The Joe Walker Podcast - Robert Boyd & Peter Richerson — How Ice Age Climate Chaos Made Humans Cultural Animals
Episode Date: August 13, 2024Robert Boyd and Peter Richerson are anthropologists based in America. Their partnership was central to the development of Dual-Inheritance Theory, a framework that applies Darwinian evolution to cultu...re and explains how genes and culture have intertwined to shape our species. This is their first ever joint interview. Full transcript available at: josephnoelwalker.com/boyd-and-richerson/See omnystudio.com/listener for privacy information.
Transcript
Discussion (0)
Today, it is my distinct honor to be speaking with Peter Richardson and Robert Boyd.
They are two anthropologists widely regarded as the co-founders of the field of cultural evolution.
And they've made probably the most significant augmentation to Darwinian evolution since
the Neo-Darwinian synthesis, which merged Darwinian evolution
and Mendelian genetics. So it's really special to speak with him today. And in fact, it's actually
a triple honor because I think this is also, we were speaking beforehand, but I think this is
your first ever podcast interview, maybe even your first ever joint interview.
That's right.
Yeah.
So Pete and Rob, welcome to the podcast.
Thank you.
Happy to be here.
So, first question, the oldest tools we've found are about 2.6 million years old, the
Alderwin tools.
Yeah, some people think about 3.1 or 3.3.
3.3 at Lomekwi.
For the Alduan tools?
They're not Alduan.
They're even simpler than Alduan tools.
Interesting.
They were probably made by the way Kanzi made tools by flinging stones at a hard surface and picking up the sharp flakes and using the flakes.
Napping.
No, not napping in the sense that napping involves using two hands
to hold the core in one hand and knock off flakes.
That's an older one.
There's a term for this, for flinging.
I forget what it is.
So even simpler than nothing.
Even simpler.
And the cores are huge.
They're like two and a half feet across.
And there was some controversy originally about whether they had the dates right because of, you know,
geological considerations at the site.
But now I think pretty much everyone agrees that there are tools there at 3.3
and there are bones with cut marks just a few kilometers away at another site in Ethiopia.
And so 3.3. 3.3 yeah that's fascinating i didn't know we'd found evidence of tools from that long ago yeah but i did certainly know about
those cut marks so they're they found cut and scrape marks on the bones of bovid and an ungulate in this site in Ethiopia. Yeah, Ghana. Right.
So something was using a tool to cut flesh from bones 3.4 million years ago.
Right.
We also know from fossils found about 3.2 million years ago
that australopiths had evolved precision grip in their hands.
So clearly something was applying selection pressure on their
hands to become more dexterous. So my first, I guess, substantive question is,
how likely is it that cumulative cultural evolution was already underway with Australopithecines?
There is, I've just read a paper, I forget the authors. They decided, it seemed kind of arbitrary to me, that it takes, if a cumulative culture, the signature of it is more than six steps in the manufacture of a tool.
Now, why six rather than two?
I don't know. It wasn't convincing to me. So, by their account, by the olden times or later even, the tools were too simple to, or something that somebody could reinvent for themselves.
That's the argument. Now, I'm not sure I quite believe it, but so that's a kind of approach
that people are taking to trying to find a signature for cumulative culture. Go ahead.
The other bit of evidence, so I think it's a mistake to think of cumulative
culture as either there or not there. I mean, when the psychology evolved, presumably there's
a bunch of steps. And so it's a fact that there are lots of australopithecine sites with no cut marks and no tools and australopithecine fossils.
So one model you could have that fits the data is there was some kind of psychology that let little bubbles of technology evolve,
and then they would persist for some period of time and then they'd be lost.
And lots of models have that property.
If the error rate of learning is high enough, you can easily go backwards.
And maybe the technology was only useful in certain habitats, certain things.
And that's also possible.
I mean, we know that chimpanzees and other tool-making animals sometimes have two or three or more steps involved in making the tools.
So, by my way of thinking, it's just a continuum.
And great apes are pretty smart.
Australopithecines must have been pretty smart.
And they probably made all sorts of tools that leave no record.
And so we don't have, I mean, the stone tools are sort of like looking at the cumulative culture record through a keyhole.
They're wonderful things because stone is so durable, but they're a tiny window into probably what they were doing just to look at chimpanzees and other tool-making modern creatures. And as you say, the hands of Australopithecines became rather modern looking.
So to me, it's a big mystery what most Australopithecines were doing with,
as Rob said, most of them weren't making any tools that we recover anyway.
Like stone tools.
Exactly.
So what were they doing with those hands
well chimps make all kinds of stuff and right you know
and they don't have hands that are really well adapted to uh to uh making things like uh like
stone tools and when you get to early homo so that's say two million years ago 1.8 million years ago
or something like that then you never find fossils without stone tools so there's a difference
between the australopithecine archaeological record and early homo homo erectus depending
on how you want to it's unclear how to categorize all those fossils.
So what were australopithecines doing with their hands?
That's an excellent question.
I mean, pretty clearly, at least sometimes,
they were using them to make tools.
But they might have been using them to carry things.
Rob had this idea a long time ago that australopithecines are, you know, small, not very fast, out in fairly open countries.
So what they might have been doing is going around in big mobs,
carrying sticks and stones to defend themselves against lions and giant hyenas and the other nasty predators who are out there.
Carl Butzer has an old book in which he has a plate in it of a drawing of a
chimpanzee with a great big rock.
I mean, a chimpanzee, an Australopithecine with a great big rock,
threatening to chuck it at something.
There is this idea about throwing.
So what's that guy's name at Stony Brook?
The stone throwing hypothesis?
Yeah, exactly.
My proper now module is failing on me.
But people like Leslie Aiello and people who know the anatomy will tell you that the shoulder and back has been reorganized by early Homo and maybe Australopithecines for the purposes of, you know.
Throwing.
Overarm throwing.
Overarm throwing.
And, you know, there's lots of, you can tell lots of stories.
We have, we don't know.
But I often thought, so I, when I was a young lad, I, we had rock throwing wars.
You know, we would get out.
There was this pond near my parents' house and different groups of kids start throwing rocks at each other.
And, you know, you can really get hurt with a sizable cobble. And if I were trying to scavenge from lions or hyenas,
throwing rocks at them would be a good strategy because they don't want to come chase you away
because they want to stay there eating. And there's a big free rider problem and you could stand off and make trouble for them
but again you know just who knows so pete you mentioned somebody has this argument that
it's unlikely australopithecines had achieved cumulative cultural evolution because
the tools that we can conclusively connect to them
are things that seemingly could be created
in the lifetime of one individual.
So individual learning is kind of sufficient
to explain those innovations.
Or at least it's at the margin.
At the margin.
So my rebuttal to that,
if I wanted to take the other side of that argument,
would be, sure, but their brains were much smaller than early human brains. Their brains were only a little bit larger than that of a chimpanzee. And so perhaps those tools weren't cultural creations that could be devised in the space of an individual lifetime for an individual with a brain of that size?
What would you make of that rebuttal?
It's quite possible, it seems to me.
And also, there's a kind of a quantitative.
Maybe one in a hundred australopithecines would invent a particular tool, but the other 99% of them got it from cultural transmission or from mom.
But no accumulation.
But not much accumulation at any rate.
So it just seems to me that we'd expect this to evolve gradually and there wouldn't be any,
any cut point.
We put it on cumulative culture,
be a bit arbitrary.
So that's my picture of it is as a brain's got bigger,
more information processing was possible.
And of course that would also mean that the
Homo erectus was probably more inventive. They could probably master things individually that
an Australopithecine, the average Australopithecine at least might depend upon
acquiring culturally. Yeah, and their brains were, I mean, depends who's doing the estimating,
but about 30% bigger corrected for body size than chimps.
Right.
But they show no trend.
So Australopithecine encephalization is completely flat
from three and a half million years ago to two million years ago.
And as soon as you get to early Homo, there's quite a steep slope.
So by a million years ago, you got creatures with brains, you know,
1,000 cc, 1,100 cc, something like that.
So something happened at the transition between Australopithecines and early Homo, which early Homo is a real mixed bag.
I mean, there are little tiny ones that are sort of Australopithecine size and there's big ones.
And it's not completely clear what was going on, you know, in terms of one species, several species.
That's all up in the air right now.
Yeah.
One of the things that is a potential causal factor is the onset of the Pleistocene climate fluctuations.
We still don't have any high-resolution data from between 3 and 1.5 million.
There's just a recent paper that reports a core that's 1.5 million years old.
And it's pretty clear that the millennial and sub-millennial scale variation is present in the 1.5 million years ago, but it's gotten more intense across the whole Pleistocene, or at least the last two-thirds or three-quarters of the Pleistocene.
And we don't know anything about the high-frequency variation before the onset of the Pleistocene.
In the Pleistocene.
There's a big gap in our knowledge at that critical period when the transition occurred.
But at least my speculation is that it was the onset of that millennial
and sub-millennial scale variation that started to push the encephalization.
Yeah. It's such a fascinating speculation.
I want to come back to that, and I have a few questions on that.
But before we do, a couple of other things first.
So I wanted to test an intuition with you.
Sometimes you hear people talk about the prospect of chimp scientists
and whether cumulative cultural evolution could
allow the possibility of having like chimp or australopith scientists. But my understanding
of the way the process works is that that is not how it would play out because as soon as the
autocatalytic feedback loop of social learning to cumulative cultural evolution spins into motion that puts
selection pressure on brains to grow larger and then by the time whatever the creature was
has evolved into being a scientist the species is no longer what it originally was whether that
was a chimp or an australopith, whatever.
Is that correct?
Science.
So, I mean, science as we know it is only a few hundred years old.
Yeah, I guess.
It's a particular.
That's a high standard.
That's a high standard.
Well, I mean, it's a particular social institution.
Right.
That has a bunch of rules about credit and priority
and other things that generated the world we live in, basically.
But before that, there are people doing experiments
and the cumulative culture all the way back.
So that's what you mean by science.
You mean the more could so could you have could you have a creature with a
small brain of like 400 centimeters cubed like a chimp or an australopithecine that because it
evolves cultural learning it can simultaneously be a small brain hominid and evolve some kind of cultural complex as intricate as the scientific
method. And I guess the intuition I had was that those things aren't compatible because
by the time it evolves the intricate cultural complex, it's already a different species
because of the pressure that that feedback loop would place on its brain size.
So, do you know the argument of this South African student
of tracking, Louis Lieberman?
I don't think so.
He'd be a great guy to interview.
Yeah, he'd be.
I don't know him.
I don't know him.
I've had some.
Yeah, go ahead.
Anyhow, he studied tracking by the Kalahari San people. And he argues that the roots of science are in the kinds of practices that they have.
So usually it's two, three, four guys tracking an animal.
And the signs are very ambiguous and slight, you know, bent grass blades and a strike of a hoof here and a bent twig where something brushed past a bush or something like that.
And so these guys are talking to one another all the time.
And their natural history knowledge is quite good, very good.
And so they know the animal that they're tracking.
They know what species it is.
They know how it moves and what it's likely to do.
And so they're using that background knowledge to generate hypotheses about what this particular animal is doing right now.
And they are using this to reach a decision about, do we continue to pursue this animal?
If they're tracking it, they don't have it in visual, they don't have it in sight.
So they're depending upon these thin clues to try to infer what it's doing.
Now, if they think it's wounded or old, then they might continue this pursuit for quite some time.
Days.
Even.
And on the other hand, if they think it's a perfectly healthy animal and can run four times as fast as they can,
they say, yeah, give up on this one.
We'll never catch it.
So they're prepared to spend a huge amount of effort if they think they've got a reasonable chance of success, but not otherwise.
And so, and, you know, they're just talking it out as they're running along, looking one sign after another.
And so this, he thinks, and the big thing is that the background knowledge they've got
a they've got a theory about what this animal uh might be doing and so they're testing that theory
with this rather skimpy evidence that they're they're running across and right so they're using
a form of like deductive inference yeah it's a scientific method kind of on the fly, out in the bush chasing, you know, a willow beast or something.
They help with these little low-powered bows, you know, tiny little 40, 50-pound full bows and poisoned arrows.
So they often have to track animals that are dying from the poison for long periods, for hours and hours.
And so it's a big part of their hunting strategy.
It's not like other people have bows that kill the animal on the spot, but not the huds, not the kung.
Actually, it's not the kung.
It's Central Kalahari people.
I can't remember the name of the group.
I can't pronounce it.
Exclamation point X-O.
And if you show cold, cold.
But we need Polly.
That's so interesting.
So, okay.
Last point for now on the kind of history of early humans,
but what's your kind of lower bound for when cumulative cultural evolution was
achieved in our lineage?
Like when can we say with some certainty that it's clear in the record?
Three to 500,000 years ago.
Middle Paleolithic.
Middle Paleolithic.
Yeah. You get prepared core tools
and lots of evidence, I think,
of people know how to make fire.
Yeah.
And this is with hymo erectus?
No, this is...
A little later than that, probably.
You know, who this is,
you can get in fights about what to call them.
But these are these robust, so sometimes people call them archaics.
So, proto-Neanderthals, proto-Denisevans.
Right.
Is this sometimes called Heidelbergensis?
Yeah, it's one term.
Sometimes people call it Heidelbergensis.
You can, in certain circles
you can get in a big fight about what to call these guys but uh but you're not gonna find rob
and i in that i mean you know they were morphologically they were heavyset compared
to us they made you know they're quite distinctive but you, what they were like cognitively, their brains are a little smaller, but not very much.
And by the end, they're bigger, actually, than our brains.
And so for then, for sure, if I had to bet, you know,
somebody introduces a time machine and makes me bet,
I would bet at least a million.
At least a million? At least a million?
At least a million.
But I think the evidence, there's this one site in Israel.
Yesha, kind of Yaakov or something?
Yeah, exactly.
That has all the organic remains.
Has organic remains.
There's a skeleton of an elephant.
They're eating acorns.
Fish, turtles. Fish, turtles.
Fish, turtles.
But eating acorns, depending on what kind of acorns they were, that can involve quite a bit of detoxification.
Yeah, you've got to leach them.
Leach the tannins out of the acorns.
Now, there are acorns without sweet acorns.
Sweet acorns.
So it could be that.
We don't know for sure.
But anyway, it's a site, but it's unique.
It's this anoxic site that preserved all this stuff.
And I watch guys making hand axes.
We have some very skilled nappers in my department,
and it looks pretty complicated. I don't think we know, actually. Right. we have some very skilled nappers in my department and
it looks pretty complicated
you know
I don't think we know actually
right
yeah
interesting
okay so
I'm going to break
all of my podcast rules
by getting you to ask
a pretty
sorry getting you to answer
a pretty basic question
yeah
not only that
but a three part
question
oh my goodness
just to so the audience has to remember three parts yeah exactly and you guys too basic question. Not only that, but a three-part question. Oh my goodness.
Just to... So the audience has to remember three parts.
Yeah, exactly.
And you guys too.
So we don't have to spend too much time on this,
but just to give everyone context before we move on,
could you briefly outline,
firstly, cultural evolution,
secondly, cumulative cultural evolution,
and thirdly, gene culture co-evolution sure
okay i'll take a crack and you can you know like in the wrestling you know
tag me and i'll you can jump in the ring but um uh so cultural evolution occurs when
uh an organism gets useful information from other organisms through some kind of social learning process.
So I think chimps have cultural evolution.
Lots of songbirds do.
Lots of animals have some kind of cultural evolution occurs when the process is sufficiently faithful or accurate, the social learning process, so that some individual invents something. individual learns enough about what that individual made and now changes it and other
individuals can learn the new variant. So lots of social learning processes don't involve the
transmission of variation within a population. You just get, you know, I mean, that's a great story.
Should I tell you?
So Hans Kummer was a very famous primatologist,
and he had a cat colony outside of Zurich.
And there were crab apples along the outside of the cage,
and the crab apples would drop,
and the monkeys were very interested in these crab apples.
And one of the monkeys learned to get a stick to drag the crab apples into the cage and eat them. This attracted a lot of attention and monkeys got very interested in sticks. So this is what's
called stimulus enhancement.
The fact that the sticks were associated with something that monkeys really wanted, the crab apples, made them, but no other monkey could put it together.
And look at monkey, the first monkey used the stick and drag it back.
This is a really rudimentary form of social learning.
This is real.
It's called stimulus enhancement.
It's really common in non-human animals.
Probably common enough in humans. It's common stimulus enhancement. It's really common in non-human animals. Probably common enough in humans.
It's common in humans too. Right.
But the thing is that unless I see, so you've innovated
something, unless I can copy
that innovation, not the
fact that you use sticks or the fact
that there's crab owls to eat, but the fact that you did
a particular thing,
that's what you need for accumulation.
Because now somebody else makes another improvement, somebody else makes another improvement.
Somebody makes it stick into a rake.
Yeah.
And you end up with something that's beyond the inventive capacity of individuals.
There's a very colorful psychologist, Claudio Teni, who has studied this a lot.
And he calls it the zone of latent solutions.
So the zone of latent solutions is that space of behaviors that individuals can generate based on the behavior in their lives with some social input.
You know, sticks are important.
But humans are able to get the details or the variants that somebody else
invents. And that leads to accumulation. And gene culture coalition happens anytime
culture of either kind changes the selective regime so that gene variants that weren't favored when in the
acultural, non-cultural species are now favored.
So, I mean, and that canory ungulates that have to learn their
migratory patterns by some form of social uh learning and there are these natural experiments
where the some migratory route has been stopped for some reason. Somebody built a fence, and then the fence gets torn down.
And it takes them a few generations to recover their original migratory pattern.
So this is something that's very important to their lives.
And yet, it's still a very simple form of culture.
How far do you migrate?
When do you do it?
Yeah, domain-specific.
Yeah, rather domain-specific.
Right. And then it seems to me that gene culture co-evolution comes in two flavors.
One, originally pointed out by E.O. Wilson, Wilson is that culture puts pressure on genes to control the cultural evolution, to influence
cultural evolution so that it increases genetic fitness.
And then there is, I think of it as culture-led gene culture coevolution, where the cultural variation is impressed on the genes
because the cultural environment generates selection on the genes that favors the cultural system.
I think of, well, modern penal institutions have this effect that if you're incarcerated, you're
less likely to reproduce.
So I think of chimpanzees as basically a society of psychopaths.
And in humans, we've got psychopathy down to a low roar, something like two or three percent of humans are clinically diagnosed as psychopathic.
And that's because I think for hundreds of thousands of years, if not longer, we have punished people who don't cooperate and rewarded people who do cooperate.
And this has transformed our social psychology. The difference between chimps and humans is that
humans are adapted to make massive use of cultural adaptations. And to do that, you need to have large societies. And one of the ways that we mobilize a lot of, well, two things I think are critical
to mobilizing resources.
One is technology and the other is social organization.
So we make a lot of profit out of being able to cooperate in ways that our competitors
can't. And a big, I mean, on a much sort of more simple scale,
a big fraction of the new genes that have been,
so you can identify genes that have been under selection
by looking for long haplotypes that show evidence of a gene being favored by selection
and carrying a bunch of the lateral genes along with it.
Yeah.
You know, I mean.
Linked genes, yeah.
Yeah, so there's a bunch of the lateral genes along with it. Yeah, link genes. Yeah, so there's a bunch of link genes.
And if there's some highly favored gene,
it drags a bunch of link genes with it.
And by looking for those long segments of chromosomes,
you can tell which genes were under selection.
And how long ago.
And how long ago.
And a lot of the ones that have been under recent selection
the last 10,000 years are genes that the been under recent strong selection
are ones that are plausibly related to environmental changes
that were culturally evolved.
That's really interesting because so often I'll hear cultural evolutionists
make a claim like, you know, ever since our species crossed the Rubicon
into cumulative cultural evolution, maybe that was a million years ago or whenever.
Yeah.
Ever since that point, the culture we've accumulated has become the main selection
pressure on our genes.
But my question was,
how is that statement actually quantified or quantifiable?
But maybe that suggests a way that you could...
Yeah, so we can't detect selection.
Well, there are ways,
but recent selection is a lot easier to detect
than selection 50,000 years ago.
And that's just because recombination
breaks up all these long haplotypes.
The long haplotypes are reduced to...
You can't see them.
There are other methods,
but they're much less reliable, I think.
So it could be that in spreading across the world
60,000 years ago,
there was lots of selection for all kinds of stuff,
but we have a hard time seeing it,
detecting it with genomic methods.
I mean, I think almost certainly moving from the Horn of Africa to the high Arctic must
have selected for some stuff, right?
I mean, well, or the morphological differences.
Yeah.
And cold adaptations.
Cold adaptations.
Well, the high altitude adaptations is another good story. But who knows how much historical information the geneticists will eventually be able to extract.
The innovations have been spectacular.
Well, ancient DNA, for example, spectacular innovations in the last few decades.
If we had populations of ancient DNA,
which is not out of the realm of possibility,
then we could do long haplotypes with ADNA.
I'm aware of that study Kevin Laland and a few others did a few years ago
where I think they found over 100, maybe it's more now,
but they'd found over 100 genes that plausibly had been shaped by or selected yeah by culture yeah yeah just to quickly go back to the definition of
cultural evolution how how much loss would there need to be in transmission before you said okay
that's no longer an evolutionary process i mean that depends, right? So, in experiments, there's a lovely experiment by a young cognitive scientist at Berkeley, Bill Thompson. had people learn to do a sorting algorithm. And then he offered them in one treatment,
well, there are three treatments. One, they had to do it on their own. Only a few people figured
it out, a few percent, 10% or something. And then they had social learning and they could see
other people. And then he gave them social learning plus the payoffs that the individuals who saw everybody's payoff, depending on what algorithm they used.
And the error rate in that experiment was about 50%.
So 50% of the people who copied, because he knows who copies who from the way the experiment was run, failed to get it right.
But if they were given the payoff information,
the best algorithm spread throughout the whole population
because the flow of information in was high enough to compensate
for this very high error rate.
Mutation rate, so to speak.
Whereas if you didn't get payoff information, the best algorithm just percolated along what
the invention rate was.
So people could use the success rate of others as a basis for biasing their…
And they just completely focused on the people who had the best algorithm.
And that whole process...
So I don't think it's just the accuracy.
You have to look at the processes
that are degrading information
as it's transmitted through time,
and then processes that are increasing
the quality of information.
So it's just a kind of a budget problem.
Yeah, I don't...
In-go and out- going, and the sort of equilibrium depends upon the relative rates of those two things.
I think you've got cumulative cultural evolution when things get better through time.
Now, obviously, once everybody's learned the good algorithm, they're going to stop getting better.
But it'll reach some steady state. Now, obviously, once everybody's learned the good algorithm, they're going to stop getting better.
But it'll reach some steady state. But if the potential is there for gradual improvement over generations, then I count that as cumulative cultural evolution.
Yeah.
Yeah.
Yeah, interesting.
So, back to gene culture, co-evolution.
Yeah. back to gene culture co-evolution yeah are there any for you any particularly surprising or
counterintuitive examples of gene culture co-evolution i think the one that fascinates
me the most is that the sclera of our eyes have probably evolved to aid with teaching because you can track the gaze of someone who's
teaching you if you can see the whites of their eyes and I guess it helps with social referencing
and infants as well but are there any other examples that I know the canonical you find
that counterintuitive not counterintuitive but just surprising or kind of fascinating. One of my favorite examples, it's a little bit peculiar,
but Japanese people eat seaweed in a fairly large part of their diet.
And it's not human genes that change,
but they harbor a bacterium in their gut that degrades the polysaccharides in seaweed.
And I mean, this gut flora that we have is a whole ecosystem that is presumably co-evolving with our genes and with our diets.
And obviously diets are culturally determined in large part. So that's a whole other field of co-evolution.
If you want, we've got this giant domesticated ecosystem in our guts.
But that hasn't genetically evolved.
That's downloaded through culture, approximately?
Well, who knows?
In other words, people in Japan are eating these seaweeds with polysaccharides that humans can't digest very well.
And is it they just acquire a wild bacterium that happens to have this capacity or does a capacity has a capacity evolved in human guts i don't i don't know i don't imagine anybody does but uh it's a
another whole dynamic system that involves uh gene culture co-evolution, not human genes. And I suppose it's a lot like domestic animals.
So there's a whole nother domestic plants and animals
or a whole nother co-evolutionary process
that involves human artificial selection.
Now, it's not deliberate selection in our gut,
I don't suppose, but...
Most domestication isn't either.
So the Siberian fox experiments,
probably the classic example there, right?
Maybe 40 generations.
Oh no, way fewer.
They got them.
There's a bunch of controversy about that now.
Oh, really?
Yeah.
What's the source of the controversy? Well, the story is that Vavilov, is that his name?
No, Vavilov was a plant guy.
What is, I can't remember.
Anyway, it doesn't matter.
They were just selecting for, they had one selection criteria, which was flight distance, basically, people would approach.
How close would they let people get?
And that they started with wild foxes.
And there's been a bunch of stuff recently saying neither of those things is really true.
There's been a lot of domestication of these foxes already.
And I haven't kept up with that carefully but but i mean we know that you know
dogs and cats and and cows and you know there's been lots of domestication and mainly by
stone age farmers who who weren't trying to domesticate anything, right? They were just keeping the ones they liked. And so, yeah.
Yeah, a friend of mine, his father was a superintendent of the National Bison Range in Montana or Wyoming.
So he grew up on this federal facility that was trying to propagate bison.
The only problem is that the bison are just wilder than can be compared to a cow.
And so they discovered that they were inadvertently domesticating the bison because particularly the bulls, a lot of them were really, really scary. And,
and you know, they broke out of fences, they squashed the cowboys.
They were, and so those are the ones that somehow ended up at the butcher more
often than the tamer was. And so they were trying to maintain the wild type because they wanted to release them into
the wild.
And ability to deal with predators would have been a desirable characteristic.
But their ability to deal badly with cowboys was costing them their lives.
So that's a domestication
story that I find fascinating.
It's hard not to
when you're dealing
with wild animals, it's hard not
to inadvertently
select them for
traits that make it easy to deal with them
at least, if not other things.
Yeah, that's really interesting.
Okay, so I promised we'd come back to Pleistocene climate variation.
So this is both fascinating and crucial for the mathematical models
you guys developed in the 70s and 80s.
They were, I guess, most famously published in the 85 book,
Culture and the Evolutionary Process.
I've learned a lot about pleistocene climate variation through reading your your work so you had these macro cycles in the first 1.5 million years those cycles lasted for
about 41 000 years in the last 41 000 so uh 23 000 years uh was a dominant cycle before the pleistocene
yeah and then the uh play applies to seeing transition uh brought on the uh dominance of
the 41 000 year cycle and then in the middle of the pleistocene, by about 800,000 years ago, the 100,000-year cycle became clearly dominant.
Yeah.
And then superimposed on those macro cycles, you had these shorter cycles.
And this is so interesting.
I didn't realize that every, say, 1,000 or so years in the shorter cycles in what we know as the ice ages like the
last ice age the temperature could actually go from glacial to interglacial over like a
almost interglacial not quite but so i mean what we think of as ice ages were actually interspersed
with quite quite warm periods so a huge amount of variation. But I have a bunch of questions about this.
But before we get to those, could you just paint a picture of what that would have been like on the ground in terms of the temperature, the rainfall in Africa, and then the variation in those dimensions?
Yeah, my picture of it is it was just completely chaotic.
The original paleontologists and paleobotanists that looked at this, they called them anomalous distributions.
So they would find things like reindeer and bison and horses in the same fossil beds.
Now, whether these were exactly contemporaneous or not is hard to say, but with those millennial and sub-millennial scale, high amplitude sub-millennial and millennial scale variation, the vegetation
and the rest of the ecology couldn't come to any kind of equilibrium insofar as there was ever any equilibrium.
So when the transition to the Holocene occurred and the climate got very much quieter,
it took a couple of 3,000 years before we got the modern biomes organized.
So the boreal forest and the tundra and the steppe, these stripes.
So the term of art was plaid versus striped patterns.
And so the Pleistocene, the last ice age, it seems, at least, was just hugely chaotic, particularly the last half of it.
But it's fair to say that the coal periods are drier, right?
Well, and I mean, a bunch of things.
He's the expert on this.
Well, I'm going to advance the amateur. But so the picture of it is, yeah, it was colder on average, drier on average, and of course, less CO2, which impacts plant growth rates. So it was, and it was this wildly varying system on a fairly short time scale.
Really short time scale.
And so that impacted humans.
Now, what the last ice age, of course, was the first place that this was discovered.
Well, Rob and I talked about saying something about this in the 85 book, and Rob talked
me out of it because the data was lousy.
There just wasn't a decent high-resolution record that resolved these millennial and
sub-millennial scale variations until, well, the publications
are in the early 90s.
The ice cores from Greenland were raised in the latest 80s, and then it took a few years
to get the data analyzed.
And so in 92, 93, this data came out.
And I mean, I thought we couldn't ask for better data than that.
It was just what the model suggested should be happening.
Yeah, so we were, maybe we should, well, in retrospect, we should have been bolder.
We should have predicted the millennial and sub-millennial scale variations on the grounds that humans wouldn't have evolved without them.
But what gave you, because that data, as you said, didn't come out until the mid-90s,
what gave you the hunch?
Well, there were cores drilled in Greenland, mostly along the edge of the ice because logistically to get out on the ice divide in the middle of
the Greenland ice sheet is a logistical big project.
And so the Europeans and the Americans in the late 80s organized the resources to go
out there and drill those two-mile-long cores down through.
So they drill on the ice divide.
So on the ice divide, the ice is not moving horizontally.
It's just the layers are getting thinner and thinner as you go down.
But away from the ice divide, it's, it's basically turbulent that, you know,
the ice is flowing and it's,
it's overturning and mixing up as it goes along.
So the, by the time you get to the ice margin, the record is lousy.
But it did seem to show a few cores did seem to show these big excursions,
but it was, as I say, it was not the kind of data that would convince a skeptic at all.
And also, I mean, you always argued that there is these, you know, variance propagation from
the fluid guys have from long timescales to shorter timescales.
And we knew that there was much more variation on 100,000-year timescales
than there ever had been before.
And so it seemed plausible.
I mean, Pete can come talk me into it.
There are these so-called variance cascades that I learned about this
because I collaborated with physical environmental sciences to get tenure, basically.
And so they taught me a bunch of stuff that I would never otherwise have learned.
So in the turbulent motions in the ocean, there's a huge amount of energy in the major currents, like the California current off our coast here.
And that energy then forms eddies that break down.
And so ultimately, at the bottom, at small scales, at centimeter scales,
friction absorbs all of that energy that's being put into the ocean,
basically by the wind. And so that there's a quite orderly cascade of variance from
low frequencies to high frequencies. And so you could imagine the same thing occurring on these 100,000-year timescales.
There'd be a variance cascade that would affect the higher frequencies.
But it's a metaphorical argument.
It's not a real – you're a physicist.
It wasn't really a mechanistic argument.
It was an analogy, if you want.
But it turned out to be right.
Turned out to be right.
Yeah. In a big way.
So, explain how culture is an adaptation to this climate variation.
Well, so, the culture evolves faster than genes.
That's the basic starting point of the argument. On the other hand,
culture seems to require this massive, at least the kind of culture we have, cumulative culture,
that requires this massive brain that's extremely costly in metabolic terms. And your head's fragile.
It could easily get hurt.
Some people even think that insanity comes from having too big a brain.
It gets confused sometimes.
So there are these major costs to having this system of cumulative culture and so the the question that rob and i posed in
our 85 book is what could this thing be good for i mean uh the way i phrase it in talks or to
students is the dinosaurs had it right you should have a smaller brain as you can get away with
uh it's too expensive to mess around with brains unless they're doing some real work for you.
So what could the real work be?
Brain tissue is like 20 times more energetically expensive than muscle tissue.
Yeah, something on that order.
Yeah.
And it contributes a lot to basal metabolism.
In other words, there's not muscles if you don't use them like we're sitting here.
We're not.
Our muscles are just ticking over, but our brain seems to be generating a lot of metabolic activity all the time.
I recall in your book, Not By Genes Alone, I think you have a statistic about the brain absorbs about 16% of a human's basal metabolic rate, but only about 3% in an average mammal.
Yeah.
Yeah.
That's 5%.
I think I can't remember the exact numbers.
So 3% in efficient mammals like opossums and the other small-brained mammals,
the little-brained mammals.
Yeah.
So the question then is how do we pay that overhead cost?
And so the one thing that will generate that overhead, pay that overhead cost is adaptation to spatial and temporal variation. If we can adapt more finely to
high frequency variation in time or adapt more efficiently to spatial variation on a finer scale,
then that will, in the models we made in the 85 book, that would be something that would
tend to pay that overhead cost. And especially in sort of multi-generation time scales. So,
the thing about ordinary learning will do stuff for you on individual lifetime scales or generational time scales.
But it gets lost, right, if you don't have cultural transmission.
And a lot of the variance seems to be in these kind of thousand-year, hundred-year time scales where culture is fast enough to keep up but preserves things long enough to be useful on those times.
That's so interesting.
So in a sense, then, cultural evolution economizes on individual learning.
We learn things socially, and the cost of innovation is spread over the whole population.
Right.
Everybody's doing a little bit of innovating and then sharing with the rest of us.
And that will pay this overhead cost.
That's really interesting.
So if the environment is varying slowly enough, be that in space or time.
Genes keep up fine.
Genes keep up fine.
If it's varying too quickly, the only way to keep up is individual learning.
Right.
Or individual phenotypic flexibility of one kind or another.
Right, right.
But if it's varying in this sweet spot of like a millennial or sub-millennial scale,
then culture is the best way to keep up.
That's our argument.
That's our story.
That's really interesting.
We're sticking to it.
Actually, there's maybe one other interesting timescale here,
which is if it's varying on an extremely quickly timescale,
like an hour-by-hour basis,
then that would go back to favoring genetic evolution,
right?
Oh, well, I think...
It would take like the average.
Oh, well, depends.
Yeah, that might...
Yes, so if the temperature of your environment goes up permanently, then,
but yes, if it's varying on
too rapid a timescale, then if you can adapt to the average, I mean,
if the temperature were going from, from,
from freezing to a thousand degrees on an hourly basis, I don't think anything could manage that.
But if it's something that you can buffer, we have rather large body size, so we can buffer
high frequency variations in temperature just because we have a large body size.
But you're thinking about things, I mean, it really has to do with the timescale of
individual learning. So, I mean, you think about, I mean, our irises adapt
to a varying environment on really short timescales because it's a really simple mechanism.
Genes have provided us with, you us with a photometer that does that.
But if it was about something else, it took several days to learn,
then things that vary on an hourly scale, then you're right.
It would just be a genetic adaptation to whatever the average was.
Right.
Or some, as Rob says about irises of eyes,
we have a lot of different phenotypic flexibility mechanisms.
So, you know, if you see a bus coming down the sidewalk,
you panic and get the hell out of the way.
So it doesn't have much to do with culture or individual learning.
It's just emergency decisions. Got it. Got it. So I find this functionalist analysis of culture so
appealing and interesting, but do you remember how you came up with it? Like, what was the genesis of the
idea? Well, on my part, I was a freshman faculty member, first-year faculty member in this
new department, and it was an interdisciplinary department meant to deal with environmental problems.
And one of my senior colleagues was a sociologist, and he'd been part of the planning for this new department.
And he put a course on the books called Principles of Human Ecology. And so then he got, he was designated
to teach it, but he decided he needed a natural scientist to help him teach this course. And so
I'd worked on a postdoctoral project with him. And so I'd done a certain amount of reading in
social sciences. And I knew that
there were these people who call themselves cultural ecologists who talked about cultural
adaptations. And so we decided, we had decided to make adaptation a central part of this course.
And so I went off to find out what these cultural ecologists had to say about how cultural adaptations work.
My training in evolutionary biology was fairly adequate.
And so I sort of knew where adaptations came from, genetic adaptations came from.
So where did cultural adaptations come from?
And what these guys, they had the concept of cultural adaptations, but they didn't have the concept of cultural evolution, at least not in anything like the way evolutionary biologists did.
Rob and I were teaching a different course together.
And so I was bugging him about this.
And one thing led to another.
Yeah. And we came upon, it was kind of in the air.
There were other people who had kind of related ideas.
Don Campbell.
Yeah.
Mark Feldman.
You introduced us in a way that some people would disagree with,
that we aren't the first or the most important.
You know, Mark and Luca would think of themselves that way.
And I'd read their papers,
and I actually don't think they had a theory of cultural adaptation at that point.
They weren't very interested in adaptation.
No, they were.
Anyway, so, and then Pete and I just started talking,
and we weren't very, so people likerew vita and and um skip rapaport and
marvin harris julian stewart lots of data on cultural adaptation lots of interesting stuff
but no mechanism that is plausible to us and so we just started talking and out it came
i think of it as just a sort of Darwinian straight and narrow.
Just marched down the same path that the geneticists had in the neo-Darwinian synthesis, in the modern synthesis.
So I want to play a counterfactual. Cool. Imagine in the next few years, we get some new data that stretches all the way back into the
Pleiocene that shows that climate variation has actually been continually increasing since
the Pleiocene right up through the Pleistocene. What's the most important story that would enable
us to tell? Would it enable us to tell some kind of grand story about how climate variation has been driving the evolution of brain size and intelligence on Earth? expect. In other words, doing what Rob and I didn't do in the 85 book, turning the question
around and arguing that brain size is a kind of a paleoclimate indicator. In other words,
brains are for coping with variable environments on different timescales, culture being, as you say, in the sweet spot. There is a sweet spot for culture.
So what, well, to go even further back, the human, excuse me, brain size in mammals generally
has been increasing for the last 65 million years
since the dinosaurs since the dinosaurs yeah and birds probably too but the birds don't uh
fossilize very well because they're so delicate to fly you have to have light bones and light
bones aren't very rugged and so the bird record is poor. And the mammal record is
quite poor. A guy named Harry Jerison at UCLA in
1973, his book I think, he
used naturally occurring fossil endocasts
to build a record of
brain size evolution for the last 65, 70 million years.
But it's a really crude record because fossil endocasts aren't very common.
And nowadays, somebody could go back and take all the skulls
and all of these collections and put them in an x-ray machine
and do computerized tomography and develop a much finer resolution set of data.
But I keep looking for this, and so far nobody that I have run across
has actually tried to do this. It's complicated because the other response to climate variation is to go small.
I mean, you know, so apes, there were hundreds of species of apes 20 million years ago.
And by 8 or 10 million years ago, most of them were gone and replaced by monkeys.
So apes came first and monkeys replaced apes mainly, not completely.
So, you know, if you can't predict, then just make a lot of offspring,
you know, the kind of strategy and,
and that's going to be overlaid on top of, so it, it, it.
Well, yes. And, and Garrison Garrison's data, how did he summarize it?
Many mammalian lineages got bigger brains, but by no means all of them.
So what's been driving that?
If it's been happening for 65 million years?
Well, I mean, what I think our models suggest is that, and models like them suggest, is that the world has been getting more variable on short timescales.
Now, the trouble is that there are no short timescale records that I know of that certainly not to cover the whole last 65 million years.
Now, I don't think you need that.
You could just have samples.
You could look at fossil tree rings or something like that
and develop samples of high-frequency climate variation,
old lake sediments, old marine sediments,
and not try to have a complete record, just spot samples would give you some indication.
You'd like to know the slope of the grain size versus time curve, right?
Because I think we just know the average. Well, Jarrison suggested that there was a long, slow, and then the transition from that to the Pleistocene was our story.
Myosin, Eosin variation being not that big, warm, wet planet.
And then all of a sudden in the late Myosin, things go to hell, basically.
And it gets colder and drier and more variable.
So, I mean, until 20 million years ago, there were tropical rainforests in Moscow.
Temperate rainforests in Antarctica.
Yeah, exactly.
So, it was a much more, now this is, of course, about primates who are forest specialists.
But then all of a sudden, you know, in the Miocene, things dry out.
You can't just live in the forest anymore.
It's a lot more variation.
And it's true.
I didn't know this about Gerasim's data.
If there's an acceleration.
Well, again, the crudity of that data might cause some skepticism, but that's what he
says.
Yeah.
That's what I predict.
Okay.
So interesting.
Let's stick my neck out here.
I like it.
That's so interesting.
Okay.
There is a great paper that you might look up by a geochemist at UC Santa Barbara and his co-authors,
I forget his first name, Zachos, C-A-C-H-O-S,
2001 paper in science, I think.
If you have trouble finding it, just drop me an email
and I'll give you the full reference.
And I don't know how he derived it, but he and his co-authors derived it.
But they have, they have in the graph that they have, they have a pattern of dots around the mean curve that they use to represent their impression of what the variation in climate has been.
They don't specify timescales.
I know this.
Yeah, I know this paper.
It's a great paper.
I think the dots are just data points.
I don't know what data they are.
It's all from deep sea cores, yeah?
Could be, yeah.
Yeah, I think that's right.
I think it's all from deep sea cores.
And the variant, the cloud of points around the mean gets much wider.
As you get to it.
And particularly during the Pleistocene, it just explodes.
But I think this would be the orbital scale variation.
If it's real data, it's orbital scale.
Yeah.
Milankovitch.
100,000 years. 20,ovitch. 100,000 years.
20,000 years, 41,000 years, 23,000 years, the shortest.
They aren't real cycles.
They're more complicated than that.
But they're orbital perturbations that come from the gravitational effect of mainly Jupiter on the Earth.
So that to a first approximation, the Earth and the other planets follow these orderly orbits, but under the massive influence of the gravitation of the sun. But Jupiter has enough gravitational force on the other planets
to perturb their orbits in a systematic but not exactly cyclical way.
And that's where you get the 100,000-year ellipticity
and the 41,000-year tilt and the the 23,000 year recession of the equinoxes
by the gravitational effects.
I suppose that Saturn and the other big planets also may have a measurable effect.
I didn't know that, that we could connect the variation back to the orbital perturbations.
But the orbital perturbations then are somehow filtered by,000-year, and the 23,000-year quasi-cycles, they sometimes say, they haven't changed as the Earth moved from the Pliocene to the Pleistocene, the early Pleistocene, and then the later Pleistocene.
Those orbital scales didn't change.
Something about the way the Earth responded to them is what generated the dominance of one cycle versus the other.
Right.
So, and just exactly what that all amounts to is at least still a mystery to me. I don't know if it's a mystery to
the paleoclimatologists or not, but I think it is. And what generates the millennial and
sub-millennial scale variation and why has it been increasing over at least the last 1.5 million years. One hypothesis I saw is speculative, I think, but it's that the North
American glacier is sort of the dominant ice mass during the high glacial episodes. And over the successive glacial cycles,
it's gradually been pointing the North American continent flatter.
So it reduces the friction.
So the reason that the amplitude and frequency have gone up is because the
sliding of the glacier off the North American continent has gotten faster and faster.
That's sort of, you know, for want of a nail, the kingdom is lost kind of causation.
But it easily could be.
Yeah, it's a mechanistic hypothesis.
Whether it's true or not, I'm not an expert enough to to have a uh
what do i want to say i'm i'm not enough of an expert to have an expert opinion
yeah that's fascinating nonetheless so everything we've spoken about thus far raises the for me
raises the question of contingency so it like, you can correct me if this is inaccurate, but for
cultural brains to evolve took at least three, if you like, exogenous shocks. One was apes becoming
bipedal about 5 million years ago. The second was the large amount of predation on the savannas in Africa.
I think I read that there was about twice the level of predation there is today,
both in terms of the number and the type of predators.
And then the third factor is the Pleistocene climate variation we've spoken about.
Do you see all of those ingredients as just independent factors? And we
kind of won the lottery in a sense in that we had these pre-adaptations, our lineage had become
bipedal apes, were under the threat of lots of predation and the selection pressures that was creating for
grouping socially. And then just as that was happening, we entered this period of
climate variation on a millennial and sub-millennial scale.
Or are those three factors somehow correlated? Maybe bipedalism was driven by the climate variation in some sense.
Maybe apes had to change their foraging strategies because of
the forests growing and shrinking or something like that.
So, how contingent was the evolution of cultural brains?
I think it's really contingent. I'd add a bunch of other things.
What would you add?
Well, I think this, so humans, australopithecines are bipedal and that plays constraints on brain size because as brains get bigger, there's more obstetric complications. And that led to, so in early Homo,
there seems to have been a shift to a more predatory lifestyle.
So now we've got apes out on the savannah.
They're bipedal.
Heads are little, so that's not a problem. Then they start hunting they start hunting females can't hunt with highly dependent
offspring and that leads to changes in social organization and cooperative breeding so
cooperative breeding that you know that's probably potentiates cultural transmission because there's more individuals together and in social groups.
Also, it brings males more firmly into the...
Exactly.
So, males have to provision females.
And fathers have to teach their kids or boys to hunt.
Yeah.
So, there's a bunch of things, I think, all of which could have gone some other way.
If, you know, it's, when I teach this in class, I say it's like little Eliza jumping from one ice flow to another.
You know, it's just a bunch of stuff happened and it ended up, you know, where we are.
But it didn't have to be that way at all
yeah wow that's better be my story yeah i would say basically the same thing so but if you look
at any evolving lineage you look backwards it's you know it's one crazy thing after another. So, you know, horses started as these little forest browsers and as the climate got drier, more open, then you had a whole revolution, Miocene revolution in mammals. origin of the antelope and bovids and all these efficient grass-eating herbivores that
exploited the more open vegetation browsers that could, if the trees have their leaves
way the hell above the ground, then mammals, there are no mammals that get up there very much.
But if you've got low trees and shrubs and things, then you've got a bunch of browsing specialists that can get after the vegetation.
And then there are top-down effects of the grazers and the browsers on the ecosystems.
And so if you look back in history, it's just one damn thing after another.
So historical contingency plays a big role.
And that's not opposed to an adaptationist perspective at all, I don't think. I mean, Gould kind of, back in the day, tried to make those, you know,
if you're an adaptationist, then you should think you always end up in the same place.
And I just, I think there's no reason to think that.
You know, all selection cares about is now.
And so it goes, you know, on the surface, you know, Wright had this idea of adaptive topography.
It's got peaks everywhere and you're just going.
And they're dynamic.
And they're moving around like the ocean.
And you're climbing.
Selection is causing lineages to climb as best they can.
And where they climb depends on where they are.
And so, I just, yeah, I think.
Yeah.
Yeah.
I don't, you know, so this guy at Oxford,
what's his name, Simon Conway Morris,
isn't he the guy who thinks that, you know,
if dinosaurs hadn't got extinct,
we'd have bipedal big brain dinosaurs.
And maybe, I doubt it though, myself.
Yeah.
Well, it's interesting that uh people challenged
me about this I suppose Rob too I mean if your story is correct why don't why don't we have lots
of uh uh animals with big brains and cultural cumulative cultural adaptations we're all
all of us are living in the all in the same Pleistocene environment.
Why is it just humans that have got this massive?
Yeah, absolutely.
And that has to do with something like, you mentioned it, ape pre-adaptations,
where apes are already fairly big-brained.
They already have a certain amount of culture, we assume, in the last common ancestor with the living apes.
The apes all live in fairly large, fairly sophisticated societies.
All of these things, you can imagine, are potential pre-adaptations. And then we get the australopithecines that are bipedal and their forelimbs can turn into technology making and technology using devices.
And carrying.
And carrying.
And carrying is a big deal, right?
Because you invest in a tool and you're a quadruped, you got to throw it away.
Unless you carry it in your mouth, you're done.
And whereas humans could carry a spear around all day. throw it away when you know unless you carry it in your mouth you're done and uh whereas you know
humans could carry a spear around all day and uh um yep chimps actually carry stuff like this
kevin was telling me oh really between the cheek and their shoulders oh Oh, I see. Yeah. They carry, they make sticks and
and
stones they use
to crack nuts
and stuff.
And,
but they can't carry them
very far.
It's,
you know,
it's too,
it's too inefficient.
And,
so,
yeah,
I mean,
I would think
life's like that, right?
It's my picture of evolution is it's endlessly dynamic.
Yeah. this progressive idea of evolution, which is, I think, widespread in the public,
is that it just took a long time to get humans starting with bacteria.
And everybody be a human if they could.
Every lineage would have a big brain if they found a way to get there.
This seems to me to be the wrong way to think about it.
We have this extremely expensive adaptation that evolved in response to these climate
variations, and it's not the best adaptation in some absolute sense.
It's just something that works in the present-day environment.
So I've got some questions about intelligence and artificial intelligence,
and then some questions about the fertility crisis.
But to start with intelligence,
one of the interesting things that's been happening with human brains is that they've been shrinking over the last 10,000 years.
Controversial.
The latest paper I saw on that attempted to do the statistical analysis right.
If I understand it correctly, there are two major problems.
One is that there just aren't that many Pleistocene last ice age skulls.
And even worse, there are very few last ice age post-cranial skeletal materials.
So, Pleistocene people were probably quite a bit bigger on average.
Meaty diets, hunting and gatherings, athletic occupation.
And so, they were probably, I think they're known to have been quite a bit bigger, but not just how much is a problem. So those are the statistical difficulties that make it really hard to be sure.
So any difference is pretty slight.
So that, I think, is the problem.
Oh, interesting.
And the Pleistocene fossil record is heavily biased to Europe. Oh, interesting. And so I think you have to be really careful with what was going on in Southeast Asia or tropical Africa 30,000 or 40,000 years ago.
We have only the slimmest record because those environments don't produce fossils very readily.
They don't produce archaeologists very well.
Well, that's the other thing.
There's the croissant effect, which is it's a lot more fun to work, dig up fossils in
southern France than it is in the Congo Basin for lots of archaeologists.
And so I read once that there were 50 sites in Europe for every site in Sub-Saharan Africa, archaeological sites.
So, you know, I would be skeptical.
Generalizations about world phenotypes.
But go ahead.
It's a good story.
It should be true, according to our theory. According to the cultural brain hypothesis, because culture is substituting for encephalization.
So, on the other hand, what has transpired in the Holocene, I think, compared to the last Ice Age,
is people have adapted ever more finely to local ecological variation.
So we have all of these agricultural systems that are finely adapted to local environments.
And hunting and gathering is a much more generic kind of subsistence strategy that isn't so highly tuned to particular environments.
So, I think that we, so to speak, in the Pleistocene, at least in the last ice age, our adaptive
effort, so to speak, was mainly adapting to temporal variation. And in the Holocene, it switched to mainly being focused
not on the slight temporal variation, but the geographical variation.
As we spread across the Earth.
Yeah, well, we were already pretty widespread,
except for the Americas until the very end of the Pleistocene.
Yeah.
But, yeah, particularly agricultural crops and animals are pretty sensitive to the local environment right down to the variety level.
So when you adapt to a new environment, you have to adapt your starchy staples and you have to select for new varieties that will be adapted to the local environment.
So, that may have had a tendency to keep our brains from atrophying too dramatically.
Our teeth have also gotten much smaller.
Right.
Over the same time scale. because of our addiction to cooking
so
if the birth canal is the major constraint on encephalization in humans at the moment. What happens when... So I think in the last couple of decades, cesareans have increased
from about one quarter to one third of births in America, and they've maybe doubled worldwide.
Obviously, they still carry a lot of risks. Maybe some even better technology is diffused,
like artificial wombs or something, who knows,
something like that. What would happen at that point to human brain sizes? Would
selection pressures continue to push for larger and larger brains, or would it break the other way?
And because of cumulative culture, the pressure for larger and larger brains is no longer
what it was.
So if we remove the constraint of the birth canal, what happens to brain sizes?
Well, that's only one constraint.
There's a metabolic constraints in the other ones
that we talked briefly about earlier,
fragility of the skull and things like that.
But presumably culture helps us obviate those other...
We're pretty well fed these days in some cultures anyway.
Right.
Some countries.
Well, famously, evolution is not a very successful predictive science.
So I think you're asking a very hard question that contingencies will intervene. And even if we knew exactly how we would respond
to a particular selective pressure,
we would then have to predict the selective pressures.
And that is hard to do.
I mean, we can make short-term predictions
about the evolution of pathogens, for example.
COVID is a guy named Paul Ewald.
Have you heard his story about this? I know Paul Ewald, but I don't know his story about COVID.
Well, it wasn't a story about COVID. It was a story about respiratory diseases in general. This was 10 or 15 years ago. He and I, a student, I don't know, he had a
co-author, and they looked, the critical variable is how long the infectious agent persists in the
environment. If it persists a long time in the environment, then it doesn't have to care very much about the health of the individual that gets sick.
So anthrax forms a spore, and so it lives for a very long period of time in the environment.
And it's a devastating disease if you catch it. Things like the common cold don't persist in the environment very long,
so they have an interest in not harming their host too much. And COVID, the initial, the time
that COVID spends alive in the environment is, I think, just a little longer than the average flu virus.
So the prediction from their model would be that COVID would evolve from being a very virulent disease
to one that was much less virulent, something on the order of the seasonal flu,
which is more or less what's happened. So those kind of short-term predictions can be quite successful.
And the evolution of certain kinds of adaptations that are tightly constrained by mechanical
principles, flying and swimming.
You can predict what sort of a pursuit strategy hawks have from the size and shape of their
wings.
In other words, if they're maneuvering tightly to catch things in a forested environment,
they'll have short, blunt wings.
If they're diving from great heights like peregrine falcons in an open environment,
they take a completely different strategy.
And so lots of things are predictable, but brain size in humans, I don't know.
And what the selective pressures are.
What the selective pressures will be, yeah.
I mean, you're going to talk about the fertility crisis.
It's not clear that
what it is that's driving differences in fitness.
Right.
Whether it's how clever you are or not.
Yeah.
I mean.
Oh, interesting.
Interesting.
I hadn't made that connection, but that makes a lot of sense. Yeah.
I mean, when it is how clever you are, then you might think there'd be the selection gradient would lead to larger brains and it, you know, there would be some kind.
It depends on everything, right?
On energy availability and everything.
But in modern societies, at least urban ones, I don't think it's so clear what selection is doing.
Well, I mean, we've all got all these crazy scientists running around publishing papers and neglecting their duties as fathers and
mothers. So the big brains are leading people to adopt, so to speak, crazy hobbies that
detract from their fitness. Climb mountains. Yeah, as you'll be doing next week, I guess.
Yeah.
Yeah.
So, maybe I'll, because we've segued into the fertility crisis, let's talk about that
now and then I'll come back to artificial intelligence after that.
So, your way of making sense of the demographic transition is one of the most fascinating and compelling that I've encountered in my reading on the topic.
Could you outline how you make sense of the demographic transition?
And then I'll ask some more specific, sophisticated questions about it.
I think this is your idea, Pete.
I think you...
Okay.
Well, so that in our 85 book, we have a little vignette about the demographic transition
in which we attribute it to basically to careers open to talent, that if education becomes a major source of economic
success and prestige, then people who spend a long time furthering their education and
start their families late will have an advantage in getting into prestige
positions like army officers and teachers and government officials and things like that.
So you're sacrificing your genetic fitness for your cultural fitness, so to speak. And that was our basic idea in the 85 book.
I think that the other thing that's in there is that it has to do with modernization, right?
So it's not wealth per se, income, that is the key thing.
It's the rearrangement of the economy and the society such that prestige roles are required delayed marriage and investment in credentials and stuff. The key difference there being you can become prestigious not through birthright,
but through effort and merit.
Yeah, but I think that goes, I mean, I wouldn't think it about kings and earls and stuff like that.
I mean, think about a village in a modernizing society.
Who are the prestigious people?
You know, the guys that have an outboard on their boat.
And I worked in Fiji a little bit.
And, you know, it's the teacher.
It's the local policeman.
It's people that have some cash salary.
It's not the best farmer in town.
That's where it was before.
It used to be the guy who could really grow yams better than anybody else.
But now- Feed six kids instead of four.
Feed six kids instead of four. And to get those roles, you have to go to high school in Fiji,
you have to learn English. All these things require investments that are different from the investments in producing kids. And so the data suggests that modernization is a better predictor of the demographic transition than GDP per capita or something like that.
So the former Soviet countries, for example, never got very rich, but they modernized in the sense that education became universal.
Prestige roles were achieved through education.
So, the key change is not so much moving to meritocracy, but it's opening up those channels of non-parental transmission.
Exactly.
Yeah, I mean, in the old days, it was just the village, right?
And now people are going to town sometimes,
and they're seeing who's in charge there and how cool it is.
And I don't think, I mean, these are still highly kin-based.
I mean, relatedness still matters for a lot of things, but less than it used to.
Yeah. is that sort of a generalization of the original argument that we had, I think,
is that social networks have changed dramatically.
So the proportion of kin and social networks is declining,
and the proportion of factory mates, office mates, friends
that don't have much interest in your reproductive success.
In fact, that might be a handicap on your friendship with the guys you go to the bar
with on Saturday night.
Can't buy as many beers.
Yeah, those dads that stick at home and don't buy their buddies beers anymore.
We're not going to encourage that kind of behavior.
Yeah.
So, as Rob says, I think modernization is the sort of master variable here.
The switch from subsistence farming or nearly subsistence farming to factory work and office work and urban life.
One of the predictions that I would love to see somebody test would be, I'd predict the same thing for urban expansions in the past.
So if we could go back to Tenochtitlan or rome and get good measures of of
fertility i'd predict the urban people would get sucked up into the same thing because they have
these wider social networks prestige networks are um yeah yeah i think rome was really good at just
continuing to suck the population in.
All those cities were because they were death traps.
Yeah.
So that's partly.
That confounds a little bit.
Moving to the city was a little bit like a suicide.
But still.
Still is to an extent. You know, I mean, and some of these, you know, one of my, I'd like to see somebody, there is demographic data for some of those places.
Somebody needs to really work that out. than I, that isn't there some decent data for early modern or medieval Jews in Italy that they underwent a precocious demographic transition that they had?
Maybe exactly who you'd predict.
That would be the kind of people that you would predict. On the other hand, we have these North American Anabaptists that are
going after it. I did a back-of-the-envelope calculation once, and I guessed that
projecting present demographic parameters that in about 200 years, half of North Americans
will be Anabaptists. They'll be Mormons instead.
No, the Mormons.
I looked at the Mormons when I first got interested in this.
And the Mormon demographic transition is happening.
It's mostly happened by now.
My former student who's a Mormon, an observant Mormon, has three kids.
Two kids.
What's that?
He has two kids?
Three kids.
Three kids.
Adrian, yeah.
So he's still a little above the average, but not dramatically. One argument about the urban phenomenon is that there's some deep genetically evolved switch in our brains that once we're surrounded by density and crowds of people, it causes us to downregulate our fertility in response.
Does that seem likely to you?
Well, one of the observations that people make of hunter-gatherer subsistence camps is they're tightly crowded together.
They don't live miles away from each other.
In a given camp, they're tight.
They're often 50 people.
Yeah.
Sort of living in each other's hair, so to speak.
Yeah.
Cheek by jowl.
It's denser than to speak. Yeah. Cheek by Joe. Denser than the village.
Yeah.
Well, I guess that largely dispenses with that hypothesis.
Yeah, it depends.
They could be averaging.
I mean, they're off.
They're quite mobile.
I don't know.
Yeah.
I'm skeptical of those kind of arguments.
So, in your model of or your understanding of the demographic transition, there are different cultural forces that are contributing to the spread of the maladaptive fertility-reducing cultural variant.
One being content biases of which Gary Becker's kind of rational choice model would be a special case.
Yeah. of which Gary Becker's kind of rational choice model would be a special case. Am I right in thinking that prestige bias is the most important cultural force relative to the others?
We don't know.
It'd be interesting to try to actually put numbers on that because Leslie's kin influence hypothesis also suggests that when you're having beers with your workmates rather than with your brother, it's not that your workmates have any particular prestige, but they're, if you, but they're diluting the,
your social network and people that are, that are interested in your, in your fitness. In other
words, your relatives tend to, they, they ask you, geez, you know, you guys should have a kid,
shouldn't you? You don't get that kind of, you get that thing from grandmothers and
mothers and fathers and, and maybe uncles and brothers and things. You don't get that kind of, you get that thing from grandmothers and fathers and maybe uncles and brothers and things.
You don't get that from your workmates so much.
So it isn't necessarily only a prestige thing.
So it'd be interesting to try to get a quantitative handle.
It might be something you could get a quantitative handle on. Now, the Anabaptists, to take an example where the demographic transition is not happening,
I mean, they do both things.
They seal off internal prestige hierarchy, and they seal their prestige system off from the rest of us quite tightly.
I mean, things like movies, I mean, they seal themselves culturally off, and they maintain small communities that are rich in kin.
So they are working on both of those things simultaneously.
Some, like the Hutterites, are quite well off.
I mean, economically.
Yeah, well, they have to be economically well off to raise seven kids.
And buy all the property of the kids.
Well, the Hutterites, actually, I've read.
So the demographers don't do retail science.
I mean, they're not like anthropologists.
They don't go out there and count heads in the villages.
They depend upon government data. So the actual data on the Anabaptists is
quite poor. But I read it said that the Hutterites are actually reducing their fertility some because their style of farming is very modern and requires a huge capital investment.
They don't only got to buy land, they got to buy tractors and all of the pickup trucks and all of the accoutrements of modern farming. And they have just been growing too rapidly
to be able to divide the colony on the timescale
that they think is appropriate.
So they're scaling back their fertility some,
not a lot, but some.
So completed family size is more like five or six than seven,
something on that order.
The Amish, at least, have gone into wage labor on a large scale.
So what they do is they make a deal with some capitalist entrepreneur that they'll furnish the labor force for some kind of factory.
And Amish are, you know, they're pretty skilled people.
They grow up on a farm.
They can fix machinery and they can build stuff.
And so they build things like Winnebago's and other semi-skilled manufacturing jobs that are consistent with their relatively low level of education.
So, and the tourism in some parts of the Amish land like Lancaster County, Pennsylvania, that is a big absorber of labor. So they've, to some extent, shifted out of the farming business,
maybe in part because of land costs, although some of them have moved further east, excuse me,
further west into Ohio and places like that, where they, Illinois, where they can more easily afford
the land. But they still isolate themselves from... The cultural isolation.
So the whole deal on these factories is that the entrepreneur will
hire only Amish labor. So that the Amish don't have to mix with
non-Amish on the job. The entrepreneur sometimes I think is not Amish, but other than that,
they don't have to
their cultural isolation is not threatened on the job.
Right. Interesting. I read their population
I think is doubling every 20 years or something like that. Something like that.
Yeah. It'd be interesting to see whether the world's The population, I think, is doubling every 20 years or something like that. Something like that, yeah.
It would be interesting to say.
Completed family.
One of the world's Amish in a few centuries.
Yeah, well, I mean, the other group that's a little like this is the ultra-Orthodox Jews. And they're numerous enough now in Israel to be a political problem or a political force.
Let's put it that way.
Not to prejudge, but who knows?
It's hard to predict what will happen to that whole phenomenon. But the way that... I just want to focus on the prestige by a story
because I do find this a very
powerful explanation.
The way it works is
in modern society
to become prestigious
it helps to have...
It often helps to have fewer kids
because then you can extend your education,
focus more of your time and efforts
on career advancement.
And the people who become prestigious
are the people who are most likely to be imitated.
And so in modernity, the people being imitated
happen to be people who typically have fewer children
than in pre-modern societies.
Just so I'm clear, is the imitation happening for reduced fertility per se,
or is it more happening for the package of behaviors that produce success
of which reduced fertility is a byproduct.
I'd bet with number two.
Number two.
Byproduct.
Yeah, that's certainly the argument we made in the 85 book.
Interesting.
Because at least today, anecdotally, if I speak to female friends,
they'll tell me that there is a sense that having lots of kids early is uncool.
Calling it socially taboo is probably pushing it too far. But it does seem like there has also
been a norm that's kind of congealed around having less kids per se, in addition to the
package of things that lead to career success.
Yeah, especially in East Asia seem to have carried this to Koreans.
So isn't, I think Korea has the, maybe the lowest or one of the lowest TFRs in the world.
It's down to like one point something.
Or even below one.
Sorry, I think it might be 0.9.
Yeah, that's what my recollection is.
So, I mean, again, there's the prestige angle
and then there's the social network effect.
And those two
were partly decoupled
so they're both effects are
operating in the same direction
and things get
turned into norms
I mean I don't know if this
is so that
there's quite a bit of evidence that people just look
around and they say what's everybody doing
you know so that must be the right thing to do.
And so common behaviors become moralized or prestigized.
And there's, I mean, guys like Granta Veteran,
there's a bunch of sociologists that have unmotivated empirical data that
suggests that's, you know, they don't start from where we're starting.
I noticed this in myself.
Somebody tells me that they have five kids.
You think, wow, that's a lot.
So the data on things are, you know, so if you tell kids, oh, you shouldn't drink, you know, drinking's bad, that has no effect.
But if you tell kids nobody's drinking, that has a big effect on people who've done experiments.
And there's a bunch of pluralistic ignorance. So everybody, well, not everybody, but the average kid on college campus thinks that other people are having a lot more fun than they are and are drinking more and having more sex and all this stuff.
So if you tell people what's actually going on, it motivates them to drink less.
Yeah.
Maybe the process starts with model-based transmission and then conformist bias puts the final nail in the coffin.
There's conformist bias too.
Conformist bias would just make it more common.
But I think we haven't written anything about this,
but I mean, a good general rule would be
if you're a little kid and you're growing up,
if everybody's doing something,
that must be what we're supposed to do around here.
Yeah, just like a Bayesian.
Yeah, exactly.
Yeah, yeah.
So I've got three more questions on the fertility crisis.
I think these are even the most important questions.
So, okay, a few disturbing facts.
The demographic transition began in the West about 1870,
but obviously it happened unevenly across countries.
As far as I know, no Western country has reversed its demographic transition.
I believe that's true, yes.
Moreover, fertility declines have also penetrated
into the lower socioeconomic levels of society within
western countries and then in addition lots of non-western countries have begun their demographic
transitions almost all almost all china and india are below replacement you mentioned south korea
vietnam bunch of bunch of countries in south america just not af. Sub-Saharan Africa. Yeah, Africa seems largely unaffected at this stage.
I'm not sure that's true.
The data I've seen suggests that it's proceeding much more slowly in Africa
than it has proceeded very rapidly in East Asia, but it's very slow, if not stalled, in parts of sub-Saharan Africa.
Modernization is much slower in sub-Saharan Africa, too.
Yeah, right. reproducing behaviors don't seem to be being fixed very quickly imply that cultural group
selection has weakened and that we have low variation. Why would cultural group selection
favor larger families? That's the assumption behind that question maybe there's a different
path into the question
I think the assumption
is
if this
isn't being
fixed quickly
does that imply
that we're in some
kind of like global
macro culture
Mac
I'm sorry
oh I see
I guess the archetype
is like the Davos-style elite.
But because we're all interconnected through telecommunications technologies.
Yeah.
We do have a macroculture in the sense that urban life, bureaucratic government.
Hollywood movies.
Educational systems that take a lot of time.
That's shared more or less to varying degrees across the world.
And development agencies are trying as hard as they can to make everybody else do it.
So am I making an analytical error?
Could we infer from the fact that the demographic transition isn't being fixed in all these different countries
that cultural evolution is happening too slowly and therefore there must be low variation?
Well, the trouble is you mentioned non-parental transmission.
So, culture in lots of cases will result in
adaptations that would fix problems with genetic fitness, but that's not guaranteed at all. So, cultural evolution can favor fitness diminishing behavior.
Selection on cultural variation will not necessarily be correlated with selection on genetic fitness.
For sure. Yeah. And so, I mean, globally, overall, in human evolution,
it had to have been true that there was some kind of correlation
between cultural fitness and genetic fitness,
or else culture wouldn't have evolved, right?
It would have been extinguished by selection.
That doesn't have to have been true for even the last
10,000 years. No.
Especially if you think brains are getting smaller.
I mean,
I don't think, I don't see any
clear reason to predict
that cultural evolution should favor
higher genetic fitness, except
that our brains have evolved to,
you know, we like sweet things
and we don't want to die
and lots of fitness-enhancing things.
But if cultural evolution has finessed that
by creating prestige systems,
I don't see any reason it can't persist.
It might persist to extinction of humans.
Well, I mean, there's all these,
you know, there's the so-called Irish elk case,
which is probably not true,
but, you know, sexual selection can lead
to ridiculous ornaments that reduce average fitness.
Like the peacock's tail is the classic example.
Yeah, in theory.
I don't think there are actually good examples of this.
Right.
But, and the classic example is the Irish elk,
which is this extinct species of elk that had these,
I mean, truly enormous antlers.
And some people have, but I don't,'t you know they're long gone and who knows but
yeah um lots of animals became extinct at the end of the pleistocene um
mostly at the end of a spear i imagine but yeah so anyway i i just don't
so on the you know on the other hand uh if you want to think about the Anabaptists, the correction is going right along, right?
Yeah, but...
They only take less than a millennium.
Yeah, and they're much more powerful, technologically much more powerful.
I mean, this may happen in Israel, right?
That secular Israelis will get tired of this and squash them.
Yeah, so that's one scenario.
Who knows? Who knows?
But yes, I don't think there's any law-like process that will force a strong correlation between cultural fitness and genetic fitness.
For sure.
They're, I think, important weak forces.
I think of the emotional system, for example, as being the main sort of corrector.
Appetites and emotions.
You mentioned food.
Kids.
I mean, people love kids.
Yeah, people do love kids.
We love our kids.
Yeah, absolutely.
Not so many of them, but so, I mean, most people who have kids will tell you it's the
most important thing they ever did, even if they only had a couple. So if selection created a mechanism where you can get a lot of satisfaction from two kids, then it won't work very well.
It might have worked fine when there was natural fertility, but it doesn't work anymore.
We've got a lot of people in the world, so uh uh we got a lot of scope for uh
something happening before we go extinct right yeah so there is there is indeed some variation
there are also a lot of projections that show global population peaking around but no there's
there's a un projection shows it peaking around 2080.
How much does that worry you from the perspective of we need a large and increasing cultural
brain, collective brain rather, in order to sustain a technologically advanced civilization?
I know, Rob, you did some work on fishing technologies in Oceania and the islands that were more populous and better connected
had a greater number of fishing technology types
and more complex fishing technologies. That's all
true. There's also obviously the salutary lesson of the
Tasmanian Aborigines in Australia when
the sea levels rose and cut them off from the mainland, fully isolated them about 8,000 years ago.
They slowly lost their technologies.
By the time Europeans encountered them, they had a toolkit of about 24 tools, which is the smallest toolkit in human history.
No boats.
Yeah, yeah.
And as an interesting
I guess like
control as a natural experiment just across
the Basque Strait when the
local Aboriginal
clan or tribe there was
when European
contact was made with them,
they had like hundreds of tools in their toolkit.
So to the extent that we need a really large collective brain
to sustain a technologically advanced civilization,
a fertility declines worry,
or I guess we're recording this today
in the Internet Archive headquarters.
Now that we've digitized so much of culture,
is that like a countervailing force? how do you think about that problem an institutional i mean now in the
last when we talked about science a bit earlier but we now have institutions for innovating and
cultural evolution that are evolving on their own you know know, the engine of scientific and engineering progress,
I don't think depends so heavily on population size as the diffuse the exposure of any given individual to cultural ideas, particularly with regard to things like technology. So somebody who's got a double E degree or a computer science degree has been exposed to a huge amount of technical knowledge that they'd never pick up without formal education.
And much more efficiently than...
At least that's what we college professors claim. Pardon least that's what we college professors claim.
Pardon?
It's what we college professors claim.
Yeah, well.
Some people are skeptical.
ASU is trying to measure what kids actually learn.
Seems like a bad idea to me.
Might not like what you find out.
I don't know.
The other thing is the models, Joe's model and the Shannon.
Joe Henrich.
Joe Henrich has this, what he calls a treadmill model of this. And Steve Shannon and Mark Thomas and those guys had a more like a drift-based,
a random loss model.
Joe's, well, it doesn't matter.
The point is they both show very striking diminishing returns.
So you get a big effect for when populations go from 100 to 1,000,
a smaller effect, 1,000 to 10,000.
Oh, interesting.
And we saw that in our, so those plots you see in our data are on log-log scales.
Oh, okay.
And if you actually plot them out in linear scales, they nose over because the sampling, so they're both based on sampling.
So that Joe's model works.
You learn something. But, you know, as we know,
students don't learn everything from their teachers. So, things tend to go backwards a little bit through each cultural transmission process. But it's just like the experiment I
talked about earlier. That's exactly like the experiment I talked about earlier. By copying the most successful people, that brings the mean back up again, and there's some balance.
There's a square root of n factor in the models, literally?
So Joe's model is very stylized, actually it has a threshold and either
technology goes off to infinity
or it goes to zero. But if you put
in
it takes
longer to learn more complicated things.
So Alex Massoudi
modified the model in that way
and it shows
diminishing returns just
like you'd think., to population size.
And so I think $10 billion is a lot of people.
And if we were down to $100,000, I would be more worried.
Interesting.
Interesting.
And then couple that with the fact that we've made this, I think, made this, well, I know, we've made the system much more efficient by routinizing it and institutionalizing it and made it less sensitive, I think, to these diminishing returns kinds of things.
Interesting. I mean, I think specialized institutions where incentives have been norms and incentives have evolved to create more efficient accumulation on the Internet has been a big revolution to me.
I mean, I can read far more papers if I can harvest them off the Internet than I have to traipse down to the library and photocopy them.
The citation rate of articles in books.
Yeah, because you can't grab them off the internet whereas journal articles you know yeah so yeah so a couple of questions about
artificial intelligence does your understanding of how human intelligence evolve give you any unique insights into how artificial intelligence might
be created well uh i attended a conference on ai uh about six weeks ago or so and so i boned up
just a little bit uh uh and allison gottnick do you know who she is? She's a developmental psychologist here at Berkeley,
and she argues in a paper that artificial intelligence is nail culture,
that artificial intelligence is, you know,
these large language models have made the accumulated wisdom of the world
available in a very efficient way.
Now, it's also made the bullshit factor in culture equally exaggerated, right?
So it's easy to get.
That's why the large language models have these flaws is because they're tapping into the craziness? Individual-like learning models, models of innovation.
But those aren't integrated with the large language models.
And the kinds of success-based filters that we think are so important in making culture adaptive.
It's not clear that they're built into the current thinking on AI.
And other people make the point that AI has this problem
that it's computationally extremely expensive. So if we say that with Gopnik that AI is ace, the cultural part of it,
what about the cultural transmission kind of part of it?
What about the innovative part of it?
What about the quality filters part of it?
Can we integrate those into AI without making it prohibitively expensive?
Very unschooled thoughts.
Interesting nonetheless.
Do you have anything to add to that?
AI will be cultural.
Yeah.
It's got to be.
But how it'll all work is a mystery to me.
Yeah.
One thing I was contemplating was,
so I think the modal scenario for a lot of people
is you basically have billions of copies of AGI agents.
For example, if we create AGI.
And presumably, they will all be interacting
both with each other and
with humans and presumably they will be participating in our cultural evolution but
what's interesting is that they might have different like the forces of cultural evolution
might change in that scenario so presumably they will have less
transmission bias than humans and perhaps content how their learning psychology gets how teaching
psychology or psychology is are they teaching each other or people teaching them i mean who knows i mean yeah yeah i mean it could
easily be very pristine you know they they look around and see which bot is the most successful
and copy that one it could be yeah you should read allison's paper it's she's she's kind of a wicked
lady oh she's a smart one yeah she is and the guy i I mentioned earlier, Bill Thompson's part of the same group, intellectual stream.
Yeah.
And also a Berkeley.
And there's a whole bunch of those guys that are.
So Allison is a child development person. So she argues you're trying to replicate babies, and babies are already a much more efficient way of doing this than server farms burning fossil fuel as if it were not causing any real problems.
Right.
So that's why I say she's got a little wicked sense of humor.
You might enjoy that paper.
Yeah, yeah. She's got a great book about child development from a cognitive point of view with Laura Schultz.
And it's written, the first, the great part of the book, it's a bunch of papers too, but the great part of the book is this long intro that she and Laura wrote in the form of an email exchange between, so it's like a dialogue, right?
Between a mathematical cognitive scientist and a,
you know, experimental child development person like Allison.
And it's great.
I mean, highly recommended.
I'll check it out.
I don't know that.
Yeah.
Thanks for the recommendation.
She's a good one.
So I have a million
more questions for you guys which is obviously an indication of just how interesting and rich
your work is but we are running out of time we're about to get kicked out of this beautiful room
that we're recording in so my final question is you two for me are a very successful scientific dyad and there are many interesting examples of
other scientific dyads watson and crick being a famous one marion pierre curie there seems to be
something special about the number two that in certain contexts is perhaps more productive than
either lone researchers or group sizes of three or more.
And firstly, I'd like to just invite you to speculate on why that might be from an evolutionary perspective.
Like, is it maybe piggybacking on pair bonding
or is it a culturally evolved institution, the scientific dyad?
And then secondly, I'd like to understand how each of you thinks
about the other's comparative advantage in your scientific partnership.
So, Pete, how you think about Rob's comparative advantage and Rob, how you think about Pete's comparative advantage?
I think we're friends, right?
That's the forever.
We have been for 50 years.
Yeah, 50 years.
And 50...
Actually, 54 years.
Bill Hamilton's living room.
Yeah, right.
You met in Bill Hamilton's living room?
No, different Bill Hamilton.
Different Davis Bill Hamilton.
Oh, no.
Rob was a first-year graduate student the year I was a first-year faculty member and we met at a seminar at this eccentric professor's uh uh house this is high
70s orange shag carpet conversation pit a whole nine yards and uh well bill hamilton's another
whole story but i i think that's the we enjoy each other's company. But I think that's the, we enjoy each other's company.
And I think that's the main thing that, I mean, we have this great project that we put together.
But we could, I mean, it'd be easy to feel resentful that Rob got more credit than I did or vice versa.
And both of us have a certain eccentricities that would, in a different context, lead to
resentments or, I'm tired of this son of a bitch.
But we never went that way.
So I think that is really important.
I don't think it's, maybe it's evolution in some deep sense, but I think increasing the number of collaborators increases the diversity of skills and knowledge.
And that's very beneficial.
And we're going to talk about that in a second, I guess.
But the bigger you get,
the more the free rider problem raises its head.
And that, you know, that people get lazy,
people want credit, all those kinds of things.
And two may be somehow in the sweet spot between... Well, if someone's not pulling their weight in a partnership,
you know who it is.
Yeah, exactly.
Whereas if it's even three or four, then, I mean, you know, projects and stuff.
Both of us have participated in far larger projects.
Yeah.
Quite productive ones.
People goof off and stuff.
So there's that.
And I guess I'll start on the second part of your question, which is we bring, I think, somewhat different
complementary, but overlapping, but quite different skills. I'm more of a modeler and Pete knows
more than anybody I've ever met about everything. And at least everything important. Maybe not who wrote what poem, but, but, uh, but, uh, uh, you know,
science and especially the more environmental kinds of how, how the world works science.
I think bringing those two skills together was quite productive. Yeah, I agree with that. Yeah, when I had a friend in graduate school who was a quite accomplished population geneticist.
He's in the National Academy by now. And so when I got interested in cultural evolution, I knew my math skills were not up to, not without a hell of a lot of work, were not up to what was required.
So that's one of the reasons I first tried to interest Rob in this project.
Well, it wasn't a project in the beginning.
I was just looking for material for one lecture and it turned into a project.
Yeah.
Cavalli, Swartz, and Feldman.
Yep.
And so then it turned out that Rob's a hell of a good scientist.
You know, he thinks like a scientist is supposed to think. Skeptical
when he needs to be skeptical and
never takes any
bullshit off anybody.
Yeah, it's just, you know,
people are complicated
and the fit was good, I think.
And then we, by chance, I think, kind of,
we had a good idea.
Yeah. No, I think that's.
That's the main.
That is, I just stumbled on this thing.
Yeah, exactly.
And you could just see when we started talking about this, you could just see there's all this low-hanging fruit. and the evolutionary biologists were 75 years ahead of the social scientists
in this business of what a cultural adaptation was.
They were in the 19th century.
But they decided they weren't going to talk about people.
I mean, that's one of the things in…
The evolutionary biology.
Yeah.
So the evolutionary…
And the population geneticists don't talk about adaptations very much.
Very much.
And the evolutionary biologists who do talk about adaptations were in the thrall of the modern synthesis, I think.
They couldn't imagine that they needed anything fundamental besides genes as an information carrier. They could see that culture could do some things,
but the idea that it could be a fundamental force in human evolution
was just beyond them.
There's still plenty of geneticists and evolutionary biologists
who think that way, but it's changing.
I mean, the cultural evolution is getting, you know, when we started, it was like Chariots of the Gods, right?
I mean, it was crazy talk.
What do you mean cultural evolution?
And now it's, you know, you see it all the time.
I mean, PNAS is having a big 50-year celebration about 50 years of cultural evolution coming around on the guitar so
I don't know it's been a big success and that's really cool yeah when you were talking about your
complementarities Rob you mentioned that Pete has this encyclopedic knowledge and you have
more of the I guess the mathematical I don't think it's so much. I mean, I'm only a mediocre mathematician.
I have some skill at figuring out
how to make simple models of things that are...
The way someone described it to me
was that in the partnership,
Pete brings the variation
and Rob brings the selection.
That's, I think, I've actually said that myself.
I agree with that. Yeah, I have a
speculative tendency. Well, it's sometimes get out of control. It serves you well. It's been a great
pleasure speaking with both of you today. Thank you so much for coming on the podcast.
I've certainly enjoyed it. Yeah, it's fun. Thank you. Done.
Thanks so much for listening.
Two quick things before you go.
First, for show notes and the episode transcript,
go to my website, jnwpod.com.
That's jnwpod.com.
And finally,
if you think the conversations
I'm having are worth sharing,
I'd be deeply grateful
if you sent this episode or the show are worth sharing, I'd be deeply grateful if you
sent this episode or the show to a friend. Message it to them, email them, drop a link in a WhatsApp
group, or even better, blast it out on Twitter. The primary way these conversations reach more
people is through my listeners like you sharing them. Thanks again. Until next time. Ciao.