Tetragrammaton with Rick Rubin - David Eagleman

Episode Date: January 8, 2025

David Eagleman is a neuroscientist at Stanford University and a bestselling author. His neuroscience research spans the areas of brain plasticity, sensory substitution, human perception, synesthesia, ...and beyond. As the co-director of the Center for Science & Law, he works at the intersection of neuroscience and the legal system, using new discoveries in neuroscience to inform lawmaking. Eagleman also applies his research findings through ventures including Neosensory, a company he co-founded to develop sensory substitution devices. Known for making complex science accessible, he is the writer and presenter of the Emmy-nominated PBS series The Brain with David Eagleman, and he now shares valuable insights on his podcast, Inner Cosmos. A prolific author, his books include Incognito, Sum, and his latest release, Livewired. David Eagleman’s upcoming events can be found here. ------ Thank you to the sponsors that fuel our podcast and our team: LMNT Electrolytes https://drinklmnt.com/tetra Use code 'TETRA' ------ Athletic Nicotine https://www.athleticnicotine.com/tetra Use code 'TETRA' ------ Squarespace https://squarespace.com/tetra Use code 'TETRA' ------ Vivo Barefoot http://vivobarefoot.com/tetra Use code 'TETRA25' ------ Sign up to receive Tetragrammaton Transmissions https://www.tetragrammaton.com/join-newsletter

Transcript
Discussion (0)
Starting point is 00:00:00 Tetrachromatism. Fundamentally, your brain is where all the action is happening. It's the densest representation of you in the body. And the reason we know this is because if you were to damage part of one of your other organs like your heart or your lung or your spleen or your kidney, you know, you might have some problems as a result of it, but it can get fixed. And in fact, the heart can get replaced entirely.
Starting point is 00:00:49 You can put in an artificial heart, you know, and they have these pulseless hearts that just are machines that run the blood through. You don't even have a pulse anymore. And yet you're the same person. But if you damage even a very tiny chunk of brain tissue, that changes who you are. That changes your hopes and dreams and aspirations and your capabilities and your ability
Starting point is 00:01:11 to recognize animals or see colors or understand mirrors or listen to music or hundreds of other things that we see in the clinics every day. And that's how we know that the brain is the important dense three pound representation of you much more than the other organs. So the way I think about it is the brain is sort of like the city center. And yes, there's this communication to the outlying areas,
Starting point is 00:01:38 but the city center is sort of where all the action is happening. There are stories of people who have heart transplants and then their emotional lives change. There are stories like that and I interpret them to be romantic stories. And again, the reason we know that is because thousands of people get artificial hearts or heart transplants every single year and they're the same person. Obviously, going through a major surgery like that can change you and make you appreciate life in a different way.
Starting point is 00:02:08 So you might come out feeling a little different on the other side, but not because it's a different heart. Explain your theory on why we dream. Okay. So it turns out the brain has to take care of all these different senses like vision and touch and hearing and so on. And it does that by sharing its real estate among these. But what's interesting is that the whole system is incredibly fluid and flexible, much more than people have ever thought. My last book, LiveWired, is all about this because I'm fascinated by how fluid the whole
Starting point is 00:02:40 system is. For example, if you were to go blind, that area that we think of is the visual part of your brain, the visual cortex. That gets taken over by hearing, by touch, by other things like that. Now, the surprise just in the last decade has been how rapidly these kinds of changes can happen. And some colleagues of mine at Harvard did an experiment where they took normally sighted people and they blindfolded them tightly and they put them in the scanner.
Starting point is 00:03:11 And then they measured things like, you know, if touch you on the hand or you hear a note of violin or something and what's going on in the brain. Well, it turns out that after an hour, they started seeing activity in the visual parts of the brain? Well, it turns out that after an hour, they started seeing activity in the visual parts of the brain. So in other words, the person is blindfolded and not seeing anything, but now hearing and touch is starting to encroach on the visual part of the brain. And it happens much more
Starting point is 00:03:38 quickly than anyone thought was possible. And I realized with a student of mine that was possible and I realized with a student of mine that the visual system faces a problem that is unique to it among sensory systems, which is that our planet rotates into darkness, for half the time, every 24 hours we're back in the dark, and in the dark you can still hear and touch and taste and smell, but you can't see anymore. And so that puts the visual system at a disadvantage compared to the other sensory systems. And so what I realized was the visual system is in danger of getting taken over, of getting its territory encroached upon every single night. And the way that the brain fights back against that is with dreaming, which is just blasting
Starting point is 00:04:27 random activity into the visual system every 90 minutes. It just sends in random volleys of activity. And because we are visual creatures, we see and because we're storytelling creatures, we impose a narrative on top of this. But fundamentally, it's about defending the visual system against takeover during the nighttime. And by the way, this happens in all animal species. To the best we can measure, all animals dream. And it turns out that we were able to demonstrate
Starting point is 00:04:59 with our science that the amount you dream has to do with how fluid your brain is, what's called brain plasticity. So if you have a brain like Homo sapiens, where the whole thing is very fluid and flexible, then you do a lot of dreaming. But if you're an animal that drops into the world and you're essentially pre-programmed, you have much less dreaming. But that is why we dream to prevent takeover of the visual system.
Starting point is 00:05:26 Since the brain essentially rewires itself all the time, are there things we can do purposefully to rewire the brain in a way to make it more advantageous to ourselves? Yes, yeah. So this is one of the key things is, we as humans think at all different levels, including long-term thinking and what
Starting point is 00:05:45 kind of person would I like to be. And essentially, any time we take on a task like, hey, I really want to become good at playing the piano or playing chess or swimming or whatever the thing is, that's what we're doing is we're saying, all right, I'm going to do all the hard work, all the brain plasticity hard work and set my sights on this sort of goal. The way we do that, of course, is by tying things into reward. There's a lot of reasons something can be rewarding,
Starting point is 00:06:14 and it can be rewarding at all different timescales. Like, oh, maybe I get attention from this person over here, or maybe I feel great about myself in this situation, or maybe my mother calls me and says, "'Wow, what a nice job you did,' or whatever the thing is. So there's a million reasons that we can motivate ourselves.
Starting point is 00:06:31 But yes, as the Spanish neuroscientist Ramon y Cajal, who won the Nobel Prize a century ago, as he put it, each of us is the sculptor of our own brains. And this is the really interesting thing about being a species that can think into the future and imagine future versions of ourselves and what we might want them to look like and then what steps we have to take to get ourselves there.
Starting point is 00:06:54 When did you go from just being a professor to doing outward facing stuff, communicating with the world instead of just the classroom? Yeah. Well, you know, as an undergraduate, I studied lots of science. That's what I was doing, but I also studied British and American literature. And I, from the time I was a little kid,
Starting point is 00:07:15 I always wanted to be a writer. And actually when I was young, I watched Carl Sagan's Cosmos and it had an enormous influence on me. And in fact, when I was growing up, my parents, instead of saying something like, oh, he's gonna be the next president, they would say, oh, he's gonna be the next Carl Sagan.
Starting point is 00:07:31 So from the time I was a kid, that was always my sort of the light on the hill for me was science communication. My father was a physician, my mother was a biology teacher. So science was always sort of part of the background radiation in the house. and I loved that, but I also really loved communicating it clearly. And so as I became a scientist, I sort of always knew that was part of what was going to be going on. I wrote my first book, which was a book of fiction called SUM, S-U-M. I wrote that starting in my postdoctoral fellowship and published that when I was an assistant
Starting point is 00:08:09 professor. You know, that was a type of public outreach, although that was fiction, as I mentioned. But then I published my first book of neuroscience called Incognito when I was still, yeah, a young assistant professor. What's interesting is that I think things have changed nowadays in terms of public communication of science, but back when I was doing that, I got a lot of warnings from older professors who told me, oh, you better not do this now. You better wait until you're tenured before you publish because people within the academic
Starting point is 00:08:39 world will think you're not being serious enough if you're talking to the public about things. Happily, I think that time is maturing. So we all have a deeper appreciation for that. But for me, I need to say my audience when I write is always crystal clear, which is I know that I'm writing for my 18 year old self. In other words, I'm writing to somebody who I know well, who loves big ideas and is amazed by them and brings tears to his eyes or her eyes,
Starting point is 00:09:15 but doesn't happen to know this particular fact or that story or that framework for thinking about things. And so I get to be the lucky one to tell that kid something that, that I've figured out or I've found in the literature that, uh, that maybe they don't know that I know will blow their hair back. How was it different writing fiction versus writing science? You know, I've thought about this a lot because I do both. I think they're kind of the same thing, which is to say,
Starting point is 00:09:43 literature and science are both ways of trying to understand the world. And the only difference is that in science, there's a very particular pathway, a model that we have for moving forward. You know, you set up experiments and you have to be replicable. And you know, you see subjected to the scientific method. Whereas in literature, you can make up a what if, and then you can just go explore it. It doesn't matter if it's true or not true. But other than that, it's the same thing. It's saying, hey, what if the framework were this?
Starting point is 00:10:17 What if the world was made of that? And it's a terrific way to break ourselves out of the internal models that we have. Essentially, your brain is locked in silence and darkness, and it's always just trying to figure out what's going on in the world out there. And the fact is we all have an internal model of what's going on out there in the world, and that has to do with a lifetime of experience teaching you, okay, this is what the world is made out of. And what both good literature and good science do
Starting point is 00:10:47 is they say, hey, what if that's not true? What if there's this other model? What is this other way of looking at it? And that's what's so special about both is they get to take you out of your daily life into a different world. And in science, we make progress by leaping to these different worlds
Starting point is 00:11:04 and seeing if we can build a leaping to these different worlds and seeing if we can build a bridge there and if it actually is true, but in literature we can just explore those islands. How different are our internal models? So different. So this has actually been one of the driving forces in my research, my whole career, is I'm absolutely fascinated by how different life is between one person's head and another person's head.
Starting point is 00:11:30 So I'll just give you a couple of quick examples. So there's something called synesthesia, where it's a blending of the senses. So somebody might look at letters on the page and it triggers a color experience for them. So A is red and B is yellow and C is blue and so on. Different for each synesthesia. About 3% of the population has this. But the point is it's not a disease or disorder. It's just an alternative way that some people
Starting point is 00:11:53 see the world. There are many different flavors of synesthesia. You might taste something and it puts a feeling on your fingertips. You might hear something, it causes a visual. Any kind of cross blending you can imagine, we found examples of that. But the point is, it's just one way in which, this person is different from that person, is different from that person, having a pretty different experience on the inside. Or take something more close to home,
Starting point is 00:12:18 like colorblindness, right? There's a fraction of people who are colorblind and others who aren't. People who are colorblind often don't even figure that out until much later in life. Just like synesthetes have no idea that other people are seeing the world differently than they are.
Starting point is 00:12:32 I've been spending a lot of time in the last years studying things like how clearly we have visual imagery. So if I ask you to imagine something like an ant crawling on a red and white tablecloth towards a jar of purple jelly, you might see that like a movie inside your head. And I don't see anything at all. It's just conceptual to me. I don't see any picture at all.
Starting point is 00:12:57 And that's the spectrum from what's called hyperphantasia to aphantasia. And it turns out that everybody is spread out somewhere on that spectrum. There's no problem. It's not as though one end of the spectrum is a disease state or pathological or whatever. It's just people have different internal experiences. My wife and I, for example, she has a very loud internal radio, as she calls it, which is she's always hearing her internal voice and having conversations with herself, and that will often drown things out.
Starting point is 00:13:28 I happen to have no internal radio at all. And so I'm just really interested in how different it is from head to head. I also, for 15 years now, I've been running a national nonprofit called the Center for Science and Law, which is about where neuroscience intersects the legal system. And one of the fascinating things to me is again, how different people can be on the inside because lots of people, millions of people in this country commit crimes, but for all kinds of reasons.
Starting point is 00:14:01 And this doesn't give anybody an excuse, but I'm just saying it's very different on the inside. Some people do it because they're tweaked out on drugs. Some people have mental illness like a psychosis. Some people are psychopaths meaning they don't care about you and what's going on with you and what your feelings are if they you know hurt you badly. Some people are doing because they're impoverished and they you know they need to put food on the table for their family. I mean there's just just, there's a million reasons. And so when a, when a brain shows up in front of the judge's bench, the important part is to figure out what kind of reality is this person having and what does that tell us about whether he is likely
Starting point is 00:14:37 to commit future crimes or unlikely? And how can we root him through the legal system instead of imagining that incarceration is the one size fits all solution? It's definitely appropriate for some scenarios, but what other possibilities do we have there? Tell me something you believe now that you didn't believe when you were young. That is the thing actually. When I was young, I thought that everyone's internal life was the same. And so, for example, my father mentioned as a psychiatrist and he was involved
Starting point is 00:15:10 in all of the big mass murder cases in the Southwest. He was a psychiatrist for those. And so I was with him, I was just a kid, I don't know, maybe 10 years old, I was with him at some party with some other adults. And someone said to my father, oh, this guy who had just murdered several people, he shouldn't get the death penalty
Starting point is 00:15:30 because surely he feels terrible for what he's done. And my father, I remember sort of the surprise on his face when the guy said that, my father said, no, that's not at all how it goes. My father had deposed this guy and spent hours and hours talking to him. He said, no, that's not at all how it goes. My father had deposed this guy and spent hours and hours talking to him. He said, no, this guy, when he thinks about going out to murder somebody, he describes it as the level of excitement that he had as a child on the night before Christmas.
Starting point is 00:15:57 You can't imagine what it is to be this guy, William Wayne Gilbert, and presumably he can't imagine what it is to be you. The lesson of psychiatry is that you can't actually stick yourself in other people's shoes. And as a child, I resisted that. I didn't believe my thought. Surely you can put yourself in other people's shoes. So that is the thing that as a child,
Starting point is 00:16:21 I thought that I don't believe anymore. That is the thing that as a child I thought that I don't believe anymore. Element Electrolytes. Have you ever felt dehydrated after an intense workout or a long day in the sun? Do you want to maximize your endurance and feel your best? Add Element Electrolytes to your daily routine. Perform better and sleep deeper. Improve your cognitive function. Experience an increase in steady energy with fewer headaches and fewer muscle cramps. Element electrolytes.
Starting point is 00:17:11 Drink it in the sauna. Refreshing flavors include grapefruit, citrus, watermelon, and chocolate salt. Formulated with the perfect balance of sodium, potassium, and magnesium to keep you hydrated and energized throughout the day. These minerals help conduct the electricity that powers your nervous system so you can perform at your very best. Element electrolytes are sugar-free, keto-friendly, and great tasting. Minerals are the stuff of life. So visit drinklmnt.com slash tetra, and stay salty with Element Electrolyte.
Starting point is 00:17:56 LMNT. How much do our memories make up who we are? I would say almost entirely. I mean there are other things too at a neighborhood level, a cultural level, a moment in time era level. But who you are locally is all about your memories, who you believe you are, the kind of person that you believe has done X, Y, Z and knows X, Y, Z and has these friends
Starting point is 00:18:29 and these parents and these neighbors and so on. So the scary part about that statement is that our memories are terrible. I mean, they're famously and empirically terrible. You can study this in the laboratory as people have for decades. And memory is a myth-making machine empirically terrible. You can study this in the laboratory, as people have for decades, and memory is a myth-making machine, and we're constantly reinventing our past to keep it consistent with who we think we are. And so the fact that our identity is pinned down to that is
Starting point is 00:18:59 quite fascinating. It makes us these sort of weird creatures in time. And one of the short stories that I wrote in my book, Some, was this story about how in the afterlife, God couldn't figure out what age he should make people live in the afterlife because people are so different at different ages. So he splits you up like a prism into all of your different ages. So there's your eight-year-old self and your 30-year-old self and your 60-year-old self. But it turns out that pretty quickly,
Starting point is 00:19:30 you know, the eight-year-olds are hanging out with each other and all the teenagers are hanging out with each other and they don't want to talk with the 60-year-olds and so on. And this is all just to demonstrate this issue about how different we are in time, even though we have the illusion of continuity. We always feel like, oh yeah, I'm same person I was. And by the way, we can often look back and say,
Starting point is 00:19:53 oh yeah, I really have changed from that time point to here. But when people look forward, they're especially terrible at imagining change. In other words, you think you're essentially, you've arrived and you, Rick, will be the same Rick as in ten years From now even though of course you you won't be yeah considering how much the brain changes is there some part of us That's unchanging Unchanging no and the reason is I mean look your biology is turning over at
Starting point is 00:20:25 And the reason is, I mean, look, your biology is turning over at such a rapid rate, all the pieces and parts of all the cells in your body are changing out and getting reconstructed. Do you know the myth of the ship of Theseus? Does that ring a bell to you? It's this ancient Greek story about the ship of Theseus pulls into dock and it's there for many, many years and eventually they have to replace this floorboard and then they have to replace that plank and they have to do, okay. And over enough time, every single piece of the ship has been replaced.
Starting point is 00:20:50 So is it still the same ship of Theseus or is it a new ship? Well, that's the situation we're in. We have every single part of our bodies has been replaced over and over and over. It's estimated about every seven years, you're an absolutely new person than you were biologically.
Starting point is 00:21:07 And so obviously something is lasting across that, but it certainly is not unchanging simply because every moment of your life, you're having new experiences, your brain is changing, it's reconfiguring, you know, the expression, you can't step into the same river twice. It's both because the river has changed, but also you have changed. You're not the same person each time you go back there. Tell me about instinct. How does it work?
Starting point is 00:21:36 Yeah, so most of what you think and do and act and believe is generated unconsciously. You don't have access to it or awareness of it or even acquaintance with it. And so that's most of our lives actually is generated in this way. The stuff that we come to the table essentially pre-programmed with,
Starting point is 00:21:58 that's what we call instinct. So, you know, withdrawing from something, slithering in the grass, that kind of thing, we come pre-programmed with. Now, the interesting thing is that what Mother Nature discovered with us, Homo sapiens, is this trick of brain plasticity, in other words, making our brains flexible, to such a degree that we drop in the world with, I would say, fewer instincts than any one of our animal neighbors and massive flexibility
Starting point is 00:22:29 such that we absorb the world around us. We drop into the world with half-baked brains. So we're not a totally blank slate, we do have instincts. And in fact, you can do things like, you can stick out your tongue at a newborn baby and the baby will stick its tongue back out at you. And that's incredible because that requires a whole wiring to the visual system and then to the motor system
Starting point is 00:22:54 and seeing that you stuck your tongue out and sticking its tongue out and so on. So it's not that we're coming blank, but we are coming with a lot more blank space than anybody else. And this is what has allowed us to take over every corner of the planet to construct skyscrapers and compose symphonies and get off the planet to the moon
Starting point is 00:23:13 and all the amazing things that we're doing and having podcasts over the internet and so on. It's all because we dropped into the world with these half-baked brains. And we can, by the time we're some number of years old, learn all the big discoveries and things about what other humans have discovered for us, and then we springboard off the top of that.
Starting point is 00:23:33 Whereas, if you're an alligator, you drop into the world, and you're doing the same thing that alligators have done for millions of years. You're eating, mating, sleeping, and that's it. You're not absorbing anything new. So yes, we do have instincts, but what makes humans really special is all the stuff that we can absorb flexibly. Can we develop our instincts or are they all just inborn?
Starting point is 00:24:00 The way that I would define instincts, those are the things that are inborn. You can obviously practice all kinds of other unconscious things. The way that I would define instincts, those are the things that are inborn. You can obviously practice all kinds of other unconscious things. For example, you can become a great soccer player. So Rick, I don't know if you play soccer, but if you wanted to, if the motivation were there, if the relevance were there for you, you could become a great soccer player. Now the first day you go out there, it's all knees and elbows.
Starting point is 00:24:23 You have no idea what's going on. But after a year of playing, you're pretty good. You have a field sense. You recognize exactly what's going on and why and whatever. So now you might call that, wow, I've got a really good instinct for soccer playing. Technically it's not instinct. What it is, is unconscious learning. And it just comes back to this thing we were talking about earlier about being the sculptor of our own brains. Because if you set your mind to, I'm going to become a soccer expert, then yep, that's how you can develop that. Tell me about our conscious mind versus our unconscious mind.
Starting point is 00:24:53 So most of the action is happening unconsciously. The conscious brain, my book Incognito is about this. And what I said in it is that you can think of the conscious brain like being a broom closet in the mansion of the brain in the sense that most of what's going on has very little to do with it. Here's how I think about it. Think about like the CEO of a giant organization, like United Airlines or Amazon or something.
Starting point is 00:25:21 If you're the CEO of that, you can't possibly know all the mechanisms of the company. Like there's giant stuff happening and you don't know where the tires are coming from or the cafeteria food is coming from or what version of the software is loaded on people's computer. Like you can't possibly know. You have to trust the machinery of the company
Starting point is 00:25:42 and your job as the CEO is to kick your feet up on the desk and wait for the phone to ring. Because that's what the CEO is for is only when something is going wrong, when something is unexpected to react to that, the other thing is to do future planning. And that's all the conscious mind is doing too. If something is happening exactly as you expect it, your conscious mind is not even involved in that. It's only when something violates your expectation that you consciously become aware of that.
Starting point is 00:26:11 For example, if I thought that I had my coffee cup right here and I reach for it and grasp it, I'm not even conscious of the whole thing, then I drink it. But if I reach for it and it's not there, then I have to look. I have to suddenly engage my attentional mechanisms and become consciously aware of what's going on to search around for it. But if everything is going as expected, you don't really have to be conscious at all. Take for example, when you're learning how to ride a bicycle, at first you're really conscious, well, what am I doing with my hands and my torso and my legs and how am I doing? But after you get good at it, which doesn't take that long,
Starting point is 00:26:45 it gets burned down into the unconscious machinery of the brain and you're not even aware of it. Then you don't even know. Then if someone asks you to describe how you ride a bicycle, you can't even answer the question. You don't even know the answer. Tell me the difference between things that we learn. Like if we learn to drive, usually,
Starting point is 00:27:03 if you don't drive for a few years and you come back to driving, you can still do it. Bicycle, you learn as a child, you don't do it for a long time, you come back to it, you can do it. But there are other things that we could learn and when we come back to it, we can't, why? Well, interestingly, it turns out there is something
Starting point is 00:27:18 in the brain called savings, which always comes as a surprise. So just as an example, when I was a young man, I don't know, probably 23 or something, I learned how to speak sign language, American sign language, because I knew a colleague of mine who was deaf. And so I learned how to speak sign language.
Starting point is 00:27:38 And then I completely forgot it after she moved away, never used it again for, I don't know, 20 years or something. And then I had an opportunity to learn it again. And what I realized is, oh, more is down there than I thought. So the speed of getting up to conversational again was much, much faster than someone actually starting at square zero.
Starting point is 00:27:57 And this is what's called savings. And we always have that. So for example, people who thought they have forgotten a foreign language, if they actually go back to that country, they realize, oh, wow, I'm actually better than I thought. Tell me about your work habits. You have an unbelievable output of compelling information. How do you manage your life? I'm always hanging on by one finger and I have been doing that for my whole life now. So I don't have any particularly good habits on that,
Starting point is 00:28:26 except that I just always work. I just crank all the time. One thing that I have always done is the lazy Susan method of getting things done, which is a term that I've stolen from Walt Whitman. He didn't actually use that term, but he had a lazy Susan in the middle of his table. If you don't know what that is,
Starting point is 00:28:47 it's this circular thing that you can turn and you can get different things like the salt and pepper or the forks or whatever, you know, you turn the thing and rotate whatever to come closest to you. I have a whole bunch of projects on my plate. And what I do is I work on whatever I'm getting the fastest, most productivity on.
Starting point is 00:29:04 And then when I start slowing down on that, I spin the lazy Susan and grab another project off, and I get tons of work done on that until I'm slowing down. And then I'm so, I'm always able to keep myself close to optimal speed that way. And in some ways, I guess you don't get burned out on any one project because when it starts feeling like you're not making the best use of your time, you move on to something else.
Starting point is 00:29:26 You know, exactly. I got to tell you, when I was an undergraduate, I had an electrical engineering teacher and I really liked this guy. And he told me one day, he said, Eagleman, I got to give you some advice. He said, life is as though you're a lumberjack and you go into the forest. And if you take one whack with your acts at each tree, you're not going to get anywhere. You have to concentrate on one tree and just keep hitting that one tree. Because I really liked this professor, I wanted to take that advice to heart, but I couldn't because it's just not me.
Starting point is 00:29:57 I'm not the one tree guy. I always felt as a young person that there was something wrong with me that I was doing multiple projects, but thank goodness that I was doing multiple projects. But thank goodness that I didn't listen to that or couldn't listen to that. And yeah, so that's why I have output on several fronts now. Do you find that working on a different project sometimes sheds light on another project
Starting point is 00:30:20 that you didn't think it was related, but you see the connections? Absolutely, that's my favorite bit. That happens all the time. I'm working on, for example, you know, so I'm doing this podcast now called Inner Cosmos, and I'm writing an episode, and then I realize, oh, my God, this is the perfect thing that I, like,
Starting point is 00:30:35 I just found this piece of data and some paper while working on this episode. That's the perfect thing for this over here. And that, by the way, you know, I'm writing a movie screenplay, and that goes, belongs right in here. And, oh, by the way, I'm launching a new company. And so that is that piece of data can actually belong over here as well. What I feel really good is when I can double leverage something or triple leverage something. Tell me about your relationship to time.
Starting point is 00:31:01 So I've always been fascinated by time, because this is a well-worn story, so I'll just mention it very briefly, which is that I fell off of a roof when I was a child and I almost died. I landed on a brick floor with my nose first and broke my whole face badly, but lived. And the thing that stayed with me about that whole experience was that it seemed to take forever while I was falling. It seemed to take a very long time. And when I got to high school and took physics
Starting point is 00:31:32 and I learned, you know, I did the calculation, I learned that it was only 0.6 seconds from the top of the roof to the floor. And I couldn't reconcile those. So when I became a neuroscientist, I devoted, and I have continued to devote a lot of my time to studying time perception. And I did these experiments some years ago where I dropped people from a 150-foot-tall tower in free fall.
Starting point is 00:31:56 You're falling backwards. And I was able to measure people's time perception by, I built a device that sat on their wrist and flashed numbers at them in such a way that you could read them at a certain speed, but not at a faster speed. And I was seeing, does time actually go in slow motion when you're in fear for your life? And it turns out the answer is no, you can't actually see in slow motion when you're in fear for your life, but you lay down denser memory. When you're in a life or death situation, you are writing every detail down because fundamentally that's what memory is for.
Starting point is 00:32:31 And so when you read that back out, when you say, what just happened? What just happened? You've got this memory, this memory, this memory. And so what you feel, what your brain presupposes is, oh, that must have taken longer. That must have been a five second event because under normal circumstances, if I have this much memory, it must have taken longer. That must have been a five-second event, because under normal circumstances, if I have this much memory, it must be five seconds. So just as a side note, I published this work, and it really moved the field forward
Starting point is 00:32:53 in terms of understanding that, OK, we don't actually see in slow motion. What happens is occasionally people come up to me and say, hey, I know that's wrong, because I was in a car accident, and I saw the hood crumple, and I saw the rear view mirror fall off, and I saw the expression on the other guy's face. I know I was wrong. Cause I, you know, I was in a car accident and I saw the hood crumple and I saw the rear view mirror fall off and I saw the expression on the other guy's face. I know I was seeing in slow motion. And I said, okay, so the person on the seat next to you, when, when you heard him screaming, did it sound like he was saying no, because if not, then time was not running in slow
Starting point is 00:33:19 motion and they have to allow that that actually wasn't what happened. And so yeah, that's been part of my relationship with time. And what that led to is a deeper understanding that time, how long you think life is lasting has everything to do with memory and the brain only writes things down if they're novel or salient and otherwise it doesn't. So if you're doing the same stuff all the time, then it will seem as though the years are passing quickly because you have no new memory. When you're a child and you experience the summertime,
Starting point is 00:33:52 it seems to last forever because everything is new. You've never experienced that. You've never been in this sort of social situation. You've never been off to this summer camp, whatever. But when you're an adult, a summer goes like that because you've seen it all before. It's the same old patterns that you are now an expert in. And so the really important thing is to seek novelty, to find ways of injecting novelty into your life. This is not to live longer. It's to make it seem as though you've lived longer. This is the important key. behind convenience store counters. Not the aggressive buzz that leaves you jittery,
Starting point is 00:34:45 but a careful calibration of clean energy and focused clarity. Athletic nicotine, the lowest dose tobacco-free nicotine available. Made entirely in the USA. No artificial sweetness, just pure, purposeful elevation. Athletic nicotine is a performance nootropic. Athletic nicotine is a tool for shifting mindsets. Athletic nicotine is a partner in
Starting point is 00:35:10 pursuit of excellence. Slow release, low dose, gradual lift, sustained energy, soft landing, inspired results. Athletic nicotine. More focus. Less static. Athletic nicotine. More clarity. Less noise. Athletic nicotine. More accuracy.
Starting point is 00:35:38 Less anxiety. Athletic nicotine. From top athletes pushing their limits to artists pursuing their vision. Athletic nicotine offers the lift you've been looking for. Learn more at athleticnicotine.com slash tetra and experience next level performance with athletic nicotine. Warning, this product contains nicotine. Nicotine is an addictive chemical.
Starting point is 00:36:02 Do you have any type of a spiritual practice? I would say it's my science, and I'll tell you why. I gave a talk some years ago called Why I Am a Possibilian, and what I described is, look, when you go into the bookstore, you've got the books by the atheists, and you've got the books by the religious, and that've got the books by the religious and that's it. It's like you got to choose one team or the other. But I've always felt like from a scientific point of view, all the religious stories,
Starting point is 00:36:35 you know, sorry to say, but those are clearly wrong. It's not that the earth is 6,000 years old. It's not that, you know, the atom was invented from a rib, whatever. Those are clearly, we're past that. We're not living 2000 years ago and making up stories like that. And on the flip side with the atheism, I'm clearly closer to that side,
Starting point is 00:36:56 but I feel like, look, actually this is a big complicated world. We don't know what is going on here. And part of the scientific mindset is really having a wide table upon which you accept all kinds of hypotheses and say, okay, look, here's a bunch of possibilities. Let's figure out which one's right or wrong. And so what I don't like sometimes when I go and I see these books by the atheist and the books by the religious, I feel like everyone's coming at this with all this certainty
Starting point is 00:37:19 saying, I know what the answer is. And I find that unconscionable because who knows what the right answer is? We are living in a cosmos filled with mysteries. And so I think there's another path. Sometimes people think agnosticism is in the middle, but agnosticism simply says, I don't know. So I defined some years ago, this new thing called possibilityism, which is actively exploring the structure of the possibility space and figuring out, okay, what are we doing here? So maybe the whole Judeo-Christian, that's like one point in the possibility space.
Starting point is 00:37:56 Maybe the idea that absolutely nothing, we're just, we're going to die and that's the end of that. That's certainly a point in the possibility space. There's lots of other stuff and we can use the tools of science to figure out the structure of this. And by the way, rule out whole sections of it, and by the way, open up new folds in the possibility space that we didn't even expect. So that's my spiritual practice,
Starting point is 00:38:18 is just trying to figure out, wow, what is possible here? What was it like growing up with a father who was a psychiatrist? I mean, he passed away four years ago, but yeah, he was just an amazingly smart and empathetic person. He was a particularly interesting guy. He was also a gun dealer and also as a part-time police officer. And he was also in the army reserve.
Starting point is 00:38:46 So he was a guy who, I guess, like me, had lots of different careers going on at the same time. I guess as a child, he once said something, I asked him, I said, what exactly do you do as a psychiatrist? And he said something like, well, I listen to what people say, and I think, what are they actually saying? And then from then on, I thought, oh shoot,
Starting point is 00:39:02 now everything I say, I'm not gonna be taken at face value. But anyway, it was wonderful. Have you ever done talk therapy? And then from then on I thought, oh shoot, now everything I say, I'm not going to be taken at face value. But anyway, it was wonderful. Have you ever done talk therapy? I actually have not. I've never done therapy. I understand from so many friends that it is extremely useful for them.
Starting point is 00:39:17 I'm married and have a wonderful wife and there's a sense in which we help each other out in those ways. Sometimes in battles and sometimes with helping and loving, but one certainly learns a lot about what one's issues are and how to move forward that way. In relationship. Yes, exactly. How often are you surprised in your work?
Starting point is 00:39:40 Every day. I mean, if I'm doing it right, every day something blows my mind. And thankfully we are living in a world now where I would say mostly because of the internet, where you've got access to anything instantly, meaning as soon as somebody makes a discovery on the other side of the world, seconds later, wow, there it is in my inbox. Here's this amazing new discovery. So I feel quite grateful about that because if we were living in the 1800s,
Starting point is 00:40:09 I'd have to wait for a mail carrier to come and someone has handwritten me a note saying, hey, here's a new discovery about this thing. Just the fact that we have things like Wikipedia or that we can have things like you strap on a VR headset and you can go down to the size of an atom and like move around there, zoom up to the size of the solar system
Starting point is 00:40:30 and see things from there. I mean, what an opportunity. And by the way, I think this is what's gonna make the next generation so much smarter than we are because they get to grow up in this world where everything that humankind has figured out, they have instant access to. And I think that's the loveliest thing that they've got such a broad diet.
Starting point is 00:40:52 I mean, this is different. You and I grew up in a different world. And I don't feel like we're that old, but my God, the world is different. Where we had our homeroom teacher, and if we wanted to know a piece of information, our mother would have to drive us to the library and you'd hope it was there in Encyclopedia Britannica. If not, you would thumb through the card catalog and hope there was a book on the thing.
Starting point is 00:41:15 But now the second a kid wants to know an answer, he or she says, hey, Alexa, tell me the answer. Hey, chat, can you tell me the answer to this? And they get the answer in the context of their curiosity. So right when they want to know the answer, they get it, which, chat, can you tell me the answer to this? And they get the answer in the context of their curiosity. So right when they want to know the answer, they get it, which is important because that's when brain plasticity happens. When you have the right neurotransmitters there and those right neurotransmitters essentially equate to curiosity if you're curious about something.
Starting point is 00:41:39 And so that's when it sticks. But of course, the way you and I grew up, we had a lot of just in case information given to us. And the teacher hoped that some of it, some tiny fraction of it would stick. How much does new technology impact your work? I would say a lot because there's a very real sense in which, let's take neuroscience, study the brain, which I'm in.
Starting point is 00:42:04 People have had really good theoretical ideas for a long time, but what we're lacking is data. And it's not like people don't have theoretical frameworks, but it's really hard to measure things from the brain. Why? Because it's encased in this armored bunker plating of the skull, and it's hard to get in there. And you can't in a human,
Starting point is 00:42:29 open up a hole in the skull and see, and even if you had permission or some excuse where you could look inside the skull and see the surface of the brain, that doesn't tell you anything either because brains communicate with this electrical activity. Every single cell in your brain, you've got 86 billion of these specialized cells
Starting point is 00:42:44 called neurons. Each neuron is popping off electrical spikes, which is essentially the language of the brain. It's going, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, brrrr, So if each spike could be represented by a photon of light, it would be blinding what is going on in your brain every second of your life. And so if you just look at a brain, though, you can't see any of that. It just looks like a pink jelly thing. So it has been very difficult to get technology to really look in there. So we have technologies like brain imaging. Many people have probably seen things like MRI, which gives an anatomical snapshot or fMRI, which shows the activity, the blood flow that's going on in the brain,
Starting point is 00:43:31 which represents where the activity was. But it's very low resolution. I mean, it tells you, oh, there was a blob of activity here. That tells you that three seconds ago, there was some of the neurons there of these, you know, this cluster of, let's say, a million neurons, some fraction of them were active, but that doesn't tell you much. And on the flip side of that, you've got things like,
Starting point is 00:43:53 you can put electrodes into the brain and measure what individual neurons are doing, or maybe tens or hundreds of them, but out of 86 billion, that doesn't tell you much either. So we're stuck in an era right now where we don't have really good technology. What we need is technology that says, here's the activity of every cell in the brain all at the same time. Then we could get somewhere. Then we'd have whole new classes of theories. There's probably 12 Nobel prizes to be had there once we get that technology.
Starting point is 00:44:20 Is it possible to create that technology? Sure, I mean, it will happen. I hope it happens in my lifetime. Like every single technological invention, it's just gonna be a matter of lots of other pieces and parts coming together and all these things converging and then bang, somebody comes up with it. Or probably if it's like most inventions, three or four people come up with it at the same time.
Starting point is 00:44:45 Once all the adjacent technologies exist and are there to be put together, a bunch of people stick it together at the same time. Tell me about collective consciousness. The idea that there's a time for information to come and there's some connection between all of us that creates that invitation. I would have a slightly less romantic view on that.
Starting point is 00:45:09 By the way, I have romantic views on lots of stuff, but in this case, it's just that the puzzle pieces are there and there are so many educated people now, as in 100, 200 years ago, very few people got education. Now it's the standard and what that means is, and we have more brains than ever on the planet now, we've got 8.2 billion brains running around, most of them getting good education. And because the internet disseminates the information instantly, what it means is, as
Starting point is 00:45:35 soon as those four or five puzzle pieces hit the ground, there are going to be lots of brains that put them together. So it's just a matter of lots of opportunities, sort of intellectual opportunities there for stuff to happen. Beyond your fall off the roof, what are other events that you'd say make you who you are? One thing I often think about is my father One thing I often think about is my father was a voracious reader and in our home in Albuquerque, New Mexico, up in the mountains, middle of nowhere, he built these library stacks. So it was just shelves after shelves after shelves of books.
Starting point is 00:46:19 And I spent most of my childhood, obviously, pre-driver's license and pre-internet and so on, wandering those stacks and reading and reading. And so that for sure made me a big part of who I am. I do wonder currently what it's going to mean for the next generation who has access to that library, but also has a million ways distractions to waste time, you know, the TikTok and Facebook and so on. So anyway,
Starting point is 00:46:46 I think that certainly made me who I am. So, you know, I had a great admiration for my parents. And then I went to graduate school and I had a grab my graduate thesis advisor, a guy named Reed Montague. I just I had and continue to have such admiration for him. He became sort of like a proxy for the next parent. And he was very hard on me, but that was useful in my life at that time for someone to just break my legs all the time and make me achieve a new level. And so I'm very grateful to him and that sort of experience. I don't recommend
Starting point is 00:47:26 graduate school to a lot of people, but for those who do go, it can be super useful because you learn to think at a deeper level, by which I mean, if you're even reasonably smart in this world, you get a lot of charity thrown to you. Like if you say something that's mostly right, people will agree or say like, oh yeah, that's right. But if you get a good graduate thesis advisor who is willing to take the time to just torture you. Nitpick. Nitpick until everything is 100% right or backable up.
Starting point is 00:48:02 That is a real opportunity that most people never get. And so it's like going through bootcamp. So I'm very grateful for that. If you're under pressure, time constraint, for example, will that make your work better or worse? I mean, look, if it weren't for deadlines, nothing would get done at all. And so that is a necessary part of things.
Starting point is 00:48:28 I would say there's probably an optimal amount of pressure. There's probably a U-shaped curve on that one. Are gut feeling and instinct the same? No. So gut feeling is more like intuition because an instinct, again, is something you're born with. You come pre-programmed to the table with, but intuition is something that you develop. It's a type of learning about the world. There are some things we learn where we have, imagine like a camera lens that's
Starting point is 00:49:00 got a very tight focus, like when you're learning chess, you have to learn, okay, when I make this move with the rook, this is exactly what's, you know, what this looks like and what happens here. That's a very tight lens. But we have things with a wider lens, which is let's say emotion, and in some cases intuition, where we say, look, I don't know why I feel uncomfortable
Starting point is 00:49:22 when I walk into this room here, but something isn't feeling good to me. There's nothing magical about it. It's just a wide lens computation about what's going on. You're picking up on some signals that whether or not you can consciously identify it, some thing about the way that person looked over at that person and something about the way the things are arranged and whatever, you feel like, wow,
Starting point is 00:49:46 this is not the kind of place I wanna be. And we use that intuition all the time to say, hey, I'm gonna go towards this, I'm gonna go away from that, this is scary, this is wonderful. I mean, certainly in relationships, this is what we do all the time, right? You don't know why you like certain people
Starting point is 00:50:00 and don't like certain people, or the person you get married to, who knows why when you met that person, you felt like, oh my God, you know, birds are singing in the sunlight, whatever. That's all stuff that you're picking up on. Her eyes, her expression, the way she looked at you, the whatever. There's a million things you're picking up on that you could not consciously identify, but with your wide angle lens, that's what's going
Starting point is 00:50:25 in there and that's what we summarize as intuition. Not-Google-Gladwell had that book called Blink, which was about the benefit of snap judgment versus pondering. And the book's a series of examples of like a person who recognized that a art piece was a forgery in a museum, whereas all the other experts, it had all the right specifics for it to be what it was claimed to be,
Starting point is 00:50:53 but by looking at it in a moment, a person could tell this is not true. Yes, but really what that story illustrates is expertise versus, you expertise versus amateurism. So it's the amateurs who look at the painting and say, yeah, it looks great to me. But what expertise is, is the years of learning that led to that guy having all that knowledge deep down burned in there that allowed him to look at that and say, something's not right there.
Starting point is 00:51:23 That looks like a forgery to me. So I think it's almost the opposite though in the book because all of the experts agree that it's real. And the one differing view was the guy who could look at it. And in an instant, without seeing any of the backup could tell it was a forgery. So here's what I'd say. I read the book a long time ago and I loved it.
Starting point is 00:51:44 I think that story is a good example of, you know forgery. and the others. So I would just suggest that the others who were called experts were obviously not experts, not at the same level that this guy had the expertise. So much of today's life happens on the web. Squarespace is your home base for building your dream presence in an online world. Designing a website is easy using one of Squarespace's best in class templates. With the built-in style kit, you can change fonts, imagery, margins, and menus. So your design will be perfectly tailored to your needs. Discover unbreakable creativity with Fluid Engine, a highly intuitive drag and drop editor. No coding or technical experiences required. Understand your site's performance
Starting point is 00:52:52 with in-depth website analytics tools. Squarespace has everything you need to succeed online. Create a blog, monetize a newsletter, make a marketing portfolio, Launch an online store. The Squarespace app helps you run your business from anywhere. Track inventory and connect with customers while you're on the go. Whether you're just starting out or already managing a successful brand Squarespace makes it easy to create and customize a beautiful website Visit Squarespace comm slash Tetra and get started today
Starting point is 00:53:41 Do you think the more decisions we make, the better we get at making decisions? Or is there some point where we've made too many decisions and we start moving down the slope instead of up the slope? There's been a claim in the psychology literature that willpower is like a gas tank that can run low. And if you have to make a bunch of decisions, and then I offer you some chocolate cookies, and even if you're on a diet, you know,
Starting point is 00:54:14 want chocolate cookies, you're gonna eat them if you've just done a lot of decision-making because you've burned a lot of energy. I mean, decisions take energy in the brain, and we are mobile creatures that run on batteries. You know, we have to constantly get food to fund ourselves. And so that's the claim is that you can actually, you know, get tired and then your decision-making gets worse.
Starting point is 00:54:38 Is all of the information held in our brain held there physically? Yes, it's all physical. This is the amazing part. So there's many different ways that the information can get held. So for example, if I say, hey here's six digits that you have to type into this website for your two-factor authentication, you're memorizing that okay three, six, nine, two, four, five, and you memorize and you repeat it yourself. That's being held
Starting point is 00:55:03 physically in spikes running around in a loop in your brain. That's how you memorize that. But the name of your fifth grade teacher, that's stored differently. That's burned down into the structure, the physical structure of your brain. So the first is an example of short-term or working memory.
Starting point is 00:55:21 The second is example of long-term memory. And of course, most of who you are is the long-term memory that you have. What was the address of your last house and so on. Before I asked you that question, that information was sitting there physically, but it was in just think of it like cold storage. And then you say, oh gosh, now I need that address.
Starting point is 00:55:40 You bring it out and you turn it back into spikes, physical electrical activity, but it is stored there. There are dozens, maybe hundreds of ways that information gets stored in the brain. The thing that's been most popular in the field for the last 50 years is this idea of changing the strength of the connections, what are called the synapses between neurons, changing the strength from minimum to maximum and anywhere in between. And it turns out you can show in a network of neurons that you can store information
Starting point is 00:56:12 that way. But almost certainly that's too cartoonish a model to actually represent the biology because way past the connections, you've got a whole universe inside of each cell with changeable parameters. And so things are changing all the way down to the genome, where you have proteins that glom onto the genome and change the way that other proteins are getting expressed. So the point is there's lots of ways that you can make changes in the system and all of that is physical and that's how memories are stored. And again, the reason we know that is because, you know, someone can get hit in the head and they lose memories or they get, let's say, a stroke and they lose memories that
Starting point is 00:56:55 they had or, you know, people take a psychedelic drug and they remember some memory that they thought they'd long forgotten or whatever. It's all in there physically. The difference between how a child's brain works versus an adult brain. Yeah, so it turns out that when you are born, you have essentially all the neurons you're ever going to have.
Starting point is 00:57:15 Neurons don't divide and reproduce like many other cells in your body. So you've got, call it 86 million neurons, but what happens is as a baby, they start reaching out and making connections and trying to find connections with others. And by the time you're two years old, it is super densely connected, more so than any other time in your life. And from then on, from about two onwards, it's all a game of pruning. You're actually reducing the number of connections. And what prunes the garden is your experience in the
Starting point is 00:57:45 world. What your neighborhood is, what your parents are, what your moment in time is. All that stuff is pruning it so that you become an expert in how to navigate your world and understand what's going on there with your language that you're surrounded with and so on. And so that's one of the main differences. Another one is that parts of the brain sort of develop and come into place like a Polaroid picture developing. The slowest part of the brain is what's called the prefrontal cortex, behind the forehead
Starting point is 00:58:17 here. And that's the part of the brain that's involved in simulating possible futures and therefore involved in things like risk-taking. So teenagers have the size and body of an adult, but not the maturity, and they don't have the capacity to simulate things out and say, okay, well, maybe I shouldn't do that because that wouldn't be a good idea for the following reasons. So the prefrontal cortex doesn't become fully developed till about 25 years old. And you know, car rental companies know this because they charge you more if you're under
Starting point is 00:58:49 25, they're never going to drive more in a crazy manner. But it's a very interesting set of decisions that we have to make as a society. When do we let people vote? When do we let people drink? What about the death penalty? You know, the Supreme Court said, okay, look, they finally concluded, if you're under 18, when you committed the crime, we can't execute you. If you're over 18, then you can't be executed. But you know, the difference between someone who's 17 and 364 days and someone who's 18 in one day, there's no meaningful difference there.
Starting point is 00:59:16 It's just the legal system always has to draw bright lines. But this is always a problem in figuring out what to do about this slow development of the prefrontal cortex. Do animals make decisions the same way we do? I think a lot of it is based on some of the same stuff, like how do I get to the food? How do I get to the mate? How do I find a safe place to sleep?
Starting point is 00:59:39 Stuff like that. But what we have as humans is this whole capacity on top of that to think about ourselves at all these different time scales. I mentioned this before, but okay, I need to do this, but also what kind of person do I want to be? And also what will my friends think if I do this? And also can I advance my career if I do that? And will I do something that gets me in trouble politically if I do that?
Starting point is 01:00:04 And will it? Whatever. I mean, there's just a million decisions that go into everything because we are social creatures who are embedded in this culture and we really care for the most part about what other people think and we navigate our behavior accordingly. There are animal species that are more social than others, but for the most part animals don't do any of that stuff that I just mentioned. They're just sort of coming up with the best, probable reward and how to get there. Is there anything that's currently being taught
Starting point is 01:00:36 in neuroscience that you question whether that's accurate or not? Well, the main thing is when you look at any introductory neuroscience textbook, it has a picture of the brain and then puts some color on it and says, okay, this is the visual part of the brain, this is the auditory part of the brain, this is for touch, this is for decision-making, and it's just, as I mentioned earlier, it's a totally fluid system.
Starting point is 01:00:59 It just so happens that that's usually how it turns out if you're born with functioning eyeballs and ears and touch receptors and whatever. But boy, it is unbelievably flexible where that can go. So I think there's a real underappreciation. I mean, everyone knows about brain plasticity and talks about it, but the degree to which this is a system that you can plug anything into and it will shape itself around that, is quite extraordinary and not the type of thing that I think is talked about upfront in any book.
Starting point is 01:01:32 And sometimes they put it in the last chapter like, oh, by the way, the brain is very flexible and you can do other things and plug other things in. So for example, one of the things that I started doing in my lab about 12 years ago is this idea of sensory substitution, which is feeding information into the brain via an unusual pathway. So it turns out that this was first demonstrated in 1969 in the journal Nature.
Starting point is 01:01:59 A scientist named Paul Bocchi-Rita took blind people and he put them in a dental chair and he put this solenoid grid against their back. So imagine these things that can poke in and out and he set up a camera and whatever the camera saw they would feel that poked into their back. So if the camera's looking at a face they feel a face poked into their back and if the camera's feeling looking at a telephone they feel the shape of a telephone poked into your back and so on. And blind people get pretty good at understanding what was happening in the visual world, just based on what they were feeling on their skin,
Starting point is 01:02:33 on the skin of their back, which is remarkable. So about 12 years ago, I started looking into whether I could build sensory substitution for people who are deaf. So I capture auditory information and I turn, I built a vest with the students in my lab and it's got vibratory motors all over it. And so the vest is capturing sound and turning into these patterns of vibration based on you know how much of this high frequency
Starting point is 01:02:57 is here and low frequencies and medium and everything in between. And deaf people could come to understand the auditory world this way by feeling the information pushed in through the skin of their torso. So I actually I ended up spinning that out of my laboratory as a company called Neo Sensory and we shrunk this down to a wristband and the wristband has vibratory motors around the wrist and this is on wrists all over the world now for people who are deaf or simply getting age-related hearing loss or who have tinnitus, which is ringing in the ears.
Starting point is 01:03:27 We have all kinds of things we're doing, but the point is it's capturing sound, it's turning things into patterns of vibration on the skin, and you can come to understand things about the world that way. And one of the things I've been pursuing for the last 10 years is can we create new senses for humans? Can we feed in a totally new kinds of information like infrared light or ultrasonic sound or other things like that
Starting point is 01:03:50 and come to understand what is happening in the world just based on what we're feeling pushed into the brain via the skin in this case. How much of what we perceive is what's actually out there? We perceive a very vanishingly small fraction of what's out there. So this isn't just a philosophical claim. You can look at something like electromagnetic radiation, what we call light. In a very narrow range, the light that bounces off and hits our eyes, we're able to pick
Starting point is 01:04:20 up on that because we have the proper receptors to pick up on that. And so that's what we call visible light That's ROYGBIV and that's what we think of is all the colors that are out there But in fact what we see is actually less than a ten trillionth of the electromagnetic radiation spectrum In other words all the light that's out there only a tiny tiny fraction of what we call visible light The rest of it is all light but things that we call like, you know, radio waves and x-rays and microwaves and cosmic rays, all this stuff is exactly the same stuff as your red, orange, yellow, blue. It's just of a different frequency that we can't see because we don't have the proper receptors for it. So it's totally invisible to us.
Starting point is 01:04:59 And it's not that it inherently has to be invisible because rattlesnakes, for example, pick up on infrared information because of the structure of their heat pits, and honeybees because of the structure of their compound eyes pick up on ultraviolet information. And of course, we build machines to pick up on radio frequencies or microwave or whatever. It's just that we can't see them.
Starting point is 01:05:20 You don't come to the table biologically able to see that stuff. And so the rest of it, until modern science came along, You can't see them. You don't come to the table biologically able to see that stuff. And so the rest of it, until modern science came along, the rest of it didn't even exist as far as we were concerned, even though it's all light. So we're only seeing a very, very tiny fraction of the world that's out there. The human foot is a true marvel of engineering. With 26 bones, 33 joints, and over 100 muscles and tendons, it's built for flexibility, balance,
Starting point is 01:05:53 and natural movement. Unfortunately, today's narrow, rigid, elevated shoes undermine this natural brilliance and weaken your feet. Vivo Barefoot shoes set your feet free. Feel every step, move naturally, allow your feet to function as nature intended with Vivo Barefoot. Thin flexible soles to keep you connected to the ground, wide toe boxes to let your toes spread and stabilize. Minimalist shoes designed to mimic the barefoot experience. Whether you're running, hiking or just going about your day,
Starting point is 01:06:32 Vivo Barefoot has you covered precisely when and where you need it most. Feel the vibration of the earth. Strengthen your feet. Improve your posture. Enhance your natural movement with Vivo Barefoot. Made from 100% recycled and natural materials, they are as healthy for the planet as they are for you. Experience the freedom of Barefoot movement with the protection from the sharp edges of
Starting point is 01:06:59 a man-made world. Natural movement for your feet, sustainable choices for our world. How is rhythm used in the brain? The brain is always trying to predict. Fundamentally, the brain is a prediction machine. And so it's always trying to say, what's next, what's next. And so I think the reason brains are so attracted to rhythm is that it says, oh, good, ooh, I got this, I got this.
Starting point is 01:07:35 Every time I'm making a prediction, it's coming true. I totally got this. And so that's, I think, why we love rhythm so much. Tell me about Surprise. How does it work? We have an internal model of the world, which is everything you've built up over every experience you've ever had about,
Starting point is 01:07:52 okay, I got it. This is how the world works. This is what people do. This is what buildings do, what dogs do, what cars do. Great. And the thing is our internal model is always a little bit imperfect. And as a result, we get surprised.
Starting point is 01:08:06 That's what surprises is where you think, Oh, I got this. I can make a good prediction about this. Whoops. That didn't happen. Then we pay attention to it. The brain is fundamentally wired to look for surprise to say, Whoa, that wasn't what I thought was going on, so I'm going to attend to that and figure out how to incorporate that into my internal model
Starting point is 01:08:25 and understand that a bit better. And so that is the key to learning. I mean, surprise leads to attention, leads to writing things down in the brain. How do you decide what's gonna be a book versus something else? You know, I have written a lot of books at this point, but I am the world's slowest book writer.
Starting point is 01:08:46 So each book that I write takes probably seven to 10 years. And what I'm doing is I'm always working on multiple books at once in this lazy Susan method. You know, I spin it, I'm working on the next book and so on. But the answer is I never know in advance. I start a book and I'm dumping everything in. The way I write a book is very different than a lot of my colleagues who sort of start on page one and make
Starting point is 01:09:09 an outline or whatever. I don't do it that way. I just start throwing everything in and the whole thing develops like a Polaroid picture. But often, I guess I'd say always, the book ends up being so different from what I originally thought it might be. So the way I write books is by a lot of experimentation and much of it doesn't work. And I would say, it feels very wasteful. I mean, you know, probably half the words I write end up getting chucked. But I think, at least for myself, that is the process.
Starting point is 01:09:38 What's happening in us when we have internal conflict? My view of the brain is that the right way to understand this is as a team of rivals. You have all these different neural networks going on. And when we talk about you, we say, oh, Rick, yeah, I know Rick, Rick's this kind of guy, whatever. We're really talking about as a whole collection
Starting point is 01:09:59 of neural networks with different drives in it. And so as an example, if I, you know, I take some creme brulee and I say, Oh Rick, why don't you, why don't you eat this? It's delicious. And maybe you say, Oh, part of your brain says, okay, well I'm on a diet. I shouldn't eat that. Party brain says, wow, that's a rich energy source. Got a lot of sugar in it.
Starting point is 01:10:19 Party brain says, okay, how about I'll eat it, but I'll go to the gym tonight and work out to cancel this out What you can argue with yourself you can cuss at yourself You can cajole you can contract with yourself and the question is who is talking with whom here? It is all you but it's different parts of you and we constantly have internal conflict We constantly say oh gosh, I really want to do this thing. Oh, but that's a bad idea Oh, what if I get caught? What if I don't get caught? What'll this lead to my, you know, we're doing that with everything in a million small ways all the time.
Starting point is 01:10:49 And that is because we are a team of rivals. I, I obviously, I took that expression from Abraham Lincoln's team of rivals cabinet that he set up. He came in as president and said, I'm going to make a cabinet of people who disagree with each other. And we'll see where we get by having you know consensus Derived from this team of rivals and that was very smart idea. Anyway, that's that's what evolution has done with us We are essentially machines that are built of conflict
Starting point is 01:11:19 How susceptible are we to group think Totally susceptible we are a very social species. We are massively influenced by What our community thinks what our era thinks what our country thinks? You know what flies at this moment in time, you know it's been a popular thing in the last decade to take down statues of people who have some thing in the last decade to take down statues of people who have some idea that seems distasteful to modern college students and it's a real shame because they have no control over what culture they lived in, what era, what moment in time they lived in, and so what this means if we keep that up is that we're constantly
Starting point is 01:12:00 taking down statues because whatever you think now, so take a student who feels that they are at the center of exactly what the right thing to think is, I guarantee in 200 years your statue will get taken down because people say, I cannot believe they did that. So whatever, we are creatures of our moment in time. We're creatures of group think. Everybody likes to believe to some degree that they're an independent thinker. But one thing you can do, I mean, you know, Rick, you and I, we haven't talked
Starting point is 01:12:29 about this yet, but you know, you and I are both so obsessed with what creativity is. What's fascinating to me about creativity is that, you know, if you were Beethoven, you could have written music that was with different semitones or, you know, used breathing in it or used some kind of other weird drum thing in it. Okay, why do we know that? Because other people in the world were writing that kind of music and this culture over here that culture there, whatever. But of course, Beethoven is not going to write that because he is a creature of what's going
Starting point is 01:12:58 on in Austria at that moment in time. Just like the person who's contemporaneously writing music over in China is writing a very different style of music because that's what music is to that brain and in Nigeria, that guy's writing music because that's what music is in that moment. We as you would know better than anybody, you know, live in this extraordinary time right now when because of the internet, we get to experience music from all different cultures that gives us such a cool opportunity for new remixes of ideas that people before us never even had the opportunity for. What makes a great teacher? I think it's just understanding what a person does and does not know and what interests
Starting point is 01:13:42 them. And so telling a story in a way, setting it up in a way that they will care about the answer. And by the way, one of the classes I teach at Stanford is called Literature and the Brain because I'm fascinated by this issue that we are so wired for story. We love stories as opposed to lists of facts.
Starting point is 01:14:02 And so I've just been very interested in the fact that, first off, you want to get any information into the brain, you have to cast it in terms of a story, and that really works to get things in there. And 10 years after you read Malcolm Gladwell's book, the part you remember is the story. You don't remember the details, and you listed something about a brain part,
Starting point is 01:14:20 whatever, it doesn't matter. But I'm also fascinated by the fact that our brains, even though the way a typical neuroscience textbook would go is say, look, this is the part of the brain that sees and hears and touches and determines where you are now, what's around you, but we're so prone to slipping into fantasy. So I can pick up a page with these symbols on it
Starting point is 01:14:43 and I'm immediately in Westeros and doing something, Game of Thrones, and I am Jon Snow in that moment. And the fact that I've still got the same eyes and ears and nose and fingertips and whatever, but I'm now not David Eagleman, I'm Jon Snow and I'm doing something else. The fact that we slip into these other worlds so easily is fascinating to me and I don't think that's been well studied.
Starting point is 01:15:08 What's the hardest part of your job? I guess the hardest part is ideas are easy but proving an idea all the way to you know running the experiment in science, this is often getting the grant money for it, writing the institutional review board to get the permission to do the experiment, writing up the results, getting it published. That's a years-long process often. And so it's very worth it, and it feels great. But sometimes the hard part is that I think of something,
Starting point is 01:15:44 and I think just something and I think just occasionally I'll think, wow, I have an intuition that that might be right, but I'm never going to be able to prove that idea because it would take me the next 10 years and it would take getting a $10 million grant and building this machinery that does XYZ. And so I feel like that part stinks sometimes to feel like, okay, I'm reaching out past the curtain of what's known. And look, I might not be right. I mean, you know, as scientists we're wrong most of the time, but occasionally you're right about some idea.
Starting point is 01:16:20 And I feel like, damn, I'm never gonna be able to show that that's true. Tell me about conversations you've had with people while they were undergoing brain surgery. Yeah, well, you can be totally awake during brain surgery because the brain is the only part of the body that doesn't have pain receptors. And so you can be poking around in somebody's brain and taking parts out and doing whatever you want. And they can be totally awake
Starting point is 01:16:46 just like doing anything else they're doing. Obviously, when you drill a hole in the skull, there's pain receptors on the skin there on the scalp. And so you have to locally anesthetize that. But once you're in the brain, there's no pain receptors. Why? Because mother nature never had to worry about that.
Starting point is 01:17:01 Mother nature never foresaw a situation where someone is mucking around in your brain. So patients get neurosurgeons all the time for epilepsy, for tumors, for different sorts of things. And you can do experiments with them and talk with them and show them, you know, here's a screen of a tablet. I want you to point at this and tell me what you see there and that kind of stuff.
Starting point is 01:17:24 And so the kind of conversations you can have with them are exactly like the kind of conversation you can have if they weren't undergoing brain surgery. Does anything come up that wouldn't come up otherwise? Yeah. Well, the main thing is if the surgeon sticks an electrode in and zaps some area that might cause a person to, let's say, laugh. And then because the brain is a storyteller, you'll come up with some reason. Okay, well, why did you laugh? Oh, it's because I just thought of something funny, things like that.
Starting point is 01:17:56 Or there's all kinds of things where a certain zap in a certain place causes a muscle movement or something. People will always have a story about why they just did something, oh, my arm hurts so I was moving it, that kind of thing. So yes, that's what can happen differently during brain surgery is that someone actually does something to your brain down in the hardware
Starting point is 01:18:15 that makes you do something, and then you retrospectively tell a story about it. So imagine you're stimulating a part of the brain, the person moves their arm, and you say, why did you move your arm? You say, I moved my arm because it was itching, so I decided. Now, does that mean that the place that got triggered is the same as the place where they would feel the itch?
Starting point is 01:18:39 So they're actually telling the truth. Do you know what I'm saying? That's a really good question, but no, in this case, you've got a map in the brain of the body. And when you stimulate those areas, you stimulate the muscles, you contract the muscles. So if you do it every time, it'll contract my bicep
Starting point is 01:18:54 every time, because it's about the muscle itself is what's getting stimulated there. I see. And the story that they would tell would not be that story. Right. I mean, it's an interesting question. If I stimulated someone's bicep over and over and over again, maybe they'd start getting used to it and say,
Starting point is 01:19:08 hey, I think you're zapping me. There are these experiments, for example, with these split brain surgeries, as they're called, where someone has an epilepsy that spreads to the other side of the head to the brain. And so what these surgeons did some decades ago is cut the superhighway of neurons that connects the two hemispheres, the two halves of the brain.
Starting point is 01:19:28 And the patient came too and the patient seemed fine, but it turns out that now they had essentially two consciousnesses. Each hemisphere was like its own person. And so you could communicate to the left hemisphere, which has language, by writing words over here on the right side, because things on the right side get analyzed on the left side of the brain. So you could write some words in the left hemisphere to understand that. The right hemisphere would not understand the words. Instead, it can only understand pictures of things. But what you can do is set up all kinds of experiments
Starting point is 01:20:01 where you get one side of the brain to do something because you're sending it a message, and then the other side of the brain will make up a story about why it's just done that thing, because it doesn't have access to what the other side saw, so it'll constantly make up stories. It's really interesting. Yes, and that's what neuroscience is about. This is why I feel like in my entire lifetime, I will never run out of moments of feeling in total awe, not only about scientifically how does a brain work and how does this come about, but I and you and everyone, we are our brains. It's not like I'm studying soil mechanics or something that's totally separate from us. This is us. This is you. It's the most
Starting point is 01:20:49 extraordinary thing that we have ever found in the universe and yet it's us. So every bit that we can understand it gives us a little better insight into who we are. Tetragrammaton is a podcast. Tetragrammaton is a website. Tetragrammaton is a whole world of knowledge. What may fall within the sphere of tetragrammaton? Counterculture? Tetragrammaton. Sacred geometry? Tetragrammaton. The avant-garde? Tetragrammaton.
Starting point is 01:21:52 Generative art? Tetragrammaton. The tarot? Tetragrammaton. Out-of-print music? Tetragrammaton. Biodynamics? Tetragrammaton. Graphic design? Tetragrammaton. Mythology and magic? Tetragrammaton. Obscure film? Tetragrammaton. Graphic design. Tetragrammaton. Mythology and magic. Tetragrammaton. Obscure film. Tetragrammaton. Beach culture. Tetragrammaton.
Starting point is 01:22:10 Esoteric lectures. Tetragrammaton. Off the grid living. Tetragrammaton. Alt. Spirituality. Tetragrammaton. The canon of fine objects. Tetragrammaton. Muscle cars. Tetragrammaton. Ancient wisdom for a new age. Upon entering. The world of science. The world of science. Fine objects. Tetragrammatin. Muscle cars. Tetragrammatin. Ancient wisdom for a new age.
Starting point is 01:22:29 Upon entering, experience the artwork of the day. Take a breath and see where you are drawn.

There aren't comments yet for this episode. Click on any sentence in the transcript to leave a comment.