan two people at the same time having an interaction
and kind of seeing what's happening in the space between two people.
Because as I said before, I can never get into your brain
or really know what you're thinking.
And that's what communication is.
It's about the space between.
And now we're seeing what's happening in your brain
when you're actually really taking in what I'm saying and vice versa.
And also as scientists, I'm sure we'd all agree. Yeah. There are things that we can't see and that
we can't understand, but there are also things that we know now as scientists, things like
pheromones. There are things that occur, for example, between two people that are not necessarily
detectable in all the other ways we're looking, but there's something that happens that, again, we don't need to write off as magical and mystical.
We may not be able to measure it yet, but yes, I think we'll get there in more ways.
Okay, so let me end the show with this exploration. Heather, you're on really, dare I say,
dangerous territory, because the more you know what the brain is and what it does,
as a scientist, what any scientist wants to do at the end of the day is control it. Yeah? That's the evidence that you know what it brain is and what it does as a scientist what any scientist wants to do at the
end of the day is control it yeah that's the evidence that you know what it is you're doing
you want to make predictions make predictions have those predictions come true and you say i know how
this thing works so if the brain is your laboratory and you say oh i've just discovered what makes two
people fall in love now you make a love potion number nine and you control people's behavior and conduct. Do you have another team of ethicists
behind you looking over your shoulder at your conduct?
Yes, it's the CIA.
Yes, it's the CIA.
I mean, there's a whole field of neuroethics, which are talking about issues.
But yes, I mean, ultimately, what we're going is from correlation to causation. When I say we're going into the brain and implanting electrodes for a good cause to treat psychiatric illness, we're controlling emotions.
And, you know, if somebody who was a psychopath...
Well, emotions that are socially regressive.
Yeah.
But if we know exactly what the right formula is for, say, love,
I mean, you can stimulate an emotion in someone and they'll label it as love.
But no one's there overlooking my shoulder saying, you know, there is an IRB.
There's a board that, like...
IRB?
Yeah, Institutional Review Board.
That makes sure that you don't make people fall in love when they're not supposed to.
Okay, good.
Exactly.
Feel super safe.
Okay, so the day would surely come where you can make someone smarter if they can afford
to pay you.
You could cure them if they can afford that.
We have all kinds of neurological disorders.
I'm just wondering, what is the dystopic future of this? And what is the utopic
future of this? Well, one issue is that it could be that only people who can afford, let's say,
the neuro implants get them, and then they have an advantage. And then everybody will have to,
so like performance enhancing drugs, you know, in sports, the reason they outlaw them is because
if one person has it, they have the advantage everyone else has to do with it. Same with mental
illness, right? In terms of access to mental health care and mental, you know, exactly. I
mean, one person has a mental illness, everybody else. No of access to mental health care and mental, you know. Exactly. I mean, one person has a mental illness.
Everybody else is like, hey, I have a mental illness.
No, but I think what you're getting at, there's a dangerous possibility of sort of narrowing
the field of who is well, who is smart, who is desirable, who is accessible, who gets
access to care, not to bring it down at the end, but that could be dangerous.
Yeah.
And so let me ask each one of you, what's the scariest future of neuroscience
you can think of?
Oh, can we end on a positive note?
On a positive note, okay, okay.
What's the happiest?
What is the greatest?
You work in this field.
What is 50 years in the future,
what is that world gifted to us
by you and your research colleagues,
50 years from now, what is that?
Wow, well, what I would hope is
that we can really have a full scale map of the human brain going all the way from the genome to
function and to mind and brain. And once we have this full map of the brain and how it works,
then we can help prevent things like Alzheimer's. So rather than looking at treatments, we're going
to look at prevention. If you can go in and tweak the genome a bit so that people don't get these kinds of mental disorders to begin with, then we don't even have to worry so much about
the treatment side of things. So that would be wonderful. So can you treat the genome in an adult?
And then that fixes their brain as an adult? That would be great if we can do that. Yeah,
if we can even do it in an adult. We've got to ask, though, from an evolutionary standpoint,
why is it that mental illness persists?
If it's really especially dangerous, wouldn't it have been eliminated from the gene pool?
So I suspect it's not in the biggest picture that deadly to have a mental illness if it's manageable.
That is to say, germs and parasites are much more likely to kill you than being mentally different.
I just wonder about this whole thing when you go to predict the future without taking into account let's say the flu that killed more people in 1918 than world war one did
the other thing is these questions i think are really only useful to a society that can afford
it right the people that are more successful at having an overall health in their tribe or their
society might do better than...
Well, there are two things there. One is that certain things do persist. Like, for example,
being a sensation seeker, being really impulsive, that's a quality that got people out there and,
you know, discovered America, right? These kinds of traits, which we might call, you know...
By the way, that probably killed more people than who actually survived and came back.
Artistic ability and mental illness, right?
Yeah. There are some positive things about being at the extreme ends of what we consider to be normal.
There is no such thing as normal.
But what studies do show is that the most amount of any disease, cancer, you know, all sorts of physiologic diseases,
the thing that causes people the lowest quality of life in distress are mental illness.
Because they don't die from it, but they have to suffer their lives.
And often they have children and pass on genes
that may be predisposing to future mental illnesses.
It's a really sad discussion.
I don't know.
We're trying to find something uplifting.
We've got to wrap this up.
So let me offer some concluding remarks here.
You know, I studied the universe,
and the interesting thing is that there's this universe inside our heads
that is perhaps less well-known
than the
universe extends back to the Big Bang in time and in space. And so it's curious, maybe the big
challenge here is you're using your own brain to study a brain. There's not some other entity
studying the brain. And that prevents some questions perhaps from even being asked. You
don't even know the right question
because the brain is asking the question about the brain.
Carl Sagan is famous for the saying
that humans, conscious humans,
are a way for the universe to know itself.
No, we're not the universe, we're in the universe,
and we use our brain power to decode the universe.
I'm intrigued by the fact that the real future here might just be in our minds,
and maybe we will learn what questions we're not asking yet.
And those are the questions that keep me going every day,
because that's the future of discovery in any field.
So, join me giving a very warm thank you to this brilliant panel.
It's Q&A time. We have some microphones on the aisles. What we prefer is just to find one person
to direct the question to, because if all
twelve of us answer, it takes all night.
So if you can be specific about it.
Sir, you're up front.
Hey, how are you doing?
Hello.
So you talked a little bit about the idea of these people, super humans, right?
And so there are people out there who have what I think is called savant syndrome, who
have some kind of amazing mental abilities.
They can learn new languages in a week or do really rapid calculations.
So what's different about these people's brains?
Is this something we all have inherently able in our brains?
And could we hack it to enable that ability?
Okay, great question.
I think we all want some of that ability, right?
So what's up with that?
How can we all become magic? With savants, they seem to be wired
slightly differently. And so they seem to have these usually these isolated areas of specialization,
but sometimes at the cost of other parts of the brain, development of other parts of the brain.
So it's not necessarily a good thing to be very, very good at just one thing and at the cost of
other things. The only way I think we can enhance
our cognitive abilities is with working at it.
It's like anything, like with losing weight,
you have to work at it.
So I don't think there's a sort of magic pill,
but it has to do with the way
that their brains are wired slightly differently.
Okay, so using the word wiring,
so in the future you can adjust the wiring.
If you have a savant,
is there an ethical question
about whether you would change that about that person?
Yeah, I think there is.
I mean, it's with anything.
Even with a psychopath, they don't come in for treatment.
That's a gesture, too.
For example.
Even psychopaths need to be asked for their...
They might enjoy being that way.
So if they're not a threat to other people
or if they're not in distress,
I don't think that we should go around
just changing people for the sake of it.
Question here, yes.
Yeah, so since we have the comedians here,
I don't know if you guys have thought a lot
about the intersection between comedy and neuroscience.
I have, yes.
Yeah.
I wrote a paper on the physiology of laughter in college.
Actually, he did.
Yeah, it was all wrong, but...
I wrote the shit out of it.
All right, perfect.
So, you know, these guys are up here
providing great, like, comedic interjection,
and not everyone can do that.
Like, not every brain works that way.
So maybe we could hear from, like,
one comedian and one neuroscientist
why... Or how about a neuroscientist
who is a comedian?
Shemya, can I add
that on the Big Bang Theory,
her timing is impeccable.
Blossom.
That's my mother.
Thank you, Mom.
That's your mom.
Wait, what?
From 1991 to 1994, she was Blossom on the show. Blossom, mom. That's your mom? Thank you. Wait, what? She had practice.
Yes, from 1991 to 1994, she was Blossom on the show.
Blossom, yes.
I mean, I-
And you were the young Bette Midler in The Rose.
I was, in Beaches.
Yeah.
Yes.
Beaches, Beaches, Beaches.
No, I mean, I'm happy to let you guys also speak to it.
I mean, in terms of what's funny and why are things funny, there is a really interesting
field of sort of understanding the timing. It's complicated social interaction, but I think for me as an actor and
as a scientist, what I'm aware of is we're constantly scanning and tracking. People can
do it better than others. You know, standups can do something that I can't do, right? I can't do
improv. There's a different set of skills that actors and performers have. But you know, the
joke that we tend to be neurotic,
that we tend to be constantly looking to make someone feel something is a very complicated
thing about being an actor. And I didn't think about it as much when I was younger, but when
you meet other actors and when you talk about process, which a lot of people laugh, they figure
like, oh, we're paid to be funny or we're paid to make people believe something. It's a very
complicated process by which I need to make you feel something. I need to make you believe something. It's a very complicated process by which I need to make you feel something. I need
to make you believe something. And I need every single person to feel that. So if you're a stand
up, you're working with a room of people. When I work in front of a live audience, I need everyone
to feel something. And it's a really complicated interaction, especially if you work with a live
audience. It's very different than not because it is a constant tracking.
Heather, are comedians subjects of your... Well, yeah, actually. So this is really exciting stuff. This is a constant tracking subjects of your well yeah
actually so this is really exciting stuff this is a new area of research we're going to is actually
the neural basis of creativity and improvisation and so what we find is that whether it's jazz
improv or comedy improv there's a certain neural signature involved when people are improvising so
you can put people in a scanner and you freestyle rappers, right? So you give them a memorized rap and then they can freestyle. Or you give a musician a memorized piece or
improv. And when they're improvising, what they find is that a part of the prefrontal cortex,
the medial prefrontal cortex becomes extra activated. And that has to do with internally
generated ideas. And the dorsolateral part of the prefrontal cortex becomes deactivated. And that
has to do with sort of self-awareness and monitoring your behavior.
So you almost go into this free-flowing state.
If you become too aware, you mess up.
You're not good at improvising.
You have to kind of lose yourself, so to speak.
And that's what the neuroimaging shows.
I have a follow-up.
Is there any connection between improv and optimism?
I don't know.
Actually, wait, one of the studies showed that when they were improvising, they had increased activation of the amygdala, which could be related to positive feelings.
That's your brain part.
That's kind of my thing.
That's interesting to know. Right here, sir.
Hi. I actually had a couple of questions, but then when you guys mentioned eugenics...
That's kind of your thing.
What's your question, Himmler?
Wow.
Wow.
All I could... Too soon.
Too soon.
Too soon.
Too soon.
That kind of shades my question, doesn't it?
I was actually... All I could think about was the movie Idiocracy.
I don't know if any of you guys have seen that.
Idiocracy.
It's kind of a scary look at what might happen if...
If dumb people keep breeding.
Thank you, yes.
Luckily, make-believe would go on.
Yeah, sort of like if, you know.
Okay, so my question is to the neuro folks over there.
I don't know if you've seen anything going in that direction or not or have you seen a genetic predisposition
predisposition in society to become idiots i didn't see the movie so i can't speak to that
well this is going to be i'm trying to be like sort of PC. Well, one thing is that with evolution in the past, right,
if it was survival of the fittest,
then those who just couldn't make it couldn't make it,
and they would die out,
and then there would be certain traits that would have evolved and survived.
But now we have all sorts of ways to keep those people around.
Exactly.
So it's kind of messing with evolution a bit.
Yeah.
So who knows what's going to happen. I've got an in-depth and specific question
I guess to the scientists and I guess to the neuroscientists here
based on quantum theories of the mind
what's your take on the idea that consciousness
originates at the quantum level?
And if you're familiar with Schrodinger's equation, the very fact that observing something
changes the outcome. So the mind observing consciousness, what's your guys' take on that?
Yeah, I can offer something on that as well. So quantum physics has sort of mysterious properties
that are real, but they defy anything your
five senses or more have ever experienced in your life.
So you say, well, that's weird, but it's real.
It happens.
Particles pop in and out of existence.
You look at it and then it changes into something else.
This is the behavior of the universe on the smallest scale.
Because it has all these mysterious properties,
what I have found in my experience
watching people think about the world
is they have this urge to insert quantum physics
where there's something where they don't otherwise
understand what it is.
So we don't really understand consciousness,
quantum physics must be at work, right?
So it's this, and in fact,
the more they invoke quantum physics,
that's the evidence
that they know least
about what it is
they're talking about.
Well, I admit that.
I don't know what I'm talking about.
No, no, but I think
the Heisenberg uncertainty principle,
I mean, I do appreciate this notion
that any observation of something
alters what it is,
how we perceive it,
and how we describe it.
I appreciate that.
However, when I observe you,
you don't pop into a different quantum state.
You're a macroscopic entity.
Large things average out these principles
so that we have a macroscopic world
that lends itself to macroscopic discussions.
If we were the size of particles, yes.
Me beaming eyes at you with a light would pop you into some other part of the universe.
Yes, but you're big, so it doesn't happen.
Is that a fat joke?
Okay. It would be cool if we were like particle size and you walked through a doorway, okay?
You would diffract, okay? Because that's what light does. I mean, it'd be funny if
Planck's constant were much larger than it is, weird stuff would happen all the time.
Okay, so, yes. Can I just bring us to the brain for a second? I just want to diffuse this theory,
because I always get this question about the quantum theory. Roger Penrose, who's a mathematician
at Oxford, and Stuart Hammerhoff, who's an anesthesiologist,
they came up with the quantum theory of consciousness, basically saying that at some level, at the structural level of the neuron, is where these quantum effects can sort of take place,
and that's where consciousness lies and the rest of it. But there's no evidence for that, and also
the temperatures at which these effects would have to occur, they'd have to be cooler than the
temperature that the brain is currently at.
So there's a lot of things that go against this idea,
but again, people tend to want to put together something mysterious with something else mysterious,
but as far as we know from the field of neuroscience, there's no evidence.
Can I ask a follow-up question about this?
Okay, almost as quick, but go.
Doesn't this imply, this idea, does that imply then that consciousness has mass?
I've got to say, you guys, as an engineer, at some level you can measure the, like with a logic analyzer or whatever,
you can measure the voltage without screwing up the circuit. At some level.
I don't understand what you're saying.
Wait, wait, wait, wait. If one day we establish consciousness as the sum of some energies within the brain,
then by E equals mc squared, consciousness has a mass equivalent.
That's what I'm saying.
That's right.
Okay.
Thank you.
Yes, sir.
Okay, so tonight's discussion was mostly centered upon human neuroscience for obvious reasons.
But there was a few mentions about
neuroscience and consciousness and whatnot of other species and we do know
that some species are quite smart and not just like our close relatives like
great apes but also toothed whales some birds like parrots and crows and even I
recently saw the video that looked like it was unedited of an elephant painting an elephant.
Was that on YouTube?
No, it was not.
I saw a dolphin playing chess. Is that real?
My real question is, what might consciousness be if it has arisen in different lineages of living things?
What might consciousness be for them?
And also, what might it be for, say, say a possible alien life? We got a Hindu here
So conscious
Big brained mammals out there. Can we learn about our consciousness by studying them? You work on lab rats to learn about other things.
I don't work on rats.
I work on humans.
But, yeah.
Oh, you work on, excuse me.
You remember the time we just said that?
I don't work on rats.
I work on humans.
Well, they're different.
They're different.
Different IRB protocol.
Yeah, exactly.
Totally different protocol.
So there are two different things.
There's intelligence.
There's consciousness.
A bee can have consciousness.
It can feel a sensation.
It can sense something.
It can do something very simplistic. So you don't need to be able to play chess to be conscious. We know that in certain
creatures like in dolphins, in whales, in humans, there are certain types of cells called spindle
cells, especially in animals that are social. And that might have some kind of link up to awareness
or at least some kind of a self-awareness. But until we have a fundamental theory of what
consciousness is, we won't be able to know for sure what something has. We only
can look at behavior. Even with humans, in the
lab, you have to just ask them, did you see that
or not? Were you aware of that or not? That's the only
measure we have. I saw a comic of two dolphins,
one swimming next to another,
and they're
referring to the humans up on the shore,
and one dolphin says to the other,
they face each other and make noises,
but there's no evidence they're actually communicating.
Right.
Yes, right here.
Yes.
First I have to say, what an awesome panel.
And Bill, you did great in the debate.
Good.
Thank you.
I'm really starstruck just being here.
But anyways, I want to ask two of the neuroscientists and to neil because
i think you've been on stage i think a lot of you guys have been on stage with them it's a rather
controversial position held by sam harris um who was one of my colleagues at ucla he was
let me preface this by saying i think he's a brilliant orator i think he communicates very
well i just know he's got some he's a great thinker too but it's trying well. I just know he's got some, he's a great thinker too, but it's trying to parse out
whether it's, whether we agree or not, or whether there's consensus. And the question is about
whether or not we have free will. Oh yeah, I can answer that. Free will. So as far as we know from
neuroscience. Sam Harris recently published a tract on the assertion that we do not have free
will, and the evidence cited for it comes from neuroscience.
I must answer this. I have no choice.
There you guys go.
Or do I?
Free will, what's up?
So as far as we know, it started with Benjamin Libet's studies in the 1980s
and all the way until modern studies now.
But the sense of free will is kind of an illusion.
Free will is an illusion in the sense that your brain decides first and you become aware of it after the fact.
And we know this from a whole series of studies from measuring neural activation.
There's even studies now where you can put somebody in a scanner and you could predict, for example,
up to 10 seconds now before they're going to decide
whether to go left or right or press the left button or right button by just reading their
brain activation. The brain pre-decides. Yeah, the brain, it's called the readiness potential.
So it kind of is gearing up and you can start to read how the neurons are firing and that it's
going to go sort of toward one way or another. At a certain point, you can make that prediction
well before the person becomes consciously aware that they've decided to go left or right.
Big N too. Talking big N. Yeah, that's a big N there. well before the person becomes consciously aware that they've decided to go left or right.
Big N, too. Talking big N. Yeah, that's a big N there.
It's pretty well established that yes.
Okay, but does that alone negate free will?
Your brain has free will, presumably, to do it.
The brain decides.
Your perception of I made the decision only comes after the brain has already kind of decided.
I made the decision is my
brain that's I making the decision.
So let me ask you,
what is this phenomenon?
I sure hope
I don't see my keys in the trunk
of my car and close the lid on it.
What is that?
You know what I'm talking about?
Why does the brain keep getting keys in the trunk? You feel that you're going to make a mistake, but you go ahead and make the mistake anyway.
So this is the thing.
Everybody's talking about consciousness, but much of our behavior, much of what we're doing
is happening outside of awareness.
So you're already halfway there before you're realizing, oh, where am I going?
What am I doing? And so if we had to be consciously
aware of every single move we made
the brain doesn't have the capacity to do that
consciousness has a limited capacity
but the unconscious processes seems to be unlimited
which is not to say we also couldn't call that
free will, right, in an unconscious way
you're unconsciously
making decisions freely all the time
good answer good we did it Unconsciously, yes, you're unconsciously making decisions freely all the time. Good answer.
All right.
Good.
We did it.
Yeah, yeah.
Sir, go.
Okay, the question is kind of following up on the issue of awareness and heightened awareness
and the use of psychotropic drugs and hallucinogenics in treatment of brain disorders.
Oh, okay.
Yeah, so.
And I have one other question.
How many people on the panel are left-handed?
Ambidextrous, yeah.
We'll get rid of them.
Eugenics.
No, just kidding.
No, no.
Once again,
the left-handed people
have certain advantages
to certain things.
Yes.
It's a hilarious old joke.
You want your kid to grow up
to be a left-handed relief pitcher
because he or she is going to, so far,
make more money. Actually, people
who are left-handed have less lateralization,
so language tends to be more lateral.
It's not completely in the left brain, but there's
more activation in one
hemisphere of the brain than the other.
So language tends to be
lateralized, or there's more activation in the left
hemisphere, but people who are left-handed, there tends to be lateralized, or there's more activation in the left hemisphere. But people who are left-handed, there tends to be more activation in the right hemisphere,
above and beyond those who are right-handed.
So there's less lateralization.
There's more sort of distributed across the brain activation.
I'm about 20% left-handed.
But the point of the psychotropic drugs, the treatment, there are actually now studies
that where I'm at, they're using ketamines as a treatment for depression,
which used to be a club drug called Special K.
And if you take too much of it, you can dissociate.
It's not good.
We're doing that later.
That's just part of it.
In the name of science.
Hey man, you guys like want to dissociate?
Got some Special K.
But I think also treatment for depression,
that's it's sort of one set, but
there is a field that's looking
at sort of understanding altered states
of consciousness and our perception of
altered states of consciousness, probably beyond the scope of
And even using drugs like
ecstasy for therapy and treatment
for PTSD. And therapeutically
with well-trained and
very elaborate supervision
it can be very helpful.
Oh, yes.
Is there any mental state beyond the one that is alert and bright that can be chemically induced
that is a greater mental state of awareness than the one you started with so because it appears
that every time you do anything chemical to the brain, it disrupts your ability to know what reality is.
First of all, even just in our non-drug-induced state, our perception is different than what's
necessarily out there in reality. So there's all sorts of illusions. You don't see what's
right in front of your face. So there's that. So anytime you add a certain kind of chemical,
you change the neurochemistry of the brain, it's not going to say that you're going to be
more aware. You're just going to have a different type of awareness and perception.
But when you have things like synesthesia, when you have senses crossing from LSD,
those kinds of experiences, these are kind of qualitative or quantitative questions, right?
Yes, you're having more experiences that are much more complicated and much more trippy
than what we're experiencing in our normal state.
But I think that's subjectible.
And you've read about these conditions.
I have.
Life mech.
So my follow-up question is,
what if the way you see blue
is different than the way I see blue?
It's like really red.
It's like the same.
Something I've always been fascinated with,
you guys talked about telekinesis, mind reading and all that.
Where does deja vu fall into the spectrum?
We've all experienced it.
We've all had that moment or many moments that we know it's coming,
but we don't know why.
Have you asked this before?
I did.
I'm waiting for one of you.
I'm sorry.
It's only because I'd already heard you ask that question.
I knew it.
Have you heard George Carlin comment
on Deja? He says, have you ever walked into a room
and you were sure
it has never happened to you before?
That's Oujade.
So what do you guys say about that?
I'm not an expert in this area,
but I think that something is triggered in the brain
where you get a sense of familiarity
and then you attribute that to what's happening around you.
And there's also studies that show
when you lose that sense of familiarity,
some people think that a person they know very well
is an imposter.
So they think like their husband or wife is an alien.
And sometimes that same neurocircuit
is activated in any situation
and they feel like they've been there before.
And I think that's sort of speaking to the sort of redundancy that occurs in the brain, that
the notion is that there are many ways to get to one destination.
And sometimes pathways get triggered that have this sense of familiarity, because you've
literally activated a pathway that is redundant.
I mean, everything's got to fit in there.
Sure, there's miles and miles and miles, but there are certain things that are going to
trigger redundant kind of pathways that also, again, have that sense of,
I've done this before, and your brain thinks it has. Yeah, and also the other thing with that
memories are malleable. Each time you re-remember something, you can reconstruct it. It's different
than the memory of your iPhone. The picture that you look at will stay the same, you know,
10,000 years from now, but every time you remember something, it slightly changes,
so we can rewrite history in a way.
The last question of the evening. Yes?
If someone was born in space,
how would that affect the way their brain develops?
Aww.
That's great.
Well played.
How old are you? Nine. You you're nine it's past my bedtime
is it past your bedtime when you see your parents let them know they're
irresponsible unless you drove here
child genius so that's a brilliant question, first of all.
Second, I can take it only so far, maybe Heather and Mayim have some other comments on that,
but we evolved to be on Earth with our force of gravity and the kind of light that's here.
So there are all these things that shape what eventually we call common sense.
Things fall down when you let go of them.
In fact, the act of saying let go means to drop it,
whereas in space, letting go doesn't drop it.
It just sort of stays there. So your awareness of what is normal is really different,
and we don't really know.
It's kind of unethical at this point to just
do that experiment. Let's see what happens to my kid after she's born in zero G. So we
don't really know. But from the experiments done, there are things about your ears. Inside
your ear, you get a sense of balance. At the fitness center I do that little balance thing so you can do it. So your sense of balance comes about because
you're in a gravity field. If you're born in space and that's all you know, and then
we put you here on earth. Did you see the end of the movie Gravity?
No.
In it, Sandra Bullock, who's been a long time in space, she-
The only accurate part of it.
She crawls up on the beach, and she realizes she hasn't had to walk in a long time.
Her sense of balance had grown accustomed to being in zero-g,
and had forgotten what it was like to walk on Earth.
Dr. T, I've got to interject this. Do it. You don't know earth. Dr. T. I got it. I got to interject this do it
You don't know that don't know what you don't know that if a kid were born in space
That he or she came back to earth couldn't figure it out. You don't know like he or she may have actually a whole different deeper
understanding of inertia and physics and friction then I admit we don't want to run that test.
Uncle Bill is right.
We don't know, okay?
Our closest guest is Sandra Bullock.
Yeah.
Yeah, best evidence is what happened
at the end of the movie Gravity.
Thank you all for coming to StarTalk Live!
Thank you all for coming to StarTalk Live!