Stuff You Should Know - How Morphic Fields Work?
Episode Date: March 3, 2020Biologist and science historian Rupert Sheldrake is known as a heretic of science, mostly for his deeply strange ideas about what connects all living things. But his pokes at science help keep the fie...ld from growing dogmatic and for that we salute him. Learn more about your ad-choices at https://www.iheartpodcastnetwork.comSee omnystudio.com/listener for privacy information.
Transcript
Discussion (0)
On the podcast, Hey Dude, the 90s called,
David Lasher and Christine Taylor,
stars of the cult classic show, Hey Dude,
bring you back to the days of slip dresses
and choker necklaces.
We're gonna use Hey Dude as our jumping off point,
but we are going to unpack and dive back
into the decade of the 90s.
We lived it, and now we're calling on all of our friends
to come back and relive it.
Listen to Hey Dude, the 90s called
on the iHeart radio app, Apple Podcasts,
or wherever you get your podcasts.
Hey, I'm Lance Bass, host of the new iHeart podcast,
Frosted Tips with Lance Bass.
Do you ever think to yourself, what advice would Lance Bass
and my favorite boy bands give me in this situation?
If you do, you've come to the right place
because I'm here to help.
And a different hot, sexy teen crush boy bander
each week to guide you through life.
Tell everybody, ya everybody, about my new podcast
and make sure to listen so we'll never, ever have to say.
Bye, bye, bye.
Listen to Frosted Tips with Lance Bass
on the iHeart radio app, Apple Podcasts,
or wherever you listen to podcasts.
If you've ever been at home and wondered,
Josh and Chuck, is it really worth
going to see them perform live?
The answer is a resounding yes.
Yes, and if you live in Vancouver, BC,
or anywhere near there, come on out to the Chan Center
on Sunday, March 29th to see us and find out for yourself.
And then the next night, if you live around Portland, Oregon,
you can go to the Arlene Schnitzer Concert Hall
and we'll be there, ready to go on Monday, March 30th.
That's right, you can get all ticket information
at SYSKLive.com.
Welcome to Step You Should Know,
a production of iHeart Radio's How Stuff Works.
Hey, and welcome to the podcast, I'm Josh Clark.
There's Charles W. Chuck Bryan over there
and there's guest producer Dylan sitting in
this fine Wednesday morning of weirdness.
Everything's out of whack and strange.
I know, right?
Hopefully our voices sound normal, Chuck,
I was worried about that.
So, not that anyone cares,
but our regular Tuesday sesh got pushed
because our computer took a dump.
Technical difficulties.
And then we said, hey, let's just do it tomorrow morning.
Had a few more technical difficulties, but here we are.
Going strong, buddy.
I'm tired, are you?
No, I'm okay, I've had enough coffee that I'm not tired.
It's weird for us to record in the morning,
it just, everything's out of whack.
I would call it eerie, I think.
Cause you know, I do the movie crush,
mini crushes on Wednesday mornings, usually.
So usually I'm just making dumb jokes
and cussing a lot with Noel right now.
Right.
I gotta switch my brain back into G-rated mode.
Yep.
To talk about Rupert Sheldrake.
That's what we're doing.
And you can curse if you want,
we'll beep it out.
Sheldrake?
Depending on, yeah.
I think that's how the scientific community refers to.
That's just one of those names
that seems like it should be yelled at, like that.
And I like to call him Rupert after Michael Cain
in 30 Rotten Scoundrels.
Oh, right.
Remember he called Steve Martin's name
at one point was Rupert?
There's a cork on the fork.
Yeah, man, that was a good movie.
It was.
So we're talking about a different Rupert
or Rupert.
Rupert.
Rupert Sheldrake, who is widely considered
in the scientific community, a heretic,
a fraud, a hoaxer, a pseudoscientist,
all sorts of things.
And normally we don't entertain that kind of stuff
or specifically people who are considered as such
because we tend to be like,
yeah, pseudoscience is not so great.
But there's something about Rupert Sheldrake.
There is.
That.
The cut of his jib.
It may be a little bit something like that,
but he is different in some ways.
He kind of stands alone.
He's got staying power to say the least.
He was first branded a scientific heretic in 1981.
And he's still around doing his thing,
ticking off the scientific establishment.
Yeah, but see, he's also in certain circles
labeled as an open-minded scientist
and someone not afraid to kind of question
the unquestionable and someone who flies in the face
of what some people call the scientific orthodoxy.
Or dogma.
Where everything is so rigid
that there is no room for new ideas to be explored.
Right, that basically the scientific orthodoxy
that you refer to, it kind of says,
we are on basically the right track.
We generally have the parameters kind of figured out.
We know the math we need to be using.
We know the places we need to be looking.
We have generally in everything from physics to biology
an understanding of the general structure.
Now it's just a matter of filling in the details.
Yeah, we've looked under the hood.
Uh-huh.
And we know what's going on generally.
We know that it's a combustion engine
and not an electric car that the universe is.
That kind of thing.
The heretical electric car.
Right, so when somebody comes along and says,
no, no, that's not even a car that you're looking at.
That's a boat.
The scientific establishment or really any establishment
really tends to get shook by that kind of stuff.
They don't like that.
And one of the reasons that I was trying to figure out
like why people get so invested in this.
I think there's a lot of people who come
and say, I am a person of science.
I believe in this.
I subscribe to it.
And they end up going so far as to pin their identity to it.
And this happens with just about any structure.
And when you pin your identity to something,
when that something, that structure is attacked,
you take it as a personal attack.
And I think that that's one of the reasons
why a lot of people are so rabidly against Rupert Sheldrake.
So should we talk about this guy?
I think we should.
I think, Chuck, we should explain
one of the reasons why he has such staying power
and what makes him different is that he is
about as trained a scientist as a scientist can be.
Yeah, and as we move through this,
you'll see that what makes him stand out
from other kooks is that he's very, very intelligent guy.
He's not a kook.
So that's why it's kind of like,
that's why certain people listen to him.
One of the other things I really want to point out
at the start of this, and this is what really differentiates
him from a lot of people on the fringes today,
is he's not in a hole.
He's very polite.
He's very calm.
He's very measured.
He doesn't engage in ad hominem attacks against his critics.
He engages with his critics.
He's actually a very congenial person.
He's just on a different side of the coin
from the scientific establishment in almost every respect.
Yeah, and when I've read articles and interviews
with other people from the establishment
that have hung out with him and done experiments,
they're all like, he's a really affable kind of fun guy.
Exactly.
Even though when you look at him,
and when you hear Rupert Sheldrake,
it doesn't scream fun and affable.
No.
But he is.
Yeah, he's got a lampshade on his head,
nine-tenths of the time.
Man, can you imagine the lab parties?
Beer bong.
You get freaky.
All right, so he started out his career,
kind of right down the middle, science-wise.
Went to Cambridge as an undergrad.
He won a botany prize there, the university botany prize.
He then went to Harvard, studied philosophy.
He studied the history of science.
That was a big one.
Went back to Cambridge.
Yeah, apparently he's just like a savant
when it comes to science history.
Went back to Cambridge, got his PhD in biochemistry,
and then a postdoc with a Royal Society
in plant development in the aging of cells.
So I think that's unassailable, unimpeachable.
It really isn't.
Had he just kind of continued along,
this was largely in the 70s,
had he just kind of continued along this path,
he probably would have been a really widely respected,
although pretty obscure plant scientist
or biologist of some sort, right?
Just one of many.
But one of the things that happened to him
was he went to India and studied
and lived at an ashram for about a year and a half,
and apparently smoked a lot of hashish while he was there.
Now, is that true or are you just goofing?
The hashish part?
Yeah.
Oh, I'm just goofing, but...
But surely he did, right?
Surely.
Yeah.
But the thing is, is around this time,
he elaborated on an idea that he'd had,
that he'd learned about probably in his history
of science classes,
that science can't explain how you can take some cells
that start out as like a seed or something like that,
and that little seed grows into an oak tree,
and that that oak tree looks startlingly similar
to other oak trees that you can dig up
from a thousand years ago,
or imagine that they'll basically look like
a thousand years from now,
or that are spread out on different continents.
They can't, science can't explain how morphology works,
that how something becomes the thing that it is,
and that that resembles something else.
And you say, well, it's genetics,
like that's kind of the common thing.
But here we get to that point where science is like,
we've got the broad strokes,
we just don't understand the details,
and genetics can possibly be the thing
that explains this later on,
but we really have no idea how this stuff works
because it's really, really intricate
how something like that happens.
Yeah, it's almost like Sheldrake was like,
Tom Hanks and Big in the boardroom
when they're talking about the toys,
and he's just like, I don't get it.
Yeah, like, cause they'll say, oh, well, it's DNA,
and he's like, yeah, but I don't get it.
Like, how does a tulip become the tulip?
Well, it's DNA.
Yeah, but that really doesn't explain it all.
Well, it's DNA.
We understand DNA.
He's like, yeah, I don't get it.
Yeah, and to him specifically,
DNA is a chemical that dictates
how other chemicals are produced, right?
He thinks it's very overrated.
He does, which that in and of itself is heretical,
but it's pretty funny too, but it is.
But with morphology, with how something
takes the shape that it has eventually
and it's a mature state, there's a lot going on there.
There's like little cells that have to set up
and arrange in a certain pattern
that later on down the road,
after all these processes play out,
will form another pattern.
So there's basically planning.
There's timing, like all of those that process
has to happen at just the right steps
and just the right stages for that end result
to be what it's supposed to be.
There's differentiation of cells
where one cell can produce a new cell
and the new cell has totally different genes
turned off or on that will allow it to specialize.
And these are the things
we don't understand what's guiding it.
And so Rupert Sheldrake kind of tapped
into a thought that started, I think,
back in the 1920s among biologists
that there must be some unseen guide or force
that basically says, I've got this.
I know what the end result is.
I can take the starting bit
and guide it into this end result.
And we don't understand what that is.
Yeah, there were a couple of scientists
in the 20s and 30s studying
what they called morphogenetic fields,
which is sort of like the idea
that there is this invisible mold
that we don't fully understand
that gives the shape to these things.
A guy named C.H. Waddington in 1936
had a paper called Morphogenesis and the Field Concept.
And then a Russian biologist named Alexander Govitz
kind of had the same thoughts,
but I think he came independently to these thoughts,
which was, hey, there's something else going on here.
We're calling it morphogenetic fields.
And this is this, like I said,
this idea that there are these invisible molds
that we don't fully get
that gives things their eventual shape.
And that's why they all look alike.
Right, so on the ashram in the late 70s, early 80s,
Sheldrake was kind of vibing on this idea
of there must be some field,
these morphic fields or whatever,
that guide the development of something living
into its mature form,
because we just don't understand it.
So, hey, maybe that's just as good an explanation
as our current understanding,
which is really non-existent.
So, he took it further though, and he wrote a book.
He did, he took it further.
He wrote a book called A New Science of Life.
It was his first book, as far as I know,
at the very least it was his first book
that really kind of made a splash.
And in it, he kind of said these morphogenetic fields,
we're gonna call them morphic fields now.
And not only do they guide the morphogenesis
of a living thing, they guide its behavior
from that moment on, from the moment of conception on
to I guess it's death.
And then when that thing dies,
the life that it's led will contribute
to this morphic resonance
that carries on to the next generation,
and the generation beyond that.
And so, you eventually have this long line of tulips
that know not only how to grow into the right shape,
but how to behave and do all the things
that tulips do because of all of the living tulips
that came before it through this process
of morphic resonance.
Yeah, and not just like that tulip growing nearby
at the same time, but he said,
what if it just was across all of space and time
and the tulip in Africa in the 19th century
has informed the tulip in Florida in the year 2020
how to grow.
Right.
And everyone went, oh, good hashish over there in India
in the 1970s, right?
Sheldrake.
Right.
So, yeah, we'll get to how it was received in a minute,
but you wanna take a break and then come back
and kind of explain how he says it works a little more?
Yeah, but I also think I totally spoiled
how it was received, but that's okay.
That's all right, man.
All right.
Oh, oh, oh, oh, oh, oh, oh, oh, oh, oh, oh, oh, oh, oh, oh.
On the podcast, Hey Dude, the 90s called
David Lasher and Christine Taylor,
stars of the cult classic show, Hey Dude,
bring you back to the days of slip dresses
and choker necklaces.
We're gonna use Hey Dude as our jumping off point,
but we are going to unpack and dive back
into the decade of the 90s.
We lived it, and now we're calling on all of our friends
to come back and relive it.
It's a podcast packed with interviews, co-stars,
friends, and nonstop references to the best decade ever.
Do you remember going to Blockbuster?
Do you remember Nintendo 64?
Do you remember getting frosted tips?
Was that a cereal?
No, it was hair.
Do you remember AOL Instant Messenger
and the dial-up sound like poltergeist?
So leave a code on your best friend's beeper
because you'll wanna be there when the nostalgia
starts flowing.
Each episode will rival the feeling
of taking out the cartridge from your Game Boy,
blowing on it and popping it back in
as we take you back to the 90s.
Listen to Hey Dude, the 90s called
on the iHeart radio app, Apple Podcasts,
or wherever you get your podcasts.
Hey, I'm Lance Bass, host of the new iHeart podcast,
Frosted Tips with Lance Bass.
The hardest thing can be knowing who to turn to
when questions arise or times get tough
or you're at the end of the road.
Ah, okay, I see what you're doing.
Do you ever think to yourself,
what advice would Lance Bass
and my favorite boy bands give me in this situation?
If you do, you've come to the right place
because I'm here to help.
This, I promise you.
Oh, God.
Seriously, I swear.
And you won't have to send an SOS
because I'll be there for you.
Oh, man.
And so will my husband, Michael.
Um, hey, that's me.
Yep, we know that, Michael.
And a different hot, sexy teen crush boy bander
each week to guide you through life step by step.
Oh, not another one.
Kids, relationships, life in general can get messy.
You may be thinking, this is the story of my life.
Just stop now.
If so, tell everybody, yeah, everybody
about my new podcast and make sure to listen.
So we'll never, ever have to say bye, bye, bye.
Listen to Frosted Tips with Lance Bass
on the iHeart Radio app, Apple podcast
or wherever you listen to podcasts.
Chop, chop, chop, chop, chop, chop, chop, chop, chop, chop, chop, chop, chop, chop, chop, chop, chop, chop, chop, chop, chop, chop, chop, chop, chop, chop.
Okay, so we're at the point where, um,
Rupert Sheldrake has published his 1981 book, A New Science of Life.
And in it, he's talking about this morphic resonance
that basically says, um, anything that self-organizes
from a molecule to a giraffe, um,
knows how to take the shape or is guided by a process
that shapes it called morphic fields.
But even more than that, it's behavior.
It's future behavior is shaped by these same morphic fields.
All of the, all of the things that the,
the giraffes that came before it learned and knew and saw
and ate and figured out becomes this kind of body of consciousness
that's passed along to every new giraffe that's born.
Yeah, I think we should read this quote.
Okay.
This is a great interview in Scientific American.
Um, who was it, was it, who was it interviewing him?
I can't remember now.
Was it Rose? No.
I'm not sure.
It was, it was a contemporary who was, uh,
more, you know, traditional mainstream science,
but he again was like, this Sheldrake guy,
he's got something, he's got equality.
Right.
So here's how Sheldrake himself answers
the question of morphic resonance.
Morphic resonance is the influence of previous structures
of activity on subsequent similar structures
of activity organized by morphic fields.
It enables memories to pass across both space and time
from the past to the greater the similarity,
the greater the influence of, of morphic resonance.
What this means is that all self-organizing systems
like molecules, crystal cells, plants,
animals and animal societies have a collective memory
on which each individual draws into which it contributes.
And here's the key here, I think he says,
in its most general sense, this hypothesis implies
that the so-called laws of nature are more like habits.
Yeah. Scientific establishment really particularly doesn't
like that last bit right there.
Yeah. Sheldrake just called out the laws,
so-called laws of nature.
Right.
So there's something in there that kind of stuck out to me
that I was curious about.
I couldn't find an answer to is that,
he says the greater the similarity,
the greater the influence of morphic resonance,
but what is the similarity say in like a giraffe embryo
that allows the morphic resonance of all the giraffes
that came before to be like this?
This is the thing we need to exert our influence on.
Like what similarity attracts that morphic
resistivity?
I took that to mean maybe not in the case of giraffes,
but in the case of like different varieties of an orchid.
Like the more similar, you know,
cause that's why they're all different ones.
I don't know, maybe not.
Yeah, but what is the initial similarity
that that morphic field recognizes in that specific
kind of orchid that says, oh, I'm going to influence you?
Or is it just-
We should call him.
It just naturally happens.
I don't know.
But these are the questions that you start to wonder
about when you read Sheldrake's stuff,
which is, I think the reason why I like him,
like he, it just makes you think.
You just start to think differently than,
than just like, oh, it's DNA.
Yeah, where are you with this guy overall?
I am sympathetic to him because I admire
that he has a tremendous amount of courage
and willingness to take tons of flak.
And I'm sure in this day and age, lots of hate and threats.
I think that I am critical of the fact
that he stopped publishing peer reviewed papers
all the way back in the mid 80s.
That makes him currently less of a scientist
and more a science communicator,
but he's also kind of making up his own science too.
So I don't know if he qualifies as a science communicator,
but I generally like him and I appreciate the role
that he plays in this, this, with science.
What about you?
I'm kind of with you there.
I admire his, his chutzpah because I don't think
that he is a charlatan out just to make money
selling books like some people think.
Yeah, I don't either.
I think he's a really smart guy who gave,
has given his whole life to deep, deep thought
and research on this stuff.
And I read some of it and I think he may be onto something.
I read other stuff and I think this sounds like magic.
And we are men of science, we are podcasters,
but we have always roundly sided with the scientific method
as sort of the baseline.
And if you can't, if you can't satisfy the scientific method,
then we typically kind of poo poo it,
but there's something again about the way he's gone about it
that just doesn't seem like he's just some wacko out there
making stuff up.
Yeah, and I think he also kind of tunes into something
that I dislike, which is, you know, he's really critical
and really challenges, you know, hardened dogma
of a lot of the scientific community
where he's like, this is just how it is.
Well, why?
Yeah.
I don't know, I was taught that,
that's just how it is and stop questioning it.
And I really dislike that.
And I like him that he challenges that as well.
Yeah, there's a rigidity in science
that turns us both off, I think.
So turned off right now.
I was gonna make a joke, but I'm not going there
because I'm in the mini crush mode, so.
Right on, keep it in, that's why I came.
All right, so let's look at a few examples
of claims that he makes about things
that he thinks morphic resonance might explain in nature,
specifically with animal behaviors.
He says things like fish schooling,
butterflies, monarchs flying thousands of miles
to the same place, homing pigeons,
termites in Africa that are blind that build,
you know, a 10 foot tall nest with ventilation structures.
He said all this stuff, or more importantly,
we'll look at this a little closer in a minute,
a dog and their owner and a dog anticipating
their owner's return, even though it might vary
on what time that happens, like the sense
that the dog knows and is waiting by the door.
He thinks that's all explained by morphic resonance.
Yeah, and I mean, like it's curious in that, you know,
how does a bee know after it makes that wax ring
in a honeycomb, how does it know to melt it
into a polygon shape rather than just a circle?
Or like those termites, like why does a termite nest
look almost identical to other termite nests?
DNA.
You know, yeah, exactly.
Like there's a lot of behaviors
that we can't quite explain that.
If you do kind of buy into this morphic resonance idea,
you could say, well, that's actually really, really interesting.
Now, and this is a real good criticism
of morphic resonance is you could also just as equally say,
magic or God or whatever, there's not,
no one's proven that morphic resonance exists.
This is just Sheldrake saying, here's a good examples
of what I'm talking about this morphic resonance stuff.
Yeah, and this is kind of important too.
He talks about the fact that humans
are not as sensitive to this because,
and this is where he kind of got me a little bit thinking,
he says, we're so distracted by technology
and we don't need collective memory of past humans
to survive anymore.
So that's why we can't really sense these fields.
And I kind of disagree with that in some ways.
Like I think if it does exist, it still is,
it still survives in humans and things like,
think about how easily the average human can pick a snake
out of the grass with peripheral vision, right?
I wasn't raised around snakes.
My parents didn't drum it into my head
to be really wary of all snakes.
And yet I'm a pro at picking a snake out in the grass
with my peripheral vision.
Are you really?
Sure.
And it's been shown that people can like pick a gun out
as quickly as they can pick out snakes and spiders.
And we're really good at picking out snakes and spiders
in our environment and this would be a pretty good example
of that if you ask me.
That is the most common descriptor I think
when people say, what's Josh like?
I'm always like, he drinks a lot of beverages,
coffee, water, you know, energy drinks.
He's a hard worker and man, you should see that guy
pick a snake out of his peripheral vision.
It's uncanny.
It makes a gunshot ricochet sound.
Like I don't go for a walk in the woods without him anymore.
Sometimes you're nice and carry me on your back
when I get tired.
So, here's a couple of things
with human morphic resonance
that this is where it gets a little wacky to me.
He says, he claims that a crossword puzzle is easier
to complete later in the day
because of all the other people
that had solved it earlier in the day.
And they are broadcasting this morphic resonance out
into the universe, I guess.
Yeah, just their general awareness of the answers.
That gets a little wacky to me.
Yeah, a little.
The other one is not as wacky
as that feeling eyes in the back of your head
like you're being stared at.
That's a thing.
He says that's morphic fields.
Yeah, that your morphic field extends beyond your head
and that it's sensitive
and is the first thing contacted by that person's stare
and it lets you know basically that you're being stared at.
Yeah, this is where he just,
he's going in the right direction, then I hear that
and I think, oh boy, that is sounds a little wacky.
I read another really good explanation for that,
that it's a self-fulfilling thing where you,
say you're in a library or whatever
and you get the sense that you're being stared at
by somebody at a table behind you
and when you start to turn around,
the movement of your head
catches that person's attention.
Interesting.
And when you finally complete that turn,
that person is looking at you.
That makes sense, especially if you're like,
ah, for God's sake, and you turn around.
Right, yeah.
Then they're definitely going to look the next time
you turn too, because they're keeping an eye on you.
That's right.
Or they're just looking for snakes.
So Charles, as you kind of said earlier,
this has not all been very well received
by the scientific community.
I get to say the least.
They tend to think of it as Hocom,
the fact that he doesn't publish peer-reviewed papers
anymore and then said,
writes books directly to the public,
the fact that they claim that his stuff
isn't falsifiable.
But if you read his explanations and descriptions,
he's like, no, actually, this all is falsifiable.
And I try to run experiments all the time.
Sometimes it comes back with positive results.
But they generally don't like the stuff that he's saying.
And in particular, there was one guy
who, looking back, made Rupert Sheldrake's career.
And his name was Sir John Maddox.
And at the time that, what it was
that the science, the new science of life.
Yeah, a new science of life.
When that book came out, Sir John Maddox
happened to be the editor of the journal Nature.
Nature and science are the two most prestigious
scientific peer-reviewed publications in the entire world.
He's knighted for God's sakes.
And this guy, right, was the editor of that.
And he got his hands on the science of a new science of life
and wrote, not just a book review,
an editorial about this book from the editors of Nature
claiming that it was an infuriating tract
and that it was the best candidate for burning
there has been for many years.
Yeah, also this, in a 1994 interview, he said,
Sheldrake is putting forward magic instead of science.
And that can be condemned in exactly the language
that the Pope used to condemn Galileo
for the same reason it is heresy.
Right.
So if you were curious about how Sir John Maddox
felt about the dogma of science,
the fact that he used the word heresy
kind of says it all, right?
And this was 13 years after that first review.
And it invoked the Pope.
Yeah, he was doubling down on this.
And he didn't mention that it turned out
Galileo was right, even though he positioned himself
in science and the Pope position in this one.
Burn.
The fact is, he used the word burning.
He and his defenders later on will say like,
no, if you read the whole thing at the end,
he says, no, we shouldn't be burning books,
but he does say that there's,
hadn't been a better candidate for burning.
But if we were to burn books,
this would be the first one on the pile.
Right.
And so that, from that point on,
Rupert Sheldrake's publishers are like,
we'll be using that on the dust jacket
of every edition of this from now on.
And it made his career.
He went from somebody who might have never been anybody
to the premier heretic of science,
thanks to that dusty old crotch, Sir John Maddox.
Yeah, here's a dusty old crotch.
Yeah, I'm not a big fan of him.
Oh man.
Or anybody who suggests we should burn books.
So here's another quote from another professor of biology
at University College London, Lewis-Walpert.
Morphic resonance is rubbish.
It is unmitigated junk and a great insult
to the people who do real work in the field.
Yeah, and I'm sure we could spend the next 20 minutes
finding quotes like that about that book.
Yeah, and Sheldrake's response has always been,
I mean, he'll go back at people for sure,
but not in a sort of a poopy pants way.
Right.
He basically is like, and you know,
in any idea that doesn't conform to this religion
of science is denounced.
And he said, it's closed-minded,
it's a closed-minded system.
It goes against the nature of what science is,
which should be discovery and investigating hypotheses
and the fact that they're valid
until they're proven or disproven by experimenting.
So get off my back with your dusty crotch.
And in fact, some scientists in the field,
or a number of fields, have kind of come
to Sheldrake's defense.
Not so much that they've criticized John Maddox.
I get the impression that you don't criticize Sir John.
Unless you're Josh Clark.
Right, yeah.
Well, I'm not a scientist.
I've got no skin in this game,
but they came to Sheldrake's defense
and they said, okay, if you're saying these are falsifiable,
let's do some experimentation.
Let's take this to the point you're putting it at
and let's apply the scientific method to this.
Yeah, here, smoke this hash.
Right, no, Sheldrake said that to them,
like, okay, for the data to make sense,
you got to smoke this first.
That's right.
And they went, oh, okay.
That explains everything.
Right, now I got it.
But that's why everybody likes to hang out with him.
Cause he's got the good stuff.
Right.
All right, so should we talk about
a little bit about what he claims
and what he's tried to prove?
Yeah, because, so again, he's run these experiments,
but there have been, I just wanna say,
there have been a few people who've come up
and been like, you know, that was BS
what Sir John said, we shouldn't be burning books.
I'm going to extend an olive branch
on behalf of the scientific community
and we're going to test some of these experiments.
Yeah, so he drilled down in a few different areas
that we're gonna talk about.
One is the one I talked about
about humans being stared at.
The other is the dogs anticipating their owners return.
Right, basically human dog telepathy.
Right.
And then, yeah, boy, as soon as that word telepathy
is thrown out there, that's a science killer.
Yeah, and that's a, I mean, that's a big,
easy criticism of children's ideas
is that they include telepathy,
that the idea that we're tuned into this general body
of conscious knowledge that was accumulated
by all the living things that came before us
and that exists outside of our minds
and we can connect to it with our minds.
That's telepathy, there's no way to put it otherwise.
Well, in Psy in general,
which we should probably do a podcast on at some point.
Sure, I mean, we've been chipping away
at it little by little, but yeah, yeah.
And then the third one that he kind of drilled down on
was the idea that successive generations of lab rats
can solve their little puzzles and problems faster
and easier than generations before
that is because of morphic resonance.
Yeah, and there's been data.
He's either carried out experiments himself
or he's pointed out to publish data
before that have shown that.
I think back in the 30s,
there was a, I guess a biologist or a psychologist
who was training rats how to run a maze
and he found to his amazement
that rats of successive generations
over like 36 generations did better initially
on these mazes than their predecessors,
which would suggest, well, a lot of things,
but apparently he controlled for genetics and environment
and said, it's possible that this is somehow being passed
down from one generation to the next outside of genes.
Yeah, so let's talk about the dog thing
because we have dogs and we love to think
that our dogs are little people
and that they sit by the door waiting on us
and look out the window and are just sad until we get home.
Sure.
And so he did this experiment
and then later on did some more experiments with a partner,
which we'll talk about here in a sec,
but he found a lady, a British woman who, her name was Pam
and she had a dog named JT, J-A-Y-T-E-E.
Very weird.
And she said, hey, use me because I got this dog
who waits by the window before I come home,
no matter when I come home.
Right.
So it doesn't matter if I come home at five
or 10 o'clock at night or three in the afternoon,
this dog is by the window.
So I think there's some telepathy going on
and Sheldrake said, well, step right up
and let's see what's going on here.
Yeah, and it wasn't just that her dog sits by the window
the whole time she's gone,
it's that people had noticed
that her dog would suddenly sit up, go to the window
and then within a few 10 minutes
or something like that, Pam would come home.
Right, and started singing True Colors by Cindy Lauper.
Right.
And she would come home at different times of the day,
like this is a pretty, it was a remarkable thing.
So apparently over a hundred different tests,
Sheldrake found that 84 out of a hundred times,
this dog accurately predicted when Pam was coming home.
And Sheldrake's whole hypothesis.
Within 11 seconds, well, people should understand
not that this dog hears the car pull in,
within 11 seconds of her leaving to go home.
Right, leaving her office, I think, miles away.
Exactly.
And again, this is at different times of day,
they apparently experimented so that she would come home
in different kinds of cars, including taxis,
so that the dog couldn't somehow like hear
this particular calm of Pam's motor or something like that.
But he controlled for a lot of stuff.
And this is something you gotta understand
about Rupert Sheldrake.
He carries out scientific experiments,
like under the scientific method.
What people disagree with is his interpretation
of the data typically.
But he controlled for all this different stuff
and he found that 84 times out of a hundred,
JT accurately predicted roughly when Pam was gonna come home
by getting up and going to the window to wait for her, right?
Yeah, I think that first one though
was not quite so scientific.
Wasn't that the deal?
This is why they redid it?
No, no, they didn't redo it because it wasn't scientific.
They redid it because one of his greatest critics,
Richard Wiseman, who is a, I think a psychologist,
but also like a professional skeptic,
said, this is BS, but let me carry out,
let me replicate your experiment
and see if I get the same results.
Oh, okay.
Cause he had an Austrian documentary crew
and they said that the test wasn't scientific.
Oh, I'm sorry.
Right.
Like they're filming what they did was not scientific,
but he had already previously carried out.
In private that no one saw.
Yeah, but I mean, you know,
the scientists do that all the time.
I don't think that he's,
I don't think he has been accused
of fudging his methodology.
I think he's just roundly accused of cherry picking data
or misinterpreting the data
or interpreting the data to suit his needs,
that kind of stuff.
But I don't get the impression
that a lot of people are like this data
on its, at its core is Hockham.
All right.
Well, Wiseman, like you said, noted skeptic,
professional poo pooer of things, experimental psychologist.
He comes in and says, all right, let's do this together.
He said, all right, this dog did not,
in four different occasions or four different experiments,
this dog failed.
Right.
And this dog is going to the window a lot.
He's a window hanger outer.
Yeah.
This dog loves that window.
And so they said, all right,
let's rule out some of these false positives.
And let's say, let's define what the real signal would be.
That's if this dog stays at the window for two minutes,
not just pops up to see if it's raining
or to sing a stanza of true colors,
but really sits there for two minutes.
And then let's see what happens.
They did that.
And Wiseman said, all right,
and we gotta also say it's gotta be within 10 minutes
of her leaving from home, not 11 minutes.
Right.
One minute.
And in all four of these experiments,
this dog gives a signal before that 10 minute period,
before she even started for home.
So Wiseman said, failed.
So if you read Sheldrake's rebuttal to Wiseman's findings,
and Wiseman ran around not just saying failed,
he like gave, I think, four different talks
about this experiment and how like it was,
it didn't amount to anything.
And so Sheldrake responded to it and he was like,
well, this two minute duration
was an arbitrary signal that you came up with.
That wasn't part of my original methodology.
And then also, and I think all four of those experiments
are maybe all four.
So the dog went to the window early
and then afterward, if he went to the window again,
which apparently he did to wait for Pam,
that was thrown out
because he'd already gone to the window before.
He's like, well, I never said the dog
only went to the window when Pam was coming home.
I just said he would go to the window to wait for Pam
within some certain timeframe of her leaving.
And apparently the dog continued to do this,
but it wasn't included in these tests
because he had already gone to the window.
So it's really detailed and you can read it yourself
if you want to, but he has a good explanation
for why Wiseman's interpretation of the data was,
or his methodology was flawed.
But it's all very civil, like you were saying before.
He's not like, Wiseman's a moron who couldn't do science
if it sat on him and caused him to stop respirating
or anything like that.
Yeah, and I think it's the other thing,
because the thing that Wiseman poo pooed
was the fact that the dog started this behavior
before she started for home.
And Sheldrake was like, hey,
I think that further proves it actually,
because I think that Pam is sending signals
before she starts for home that she doesn't even realize
like maybe she gets her coat
and goes to the restroom for a few minutes or something even.
Well, she was with-
And that's the beginning of the going home process.
Yeah, or she was with Wiseman's assistant.
And Wiseman's assistant was the one
who knew what time they were going home.
So he said maybe Pam was picking up on the guy
looking at his watch or something like that
and knew when she was gonna go home anyway.
There's a lot of, he has a lot of explanations for it.
It's very interesting to kind of read the back and forth.
But Wiseman won that one
because everyone wanted Wiseman to win that one.
And I think that's kind of par for the course for Sheldrake.
He's like, well, no, here's all these other explanations
for this interpretation and people just kind of ignore it
unless you want to believe what Sheldrake has to say.
That's right.
If you don't want to believe what Sheldrake has to say,
people like Wiseman and other skeptics provide,
here we carried out this experiment
and now this is disproven, right?
Another guy who did that is a really big critic of Sheldrake.
His name is Steven Rose.
I think he's a, no, I'm sorry, he's, yeah,
he's a biologist and a neuroscientist, Steven Rose.
And he carried out another experiment
about how chicks might be able to learn
kind of like that lab rat experiment,
how they successive generations learned how to do a maze.
Well, they did this with chicks
and they did this experiment together
and they had different interpretations of the data
and it went back and forth
in different journals or whatever.
But the fact is there are scientists out there,
skeptics who are critics of Sheldrake
and his ideas and methods,
but still scientists that are willing to engage his ideas.
And I think that that's healthy,
even if they are coming at it from the standpoint
like this is bunk, this is Hockham,
they're still willing to go through with these experiments
and I respect that.
That's right.
You want to take another break?
Yes.
All right, Chuck.
We're going to take another break everybody
in case you didn't hear.
["Hey Dude", by David Lashor and Christine Taylor plays in the background."
On the podcast, Hey Dude, the 90s called David Lashor
and Christine Taylor, stars of the cult classic show, Hey Dude,
bring you back to the days of slip dresses and choker necklaces.
We're going to use Hey Dude as our jumping off point,
but we are going to unpack and dive back into the decade of the 90s.
We lived it and now we're calling on all of our friends
to come back and relive it.
It's a podcast packed with interviews,
co-stars, friends and non-stop references to the best decade ever.
Do you remember going to Blockbuster?
Do you remember Nintendo 64?
Do you remember getting frosted tips?
Was that a cereal?
No, it was hair.
Do you remember AOL Instant Messenger
and the dial-up sound like poltergeist?
So leave a code on your best friend's beeper
because you'll want to be there when the nostalgia starts flowing.
Each episode will rival the feeling of taking out the cartridge
from your Game Boy, blowing on it and popping it back in
as we take you back to the 90s.
Listen to Hey Dude, the 90s called on the iHeart radio app,
Apple Podcasts, or wherever you get your podcasts.
Hey, I'm Lance Bass, host of the new iHeart podcast,
Frosted Tips with Lance Bass.
The hardest thing can be knowing who to turn to
when questions arise or times get tough
or you're at the end of the road.
Ah, okay, I see what you're doing.
Do you ever think to yourself,
what advice would Lance Bass and my favorite boy bands
give me in this situation?
If you do, you've come to the right place
because I'm here to help.
This, I promise you.
Oh, God.
Seriously, I swear.
And you won't have to send an SOS
because I'll be there for you.
Oh, man.
And so my husband, Michael.
Um, hey, that's me.
Yep, we know that, Michael.
And a different hot, sexy teen crush boy bander
each week to guide you through life, step by step.
Oh, not another one.
Kids, relationships, life in general can get messy.
You may be thinking, this is the story of my life.
Oh, just stop now.
If so, tell everybody, yeah, everybody
about my new podcast and make sure to listen
so we'll never, ever have to say bye, bye, bye.
Listen to Frosted Tips with Lance Bass
on the iHeart Radio App, Apple Podcast,
or wherever you listen to podcasts.
Cha, cha, cha, cha, cha, cha, cha, cha, cha, cha, cha, cha,
cha, cha, cha, cha, cha.
All right, so Rose, Steven Rose, and this quote kind of really
puts the nail on the head of the critics of Sheldrake.
And this actually is one that spoke to me
because it's not an attack on Sheldrake.
It's more of a sympathetic view, which is this.
Sheldrake is so committed to his hypotheses
that it is very hard to envisage the circumstances in which
he would accept its disconfirmation.
So it's a very sweet way of saying this guy really, really
believes this stuff so much that I
don't think he is able to look at the data in a level-headed,
unbiased way.
Right, and it says it all.
I mean, that's tough to, it's a very cutting criticism
because how do you show that's not true, other than to say,
Sheldrake just said no.
Well, no, these are wrong.
You have to admit that your hypothesis is wrong
to get around that.
And then once you've done that, you've just lost anyway.
So it's a tough, it's a very shrewd criticism.
Yeah, so Sheldrake over the years has,
he's written a lot of books.
He has been accused of, he's been accused by some of like,
hey, this guy is just out there writing books to make money.
And has sort of made himself the superstar of the alt side
of science.
That seems to be the biggest explanation
for why he's doing what he's doing,
that he found an easier and quicker path to fame
and recognition and probably money writing these books
about his own made up ideas rather than writing,
you know, academic papers like everybody else.
Right, and on Sheldrake's side, he's like, listen,
this, what he calls the default worldview of science
and these dogmas, he said they should be
sort of pushed back against.
And what about?
Questioned.
Yeah, questioned.
What about the big bang?
He's like, everyone thinks they have it all figured out
and all laws in the universe are constant.
Well, except for the big bang, and then, you know,
we can't fully explain that.
And that, there's another great quote from a philosopher
named Terence McKenna.
I love this quote.
Is, give us one free miracle and we'll explain the rest.
Yeah.
And you know, when it comes to things like the big bang,
that kind of holds true.
Yeah, specifically with the big bang,
I think is what he's talking about,
that if you can just allow for there to have been
nothing that came before and all of a sudden,
all the matter and energy in the universe suddenly existed,
then we can pretty much explain all other physics
from that point on or we can use that.
And that's the big question is what happened
right before the big bang.
Yeah, yeah, exactly.
But we also have other questions about the universe,
like is it inflating, is there gonna be a big bang
or is there gonna be a big crunch or we have a lot
of questions and a lot of misunderstanding about it too.
But we, physics needs the big bang to have happened
the way that we think it might have happened.
But even still, the way we think it might have happened
doesn't follow the physical laws as we understand them.
And so, Sheldrake and others point to that one
and they're like, come on guys, like this is just one
of several examples of science just saying,
this is the way it is, even though we don't fully
understand it or the data we're getting suggests otherwise.
Right, and oh man, how can I say this in a way
that's not like controversial?
There isn't one.
It's not the same as when, let's say a creationist saying,
well, you can't really explain the big bang.
So, it's all magic and that's okay.
It's not along those same lines or something different
about what Sheldrake is saying.
I think what he's saying is, yeah, in some cases,
he's like, we don't understand this.
So, here's my interpretation.
I think in his more recent book, The Science Delusion,
which came out in 2012 in the US is called Science Set Free.
He's saying, here are some essential dogmatic beliefs
of science that are worth challenging.
And that if we don't challenge them,
we might end up going down this wrong path
of scientific inquiry and we need to be a little more free
to differing ideas because we don't understand these things
like everybody generally believes we do.
Yeah, I think he kind of, and the book title itself,
you said in America, Science Set Free,
that kind of encapsulates, I think he sees himself
as some kind of emancipator of science,
rather than a cuckoo who believes in telepathy.
Right.
Even though he kind of, I mean, that stuff,
and Dave Ruse helped us with this research.
Yeah, he did a great job.
He did, and he points out, and he's right,
morphic resonance sounds very strange and weird.
Yeah.
And it also sounds like something that would sell a book.
Right, right.
But to throw him in there, I think it's,
Rose said he's basically no better
than someone who endorses crop circles and creationism.
Right, that is pseudoscience.
Yeah, and I don't think that's necessarily fair.
No, and even so, if you take away things
like morphic resonance and things like,
well, morphic resonance basically,
if you take that stuff away and just look at him
as like a challenger to scientific dogma,
you can appreciate him on that level as well.
Like you can peel back different layers of this guy
and appreciate different parts
and also disagree with different parts.
But so in his most recent one, the science solution,
he basically says, here are some things
that science believes that we shouldn't necessarily believe
like that matters unconscious.
And there are people out there,
including physicists who are like,
if consciousness were just a property of all matter
and that the more matter you put together,
the more sophisticated consciousness you got,
that would explain a lot of stuff,
including human consciousness.
That's just an emergent property of all these particles
that came together to form human beings.
But currently, scientific establishment says,
no, matters unconscious, that's just our understanding of it.
That's the way it is.
Although that is being challenged
by more people than just Rupert Sheldrake.
And then there was another one, he gave a TED Talk,
a TEDx Whitechapel Talk.
That I think was later banned, right?
Or not banned, but removed?
They took it from their YouTube channel
and then inserted it into a blog post.
You can still see it, but they put it in a blog post
because their science advisors have been like,
this is pretty heretical
and I don't think you should just be presenting it.
Like it's just, you need to couch it in some language.
So they did and they put it in a blog post,
but you can still see it.
It's not like they just took it down all together.
But it's a really interesting talk.
It's only like 20 minutes long.
But in it, he makes a really good case
about how the laws of nature like the gravitational constant
or the speed of light aren't actually constant.
And physics needs those things to be constant
for it to do its inquiries,
for it to do its formulae and equations
for the current theories to work.
And he's saying, no, there's been periods in history
where we've measured these things
and gotten different measurements.
And that during that same period,
all these different scientists around the world
were getting roughly the same different measurements
from what we thought it was before.
How do you explain that?
And I think that point is really important
because if something like the speed of light does change
and understanding that it does and how it does
could give us an even greater understanding of physics.
And that right there, I think is the greatest role
that Rupert Sheldrake plays is to say,
no, stop looking at it through this lens.
This lens is possibly incorrect at the very least.
Don't burn all your old stuff, don't throw it away,
but just step to the side and approach it
from a different way just to see if that's the truth.
And if it is the truth,
then we'll have a greater understanding
of how things actually work.
Yeah, because the unexplainable are only unexplainable
until they can be explained.
You're right.
And at various points throughout history,
there were a lot of claims
that things were unexplainable until they figured it out.
Yep.
And again, we're not touting pseudoscience here
because we have a pretty good track record
of roundly siding on the side of science.
Yes, and expertise too.
Like we both have a tremendous amount of respect
for expertise and people who go study things
for years and years and years and apply themselves
to that understanding that one thing,
that's an expert and they should generally be listened to.
That's right.
Aldrake has proven himself out enough
to be listened to, I think.
He's not alien's man, whoever that guy was.
Yeah.
So yeah, I mean, make up your own mind.
Go read about him.
Go read both sides about him.
If you're interested in this, don't just listen to us.
Like he's definitely one of those people
you should make your own mind up.
And if you disagree, great.
If you agree, fantastic.
We're just kind of,
we just kind of admire thinkers like that.
That's right.
Are you got anything else?
I got nothing else.
Rupert Sheldrake, that was it.
If you want to know more about him, go read.
I go, go read his books, go do what you want.
And in the meantime, since I said go do what you want,
it's time for Listener Mail.
Yeah, this one is a little long,
but this was a firsthand account from the Iowa caucus.
So I thought it bared reading.
This is from Lauren, a student at University of Iowa,
participated in her first Iowa caucus.
She said I went.
And last.
Well, she did say that.
I went because it's such a big deal here.
And since I'm graduating,
I thought it might be my only chance.
However, this week's events,
it's looking like it may be the last time anyone
is going to participate.
There were several logistical issues in my opinion
that led to issues where I was participating.
My caucus location was downtown Iowa City
at the Englert Theater.
My roommate and I arrived about 45 minutes before
to make sure we could be in the door before seven
since we had been told if you weren't in the door,
then TS for you.
Two lines for the caucus wrapped around the block.
One for people who registered in the correct precinct
and one for people who needed to change their registration
to the correct precinct.
There were over 700 people in our caucus location alone,
far more than the Democratic Party of Iowa had expected.
Since my caucus location was a theater,
it was almost impossible to distinguish
where different candidates were in the room.
The Bernie and Warren groups were so large
they had locations on the floor
and had to have satellite spots in the balcony.
When it came time to do the head count,
the overcrowded space led to issues tallying people.
All of the campaign volunteers had reported numbers.
Caucus Delegate informed us that the total number
of people under each candidate was about 50
under the amount of total people checked in.
So they assumed that 50 people had chosen to leave
before the votes were all tallyed,
but the campaign volunteers demanded a recount.
All in all, it took about two and a half hours
to get through the first round of caucusing
to find out which candidates would be viable.
My prediction is that because Iowans are so passionate
about their caucus system,
they'll probably happen again next election cycle.
But since their faux importance is brought
to public consciousness this year,
they'll eventually die away to a primary system soon.
That is from Lauren Cheshire.
Lauren, that was a great account.
You're basically like the Hunter Thompson correspondent
of Stuff You Should Know.
We appreciate that big time.
That's right.
She took some AMO nitrates after that.
Right.
The name of the subject line of the email
was the Iowa caucus is depraved and decadent.
Very nice.
So if you want to get in touch with us
to let us know something that's going on
in your neck of the woods, well, we want to hear about it.
You can go on to StuffYouShouldKnow.com.
Don't know why you would want to these days.
Instead, just send us an email.
Wrap it up, spank it on the bottom
and send it off to StuffPodcast at iHeartRadio.com.
Stuff You Should Know is a production
of iHeartRadio's How Stuff Works.
For more podcasts from iHeartRadio,
visit the iHeartRadio app.
Apple podcasts are wherever you listen
to your favorite shows.
On the podcast, Hey Dude, the 90s called,
David Lasher and Christine Taylor,
stars of the cult classic show, Hey Dude,
bring you back to the days of slip dresses
and choker necklaces.
We're gonna use Hey Dude as our jumping off point,
but we are going to unpack and dive back
into the decade of the 90s.
We lived it and now we're calling on all of our friends
to come back and relive it.
Listen to Hey Dude, the 90s called
on the iHeartRadio app, Apple podcasts
or wherever you get your podcasts.
Hey, I'm Lance Bass, host of the new iHeart podcast,
Frosted Tips with Lance Bass.
Do you ever think to yourself,
what advice would Lance Bass
and my favorite boy bands give me in this situation?
If you do, you've come to the right place
because I'm here to help.
And a different hot, sexy teen crush boy bander
each week to guide you through life.
Tell everybody, yeah, everybody,
about my new podcast and make sure to listen
so we'll never, ever have to say bye, bye, bye.
Listen to Frosted Tips with Lance Bass
on the iHeartRadio app, Apple podcast
or wherever you listen to podcasts.