The Daily - The Sunday Read: 'Beauty of the Beasts'

Episode Date: March 21, 2021

The bright elastic throats of anole lizards, the Fabergé abdomens of peacock spiders and the curling, iridescent and ludicrously long feathers of birds-of-paradise. A number of animal species possess... beautifully conspicuous and physically burdensome features.Many biologists have long fit these tasking aesthetic displays into a more utilitarian view of evolution. However, a new generation of biologists have revived a long-ignored theory — that aesthetics and survival do not necessarily need to be linked and that animals can appreciate beauty for its own sake.Today on The Sunday Read, a look at how these biologists are rewriting the standard explanation of how beauty evolves and the way we think about evolution itself. This story was written by Ferris Jabr and recorded by Audm. To hear more audio stories from publishers like The New York Times, download Audm for iPhone or Android.

Transcript
Discussion (0)
Starting point is 00:00:00 There's a really beautiful flower called the snake's head fritillary. It blooms in the spring with these wine-colored nodding petals that have checkers on them. They almost look like a snake's skin. I grow them in my garden. Sometimes I just want to stare at them all day. Flowers have completely mesmerized people throughout history and across cultures. But our obsession with flowers is puzzling. We're not bees or hummingbirds.
Starting point is 00:00:46 There's no obvious evolutionary advantage for our species to be so fixated on flowers. And yet we're still so drawn to them. Flowers are just beautiful. I'm Ferris Jaber. I'm a contributing writer for The New York Times Magazine. And I wrote a story about the quest to explain the origins and purpose of beauty in nature. You know, the peacock is sort of the mascot of animal beauty. Male peacocks have these massive, fanning, many-eyed tails with stunning shades of gold and turquoise that help them attract mates. with stunning shades of gold and turquoise that help them attract mates.
Starting point is 00:01:24 But that plumage has evolved to be so big that it literally weighs the males down. It makes them more noticeable and vulnerable to predators. So why would they carry such a big burden? The usual explanation has been that beauty in the animal kingdom is a kind of code. Ornaments like the peacock's tail are supposed to convey useful information about a potential mate's health and fitness. But that's not the only possibility.
Starting point is 00:01:54 I wrote about Richard Prum, who's an ornithologist at Yale University. Prum says animals simply prefer certain colors, shapes, and sounds, just as we do. You might like a sunflower, or in my case, and sounds, just as we do. You might like a sunflower, or in my case, a snake's head fritillary. To Prum, beauty does not have to be useful. It doesn't have to be coupled with survival. Peacocks are beautiful because they're beautiful to themselves.
Starting point is 00:02:19 They like the way they look. And that's a controversial idea because it highlights personal preferences over the demands of an organism's external environment. But in thinking about beauty in these new ways, we can actually learn something really profound about how evolution itself works. So here's my story, Beauty of the Beasts, read by McLeod Andrews. The New York Times, The New Yorker, Vanity Fair, and The Atlantic. A male flame bowerbird is a creature of incandescent beauty.
Starting point is 00:03:18 The hue of his plumage transitions seamlessly from molten red to sunshine yellow. But that radiance is not enough to attract a mate. When males of most bowerbird species are ready to begin courting, they set about building the structure for which they are named, an assemblage of twigs shaped into a spire, corridor, or hut. They decorate their bowers with scores of colorful objects like flowers, berries, snail shells, or, if they are near an urban area, bottle caps and plastic cutlery. Some bowerbirds even arrange the items in their collection from smallest to largest, forming a walkway that makes themselves and their trinkets all the more striking to a female,
Starting point is 00:04:01 an optical illusion, known as forced perspective, that humans did not perfect until the 15th century. Yet even this remarkable exhibition is not sufficient to satisfy a female flame bowerbird. Should a female show initial interest, the male must react immediately. Staring at the female, his pupils swelling and shrinking like a heartbeat, he begins a dance best described as psychotically sultry. He bobs, flutters, puffs his chest. He crouches low and rises slowly, brandishing one wing in front of his head like a magician's cape. Suddenly, his whole body convulses like a wind-up alarm clock. If the female approves, she will copulate with him for two or three seconds. They will never meet again. The bowerbird defies traditional assumptions about animal behavior. Here is a creature that spends hours meticulously curating a cabinet of
Starting point is 00:04:53 wonder, grouping his treasures by color and likeness. Here is a creature that single-beakedly builds something far more sophisticated than many celebrated examples of animal toolmaking. The stripped twigs that chimpanzees use to fish termites from their mounds pale in comparison. The bowerbird's bower, as at least one scientist has argued, is nothing less than art. When you consider every element of his courtship, the costumes, dance, and sculpture, it evokes a concept beloved by the German composer Richard Wagner, Gesamtkunstwerk, a total work of art, one that blends many different forms and stimulates all the senses. This extravagance is also an affront to the rules of natural selection. Adaptations are meant to be useful, that's the whole point, and the most successful
Starting point is 00:05:46 creatures should be the ones best adapted to their particular environments. So what is the evolutionary justification for the bowerbird's ostentatious display? Not only do the bowerbird's colorful feathers and elaborate constructions lack obvious value outside courtship, but they also hinder his survival and general well-being, draining precious calories and making him much more noticeable to predators. Numerous species have conspicuous, metabolically costly, and physically burdensome sexual ornaments, as biologists call them. ornaments, as biologists call them. Think of the bright elastic throats of anole lizards, the Fabergé abdomens of peacock spiders, and the curling, iridescent, ludicrously long feathers of birds of paradise. To reconcile such splendor with a utilitarian view of evolution, biologists have favored the idea that beauty in the animal kingdom is not mere decoration. It's a code.
Starting point is 00:06:47 According to this theory, ornaments evolved as indicators of a potential mate's advantageous qualities, its overall health, intelligence, and survival skills, plus the fact that it will pass down the genes underlying these traits to its children. A bowerbird with especially bright plumage might have a robust immune system, for example, while one that finds rare and distinctive trinkets might be a superb forager. Beauty, therefore, would not confound natural selection. It would be very much a part of it. Charles Darwin himself disagreed with this theory. Although he co-discovered natural selection and devoted much of his life to demonstrating its importance, he never claimed that it could explain everything. Ornaments, Darwin proposed, evolved through a separate process he called sexual selection.
Starting point is 00:07:40 Females choose the most appealing males according to their standard of beauty, and as a result, males evolve toward that standard, despite the costs. Darwin did not think it was necessary to link aesthetics and survival. Animals, he believed, could appreciate beauty for its own sake. Many of Darwin's peers and successors ridiculed his proposal. To them, the idea that animals had such cognitive sophistication and that the preferences of capricious females could shape entire species was nonsense. Although never completely forgotten, Darwin's theory of beauty was largely abandoned.
Starting point is 00:08:21 Now, nearly 150 years later, a new generation of biologists is reviving Darwin's neglected brainchild. Beauty, they say, does not have to be a proxy for health or advantageous genes. Sometimes beauty is the glorious but meaningless flowering of arbitrary preference. Animals simply find certain features, a blush of red, a feathered flourish, to be appealing. And that innate sense of beauty itself can become an engine of evolution, pushing animals toward aesthetic extremes. In other cases, certain environmental or physiological constraints steer an animal toward an aesthetic preference that has nothing to do with survival whatsoever. These biologists are not only rewriting the standard explanation for how beauty evolves,
Starting point is 00:09:14 they are also changing the way we think about evolution itself. For decades, natural selection, the fact that creatures with the most advantageous traits have the best chance of surviving and multiplying, has been considered the unequivocal centerpiece of evolutionary theory. But these biologists believe that there are other forces at work. Modes of evolution that are much more mischievous and discursive than natural selection. It's not enough to consider how an animal's habitat and lifestyle determine the size and keenness of its eyes, or the number and complexity of its neural circuits. We must also question how an animal's eyes and brain shape its perceptions of reality, and how its unique way of
Starting point is 00:09:59 experiencing the world can, over time, profoundly alter both its physical form and its behavior. There are really two environments governing the evolution of sentient creatures, an external one, which they inhabit, and an internal one, which they construct. To solve the enigma of beauty, to fully understand evolution, we must uncover the hidden links between those two worlds. Perhaps no living scientist is as enthusiastic or doctrinaire, a champion of Darwinian sexual selection, as Richard Prum, an evolutionary ornithologist at Yale University. In May 2017, he published a book, The Evolution of Beauty, that lucidly and passionately
Starting point is 00:10:48 explains his personal theory of aesthetic evolution. It was nominated for the Pulitzer Prize for general nonfiction, but within the scientific community, Prom's ideas have not been as warmly received. Again and again, he told me, he has asked other researchers for feedback and received either excuses of busyness or no reply at all. Some have been openly critical. In an academic review of Prum's book, Gerald Borgia, one of the world's foremost experts on bowerbirds, and the ethologist Gregory Ball described the historical sections as revisionist and said Prum failed to advance a credible case for his thesis. Once, over a lunch of burritos, Prum explained his theory to a visiting colleague
Starting point is 00:11:35 who pronounced it nihilism. Last April, Prum and I drove 20 miles east of New Haven to Hammonasset Beach State Park, a 900-acre patchwork of shoreline, marsh, woodland, and meadow on Long Island Sound with the hope of finding a hooded warbler. Birders had recently seen the small but striking migratory species in the area. Before he even parked, Prum was calling out the names of birds he'd glimpsed or heard through the car window. Osprey, Purple Martin, Red-Winged Blackbird. I asked him how he was able to recognize birds so quickly
Starting point is 00:12:13 and sometimes at such a great distance. He said it was just as effortless as recognizing a portrait of Abraham Lincoln. In Prum's mind, every bird is famous. Binoculars in hand, we walked along the park's winding trails, slowly making our way toward a large stand of trees. Prum wore jeans, a quilted jacket, and a beige hat. His thick eyebrows, round spectacles, and sprays of white and gray hair gave his face a vaguely owlish appearance. In the course of the day, we would see grazing mallards with emerald heads, tree swallows with iridescent turquoise capes, and several sparrow species, each distinguished by a unique ornament.
Starting point is 00:12:56 Swoops of yellow around the eye, a delicate pink beak, a copper crown. On a wooded path, we encountered a lively bird flinging leaf litter into the air. Prom was immediately transfixed. This was a brown thrasher, he told me, describing its attributes with a mix of precision and fondness. Rufous brown, speckled on the breast, yellow eye, curved beak, long tail. Then he reprimanded me for trying to take a picture instead of observing with my binos. About two hours into our walk, Prom, who was a fast and fluid talker, interrupted himself mid-sentence. Right there, right there, he said. There's the hooded, right up against the tree. Something gold flashed across the path. I raised my binoculars to my eyes and scanned the
Starting point is 00:13:47 branches to our right. When I found him, I gasped. He was almost mythological in his beauty. Moss green wings, a luminescent yellow body and face, and a perfectly tailored black hood that made his countenance even brighter by contrast. For several minutes, we stood and watched the bird as it hopped about, occasionally fanning white tail feathers in our direction. Eventually, he flew off. I told Prum how thrilling it was to see such a creature up close. That's it, Prum said. That moment is what bird watching is about. That moment is what birdwatching is about. As a child growing up in a small rural town in southern Vermont,
Starting point is 00:14:32 Prum was, in his words, amorphously nerdy, keen on reading and memorizing stats from the Guinness Book of World Records, but not obsessed with anything in particular. Then, in fourth grade, he got glasses. The world came into focus. He chanced upon a field guide to birds in fourth grade, he got glasses. The world came into focus. He chanced upon a field guide to birds in a bookstore, which encouraged him to get outdoors. Soon he was birding in the ample fields and woods around his home. He wore the grooves off two records of bird calls.
Starting point is 00:15:05 He befriended local naturalists, routinely going on outings with a group of mostly middle-aged women. Conveniently, they had driver's licenses. By the time Prom was in seventh grade, he was leading bird walks at the local state park. In college, Prom wasted no time in availing himself of Harvard University's substantial ornithological resources. The first week of his freshman year, he got a set of keys to the Museum of Comparative Zoology, home to the largest university-based ornithological collection in the world, which today has nearly 400,000 bird specimens. I've been associated with a world-class collection of birds every moment of my adult life, he says. I joke with my students, and it's really true. I have to have at least
Starting point is 00:15:45 100,000 dead birds across the hallway to function intellectually. He is now the head curator of vertebrate zoology at Yale's Peabody Museum of Natural History. He wrote a senior thesis on the phylogeny and biogeography of toucans and barbits, working on a desk beneath the skeleton of a moa, an extinct emu-like bird that stood 12 feet tall and weighed 500 pounds. After graduating from Harvard in 1982, Prum traveled to Suriname to study mannequins, a family of intensely colored birds that compete for mates with high-pitched songs and gymnastic dance routines. In 1984, he began graduate studies in biology at the University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, where he planned to reconstruct the evolutionary history of mannequins through careful comparisons of anatomy and behavior. In the process,
Starting point is 00:16:36 a colleague introduced him to some research papers on sexual selection, piquing his interest in the history of this fascinating yet seemingly neglected idea. Darwin was contemplating how animals perceived one another's beauty as early as his 30s. How does Hen determine which most beautiful cock, which best singer? He scribbled in a note to himself sometime between 1838 and 1840. In The Descent of Man, published in 1871, he devoted hundreds of pages to sexual selection, which he thought could explain two of the animal kingdom's most conspicuous and puzzling features, weaponry and adornment. Sometimes, males competing fiercely for females would enter a sort of evolutionary arms race,
Starting point is 00:17:26 developing ever greater weapons, tusks, horns, antlers, as the best endowed males of each successive generation reproduced at the expense of their weaker peers. In parallel, among species whose females choose the most attractive males based on their subjective tastes, males would evolve outlandish sexual ornaments. It's now well known that all sexes exert numerous different evolutionary pressures on one another, and that in some species, males choose ornamented females. But to this day, many of the best studied examples are of female preference and male display. Unlike natural selection, which preserved traits that were useful in the struggle for life,
Starting point is 00:18:10 Darwin saw sexual selection as exclusively concerned with reproductive success, often resulting in features that jeopardized an animal's well-being. The peacock's many-eyed oriole, mesmerizing yet cumbersome, was a prime example and remains the mascot of sexual selection today. A great number of male animals, Darwin wrote, as all our most gorgeous birds, some fishes, reptiles and mammals, and a host of magnificently colored butterflies, have been rendered beautiful for beauty's sake. have been rendered beautiful for beauty's sake. Darwin's peers embraced the idea of well-armed males dueling for sexual dominance, but many scorned the concept of animal aesthetics,
Starting point is 00:18:58 in part because it was grounded in animal consciousness and female desire. In one critique, the English biologist St. George Mivart stressed, the fundamental difference which exists between the mental powers of man and brutes, and the inability of vicious feminine caprice to create enduring colors and patterns. The English naturalist Alfred Russell Wallace, who independently formed many of the same ideas about evolution as Darwin, was also deeply critical. Wallace was particularly tormented by Darwin's suggestion of beauty without utility. The only way in which we can account for
Starting point is 00:19:32 the observed facts is by the supposition that color and ornament are strictly correlated with health, vigor, and general fitness to survive, Wallace wrote. In other words, ornamentation could be explained only as a heuristic that animals use to judge a potential mate's fitness, a view that came to dominate. In the early 1980s, while researching the history of sexual selection, Prom read a seminal 1915 paper and a 1930 book on the subject by the English biologist and statistician Ronald Fisher, who buttressed Darwin's original idea with a more sophisticated understanding of heredity.
Starting point is 00:20:14 At first, Fisher argued, females might evolve preferences for certain valueless traits, like bright plumage, that just happened to correspond with health and vigor. bright plumage that just happened to correspond with health and vigor. Their children would tend to inherit the genes underlying both their mother's preference and their father's trait. Over time, this genetic correlation would reach a tipping point, creating a runaway cycle that would greatly exaggerate both preference and trait, glorifying beauty at the expense of the male's survival. In the early 1980s, the American evolutionary biologists Russell Landy and Mark Kirkpatrick gave Fisher's theory a formal mathematical girding, demonstrating quantitatively that runaway sexual selection could happen in nature
Starting point is 00:20:59 and that the ornaments involved could be completely arbitrary, conveying no useful information whatsoever. Although Fischerian selection was certainly not ignored, it was ultimately overshadowed by a series of hypotheses that seemed to rescue beauty from purposelessness. First, the Israeli biologist Amot Sahavi proposed a counterintuitive idea called the Handicap Principle, which put a new spin on
Starting point is 00:21:26 Wallace's utilitarian explanation for sexual ornaments. Extravagant ornaments, Zahavi argued, were not merely indicators of advantageous traits, as Wallace had said. They were a kind of test. If an animal thrived despite the burden of an unwieldy or metabolically expensive ornament, then that animal had effectively demonstrated its vigor and proved itself worthy of a mate. Similarly, in 1982, the evolutionary biologists W.D. Hamilton and Marlene Zuck proposed that some ornaments, in particular bright plumage, signaled that a male was resilient against parasites and would grant his children the same protection. Many scientists began to think of sexual selection as a type of natural selection. Scores of researchers joined the hunt for measurable benefits of
Starting point is 00:22:18 choosing an attractive mate, both direct benefits, like better parenting or more desirable territory, Both direct benefits, like better parenting or more desirable territory, and indirect benefits, namely some evidence that more alluring males really did have good genes underlying various desirable qualities, like disease resistance or higher-than-average intelligence. After more than 30 years of searching, most biologists agree that although these benefits exist, their prevalence and importance is uncertain. A few compelling studies of frogs, fish, and birds have shown that females who choose more attractive males typically have children with more robust immune systems and a greater chance of survival. On the whole, however, the evidence has not equaled the enthusiasm. A 2012 meta-analysis of 90 studies on 55 species found only equivocal support for the good genes hypothesis. Prom thinks the evidence for the heritable benefits of choosing a beautiful mate is scant
Starting point is 00:23:21 because such benefits are themselves rare, whereas arbitrary beauty is nearly ubiquitous. Over the years, the more he contemplated runaway selection, the more convinced he became that it was a far more powerful and creative evolutionary force than natural selection, which he regards as overhyped and boring. Animals are agents in their own evolution, he told me during one conversation. Birds are beautiful because they are beautiful to themselves. In the summer of 1985, around the same time that biologists were rekindling their interest in sexual selection,
Starting point is 00:24:02 Prum and the nature documentarian Anne Johnson, who would later choose him as her husband, traveled to Ecuador to continue studying mannequins. The first morning, while hiking through a cloud forest, Prum heard odd bell-like notes, which he took to be
Starting point is 00:24:17 the murmurings of parrots. Later that day, on the same trail, he heard the strange sounds again and followed them into the forest. He was astonished to find that the source was a male club-winged mannequin, a small cinnamon-bodied species with a red cap and black and white mottled wings. The mannequin was jumping around in a showy manner that suggested he was courting females.
Starting point is 00:24:41 Instead of singing with his throat, he repeatedly lifted his wings behind his back and vibrated his feathers furiously against one another, producing two electronic blips followed by a shrill buzzing ring, a sound Prom describes as bip-bip-wang. At the time, Prom had not fully developed his evolutionary theory of beauty, but he immediately suspected that the club-winged mannequin was emblematic of nature's capacity for pushing creatures to aesthetic extremes. The bird's singular vibrato haunted him for years. In the early 2000s, when Prom had become a professor of biology at the University of Kansas, he and his graduate
Starting point is 00:25:22 student, Kimberly Bostwick, revealed that the demands of courtship had drastically altered the bird's anatomy, turning it into a living violin. Male club-winged mannequins had feathers with contorted shafts that rubbed against each other 100 times a second, faster than a hummingbird beats its wings. Whereas a vast majority of birds have light, hollow bones in service of flight, Bostwick has recently shown via CT scans that male club-winged mannequins have solid ulnas, wing bones, which they need to withstand the intense quivering. Female mannequins have inherited related anomalies as well. Although there are no published studies of the club-winged mannequin's aeronautics, Prom says it's obvious from observation that the birds fly awkwardly,
Starting point is 00:26:11 even the females. The self-perpetuating pressure to be beautiful, Prom argues, has impeded the survival of the entire species. Because the females do not court males, there can be no possible advantage to their warped bones and feathers. Some of the evolutionary consequences of sexual desire and choice in nature are not adaptive, Prum writes in his recent book. Some outcomes are truly decadent. In the following decade, as Prum's hearing declined, he withdrew from field research and birding. But he still managed to make a series of groundbreaking scientific discoveries. He helped confirm that feathers evolved in dinosaurs long before the emergence of birds.
Starting point is 00:26:57 And he became one of the first scientists to deduce the colors of a dinosaur's plumage by examining pigment molecules preserved in fossilized feathers. All the while, he never stopped thinking about sexual selection. Prum formally presented his theory of aesthetic evolution in a series of scientific papers published between 1997 and 2015, proposing that all sexual ornaments and preferences should be regarded as arbitrary until proven useful. Despite his recent Pulitzer nomination, Prum still stings from the perceived scorn of his academic peers. But after speaking with numerous researchers in the field of sexual
Starting point is 00:27:39 selection, I learned that all of Prum's peers are well aware of his work and that many already accept some of the core tenets of his argument namely that natural and sexual selection are distinct processes and that, in at least some cases beauty reveals nothing about an individual's health or vigor At the same time, nearly every researcher I spoke to said that Prum inflates the importance of arbitrary preferences and fisherian selection to the point of eclipsing all other possibilities. In conversation, Prum's brilliance is obvious, but he has a tendency to be dogmatic,
Starting point is 00:28:18 sometimes interrupting to dismiss an argument that does not agree with his own. Although he admits that certain forms of beauty may be linked to survival advantages, he does not seem particularly interested in engaging with the considerable research on this topic. When I asked him which studies he thought offered the strongest support of good genes and other benefits, he paused for a while before finally responding
Starting point is 00:28:42 that it was not his job to review the literature. Like Darwin, Prom is so enchanted by the outcomes of aesthetic preferences that he mostly ignores their origins. Toward the end of our bird walk at Hammonasset Beach State Park, we got to talking about club-winged mannequins. I asked him about their evolutionary history. mannequins. I asked him about their evolutionary history. Prom thinks that long ago, an earlier version of the bird's courtship dance incidentally produced a feathery susurration. Over time, this sound became highly attractive to females, which pressured males to evolve adaptations that made their rustling feathers louder and more noticeable, culminating in a quick-winged strumming. But why, I asked Prom, would females be attracted to those particular sounds in the first place?
Starting point is 00:29:30 To Prom, it was a question without an answer, and thus a question not worth contemplating. Not everything, he said, has this explicit causal explanation. Prum's indifference to the ultimate source of aesthetic taste leaves a conspicuous gap in his grand theory. Even if we were to accept that most beauty blooms from arbitrary preferences, say, a warbling mating call or bright yellow feathers, and that these predilections would have nothing to do with advantageous genes. Yet such inclinations are inarguably the product of an animal's neurobiology, which is itself the result of a long evolutionary history that has adapted the animal's brain and sensory organs to specific environmental conditions. In the past two decades, a cohort of biologists have dedicated themselves
Starting point is 00:30:51 to studying how an animal's sensory bias, its ecological niche, and its particular way of experiencing the world, sculpts its appearance, behavior, and desires. Like prom, they don't think beauty has to be adaptive. But where prom celebrates caprice, they seek causality. Molly Cummings, a professor of integrative biology at the University of Texas at Austin, is a leading researcher in the field of sensory ecology. When I visited her last spring, she drove us to one of her field laboratories, a grassy clearing populated with several large concrete basins. The surface of one basin was so packed with woolly algae and pink-flowered water lilies that we could hardly see the water.
Starting point is 00:31:38 Cummings began pushing some of the vegetation out of the way, forming shady recesses that permitted our gaze at the right angle. Let me see if I can find a big, beautiful boy, she said. A paperclip-sized fish swam toward us. I leaned in for a closer look. His silver body was decorated with a single black dot and a stripe of iridescent blue. His lengthy tail, shaped like a knight's blade, was streaked with yellow. Oh yeah, there's a guy, Cording, Cummings said. He's coming up to that female trying to impress her. The fish, a male swordtail, seemed almost manic in his effort to be noticed. He darted back and forth in front of the female, shimmying as he went, his scales reflecting whatever light managed
Starting point is 00:32:25 to breach the murk. A little while later, we drove the few miles back to her campus laboratory, where shelves of fish tanks lined several rooms and Ernst Haeckel's resplendent illustrations of jellyfish undulated across the walls. As we toured the facilities, Cummings told me about the arc of her career. While an undergraduate at Stanford University, she spent a summer scuba diving in the giant kelp forests at Hopkins Marine Station, adjacent to the world-renowned Monterey Bay Aquarium. where she studied marine ecology and discovered the work of the biologists John Lithgow and John Endler, both of whom were interested in how the type of light in an animal's environment shaped its visual system. Cummings thought about the fish she had observed in California and Australia. She was astounded by the dynamic beauty of surf perch in the kelp forest,
Starting point is 00:33:31 the way they communicate through the color and brightness of their skin, flashing blue, silver, and orange to attract mates. Equally impressive was the diversity of their aquatic habitats. Some patches of water were sparkling and clear. Others were cloudy with algal muck. In Australia, sunlight bathed the many vibrant species of reef fish almost constantly, but they lived against a kaleidoscopic backdrop of coral. How did fish evolve effective and reliable sexual ornaments if the lighting and scenery in their homes were so variable? After earning a postgraduate degree in Australia in 1993, Cummings began a PhD at the University of California, Santa Barbara.
Starting point is 00:34:06 For several years, she studied various species of surf perch, repeatedly diving in the kelp forests with a plexiglass-protected spectrometer to quantify and characterize the light in different habitats. At night, she would use powerful diving lights to stun surf perch and take them back to the lab, evading the hungry seals that routinely trailed her in hopes of making a meal of the startled fish. After hundreds of dives and careful measurements, Cummings discovered that water itself had guided the evolution of Piscine beauty. A female's preference for a blaze of silver or blue was not arbitrary. It was a consequence of the particular wavelengths of light
Starting point is 00:34:46 that traveled farthest through her underwater niche. Whichever males happened to have scales that best reflected these wavelengths were more likely to catch the eye of females. In her studies, Cummings showed that surf perch living in dim or murky waters generally preferred shiny ornaments, while surf perch inhabiting zones of mercurial brightness favored bold colors. Later, Cummings found that Mexican swordtails occupying the upper layers of rivers, where the clear water strongly polarized incoming sunlight,
Starting point is 00:35:19 had ornaments that were specialized to reflect polarized light, like a stripe of iridescent blue. These findings parallel similar studies, suggesting that female guppies in Trinidad prefer males with orange patches because they first evolved a taste for nutritious orange tree fruits that occasionally fell into the water. Some people think female preferences just somehow emerge, Cummings says. But what has been overlooked is that in many cases it's a result of environmental constraints. It's not always random. What a creature finds attractive depends on more than the unique qualities of its environment, however.
Starting point is 00:36:04 Attraction is also defined by which of those qualities cross the threshold of awareness. Consider the difference between what we see when we look at a flower and what a bumblebee sees. Like us, insects have color vision. Unlike us, insects can also perceive ultraviolet light. Many plants have evolved flower parts that absorb or reflect ultraviolet light, forming patterns like rings, bullseyes, and starbursts. Most creatures are oblivious to these ornaments, but to the eyes of many pollinators, they are unmistakable beacons. There is an entire dimension of floral beauty invisible to us,
Starting point is 00:36:42 not because we are not exposed to ultraviolet light, but because we do not have the proper biological hardware to perceive it. Michael Ryan, a professor of zoology whose lab and office are just a few floors below Cummings, has spent more than 30 years investigating how the quirks of an animal's anatomy determine its aesthetic preferences, a career he details in his recent book, A Taste for the Beautiful. Since 1978, Ryan has been traveling to Panama to study a mud-colored frog called the Tungara. Like the club-winged mannequin, the Tungara has a unique form of beauty that is not visual, but aural. At dusk, male Tungara frogs gather at the edges of puddles
Starting point is 00:37:27 and sing to seduce females. Their mating call has two elements. The main part, dubbed the wine, sounds precisely like a miniaturized laser gun. Sometimes this is followed by one or more brief barks, known as chucks. A long and complex mating call is risky. It attracts frog-eating bats. Yet there is a high payoff. Ryan has shown that wines, followed by chucks, are up to five times as appealing to females as wines alone. But why? According to the adaptive model of beauty, the chucks must convey something about the male's fitness. As it happens, larger males, which produce the deepest and sexiest chucks, are also the most adept at mating because they are closer in size to females.
Starting point is 00:38:17 Frog sex is a slippery affair, and a diminutive male is more likely to miss his target. Moreover, the Tungara frog has an inner organ tuned to 2,200 hertz, which is close to the dominant frequency of a chuck. Together, these facts seem to indicate that the Tungara's puddle-side serenade is an example of adaptive mate choice. Females' evolved ears tuned to chucks because they indicate the biggest and most sexually skilled males. Ryan's research revealed a stranger's story.
Starting point is 00:38:51 When he examined the Tungara frog's family tree, he discovered that eight frog species closely related to the Tungara also have inner ear organs sensitive to frequencies of about 2,200 hertz. Yet none of them produce chucks in their mating call. Ryan thinks that eons ago, the ancestor of all these species probably evolved an inner ear tuned to roughly 2,200 hertz for some long-abandoned purpose. The Tungara later revived this neglected auditory channel, probably by happenstance. Male frogs that happened to burp
Starting point is 00:39:25 out a few extra notes after whining were automatically favored by females, not because they were more suitable mates, but simply because they were more noticeable. Like the glistening scales on the surf perch and sword tails that Cummings studied, the Tungara's costly mating call did not evolve to convey any pragmatic information about health or fitness. But that doesn't mean that these traits were arbitrary. They were the result of specific, discernible aspects of the animal's environments, anatomy, and evolutionary legacy. I took a real beating when I suggested this idea in 1990, Ryan says. It was very widely criticized, but now sensory bias is considered an important part of the evolution of these preferences. During our walk at Hammonacid,
Starting point is 00:40:13 while admiring seabirds from shoreside cliffs, I asked Prum about sensory bias. He said it could not possibly explain the staggering diversity and idiosyncrasy of sexual ornaments. The fact that every closely related sparrow species has a unique embellishment, for example. Prom sees sensory bias as just another way to maintain the predominant adaptive paradigm that refuses to acknowledge his theory of aesthetic evolution. Tellingly, Prom and Ryan do not discuss each other's work in their recent books. While mulling over the similarities and discrepancies between Prum's ideas and those of his peers, I kept returning to a passage in his book. In 2010, Prum and his colleagues revealed
Starting point is 00:40:58 that a crow-sized dinosaur called Ankyornis huxleyi was beautifully adorned. Gray body plumage, an auburn mohawk, and long white limb feathers with black spangles. Why dinosaurs originally evolved feathers has long perplexed scientists. At first, layers of fuzzy filaments, similar to a chick's down, most likely helped dinosaurs repel water and regulate body temperature. But what explains the development of broad, flat feathers like those found on Anchiornis? Flight is the intuitive answer, but the first planar feathers were probably too primitive for flight or gliding, lacking the distinct asymmetry that makes birds' feathers aerodynamic. In his book, Prum advocates for an alternative hypothesis that has been gaining support.
Starting point is 00:41:47 Large feathers evolved to be beautiful. The aesthetic possibilities of fuzzy down are limited. The innovative planar feather vein, however, creates a well-defined two-dimensional surface on which it is possible to create a whole new world of complex color patterns within every feather, Prom writes. Only later did birds co-opt their big, glamorous plumes for flight, which is probably a key reason that some of them survived mass extinction 66 million years ago. Birds transformed what was once mere frippery into some of the most enviable adaptations on the planet, from the ocean-spanning breadth of an albatross to the torpedoed silhouette of a plunging falcon.
Starting point is 00:42:31 Yet they never abandoned their sense of style, using feathers as a medium for peerless pageantry. A feather, then, cannot be labeled the sole product of either natural or sexual selection. not be labeled the sole product of either natural or sexual selection. A feather, with its reciprocal structure, embodies the confluence of two powerful and equally important evolutionary forces, utility and beauty. Most of the scientists I spoke with said that the old dichotomy between adaptive adornment and arbitrary beauty, between good genes and Fischerian selection, is being replaced with a modern conceptual synthesis that emphasizes multiplicity. Beauty is something that arises from a host of different mechanisms,
Starting point is 00:43:16 says Gil Rosenthal, an evolutionary biologist at Texas A&M University and the author of the new scholarly tome, Mate Choice. It's an incredibly multi-layered process. in maladaptive ways. Beauty reveals that evolution is neither an iterative chiseling of living organisms by a domineering landscape nor a frenzied collision of chance events. Rather, evolution is an intricate clockwork of physics, biology, and perception in which every moving part influences another in both subtle and profound ways. Its gears are so innumerable and dynamic, so susceptible to serendipity and mishap, that even a single outcome of its ceaseless ticking
Starting point is 00:44:12 can confound science for centuries. On my last day in Austin, while walking through a park, I encountered a common grackle hunting for insects in the grass. His plumage appeared black as charcoal at first, but as he moved, it shimmered with all the colors of an oil slick. Every now and then, he stopped in place, inflated his chest, and made a sound like a rusty swing set. Perhaps dissatisfied with the local fare or uncomfortable with my presence, he flew off. In his absence, my attention immediately shifted to something his presence had obscured. A golden columbine bush. From a distance, its flowers resembled medieval illustrations of comets,
Starting point is 00:45:01 big and bold with long trailing streamers. Up close, I was struck by the complexity of a single blossom. A large yellow star wreathed a cluster of five tubular petals, shaped like angels' trumpets and pooled with nectar. A tuft of pollen-tipped filaments fizzed through the very center. Viewed from above, the flowers looked like
Starting point is 00:45:22 huddles of tiny birds, with their beaks pressed together and wings flared. The name Columbine comes from the Latin for dove-like. Why are flowers beautiful? Or more precisely, why are flowers beautiful to us? The more I thought about this question, the more it seemed to speak to the nature of beauty itself. The more I thought about this question, the more it seemed to speak to the nature of beauty itself. Philosophers, scientists, and writers have tried to define the essence of beauty for thousands of years.
Starting point is 00:45:55 The plurality of their efforts illustrates the immense difficulty of this task. Beauty, they have said, is harmony, goodness, a manifestation of divine perfection, a type of pleasure, that which causes love and longing. And M equals O over C, where M is aesthetic value, O is order, and C is complexity. Evolutionary psychologists, eagerly applying adaptive logic to every facet of behavior and cognition, have speculated that the human perception of beauty emerges from a set of ancient adaptations. Perhaps men like women with large breasts and narrow waists because those features signal high fertility. Symmetrical faces may correlate with overall health. Maybe babies are irresistibly cute because their juvenile features activate the caregiving circuits in our brains.
Starting point is 00:46:50 Such claims sometimes verge on the ludicrous. The philosopher Dennis Dutton has argued that people around the world have an intrinsic appreciation for a certain type of landscape, a grassy field with copses of trees, water, and wildlife, because it resembles the Pleistocene savannas where humans evolved. In a TED Talk, Dutton explains that postcards,
Starting point is 00:47:13 calendars, and paintings depicting this universally beloved landscape usually include trees that fork near the ground because our ancestors relied on their conveniently low branches to scramble away from predators. Of course, it is undeniable that we, like all animals, are products of evolution. Our brains and sensory organs are just as biased as any other creatures. Our inherited anatomy, physiology, and instincts have undoubtedly shaped our perception of beauty. In their recent books, Richard Prum and Michael Ryan synthesize research on animals and people, exploring possible evolutionary explanations for our own aesthetic tastes. Ryan is particularly interested in the innate sensitivities and biases of our neural architecture.
Starting point is 00:48:02 He describes how our visual system, for example, may be wired to notice symmetry. Prom stresses his conviction that in humans, as in birds, many types of physical beauty and sexual desire have arbitrarily co-evolved without reference to health or fertility. What complicates their respective arguments is the overwhelming power of human culture. As a species, we are so thoroughly saturated with symbolism, ritual, and art,
Starting point is 00:48:31 so swayed by rapidly changing fashions, that it is more or less impossible to determine just how much an aesthetic preference owes to evolutionary history as opposed to cultural influence. Perhaps more than any other object of aesthetic obsession, flowers expose the futility of trying to contain beauty in a single theoretical framework. Consider how flowers came to be and how we grew to love them. 150 million years ago, many pollen-producing plants depended on the wind to spread their pollen and reproduce. But certain insects, perhaps beetles and flies, began to eat those protein-rich pollen grains,
Starting point is 00:49:13 inadvertently transporting them from one plant to another. This proved to be a much more efficient means of fertilization than capricious air currents. Plants with the richest and most obvious sources of pollen were especially successful. Likewise, insects that were particularly adept at finding pollen had an advantage over their peers. Through a long process of coevolution, plants and pollinators transformed one another. Some plants began to modify their leaves into proto-flowers, little flags that marked the location of their pollen. Bold colors and distinctive shapes helped them stand out in a tangle of green. Strong aromas and ultraviolet beacons played upon
Starting point is 00:49:57 pollinators' senses. Nectar sweetened the deal. Insects, birds, and mammals began competing for access, evolving wings, tongues, and brains better suited to the quest for floral sustenance. As the pressure from both parties intensified, plants and their pollinators formed increasingly specific relationships, hurtling each other toward aesthetic and adaptive extremes. A bird that hums and hovers like an insect, an orchid that mimics thes and hovers like an insect. An orchid that mimics the appearance and scent of a female bee.
Starting point is 00:50:34 Many millions of years later, flowers enchanted yet another species. Perhaps the initial attraction was purely utilitarian. The promise of fruit or grain. Maybe we were captivated by their consonants of color, form, and aroma. Whatever the case, we adopted numerous flowering plants into an expanding circle of domesticated species. We brought them into greenhouses and laboratories, magnifying their inherent beauty, creating new hybrids and tailoring their features to our individual tastes. We contracted orchid delirium and tulip mania, and we have never fully recovered. The flower began as a plea and became a phenomenon. If there is a universal
Starting point is 00:51:15 truth about beauty, some concise and elegant concept that encompasses every variety of charm and grace in existence, we do not yet understand enough about nature to articulate it. What we call beauty is not simply one thing or another, neither wholly purposeful nor entirely random, neither merely a property nor a feeling. Beauty is a dialogue between perceiver and perceived. Beauty is the world's answer to the audacity of a flower. It is the way a bee spills across the lip of a yawning buttercup. It is the care with
Starting point is 00:51:54 which a satin bowerbird selects a hibiscus bloom. It is the impulse to recreate water lilies with oil and canvas. It is the need to place roses on a grave. © transcript Emily Beynon

There aren't comments yet for this episode. Click on any sentence in the transcript to leave a comment.